You are on page 1of 14

Journal of Planning Education and Research

http://jpe.sagepub.com/

Forgetting to Plan
Howell S. Baum
Journal of Planning Education and Research 1999 19: 2
DOI: 10.1177/0739456X9901900101

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://jpe.sagepub.com/content/19/1/2

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of:

Association of Collegiate Schools of Planning

Additional services and information for Journal of Planning Education and Research can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://jpe.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://jpe.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Citations: http://jpe.sagepub.com/content/19/1/2.refs.html

>> Version of Record - Sep 1, 1999

What is This?

Downloaded from jpe.sagepub.com at DALHOUSIE UNIV on June 26, 2014


Forgetting to Plan
Howell S. Baum

Those who cannot remember the past are condemned to repeat it.
George Santayana, The Life of Reason
ABSTRACT I can swim like the others, only I have a better memory than the others. I
Conventionally, planning for the future de- have not forgotten my former inability to swim. But since I have not forgot-
pends on collecting information and analyzing ten it, my ability to swim is of no avail, and I cannot swim after all.
it rationally in order to control contingency.
In reality, contingency persists, and communi- Franz Kafka, Parables and Paradoxes
ties react in ways that defeat planning. Seeing
The patient is cured in psychoanalysis when, among other things, he contin-
problems and uncertainty, communities often
retreat to a past they remember in idealized plan for the future knowing he is unable to do so.
ues to

ways to find gratification they do not expect Adam Phillips, On Flirtation


from the future. They choose nostalgia or
fantasy rather than looking realistically at
current conditions. Thus communities resist
s TWO VIGNETTES
planning not out of ignorance, but out of Two vignettes illustrate how communities hold on to memories of the past in
knowledge. They know the past, its satisfac- 1
tions, and its centrality to their identity, and ways that hinder planning.
they want to maintain it against change. Activists in a historically working-class white ethnic community convened a plan-
Hence planning depends on forgetting: forget-
ning council to develop a community plan. As in other urban areas, jobs had gone
ting a static image of the past in order to re- as manufacturers left or cut back; the quality of schools had declined as the city tax
member the past as a time of contingency and
to see the past linked in time to the present
base dwindled; families with the means to move had left for the suburbs; and low-
and the future. income renters were taking the place of long-time blue-collar homeowners. At the
same time, the racial make-up had changed from almost completely white to one-

fourth black. Residents were increasingly poorer, less well educated, less skilled so-
cially or economically, and more dependent on others. The working class was no
longer secure, and their ethnic identity was weakened.
Planning participants acknowledged these changes as the impetus for acting. Yet
they could not imagine another direction for the community. They did not ask
what the best realistically possible future would be. As ethnicity was losing sway,
they talked of promoting ethnic diversity. Facing changes in work and a growing
mismatch between residents and jobs, they struggled to create new blue-collar jobs.
As low-income and black renters replaced working-class white homeowners, they
called for more homeownership and said nothing about the housing needs of the
poor and renters who would inevitably live in the area. These policies represented
pragmatic choices among unappealing alternatives, and it is hard to fault commu-
nity members for not transforming the economy and housing market. Still, they
were more clearly oriented to the past, which they idealized, than toward the future,

which seemed depressingly unpromising.


Howell S. Baum is professor In the Urban Stud- Interviewing an economic development officer in this community, I asked,
ies and
&dquo;You’ve described the exodus of industries from this community over the past two
Planning Program at the Unzversity of
Maryland College Park; hb36@umall.umd edu decades. Now you’re responsible for economic development. Let’s assume you are

Downloaded from jpe.sagepub.com at DALHOUSIE UNIV on June 26, 2014


3

successful, and we get back together here in 20 years. What nate contingency. At the same time, some of our de-
their
will we see?&dquo; The officer, a third-generation community sires, deepest ones, remain hidden from us, from our
our

resident, said, &dquo;It will be just the way it was before!&dquo; The conscious selves: We do not always know what we want.
economic development officer spoke for many in expressing Moreover, we often want what we know, what we remem-
the wish that, somehow, things might once again be as they ber familiarly, rather than anything strange, much less any-
had been. thing ultimately unpredictable.
In the Jewish community across town, activists in the We know these things, and yet we do not want to know
central organization initiated strategic planning to set direc- them. The Enlightenment faith in the power of reason to
tions for the organization and, by extension, the commu- understand and govern society offers a defense against this
nity. Though the community and the organization were recognition (see Levine 1985). It assures us learning is the
economically quite secure, leaders openly worried about de- remedy. While we contemplate the contingent, we can pre-
clines in ethnic identity similar to those in the working-class tend that, if only we can know more, we can overcome its
community. As assimilation had become possible, Jews par- dominion. Yet, simultaneously, we doubt the efficacy of the
ticipated more in the larger society. Philanthropists gave to project, and we hold on to our memories. Learning is im-
secular causes as well as, or instead of, to the Jewish commu- periled, and realistic planning is improbable.
nity organization and its agencies. Middle-class people affili- Learning matters, but it is not a simple matter. What we call
ated less with Jewish religious or ethnic institutions. Many learning depends on forgetting-giving up memories of some
married non-Jews and did not raise their children as Jews. things in order to remember others. We must let go of cer-
Community leaders were concerned about the continuity of tain configurations of remembered events in order to recollect
their community. (re-collect) others. Thus we find new meanings in the past.
Yet planning participants, while lamenting these trends, Some events seem less important; their links to others dissolve;
aimed to restore an idealized past. They recalled a time perhaps they never existed. Other events come to mind or
when community was strong, religion mattered, ethnicity seem more important and combine in new ways. Crucially,

was forceful, and a measure of anti-Semitism reinforced this work is neither disinterested nor merely cognitive. It
Jews’ tendencies to identify unconditionally with one an- depends on giving up, and mourning, emotional attach-
other. They did not ask what were the attractions of the ments to memories-remembering the memories as memo-
world outside their community, why religion mattered less, ries but forgetting them as active, veridical accounts of the
or why Jews were not marrying Jews or raising their children past.
as part of the community. Instead, they simply advocated This article examines the essential role of forgetting in
community &dquo;continuity&dquo;-continuity of the distant past, as practice. The next two sections describe related obstacles to
if the recent past had not occurred-and they urged people planning: contingency and the character of human memory.
to resume patterns of worship and socialization associated The section following analyzes what communities need to
with earlier generations. They did not consider how devel- forget and remember in order to plan. The final section dis-
opments suggested new models for the community or their cusses implications for planning practice.

organization, and they did not develop realistic strategies for N CONTINGENCY
responding to the trends that concerned them.
In both instances, community planning participants re- Contingency is at once the reason for planning and its
acted to threats to their community by remembering and nemesis. People will have bad housing, they will be poor,
trying to restore a version of the past. Neither organization and noxious substances will sicken them, for example, unless
developed a realistic image of a future that would grow out we can find ways to control events. If we can anticipate the

of the past while diverging from it. As a result, their plans conditions that will put people out of work-and if we can
diluted strategizing with wishful thinking. manage those conditions-we can keep people employed.
0 PLANNING
Failing that, if we can identify responses to unemployment
that will put people into good jobs, we can minimize the
Planning is a mental activity that aims to affect the exter- costs of economic changes.
nal world. Directed toward the future, planning concerns A great many human events are sufficiently probable to
imaginary events, that, however much one might desire let us predict their occurrence. Not only will taxes be due
them, have never taken place, have uncertain precedent, and each year, but the mail will be delivered daily except on
might never occur. Given the unpredictability of human Sundays and holidays and in extreme weather; most stores
events, people find it reassuring to imagine that the future will be today where they were yesterday and will sell similar
repeats the remembered past. Thus, the mental activity es- goods; most firms will act in ways that keep them solvent
sential to planning is always in jeopardy. and, if possible, turn a profit; and most parents will do the
Uncertainty reigns in human affairs: We can calculate best they can to raise their children.
reassuringly plausible odds of events, but we cannot elimi- These statements are all arguably true; yet one senses they

Downloaded from jpe.sagepub.com at DALHOUSIE UNIV on June 26, 2014


4

are arrayed in order of diminishing predictive value. They methods. And future planners learned that research was the
move from describing virtually all actors or conditions to de- essence of planning (see Baum 1997b).

scribing most or many, and they grow increasingly abstract. The teachers, students, and planners were not wrong to
Moreover, the conditions that make it harder to predict spe- take research seriously: It is good to understand conditions
cific events also make it harder to control them. On the one better. But many assumed the only legitimate way to study
hand, tradition and economic necessity make it virtually cer- the phenomena of planning was in a manner considered
tain that legislatures will continue to require payment of taxes. scientific after an image of the natural sciences: with intel-
On the other hand, firms’ financial security depends on their lectual, emotional, and moral detachment.3 Moreover, fix-
making complicated forecasts and decisions about goals, strate- ing their attention on what science apparently allowed them
gies, resources, demands, and constraints. to see, they focused on what was easy to measure and quan-
In fact, most of the areas of life that matter are like the tify, what they could observe from a distance, perhaps
latter. Human activity is subject to various influences, im- through secondary data, without personal involvement, and
perfectly understood, and difficult to control. Crucially, they avoided messy problems. It would be easy, for example,
though we hesitate to acknowledge it, some of the influences to measure how many dwelling units within certain geo-
that matter most are accidental, beyond our predicting. De- graphic boundaries conformed to the housing code, but it
cision makers can speculate about what is possible and can would be daunting to understand the interplay of assump-
nurture new possibilities, but it is futile to calculate prob- tions, intentions, and actions that produced current condi-
abilities (Shackle 1958, 1969). Those who recognize uncer- tions or those that might later alter them.
tainty may reduce or reallocate its costs but cannot avoid its There were good reasons for some interests in so-called
certainty (Mack 1971; Marris 1996). objectivity. The more a researcher is involved with the ob-
Contingency presents us with a paradox. We cannot es- jects of study, the harder it is to ascertain whether the re-
cape it, but we must significantly reduce our experience of it searcher is seeing them more or less as they are or is simply
to give ourselves the confidence that we can act intention- observing his or her own shadow and influence. Yet the am-
ally. Yet we must not so reduce apparent contingency as to bition to exclude all personal influence was so thorough and
persuade ourselves that little is left within our grasp-that unrealistic as to suggest it was a cover for certain fantasies
things are rigged. At its realistic best, planning is an inven- (see Seeley 1967).
tive art, concerned with diminishing the domain or at least One fantasy involved the external world: The scientific
the consequences of uncertainty while holding open possi- method could comprehend complexity and control uncer-
bilities for acting. Planning is jeopardized by efforts to see tainty. The hope was naively embodied in large-scale model-
everything as certain, even-perhaps particularly-certainly ing, built on faith in the capacity of computers to manage
good.22 tremendous amounts of information. But the Achilles heel
Sometimes planning is a defense against the discovery of was irreducible
ignorance about how to understand the per-
contingency-irreducible uncertainty, accident-and the verse complexity of the world (Lee 1973). The usual result

sense of impotence it can engender. In this context, the alli- was an oversimplification of past conditions and projection

ance between planning and the social sciences is not acciden- of them onto the future. Black (1990) describes the result in
tal. The conventional interpretation of this link focuses on the the Chicago Area Transportation Study:
establishment of academic planning programs (Hoch 1994;
The staff often talked about the future, but it was
Krueckeberg 1984; Sch6n 1983). As planners sought profes- a future that extrapolated the past and main-
sional status, they promoted university training as a substitute
tained the status quo they can be reasonably
....

for apprenticeship. Academic culture, in turn, shaped what


criticized [for getting things wrong] on the issue
future planners were taught and who taught them. Research
of highway versus transit .... The emphasis on
universities held the most prestige among institutions of higher
education, and doctorate faculty were the norm. Doctoral rationality, on trying to be scientific, disposed
the CATS staff to this limited vision .... The fu-
planning programs were established to prepare faculty to teach ture came to be seen as inevitable. The staff did
planning students. These programs mimicked the dominant not try to change the future; they did not try to
model of the social sciences, which aspired to the precision and
status of natural science research. change policies, because these involved values,
which were beyond the pale (36).
Their graduates, few of whom had practiced planning,
became practicing social scientists. Though many were Planning theorists who have recognized such difficulties
eclectic and some invoked a lineage in the humanities, most have responded in two ways. Some have simply insisted on
were evaluated and rewarded for their work as social scien- trying harder, proposing a &dquo;contingency theory for plan-
tists-perhaps applied, if not basic, researchers, but re- ning&dquo; that would give closer attention to contexts and their
searchers. They modeled and taught social science. When constraints (for example, Alexander 1996). Yet the highly
they spoke of planning &dquo;methods,&dquo; they meant research abstract language of such efforts only echoes earlier scien-

Downloaded from jpe.sagepub.com at DALHOUSIE UNIV on June 26, 2014


5

tific ambitions without making an advance. that we could stop ourselves from dying. We can attribute
A second approach, which seems to depart from science death to external sources and bad luck, but we sense we have
and embraces subjectivity, is known as communicative plan- impulses that move us toward it. If only we could control
ning (for example, Forester 1989; Healey 1997; Innes 1997; these wishes, we imagine, we could keep ourselves alive.55
Mandelbaum, Mazza, and Burchell 1996; Sager 1994). In short, contingency frustrates human ambitions. We
Those who take this stance acknowledge limits in what can reduce contingency and its costs by
understanding the
people may know about the world. They put their hope for world better. Yet we want to know and control more than
control in what people may tell one another and agree on. we ever can. In particular, we expect scientific research and

Emphasizing agency, they focus on possibilities for collective planning to contain what will elude human dominion: ag-
action and play down or overlook structural constraints.4 gression, desire, envy, fantasies, and the conflicts to which
They argue that, if only people speak honestly and carefully, they give rise. The trouble with simply redoubling normal
they can increase their shared knowledge and create under- scientific and planning efforts is that they will fail to address
standings, policies, and institutions that expand their con- some of our most urgent concerns. As Enlightenment

trol over the world. Though none of them would unabash- projects, science and planning encourage us to define issues
edly invoke the language of scientific research or modeling, rationally. For example, we may deal with anxiety about
many share the wish that motivated those pursuits: the de- dying by talking about preventing and treating disease and
sire to reduce uncertainty. In place of the scientific method, spending billions of dollars on biomedical research. Yet no
they substitute undistorted communication. Instead of try- matter how impressively we succeed in curtailing illness and

ing to wield a lever over an external world, they bring the lengthening life, we will never eliminate death; our underly-
world inside a community of communicators and seek ways ing worries will continue to trouble us because we do not
of taming it by taming themselves. address them. Unless we can accept the limitations in what
This latter hope is one of several expressions of a second we can control, we cannot realistically understand our con-

fantasy motivating interests in &dquo;objectivity.&dquo; Those who put ditions or plan to improve them.
their faith in science imagine that, if only research were fully Thus planning depends on making sense of the world in
governed by impersonal principles and methods, human ways that simultaneously appreciate contingency and imag-
imperfections would be removed, and research could pro- ine possibilities of acting. However, as the next section
duce perfect knowledge-infallible and complete. Indi- shows, anxiety about contingency influences how we re-
vidual cognitive limitations and biases could be overcome. member and learn and hinders realistic planning.
Some who write about communication do not believe these
limitations and biases can be eliminated, but hope that N MEMORY

good-faith reasoned discussion could reduce their extent and The familiar quotation from Santayana expresses the
lead to agreement based on explicit recognition of them. comforting convention that remembering is the key to
Still, these reasonable, if not necessarily realizable, hopes learning. If we keep in mind what went before, we can draw
give voice to wishes about other, more unsettling human analogies from the past to the present and make probabilis-
imperfections. The intensity of individual and collective tic inferences about the consequences of choices. From
concerns about contingency betrays general anxiety about negative and positive examples, we can conquer contingency
the future. People want good housing, a dependable in- and invent a secure future. Yet what we remember is often
come, and security for their children. And yet people worry an account of failures of memory or apparent failures to

about having so much more; we do not draw sure lines be- learn from memory. Not only should we have known better,
tween necessities and luxuries. And those who have much but, so it seems, we could have known better.
can worry as much as those who have little. Having things, Personal memory is like written history: incomplete and
having control over things, offers hope that one might have selective. Carr (1961) distinguishes the facts of history from
control over certain intangibles. The effort to know every- facts about the past. The universe of facts about the past
thing represents a wish for immunity from change and loss. exceeds any capacity to collect or organize. Historians must
Beneath that, it expresses a desire for immortality. assemble these facts selectively. But they do not do so ran-
There are external challenges to control, success, well- domly : A fact about the past becomes an historical fact if it
being, and health-for example, in the economy and in na- fits into an interpretation that historians consider valid and
ture. These are adversaries that knowledge and action may significant. Orwell (1961 [ 1949] ) portrayed the worst possi-
thwart. Yet there are also internal sources of change-in our bilities, where the Party dispatched deviant facts to the
bodies, which get sick and age. Many, particularly children oblivion of the insidiously named &dquo;memory hole.&dquo; Yet pro-
of the Enlightenment, may imagine the body, too, as some- fessional historians may do similarly, ignoring or rejecting
thing external, that may be controlled. And yet we know not just facts that conflict with an interpretation but also

that our body is ourselves, its vicissitudes somehow our do- those that are embarrassing or painful.
ing. The desire to know and control all expresses the fantasy We do the same in constructing personal memories. It is

Downloaded from jpe.sagepub.com at DALHOUSIE UNIV on June 26, 2014


6

uncertain whether we have the neurological capacity to re- some of the available data in terms of current perceptions
member all we have experienced, but we know we have forgot- (Modell 1990). Memory is transformational, rather than repli-
ten a great deal. What, for example, did we do during the cative. Though we may seem to know more of our past, we are
365 days of our third year of life? We forget for many rea- re-collecting, re-calling, and re-membering items, not recol-
sons. To begin with, our first experiences precede words. lecting, recalling, or remembering everything.
Infants endure an eternity before they learn a language oth- These peculiarities of memory are found in groups, orga-
ers can understand. But then the words are nizations, and communities. Groups may choose a view of
inadequate to
record the experiences that went before them, in a time their history that relieves them of responsibility for con-
when experience ineffably combined touch, taste, smell, fronting difficult tasks (Bion 1961). In &dquo;groupthink,&dquo;
hearing, and sight. Later in life we rediscover the inadequacy members agree on any account of the past that helps them
of language when we have difficulty portraying the beauty of get along, even when contradictory information is available
a scene, conveying our feelings, or remembering our dreams. (Janis 1982). Organizational cultures often include elabo-
Dreams epitomize problems with language. Dreams are rate accounts of the past; sometimes subordinates contest

metaphorical, combining images, sounds, and sensations. an official history with one of their own (Deal and
Words in dreams are notoriously unreliable, puns or ono- Kennedy 1982; Frost et al. 1985; Schein 1992). The Chal-
matopoeia, never what they seem. This is the wordless lan- lenger disaster dramatically shows what happens when
guage we originally engaged in as infants and, crucially, the members of an organization prefer an idealized memory of
way we understand the world in the moments before we perfection to an accurate one of increasing mistakes
find words with which to think, talk, and act. Unconscious (Schwartz 1990). Societies create monuments to benign
thinking deeply shapes what we see as conscious calculation. images of the past in efforts to put disruptive versions to
Thus the store of facts about the past accessible to us is rest (Sturken 1997). In many ways, groups choose to re-

inevitably reduced in several ways beyond any neurological member their past so as to make sense of current conditions
limitations. Reliance on language forces us to forget experi- or resolve contemporary conflicts (Halbwachs 1992).

ences that resist words. On top of this, just as professional These observations do not assume that collectivities
historians, we choose to forget experiences that are inconsis- think with a single mind. Not everyone in an organization
tent with how we want to think of ourselves-in particular, or community, for example, thinks the same, nor is every

experiences that would embarrass us, cause us to feel guilty, member wholly in the group, without autonomy or outside
lead us to fear punishment, or otherwise arouse anxiety. In loyalties. Moreover, formally and informally, collectivities
various ways we repress what we once knew about ourselves, divide up labor. Differences and conflicts of both percep-
pushing certain facts from awareness. With respect to our tion and interest abound. Still, there are moments when
conscious thinking, we have forgotten things. However, as many, if not all, members identify with one another and act
with experiences that exceed the potentials of language, we on common assumptions, when even those who disagree

remember them unconsciously, in ways we do not recog- find it politically, culturally, or psychologically difficult to
nize, and we interpret, evaluate, and act on the world in think or act differently.
their terms. Sometimes people deliberately deceive others about the
We forget in other, more subtle ways. We may keep an inci- past, but these examples suggest that most inaccuracies fol-
dent in mind but re-write its script, altering the plot and the low good-faith efforts to remember things in ways that fit
characters’ moral valences. We may substitute an innocuous or contemporary desires and anxieties. Still, the result is the
benign image for what was upending.~ We may condense sev- same: Memory is an imperfect instrument for managing

eral episodes into one, perhaps covering over a dangerous situ- contingency. The obvious flaw is that events and actors
ation with one in which we were safe or even heroic, remem- change. Yet a more subtle disability derives from a way that
bering our past in a way that reassures us about our potential people use memory to cope with contingency. They choose
to do and be good. Thus, in many ways we diminish our ac- to remember things in ways that could not have been oth-

cessible memory by what we choose to forget. erwise. Conscious memory contains a world without acci-
Spence (1982), writing of psychoanalytic patients’ efforts dents : satisfaction or frustration, success or failure, events
to recall and learn from their experiences, echoes Carr in have sure causes of someone’s making. Seeing the past as
distinguishing historical truth from narrative truth. We may determinate serves the purpose of reassuring that the future,
aim for the former, an inclusive veridical account, but we too, can be under control.
construct only the latter, a story that makes sense of avail-
able facts and satisfies the pragmatic test of contributing to s TRAUMA AND NOSTALGIA
effective action. With effort, we may remember more of our Anxiety about contingency encourages people to distort
past. But remembering is not a matter of journeying men- what they remember and stop time. When the future seems
tally to a vault where all the data of our lives are deposited, overwhelming, when security seems contingent on more
ready to be checked out. Rather, it is a way of recategorizing than can be controlled, people may hold fast to the past,

Downloaded from jpe.sagepub.com at DALHOUSIE UNIV on June 26, 2014


7

stuck onmemories that resist change. Thus, living in the The tone of these memories is similar to that of some
past, they avoid experiencing a present and cannot learn trauma victims, but people who experience less severe upset
from current events. have greater freedom in ordering their memories. In contrast
Trauma offers an example. Psychologically, trauma is an with trauma victims, who are completely absorbed by the
experience that arouses anxiety and overwhelms the ability past, people who are nostalgic treat their memories in a
to think realistically and act planfully. The experience may compartmentalized manner. In many ways they act realisti-
arise from an external event or an inner impulse (such as a cally in the contemporary world, while in one area holding
wish to harm someone or a desire to be with someone) that onto a glorious past. Though cognitively they know times
leads to great disappointment or danger. Trauma is not an have changed, emotionally they feel they are in some ways
inherent quality of an event but derives from how the event the same as before.
is experienced. People vary in how they understand, react Comparison with the present is the heart of nostalgia. The
to, and cope with what may look like similar conditions past seems more exalted than its contemporary shadow, but it
(Cooper 1986; Freud 1977 [1920]; Yorke 1986). Individu- is also irrecoverably gone, separated forever from the present
als may experience trauma, for example, when they face by some watershed event. Hence &dquo;the golden era&dquo; is also &dquo;the
combat, lose a job, or are forced to move. Communities world we have lost.&dquo; In nostalgic memories, the past is not
may be traumatized by urban renewal, the loss of a major continuous with the present. This isolation makes it possible
employer, or natural or environmental disasters (Erikson to idealize the past without challenge from other memories,
1994; Fried 1963; Gans 1982). and it can then compensate for present disappointments.
When people are traumatized, they try to care for them- For example, a community may face hard times when
selves in several ways. One is to forget what happened. manufacturers cut back operations, but residents can feel good
They may suppress conscious awareness of the episode and about themselves by recalling days when proud immigrants
try to act as if it never occurred. At best, the strategy can be worked tirelessly in factories to buy homes, raise families, and
partly successful. On the one hand, they forget what hap- establish themselves as Americans. People who feel their lives
pened. But, unconsciously, they continue to remember the are isolated or undistinguished can find apparent community

injury, associate it with other things, and then try to avoid or specialness in neo-urban developments, theme parks, and

thinking about all related matters or acting in any way that their variations that offer encapsulated experiences of idealized
evokes them. As a result, they narrow their thinking and past moments (Gottdiener 1997; Sorkin 1992). Professionals
action by tacitly ruling these dangerous areas off limits who spend their days in nondescript bureaucracies can enjoy
(Fenichel 1945; A. Freud 1946; S. Freud 1957 [1915]; former factory buildings that have been turned into bookstores
Klein 1976). Paradoxically, they forget but also remember or coffee houses with exquisite architectural detail-and with-

in a way they cannot recognize and from which they cannot out references to the hazardous, poorly compensated working
learn. Unconsciously, they stay in a past they remember. conditions of those who labored there (Lowenthal 1985).
Members of a community decimated by flood, for example, Disappointment with the present makes future security
may be in shock, holding only the sketchiest memories of uncertain. Trauma instantly coats everything with contin-
what happened. gency. In different ways and to different degrees, people re-
People may find a trauma so incapacitating that they try spond by trying to forget what has upset them, what is asso-
to forget it in a different way, by pretending it never hap- ciated with that situation, and perhaps what followed it. To
pened and retreating mentally to an earlier, more pleasur- repair the damage, they want to remember events in a way
able period in their lives (A. Freud 1946; S. Freud 1963 that assures them conditions will be secure and gratifying.
[1936]). For example, when a company town is abandoned Nostalgia epitomizes these defenses against contingency in
by its main employer, residents may focus on documenting the ways it avoids a present where things change, as well as
and celebrating the events of an earlier time (Hinsdale, the passage of time itself. People forget certain things in or-
Lewis, and Waller 1995). They try to push traumatic events der to remember certain others.
from consciousness, but it is their continuing unconscious Thus the experience of contingency can foster intentions
memory that forces them to retreat to an earlier time. They to distort memory, such that less realistic information is
have stakes in consciously remembering a distorted view of available for analysis and action that might reduce contin-
the past that keeps them from assimilating current events. gency and its costs. Moreover, when people imagine they
Nostalgia is a variation on this second response. What we can bring about what they idealize, make it real simply by

commonly call &dquo;nostalgia&dquo; seems to follow upon losses that thinking of it, they have no motivation to act. When com-
are less than traumatic. People find present conditions dis- munities do engage in formal planning, as in the vignettes
appointing, they see little likelihood of improvement, and above, they may try to project an imaginary past onto the
they harken back to gratifying memories of the past-the future through wishful thinking, rather than creating a
good old days, the golden age when all was wonderful workable future through strategic action (Baum 1997a,
(Gabriel 1993; Kaplan 1987; Werman 1977). 1998; Marris 1975).

Downloaded from jpe.sagepub.com at DALHOUSIE UNIV on June 26, 2014


8

there is no time, where past, present, and future are indistin-


N REMEMBERING AND FORGETTING substitute and wishful
guishable. They nostalgia thinking
The problem of individuals and communities stuck in the for analysis and strategic action.
past and unable to plan is not that they do not remember For example, public housing residents who live in deso-
enough, but that they remember too much. They remember late, crime-ridden neighborhoods may resist relocation or
certain experiences too vividly and rigidly. They identify so demolition and replacement of their projects, even when
strongly with particular good or bad_ memories that much of they can move into sounder, more attractive housing. They
their identity is attached to being part of some time in the have worked out relationships and routines with people and
past; they cannot imagine themselves otherwise. The remedy places, and they are wary of new situations over which they
is not to remember everything, or even just to remember expect little control (Vale 1997; Varady and Walker 1997).
more. It is to remember differently-and to forget.
Many residents of the working-class neighborhoods de-
The challenge is to get free of these memories by con- scribed in the first vignette prefer remembering vigorous
structing a new narrative about the past where, whatever early immigrant communities to grappling with economic
happened, it is confined to the past. The emotional valence and racial changes. Some talk of establishing a museum of
of memories changes as in the idiom that one &dquo;forgives but ethnic history. Jewish leaders in the
second vignette invoke
does not forget.&dquo; One may remember experiences but forget images of that community’s past and call for &dquo;Jewish conti-
some meanings in order to remember new details and mean- nuity&dquo; while giving superficial attention to reasons why
ings. Under these conditions, memories of the past continue many find other identities more attractive. They resist
but no longer arouse anxieties or desires that control present changing their institutions to accommodate social and cul-
actions. We speak of being &dquo;less attached&dquo; to certain events tural trends that draw members away.
or memories: We may continue to recall episodes but, leav- And yet individuals and group do change. They adapt to
ing them in the past, consider them less central to our iden- new conditions, they give up old ties, and they overcome

tity and refer less to them in defining our contemporary re- injury. They recognize costs to past ways of thinking, and
lationships. they see gains in new choices. They succeed in mourning
Yet it is not enough just to insist that a group &dquo;be realis- the loss of past attachments, reconciling them to present
tic,&dquo; for there are moments when the past seems real enough realities. Marris (1975) describes widows’ grieving as a pro-
to those who hold fast to it. People will decide to remember totypical process:
things differently for the same reasons they remembered [G]rief works itself out through a process of re-
them originally: Pragmatically, memories make sense of ev-
formulation, rather than substitution.
eryday experiences and actions. Hence, the strongest motive Confidence in the original commitment is re-
for forgetting certain memories and recollecting others is
stored by extracting its essential meaning and
that the apparent reality described by prevailing memories
conflicts with present realities. Change depends on seeing grafting it upon the present. This process in-
volves repeated reassurances of the strength and
costs in adhering to particular memories. For example, bask-
inviolability of the original commitment, as
ing in warm thoughts of a community’s golden age may much as a search for the terms on which reat-
hinder planning for improving housing stock and schools.
tachment would still make life worth living. Un-
Mourning, Playing, and Working Through til this ambivalent testing of past and future has
retrieved the thread of continuity, it is itself the
Individuals, organizations, and communities are conserva-
only deeply meaningful activity in which the be-
tive, maintaining familiar ways of thinking, social relation- reaved can be engaged (98).
ships, places, and practices that have given them meaningful
identities. When their identity is injured, when they feel they Successfulmourning depends on two related achieve-
have lost control over the relevant world, they seek anchors in ments. simultaneously accepting the passing of
One is
the past and hold fast against the depredations of change. people, things, or conditions while continuing to care for
People experience change-even when it improves their them. The other is recognizing continuity between the past
lives, even when they choose or enact it-as loss. Without and present, setting time in motion again. These accom-
consciously thinking, we react with dual impulses of seeking plishments make it possible to learn from the past to think
to return to a time when we were secure and trying to forget about the present and to contemplate and take responsibility
altogether what has gone. Each is an effort to stop time, to for the future.
separate the past from the present. Giving up the past can Psychoanalysts describe this process as &dquo;working through&dquo;
seem to mean disavowing oneself, committing existential past memories to free up the present (Brenner 1987; Freud
suicide. Thus individuals and collectivities try to preserve 1958 [1914]; Greenacre 1956; Greenson 1965). Those who
themselves by imagining the future will recapitulate the are stuck in the past-for example, holding on to the mo-

past. They retreat to fantasies in the unconscious, where ment of a trauma or injury or grasping at the last apparently

Downloaded from jpe.sagepub.com at DALHOUSIE UNIV on June 26, 2014


9

good time before that-must find ways to reconstruct the everyday role obligations, tenets of faith, and time. They
old experience. In psychoanalysis and psychotherapy, some- must be able to try out different ways of thinking about and
one assists another in working things through. However, looking at conditions without feeling that asking questions
often, as in mourning the loss of a loved one, individuals and considering unwonted answers is irresponsible, disloyal,
adjust less formally, by talking with friends and relatives and or dangerous. Then they can engage in two activities of

trying variously to get on with their lives. Even so, people changing.
take themselves through mourning in a way that mirrors the
course of psychoanalysis (Parkin 1981), as if there is a
logic Learning to Forget to Learn
to changing that the mind tacitly follows. Studies of modifi- The first activity is tentatively to adjust to the past. Under
cations of group beliefs (Lewin 1951), rites of passage (Van these conditions, people can provisionally take up new ver-
Gennep 1960 [1908]), other rituals (Turner 1969), and en- sions of events, remembering them differently, without giv-
try into organizations (Baum 1990) all find a similar three- ing up old memories. Holding various accounts at once,
phase process that individuals and collectivities naturally they can analyze why they may prefer memories that are
follow in changing ideas, roles, or identities. unrealistic or self-defeating. For example, why do residents
Lewin’s (1951) simple formulation of group change cap- of an old coal town continue to act as if the mine is still op-
tures the essence of the stages. First, people must &dquo;unfreeze&dquo; erating when it closed five years ago?
old assumptions from past mental and social associations. Yet asking such seemingly straightforward questions may
Second, they can move to different points-of-view and social be difficult because giving up the past is painful. People
relations. Finally, they can &dquo;freeze&dquo; their thoughts again in need to be able to play with their memories in a special way.
associations that hold them to the new position. For example, when people are injured, they may symboli-
Many formal rituals, particularly in traditional societies, cally repeat their experience many times in an effort to mas-
are divided into three distinct stages, corresponding to these ter it. They try to convert a passively experienced injury into
tasks. They begin by stripping the participants of their ac- a symbolic mental equivalent over which they have active

customed identities and roles and then thrust them into a mastery, as a way of curing themselves (Freud 1977 [1920];
transitional middle stage that does the work of change. Here Klein 1976; Waelder 1933). A shell-shocked soldier, for
they are exposed to a condition where old statuses and trap- example, may repeatedly watch war movies or engage in
pings no longer apply and everything is more or less up for competitive physical sports in an effort to find a way to
grabs (Turner 1969; Van Gennep 1960 [1908]). They can think of himself as a victor rather than a victim. A commu-
leave only by taking a desired new identity. They manage in nity abandoned by its major employer may re-enact the sur-
this experience because they know it is part of a larger struc- rounding events in public theater in an attempt to direct
ture, preceded and certain to be followed by structure. and play lead roles in what happened (Hinsdale, Lewis, and
Moreover, especially in traditional communities, the stages Waller 1995).
may be elaborately ritualized and new points of view may be Changing depends on the possibility of playing with
pre-ordained. memories, mentally picking them up, turning them over,
Everyday efforts to change in modern society are less de- putting them down, picking them up again and looking at
fined. Participation is voluntary. People calculate the costs them from a different aspect, grouping them together in
and benefits of unfreezing old memories, and they weight new combinations, and so forth until one knows them dif-

the past much more heavily than the present. If they con- ferently. When people feel that imagining new accounts of
template change, they assess the likely process. Will it be the past does not unmoor them or betray anyone, they can
painful? Will it harm them irreparably? Will they come out choose new memories. What if the town’s major employer
safely? Will life be better as a result? Individuals and collec- really has been gone for five years?
tivities vary in both their anticipations and their ability and When people feel free to select their memories, they can
willingness to tolerate anxiety associated with change (Dia- engage in the second task of changing, to live hypothetically
mond 1993; Stein 1994). In practice, boundaries between the in the future. A transitional space allows people to experi-
tasks of changing are often fuzzy, and participants may move ment with the imaginary consequences of thinking or acting
back and forth among them. They may declare their inten- as if something unreal or not yet real were real. People can

tions to change but resist unfreezing an old position until, consider possibilities that are realistic but unprecedented
somehow, they experience moving safely to a new one. Mov- when they feel free to think without having to follow with
ing in one respect may motivate unfreezing in another. Finally, commitments to action. They can play with roles that give
in contrast with many formal rituals, the length, course, and them control and situations that bestow dignity. What if the
outcome of change activities are difficult to predict. town asked the state economic development agency to help

Still, the conditions that enable individuals and collectivi- people leave for opportunities elsewhere? What if residents
tiesto change are the same that make the transitional stage raised funds to send their children away to college? What if
of formal rituals effective. People need a space that is free of they tried to get a new federal prison built in the town?

Downloaded from jpe.sagepub.com at DALHOUSIE UNIV on June 26, 2014


10

Thus learning to accept past events as real so as to act re- ing future will not take the place of the idealized past.
alistically toward the future in the present depends on re- Mourning, playing, and working through are processes by
membering and forgetting. We may recall new details about which individuals and collectivities try to give up the past
the past, and they may overshadow others we gradually lose they love and struggle with a disquieting present to create a
track of. But, centrally, we must forget memories as present future in which they might realistically hope. They suggest
events and leave them to be rediscovered in the past tense. principles for designing planning processes.
We must forget that people, places, and things are as they
were. We must remember that things have changed, that
The Aims of Planning
changes have brought injuries and losses. We must forget Planning should enable a community to imagine its best
that we will somehow perish if we lose certain things. We possible futures. Another way to say this is that planning
must remember that we cared so deeply about them that we should help a community experience itself in time. In this
wished to keep them with us forever. project, members must be able to balance imagination and
There is an old joke, where someone tells another, &dquo;Don’t reality without yielding either.
think about elephants.&dquo; Remembering and forgetting are Planning should help them reconstruct a community
linked in this paradoxical way. Their relationship is not di- ideal-an image of themselves as they would like to be-to
rect, linear, unidirectional, or fully conscious. There are no motivate the remembering and forgetting that will let them
even exchanges where, for example, community members wrest the future from the past. Individuals and groups alike
agree to forget something in order to remember something normally form tacit idealized self-portraits, representing the
else. Forgetting and remembering are intricately linked mo- best they can imagine themselves: virtuous, powerful, special,
ments in a high-stakes struggle over how to interpret the and loved.The positive function of these ideals, with their
past and the present. It is possible to prepare the stage for admixture of fantasy and realism, is to assert that one is worth
their play, but it is difficult to script the action or be sure of something, to define ways of being valued, and to motivate
the denouement. efforts to improve, to become more like the ideal. An ideal
becomes problematic when people believe it requires them to
0 PLANNING TO FORGET TO PLAN make themselves perfect and they find all real accomplish-
Planning is an effort to help groups realistically assess ments flawed. Then they become depressed and give up all
their conditions and choose future actions that could im- effort or turn to fantasy to perfect themselves by acts of imagi-
prove them. The two vignettes show that remembering and nation. Neither avenue engages the external world. Thus indi-
forgetting have practical consequences for planning. Contin- viduals and groups live with the challenge of shaping an ideal
gency always puts planning at risk; the future will elude our that calls them to become noble and yet allows them to admire
full mental grasp. In addition, the peculiarities of human themselves in what they can accomplish.
memory, our anxieties about contingency, and our invest- In this effort, communities must remember their past to
ments in forgetting certain things while remembering others be good enough to justify faith in an ideal and to motivate
make it difficult to confront contingency with our strongest planning. They must also remember the past realistically
mental armamentarium. enough to differentiate events in it and to see those events as
Communities find it hard to surrender the past. Even the products of both deliberate actions and contingency.
while apparently engaging in planning, members may recall Then they can think of themselves as trying to choose the
another era and imagine that remembering it will keep it past, as responsible for their intentions, though not necessar-
alive or resurrect it. Fanciful thinking easily displaces analy- ily what followed. Thinking in this way, they can see them-
sis. Communities may go through elaborate planning pro- selves in the past, but not wholly of the past. They can find
cesses only ritualistically, assuming that the effort will magi- themselves as actors in time-not just the past, but, now,
cally put the past into the future. the future. They can begin to move back and forth between
They may seem to look at present conditions, but do so a good-enough portrayal of themselves in the past and a re-

with a pseudo-realistic mania-constantly preparing and alizable ideal for the future-one that calls them for great
holding meetings, obsessively collecting and analyzing data, things but allows them to approach greatness strategically,
continually composing problem statements, cataloging incrementally, and fallibly.88
needs, inventorying resources, looking at every detail-do- In other words, planning should help community mem-
ing everything but facing facts, placing them in realistic per- bers recognize their human complexity-acknowledging the
spectives, constructing and comparing big pictures, and ambiguity and multiplicity of their motivations and reasons,
making difficult, unavoidably imperfect choices. The prod- including those knowable, those known, those unknown,
uct may be a plan that is intricately detailed but, neverthe- those unknowable, and those that, whatever the knowledge,
less, disconnected from implementation. Indeed, it may be imagination, and effort, remain outside their control-their
unimplementable, and its unimplementability may tacitly own accidents waiting to happen. Community members

reassure community members that a realistic but disappoint- must learn to reconcile themselves to planning in the face of

Downloaded from jpe.sagepub.com at DALHOUSIE UNIV on June 26, 2014


11

the impossibility of doing so with full success. Contingency, them as believers. They present a single narrative without
in others’ actions and their own that elude their grasp, will acknowledging alternatives or choices and without arguing
foil planning, but it is the reason for making the attempt. why one might be superior to another. Simply, they deny
contingency. Part of the explanation is that planners, misun-
Telling a Story derstanding professionalism, assume their credibility de-
Planning is an effort to tell a story about the future. Of- pends on pretending to know everything. They may imag-
ten the text is explicit and its drafting deliberate, as with a ine they can influence future affairs if only they assert a
plan (Mandelbaum 1990, 1991). As an account of the fu- single story to be unimpeachable truth (Benveniste 1989).
ture, a plan can be neither true nor false, though it may be Interested parties may push planners to cast things in a spe-
confirmed by subsequent events. As a declaration of inten- cific light. These are all expressions of the wish to see a uni-
tions, it may be written in good faith, and it may be realized verse without contingency.

later on; this is the ambition of planning. In these respects, Planners must help community members understand and
plans are pragmatic documents (see Krieger 1981). give voice to these fantasies in order to resist them. Sometimes
Written plans normally codify conversations about the planning processes do engage participants in confronting
past, present, and future. Scarce time, money, and attention themselves, contesting stories about their past and possibilities.
limit the length of plans, and they inevitably exclude much. In these cases, a written plan is complemented orally by a rich
Their authors emphasize what seemed most important at and efficacious aggregation of stories, arguments, and frag-
the end, and they draft documents that take coherence at ments that comprise a fittingly complex narrative about the
the expense of accuracy. Plans rarely report ideas that were community. A published document may give planning formal
rejected, much less those ignored, and they usually offer no authority, while the oral plan gives it force. Planners should
record of the discussion that preceded the conclusions. The encourage such conversations, in which community members
conclusions, lucidly presented, seem to stand on their own, can reveal and discover their desires, fears, doubts, and am-

bolstered by statements about the people or places whose bivalence about changing. Written documents can more or
interests they putatively serve, but separated from any testi- less represent these intentions, but planners must take the dis-
mony regarding how their advocates came to believe them. cussions as a central part of the plan.
The stylistic norm for plans is to claim truth for an account
beyond those who subscribe to it. Planning for the Future
The proclaimed purpose of plans is to motivate action, to Planners, as partisans of the Enlightenment, promote

find an audience and encourage it to realize a vision. The planning that is change-oriented, rational, logical, and com-
pragmatic test of a plan is whether it can convert readers prehensive. Most speak unambiguously of change as im-
into collaborators. What kind of account of past, present, provement (see Lasch 1991). Yet change depends on stabil-
and future could have this effect? It must help construct nar- ity. People more or less willingly change when they see
rative truth-a plausibly coherent account of the past that is themselves, nevertheless, continuing past attachments.
linked to the future and offers freedom to shape it (Spence These connections maintain their sense of identity. Planners
1982).~ It should portray the past as a domain of contingent often treat planning as if it involved a straightforward cogni-
possibilities, where people tried to bring about certain con- tive process, imagining alternative futures. However, think-
ditions, but where they might have made other choices, ing about future possibilities must struggle against inclina-
where the results reflected not only their choices and efforts, tions to repeat the past.
but also those of others as well as brute contingency, and Hence a planning process must create a transitional space,
where things might have been different under other, even between past and future, where participants can share the
kindred circumstances (Mandelbaum 1985). And it should illusion of being apart from time.&dquo; They need to imagine
acknowledge the possibility of understanding the past in stepping away from past memories without feeling they have
different, divergent ways. lost their identity or betrayed the objects of memory. They
Constructing a narrative in this way can enable a commu- need freedom from everyday responsibilities without feeling
nity to experience the past vitally for the first time. Mem- irresponsible. They must be able to imagine alternative fu-
bers can take it as an object for reflection, find themselves in tures without feeling obligated to enact any of them. By

it, imagine alternative positions for themselves, and see vari- stepping out of time they can re-enter it with new possibili-
ous ways of remembering themselves. In gaining freedom in ties. Planners should secure the process with rules that en-
the past-specifically, in becoming able to reject certain nar- courage, allow, and protect such serious play without imme-
ratives-they come into freedom in the future. Members diate serious consequences. 11
can contribute consciously to a community ideal, and they In contemplating a new future, community members do
can imagine ways to realize it. more than simply declare the past no longer matters. It will

Most written plans are too distilled to be powerful in continue to matter, but differently. Planners must let people
these ways. They offer readers a belief without engaging try to forget parts of the past and remember others. They

Downloaded from jpe.sagepub.com at DALHOUSIE UNIV on June 26, 2014


12

need to mourn for what they decide to give up. They must about comprehensiveness too narrowly, and comprehensive
talk about what they care about in the past, how they loved change, whatever its virtues, seems too easy. Planners can
it, how it made their lives meaningful, how it made them help communities change by helping them keep many
feel special, and how they don’t want to give it up. And they things the same.
must talk about how they want to give it up, how they will A planner has instrumental roles in encouraging talk and
miss it, how they will feel guilty about surrendering it, how reflection. In addition, participants thrust a planner into
they will be angry at themselves for letting go of something symbolic roles. To them, the planner is the one who stirs
they care deeply about, and yet how they must let go, be- things up, talks about change, and urges them to give up
cause it is really gone, because holding on holds them back, what gratifies them. Community members may do battle
because it is an illusion, and for any other reasons. And they with planners as a way of expressing mixed feelings about
must talk, once more, about how they will remember what changing: If only they could beat down the planner who
they are giving up, in a different way. represents an untested future, they wouldn’t have to give up
Thus planning must let people argue with themselves and the past. At the same time, they may associate the planner
be inconsistent. Planners who insist that participants be ra- with the past, for example, as the symbolic agent of the
tional or that discussions follow a logical order elicit only changes that eroded past glories. In a convoluted way, by
superficial participation. Told to be rational, people assume engaging the planner in conflict, they continue to keep
they have been told not to be themselves. They may feel themselves in the past they want to remember. Planners
relieved: Planning will not require them to reveal or risk need to recognize when communities fight them as a way of
what matters. They may let planners believe they have par- managing their anxiety about changing. They should help
ticipated, but they will not have planned. people discuss their fears as a way to see past them toward
Conflict arises when people disagree with one another the future.
about what things are or who should get what. Conflict also One might protest that the planning proposed here is im-
arises when people disagree with themselves, when they are practical. Who would tolerate seeming free-for-all in public
of two (or more) minds about something. Marris (1975) meetings? What would happen if hundreds of people each
notes, for example, that when community members are am- claimed to have part of an oral plan? Wouldn’t talk about
bivalent about changing, unconsciously they may form op- the past just open up avoidable conflicts and divert attention
posing groups and do battle. The conflict may turn on sub- from current conditions? Wouldn’t all the experimental talk
stantive issues, perhaps of considerable importance, but the of a transitional space just help people avoid thinking seri-
sides tacitly represent different stances toward change. One ously about practical action? How are planning commis-
argues for leaping into the future, whereas another insists on sions, legislatures, and administrators supposed to make de-
holding onto the past. Each represents part of a community cisions out of all this?
mind with respect to remembering and forgetting. If they These are good questions. The vignettes at the beginning
can resolve their differences, one effect will be to give up the argue that the easy answers are bad answers because they are
past sufficiently to approach the future. Planners who try to not practical. Communities often have difficulty thinking

suppress conflict or to resolve it on the basis only of substan- about the future. Written plans frequently represent agree-
tive issues can sabotage the work of grieving. Planners ments about superficial matters that do not touch the prob-

should not only expect conflict, but encourage it when it is lems bothering people. We have examined many of the rea-
part of mourning. Negotiations that produce agreements sons-normal human reasons. Taking them seriously does
can help groups work through common losses of past rela- not quickly lead to simple accommodations. Yet these con-

tionships.l2 cerns are not utopian. We can find encouraging examples in

When communities do decide to change, they want to disparate places-participatory planning, alternative dispute
change only a little. Cognitively, they can take in only so resolution, and organizational consultation, for instance
much of the world at once (Miller 1975). Practically, they (Diamond 1993; Forester 1989; Medoff and Sklar 1994;
can affect only a small
part of what they see. Centrally, Stein 1994; Susskind and Cruikshank 1987; Weisbord et al.
people can tolerate only a little change without feeling lost. 1992). The impracticality of many planning practices
Emotionally, human beings are incrementalists. Yet compre- obliges us to search on.
hensiveness is a tenet of planners’ faith: If something should
Author’s Note- John Forester, Sanda Kaufman, Martm Krzeger, and Seymour
change, everything connected to it should change, at once. Mandelbaum have helped clarify the ideas here.
Sometimes planners’ thinking is aesthetic: The parts of a
picture should fit together. Other times, their reasoning is
empirical: The parts of a society work together. However,
what planners often leave out is the humans in the build-
ings, places, or institutions. They must change for land use
(that is, how they use land) to change. Many planners think

Downloaded from jpe.sagepub.com at DALHOUSIE UNIV on June 26, 2014


13

Bion, W. R. 1961. Experiences in Groups. New York: Basic Books.


Black, Alan. 1990. The Chicago Area Transportation Study: A case study
NOTES of rational planning. Journal of Planning Education and Research
1. Baum (1997a, 1998) describes these Balumore, Maryland, cases of the 10(1):27-37.
Southeast Planning Council and The Associated, respectively. Brenner, Charles 1987. Working through: 1914-1984. Psychoanalytic
2. Seymour Mandelbaum helped clarify these issues. Quarterly 56:88-108.
3. Mitroff (1974) provides an excellent study of the practice of natural Brown, Norman O. 1959. . Life Against Death Middletown, Conn.:
science, which contrasts with ideal norms in being highly subjective. Wesleyan University Press.
4. To varying degrees, they recognize that external, political economic, Carr, Edward Hallett. 1961. What is History?
New York: Vintage Books.
structures limit individual or community action. Few say much about Chasseguet-Smirgel, Janine. 1985. The Ego Ideal. Translated by Paul
internal structures—personality and cognitive processes—and how they Burrows. New York: W.W. Norton.
limit action possibilities. Cooper, Arnold M. 1986. Toward a limited definition of psychic trauma.
5. Freud (1977 [1920]) postulated the existence of a death instinct. Death, In The Reconstruction , of Trauma ed. Arnold Rothstein, 25-41.
he observed, is the end of biological life—not just its termination, but Workshop Series of the American Psychoanalytic Association,
also its aim. He suggested the life course is a contest between life- Monograph no. 2. Madison, Conn.: International Universities Press.
sustaining impulses and the death instinct, in which the latter is
Deal, Terrence E., and Allan A. Kennedy. 1982. Corporate Cultures.
ultimately the victor. His formulation can be considered metaphorical, Reading, Mass.: AddisonWesley.
but it captures the sense of being responsible for personal setbacks as well Diamond, Michael A. 1993. The Unconscious Life of Organizations.
as one’s eventual demise. See Brown (1959). Westport, Conn.: Quorum.
6. Freud (1962 [1899]) gave the name "screen memories" to innocuous Erikson, Kai. 1994.
A New Species of Trouble; Explorations in Disaster,
images of past episodes that people unconsciously construct to conceal
Trauma, and Community. New York: W.W. Norton.
other, anxiety-provoking memories from consciousness. The clarity of Fenichel, Otto. 1945. The Psychoanalytic Theory . of Neurosis New York:
screen memories insidiously confuses us into thinking we are remember-
W.W. Norton.
Forester, John. 1989. Planning in the Face of Power. Berkeley, Calif..
ing something when, in reality, we are forgetting other things.
7. Children draw these idealized pictures, called the ego ideal, from what University of California Press.
they remember of the earliest, seemingly unblemished moments of life, Forester, John. 1994. Political deliberation, critical pragmatism, and
traumatic histories: Or on not leaving your pain at the door. Paper
seeing themselves as perfect, omnipotent, omnicompetent, omniscient,
and the center of a loving world (Chasseguet-Smirgel 1985; Freud 1959 presented at the Association of Collegiate Schools of Planning Annual
[1921],1962 [1923]). Adults carry a modification of it into their Conference, Phoenix, Ariz., November 5-8.
endeavors. For example, they may idealize themselves as part of an Freud, Anna. 1946. The Ego and the Mechanisms of Defense. Translated by
Cecil Baines. New York: International Universities Press.
organization (Schwartz 1990) or community (Baum 1997a).
8. "Future search conferences," for example, begin a planning process by Freud, Sigmund. 1957. Instincts and their vicissitudes. 1915. In The
Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud,
helping participants construct a common history (Weisbord et al. 1992). translated by James Strachey, (14):117-140. London, U.K.: The
9. Carr (1961) writes, "To enable man to understand the society of the past
and to increase his mastery over the society of the present is the dual Hogarth Press and the Institute of Psycho-Analysis.
function of history" (69). Mandelbaum (1977, 1980, 1984, 1991) has Freud, Sigmund. 1958. Remembering, repeating and working-through.
articulated a view of the pragmatic uses of history in planning. (Further recommendations on the technique of psycho-analysis II.)
10. This discussion draws on Winnicott’s (1953, 1967) concept of the 1914. The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of
transitional object. The prototype is the infant’s blanket, which Sigmund Freud, translated by James Strachey, 12 147-156. London.:
The Hogarth Press and the Institute of Psycho-Analysis.
symbolizes the relationship with the mother. Manipulating the
blanket—holding it, putting it aside, picking it up again—allows a Freud, Sigmund. 1959. Group Psychology and the Analysis of the Ego. 1921.
child to contemplate alternative relationships with the mother. The Translated by James Strachey. New York: W.W. Norton.
transitional object creates an ontologically ambiguous space that allows Freud, Sigmund. 1962. Screen memories. 1899. The Standard Edition of
the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud, translated by James
experimentation with new positions without giving up old ones. A
planning process can provide a similar space for illusions. Strachey, 14:301-322. London, U.K.: The Hogarth Press and the
11. John Forester notes that the purpose of ground rules in mediated Institute of Psycho-Analysis.
Freud, Sigmund. 1962. The Ego and the Id. 1923. Translated by Joan
negotiation and consensus building is to create such transitional space. Riviere. New York: W.W. Norton.
12. In planning by The Associated: Jewish Community Federation of
Baltimore, community groups disagreed about the authority of Freud, Sigmund. 1963. The Problem . of Anxiety 1936 Translated by Henry
different memories and the legitimacy of alternative futures. Planners Alden Bunker. New York: Psychoanalytic Quarterly Press and W.W.
tried to suppress conflict in order to plan harmoniously. Instead, the Norton.
conflicts, undiscussed and unaddressed, persisted, and the organization Freud, Sigmund. 1977. Beyond the Pleasure Principle. 1920. Translated by
could not plan realistically (Baum 1997a). Forester (1994) shows the James Strachey. New York: Bantam Books.
importance of explicitly addressing traumas and grievances. Fried, Marc. 1963. Grieving for a lost home. In The Urban Condition, ed.
Leonard J. Duhl, 151-171. New York: Basic Books.
0 REFERENCES Frost, Peter J , Larry F. Moore, Meryl Reis Louis, Craig C. Lundberg, and
Joanne Martin. 1985. Organizational Culture
. Newbury Park, Calif.: Sage
Alexander, Ernest R. 1996. After rationality: Towards a contingency theory Publications.
for planning. In Explorations in Planning Theory, eds. Seymour J. Gabriel, Yiannis. 1993. Organizational nostalgia: Reflections on ’the golden
Mandelbaum, Luigi Mazza, and Robert W. Burchell, 45-64. New age.’ In Emotion in Organizations, ed. Stephen Fineman, 118-141.
Brunswick, N.J.: Center for Urban Policy Research. Newbury Park, Calif.: Sage Publications.
Baum, Howell S. 1990. Organizational Membership. Albany, N.Y.: State Gans, Herbert J. 1982. The Urban Villagers, updated and expanded ed. New
University of New York Press. York: Free Press.
Baum, Howell S. 1997a. The Organization of Hope: Communities Planning Gottdiener, Mark. 1997. The . Theming of America Boulder, Colo.: Westview
Themselves. Albany, N.Y.: State University of New York Press. Press.
Baum, Howell S. 1997b. Teaching practice. Journal of Planning Education Greenacre, Phyllis. 1956. Re-evaluation of the process of working through.
and Research 17(1):21-30. International Journal of Psychoanalysis 37:439-444.
Baum, Howell S. 1998. Ethical behavior is extraordinary behavior; it’s the Greenson, Ralph R. 1965. The problem of working through. In Drives,
same as all other behavior: A case study in community planning. Journal
Affects, Behavior, Vol. 2, Essays in Memory of Marie Bonaparte, ed. Max
of the American Planning Association 64(4):411-423. Schur, 277-314. New York: International Universities Press.
Benveniste, Guy. 1989. Mastering the Politics .
of Planning San Francisco: Halbwachs, Maurice. 1992. On Collective Memory, ed. and transl. Lewis A.
Jossey-Bass. Coser. Chicago, Ill.: University of Chicago Press.

Downloaded from jpe.sagepub.com at DALHOUSIE UNIV on June 26, 2014


14

Healey, Patsy. 1997. Collaborative Planning. Vancouver, Canada: UBC Mitroff, Ian I. 1974. The Subjective Side .
of Science New York: American
Press. Elsevier.
Hinsdale, Mary Ann, Helen M. Lewis, and S. Maxine Waller. 1995.
It Modell, Arnold H. 1990. Other Times, Other Realities. Cambridge, Mass.:
Comes from the People. Philadelphia, Pa.: Temple University Press. Harvard University Press.
Hoch, Charles. 1994. What Planners Do. Chicago, Ill.: Planners Press. Orwell, George. 1961. 1984. 1949. New York: New American Library.
Innes, Judith E. 1997. Information in communicative planning. Journal Parkin, Alan. 1981. Repetition, mourning, and working through.
of the American Planning Association 64(1):52-63 International Journal of PsychoAnalysis 52:271-281.
Janis, Irving L. 1982. Groupthink. 2nd edition. Boston, Mass.: Houghton Sager, Tore. 1994. Communicative Planning Theory. Aldershot, U.K.:
Mifflin. Avebury.
Kaplan, Harvey A. 1987. The psychopathology of nostalgia. Psychoana- Schein, Edgar H. 1992. Organizational Culture and Leadership. 2nd
lytic Review 74(4):465486. edinon. San Francisco, Calif.: Jossey-Bass.
Klein, George S. 1976. Psychoanalytic Theory. New York: International Schon, Donald A. 1983. The Reflective Practitioner. New York: Jossey-Bass.
Universities Press. Schwartz, Howard S. 1990. Narcissistic Process and Corporate Decay. New
Krieger, Martin H. 1981. Advice and Planning. Philadelphia, Pa.: Temple York: New York University Press.
University Press. Seeley, John R. 1967. Social science: Some probative problems. In The
Krueckeberg, Donald. 1984. Planning and the new depression in the Americanization ofthe Unconscious, 149-165. New York: International
social sciences. Journal of Planning Education and Research 3(2):78-87. Science Press.
Lasch, Christopher. 1991. The True and Only Heaven. New York: W.W. Shackle, G.L.S. 1958. Time in Economics. Amsterdam, Neth.: North-
Norton. Holland Publishing Company.
Lee, Douglass B. 1973. Requiem for large scale models. Journal of the Shackle, G.L.S. 1969. Decision, Order, and Time in Human Affairs. 2nd
American Institute of Planners 39(3):163-178. edition. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.
. Chicago, Ill.:
Levine, Donald N. 1985. The Flight from Ambiguity Sorkin, Michael, ed. 1992. Variations on a Theme Park. New York: The
University of Chicago Press. Noonday Press.
Lewin, Kurt. 1951. Frontiers in group dynamics. In Field Theory in Social Spence, Donald P. 1982. Narrative Truth and Historical Truth
. New York:
Science, ed. Dorwin Cartwright, 188-237. New York: Harper and W.W. Norton.
Brothers. Stein, Howard F. 1994. Listening Deeply. Boulder, Colo.: Westview.
Lowenthal, David. 1985. The Past is a Foreign Country. Cambridge, U.K: Sturken, Marita. 1997. Tangled Memories. Berkeley, Calif: University of
Cambridge University Press. California Press.
Mack, Ruth P. 1971. Planning on Uncertainty. New York: Wiley- Susskind, Lawrence, and Jeffrey Cruikshank. 1987. Breaking the Impasse.
Interscience. New York: Basic Books.
Mandelbaum, Seymour J. 1977. The past in service to the future. Journal Turner, Victor. 1969. The Ritual Process. Ithaca, N.Y.: Cornell University
of Social History 11:193-205. Press.
Mandelbaum, Seymour J. 1980. Urban pasts and urban policies. Journal Vale, Lawrence J. 1997. Empathological places: Residents’ ambivalence
of Urban History 6(4):453-483. toward remaining in public housing. Journal of Planning Education and
Mandelbaum, Seymour J. 1984. Temporal conventions in planning Research 16(3):159-176.
discourse. Environment and Planning B: Planning and Design 11:5-13. Van Gennep, Arnold. 1960. The Rites of Passage. 1908. Translated by
Mandelbaum, Seymour J. 1985. Historians and planners: The construc- Monika Vizedom and Gabrielle L. Caffee. Chicago, Ill.: University of
tion of pasts and futures. Journal of the American Planning Association Chicago Press.
51(2):185-188. Varady, David P., and Carole C. Walker. 1997. The rise and fall of Eutaw
Mandelbaum, Seymour J. 1990. Reading plans. Journal of the American Gardens Apartments, Baltimore, Maryland: A case study of a distressed,
Planning Association 56:350-356. subsidized private housing development. Paper presented at the 39th
Mandelbaum, Seymour J. 1991. Reading old plans. In Urban Public Annual Conference of the Association of Collegiate Schools of
Policy, ed. Martin V. Melosi, 189-198. University Park, Pa.: Planning, Fort Lauderdale, Florida, November 6.
Pennsylvania State University Press. Waelder, Robert. 1933. The psychoanalytic theory of play. Psychoanalytic
Mandelbaum, Seymour J., Luigi Mazza, and Robert W. Burchell, eds. Quarterly 2:208-224.
1996. Explorations in Planning Theory. New Brunswick, N.J.: Center Weisbord, Marvin R., et al. 1992. Discovering Common Ground. San
for Urban Policy Research. Francisco, Calif.: Berrett-Koehler Publishers.
Marris, Peter. 1975. Loss and Change. Garden City, N.Y.: Anchor Books. Werman, David. 1977. Normal and pathological nostalgia. Journal of the
Marris, Peter. 1996. The Politics of Uncertainty. New York: Routledge. American Psychoanalytic Association 25:313-320.
Medoff, Peter, and Holly Sklar. 1994. Streets of Hope. Boston, Mass.: Winnicott, D.W. 1953. Transitional objects and transitional phenomena.
South End Press. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis 34:89-97.
Miller, George A. 1975. The Psychology of Communication. New York: Winnicott, D.W. 1967. The location of cultural experience. The
Basic Books. International Journal of Psychoanalysis 48:368-372.
Yorke, Chfford. 1986. Reflections on the problem of psychic trauma.
Psychoanalytic Study of the Child 41:221-236.

Downloaded from jpe.sagepub.com at DALHOUSIE UNIV on June 26, 2014

You might also like