You are on page 1of 7

Quality Improvement

Checklists to Reduce Diagnostic Errors


John W. Ely, MD, Mark L. Graber, MD, and Pat Croskerry, MD, PhD

Abstract
Diagnostic errors are common and can care units. This article extends the evaluation. The purpose of this article is
often be traced to physicians’ cognitive checklist concept to diagnosis and to argue for the further investigation and
biases and failed heuristics (mental describes three types of checklists: (1) a revision of these initial attempts to apply
shortcuts). A great deal is known about general checklist that prompts physicians checklists to the diagnostic process. The
how these faulty thinking processes lead to optimize their cognitive approach, (2) basic idea behind checklists is to provide
to error, but little is known about how to a differential diagnosis checklist to help an alternative to reliance on intuition and
prevent them. Faulty thinking plagues physicians avoid the most common cause memory in clinical problem solving. This
other high-risk, high-reliability of diagnostic error—failure to consider kind of solution is demanded by the
professions, such as airline pilots and the correct diagnosis as a possibility, and complexity of diagnostic reasoning,
nuclear plant operators, but these (3) a checklist of common pitfalls and
which often involves sense-making under
professions have reduced errors by using cognitive forcing functions to improve
conditions of great uncertainty and
checklists. Recently, checklists have evaluation of selected diseases. These
limited time.
gained acceptance in medical settings, checklists were developed informally and
such as operating rooms and intensive have not been subjected to rigorous

Editor’s Note: A commentary on this article appears reduce cognitive errors have been checklists in hospitals, clinics, and
on page 279. implemented or even proposed. Decision emergency rooms.
support tools can be helpful, but unless
D iagnostic errors occur in medicine at
they are well integrated in the workflow,
they tend to be underused.6,7 Other Cognitive Processes in Diagnosis
an appreciable, though unknown, rate,
estimated to be in the range of 10% to suggestions include reflective practice8,9 Some insights on how checklists work
15%.1,2 Many of these errors are and training in metacognitive skills to come from studies in cognitive
inconsequential, but others result in recognize flaws in the intuitive “thinking” psychology related to the “dual-process”
substantial harm to patients. Diagnostic that underlies a substantial fraction of model of thinking and reasoning (Figure
errors are more likely to be preventable our diagnoses.10 1).18 This model proposes two basic modes
and more likely to result in patient harm of thinking. Type 1 processes are fast,
than are other types of medical errors.3,4 Given their success in other settings, it is reflexive, intuitive, and may operate at a
reasonable to suggest that checklists subconscious level. We perform many tasks
Diagnostic errors reflect breakdowns in might help reduce diagnostic errors. that involve complex decision making
our health care systems, our clinical Checklists are used by airline pilots in all without giving them much attention or
reasoning, or both.5 Solutions for the aspects of their work, but were not used thought, such as driving a car or
system-based problems are relatively easy routinely until the crash of a Boeing 299 performing a neurological exam. Provided
to envision, but few interventions to bomber in 1935, which resulted from a they are repeated on a regular basis, these
pilot’s simple oversight—failure to tasks are relegated to an automatic
release the elevator locks.11 Checklists are subconscious level, and if everything is as it
Dr. Ely is professor, Department of Family Medicine, seems, we perform well. In contrast, Type 2
University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa. used by other high-risk, high-reliability
professions, such as submarine crews and processes are analytic, slow, and deliberate.
Dr. Graber is chief of medicine, Department of They require focused attention.
Veterans Affairs Hospital, Northport, New York, and nuclear plant operators, to ensure
professor and associate chair, Department of Internal safety.12,13 Recently, physicians and
Medicine, State University of New York, Stony Brook, Clinical work involves many behaviors,
nurses have developed checklists to
New York. but most are overlearned and executed
ensure the completion of critical
Dr. Croskerry is professor of emergency medicine, through Type 1 processes. However, as
procedures in hospitals.11 For example, useful as Type 1 thinking can be, it is
Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada.
intensive care unit staff use checklists to vulnerable to error. When we are in
Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Ely,
Department of Family Medicine, 01291-D, PFP,
help prevent bloodstream infections and clinical situations that seem familiar, we
University of Iowa Carver College of Medicine, 200 ventilator-associated pneumonia,14 –16 are comfortable with our thoughts and
Hawkins Drive, Iowa City, IA 52242; telephone: and a recent international project cut
(319) 384-7533; fax: (319) 384-7822; e-mail: john-
may become overconfident.2,19 It is
ely@uiowa.edu. surgical deaths by half after introducing a exactly under these circumstances that
19-item checklist for operating rooms.17 checklists prove effective. For diagnosis,
Acad Med. 2011;86:307–313.
First published online January 18, 2011
The purpose of this article is to describe a generic checklists could force a reflective
doi: 10.1097/ACM.0b013e31820824cd potential role for checklists in avoiding check, and specific checklists could force
Supplemental digital content for this article is diagnostic errors and to argue for the consideration of “must-not-miss”
available at http://links.lww.com/ACADMED/A38. further development and evaluation of diagnoses.

Academic Medicine, Vol. 86, No. 3 / March 2011 307


Quality Improvement

Hard wiring diagnostic errors often result from a


Ambient conditions/Context
Task characteristics
previous incomplete or misleading
Age and Experience history. They can also result from
Aff ti state
Affective t t
Gender “upstream” problems—those involving
Personality previous encounters—such as
succumbing to the framing bias imparted
Type
1 (fast, by a previously suggested diagnosis. A
RECOGNIZED
reflexive) diagnosis acquires enormous inertia once
Processes
it is proposed and communicated, to the
Pattern
extent that subsequent physicians may
Patient Pattern Recognition Executive Dysrationalia discount or fail to consider other possible
Presentation Processor override Override*
Calibration† Diagnosis
diagnoses. A related problem involves
Repetition
“groupthink,” in which the chances of
error increase when the impressions of
Type one member of a group are too quickly
NOT
RECOGNIZED 2 (slow,
analytic) adopted by the others.23 Although there
Processes may be occasions when an excess of facts
Education
Training
and data can be deleterious,24 the more
Critical thinking common problem for busy clinicians is
g
Logical competence
p
Rationality insufficient time to obtain a
Feedback
Intellectual ability
comprehensive medical history, which
remains the foundation of reliable
Figure 1 A model for diagnostic reasoning based on dual-process theory. Adapted with diagnosis.
permission from Croskerry P. A universal model for diagnostic reasoning. Acad Med. 2009;84:
1022–1028. Type 1 thinking can be influenced by multiple factors, many of them subconscious
Perform a focused and purposeful
(emotional polarization toward the patient, recent experience with the diagnosis being
considered, specific cognitive or affective biases), and is therefore represented as multiple- physical exam. The initial hypotheses
channeled, whereas Type 2 processes are, in a given instance, single-channeled and linear. Type 2 that inevitably come to mind during the
override of Type 1 (executive override) occurs when physicians take a time-out to reflect on their first moments of the patient encounter
thinking, possibly with the help of checklists. In contrast, Type 1 may irrationally override Type 2 should identify elements of the
(dysrationalia override) when physicians insist on going their own way (e.g., ignoring evidence- subsequent physical exam that need
based clinical decision rules that can usually outperform them). special attention. However, we must also
* “Dysrationalia” denotes the inability to think rationally despite adequate intelligence.68 look for signs that might suggest alternate

“Calibration” denotes the degree to which the perceived and actual diagnostic accuracy correspond. diagnoses.25

Checklists could help us resist the biases errors in hospitals, clinics, and emergency Generate and differentiate initial
and failed heuristics that lead to rooms. The content of these checklists hypotheses with further history,
diagnostic errors20 (Table 1), and they will seem familiar and possibly even physical exam, and diagnostic tests.
could facilitate proposed techniques for insultingly obvious (e.g., “Obtain your Diagnostic errors commonly involve
improving diagnostic reasoning.2,10 Using own complete history”), but their routine problems related to diagnostic testing,26
generic and specific checklists, we are use in practice would be a major change and in a recent study testing-related
encouraged to for most physicians. After all, pilots no problems were a factor in over half the
longer feel insulted when reminded by cases.27 These problems can result from
• decrease reliance on memory, an error in the laboratory or radiology
their copilots to release the elevator locks.
• consider a comprehensive differential department itself, occurring at rates of
diagnosis for common symptoms, 2% to 4%, or an error in the pre- or
The general checklist
posttest period, occurring at rates of 10%
• step back from the immediate problem A general checklist provides a to 20%.28 For example, the wrong test
to examine our thinking process reproducible approach to diagnosis.21 List was ordered, the result was lost, or the
(metacognition), 1 offers an example of such a checklist. physician misinterpreted the result.28
Some of the items may seem overly basic,
• develop strategies to avoid predictable but many errors result from failures in Pause to reflect—take a diagnostic
bias (cognitive forcing functions), these areas.22 We sometimes forget the “time-out.” Short of seeking a second
• recognize our altered mood states that “dumb steps” in our work, precisely opinion in every case, reflecting on the
arise from fatigue, sleep deprivation, or because they are dumb—we do not plausibility of the working diagnosis may
other conditions and develop strategies articulate them, and we take them for be our best tool to avoid error.8,9 The two
to reduce their negative consequences granted.11 Each of the steps in this most common cognitive errors are
(affective forcing functions). checklist is discussed in detail in this context errors and premature closure.5,26
section. Context errors arise when a critical signal
is distorted by the background against
Diagnostic Checklists Obtain your own complete medical which it is perceived.24 A typical context
Here, we describe three types of checklists history. There is no substitute for error would be the assumption that
that could potentially reduce diagnostic obtaining your own history because abdominal pain reflects a problem with

308 Academic Medicine, Vol. 86, No. 3 / March 2011


Quality Improvement

Table 1
Cognitive Biases and Failed Heuristics Addressed by Diagnostic Checklists

Bias or heuristic Definition* Role of checklist


Anchoring The tendency to perceptually lock on to salient features Prompt physician to consider diagnoses other than the
of the patient’s presentation too early in the diagnostic initially favored one.
process and failing to adjust this impression in light of
later information.
...................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Availability The disposition to judge things as being more likely or Prompt physician to consider diagnoses other than
frequently occurring, if they readily come to mind. those that readily come to mind.
...................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Base-rate neglect The tendency to ignore the true prevalence of a Remind physician of the relative prevalence of diseases
disease, either inflating or reducing its base rate and in primary care for the patient’s complaint.
distorting Bayesian reasoning.
...................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Premature closure The decision-making process ends too soon; the Prompt physician to reopen the diagnostic process and
diagnosis is accepted before it has been fully verified. consider alternative diagnoses before discharging the
“When the diagnosis is made, the thinking stops.” patient.
...................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Representativeness restraint The physician looks for prototypical manifestations of Prompt physician to consider causes for the symptoms
disease (pattern recognition) and fails to consider other than the ones that readily fit the pattern.
atypical variants.
...................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Search satisficing The tendency to call off a search once something is Prompt physician to consider additional causes of the
found. complaint after something is found.
...................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Unpacking principle The failure to elicit all relevant information in Prompt physician to ask questions that might confirm
establishing a differential diagnosis. or rule out alternative diagnoses.
...................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Context errors The critical signal is distorted by the background Encourage physician to rethink assumptions and
against which it is perceived. maintain objectivity.
* Source: Croskerry P. Cognitive and affective dispositions to respond. In: Croskerry P, Cosby K, Schenkel S, Wears
R, eds. Patient Safety in Emergency Medicine. Philadelphia, Pa: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2009:219 –227.

the gastrointestinal tract without made at all, or if it can wait, because correlation between our perceived and
considering other possibilities, such as other decisions may take priority, such as actual diagnostic accuracy.
pneumonia, lead poisoning, or diabetic empiric therapy or hospital admission. A
ketoacidosis. Premature closure is our patient’s presentation often changes over Differential diagnosis checklists
tendency to stop considering problems time as the symptoms evolve. It may be The final common pathway for most
after we find an apparently adequate wise to hold off making a diagnosis32,33 diagnostic errors is our failure to consider
solution.29 Taking a diagnostic time-out and write “NYD” (not yet diagnosed) in the correct diagnosis.5,26 We argue that
would provide the opportunity to the record after the presenting this can be addressed by using a set of
• Consider the opposite: “Why can’t this symptom.34 We should avoid any differential diagnosis checklists. The
be something else?” Tests that rule out diagnostic label until our certainty is high differential diagnosis checklists
alternative possibilities are often more because dialogue and thinking often stop
valuable than tests that confirm our the instant a label is applied.35,36
original suspicion.25 List 1
• Use “prospective hindsight”: Derived Embark on a plan, but acknowledge Proposed General Checklist for
uncertainty and ensure a pathway for Diagnosis
from military planners, this technique
asks us to look into the future and see follow-up.37,38 We often just play the
• Obtain your own complete medical history.
what would happen if our diagnosis odds when we make a diagnosis.
Certainty is not a realistic possibility. The • Perform a focused and purposeful physical
was wrong. What did we miss, and exam.
what else should we have correct diagnosis often emerges over time
• Generate initial hypotheses and
considered?30,31 as test results become available or as the differentiate these with additional history,
patient’s symptoms and signs evolve. This physical exam, and diagnostic tests.
• Apply decision support tools: A growing longitudinal aspect of diagnosis mandates • Pause to reflect—take a diagnostic “time
number of Web-based differential that we reconsider an initial diagnosis at out.”
diagnosis generators are available, such as
later points in time.39 We strongly 䡩 Was I comprehensive?
DXplain (http://dxplain.org/dxp/dxp.pl),
advocate including the patient in this 䡩 Did I consider the inherent flaws of
Isabel (http://www.isabelhealthcare.com), heuristic thinking?20
process. We should tell the patient our
VisualDx (http://www.visualdx.com), 䡩 Was my judgment affected by any other
initial thoughts, make clear any
and PEPID (http://www.pepid.com). The bias?
low-tech counterpart is to employ a uncertainties, and lay out a concrete plan
䡩 Do I need to make the diagnosis now, or
systematic approach, which might for follow-up.37 Closing this loop by can I wait?
include a checklist. ensuring follow-up is a strategy that can 䡩 What is the worst-case scenario?
help improve the reliability of diagnosis • Embark on a plan, but acknowledge
An appropriate step at this point is to and provide key feedback to help uncertainty and ensure a pathway for
consider whether a diagnosis needs to be improve our “calibration”—the follow-up.

Academic Medicine, Vol. 86, No. 3 / March 2011 309


Quality Improvement

highlighted in List 2 and detailed in the checklists for 46 presenting complaints, beneficial to review the checklist for
Supplemental Digital Appendix (http:// such as chest pain, fatigue, cough, patients who do not respond to initial
links.lww.com/ACADMED/A38) have a dizziness, and so on. The checklists were treatment.
single purpose: to prompt the physician revised during two years of use in clinic.
to consider a comprehensive list of causes A six-minute video that demonstrates use We have not formally evaluated the
for the complaints that commonly of the differential diagnosis checklist is differential diagnosis checklists, but one
present diagnostic challenges. The available on YouTube (http://www. of the authors (J.E.) has noted anecdotal
checklists highlight diagnoses that should youtube.com/watch?v⫽uHpieuyP1w0). success from two years of using the
not be missed and those that are, in fact, The diagnoses are ordered according to checklists in practice. For example, a 90-
commonly missed.26 The development prevalence in primary care, despite the year-old woman with chronic obstructive
and focus of the differential diagnosis lack of supporting data, because pulmonary disease, coronary artery
checklists were based on published data prevalence may provide more disease, and metastatic ovarian cancer
and the authors’ experiences.26,40 diagnostically helpful information than presented to clinic with dyspnea. The
more traditional variables such as resident noted wheezes which cleared
One of the authors (J.E.) used published anatomy,43,45,46,48 pathophysiology,41,45 after two albuterol nebulizer treatments,
differential diagnoses41–48 to develop body system,44,45,47,48 or medical but the patient continued to complain of
specialty.42,48 dyspnea. She had been seen four days
earlier with a “COPD exacerbation” and
List 2 It would require thousands of checklists was discharged from clinic after
Example of Differential Diagnosis to cover 100% of presenting complaints. symptomatic improvement with a single
Checklist albuterol treatment. The attending
Instead, we aimed to cover 99% of those
patients who present diagnostic physician (J.E.) reviewed the dyspnea
Sinus tachycardia
challenges with a small number of checklist with the resident, and this
• Anxiety, emotional stress prompted a d-dimer test. The d-dimer
checklists. And within each checklist, our
• Pain
goal was to cover at least 99% of patients was 13.89 ␮g/mL (normal: ⬍0.50 ␮g/
• Recent physical exertion with a short list of causes for the mL). A computerized tomographic
• Chronic obstructive pulmonary disease complaint. We excluded complaints in angiogram showed pulmonary emboli,
• Infections, fever*† which the focus is more on treatment and the patient was admitted to the
• Pregnancy (10 to 20 beats per minute at than diagnosis, such as diabetes and hospital and started on heparin.
term)* hypertension, and we excluded However, this example should be viewed
• Drugs (alcohol, amitriptyline, complaints for which a list of causes cautiously because it occurred against a
amphetamines, amyl nitrite, would be unlikely to benefit clinicians, background of many checklist reviews
anticholinergics, atropine, beta-blocker that did not alter the initial diagnosis and
withdrawal, bupropion, caffeine, cilostazol, such as constipation and breast lumps.
cocaine, ephedrine, epinephrine, many that led to further testing with
isoproterenol, nicotine, tobacco)† We lumped diagnoses into clinically negative results.
• Diabetic cardiovascular autonomic relevant groups rather than splitting
neuropathy them into distinct pathologic entities Cognitive forcing checklists for specific
• Myocardial infarction*† (e.g., “pneumonia” rather than diseases
• Pulmonary embolus*† “pneumococcal pneumonia,” “klebsiella Checklists can serve as cognitive forcing
• Pneumonia*† pneumonia,” and so on). We also functions— critical elements in the
• Anemia*† grouped presenting problems (e.g., execution of a process to ensure that a
• Hemorrhage* “abdominal/pelvic pain” rather than correct procedure is followed, or to
“right-upper-quadrant pain,” “right- prevent an untoward event.49 For
• Hypotension, shock*
lower-quadrant pain,” and so on) example, a customer using an automatic
• Hypovolemia, dehydration*
because we wanted to avoid redundancy. teller machine cannot withdraw cash
• Hyperthyroidism*†
For example, if we did not group until the card is removed. Thus, the error
• Hypoglycemia* presenting problems in this way, of leaving the card in the machine is
• Heart failure, pulmonary edema* pneumonia would have to appear on the avoided. If the checklist is always built
• Cardiomyopathy, myocarditis* right-upper-quadrant-pain checklist, the into diagnostic thinking, then it becomes
• Pericarditis* right-lower-quadrant-pain checklist, and a forcing function—the final diagnosis
• Acute mitral regurgitation* many others. cannot be made until the checklist has
• Pneumothorax* been reviewed. Cognitive forcing can be
• Aortic insufficiency* Although the checklists were developed generic or specific. In the generic sense,
• Hypoxia* in the outpatient setting, they may also an overarching planning principle is
improve diagnostic accuracy for applied (List 1). For example, the
• Serotonin syndrome*
inpatients. Admitted patients generally “ROWS” (rule out worst-case scenario)
• Inappropriate sinus tachycardia
come with “admitting diagnoses,” but strategy ensures that the worst
• Postural orthostatic tachycardia syndrome
hospitalist physicians could review the possibilities always receive consideration.
• Chronic nonparoxysmal sinus tachycardia checklist at the time of admission to help In the specific sense, checklists may help
• Pheochromocytoma determine whether further history taking, avoid predictable pitfalls for particular
* “Don’t-miss” diagnosis. physical exam, or diagnostic testing is diseases (List 3). Although errors of

Commonly missed diagnosis. indicated. They also might find it commission are typically more visible

310 Academic Medicine, Vol. 86, No. 3 / March 2011


Quality Improvement

address treatment rather than diagnosis. use before further revision based on
List 3 Diagnostic algorithms help physicians rigorous methods. Instead we are
Example of a Disease-Specific make rapid testing decisions, but they promoting the need to study and test
Cognitive Forcing Checklist usually do not provide comprehensive checklists as a potential method for
differential diagnoses. Differential preventing diagnostic errors. Checklists
Ankle injury diagnosis textbooks contain more than of actionable procedures might have
• Differential diagnosis simple lists, and their purpose goes enough face validity to make such testing
䡩 Ankle sprain beyond simple prompting. Commercial unnecessary or even unethical.60,63 For
䡩 Delayed onset muscle soreness decision support tools, such as Isabel example, Balas and colleagues60
䡩 Achilles tendon injury (partial or (www.isabelhealthcare.com) and questioned the ethics of allowing patients
complete) and tendinitis Problem-Knowledge Couplers to participate in a usual-care arm (i.e., no
䡩 Ankle or foot fracture (www.pkc.com), use patient-specific data safety intervention) in clinical trials of
䡩 Acute gout to provide patient-specific differential safety innovations.60 Similarly, airline
䡩 Peroneal tendon syndromes (tendinitis, diagnoses.6 These tools seem superior to pilots did not formally test their
subluxation, tears) generic checklists because they narrow checklists before adopting them. Instead,
• Forcing functions the list of diagnoses to those that are they learned from their mistakes and
䡩 Assess for neurovascular compromise most likely for a particular patient. made thousands of incremental changes
(cold foot or paresthesia) However, decision support systems have to prevent them.63 However, diagnostic
䡩 Consider stress films for ankle stability not been widely adopted in practice,52,53 checklists may have a greater potential for
䡩 Ankle and foot X-rays if indicated they suffer from an inadequate harm than preflight or surgical checklists.
䡩 Anterior drawer test knowledge base,6 they can be difficult to For example, they could lead to excessive
䡩 Talar tilt test incorporate into the workflow,6,54 –56 and consultation or needless testing (although
䡩 Squeeze test their ability to improve diagnostic most serious errors result from doing too
䡩 Thompson test performance is promising but still being little rather than too much22).
䡩 Peroneal tendon stability test
evaluated.6,57,58
For most patients, diagnostic checklists
• Pitfalls
Other interventions similar to checklists seem unnecessary. Preflight checklists
䡩 Missed neurovascular injury (suspect if
cold foot or paresthesias) include chart reminders,59 preventive also seem unnecessary in most cases
care prompts,60 medical record because experienced pilots could recite
䡩 Underappreciated ankle instability
templates,61 and mnemonic devices them from memory. But pilots have
䡩 Missed associated fracture (especially
navicular or metatarsal stress fracture) (mental checklists).62 These interventions learned not to rely on their memories. In
䡩 Missed Maisonneuve fracture (proximal
have various purposes, formats, and contrast, physicians value superior recall
fibula) organizational structures that differ from and shoot-from-the-hip decisions more
䡩 Missed Achilles tendon rupture (partial or diagnostic checklists. than mental crutches, reflective thought,
complete) or disciplined task performance.
䡩 Missed complex regional pain syndromes Limitations of checklists Diagnostic expertise defines the medical
䡩 Missed peroneal tendon syndromes Recent success in adapting preflight-style profession. But as Donald Berwick said,
checklists to medical procedures has “Genius diagnosticians make great
received justifiable interest,14,17 but stories, but they don’t make great health
and detectable than errors of omission, checklists for diagnosis may be a “bridge care.”64 Checklists were not adopted
the latter tend to predominate,22,50 and too far.” The analogy between actionable without struggle in operating rooms,
forcing strategies will inevitably focus on procedures in aviation and cognitive intensive care units, or even airplanes.
them. procedures in diagnosis is not tight.
Thoughts are less tangible than actions, Checklists could produce a false sense of
and it is more difficult to determine reassurance that leads to complacency,
Further Considerations and
whether they have been completed. In evades the cognitive work required to
Cautions
both medical and nonmedical settings, make a correct diagnosis, neglects
Previous investigators have proposed checklists are read aloud by teams rather patient-specific factors, and obscures
checklists as a concept that might reduce than silently by individuals.11 But aspects of care unrelated to diagnosis.
diagnostic errors.10,21,51 To move this diagnosis is usually silent, lonely work, Similar concerns were raised with clinical
concept forward, we developed three and a natural pause point11 to review the algorithms. It was feared that physicians
kinds of checklists, which we have used in checklist, such as before takeoff or before would rigidly follow algorithms without
our own practices. Each checklist has a incision, does not exist in diagnosis, accounting for individual patient
different function, and each requires which can stretch over hours, days, or differences, but investigators found few
further development and evaluation. even months. data to support these concerns when
algorithms were studied in practice.65,66
Related studies Diagnostic checklists have not been
Diagnostic support tools include practice formally tested in practice to determine The key to reducing diagnostic errors
guidelines, clinical algorithms, whether they are beneficial. The may be less tied to checklists than to a
differential diagnosis textbooks, and checklists in this article were not derived diagnostic time-out—a brief pause to
computerized decision support. using rigorous or reproducible methods, reflect on our diagnostic reasoning and
However, most evidence-based guidelines and we are not promoting them for wider affective state. But rather than unfocused

Academic Medicine, Vol. 86, No. 3 / March 2011 311


Quality Improvement

attempts to think harder or recognize a “thunderclap” headache, “worst-ever” 12 Karl R. Briefings, checklists, geese, and
distracting mood, we could review a headache, and stiff neck. surgical safety. Ann Surg Oncol. 2010;
17:8 –11.
diagnostic checklist and document this
13 Reason J. Human Error. Cambridge,
procedure in the medical record: During Most missed diagnoses result from our
England: Cambridge University Press; 1990.
an active diagnostic time-out, I reviewed failure to consider the correct diagnosis 14 Pronovost P, Needham D, Berenholtz S, et al.
each item in the general checklist and as a possibility. Checklists could An intervention to decrease catheter-related
considered each item in the chest pain potentially help us avoid this and other bloodstream infections in the ICU. N Engl
checklist. I considered but rejected common errors that lead to missed J Med. 2006;355:2725–2732.
diagnoses. We should feel a sense of 15 Gawande A. The checklist. The New Yorker.
pulmonary embolus because I judged the December 10, 2007:86 –95.
risk of harm from excessive testing and urgency to explore this potential in 16 Berenholtz SM, Pronovost PJ, Lipsett PA, et
pursuit of false-positive results to be practice because harmful diagnostic al. Eliminating catheter-related bloodstream
greater than the risk of missing that errors are common, and they are infections in the intensive care unit. Crit Care
diagnosis in this patient. commonly preventable. Med. 2004;32:2014 –2020.
17 Haynes AB, Weiser TG, Berry WR, et al. A
Acknowledgments: The authors are indebted to surgical safety checklist to reduce morbidity
Amy Miranda, Grace Garey, Mary-Lou Glazer, and mortality in a global population. N Engl
Conclusions J Med. 2009;360:491–499.
and Wendy Isser for their expert administrative
We should ask many questions before and bibliographic support. 18 Sloman S. The empirical case for two systems
adopting diagnostic checklists: (1) Will of reasoning. Psychol Bull. 1996;119:3–22.
Funding/Support: None. 19 Croskerry P. Clinical cognition and
they prevent diagnostic errors? Could diagnostic error: Applications of a dual
they do more harm than good? (2) What Other disclosures: None. process model of reasoning. Adv Health Sci
is the optimal content and organization Ethical approval: Not applicable.
Educ Theory Pract. 2009;14(suppl 1):27–35.
for checklists? (3) Who should review the 20 Croskerry P. Cognitive and affective
checklist: physicians, nurses, patients, dispositions to respond. In: Croskerry P,
Cosby K, Schenkel S, Wears R, eds. Patient
family members, dedicated staff?15 (4) References Safety in Emergency Medicine. Philadelphia,
Will checklists be valued or even accepted 1 Elstein AS. Clinical judgment: Psychological Pa: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2009:
by busy physicians? How should they be research and medical practice. Science. 1976; 219 –227.
assimilated into the workflow? (5) How 194:696 –700. 21 Graber ML. Educational strategies to reduce
2 Berner ES, Graber ML. Overconfidence as a diagnostic error: Can you teach this stuff?
should checklists be presented: card in
cause of diagnostic error in medicine. Am J Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract.
the pocket, poster on the wall, computer Med. 2008;121(5 suppl):S2–S23. 2009;14(suppl 1):63–69.
on the desk, or computer in the pocket? 3 Bhasale AL, Miller GC, Reid S, Britt HC. 22 Hayward RA, Asch SM, Hogan MM, Hofer
(6) When should checklists be Analysing potential harm in Australian TP, Kerr EA. Sins of omission: Getting too
reviewed— before, during, after, or general practice: An incident-monitoring little medical care may be the greatest threat
study. Med J Aust. 1998;169:73–76. to patient safety. J Gen Intern Med. 2005;20:
remote from the patient encounter? (7) 4 Leape LL, Brennan TA, Laird N, et al. The 686 –691.
Should we use checklists routinely or nature of adverse events in hospitalized 23 Croskerry P. Timely recognition and
selectively? If selectively, what should patients: Results of the Harvard Medical diagnosis of illness. In: MacKinnon N, ed.
trigger their use? Practice Study II. N Engl J Med. 1991;324: Safe and Effective: The Eight Essential
377–384. Elements of an Optimal Medication-Use
5 Graber ML, Franklin N, Gordon R. System. Ottawa, Ontario, Canada: Canadian
Checklists are mandatory for pilots. Diagnostic error in internal medicine. Arch Pharmacists Association; 2007:79 –93.
Should they be mandatory for physicians? Intern Med. 2005;165:1493–1499. 24 Croskerry P. Context is everything or how
Diagnostic errors are common enough 6 Miller RA. Computer-assisted diagnostic could I have been that stupid? Healthc Q.
that mandatory checklists might be decision support: History, challenges, and
2009;12 Spec No Patient:e171–e176.
possible paths forward. Adv Health Sci
reasonable if they can be shown to work. Educ Theory Pract. 2009;14(suppl 1):
25 Taleb NN. The Black Swan. New York, NY:
Pilots do not have the option of skipping Random House; 2007.
89 –106.
26 Schiff GD, Hasan O, Kim S, et al. Diagnostic
their checklists when the risk is low 7 Rosenbloom ST, Geissbuhler AJ, Dupont
error in medicine: Analysis of 583 physician-
(sunny day, familiar airport, experienced WD, et al. Effect of CPOE user interface
design on user-initiated access to reported errors. Arch Intern Med. 2009;169:
crew). However, any recommendation to 1881–1887.
educational and patient information during
physicians to “use this checklist exactly clinical care. J Am Med Inform Assoc. 2005; 27 Kachalia A, Gandhi TK, Puopolo AL, et al.
when you think you don’t need it” will 12:458 –473. Missed and delayed diagnoses in the
emergency department: A study of closed
likely be met with skepticism. It would be 8 Singh H, Petersen LA, Thomas EJ.
Understanding diagnostic errors in medicine: malpractice claims from 4 liability insurers.
tempting to use checklists only when we Ann Emerg Med. 2007;49:196 –205.
A lesson from aviation. Qual Saf Health Care.
lack confidence in our diagnoses, but 2006;15:159 –164. 28 Plebani M. Exploring the iceberg of errors in
confidence is a poor predictor of 9 Mamede S, Schmidt HG, Rikers R. Diagnostic laboratory medicine. Clin Chim Acta. 2009;
diagnostic accuracy.67 Future studies errors and reflective practice in medicine. 404:16 –23.
might identify “red flags” that should J Eval Clin Pract. 2007;13:138 –145. 29 Elstein AS. Clinical reasoning in medicine. In:
10 Croskerry P. The importance of cognitive Higgs J, Jones MA, eds. Clinical Reasoning in
prompt a time-out and checklist review. the Health Professions. Woburn, Mass:
errors in diagnosis and strategies to minimize
Generic red flags might include failure to them. Acad Med. 2003;78:775–780. http:// Butterworth-Heinemann; 1995:49 –59.
respond to initial treatment, second visit journals.lww.com/academicmedicine/ 30 Mitchell DJ, Russo JE, Pennington N. Back to
to the emergency department for the Fulltext/2003/08000/The_Importance_of_ the future: Temporal perspective in the
same problem, or presenting symptoms Cognitive_Errors_in_Diagnosis.3.aspx. explanation of events. J Behav Decis Making.
Accessed January 4, 2011. 1989;2:25–38.
that are commonly associated with 11 Gawande A. The Checklist Manifesto—How 31 Kahneman D, Klein G. Conditions for
diagnostic errors. Complaint-specific red to Get Things Right. New York, NY: intuitive expertise: A failure to disagree. Am
flags for headache might include Metropolitan Books; 2009. Psychol. 2009;64:515–526.

312 Academic Medicine, Vol. 86, No. 3 / March 2011


Quality Improvement

32 Wears RL. What makes diagnosis 47 Smith DS. Field Guide to Bedside Diagnosis. 59 McPhee SJ, Bird JA, Fordham D, Rodnick JE,
hard? Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract. Philadelphia, Pa: Lippincott Williams & Osborn EH. Promoting cancer prevention
2009;14(suppl 1):19 –25. Wilkins; 1999. activities by primary care physicians. Results
33 Kovacs G, Croskerry P. Clinical decision 48 Louis AA. Handbook of Difficult Diagnosis. of a randomized, controlled trial. JAMA.
making: An emergency medicine perspective. New York, NY: Churchill Livingstone; 1990. 1991;266:538 –544.
Acad Emerg Med. 1999;6:947–952. 49 Lewis C, Norman DA. Designing for error. 60 Balas EA, Weingarten S, Garb CT,
34 Campbell SG. Advances in emergency In: Norman D, Draper S, eds. User Centered Blumenthal D, Boren SA, Brown GD.
medicine: A 10-year perspective. Can J Diag. System Design: New Perspectives in Human- Improving preventive care by prompting
2003;20:115–118. Computer Interaction. Hillsdale, NJ: physicians. Arch Intern Med. 2000;160:301–
35 Croskerry P. Avoiding pitfalls in the Erlbaum; 1986:411–432. 308.
emergency room. Can J Contin Med Educ. 50 Wilson RM, Runciman WB, Gibberd RW, 61 Marill KA, Gauharou ES, Nelson BK,
April 1996:63–76. Harrison BT, Newby L, Hamilton JD. The Peterson MA, Curtis RL, Gonzalez MR.
36 Vickers AJ, Basch E, Kattan MW. Against Quality in Australian Health Care Study. Med Prospective, randomized trial of template-
diagnosis. Ann Intern Med. 2008;149:200 – J Aust. 1995;163:458 –471. assisted versus undirected written recording
203. 51 Newman-Toker DE, Pronovost PJ. of physician records in the emergency
37 Schiff GD. Minimizing diagnostic error: The Diagnostic errors—The next frontier for department. Ann Emerg Med. 1999;33:500 –
importance of follow-up and feedback. Am J patient safety. JAMA. 2009;301:1060 –1062. 509.
Med. 2008;121(5 suppl):S38 –S42. 52 Trowbridge R, Weingarten S. Clinical 62 Lieberman P, Decker W, Camargo CA Jr,
38 Redelmeier DA. Improving patient care. The decision support systems. In: Shojania K, Oconnor R, Oppenheimer J, Simons FE.
cognitive psychology of missed diagnoses. Duncan B, McDonald K, Wachter R, eds. SAFE: A multidisciplinary approach to
Ann Intern Med. 2005;142:115–120. Making Health Care Safer: A Critical Analysis anaphylaxis education in the emergency
39 Crandall B, Wears RL. Expanding of Patient Safety Practices. Rockville, Md: department. Ann Allergy Asthma Immunol.
perspectives on misdiagnosis. Am J Med. Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality; 2007;98:519 –523.
2008;121(5 suppl):S30 –S33. 2001. 63 Leape LL, Berwick DM, Bates DW. What
40 Shojania KG, Burton EC, McDonald KM, 53 Payne TH. Computer decision support practices will most improve safety? Evidence-
Goldman L. Changes in rates of autopsy- systems. Chest. 2000;118(2 suppl):47S–52S. based medicine meets patient safety. JAMA.
detected diagnostic errors over time: A 54 Patterson ES, Doebbeling BN, Fung CH, 2002;288:501–507.
systematic review. JAMA. 2003;289:2849 – Militello L, Anders S, Asch SM. Identifying 64 Gaither C. What your doctor doesn’t know
2856. barriers to the effective use of clinical could kill you. Boston Globe. July 14, 2002.
41 Adler SN, Adler-Klein D, Gasbarra DB. A reminders: Bootstrapping multiple methods. http://www.boston.com/news/globe/reprints/
Pocket Manual of Differential Diagnosis. 5th J Biomed Inform. 2005;38:189 –199. 071402_whenyourdoc/. Accessed December
ed. Philadelphia, Pa: Lippincott Williams & 55 Bates DW, Kuperman GJ, Wang S, et al. Ten 25, 2010.
Wilkins; 2008. commandments for effective clinical decision 65 McDonald CJ, Wilson GA, McCabe GP Jr.
42 Greenberger NJ. Handbook of Differential support: Making the practice of evidence- Physician response to computer reminders.
Diagnosis in Internal Medicine: Medical Book based medicine a reality. J Am Med Inform JAMA. 1980;244:1579 –1581.
of Lists. 5th ed. St. Louis, Mo: Mosby; 1998. Assoc. 2003;10:523–530. 66 Shoemaker WC, Corley RD, Liu M, et al.
43 Wiener SL. Differential Diagnosis of Acute 56 Johnson CW. Why did that happen? Development and testing of a decision tree
Pain by Body Region. New York, NY: Exploring the proliferation of barely usable for blunt trauma. Crit Care Med. 1988;16:
McGraw-Hill; 1993. software in healthcare systems. Qual Saf 1199 –1208.
44 Stern S, Cifu A, Atkorn D. Symptom to Health Care. 2006;15(suppl 1):i76 –i81. 67 Friedman CP, Gatti GG, Franz TM, et al. Do
Diagnosis: An Evidence-Based Guide. 2nd ed. 57 Graber MA, VanScoy D. How well does physicians know when their diagnoses are
New York, NY: Lange; 2009. decision support software perform in the correct? Implications for decision support
45 Siegenthaler W. Differential Diagnosis in emergency department? Emerg Med J. 2003; and error reduction. J Gen Intern Med. 2005;
Internal Medicine: From Symptom to 20:426 –428. 20:334 –339.
Diagnosis. New York, NY: Thieme 58 Garg AX, Adhikari NK, McDonald H, et al.
Publishers; 2007. Effects of computerized clinical decision Reference cited only in figure
46 Collins RD. Differential Diagnosis in Primary support systems on practitioner performance 68 Stanovich KE. Dysrationalia: A new specific
Care. 4th ed. Philadelphia, Pa: Lippincott and patient outcomes: A systematic review. learning disability. J Learn Disabil. 1993;26:
Williams & Wilkins; 2008. JAMA. 2005;293:1223–1238. 501–515.

Academic Medicine, Vol. 86, No. 3 / March 2011 313

You might also like