You are on page 1of 17

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/11534936

Referral rates for functional vision screening


among a large cosmopolitan sample of
Australian children

ARTICLE in OPHTHALMIC AND PHYSIOLOGICAL OPTICS · JANUARY 2002


Impact Factor: 2.18 · DOI: 10.1046/j.1475-1313.2002.00010.x · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

44 83

4 AUTHORS, INCLUDING:

Barbara Junghans David Crewther


University of New South Wales Swinburne University of Technology
37 PUBLICATIONS 703 CITATIONS 242 PUBLICATIONS 2,138 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Sheila Crewther
La Trobe University
249 PUBLICATIONS 2,156 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All in-text references underlined in blue are linked to publications on ResearchGate, Available from: Sheila Crewther
letting you access and read them immediately. Retrieved on: 08 January 2016
Ophthal. Physiol. Opt. 2002 22 10±25

Referral rates for a functional vision screening


among a large cosmopolitan sample
of Australian children
Barbara Junghans1, Patricia M. Kiely2, David P. Crewther2 and Sheila
Gillard Crewther2
1
School of Optometry, The University of New South Wales, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
2052, 2School of Psychological Science, La Trobe University, Bundoora, Victoria, Australia 3083

Abstract
The aim of this study was to investigate the incidence of functional vision problems in a large unselected
cosmopolitan population of primary school-age children and to investigate whether constant clinical
criteria for functional vision problems would be implemented by the practitioners involved in the
screening. Refractive errors, near point of convergence, stereopsis, strabismus, heterophoria and
accommodative facility were assessed for 2697 children (3±12 years) of varying racial backgrounds
living in Australia. The spherical component of the refractive error ranged from )7.75 to +9.50 D (mean
+0.54 D, ‹0.79) with a distribution skewed towards hypermetropia; astigmatism ranged from 0 to
4.25 D (mean )0.16 D, ‹0.35). There was a trend towards less hypermetropia and slightly more
astigmatism with age. Mean near point of convergence was 5.4 ‹ 2.9 cm, heterophoria at far and near
was 0.12 ‹ 1.58D exophoria and 1.05 ‹ 2.53D exophoria, respectively, 0.55% of children exhibited
vertical phoria at near >0.5D, accommodative facility ranged from 0 to 24 cycles per minute (cpm)
(mean 11.2 cpm, ‹3.7), stereopsis varied from 20 to 800 s (¢¢) of arc with 50% of children having 40¢¢ or
better. The prevalence of strabismus was particularly low (0.3%).
Twenty percent of the children were referred for further assessment based on criteria of one or more of:
stereopsis >70¢¢, accommodative facility <8 cpm, near point of convergence (NPC) >9 cm, near
exophoria >10D or near esophoria >5D, shift in eso or exophoria ³ 4D between distance and near,
astigmatism ³ 1 D, myopia more than )0.75 D, or hyperopia >+1.50 D.
Post-hoc analysis of the record cards seeking the reason for further assessment indicates that referrals
appear to have been based upon clinical intuition rather than on a set number of borderline or
unsatisfactory results.

Keywords: binocular vision, clinical pro®le, referral, refractive error, vision screening

and ocular motor problems make ocular or visual


Introduction
de®cits the fourth most common childhood disability
Vision is the core sensory input for humans throughout and the most prevalent handicapping condition in
life and particularly during early childhood (Atkinson, childhood (cited Ciner et al., 1998). However, as such
2000). Disorders such as refractive errors, amblyopia visual anomalies are not regarded as signi®cant health
problems, vision screenings are seldom included in
medical assessments (Thomson and Evans, 1999) despite
Received: 14 July 2000
the evidence that systematically based ocular disease
Revised form: 6 June 2001
Accepted: 26 September 2001 often leads to the development of refractive error
(Nathan et al., 1985, 1986), further contributing to the
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Barbara M. Junghans. visual handicap.
Tel.: +61 2 9385 4237; fax: +61 2 9313 6243 Although there is considerable controversy among
E-mail address: b.junghans@unsw.edu.au
ophthalmic authorities as to whether such visual han-

Formerly of School of Optometry, UNSW. dicaps have any in¯uence on learning as a whole (Joint

10 ã 2002 The College of Optometrists


School screenings: B. Junghans et al. 11

Organizational Policy Statement of the American Acad- e€ectiveness of some screening procedures (Ciner et al.,
emy of Optometry, 1997; Committee on Children with 1998).
Disabilities: American Academy of Pediatrics, American Previous screenings have generally been performed on
Academy of Ophthalmology, American Association for predominantly Anglo-Saxon populations and examined
Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus, 1998), edu- refractive errors within general populations of children
cational authorities in Australia regularly seek visual (Hirsch and Weymouth, 1991; Zadnik, 1997). However
assessments for children diagnosed as having learning ethnicity is a factor likely to a€ect population param-
disabilities with the aim of lessening near visual eters for various ocular characteristics (Bear, 1991;
discomfort and increasing the likelihood of reading Zadnik, 1997), e.g. the prevalence of myopia in Hong
practice and preventing the development of learning Kong for 6 years old is 30% (Lam and Goh, 1991)
associated behavioural problems. This practice is sup- compared with a prevalence of myopia of 4% for a
ported by the existence of signi®cantly increased bin- similar age group in San Francisco (Zadnik et al., 1993).
ocular visual handicaps in children with mild to Certainly the increasingly cosmopolitan nature of cities
moderate intellectual disability such as Fragile X, in the UK, USA and Australia and the change in visual
Velo-Cardio±Facial syndrome and Down syndrome demands in a society with greater need for academic
(Hagerman and Cronister, 1996; Crewther et al., 1998; persistence, has raised the question of whether the
Tsiaras et al., 1999; Cregg et al., 2001). parameters used in the past are still appropriate.
The majority of evidence of a positive association Criteria de®ning `normal' or `satisfactory' oculo-
between the e€ects of vision therapy and learning is visual parameters should be based on ranges of values
indirect and anecdotal, however, there are some well which allow adequate and comfortable behavioural
designed experiments showing associations between visual functioning at far and near, rather than on
visual function and learning diculties, particularly boundaries based on the statistical normal distribution.
reading ability (Bennet et al., 1982; Grisham and The concept of functional limits was applied to the study
Simons, 1986; The 1986/87 Future of Visual Develop- of 12 000 young Nova Scotian children by Lam et al.
ment/Performance Task Force; Simons and Grisham, (1996) who used pre-determined criteria to select 162
1986; Simons and Gassler, 1988; Rosner and Rosner, Nova Scotian children as `normal' to establish standards
1994; Garzia and Franzel, 1996; Kulp and Schmidt, for childhood ophthalmological variables after screen-
1996). Signi®cant correlations have been shown between ing 12 000 children aged 4.5±5.5 years. Inclusion criteria
reading accuracy, reading comprehension, reading rate were distance acuity £6/9)3; stereoacuity £702; NPC
and numeracy and one or more of unaided distance £ 9 cm; refractive errors between )0.50 and +3.50 D;
vision, binocular distance acuity, retinoscopy, conver- astigmatism £1.25 D; anisometropia £1.00 D (a range
gence, phoria, ®xation disparity and colour vision of refractive errors consistent with unaided visual acuity
(O'Grady, 1984). Meta-analysis has also shown signi®- of 6/9). However, this study did not address monocular
cant associations between reading performance and accommodative amplitude, or accommodative-conver-
hyperopia, exophoria at near, vertical phoria, aniso- gence relationships by using accommodative facility
metropia and aniseikonia (Simons and Grisham, 1987). tests at near.
Signi®cant correlation between reading problems and The concept of referral from a screening requires the
poor fusional vergence reserves, convergence insu- determination of a ®nal pass/fail, which is obviously
ciency and ®xation disparity (Simons and Grisham, more complex than for a single clinical test but has
1987), binocular instability and low amplitude of seldom been considered in the literature. Children may
accommodation (Evans, 1998; Evans et al., 1999), and show a number or cluster of borderline results, each
accommodative facility (Hennessey et al., 1984) have insucient alone to clearly delineate a fail or to yield a
also been reported. The continuing association of subtle clear clinical diagnosis, but taken together indicating
impairments in visual function and reading disability in that the vergence system and/or the accommodative
the scienti®c literature is no longer controversial system is under stress when performing simple binocular
(Fowler, 1991). Twenty years of research into reading tasks. Yet even reported studies of single inadequacies of
disability has established an extensive literature on the visual system are often inconsistent in their recom-
impaired M-pathway functions in children with devel- mendations because a battery of tests and associations
opmental dyslexia (Lovegrove, 1996). considered essential by one clinician, are not accepted as
Although the type of vision screening and the choice the `gold standard' by others. For example, see Rouse
of tools to be included should be appropriate for the et al.'s (1998) discussion of the measurement and
age of the individual, and in the case of a child, analysis of convergence insuciency by di€erent inves-
appropriate for their continuing educational needs, tigators. In addition single disorders of the visual system
there is little agreement which are the most e€ective are often interpreted within a research context only after
screening methods and little scienti®c evidence of the having controlled for other parameters by applying a set

ã 2002 The College of Optometrists


12 Ophthal. Physiol. Opt. 2002 22: No 1

of `normal' standards for entry into the study (e.g. screening and written consent was obtained prior to a
Rouse et al., 1998). child's participation.
Although Ciner et al. (1998) have recently concluded
that `there is little agreement concerning the most
Subjects
e€ective screening methods and limited recommenda-
tions of referral criteria', Bailey's review (Bailey, 1998) A total of 2697 children aged 3±12 years participated
states that the good predictive value of the Orinda Study in the study (2612 from NSW, 85 from Victoria),
and its `Modi®ed Clinical Technique' (MCT) (Blum although not all were administered all tests. There were
et al., 1968) has gained broad acceptance. However, the 1452 boys and 1195 girls and 50 children for whom
MCT does not address non-strabismic binocular dys- gender was not recorded. In both states there was
function and requires ophthalmic-trained personnel. In considerable variation in ethnic background of the
a recent development to address issues of time, cost± children but ethical constraints prevented us from
bene®t, facilities required and training of personnel, determining ethnic background. New South Wales
Thomson and Evans (1999) have moved towards imple- children came from primary schools in the Randwick
mentation of a screening with some computer-based area of Sydney where 39.5% of people in Randwick
elements with a sensitivity of 93.8% and speci®city of were reported to have been born overseas, with 90.7%
96.1%: screening for symptoms via a questionnaire of these from 36 countries covering all continents (1996
completed by parents at home, computer-based mon- Census data: The People of New South Wales: Statistics
ocular distance acuity, computer-based stereopsis and from the 1996 Census, 1998). In this area recent
normal Ishihara colour vision plates. immigrants included 42.9% from eastern or middle-
One aim of the current study was to examine eastern Europe, 22.2% from Asia and 13.7% from
statistical population norms for refractive errors and western Europe.
selected binocular parameters believed in Australia to be
associated with comfort in reading [stereopsis, hetero-
Protocol
phoria, accommodative facility and near point of
convergence (NPC)] for a large random cosmopolitan Although a comprehensive eye examination comprising
population. The screening was implemented as part of of age-appropriate tests by Australian standards was
the clinical curriculum for students of Optometry at the performed on most children, the term `screening' is used
University of New South Wales. Given the constraints as parents were not present (see Bailey, 1998). In NSW
of the test battery used and the student±clinic setting, the screening was administered by ®nal year optometry
the second aim was to analyse what `operational' criteria students under the supervision of optometrists with
are used by experienced clinicians to determine referral considerable paediatric experience. The results presented
with regard to binocular function during a large scale are those of the supervisor. The Victorian screenings
screening, with a view to re®ning the screening protocol were administered in schools by one of two registered
and planning manpower for further screenings. optometrists (SGC and PMK). Protocols and instru-
mentation di€ered between the two sites only in that
‹2.00 ¯ippers and a Randot Stereo Test were used in
Methods
Victoria. Parents and teachers in NSW were asked to
complete a simple questionnaire prior to the screening,
Ethics
regarding the presence of signs or symptoms of oculo-
Approvals were obtained from the Committees for the visual problems (see Appendix).
use of humans in research at the University of New The screening included visual acuity, cover test for
South Wales (UNSW), Sydney, and La Trobe Univer- strabismus, motilities, saccades, pupil reactions, retino-
sity, Melbourne, Australia. Permission to approach scopic determination of the refractive error, NPC,
schools to invite them to send entire classes to the heterophoria, stereopsis (Titmus or Randot) and
Vision Education Centre (VEC) (Junghans and Crew- accommodative facility (the latter four tests were done
ther, 1992), School of Optometry, UNSW was obtained with the habitual correction unless a signi®cant change in
from the NSW Department of Education and the NSW refraction had been found), colour vision and ophthal-
Catholic Education Oce and School Principals. In moscopy. The supervising optometrist was responsible
Victoria, permission to perform vision screenings on for the recommendation for referral for further assess-
children as part of other reading-related studies was ment. The recommended guidelines for determining
obtained from the Victorian Catholic Education Oce borderline and failed performance on each test are
and The State Directorate of School Education and shown in Table 1. These guidelines were determined by
school Principals. In both states parents/guardians were two senior sta€ optometrists specialising in children's
provided with an information sheet detailing the vision vision, who drew on the literature and their own

ã 2002 The College of Optometrists


School screenings: B. Junghans et al. 13

Table 1. Criteria used to determine degree of acceptability of oculo-visual parameters (exo ˆ exophoria, eso ˆ esophoria)
Criterion

Function Satisfactory Borderline Unsatisfactory

Near point of convergence <7.5 7.5±9 cm >9 cm


Phoria (distance) 3 m: 2 eso ± 4 exo 3 m: >3 eso, >5 exo 3 m: >5 eso, >8 exo
Phoria (near) 33 cm: 2 eso ± 8 exo 33 cm: >3 eso, >9 exo 33 cm: >5 eso, >10 exo
Near phoria minus distance phoria <4D ± ³4
Accommodative facility >8 cpm 7±8 cpm 8 years+ <7 cpm
8 years) <6 cpm
Stereopsis £ 70 s 80 s ³ 100 s
Colour vision 0±1 plates missed 2 plates missed
Rx sphere < +1.50 DS +1.50 DS >1.50 DS
< )0.50 DS )0.50 DS >) 0.75 DS
Rx cylinder < 0.75 DC )0.75 DC >)0.75 DC
Anisometropia 1.00 DS >1.00 DS

experience (one with a background in behavioural


Statistical analyses
optometry and the other a more traditional background).
Retinoscopy without cycloplegia was performed with Data were analysed using Statistical Package for Social
the child ®xating a distant target; in NSW these ®ndings Sciences (SPSS) for Windows software. Multivariate
were re®ned by subjective refraction and visual acuity interpretations were analysed by ANOVA. Incomplete
determined with the refractive correction. Near point of data sometimes occurred because of time constraints.
convergence was assessed using the push-up method to This has led to varying sample sizes for di€erent
determine break subjectively and objectively whilst parameters.
viewing a ®ne high contrast target (objective results
were recorded where there was disagreement). Hetero-
Results
phorias were measured at distance (3 m) and near
(33 cm) using a Howell Phoria Card (Cyclopean Design, Refractive error results include all children. The stra-
Melbourne, Australia) which is an adaptation of a bismic children were excluded from the binocular vision
principle attributed to Thorington (or Prentice) (Borish, analysis. Correlations of poor ®ndings with the existence
1975). Vertical phorias were measured at near only. The of symptoms could not be determined accurately in the
Howell Phoria Card is oval to decrease fusional cues current study due to lack of information provided on
from horizontal/vertical borders and measures from parent/teacher questionnaires. 2490 children received
10 exo to 9 eso at far, and 20 exo to 21 eso at near. The sucient testing to qualify for inclusion in the ®nal case
black lines bordering the top and bottom edges of the analyses. For 1845 children, data were available on the
horizontally elongated yellow and blue target are four aspects of functional vision: NPC, accommodative
designed to provide optimal stimulation for foveal facility, stereopsis, and the di€erence between far and
blur-driven accommodation (E.R. Howell, personal near phorias.
communication). Stereopsis was assessed using either
the Titmus Stereotest (¯y/animals/Wirt circles) measur-
Refractive error
ing to 40¢¢ (NSW) or the Randot Stereotest (shapes/
animal/circles) measuring to 20¢¢ (Victoria). Accommo- Spherical component. The distribution of the means of
dative facility was assessed over 1 min with ‹1.50 D right and left eyes from each subject for the spherical
¯ippers (NSW) or ‹2.00 D ¯ippers (Victoria) while the component of the refractive error (the more positive
child binocularly ®xated a row of N6 letters held at meridian, not spherical equivalent) is skewed towards
33 cm. In UNSW, ‹1.50 ¯ippers were used to reduce hypermetropia (see Figure 1) with an overall mean of
the number of young children experiencing diculties +0.54 D, ‹0.79 (range )7.25 to +9.25 D, n ˆ 2697
(an interpretation of the data from Scheiman et al., children). Eighty-nine percent of eyes had spherical
1988). In Victoria ‹2 D ¯ippers were used at 40 cm as a components between 0 D and +1.50 D; 97% exhibited
more demanding task for younger children holding spherical components between )1.75 D and +2.00 D;
books. A zero value was recorded for children unable to 5.3% of eyes were myopic ³0.50 D and 7.7% of eyes
see clearly through either of the lens powers. Colour were more than 1.50D hypermetropic. Only 1.1% of
vision was tested using Ishihara plates (1979, 1980, or eyes exhibited a myopic spherical component ³2.00D;
1985 versions). 2.0% of eyes exhibited hypermetropia >+2.00 D. One

ã 2002 The College of Optometrists


14 Ophthal. Physiol. Opt. 2002 22: No 1

Figure 2. Distribution of the mean cylindrical component of the


refractive error from the two eyes of each child.

Figure 1. Distributions of the mean spherical component of the


refractive error from the two eyes of each child.
There was moderate to high correlation between the
amount of astigmatism in right and left eyes for all age
in 14 of all children assessed (7.1%) already had been groups (see Table 2). Although the older age groups
prescribed spectacles, and one of ®ve (20.9%) were appeared to demonstrate more astigmatism, there was
recommended to return to their practitioner for a review only low correlation with age (r ˆ )0.041, p < 0.05)
of their prescription as a change of at least 0.50 dioptres and the magnitude was below the minimum step used
appeared necessary. clinically.
There was a high correlation (p < 0.0005) between the Of the 1680 (28.5%) eyes with astigmatism >0.25 D
values for mean spherical component for right and left in the current study, approximately 55% exhibited
eyes at all ages (see Table 2), with only 1.4% of 2697 with-the-rule astigmatism (WTR: negative axis within
children showing anisometropia >1.00 DS. There was a 30° of the horizontal), nearly 8% exhibited oblique
trend towards less hypermetropia with age. astigmatism (axis 30±60° or 120±150°) and the remain-
ing 38% exhibited against-the-rule astigmatism (ATR:
Astigmatism. The distribution of the average cylindri- axes 60±120°). Over 80% of children with measurable
cal component for right and left eyes (n ˆ 2697) has a astigmatism in both eyes (n ˆ 589 children) showed
mean of )0.16 D, ‹0.35 (range 0 D to )4.25 D) (see similar axes in the two eyes (r ˆ 0.724, p < 0.01).
Figure 2). Seventy-one per cent of eyes exhibited 48.9% had binocular WTR astigmatism; 0.9% had
negligible astigmatism (i.e. £0.25 DC) and 27% binocular symmetrical (mirror-image) oblique astig-
showed astigmatism between )0.25 D and )1.0 D. matism; 33.2% had binocular ATR astigmatism; 6.1%

Table 2. Spherical and cylindrical components of the refractive error and correlation between right and left eyes
Sphere Cylinder

Age RE DS LE DS RE DC LE DC
(years) (Mean ‹ S.D.) (Mean ‹ S.D.) Correlation (r) (Mean ‹ S.D.) (Mean ‹ S.D.) Correlation (r)

3 0.85 ‹ 0.38 (12) 0.83 ‹ 0.36 (12) 0.898* )0.02 ‹ 0.07 (12) )0.02 ‹ 0.07 (12) 1.00*
4 0.59 ‹ 0.52 (123) 0.62 ‹ 0.54 (123) 0.850* )0.15 ‹ 0.25 (123) )0.13 ‹ 0.21 (123) 0.619*
5 0.64 ‹ 0.53 (348) 0.66 ‹ 0.61 (347) 0.743* )0.17 ‹ 0.39 (348) )0.15 ‹ 0.30 (347) 0.712*
6 0.56 ‹ 0.59 (221) 0.57 ‹ 0.57 (221) 0.883* )0.15 ‹ 0.32 (221) )0.15 ‹ 0.29 (221) 0.690*
7 0.64 ‹ 0.79 (330) 0.64 ‹ 0.72 (330) 0.796* )0.15 ‹ 0.27 (330) )0.13 ‹ 0.34 (330) 0.469*
8 0.60 ‹ 0.91 (471) 0.59 ‹ 0.80 (470) 0.853* )0.16 ‹ 0.35 (471) )0.16 ‹ 0.37 (470) 0.741*
9 0.52 ‹ 0.80 (214) 0.58 ‹ 0.93 (212) 0.873* )0.16 ‹ 0.38 (214) )0.14 ‹ 0.36 (212) 0.845*
10 0.45 ‹ 0.80 (280) 0.48 ‹ 0.87 (279) 0.929* )0.15 ‹ 0.33 (280) )0.14 ‹ 0.35 (279) 0.710*
11 0.36 ‹ 0.86 (532) 0.41 ‹ 1.02 (531) 0.875* (530) )0.18 ‹ 0.36 (532) )0.16 ‹ 0.37 (531) 0.712* (530)
12 0.37 ‹ 0.77 (166) 0.42 ‹ 0.91 (166) 0.916* )0.25 ‹ 0.50 (166) )0.21 ‹ 0.41 (166) 0.843*
all 0.53 ‹ 0.76 (2697) 0.55 ‹ 0.82 (2691) 0.859* (2690) )0.17 ‹ 0.35 (2697) )0.15 ‹ 0.35 (2691) 0.711* (2690)
Numbers in brackets indicate number/pairs of eyes; *denotes signi®cant correlation: p < 0.0005.

ã 2002 The College of Optometrists


School screenings: B. Junghans et al. 15

had WTR in one eye and ATR in the other. The The mean di€erence between far and near horizontal
remaining 10% had asymmetrical oblique axes in the phorias was relatively exo (0.92D ‹ 2.34, n ˆ 2126) (see
two eyes. Table 3) with a range from 20 more exo to 20D more eso
(Figure 6). The number of children exhibiting a far to
near exo shift ³4 was 212 (9.9%), and 23 (1.1%) showed
Near point of convergence
an eso shift >4. An eso shift from far to near of any
Mean values for NPC for di€erent age groups are shown magnitude was found in 16.7% of children. There was
in Table 3 and the distribution is shown in Figure 3. The no signi®cant change in phoria measures at far or near
NPC (break) ranged from 2 cm (`to the nose') to 30 cm with increasing age.
(mean 5.4 cm, ‹2.9, n ˆ 2524). Analysis of the variance Twenty-nine children exhibited horizontal phorias
across all age groups indicated that there was no main ³10D exo at near (1.3% of 2280), and 33% of these also
e€ect for NPC to vary with age, although there was a revealed an NPC greater than 10 cm. Of the 212 (10%
mild trend for NPC to be more remote with increasing of 2126) children with a distance to near exo shift ³4, 60
age. However, 17.2% of children had NPC ³7.5 cm, (20.3%) exhibited an NPC ³7.5 cm (2.1% of total).
11% exhibited values ³9 cm and 2.6% of children 14.2% of myopes ³)0.50 D were eso at near.
exhibited values ³12 cm.
Stereopsis
Heterophoria and strabismus
The distribution of stereopsis ®ndings from 2697 chil-
Only eight children (0.30% of 2697 children) were dren is shown in Figure 7. The median value for
reported to have strabismus (one speci®ed as intermit- stereopsis was 40¢¢. In this situation the median is more
tent). The distributions for distance horizontal hetero- representative of central tendency than the mean
phoria, near horizontal heterophoria and the di€erence because of the skewed non-parametric demand of the
between the two are shown in Figures 4±6. Distance tasks in the Titmus ¯y and Randot tests. However,
horizontal phorias showed a leptokurtotic distribution 26.9% of children showed stereopsis less than 70¢¢.
with a mean of 0.12D exo, ‹1.58 (see Table 3) with Notably, ®ve of the eight children listed as strabismic
range 10D exo to 20D eso, n ˆ 2239. 1.0% of the children recorded some level of stereopsis (2 at 400¢¢, 1 at 200¢¢, 1
were >5D exo at 3 m and 0.3% >8D exo; 27.3% dem- at 100¢¢ and 1 at 40¢¢). In addition, four of the ®ve
onstrated some degree of eso; however, only 0.5% children with signi®cant vertical phoria (all around 2D)
were >5D eso. The mean phoria at near was 1.05D exo, recorded between 40¢¢ and 140¢¢ stereopsis. There was a
‹2.53 (n ˆ 2282) (see Table 3) with a range from mild improvement in stereopsis with age (p ˆ 0.0194).
22D exo to 21D eso. At near, 11.9% children displayed
>3D eso; 1.3% of children had exo >9D, and 0.5% had
Accommodative facility
>5D eso.
Five children of the 896 (0.55%) tested for vertical The mean values for the accommodative facility of the
phoria at near were found to have latent deviations NSW children using ‹1.50 D ¯ippers are shown in
greater than 0.5D. Only one of these was classi®ed as Table 3, and the distribution of these is shown in
strabismic (9D hyper). Figure 8. The addition of the results of the 79 Victorian

Table 3. Mean values for near point of convergence (NPC), distance phoria, near phoria and the difference between the near and the distance
phorias, and accommodative facility for different age groups
Age Accommodative
(years) NPC (cm) Distance phoria (D) Near phoria (D) Near±distance phoria (D) facility (cpm)

3 4.2 ‹ 2.1 (11) ± ± ± 9.3 ‹ 0.6 (3)


4 4.6 ‹ 3.1 (106) 0.15 ‹ 1.41 (62) )0.74 ‹ 2.17 (72) )0.89 ‹ 2.43 (62) 11.4 ‹ 3.6 (33)
5 4.7 ‹ 2.2 (310) )0.20 ‹ 1.28 (304) )1.24 ‹ 2.05 (267) )1.04 ‹ 2.18 (225) 11.3 ‹ 3.4 (197)
6 5.0 ‹ 2.4 (208) )0.15 ‹ 1.36 (179) )1.07 ‹ 2.09 (196) )0.86 ‹ 2.03 (178) 11.0 ‹ 3.2 (175)
7 5.5 ‹ 3.2 (310) )0.15 ‹ 1.46 (309) )1.42 ‹ 2.94 (332) )1.19 ‹ 2.50 (308) 10.8 ‹ 3.5 (303)
8 5.3 ‹ 2.7 (443) )0.20 ‹ 1.49 (376) )0.90 ‹ 2.32 (382) )0.76 ‹ 2.23 (363) 11.1 ‹ 3.8 (427)
9 5.4 ‹ 2.9 (200) )0.03 ‹ 2.01 (175) )0.83 ‹ 2.79 (183) )0.82 ‹ 2.40 (172) 11.2 ‹ 3.7 (199)
10 5.6 ‹ 2.7 (263) )0.16 ‹ 1.57 (189) )0.93 ‹ 2.72 (189) )0.72 ‹ 2.25 (185) 11.0 ‹ 4.1 (259)
11 6.0 ‹ 3.1 (518) )0.03 ‹ 1.83 (468) )0.93 ‹ 2.63 (485) )0.92 ‹ 2.53 (461) 11.3 ‹ 3.7 (494)
12 6.5 ‹ 3.5 (155) )0.21 ‹ 1.29 (177) )1.12 ‹ 2.52 (176) )0.93 ‹ 2.24 (172) 11.8 ‹ 3.7 (156)
All 5.4 ‹ 2.9 (2524) )0.12 ‹ 1.58 (2239) )1.05 ‹ 2.53 (2282) )0.92 ‹ 2.34 (2126) 11.2 ‹ 3.7 (2370)
Numbers in brackets refer to the number of children.

ã 2002 The College of Optometrists


16 Ophthal. Physiol. Opt. 2002 22: No 1

Figure 3. Distribution of values for the near point of convergence


(cm). Figure 6. Distribution of values for the difference between distance
phoria and near phoria (D). A negative value indicates that the near
phoria is relatively more exophoric than the distance phoria.

Figure 4. Distribution of heterophoria values at distance (D) (neg-


ative values indicate exophoria, positive values indicate esophoria).
Figure 7. Distribution of stereopsis values (¢¢arc).

Figure 5. Distribution of heterophoria values at near (D) (negative


values indicate exophoria, positive values indicate esophoria). Figure 8. Distribution of values for accommodative facility (cpm).

ã 2002 The College of Optometrists


School screenings: B. Junghans et al. 17

children (for whom ‹2 ¯ippers were used) does not children (n ˆ 5). In summary, 978 children (53.0% of the
alter the values. The mean values for NSW and Victoria 1845) demonstrated between one and four poor results
did not di€er signi®cantly from each other, being on the battery of four tests of functional vision.
11.2 ‹ 3.7 cpm (range 0±24 cpm, n ˆ 2370) and
10.2 cpm, ‹4.1 (range 0±24 cpm, n ˆ 79), respectively,
Decision to take further action
with an overall mean of 11.2 cpm.
Of the 416 children who had NPC ³ 7.5 cm and for In all cases the decision to refer was made by the
whom accommodative facility was also measured, 41 or supervising optometrist. This decision was based on the
9.8% had accommodative facility <7 cpm (1.8% of the complete clinical pro®le of each child; that justi®cation
2303 who had both functions measured). Of the 72 of the reasoning was not recorded. Reports sent to
children with signi®cant near phoria (>10 exo or parents recommending further assessment classi®ed
>4 eso) only ®ve children (6.9%) rated poorly on problems in broad layperson's terms, e.g. `focusing',
accommodative facility (<7 cpm). `eye co-ordination', or `ocular health', rather than giving
a speci®c diagnosis. Thus, the referral data shown in
Table 5 represent a post hoc appraisal of the record
Colour vision
cards for the 495 children referred (19.9% of 2490).
One hundred and seventy-nine of 2330 (7.68%) children Uncorrected anisometropesP1.00 DS made up 3.0%
failed more than two colour plates on the Ishihara Test. of those referred. Poor binocular co-ordination alone
was one reason for referral (42.0% of 495, 8.35% of
2490). However, 68 children who were referred for
Analysis of groupings of functional vision problems
signi®cant refractive error also showed mildly impaired
The frequency with which children failed one or more of binocularity on at least one test and 116 referred for
the test of binocular vision is presented in Table 4. Of refractive error showed unsatisfactory binocularity on
the 1845 children who completed all four binocular tests, at least one test. In particular, of those who were
39.2% (724 children) demonstrated inadequate function signi®cantly anisometropic or astigmatic (n ˆ 98) there
on one of the tests: 17% exhibited unusual phoria shifts were 78 (80%) who had at least one poor aspect of
from distance to near (11.3% a relative convergence, functional vision. For 44 children the reason for referral
5.7% a relative divergence of ³ 4), 13.3% had stereopsis was not clear from the record card, and for 11 (0.4% of
³80¢¢, 6.8% had binocular accommodative facility below the total number of children seen) there was ocular
7 cpm, and 2.2% had a NPC > 10 cm. pathology.
Two hundred and ten children (11.4% of 1845) failed Table 6 summarises the ®ndings for the 2490 children
to meet clinical criteria for two aspects of functional considered to have received a full `screening'. Of these,
vision: stereopsis was poor in 74.9% of these cases (158), 177 (7.1%) were already wearing spectacles, 37 of whom
vergence errors from far to near were present for 63.6% (20.9% of those already wearing spectacles) were in need
(134) of these cases, and accommodative facility was of a re®nement to their refractive correction of at least
poor in 43.1% (91) of the 210 cases. Only 2.1% of the 0.50 D. A further 34 children were receiving care: three
1845 (n ˆ 39) performed poorly on three tests of apparently for monitoring of low myopia, 12 for
binocularity; of these 51.3% (n ˆ 20) showed poor monitoring hypermetropia P 1 D, seven for previous
accommodative facility, poor stereopsis, and excessive pathology (representing 0.3% of 2490), 10 probably for
vergence shifts from far to near, whilst almost a further binocular anomalies (deduced either by the child
quarter (23.1%, n ˆ 9) manifested the same cluster of describing the prescription of `eye exercises' or from
problems except that the vergence change was excess- their poor performance at our screening) and two
ively exophoric. A poor performance on all four children reported being examined for colour vision
binocularity tests was found in only 0.3% of the 1845 defects.
In summary, further assessment was recommended
for 19.9% of the total of 2490 children screened with
Table 4. Failure rates on near point of convergence, heterophoria,
stereopsis and accommodative facility for the 1845 children
8.47% reporting being under care already.
assessed for all four binocular parameters
Number of tests failed n % Discussion
None 867 47.0 The data presented in the current study re¯ect real
One 724 39.2 population norms as the population is non-self-selecting
Two 210 11.4 as opposed to norms established from a clinical prac-
Three 39 2.1
tice. This study aimed to report not only the parameters
Four 5 0.3
of individual tests for a group of children with a large

ã 2002 The College of Optometrists


18 Ophthal. Physiol. Opt. 2002 22: No 1

Table 5. Analysis of ocular parameters for


% of 495
all children deemed to require further
Parameter n children
assessment
Already in spectacles 37 7.47
No previous Rx given 458 92.53
Refractive error Hyperopia > 1.50D 81 16.36
Hyperopia of 1.50D 21 4.24
Myopia > 0.75D 102 20.61
Myopia of 0.50D 15 3.03
Anisometropia > 1.00 DS 37 7.47
Astigmatism > 0.75 DC 44 8.89
Astigmatism of 0.75 DC 18 3.64
with binocular vision problems 164 33.13
Binocular vision problems 1 borderline result 92 18.59
2 borderline results 46 9.29
1 unsatisfactory result 94 18.99
2 unsatisfactory result 29 5.86
1 borderline + 1 unsatisfactory 60 12.12
>2 borderline or unsatisfactory 50 10.10
Pathology, colour vision or unclear 68 13.73

Table 6. Summary of children either refer-


Number of % of 2490
red for, or presenting with, signi®cant
children complete cases
visual problems
Referred refractive error alone 52 2.09
Referred refractive error and binocular vision anomaly 167 6.71
Referred binocular vision anomalies alone 208 8.35
Referred other reason (pathology, colour vision only, 68 2.73
or unclear from record card)
Subtotal 495 19.88
Already under care, not wearing spectacles 34 1.36
Already under care, wearing appropriate spectacles 140 5.62
Total 669 26.86

and diverse ethnic base, but also to investigate reasons referral rates. There was heavy reliance on class teachers
for referral and examine the referral rate due to to identify the child's symptoms. This does not detract
abnormal binocular function on a particular set of tests from the results presented, but emphasises the need for
which are intrinsically likely to a€ect academic per- school screenings in multi-racial settings to be wisely
formance. planned and properly marketed to ensure sound out-
The screening was carried out by optometry students comes.
who were within 6 months of graduation. The fact that
this screening was part of a programmed school
Refractive errors
excursion might easily have led to boredom and
distractions, and hence more readily revealed anomalies, Refraction and age. A relatively predictable change in
or, it may have led to subtleties being missed as a result refractive status occurs during childhood (Hirsch and
of increased visual interest and attention engendered by Weymouth 1991). A similar result has been found in the
the surroundings. In addition, the battery of tests was current investigation.
designed to re¯ect Australian practice in the early 1990s,
and the needs for our students of optometry to Spherical component. One in every 14 children seen was
experience paediatrics. The battery was not targeted as already wearing a refractive correction, but one of ®ve
`the optimum' screening on a cost±bene®t basis, of these in our opinion were no longer adequately
although some of the conclusions might have applica- corrected. Eighty-nine percent of the children screened
tion to the design of such a test battery. aged between 3 and 12 years demonstrated a spherical
The non-English speaking background of most of the component for the refractive error (expressed in negative
families in this study diminished the usefulness of cylinder form) between 0 and +1.50 D, 2% showed
presenting symptoms as part of a problem-based hypermetropia greater than +2.00 D, 5.3% were ³ 0.50
approach to case analysis and may have impacted on myopic and only 1.1% showed myopia ³ 2.00 D.

ã 2002 The College of Optometrists


School screenings: B. Junghans et al. 19

Morgan and Kennemer (1997) analysed photorefrac- (Amigo et al., 1976). The tendency for an increase in
tion photographs of 14 000 US primary-school children astigmatism with age (0.23 D cyl from age 3±12) in our
of which 4.6% displayed signi®cant anomalies (hyper- group of children was similar to that observed by Hirsch
opia ³2.5 D, myopia ³1.0 D, anisometropia ³1.0 D, (cited in Lyle, 1991) who noted that 81% of 6 year olds
media opacity ³1.0 mm in diameter, ocular misalign- but only 72% for 12 year olds exhibited negligible
ment ³5°/10 prism dioptres), and a further 9.7% showed astigmatism. Like the study of Lyle (1991), the current
possibly signi®cant ®ndings (same signs but lesser study found that more eyes of school-age children (55%)
degree). Of these, 4.5% were myopic, 3.1% anisome- exhibit with-the-rule refractive astigmatism than
tropic and 2.9% hypermetropic. Lam et al. (1996) against-the-rule astigmatism. However, a recent study
studying children in Nova Scotia, reported mean values by Chua et al. (2001) reports that in Singaporean school
of +0.69 D, ‹0.53 for the spherical component children aged 7±9 years almost 94% have with-the-rule
(+1.01 D using negative cylinder form). Ninety-six astigmatism if astigmatism is present.
percent of eyes were between +0.07 and +2.07 (using
negative cylinder form); indicating a skewing towards
Functional vision
hyperopia. Moreover, 48.1% of children had no meas-
urable di€erence between the two eyes for the spherical As this project comprised of an one-o€ screening of each
component, while 98.1% exhibited less than 0.75 D child, the data portrays the status of near vision
di€erence. In comparison, our ®nding that 97% of captured as a re¯ection of each child's habitual vision,
errors lie between )1.75 D and +2.00 D indicates a though, for the few for whom a signi®cant (>0.50 D)
considerable myopic shift in our population which may change in refractive error was detected, the functional
be attributable to the high number of Asian children in tests were carried out through the new prescription.
our study. Therefore, 1.4% of the results presumably showed less
Other Australian studies of non-selected children have binocular problems than had been operating in the
included assessment of refractive errors. Walters general population, though this may not necessarily be
(1984a,b) studied a group of 5597 Victorian rural the case (Dwyer, 1992).
children aged 4±14 years (mean 10.4 ‹ 1.2 years) from It might be argued that the battery of tests selected for
predominantly Caucasian low socio-economic back- use at VEC UNSW does not represent the best selection
grounds. Refractive errors ranged from high plus to of tests to detect unstable binocularity for near vision,
high minus, however, 90% showed retinoscopic ®ndings however, at the time of the design of the screening this
between )0.25 and +1.25 D (mean +0.39 D, ‹0.89). was not the primary aim.
Similarly, Amigo et al. (1976), studying rural underpri- In retrospect for example, it could be argued that the
vileged children from NSW (n ˆ 1166), found 91% with aligning prism required to eliminate any ®xation dis-
retinoscopic ®ndings between 0.00 and +1.25 (mean parity on a Mallett unit (associated phoria) as predom-
approximately +0.5 D). By contrast, Robbins and inantly used in the UK should be measured in preference
Bailey (1975) studied a relatively high socio-economic to dissociated phoria. Wick and London (1987) argued
group of children (n ˆ 1243) aged 3±12 years, and found for the use of ®xation disparity testing, amongst other
that only a total of 6.6% were myopic ³0.50 D and only tests, to elucidate which component of binocularity
9.0% were hyperopic ³1.50 D, which raises the question contributes most to visual distress. Yekta et al. (1989)
whether the di€erent ethnic background for our popu- also found a relationship between near point symptoms
lation is as signi®cant a refractive problem as one might and associated phoria using a modi®ed Mallett unit,
have anticipated. rather than between dissociated heterophoria and symp-
toms. They studied a sample of 187 Iranians aged 10±
Astigmatism. The degree and orientation of astigmatism 65 years visiting an optometry clinic, although in that
are important components of refractive error, although study almost no ®xation disparity (0.09 ‹ 0.78¢) was
many studies mask the degree of astigmatism by found in those aged under 20 years. By comparison,
reporting only the equivalent sphere (Hirsch and Wey- Sheedy and Saladin (1977) found that dissociated
mouth, 1991). In the current study, 71% of all eyes heterophoria was a better discriminator than ®xation
exhibited negligible astigmatism and only 2% of all disparity for esophores with symptoms amongst optom-
children had astigmatism greater than 1.00 DC. This etry students, although their later study favoured the use
supports the observations that over 72% of eyes of of ®xation disparity (Sheedy and Saladin, 1978). The
children exhibit no astigmatism (Hirsch, cited in Lyle, clinic in the School of Optometry, UNSW, did not use
1991), and that over 90% have less than 1.00 D of ®xation disparity as a tool, as Alexander (1990) found
astigmatism (Lam et al., 1996). Similarly, only 8% of that there was a 7 min of arc calibration error on the
non-clinically sourced Australian children aged 5± Sheedy Fixation Disparometer. Furthermore, it is well
14 years showed greater than 0.50 D of astigmatism known that ®xation disparity may be present with an

ã 2002 The College of Optometrists


20 Ophthal. Physiol. Opt. 2002 22: No 1

absence of symptoms (Jenkins et al., 1989). This clouds symptoms often cannot be clearly established. They
the usefulness of ®xation disparity as a screener in a noted the relevance that the input of accommodative
population of children for whom the symptomatology is vergence plays in the choice of target, and hence NPC. A
unclear and when time is a factor. We cannot comment possible reason that our ®ndings yielded a more remote
on the ecacy of the tests chosen for the screening, as average NPC of 5.7 cm is that this test was one of a
our cohort of children have not been followed to large battery of tests in the screening (and often
determine whether the recommendations to refer were performed after other tests), whereas in Hayes et al.
satisfactory or not. (1998) normative study of NPC, the attention of the
However, it is hoped that the discussion which follows clinician and subjects was more clearly on the accurate
on each of elements of the screening, plus the analysis of conduct of the test.
the clusters of abnormal results along with the decision
to refer or not, helps in shaping decisions of how to Heterophoria. The data presented here represent the
handle mass vision screenings which target functional largest evaluation of horizontal heterophoria using the
vision. distance (3 m) and near (33 cm) Howell Phoria Cards
on children, and concur with the unpublished data
Strabismus. The presence of strabismus in this cohort of collected by Howell himself (personal communication).
children was found to be extremely low (0.3%) com- Distance lateral phoria was marginally exophoric, and
pared with other studies, e.g. 1±2% (Ciner et al. 1998) near was less than 1.5 more so. The results were not
up to even 8% (The 1986/87 Future of Visual Develop- signi®cantly di€erent from those of Walline et al. (1998),
ment/Performance Task Force, 1988). A probable who used subjective evaluation of the cover test on 1495
explanation is that our supervising clinicians are reluct- children in kindergarten, second and ®fth grades. Both
ant to apply the term `strabismic' if children with studies have shown that nearly all primary-school
misalignment of the visual axes as indicated by the cover children are orthophoric sofar, and that there is a slight
test are able to compensate sucient to demonstrate exo but more variable shift for near. As in Walline's
stereopsis of 100¢¢ arc or better. study, there was no evidence of signi®cant change of
distance phoria across age groups, and we found also
Stereopsis. The median value for stereopsis in the that 14% of myopes are esophoric at near. In another
current study using the Titmus (contoured) Stereotest Australian study, the majority of eyes of school-aged
was 40¢¢ of arc (range 20±800¢¢). 73.1% of children had children exhibited a low heterophoria: between 1 eso
stereopsis of 70¢¢ or better, which is considered normal and 4 exo at near, with the peak of distribution also at
by Simons (Simons 1986) and Lam (Lam et al. 1996), 1 exo (Walters, 1984a).
and better than normal by Osipov (Osipov 1996). Lam A signi®cant increase in exophoria when viewing at
et al. (1996) found that 83.3% of children achieved 50¢¢ near is central to the diagnosis of convergence insu-
or better using the Randot Stereotest (circles) which is a ciency according to Duane (reviewed Rouse et al.,
local test measuring down to 20¢¢. However, in the main 1998). Overall 9.9% of children in the present study
we used the Titmus Fly test which measures only down manifest a near exophoric shift ³ 4. These ®gures
to 40¢¢. Thus there is a possibility that the Titmus Fly compare well with those of Rouse et al. (1999), who
test may underestimate stereopsis in children. The values found 7.4% of white children 9±13 years old and greater
of 20¢¢ in the current study were obtained for Victorian proportions of black, Hispanic or Asian children
subjects where the Randot test was used. (although few children were of Asian origin) presented
with low suspect CI (near exophoria plus one of:
Near point of convergence. In the current study 82.8% excessive relative exophoria at near, a remote NPC or
of children could maintain single vision as close as poor fusional reserves). However, only 2.1% of 2078
7.5 cm using an accommodative target and 89% of children in our study also exhibited a near point of
values for NPC (break) were less than 10 cm. Walters convergence ³ 7.5 cm, which falls between the divisions
(1984a) similarly found that 89% of schoolchildren have of low and high suspect convergence insuciency by
NPC £ 8 cm. However, Hayes et al. (1998) found that Rouse et al. ¢s criteria. This appears to be substantially
85% of elementary school children who had already lower than the ®gure of 8.7% for white children of
passed a Modi®ed Clinical Technique screening had high suspicion for having CI in Rouse's study, and
NPC £ 6 cm when taken as an average of three repeti- considering the high numbers of Asians in our sample,
tions using a target on nearpoint rule, with a mean of very di€erent to their observation that Hispanics and
approximately 4 cm. They concluded that a cut-o€ of Asian are even more likely to have CI. Rouse et al.
6 cm should be used for NPC break in a clinic setting, (1998) reported a frequency of 8.4% for `de®nite'
but that a looser 6±10 cm pass/fail criterion should be convergence insuciency in New York (428, 8±12 years
used in screening situations where the coexistence of olds), but a frequency of only 4.9% in Southern

ã 2002 The College of Optometrists


School screenings: B. Junghans et al. 21

California and considered that the di€erence was the acuity/suppression slide (VO/9), and a bar-reader with a
result of the di€erent ethnic proportions in the two di€erent age-appropriate word scrolled into view after
groups with a greater proportion of African American each ¯ip. The bar reader consisted of a 2-mm wide
(53%) and Hispanics (23.3%) in New York than in vertical bar placed approximately 30 mm in front of a
Southern California (3.8 and 9.2%, respectively). In square box with a 15 ´ 6 mm window which allowed
non-clinical populations of children aged 9±13 years scrolling of single words drawn from a list of 16 words
in three states in northern America Rouse et al. (1998) taken from the composite Dolch and Hillerich Basic
found that only 4.2% were de®nite CI (near exophoria Sight Word list each of six letters and printed in upper
and ³ 4 exophoric from far phoria, NPC > 7.5 cm case ®ve point New York font with a space between
break, poor fusional reserves), 8.8% were low suspects each letter. Only children aged 9±12 years (n ˆ 72) who
and 8.4% were high suspects. There was a signi®cant had passed all aspects of the VEC examination other
ethnic di€erence with respect to classi®cation of CI (9% than accommodative facility were included. Testing was
were Afro-American, 5% Hispanic and a little over over 1 min for each target, with randomised order of
1% Asian-Paci®c). Using the same classi®cation, only targets.
2.1% of the children in the current study are de®nitely Analysis of variance across the three versions of the
CI and 5.9% are low CI. These di€erences may re¯ect accommodative facility test indicated that the order of
the di€ering ethnic diversities of the populations (there presentation of the tests was insigni®cant, as was the
are few Afro-American or Hispanics in Australia but di€erence between binocular accommodative facility
many Asians), or it may be because of the di€erent age with ‹1.50 ¯ippers using a polarised target (mean
range in the two studies. 8.0 ‹ 3.5 cpm) and a bar reader (mean 8.8 ‹ 3.1 cpm).
However, the simple black and white target without
Accommodative facility. Accommodative facility is a suppression checks resulted in a mean facility of
measure of the continuing ability of accommodation to 11.0 ‹ 3.6 cpm which was signi®cantly di€erent
change rapidly in response to stimulation and inhibi- (p < 0.05) from the other two versions of the test
tion when vergence is ®xed. In the current study using (Fisher LSD protected Student t-test). This demon-
‹1.50 ¯ippers the mean value for accommodative strates that targets without a suppression check lead to
facility was 11.2 ‹ 3.7 cpm (2330 children) with a less demand on the patient to perform the task under
range from 0 to 24 cpm. Low values of accommodative conditions of true binocularity, therefore resulting in
facility have been associated with symptoms related to measures of accommodative facility which more closely
near-point asthenopia in young children (Siderov, resemble monocular values. Thus we believe that a
1990). Mean binocular accommodative facility (‹2 D comparison between accommodative facility testing
¯ippers, polarised acuity suppression target) has been using targets with and without suppression checks, with
reported to be 7.7 ‹ 5.2 cycles per minute (cpm) for due regard for a learning e€ect, may be useful in
subjects aged 18±30 with normal binocular vision assessing the degree to which a child suppresses under a
(Zellers et al., 1984), whereas much lower results have visually demanding task.
been reported for over 500 children by Scheiman et al. It is not possible to ascertain why only 68% of
(1988) who found 5 ‹ 2.25 cpm binocularly for the 8± children had all four functional tests completed,
12 years olds and 3 ‹ 2.5 binocularly for the 6 years although there is a trend for fewer of the youngest
olds when the children were required to read out children to be in this group (especially those under the
several letters as con®rmation that sucient clarity had age of 6 years). One could speculate that results of
been achieved. McKenzie and colleagues used similar qualitative observations during the screening drove the
conditions to those of Zellers et al. (1984) and found inclusion of near point testing for some, but resulted in a
that 26% of a group of 140 children aged 8±12 years, `reasoned' omission for others.
failed accommodative facility when the criterion was The current study has shown that 53% of the children
8 cpm or better (McKenzie et al., 1987). The mean for who completed the battery of four functional tests had
binocular accommodative facility in the current study at least one poor ®nding. However, only 13.8% had at
was signi®cantly higher than the 7±8 cpm using least two problems and 2.4% at least three. This result is
‹2.00 D ¯ippers noted by McKenzie et al. (1987), considerably higher than the prevalence of any form of
although only a high contrast black and white near binocular defect reported by Walters (1984a), who
letter chart with no suppression check was used in our found that only of 36.7% failed one binocular vision
screening. test in a group of probably less racially diverse Austra-
To clarify the interpretation of our data, an addi- lian children undergoing a similar range of tests.
tional study was conducted to compare binocular However, Walters used much more stringent criteria
accommodative facility for three di€erent targets: black for accommodative facility. The association of binocular
and white letters (unpolarised), the Bernell vectographic dysfunction with refractive errors in the present study

ã 2002 The College of Optometrists


22 Ophthal. Physiol. Opt. 2002 22: No 1

was similar to that found by Dwyer (1992) in a clinic- priate test set with far less risk of false negative
based setting. outcomes, but which might include items that are never
considered for a screening on a cost±bene®t basis. As
Decision to take further action. A retrospective analysis children in the current study were lost to follow-up, it
seeking why children were referred is presented in has not been possible to review the results of further
Tables 4 and 5. Despite 53% of children having failed assessment as regards the relative numbers of false
at least one aspect of functional vision testing, another positives and negatives. However, the supervising
8.8% requiring correction of signi®cant refractive error clinicians responsible for the referrals may have devi-
and 2.6% requiring counseling for defective colour ated from the original set of tests and moved towards a
vision, only 19.9% of the 2490 were deemed to have more case-appropriate eye examination using evidence-
failed the screening and require further examination. based criteria derived from their own experience. This
The most straightforward decision to refer is for those could have resulted in the mismatch between the VEC
requiring refractive correction (8.8% referrals), yet some guidelines for referral and the actual pattern of
children were handled contrary to the stated guidelines. referrals.
Regarding analysis of vision at near, when to refer is less
clear. Should one borderline result alone demand
Conclusion
further assessment? As there was no pattern as to
whether a certain number of borderline results in a The current project presents oculo-visual characteristics
referral, it appears that decisions to refer have been (including refractive and functional vision measures) for
based on clinical intuition. a large group of urban multicultural Australian children
The addition of children already under care but not aged 3±12 years.
requiring immediate attention, brought the number who By undertaking analysis of the large VEC data set as a
failed the screening guidelines to 26.86%, which is whole and referring to population norms from the
similar to the 20% reported for males and 26% for literature, it has been possible to establish problems in
females after MCT examination of a whole primary delivery of vision care during the screening which would
school by Coleman (1970), and to the 29.2% found by not have been apparent by review of individual records.
Robbins and Bailey (1975) in a large study of children One example is the di€erent accommodative facility
aged 3±11 years (also using the MCT), and the 28.1% results, which led to an examination of the actual
found by Dwyer (1983) using an expanded version of the protocol used. Another is the role of the attending
MCT that included accommodative-convergence tests. optometrist.
In some instances (approximately 2%), it was not As the highly regarded MCT does not address non-
possible to infer from the record card exactly why the strabismic binocular disorders, the current study
child was referred for further assessment. This highlights attempts to establish the suitability of a set of referral
the diculties for the supervising optometrist in articu- criteria regarding binocular vision. In their current
lating the reason why a consultation has been brought to form, the set of VEC referral guidelines do not match
a particular conclusion, given that the parent is not those used by the supervising clinicians when given
present, and, as is the case in most screenings, the freedom to use their own judgment. The lack of a clear
optometrist has not himself carried out all of the testing. pattern guiding decisions to refer in this study suggests
Indeed, when constructing the Portsea Modi®ed Clinical that, when working within a limited time frame,
Technique (Dwyer, 1983) for use at summer camps, it experienced clinicians ultimately decide on the basis of
was decided that it was more appropriate to refer based clinical intuition. This lack of a `referral formula' has
upon the clinician's opinion that `the child would bene®t implications for the stang of screenings of school
from full clinical assessment' rather than on speci®c pre- children aged 5±12 years, namely that optometrists
determined criteria. One particular diculty encoun- conversant with the appraisal of the subtleties of near
tered for our screening was the poor ability of parents of point visual performance must be in attendance and
non-English speaking backgrounds to convey symptoms have sucient time themselves to be one-to-one with
and history via a simple questionnaire. Hence, decisions each child. However, our data do not allow an evalu-
to refer have been made in the light of an unreliable ation of the ecacy of this clinical intuition in detecting
understanding of the child's background. visual anomalies.
Hoppe (1997) notes that epidemiological considera-
tions drive optometric clinical decision-making di€er-
Acknowledgements
ently when a number of tests have been carried out in
parallel as occurs in a screening, rather than when The work of Year 4 students School of Optometry
testing is based upon problems elicited during history UNSW Nuong Le and Cathy Seto on the comparison of
and symptom taking. The latter drives a case-appro- the three methods of accommodative facility testing is

ã 2002 The College of Optometrists


School screenings: B. Junghans et al. 23

gratefully acknowledged. SGC's salary was funded by Dwyer, P. S. (1983) The Portsea Lord Mayor's children's camp
National Health and Medical Research Council `A vision screening: a rationale and protocol for optometric
temporal mechanism for the acuity de®cit in strabismic screening. Aust. J. Optom. 66, 178±185.
amblyopia' 1994±96; PMK's salary was funded by Dwyer, P. S. (1992) The prevalence of vergence accommoda-
tion disorders in a school-age population. Clin. Exp. Optom.
NHMRC `A role for the outer retina in the control of
75, 18±20.
axial refractive error' 1997±99 and by Australian
Evans, B. J., Patel, R., Wilkins, A. J., Eperjesi, F., Speedwell,
Research Council `Psychophysiology of Transient Pro- L. and Du€y, J. A. (1999) Review of the management of
cessing in Reading' 2000. We also thank Patricia 3323 consecutive patients seen in a speci®c learning dicul-
Munoz, Bernice Webster and Murray Lawson for their ties clinic. Ophthal Physiol. Opt. 19, 454±466.
assistance. Evans, B. J. (1998) The underachieving child. Ophthal Physiol.
Opt. 18, 153±159.
Fowler, M. S. (1991) Binocular control in dyslexics. In: Vision
References and Vision Dyslexia ± Vision and Visual Dysfunction, Vol. 13
Alexander, J. A. (1990) Calbration of the Sheedy ®xation (ed. J. F. Stein) Macmillan Press, London, pp. 141±147.
disparometer. Optom Vis Sci. 67, 90. Garzia, R. P. and Franzel, A. S. (1996) Refractive status,
Amigo, G., McCarthy, A. and Pye, D. (1976) Visual charac- binocular vision and reading achievement. In: Vision and
teristics of an underprivileged group of Australian children. Reading, Ch 6 (ed. R. P. Garzia) Mosby, St. Louis, MO,
Aust. J. Optom. 59, 188±197. 111±131.
Atkinson, J. (ed.) (2000) The Developing Visual Brain. Oxford Grisham, J. D. and Simons, H. D. (1986) Refractive error and
University Press, New York. the reading process: a literature analysis. J. Am. Optom.
Bailey, N. R. (1998) Assessing the predicative ability of the Assoc. 57, 44±55.
test-positive ®ndings of an elementary school vision screen- Hagerman, R. and Cronister, A. (1996) Fragile X Syndrome:
ing. Optom. Vis. Sci. 75, 682±691. Diagnosis, Treatment and Research. John Hopkins Univer-
Bear, J. C. (1991) Epidemiology and genetics of refractive sity Press, Baltimore.
anomalies. In: Refractive Anomalies. Research and Clinical Hayes, G. J., Cohen, B. E., Rouse, M. W. and De Land, P. N.
Implications (eds T. Grosvenor and M. C. Flom) Butter- (1998) Normative values for the near point of convergence
worth-Heinemann, Stoneham, MA, pp. 57±80. of elementary schoolchildren. Optom. Vis. Sci. 75, 506±512.
Bennet, G. R., Blondin, M. and Ruskiewicz, J. (1982) Hennessey, D., Iosue, R. A. and Rouse, M. W. (1984) Relation
Incidence and prevalence of selected visual conditions. J. of symptoms to accommodative infacility of school-aged
Am. Optom. Assoc. 53, 647±656. children. Am. J Optom Physiol Opt. 61, 177±183.
Blum, H., Peters, H. B. and Bettman, J. W. (1968) Vision Hirsch, M. J., Weymouth, F. W. (1991) Prevalence of refractive
Screening for Schools 1968 The Orinda Study. University of anomalies. In: Refractive Anomalies. Research and Clinical
California Press, Berkeley, CA. Implications (eds T. Grosvenor and M. C. Flom) Butter-
Borish, I. M. (1975) Phorometry. In: Clinical Refraction. worth-Heinemann, Stoneham, Boston, MA, pp. 21±25.
Professional Press, Chicago, IL, USA, pp. 816±818. Hoppe, E. (1997) Use of epidemiology in clinical decision
Chua, W. H., Saw, H. M., Wu, C. Y. et al. (2001) Astigmatism making. In: Clinical Decision Making in Optometry (eds
and myopia in Singapore schoolchildren. Invest Ophthalmol E. R. Ettinger and M. W. Rouse) Butterworth-Heinemann,
Vis Sci. 42, S389. Boston, MA, pp. 65±99.
Ciner, E. B., Schmidt, P. P., Orel-Bixler, D., Donson, V., Howell, E. R., Brown, B. and Bailey, I. L. (1980) The Stimulus
Maguire, M., Cyert, L., Moore, B. and Schultz, J. (1998) to Accommodation by the Human Eye. Proceedings of the
Vision screening of pre-school children: evaluating the past, Aust. Physiol. Pharmacol. Soc. May 1980. (by Personal
looking toward the future. Optom. Vis. Sci. 75, 571±584. Communication).
Coleman, H. M. (1970) An analysis of the visual status of an Jenkins, T. C. A., Pickwell, L. D. and Yekta, A. A. (1989)
entire school population. J. Am. Optom. Assoc. 41, 341±347. Criteria for decompensation in binocular vision. Ophthal
Committee on Children with Disabilities, American Academy Physiol. Opt 9, 121±125.
of Pediatrics, American Academy of Ophthalmology, Joint Organizational Policy Statement of the American Acad-
American Association for Pediatric Ophthalmology and emy of Optometry and the American Optometric Associ-
Strabismus (1998) Learning disabilities, dyslexia, and vision: ation. (1997) J. Am. Opt Assoc 68, 284±286.
a subject review. Pediatrics 102, 1217±1219. Junghans, B. M. and Crewther, S. G. (1992) The vision
Cregg, M., Woodhouse, J. M., Pakeman, V. H., Saunders, K. education centre: a multi-level educational tool. J. Optom.
J., Gunter, H. L., Parker, M., Fraser, W. I. and Sastry, P. Education 17, 83±86.
(2001) Accommodation and refractive error in children with Kulp, M. T. and Schmidt, P. P. (1996) E€ect of oculmotor and
Down syndrom. cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. other visual skills on reading performance: a literature
Invest. Ophthal. Vis. Sci. 42, 55±63. review. Optom. Vis. Sci. 75, 682±691.
Crewther, S. G., Kiely, P. M., Kok, L.-L. and Crewther, D. P. Lam, C. S. Y. and Goh, W. S. H. (1991) The incidence of
(1998) Anomalies of genetic development as predictors of refractive errors among school children in Hong Kong and
oculo-visual abnormalities in Velo-Cardio±Facial syndrome. its relationship with the optical components. Clin. Exp
Optom. Vis. Sci. 75, 748±757. Optom. 74, 97±103.

ã 2002 The College of Optometrists


24 Ophthal. Physiol. Opt. 2002 22: No 1

Lam, S. R., LaRoche, G. R., De Becker, I. and Macpherson, Siderov, J. (1990) Improving interactive facility with vision
H. (1996) The range and variability of ophthalmological training. Clin. Exp. Optom. 73, 128±131.
parameters in normal children aged 4, 1/2±5, 1/2 years. Simons, K. (1986) Stereoacuity norms in young children. Arch.
J. Pediatr. Ophthalmol. Strabismus 33, 251±256. Ophthalmol. 28, 84±92.
Lovegrove, B. (1996) Dyslexia and a transient/magnocellular Simons, H. D. and Gassler, P. A. (1988) Vision anomalies and
pathway de®cit. the current situation and future directions. reading skill: a meta-analysis of the literature. Am. J. Optom
Aust. J. Psychol 48, 167±171. Physiol. Opt. 11, 893±904.
Lyle, W. M. (1991) Astigmatism. In: Refractive Anomalies. Simons, H. D. and Grisham, J. D. (1986) Vision and reading
Research and Clinical Implications (eds T. Grosvenor and disability: research problems. J. Am. Optom Assoc. 57, 36±41.
M. C. Flom) Butterworth-Heinemann, Stoneham, Boston, Simons, H. D. and Grisham, J. D. (1987) Binocular anomalies
MA, pp. 146±155. and reading problems. J. Am. Optom Assoc 58, 578±587.
McKenzie, K. M., Kerr, S. R., Rouse, M. W. and DeLand, P. The People of New South Wales: Statistics from the, 1996
N. (1987) Study of accommodative facility testing reliability. Census. (1998) Ethnic A€airs Commission of NSW, NSW
Am. J. Optom. Physiol. Opt. 64, 186±194. Government, Sydney, 149±151.
Morgan, K. S. and Kennemer, J. C. (1997) O€-axis photore- The 1986/87 Future of Visual Development/Performance
fractive eye screening in children. J. Cataract Refract. Surg. Task Force. (1988) The ecacy of optometric vision
23, 423±428. therapy. J. Am. Opt Assoc 59, 95±105.
Nathan, J., Kiely, P. M., Crewther, S. G. and Crewther, D. P. Thomson, W. D., Evans, B. (1999) A new approach to vision
(1985) Disease-associated visual image degradation and screening in schools. Ophthal Physiol. Opt. 19I, 196±209.
spherical refractive errors in children. Am. J. Optom. Tsiaras, W. G., Pueschel, S., Keller, C., Curran, R. and
Physiol. Opt. 62, 680±688. Giesswein, S. (1999) Amblyopia and visual acuity in children
Nathan, J., Kiely, P. M., Crewther, S. G. and Crewther, D. P. with Down's syndrome. Br. J. Ophthal. 83, 1112±1114.
(1986) Astigmatism occurring in association with pediatric Walline, J. J., Mutti, D. O., Zadnik, K. and Jones, L. A. (1998)
eye disease. Am. J. Optom. Physiol. Opt. 63, 497±504. Development of phoria in children. Optom. Vis. Sci. 75,
O'Grady, J. (1984) The relationship between vision and 605±610.
educational performance. Aust. J. Optom. 67, 126±140. Walters, J. (1984a) Portsea modi®ed clinical technique: results
Osipov, G. I. (1996) Dynamic stereovisometry. Vestn. Oftal- from an expanded optometric screening protocol for
mol. 112, 35±37. (abstract only, article in Russian). children. Aust. J. Optom. 67, 176±186.
Robbins, H. and Bailey, I. L. (1975) The organisation and Walters, J. (1984b) Portsea modi®ed clinical technique: eval-
®ndings of a school vision screening service. Aust. J. Optom. uation of an expanded optometric vision screening protocol.
58, 392±401. Aust. J. Optom. 67, 212±220.
Rosner, J. and Rosner, J. (1994) The relationship between Wick, B. and London, R. (1987) Analysis of binocular visual
ametropia and elementary school achievement. Br. J. function using tests made under binocular conditions.
Optom. Disp. 2, 8. Am .J. Optom Physiol. Optics. 64, 227±240.
Rouse, M. W., Borsting, E., Hyman, L. et al. (1999) Fre- Yekta, A. A., Pickwell, L. D. and Jenkins, T. C. A. (1989)
quency of convergence insuciency among ®fth and sixth Binocular vision, age and symptoms. Ophthal Physiol. Opt 9,
graders. Optom Vis Sci. 76, 643±649. 11±20.
Rouse, M. W., Hyman, L., Hussein, M. and Solan, H. (1998) Zadnik, K. (1997) The Glenn A. Fry award lecture (1995).
Frequency of convergence insuciency in optometry clinic Myopia development in childhood. Optom. Vis. Sci. 74,
settings. Optom. Vis. Sci. 75, 88±96. 603±608.
Scheiman, M., Herzberg, H. and Frantz, K. (1988) Margolies. Zadnik, K., Mutti, D. O., Friedman, N. E. and Adams, A. J.
Normative study of accommodative facility in elementary (1993) Initial cross-sectional results from the Orinda
school children. Am. J. Optom Physiol. Opt. 65, 127±124. Longitudinal Study of Myopia. Optom. Vis. Sci. 70,
Sheedy, J. E. and Saladin, J. J. (1977) Phoria, vergence, and 750±758.
®xation disparity in oculomotor problems. Am. J. Optom Zellers, J. A., Alpert, T. L. and Rouse, M. W. (1984) A review
Physiol. Opt. 54, 474±478. of the literature and a normative study of accommodative
Sheedy, J. E. and Saladin, J. J. (1978) Association of facility. J. Am. Optom. Assoc. 55, 31±37.
symptoms with measures of oculomotor de®ciencies.
Am. J. Optom Physiol. Opt 55, 670±676.

ã 2002 The College of Optometrists


School screenings: B. Junghans et al. 25

Appendix.
Teacher's checklist
Children with vision problems frequently show recognisable signs. Could you please tick against any of the problems below which apply to this
child.
Pupil's name: ________________
1. Eyes look red or sore _____
2. Screws up eyes when looking in the distance _____
3. Book held close when reading or sits awkwardly _____
4. Complains of: Headaches _____
Dif®culty seeing clearly in the distance _____
Blurring of vision while reading or writing _____
Seeing double _____
Eye burning or itching during or after close work _____
Parent's checklist
Children with vision problems frequently show recognisable signs. Could you please tick against any of the problems below which apply to this
child.
Pupil's name: ________________
1. Eyes look red or sore _____
2. Screws up eyes when looking in the distance? _____
3. Book held close when reading or sits awkwardly? _____
4. Has to sit very close to the television? _____
5. Complains of: Headaches _____
Dif®culty seeing clearly in the distance _____
Blurring of vision while reading or writing _____
Seeing double _____
Eye burning or itching during or after close work _____
6. Is the child taking regular medication? If so, what?
______________________________________________
7. Is your child already receiving vision care?
If so, from whom?____________________________________________

ã 2002 The College of Optometrists

You might also like