You are on page 1of 4

Acta Ophthalmologica 2008

Eye blink in newborn and


preschool-age children
Marcelo M. Lavezzo,1 Silvana A. Schellini,1 Carlos R. Padovani2
and Flavio Eduardo Hirai1
1
Department of Ophthalmology, Botucatu School of Medicine, Universidade
Estadual Pauliste, Botucatu, São Paulo, Brazil
2
Department of Biostatistics, Bioscience Institute, Universidade Estadual Pauliste,
Botucatu, São Paulo, Brazil

ABSTRACT. 2005). In normal adults, blink rates


Purpose: To investigate the dynamics of ocular eyelid movements in newborn range from two to 50 blinks ⁄ min
infants and preschool-age children. (Monster et al. 1978; Tsubota 1998).
Methods: Fifty newborn infants and 200 preschool children aged 4)6 years The blink reflex is present at birth,
were examined. Images of each child, with his or her eyes in the primary eye but spontaneous blink rates are low
position looking at an object placed at the child’s height, were recorded with a in very young children (Zametkin
digital videocamera for 3 mins. Complete and incomplete blink rates, opening, et al. 1979; Lawrenson et al. 2003).
Blink rates increase rapidly during
closing and complete blink times were calculated.
the first year of life, but remain
Results: Newborn infants presented a lower number of incomplete movements
lower than those in adults (Tsubota
than preschool children. The complete blink rate was lower in newborn infants 1998).
(6.2 blinks ⁄ min) than in preschool children (8.0 blinks ⁄ minute). Eyelid closing, The evaluation of blink rates in
opening and compete blink times were longer in newborn infants than in pre- newborn infants and young children is
school children at all observation times. difficult, particularly because of meth-
Conclusions: Newborn infants had a different pattern of eyelid movement com- odological issues. The purpose of this
pared with preschool children. Specific characteristics that are found in this study was to investigate eyelid dynam-
group of children particularly, such as immaturity of the neural system and ics in normal newborn infants and
more resistant tear film, may explain these findings in part. preschool-age children using a digital
image processing system.
Key words: eye blink – newborn – preschool children – eyelid closing time – eyelid opening time –
digital image
Materials and Methods
Acta Ophthalmol. 2008: 86: 275–278
A total of 250 children were evaluated
ª 2007 The Authors
Journal compilation ª 2007 Acta Ophthalmol Scand at the Faculty of Medicine at Botu-
catu, São Paulo, Brazil. Fifty newborn
doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0420.2007.00969.x infants (aged up to 30 days) and
200 healthy preschool children aged
4 years (66 children), 5 years (54 chil-
dren) and 6 years (80 children) parti-
cipated in this study. Children with
conditions, including time of day eyelid abnormalities (e.g. congenital
Introduction (rates increase at night) (Barbato ptosis) or diseases that might affect
There are two types of blinking: invol- et al. 2000), gender (men have higher blink rate (e.g. ocular allergies) were
untary or reflex (corresponding with rates than women) (Bentivoglio et al. not enrolled.
spontaneous) and voluntary (which 1997), cognitive processes and atten- Eye movement patterns were evalu-
depends on the individual’s intent). tion level (Bentivoglio et al. 1997) and ated by recording digital images (fron-
Blink rate influences lacrimal drainage ocular alterations (Dumery & Toi Vo tal and lateral shots) of newborn
(Sahlin & Chen 1997) and ocular sur- Van 1997). infants and children fully awake, in
face integrity (Tsubota & Nakamori Blink rates in adults vary across the primary eye position, with the
1995), and depends on various different age groups (Schellini et al. observation target placed at the

275
17553768, 2008, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1600-0420.2007.00969.x by National Institutes Of Health Malaysia, Wiley Online Library on [29/01/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Acta Ophthalmologica 2008

subject’s height at a distance of 1 m. 25


All images were taken by the same
observer under the same environmen-
tal conditions using a Sony Lithium 20
digital video camera (Sony DCR-

Median (in 3 mins)


TRV110, Sony, Manus, Brazil) and
15
8-mm videotapes. Images were trans-
ferred to an Apple MacIntosh
G4computer and processed with 10
iMovie software (Apple Computer
Inc., Cupertino, CA, USA). This soft- Complete eyelid movement
ware detects intervals of 1 ⁄ 30 sec (one 5
Incomplete eyelid movement
frame), which allowed us to capture
the high speeds of the human blink
(Dumery & Toi Vo Van 1997). 0
Spontaneous blinking was evaluated Newborns 4 years old 5 years old 6 years old
Age
over a 3-min period and included eye-
lid opening and closing times (in milli- Fig. 1. Median values of blink rate for complete and incomplete movements according to age.
seconds), complete blink time (sum of
eyelid opening and closing times in
milliseconds) and the rate of eyelid respectively). Newborn infants presen- and preschool children (8.0 and
movements per minute. In order to ted a lower number of incomplete 6.3 blinks ⁄ min, respectively).
evaluate these parameters (i.e. closing, movements than preschool-age chil- Eyelid closing time as well as eyelid
opening and complete blink times), dren. Newborn infants made five opening time was longer in newborn
one complete eyelid movement from incomplete eyelid movements during infants than in preschool children at
each minute of observation was ran- the 3-min period, whereas 4-, 5- and all observation times (p < 0.0001).
domly chosen. Eyelid movement was 6-year-old children presented 15, 18 Similar results were seen for complete
considered complete when the super- and 16 incomplete eyelid movements, blink time: newborn infants demon-
ior eyelid touched the inferior eyelid; respectively (median values, p < strated a longer complete blink time
otherwise, it was defined as incom- 0.001). The complete blink rate was than preschool children (p < 0.0001)
plete. lower in newborn infants (6.2 blinks ⁄ (Table 1).
Because blink rates can be affected min) than in preschool children
by many internal (e.g. concentration (8.0 blinks ⁄ min). The complete blink
level, humour) and external (e.g. dust, rate was higher than the incomplete
Discussion
wind, smoke) factors (Monster et al. blink rate in both newborn infants Our study evaluated the dynamics of
1978; Bentivoglio et al. 1997), we deci- (6.2 and 2.5 blinks ⁄ min, respectively) eyelid movements in a group of
ded to record eyelid movements over
a 3-min period to allow the children
to become more comfortable with the
environment. The images were also Table 1. Mean (± standard deviation) eyelid closing, opening and complete blink times (in mil-
recorded from a lateral position and liseconds) over a 3-min period, according to age.
the children were distracted from the Eyelid closing time
fact that they were being filmed in
order to capture spontaneous blink- Newborns 4-year-olds 5-year-olds 6-year-olds p-value*
ing.
Min 1 433.3 ± 92.7 225.0 ± 68.3 226.7 ± 84.3 228.0 ± 55.0 p < 0.0001
Results were analysed and presen- Min 2 435.3 ± 76.0 231.3 ± 59.3 240.3 ± 88.3 230.7 ± 57.0 p < 0.0001
ted according to descriptive statistics Min 3 424.3 ± 88.0 234.3 ± 63.0 230.7 ± 72.7 232.0 ± 75.0 p < 0.0001
(mean, standard deviation and med-
ian) and non-parametric tests were Eyelid opening time
used to compare groups.
Newborns 4-year-olds 5-year-olds 6-year-olds p-value*

Min 1 330.7 ± 64.3 192.3 ± 51.7 187.7 ± 54.7 200.7 ± 40.7 p < 0.0001
Min 2 333.3 ± 61.0 201.0 ± 49.0 194.0 ± 52.0 201.7 ± 40.7 p < 0.0001
Results Min 3 319.3 ± 60.0 200.3 ± 55.3 189.0 ± 47.0 206.0 ± 49.0 p < 0.0001
Figure 1 shows median values of blink
Complete blink time
rates according to age. Newborn
infants made 19 complete eyelid Newborns 4-year-olds 5-year-olds 6-year-olds p-value*
movements during the 3-min period,
Min 1 764.0 ± 139.0 417.3 ± 113.0 414.3 ± 127.0 428.7 ± 88.7 p < 0.0001
which was similar to the number of
Min 2 768.7 ± 124.3 432.3 ± 99.3 434.7 ± 126.3 432.3 ± 89.7 p < 0.0001
complete eyelid movements presented Min 3 743.7 ± 123.3 435.0 ± 111.0 420.0 ± 106.7 438.0 ± 117.3 p < 0.0001
by 4-, 5- and 6-year-old children
(median values of 12, 16 and 22, * p-value ¼ comparison between newborn infants and the 4-, 5-, and 6-year-old age groups.

276
17553768, 2008, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1600-0420.2007.00969.x by National Institutes Of Health Malaysia, Wiley Online Library on [29/01/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Acta Ophthalmologica 2008

healthy newborn infants and pre- adults, slowing evaporation and Conclusions
school-age children. The ocular sur- increasing tear film stability, and
face, together with the eyelids and thus preventing ocular surface drying In conclusion, blink rates were
lacrimal system, represents a func- (Isenberg et al. 1998; Lawrenson lower and blinking times longer in
tional unit that ensures the quality of et al. 2005). In addition, less ocular newborn infants compared with
the refractive surface of the eye and surface area is exposed in newborn preschool children. Specific character-
also protects ocular structures from infants (Fox 1966). The combination istics found only in this group of
noxious stimuli arising from the envi- of these factors (i.e. more stable tear children, such as immaturity of the
ronment (Rolando & Zierhut 2001). film and a smaller ocular surface neural system and more resistant tear
The preocular tear film is the most area) may in part explain why chil- film, may explain these findings in
dynamic structure within this unit, dren in this age group have lower part.
and an adequate rate of tear produc- blink rates. This idea is supported
tion and a rapid turnover is essential by Tsubota (1998), who observed
for ocular health. Tears cleanse, lubri- that an increase in blink rate repre- Acknowledgement
cate and defend the ocular surface sents an important mechanism with
This study was supported by the Con-
against infection. Furthermore, by which to compensate for the greater
selho Nacional de Desenvolvimento
smoothing out irregularities of the tear film evaporation resulting from
Cientı́fico e Tecnológico (National
corneal epithelium, the precorneal tear the increase in ocular surface
Council for Scientific and Technologi-
film creates an even surface of excel- exposure caused by ocular globe
cal Research), Brasilia, Brazil.
lent optical quality that is reformed growth.
with each blink. In newborn infants, Another hypothesis to explain the
the optical characteristics of the tear lower blink rate in newborn infants
film are important for the generation relates to the development of the vis- References
of a clear retinal image, which is an ual and central nervous systems.
essential prerequisite for normal visual Newborn infants do not achieve the Barbato G, Ficca G, Muscettola G, Fichele
development (Lawrenson et al. 2003). same level of image formation in the M, Beatrice M & Rinaldi F (2000): Diurnal
variation in spontaneous eye blink rate.
As the tear film is inherently unstable, retina and brain as older children,
Psychiatry Res 93: 145–151.
frequent blinking is normally required and external stimuli are probably less Bentivoglio AR, Bressman SB, Cassetta E,
to prevent surface drying. The repor- important to the blinking process Carretta D, Tonali P & Albanese A
ted low rate of spontaneous blinking because of the immaturity of the (1997): Analysis of blink rate patterns in
in neonates and infants (Zametkin neural circuit and the eye. Moreover, normal subjects. Mov Disord 12: 1028–
et al. 1979) thus raises important the immaturity of other systems may 1034.
questions regarding tear stability and also contribute to the lower blink Dumery B & Toi Vo Van (1997): Relation-
the ability of ocular surface receptors rates observed in newborn infants. ship between blink rate, ocular discomfort
and visual tasks. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci
to detect tear break-up. For example, afferent pathways that 38 (Suppl): 68.
In addition, a reduced rate of reflex detect a reduction in tear film on the Fox SA (1966): The palpebral fissure. Am J
tear secretion may limit the dilution ocular surface, caused by evapor- Ophthalmol 62: 73–78.
of topical ocular medication, leading ation, may not yet be fully formed. Herlenius E & Lagercrantz H (2001): Neuro-
to a longer period of contact between In addition, although the relationship transmitters and neuromodulators during
the drug and the ocular surface and between dopaminergic plasticity and early human development. Early Hum Dev
thus increasing the risk of adverse spontaneous blink frequency in new- 65: 21–37.
Isenberg SJ, Apt L, McCarty J, Cooper LL,
reactions (Isenberg et al. 1998). born infants is unclear (Meng et al.
Lim L & Del Signore M (1998):
One important finding from our 1999; Herlenius & Lagercrantz 2001), Development of tearing in preterm and
study was that the pattern of eyelid it is believed that the reduced term neonates. Arch Ophthalmol 116: 773–
movement in newborn infants clearly blink rate in newborn infants, as 776.
differed from that in preschool chil- seen in this study, may reflect a rel- Lawrenson JG, Birhah R & Murphy PJ
dren. Newborn infants, for example, atively underdeveloped dopaminergic (2005): Tear-film lipid layer morphology
had lower blink rates and longer system. and corneal sensation in the development
opening and closing times than The main limitation of our study of blinking in neonates and infants. J Anat
206: 265–270.
4–6-year-old children. concerned the difficulty of recording
Lawrenson JG, Murphy PJ & Esmaeelpour
Other authors have also reported eyelid movements in newborn infants M (2003): The neonatal tear film. Cont
that newborn infants present a because they were awake for only Lens Anterior Eye 26: 197–202.
decreased blink rate compared with short periods of time and, as a conse- Meng SZ, Ozawa Y, Itoh M & Takashima S
older children (Zametkin et al. 1979; quence, a lower number of children in (1999): Developmental and age-related
Lawrenson et al. 2003) and this rate this age group were examined. changes of dopamine transporter, and
increases until adolescence. After Although the number of newborn dopamine D1 and D2 receptors in
adolescence, it appears to remain con- infants (50) differed from that of human basal ganglia. Brain Res 843: 136–
144.
stant throughout life (Barbato et al. children aged 4)6 years (200), non-
Monster AW, Chan HC & O’Connor D
2000). parametric statistical tests allowed us (1978): Longterm trends in human eye
The lipid layer of tear film in to make comparisons between the two blink rate. Biotelem Patient Monit 5: 206–
newborn infants is thicker than in groups. 222.

277
17553768, 2008, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1600-0420.2007.00969.x by National Institutes Of Health Malaysia, Wiley Online Library on [29/01/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Acta Ophthalmologica 2008

Rolando M & Zierhut M (2001): The ocular Tsubota K & Nakamori K (1995): Effects of Correspondence:
surface and tear film and their dysfunction ocular surface area and blink rate on Silvana Artioli Schellini MD
in dry eye disease. Surv Ophthalmol 45: tear dynamics. Arch Ophthalmol 113: 155– Departamento de Oftalmologia
S203–S210. 158. Faculdade de Medicina de Botucatu
Sahlin S & Chen E (1997): Gravity, blink rate Zametkin AJ, Stevens JR & Pittman R Universidade Estadual Paulista
and lacrimal drainage capacity. Am J Oph- (1979): Ontogeny of spontaneous blinking Botucatu
thalmol 124: 758–764. and of habituation of the blink reflex. Ann Cep 18618–970 São Paulo
Schellini SA, Sampaio AA, Hoyama E, Cruz Neurol 5: 453–457. Brazil
AAV & Padovani CR (2005): Spontaneous Tel: + 55 (14) 3811 6256
eye blink analysis in the normal individual. Fax: + 55 (14) 3811 6256
Orbit 24: 239–242. Email: sartioli@fmb.unesp.br
Tsubota K (1998): Tear dynamics and dry Received on January 5th, 2007.
eye. Prog Retin Eye Res 17: 565–596. Accepted on May 13th, 2007.

278

You might also like