You are on page 1of 7

Behav Genet (2011) 41:803–809

DOI 10.1007/s10519-011-9459-0

ORIGINAL RESEARCH

Canid Mating Systems, Social Behavior, Parental Care


and Ontogeny: Are they Flexible?
Devra G. Kleiman

Received: 10 February 2011 / Accepted: 18 February 2011 / Published online: 13 April 2011
Ó Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011

Abstract Benson Ginsburg’s early studies of canid Introduction


socialization and wolf social and reproductive behavior were
focused, in part, on the degree to which there was flexibility My interest in comparative behavior and the behavioral
in social development and specifically whether there was a flexibility of canids originated from two studies in which I
critical period during development after which wolf pups participated as an undergraduate at the University of Chi-
could not be socialized to humans. My focus was the degree cago during the early 1960s. In both cases, my involvement
to which differences in canid ecology and social structure was due to Benson Ginsburg. First, Ginsburg was devel-
were correlated with differences in the plasticity of social oping techniques to socialize wild wolves (Canis lupus)
and reproductive behavior, including development. Canid and coyotes (Canis latrans) of different ages in order to
species are unusual among the Mammalia in being primarily determine whether these social mammals pass through
monogamous. Males may play an indirect or direct role in critical periods during behavioral development that
parental care, depending on a species degree of sociality. restricts their ability to develop social bonds with humans
Canid species also differ in developmental parameters, and as adults (Ginsburg et al. 1962; Woolpy and Ginsburg
reproductive suppression is common in the group-living 1967). As part of these studies, we used tranquilizers and
pack hunters. I review comparative studies of the social and habituation to humans to bring about the development of
reproductive behavior of three South American canids which human-wolf bonds.
vary in their degree of sociality and explore the degree to Second, I participated in annual observations of the
which the species are ecologically and socially flexible. breeding activity of a wolf pack housed at Brookfield Zoo
in Chicago, overseen by George Rabb. The individual
Keywords Monogamy  Behavioral plasticity  Canids  behavioral differences and differing social relationships
Sociality  Reproductive behavior within and between the sexes were especially interesting to
me (Rabb et al. 1967) and provided a window on the nat-
ural behavior of wolves that could be contrasted with the
behavior of the laboratory wolves.
From these experiences, I gained a unique knowledge of
Edited by Stephen Maxson. Correspondence to: Department of canid social and reproductive behavior and social organi-
Psychology, University of Connecticut, Stoors, CT 06269-1020, zation. Moreover, I had in-depth exposure to the nature-
USA. email: Stephen.Maxson@UConn.Edu. nurture issue during the 1960s as seen in the differing
The paper originated in a festschrift, Nurturing the Genome, to honor
approaches to animal behavior from comparative and
Benson E. Ginsburg on June 2, 2002 and is part of a special issue of physiological psychology and ethology. These undergrad-
Behavior Genetics based on that festschrift. Devra G. Klieman passed uate experiences informed nearly all of my later research as
away in 2010, and her paper is published posthumously. well as being so compelling that they derailed me from
pursuing a career in medicine. I had been planning to
D. G. Kleiman
Division of Conservation and Science, Smithsonian National become a psychiatrist. Through my career, I have main-
Zoological Park, Washington, DC 20008, USA tained a fascination with individual behavioral plasticity

123
804 Behav Genet (2011) 41:803–809

and how the internal and external environment and phy- National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Post-doctoral
logeny affect social relationships and social behavior. Fellowship to investigate the hormonal influences under-
Benson Ginsburg then provided me with an extraordi- lying sex differences in the development of social behavior.
nary opportunity. I was looking for more background and For the first time in my career, I worked with a laboratory
experience in classical ethology and to pursue studies of animal, the rat. But my interests in comparative behavior
canids. At an Ethological Conference at The Hague in and exotic animals drew me back to a zoo setting. In 1972,
1963, Ginsburg met Desmond Morris, then Curator of I became the first woman scientist hired at the Smithso-
Mammals at the Zoological Society of London (ZSL). nian’s National Zoological Park.
Morris, an ethologist and former student of Oxford Uni- Zoo research is restricted since the animal you are
versity’s Niko Tinbergen, mentioned that the ZSL had been studying is typically isolated from those critical environ-
collecting specimens of numerous canid species and that he mental characteristics in which it evolved. Thus, some
was looking for someone to study them. Through his behavior patterns like predation may never be observed.
National Institutes of Health grant, Ginsburg was able to Additionally, stereotypies and abnormal behaviors may be
support me for a year’s research in London, the purpose of common in individuals who have had limited exposure to
which was to do comparative observations of canid social natural environments, both social and asocial (Carlstead
behavior and to pursue studies of socialization, using spe- 1996).
cies other than wolves and coyotes. But zoo research also has extraordinary benefits. Ani-
I went to London for a year and remained for 5 years, mals can be observed closely, and over long time periods,
during which I conducted studies on the social and repro- even those that are not normally visible in nature. The
ductive behavior of numerous mammals and completed a social environment and enclosure characteristics can be
PhD in zoology through University College, University of manipulated and responses quantified. Many species can be
London. After a year doing canid research, I was hired by observed at once, thus providing a comparative perspective
the ZSL Wellcome Institute of Comparative Physiology as (Kleiman 1992).
a research assistant to do comparative behavior studies for Over the course of my career, I have seen extraordinary
a World Health Organization (WHO)-funded program progress in zoo husbandry aided both by the explosion in
investigating physiological delaying mechanisms in mam- knowledge of the behavioral ecology of wild mammals and
malian reproduction, including species with delayed fer- of research on captive animals. Enclosures for captive
tilization (insectivorous bats), delayed implantation mammals have changed from the bare concrete-floored
(mustelids) and prolonged pregnancies (South American cages of the 1950s and 1960s to enclosures which simulate
caviomorph rodents). One result was the first successful a natural habitat. The corresponding improvement in the
captive reproduction of hibernating insectivorous bats social and reproductive management of exotic mammals
(Racey and Kleiman 1970). has been informed by their species-specific needs and
My thesis was an in depth study of reproductive and behavior.
social behavior of a single rodent species, the green
acouchi, Myoprocta pratti (Kleiman 1969), and while
descriptive, the focus was on proximate causes of behavior. Monogamy
My early studies of wolf social behavior and exposure to
physiological psychology had driven my interest in moti- In the mid-1970s, I realized that several of the mammal
vation and hormone-behavior interactions. The ethological species with which I had worked exhibited monogamy as a
influence in London added an evolutionary focus. Thus, I mating strategy (i.e. a single male and single female mated
included a comparative component in all of my work and exclusively with each other over multiple reproductive
studied several other South American caviomorph rodents. cycles) and that they had several life history characteristics
The variety of individuals and species of mammals that I in common, which differed from the typical polygynous
observed at ZSL had another result. I gained confidence in mammal (Kleiman 1977, 1981). For example, they showed
my ability to distinguish between species characteristics less sexual dimorphism than typically polygynous mam-
and an individual idiosyncrasy, e.g. I was the first to mals and fathers tended to exhibit paternal care. There also
describe the bizarre handstand urination and scent-marking seemed to be a continuum in the expression of monogamy,
posture of the female bush dog (Speothos venaticus), depending on how social a species was (see Fig. 1). The
despite only having observed a single female in London canids were a perfect subject of study since the taxon was
(Kleiman 1967, 1972). almost exclusively monogamous, yet species varied in the
Pursuing my interests in hormone-behavior interactions level of sociality they exhibited. Wolves, African wild dogs
and proximate mechanisms, I returned to the Institute of (Lycaon pictus), dholes (Cuon alpinus) and bush dogs
Animal Behavior, Rutgers University, in 1969 on an existed in large packs, hunted cooperatively, and had

123
Behav Genet (2011) 41:803–809 805

Solitary Pair Family Pack medium-sized canids, wide-ranging throughout South


America, and feeding on small vertebrates, invertebrates,
insects and fruits (Brady 1981). Bush dogs are short stocky
& Sub-adults, canids. They are little-known, but may be seen in groups
Juveniles Juveniles,
Infants and are known to prey as a pack on vertebrates larger than
themselves, e.g. pacas (Agouti paca).
For all five of the species, there were field as well as
captive observations supporting the hypothesis that
ASOCIAL SOCIAL monogamy was the mating strategy, regardless of the
degree of sociality.
The focus of the captive canid studies was a comparison
Fig. 1 Group size and social organization of mammalian species
of:
exhibiting monogamy, depending upon the degree of sociality
(adapted from Kleiman, 1977) 1. The degree of pair bonding behavior exhibited through
an examination of the quality and quantity of affilia-
tive, aggressive and sexual behaviors.
complex social relationships. Less social species were
2. Parental care patterns, including the quantity and
typically pair-bonded and lived as a pair outside of
quality of indirect and direct care behaviors, and the
breeding season; during rearing of the litter, they could be
quality and quantity of the contribution by adult
seen as a nuclear family (Kleiman and Eisenberg 1973).
breeding males, adult breeding females and non-
I developed a series of predictions concerning the life
breeding adults and sub-adults.
history characteristics of monogamous mammals in general
3. Ontogenetic patterns, including play and the develop-
and the variation likely to be observed, depending upon the
ment of socio-sexual and feeding behavior.
degree of sociality. With support from the Smithsonian
4. Sex differences in behavior and reproduction, for
Institution and the NIMH, I then focused on testing these
example, the degree and type of reproductive suppres-
predictions with students and colleagues, using the solitary
sion exhibited by maturing sub-adults, the degree of
rufous elephant shrew (Elephantulus rufescens), the group-
inter-sexual versus intra-sexual aggression and domi-
living golden lion tamarin (Leontopithecus rosalia), and
nance, and the degree of sexual dimorphism in
three South American canid species that ranged from being
territorial behaviors such as scent-marking.
solitary to highly social.
5. Species differences in the lability of these behaviors, if
The rufous elephant shrew, a small African insectivore,
that information could be obtained.
is typically seen as a solitary individual in the wild.
Although a reproductive pair occupies a single territory, the Over the period 1975 through the mid-1980s, the canids
pair members rarely interact with one another. They have were bred and observed at the Smithsonian National Zoo-
small litters (usually 2) of highly precocial young, which logical Park’s Conservation and Research Center.
are ‘‘parked’’ in a safe site, with the mother and father Researchers, focusing on different questions, included
visiting separately and at infrequent intervals during the Chuck Brady, Ingrid Porton, Melissa Ditton Rodden,
day (Koontz 1984; Koontz and Roeper 1983; Rathbun Maxine Biben, James Dietz and the author (Biben 1982a, b,
1979). c, 1983; Brady 1978, 1979, 1981; Brady and Ditton 1979;
The golden lion tamarin, a territorial frugivorous- Dietz 1984; Kleiman 1980, 1981, 1984; Kleiman and
insectivorous primate from Brazil’s Atlantic Forest, is Brady 1978; Kleiman and Malcolm 1981; Porton 1983;
typically observed in groups of 4–6 individuals. The family Porton et al. 1987; Rodden et al. 1996; Biben et al.
group includes the mated pair, twin or singleton infants, unpublished observations). During the remainder of this
and juveniles and sub-adults of varying ages; non-repro- paper, I will not refer to individual publications, except
ductive adults are occasionally found within groups (Baker when I feel that the results need to be specifically cited.
et al. 2002). Golden lion tamarins have a mating system
and social structure much like wolves.
The three canid species, from the least to the most Pair-bonding behaviors
social, were the maned wolf, crab-eating fox, and bush dog.
Maned wolves (Chrysocyon brachyurus) are tall thin fox- The three South American canid species differed markedly
like canids primarily from the grasslands of Brazil and in many measures of social behavior that we predicted
Argentina. They feed on small vertebrates and fruits. Adult would vary with the degree of sociality. For example, when
males and females are typically seen as solitary individuals pairs are first introduced, male maned wolves may be very
(Dietz 1984). Crab-eating foxes (Cerdocyon thous) are aggressive towards females. Once paired, maned wolves

123
806 Behav Genet (2011) 41:803–809

exhibit more agonistic and defensive rather than affiliative ‘‘helpers’’ can and do perform all of these behaviors
behaviors with each other, and they rarely interact or have although the existence of helpers and paternal care is rel-
physical contact, e.g. they typically do not sleep together. atively rare among the mammals as a whole (Kleiman and
Indeed, they often carry out their asynchronous activities at Malcolm 1981). There are, of course, species-specific dif-
a considerable distance. ferences in which of these activities are expressed. For
Crab-eating fox pairs, on the other hand, remain close example, the elephant-shrew is a cursorial mammal that
together much of the time and are more synchronous in does not use shelters, dens or burrows, thus shelter con-
their activities although they hunt separately. They exhibit struction and maintenance is not in its repertoire of
sequence urine-marking regularly (Brady 1978) and exhibit INDIRECT CARE behaviors. INDIRECT CARE behav-
both defensive and affiliative behaviors in their social iors do not require physical contact with young, and since
interactions. Although aggressive at first introduction many are necessary for individual survival and fitness, it
(Biben 1982c), bush dog pairs eventually remain close to may be difficult to measure what portion might be con-
each other, often sleeping in physical contact, and fre- sidered relevant to individual fitness versus reproductive
quently interact with affiliative/submissive behaviors. Bush success through survival of offspring and kin.
dog pairs also maintain contact using a high-pitched con- Patterns of DIRECT CARE include sleeping and resting
tact call (Brady 1981), go into simultaneous submissive with young (which may contribute to thermoregulation of
behaviors (rolling on their sides) and may urine mark over altricial infants), grooming and cleaning young, retrieving
each other, the female using the unique handstand posture young, carrying or transporting young (more typical of
that I first observed in the London Zoo (Kleiman 1972; primates), babysitting for the young while the mother is
Porton 1983). They also exhibit almost completely syn- absent, feeding the young with regurgitated meat or small
chronous activity, including sequence urine-marking prey carried directly back to the den, active defense of the
(Porton 1983). young against other larger carnivores and aggressive con-
While both sexes may exhibit territorial behaviors such specifics, and playing and socializing with young (see
as scent marking and long distance calling, social com- Kleiman and Malcolm 1981).
munication behaviors in maned wolves are geared towards Among the three South American canid species, the
a more solitary existence when compared with crab-eating timing, quality and quantity of male care varied depending
foxes and bush dogs. For example, maned wolves have a upon the degree of sociality (see Table 1 for canid parental
deep-throated bark designed to carry over long distances care behaviors). Maned wolf males generally exhibit more
while bush dogs and crab-eating foxes often exhibit syn- indirect than direct parental care behaviors, especially
chronous urine marking and bush dogs vocalize during the early part of the rearing period. Female maned
simultaneously. wolves may become aggressive towards the male after
giving birth and may prevent the male from entering the
den. Indeed, some pairs of captive maned wolves need to
Parental care behaviors be separated around the time of parturition. Yet, males
separated from the mate but still in close proximity may
Kleiman and Malcolm (1981) divided mammalian parental respond to the female’s birth by exhibiting an increase in
care behaviors into two types, INDIRECT CARE which territorial and ‘‘protective’’ behaviors, e.g. they may
does not require physical contact between the adult and become quite aggressive to their human caretakers
young, and DIRECT CARE, which does require physical (Rodden, personal communication). However, once the
contact. INDIRECT CARE behaviors include all the
activities involved in territorial acquisition, maintenance Table 1 Patterns of indirect and direct parental care in canids
and defense, which provide offspring with a protected (excluding nursing)
territory in which to grow. Specific behavior patterns which INDIRECT CARE DIRECT CARE
may be expressed include inter-individual and inter-group
aggressive behavior towards conspecifics that intrude into Resource acquisition Huddle with young
the territorial integrity of a pair or group. Long distance Resource maintenance Groom and clean young
vocalizations (e.g. wolf howls) and scent marking with Resource defense Retrieve young to den
urine or glandular secretions on the borders of territories Den construction Babysit
may repel potential intruders. Sentinel and anti-predator behavior Play with young
Other INDIRECT CARE behaviors are shelter or den Care of (feed) pregnant and/or Active defense of young
lactating female
construction, sentinel and anti-predator behaviors, and
caring for the pregnant mate through guarding and feeding Carry or regurgitate food to
young
her. Except for the latter, both sexes and non-reproductive

123
Behav Genet (2011) 41:803–809 807

litter emerges from the den, male maned wolves may carry Although bush dogs play more, both maned wolves and
food, regurgitate to the young and will interact and play crab-eating foxes have a more complex play behavioral
with young. repertoire than bush dogs. Bush dog object play has few
Male crab-eating foxes become involved with the litter a elements and is characterized by group activity, e.g. 3 or
little before den emergence since the female pair mate is more pups might jointly carry and bite a wooden branch.
usually less aggressive at and shortly after parturition. Males Social play is often initiated by submissive behaviors.
may enter the den in the female’s absence although they By contrast, crab-eating foxes and maned wolf object
usually rest and sleep at the entrance. They may also carry and social play contains more behavior elements, is less
food to the female and litter, which is left outside the den rigid and deterministic in structure and social play is more
entrance. Once the cubs emerge, the male participates dominated by ‘‘Play fighting’’, with many aggressive-
equally, retrieving and grooming young. In the wild, pairs defensive behaviors. Object play is much more competitive
and their young travel together as a nuclear family unit for than in bush dogs.
about 5 months, at which time young disperse (Brady 1979). The simplicity of bush dog play is surprising in view of
The bush dog father’s parental care behavior is possibly the prevailing belief that species which are more social will
the most complex of any canid. Males are with the female have more complex social relationships as adults and more
during parturition and help remove the young from the complex play repertoires as juveniles. In fact, the differ-
birth canal as well as licking and grooming them. Males ences in the complexity of object and social play in the
may also remove and eat the placenta. The bush dog female three species may derive from foraging and feeding dif-
produces a very unusual high-pitched scream during the ferences rather differences in the degree of sociality (Biben
birth process, which seems to attract the male (Porton, 1983). Both crab-eating foxes and maned wolves feed on a
personal communication). After the birth, the male remains variety of small to medium-sized vertebrates and inverte-
in close contact with the female and litter and basically brates as well as fruits. They thus have relatively complex
does everything the female does, except for lactating. foraging and feeding behaviors that may change seasonally
Female bush dogs may be unable to rear young in the and in different habitats (see Brady 1978). Play may
absence of the male (Jantschke 1973). function more to develop the flexibility that is needed to
For all three canid species, the father interacts more with exploit multiple changing food sources.
the young than the mother, once the litter emerges from the Bush dogs, by contrast, appear to be almost exclusively
den. Young also show more submissive behavior to the pack hunters who bring their large prey (pacas and capyb-
father and fathers seem to maintain subordination behavior aras, Hydrochaeris hydrochaeris)) down as a group simply
within the litter (Biben 1983). This is especially true for by biting it until it drops. The hunting tactics are therefore
bush dogs. simple. It is important, however, that bush dogs be non-
competitive while hunting, and their object and social play
does reinforce submissive and cooperative behaviors.
Social development

Maned wolf litters (*3 young) tend to be smaller than Reproductive development and suppression
crab-eating fox or bush dog litters (*5 young) (Moehlman
1997). Very early physical development tends to be similar Maned wolves breed once yearly and young disperse prior
in all three species as well as the time course of the to the next breeding season. Crab-eating foxes may breed
development of social interactions, except that crab-eating twice yearly and thus litter dispersal likely occurs within
foxes develop more quickly (see Biben 1983). Maned wolf the first 6 months. For both species, sexual maturity and
pups spend the least amount of time in social interactions breeding occurs at the earliest opportunity. Bush dog
and bush dogs the most, with peaks occurring between females may also exhibit multiple reproductive cycles
weeks 12 and 18 in all three species. This corresponds to during the year, especially if a litter is lost, but until young
the degree of sociality as adults in the three species. females are removed from their natal group, they will not
In none of the species are clear dominance relations exhibit an estrous cycle. Indeed, unlike most canids, young
developed during social interactions and play that persist bush dog females can be brought into heat within weeks by
beyond the moment (Biben 1983). This contrasts with exposing them to an unfamiliar male (Porton et al. 1987).
observations that suggest that early fights and dominance Thus, bush dogs exhibit the same suite of characteristics
interactions have a profound effect on later social relations seen in other canid pack hunters with obligate monogamy:
(Bekoff 1978). hunting large prey, living in complex social groups, large
One of the more unusual findings was the difference in litters, extensive direct paternal care and adult reproduc-
the structure of play in the three species (Biben 1983). tively-suppressed helpers (Moehlman 1997).

123
808 Behav Genet (2011) 41:803–809

Discussion and conclusions constantly in forced close contact with a conspecific (even
a pair-mate) or humans. Indeed, within the zoo community,
The three South American canid species are all monoga- they are most often described as shy and sensitive to human
mous in mating strategy, but differ in their degree of disturbance. Maned wolves may show the most individual
sociality. This diversity more than likely derives from variability in social and parental behaviors, as described in
ecological differences, specifically species’ variation in the zoo literature, but it is difficult to separate this vari-
feeding habits, with maned wolves and crab-eating foxes ability from known variability in zoo husbandry and
feeding on a variety of fruits, small vertebrates, and management. Ultimately, it is not clear to what degree
invertebrates (including insects and crabs) that require intra-specific social bonding tendencies can be extrapolated
variation and flexibility in foraging strategies. Bush dogs, to the human-canid social bond.
however, are more strictly carnivorous, being pack hunters The basic common characteristic for the three South
like wolves and African wild dogs and bringing down prey American species is bonding with a single member of the
larger than themselves. Their group hunting strategy, while opposite sex, but the greater sociality of some species in this
obligate, may be much simpler and more stereotyped than group, e.g. bush dogs, may not necessarily derive from a need
the foraging behavior of the other two species. for greater complexity of social relations. Bush dogs appear
Social differences are reflected in many aspects of the able to hunt cooperatively simply by being non-competitive
three species’ life history characteristics from the quality and maintaining complex submissive behaviors among pack
and quantity of pair-bonding behaviors, to the timing, type members. Thus, their play and social ontogeny is consider-
and amount of paternal behavior, the degree of involve- ably simpler and more rigid than for the other species.
ment in parental care by non-reproductive adult helpers, Interestingly, in this respect they are more similar to African
social development and play, and both hormonal and wild dogs than to wolves. The latter has a more variable diet
behavioral reproductive suppression (Moehlman 1997). and foraging strategy over a wide part of its range, its group
The degree to which either social or ecological charac- size varies significantly with diet, and social structure is
teristics are flexible and whether there is a correlation generally conceded to be complex.
between the degree of sociality, complexity of social
interactions, ability to form a human-canid bond and for- Acknowledgments I would like to thank Benson Ginsburg for
giving me a start in canid research and supporting my interests so
aging strategy, is unclear. Certainly, some species like
completely. Without the opportunities he provided, my career would
coyotes are enormously flexible in their ecological have gone in a very different direction. I also wish to thank George
requirements and feeding behavior. Their prey base varies and Mary Rabb for introducing me to the complexities of life within a
tremendously; they may feed on fruits, small vertebrates, wolf pack. I would also like to thank those colleagues that contributed
so much to the canid studies, especially: Maxeen Biben, Chuck
insects, invertebrates, carrion, or hunt down small ungu- Brady, James Dietz, John Eisenberg, Ingrid Porton, and Melissa
lates. Correlated with this is considerable variation in group Rodden. These studies were supported by grants from the Smithso-
size and social structure (Kleiman and Brady 1978). nian Research Foundation and NIMH 27241-03.
Additionally coyotes are persecuted by humans. Thus
they may survive as solitary individuals, or in a pair,
nuclear family or a pack. Their adaptability to human- References
induced environmental disturbance and to persecution has
actually resulted in a huge expansion of their range and Baker AJ, Bales K, Dietz JM (2002) Mating system and group
dynamics in lion tamarins. In: Kleiman DG, Rylands AB (eds)
distribution, just in our lifetimes (Kleiman and Brady Lion tamarins: biology and conservation. Smithsonian Institution
1978). The ecological and social adaptability of coyotes, Press, Washington, DC, pp 188–212
however, has not translated into a tendency for them to Bekoff M (1978) Behavioral development in coyotes and eastern
form bonds with humans or to be tamed. Indeed, they are coyotes. In: Bekoff M (ed) Coyotes: biology, behavior and
management. Academic Press, NY, pp 97–126
present in many urban and suburban U.S. settings, but Biben M (1982a) Object play and social treatment of prey in bush
remain shy and quite distant from people. dogs and crab-eating foxes. Behaviour 79:201–211
Bush dogs require social interactions and do not thrive Biben M (1982b) Ontogeny of social behavior related to feeding in
when maintained as solitary individuals. They do easily the crab-eating fox (Cerdocyon thous) and the bush dog
(Speothos venaticus). J Zool 196:207–216
become tame and indeed were often maintained by South Biben M (1982c) Urine-marking during agonistic encounters in the
American Native Americans as pets. But their social bush dog (Speothos venaticus). Zoo Biol 1:359–362
behavior is not that complex, being focused on submissive Biben M (1983) Comparative ontogeny of social behaviour in three
behaviors. South American canids, the maned wolf, crab-eating fox and
bush dog: implications for sociality. Anim Behav 31:814–826
Both maned wolves and crab-eating foxes can be Brady CA (1978) Reproduction, growth and parental care in crag-
habituated (tamed) to humans, but certainly maned wolves eating foxes (Cerdocyon thous) at the National Zoological Park,
are often best maintained in situations where they are not Washington. Int Zoo Yearb 18:130–134

123
Behav Genet (2011) 41:803–809 809

Brady CA (1979) Observations on the behavior and ecology of the Kleiman DG (1992) Behavior research in zoos: past, present, and
crab-eating fox (Cerdocyon thous). In: Eisenberg JF (ed) future. Zoo Biol 11:301–312
Vertebrate ecology in the northern neotropics. Smithsonian Kleiman DG, Brady CA (1978) Coyote behavior in the context of
Institution Press, Washington, DC, pp 161–171 recent canid research: problems and perspectives. In: Bekoff M
Brady CA (1981) The vocal repertoires of the bush dog (Speothos (ed) Coyotes: biology, behavior and management. Academic
venaticus), crab-eating fox (Cerdocyon thous), and maned wolf Press, New York, pp 163–188
(Chrysocyon brachyurus). Anim Behav 29:649–669 Kleiman DG, Eisenberg JF (1973) Comparisons of canid and felid
Brady CA, Ditton MK (1979) Management and breeding of maned social systems from an evolutionary perspective. Anim Behav
wolves (Chrysocyon brachyurus) at the National Zoological 21:637–659
Park, Washington, D.C. Int Zoo Yearb 19:171–176 Kleiman DG, Malcolm J (1981) The evolution of male parental
Carlstead K (1996) Effects of captivity on the behavior of wild investment in mammals. In: Gubernick DJ, Klopfer PH (eds)
mammals. In: Kleiman DG, Allen ME, Thompson KV, Lumpkin Parental care in mammals. Plenum Press, NY, pp 347–387
S (eds) Wild mammals in captivity: principles and techniques. Koontz FW (1984) Sternal scent gland communication in the rufous
University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 317–333 elephant-shrew, Elephantulus rufescens Peters, with additional
Dietz JM (1984) Ecology and social organization of the maned wolf observations on behavior and reproduction in captivity. Ph.D.
(Chrysocyon brachyurus). Smithson Contrib Zool 392:1–51 Dissertation, University of Maryland, College Park, MD
Ginsburg BE, Woolpy J, Kleiman D, Edwards C (1962) Comparative Koontz FW, Roeper NJ (1983) Elephantulus rufescens. Mamm
studies of Canid behavior. III. Socialization to humans on Species 204:1–5
varying schedules of experience and of tranquilizing drugs. Am Moehlman PD (1997) Cooperative breeding, reproductive suppres-
Zool 2:l44 sion and body mass in canids. In: Solomon NG, French JA (eds)
Jantschke F (1973) On the breeding and rearing of bush dogs, Cooperative breeding in mammals. Cambridge University Press,
Speothos venaticus, at the Frankfurt Zoo. Int Zoo Yearb NY, pp 76–128
13:141–143 Porton I (1983) Bush dog urine marking: its role in pair formation and
Kleiman DG (1967) Some aspects of social behavior in the Canidae. maintenance. Anim Behav 31:1061–1069
Am Zool 7:365–372 Porton I, Kleiman DG, Rodden M (1987) Aseasonality of bush dog
Kleiman DG (1969) The reproductive behaviour of the green acouchi, reproduction and the influence of social factors on the estrous
Myoprocta pratti. Ph.D. thesis, University of London cycle. J. Mammalogy 68(4):867–871
Kleiman DG (1972) The social behavior of the maned wolf, Rabb GB, Woolpy JH, Ginsburg BE (1967) Social relationships in a
Chrysocyon brachyurus, and the bush dog, Speothos venaticus: group of captive wolves. Am Zool 7:305–311
a study in contrast. J Mammal 53:791–806 Racey PA, Kleiman DG (1970) Maintenance and breeding in
Kleiman DG (1977) Monogamy in mammals. Q Rev Biol 52:39–69 captivity of some verspertilionid bats, with special reference to
Kleiman DG (1980) The sociobiology of captive propagation in the noctule. Int Zoo Yearb 10:65–70
mammals. In: Soulé M, Wilcox B (eds) Conservation biology. Rathbun GB (1979) The social structure and ecology of elephant
Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, pp 243–261 shrews. Z Tierpsychol Suppl Adv Ethol 20:1–80
Kleiman DG (1981) Correlations among life history characteristics of Rodden MD, Sorenson LG, Sherr A, Kleiman DG (1996) Use of
mammalian species exhibiting two extreme forms of monogamy. behavioral measures to assess reproductive status in maned
In: Alexander RD, Tinkle DW (eds) Natural selection and social wolves (Chrysocyon brachyurus). Zoo Biol 15(6):565–585
behavior. Chiron Press, NY, pp 332–344 Woolpy JH, Ginsburg BE (1967) Wolf socialization: a study of
Kleiman DG (1984) Implications of monogamy for infant social temperament in a wild social species. Am Zool 7:357–363
development in mammals. In: Lewis M (ed) Social connections:
beyond the dyad. Plenum Press, New York, pp 91–108

123

You might also like