You are on page 1of 19

Received: 22 October 2018 Revised: 16 December 2019 Accepted: 30 December 2019

DOI: 10.1002/joom.1082

RESEARCH ARTICLE

The impact of disclosing inventory-scarcity messages


on sales in online retailing

Sungho Park1,2 | Elliot Rabinovich3 | Christopher S. Tang4 | Rui Yin3

1
Marketing Department, W. P. Carey
School of Business, Arizona State
Abstract
University, Tempe, Arizona To influence demand, some online retailers post messages (e.g., “5 units or less
2
SNU Business School, Seoul National left in stock”) on their product pages to signal impending stockouts. These
University 1 Gwanak-ro, Gwanak-gu,
“scarcity” messages provide consumers “partial” inventory information, reveal-
Seoul, 08826, South Korea
3
Supply Chain Management Department,
ing only an upper bound on the number of units available for sale. To examine
W. P. Carey School of Business, Arizona the impact of these messages, we obtained price and sales data from an online-
State University, Tempe, Arizona retailer website across a sample of durable goods before and after the retailer
4
Business Administration, Anderson
posted the messages over multiple inventory-replenishment cycles. We then
School of Management, University of
California, Los Angeles, California used these data to assess empirically the effect of these messages on these
products' daily sales. We find that disclosing these messages can decrease daily
Correspondence
sales by an average of 17.60%. This finding suggests that scarcity messages such
Elliot Rabinovich, Supply Chain
Management Department, W. P. Carey as these can have a negative influence on the sales prospects of durable goods.
School of Business, Arizona State We also observe, on the other hand, that price discounts are quite effective in
University, Tempe, AZ 85287-4706.
Email: elliot.rabinovich@asu.edu
increasing sales and offsetting the losses induced by scarcity messages. On
average, a reduction of 1% in stock keeping unit price increases daily sales by
Handling Editor: Michael Galbreth approximately 3%. Therefore, relative to disclosing scarcity messages, price dis-
counts are a much more effective tool at increasing inventory turns.

KEYWORDS
econometrics, inventory management, online retailing, operations management-marketing
interface

1 | INTRODUCTION retailers' goal is to induce consumers not to miss out on


these events by showing them how fast inventory is
Low search costs on the Internet enable consumers to decreasing for the SKUs. According to empirical research
compare many retail offers freely which, in turn, exacer- by Cui, Zhang, and Bassamboo (2018) and Wagner,
bates certain purchasing behaviors, including the active Calvo, and Cui (2018), exposing consumers to informa-
pursuit of bargains (Brynjolfsson & Smith, 2000; Zhang, tion on these inventory reductions significantly increases
Fang, & Sheng, 2006) and the postponement of purchases the deals' frequency of purchase.
in anticipation of future price drops (Aviv, Tang, & Yin, These findings, however, are limited to a small frac-
2009; Netessine & Tang, 2009). To counteract these tion of online retail sales, since the vast majority of e-
behaviors, many online retailers conduct “flash sales” commerce purchases involve durable goods available for
events of deeply discounted stock keeping units (SKUs) sale over indefinite time horizons and multiple inventory
for limited times (usually 24 hr) during which they make replenishment cycles. The selling strategies in these
available to consumers real-time information on every mainstream environments differ notably from those in
SKU's inventory level available for purchase. The “flash sales” settings, where retailers typically promote

J Oper Manag. 2020;1–19. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/joom © 2020 Association for Supply Chain Management, Inc. 1
2 PARK ET AL.

heavily discounted items in limited quantities (i.e., with inventory drops below this threshold, retailers can let
no inventory replenishments) and for extremely short consumers know of the maximum amount of inventory
durations to attract bargain hunters who are inherently available for sale. However, since these “scarcity mes-
interested in taking advantage of these temporary promo- sages” contain only partial inventory information, con-
tions. Consequently, these consumers are naturally more sumers (as well as competitors and suppliers) will not
likely to purchase when they observe that inventory is know the exact inventory levels available for sale at any
running low (Ferreira, Lee, & Simchi-Levi, 2016; point before purchase.
Sodero & Rabinovich, 2017). To our knowledge, no research has evaluated the
This is not necessarily the case in mainstream settings impact of these scarcity messages' disclosure on durable
involving durable goods, where consumers often make consumer goods' sales in mainstream online retail envi-
purchasing decisions based on their personal needs, as ronments. We are only aware of a study by Sodero,
opposed to opportunistically, upon encountering Rabinovich, Aydinliyim, and Pangburn (2017) that also
discounted offers available during short time windows. focused on these settings. However, this study focused on
For these consumers, low inventory levels may actually the link between purchasing frequencies (not total sales)
undermine their confidence in the retailer's quality of ser- and inventory depletion at low levels, in which
vice (Balakrishnan, Pangburn, & Stavrulaki, 2004). They inventory-level information is displayed continuously to
may also have an adverse influence in their inferences consumers at all times. Therefore, Sodero et al. (2017)
regarding products' quality in relation to other items that were unable to estimate the impact of the initial disclo-
are available for sale currently or may become available sure of scarcity information on sales because they focused
in the future. Specifically, a low inventory amount may exclusively on consumers' purchasing rates and had no
lead consumers to infer that the retailer has chosen to way of making comparisons “before” versus “after” con-
forego replenishing its inventory because the products sumers' exposure to scarcity information. Because we can
are undesirable, of low quality, or likely to become obso- evaluate the differences in sales that exist before versus
lete (Koschat, 2008). To the extent that low search costs after a retailer posts this information for its products, we
on the Web make it easy for consumers to look for prod- can expand on Sodero et al.'s (2017) analysis. Moreover,
ucts elsewhere (Bakos, 1997), low stock quantities may because the disclosure of scarcity messages is potentially
actually induce them to forego buying these items and applicable as a widespread mechanism to influence con-
reduce sales (Koschat, 2008; Wolfe, 1968). sumers' purchasing behavior, our research not only has
Another important consideration is that SKUs sold academic relevance, but also carries significant implica-
through flash sales events are offered at prices that typi- tions for practitioners in the retail industry.
cally remain fixed regardless of how fast inventory is To evaluate the aforementioned issues, we collected
depleted. This is generally not the case for products in data (prices, sales, etc.) from a sample of SKUs sold by a
mainstream environments. Because these items are avail- focal retailer that posted scarcity messages with partial
able for sale over indefinite time horizons and multiple inventory scarcity information (“5 or less left in stock”)
inventory replenishment cycles, their prices normally fluc- every time the inventory level of an SKU dropped below
tuate with inventory levels. Such conditions are not com- 6 units. Because this retailer sells durable consumer
monly present in flash sales settings, making it difficult to goods available for purchase with no time or inventory-
evaluate whether lower prices or inventory availability are cycle restrictions, we can examine the impact that post-
more effective in promoting online sales in those settings. ing these messages has on sales for each SKU over
Our goal is to address these deficiencies in the litera- extended periods. In so doing, we can partial out time
ture. A challenge in fulfilling this objective is that many effects from the effects of inventory messages' disclosure
online retailers in mainstream settings have chosen not on sales, since it is possible that the time a product has
to share with the public exact inventory level information been available for sale will influence demand. Further-
because disclosing such information may give their com- more, having no limitations imposed by the retailer on
petitors and suppliers sensitive insights into their inven- the amount of time products are available for sale means
tory management and pricing policies (Fisher, Gallino, & that we can examine the effects of these messages' disclo-
Li, 2017). However, there are retailers (e.g., Walmart and sure on sales for each SKU longitudinally while isolating
Overstock) that have chosen to disclose “partial” inven- significant differences in these effects across products
tory information contained in messages alerting con- selling in large versus small daily amounts due to season-
sumers about the maximum number of units available in ality. Finally, because prices in our data vary over time,
stock when actual inventory levels drop below this upper we can also isolate these effects from longitudinal price
bound. For instance, by posting a message alerting shop- fluctuation effects on sales. To our knowledge, no paper
pers that an SKU has “5 units or less left in stock” when has evaluated these effects in an online setting. This is
PARK ET AL. 3

despite the fact that this evaluation carries important (2017), we discussed earlier are part of this body of work.
implications about the effectiveness of scarcity messages So are papers that have examined empirically the impact
for retailers because it can quantify the equivalency of inventory displays on sales in offline settings. In gen-
between the effect of these messages on product sales eral, these studies have evaluated how perceptions of
and the effect of price discounts. inventory abundance influence shoppers' purchasing
Contrary to results in flash sales environments (Cui decisions. To evaluate this phenomenon empirically,
et al., 2018; Wagner et al., 2018) showing that consumers' these studies have had to tackle a variety of endogeneity
exposure to inventory scarcity messages improves sales, concerns (particularly those involving sales, inventory
we find that such exposure reduces daily sales by an aver- levels, and pricing) that exist in traditional, brick, and
age of 17.60%. This is consistent with research in offline mortar environments. In the fashion industry, Wolfe
settings by Wolfe (1968) and Koschat (2008), among (1968) and Boada-Collado and Martinez-de-Albeniz
others. According to this research, when consumers make (2019) examined the impact of inventory levels on sales
buying decisions for products based on demand needs, across items offered at different retail stores and found
instead of opportunistically based on the availability of that inventory levels have a positive effect on sales.
deep but short-lived discounts, low inventory levels will Koschat (2008) observed a similar effect on the sale of
influence adversely consumers' inferences regarding the magazines at retail stores. Focusing on the automotive
retailers' quality of service as well as the quality of the industry, Cachon, Gallino, and Olivares (2018) showed
products offered in relation to other items that are avail- that scarcity at auto dealers can increase sales but this
able for sale currently or may become available for sale in effect vanishes as variety increases.
the future. Akin to these studies, we are interested in the sale of
In contrast, our results reveal that price discounts are durable goods that consumers may purchase without
more effective at increasing sales. We find that a 1% time limitations and over multiple inventory cycles. In so
decrease in price leads to a 3.20% increase in daily sales. doing, we also contribute to an additional stream of liter-
Therefore, daily sales are highly sensitive to variations in ature in OM that has analyzed how sellers can benefit
product prices. One important consideration is that these from disclosing opaque inventory information for this
effects are applicable to all SKUs regardless of price and, type of goods. As part of these strategies, sellers may only
therefore, they carry greater benefit for SKUs sold at lower disclose information about the availability of a product
price points, for which discounts will be lower in absolute but not the availability of that product's variants. For
terms. Moreover, these results remain consistent while instance, a seller may reveal the exact amount of inven-
controlling for the effects of scarcity messages' disclosure tory available for a set of cloth napkins but not the exact
on sales for each SKU selling in large versus small daily amount available for specific colors. Through this strat-
amounts. As a result, price discounts will carry greater egy, sellers can discriminate among consumers who are
value in increasing inventory turns when applied to high- sensitive about the availability of inventory for each of
selling items. the product's variants versus those who are not. Jerath,
The remainder of our article proceeds as follows. Netessine, and Veeraraghavan (2010) observed that this
Section 2 presents a review of the literature and expands strategy discourages consumers from waiting to purchase.
on the theory relevant to our study. Section 3 expands More recently, Cui and Shin (2017) showed that when a
upon the study's empirical methodology. Sections 4 and 5 seller offers multiple variants of the same product and
present the statistical analyses. Finally, Section 6 presents the specific variant preferred by consumers is likely to
the study's conclusions, implications, and opportunities stock out, the seller should disclose aggregate inventory
for future research. levels across the variants to increase sales.
The type of inventory information disclosure that we
consider in our article can be interpreted as a variation of
2 | LITERAT U RE R EVI EW A N D the type of opaque information examined by Jerath et al.
THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK (2010) and Cui and Shin (2017). In our case, the timing
of the release of the scarcity message that the seller pro-
Our article is positioned broadly along with other vides to consumers and the contents of this message (“5
research at the interface between the operations manage- or less left in stock”) are uniform across products and
ment (OM) and marketing literatures that has studied a remain unchanged over time. That is, the retailer releases
variety of phenomena in Internet environments (Boyer, the same message when stock levels fall below 6 units,
Hallowell, & Roth, 2002; Rao, Rabinovich, & Raju, 2014; regardless of the SKU. Moreover, the message's content
Rosenzweig, Laseter, & Roth, 2011). The papers by Cui does not change as a function of the actual amount of
et al. (2018), Wagner et al. (2018), and Sodero et al. inventory available (5, 4, 3, 2, or 1 units) for an SKU.
4 PARK ET AL.

Thus, after the retailer posts this message, consumers will buying these items. Hence, the posting of scarcity mes-
know that the upper bound of the inventory level is sages can have a negative impact on SKU daily sales.
5 units. On the other hand, before this message is dis- The juxtaposition of these opposite forces caused by the
played, consumers will know that there are at least 6 units disclosure of scarcity messages with inventory upper bound
available for sale; however, this is of little use for con- information may result in one of two possible demand
sumers since they will not know the extent to which the effect outcomes regarding SKU sales. On one hand, this dis-
actual inventory level is above 6 (i.e., the actual inventory closure may create scarcity perceptions among consumers
level can be 7 or 7,000 units). Essentially, by posting these and increase sales if product offers are rare. Alternatively, it
scarcity messages, the retailer is effectively sharing with may undermine consumers' confidence in the retailer's
consumers' inventory information with different levels of quality of service or signal that the products are being
precision at different points in time. “Before” the scarcity phased out by the retailer because of low quality or because
message is disclosed, consumers will only know the mini- they are past their peak. As a result, it will decrease sales.
mum value in the range of inventory amounts available Hypotheses 1 and 2 summarize these opposite effects.
(i.e., 6 units). However, “after” the posting of this mes-
sage, consumers will know there is a specific range in the Hypothesis 1 The disclosure of scarcity messages with
inventory amounts available (of from 1 to 5 units). inventory upper bound information will increase
To understand the influence of this inventory informa- SKU sales.
tion on sales of durable goods in an online environment,
we focus on the “demand effect” initially identified by Hypothesis 2 The disclosure of scarcity messages with
Koschat (2008) and Balakrishnan et al. (2004); inventory upper bound information will decrease
Balakrishnan, Pangburn, and Stavrulaki (2008) in offline SKU sales.
settings. This effect reflects the changes in a product's
demand function caused by consumers' exposure to inven-
tory information (in our case, it is the information about 3 | EMPIRICAL METHODOLOGY
the maximum number of units available for sale contained
in scarcity messages). This demand effect may induce two To conduct our empirical analysis and test the aforemen-
countervailing forces that can cause a product's daily sales tioned hypotheses, we collected data from Bon-Ton (BT),
to increase or decrease. On one hand, the demand effect a multichannel retailer selling a variety of durable goods
may manifest itself in an increase in the intensity of scarcity in the apparel, accessories, home furnishings, home
perceptions among consumers after the retailer posts scar- appliances, and toy categories. Founded in 1898, BT sold
city messages with information on an upper bound for products exclusively through catalogs and stores up until
products' inventory. According to commodity theory 2005, when it added its online channel. With annual net
(Brock, 1968), these product scarcity perceptions can cre- revenues of $2.7 billion in 2017 and 20% year over year
ate a “buying frenzy” among consumers who are particu- growth in Internet sales (totaling almost $200 million
larly sensitive about possible stockouts (DeGraba, 1995) annually by 2017), BT's online channel has become a
and have a positive utility from owning products that are major source of revenue for the company.
not commonly available (Lynn, 1991). This implies that We chose to use BT as a single focal retailer for our
the disclosure of the upper bound information for prod- empirical analysis in order to hold constant various factors
ucts' inventory can “nudge” consumers to commit to their that could influence demand parameters independently of
purchases earlier, which will increase SKU sales. the demand effect caused by inventory scarcity messages.
On the other hand, scarcity messages can generate an These factors include the retailer's brand (Chevalier &
opposite force that can discourage consumer purchases Goolsbee, 2003; Smith & Brynjolfsson, 2001), Web interface
(Urban, 2005). Specifically, because scarcity messages attributes (Olson & Boyer, 2003), and ordering, fulfillment,
inform consumers that stock levels are low, they may and product return options (Rabinovich, Sinha, & Laseter,
undermine consumers' confidence about the retailer's 2011; Rao, Griffis, & Goldsby, 2011). In this sense, our
level of service (Balakrishnan et al., 2004). Consumers choice to focus on a single retailer for our empirical evalua-
may also infer that the retailer has chosen to forego tion follows a similar logic behind a variety of studies that
replenishing its inventory because the products are unde- have used this approach to study a range of phenomena
sirable, of low quality, or likely to become obsolete related to Internet operations (Olson, Belohlav, &
(Koschat, 2008). To the extent that low search costs on Boyer, 2005).
the Internet make it easy for consumers to look for other In each of its SKUs' online displays, BT posts a scarcity
products elsewhere (Bakos, 1997), the disclosure of this message as soon as inventories available for sale fall below
inventory information may induce shoppers to forego 6 units. Once stocks go below this threshold, BT displays a
PARK ET AL. 5

F I G U R E 1 Inventory scarcity
message posted on Bon-Ton's website
[Color figure can be viewed at
wileyonlinelibrary.com]

message on each product's page with limited inventory level available for each SKU, it did maintain exact
information letting consumers know that there are “5 inventory-level information in the source code for each of
[units] or less left in stock” for the SKU (please see its SKUs' webpages. This enabled Cartbound to track the
Figure 1 for an example of this type of display). Although exact inventory levels available for each SKU over time.
at the time we collected our data, BT operated an online Cartbound also tracked when BT posted its inventory
and a brick-and-mortar channel, we selected for our analy- scarcity message. By recording the actual inventory levels
sis products that were available for sale exclusively through for each SKU, Cartbound could track when reductions in
the online channel, as advertised for each product on the inventory levels occurred as well as the magnitude of
retailer's website. In doing so, we wanted to ensure that these reductions. From Cartbound's information, we
the measurement of the demand effect in the online chan- could then estimate the daily sales rate for each SKU.
nel caused by the sharing of scarcity messages with con- In total, 199 SKUs had multiple replenishment cycles
sumers was not biased by inventory availability at the in which BT posted inventory scarcity messages during
store channel. Because BT offered all SKUs in its apparel our 18-month time window. We focused on these
category through the online and store channels, we did 199 SKUs in our study so that we can evaluate the effect
not include this category in our analysis. Also, note that of BT's disclosure of inventory scarcity messages on sales.
although BT sold exclusively online the SKUs, we chose Figure 2 shows the distribution of the number of SKUs
for our study, these items or similar substitutes were avail- across different product types in our data.
able for sale elsewhere. Therefore, the SKUs' availability We measured sales for each SKU in terms of the
was not exclusive to BT. “number of units sold per day.” We used days as the unit
of measurement for this variable because the SKUs in the
sample include exclusively durable consumer goods,
3.1 | Data collection which are sold infrequently and are not subject to sharp
rates of perishability. As Cartbound monitored for
We focused on SKUs available for sale online across changes in inventory levels, it also monitored in parallel
28 different SKU categories, including accessories, home for changes in prices made by BT for each of the SKUs in
furnishings, home appliances, and toy categories. For the sample. We used this information to obtain each SKU
each SKU, we obtained data provided by Cartbound, an price charged by BT over time, including the instances
online platform that monitored pricing and inventory when sales took place. From the data, we found that, on
information posted online by BT during a period of average, BT changed the prices of the SKUs every
18 months (from August 1, 2014 to January 31, 2016). 14.2 days (SD = 7.5 days). Therefore, the unit of measure-
Although during this period BT posted a scarcity message ment in days appears to be sufficiently granular relative
(“5 or less left in stock”) every time the inventory of an to the frequency of changes in SKU prices.
SKU dropped below 6 units and did not disclose on its In Appendix A, we expand our discussion on: (a) our
product webpages information about the exact inventory- data collection methodology; (b) a variety of analyses we
6 PARK ET AL.

F I G U R E 2 Distribution of the
number of stock keeping units
across product types [Color figure
can be viewed at
wileyonlinelibrary.com]

TABLE 1 Descriptive statistics

Variable Daily sales Price Scarcity message Inventory level


Daily sales (Number of units) 1
Price (dollars) −0.014 1
Scarcity message (1 = Yes, 0 = No) −0.063 −0.030 1
Inventory level (Number of units) 0.110 0.006 −0.370 1
Average 0.77 383.50 0.28 37.38
Standard deviation 4.80 330.83 0.45 59.26
Minimum 0 19.99 0 0
Maximum 381 3,075 1 696
Number of observations 27,044 27,044 27,044 27,044

conducted to test the robustness of the measurements of fixed effects to control for any biases in our results caused
daily sales; and (c) the robustness of the estimations of by the late entry or early removal of SKUs from BT's
demand effect by scarcity messages' disclosure on daily assortment as well as to account for any other time-
sales we obtained in Section 4. Note that to generate invariant effects inherent to each SKU that might have
these measurements and effect estimations, we account been unobservable to us. By using this approach, we can
for the fact that the SKUs in our sample were not con- identify the effect of scarcity disclosure using within-SKU
stantly available for sale during the 18-month sample variability in scarcity disclosure and generate results that
period. Specifically, BT introduced 183 of the sample are independent of any potential sampling bias.
SKUs to its assortment after the start of our observation
period and removed 180 SKUs before the end of this
period. On average, SKUs were available for 142 days 3.2 | Descriptive statistics
during the observation period. Moreover, there were days
during this period when inventory for different SKUs Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics from the panel
decreased down to zero, making it impossible for con- we used to estimate the effect of posting inventory scarcity
sumers to purchase these products during these days. messages on daily sales. Associated with each SKU, the
Consequently, to prevent biases in our measures of daily measurement for daily sales corresponds to the number of
sales, we removed from consideration a total of 1,634 units sold (including zero units sold over “non-OOS” days)
SKU-day observations when these out-of-stocks (OOS) on any given day according to the selling price during that
occurred. day. As shown in Table 1, we collect sales measurements
Ultimately, the measurements of daily sales for each for each SKU (daily sales) as part of each daily observa-
SKU excluded the OOS days between the dates of the sales tion, along with the values for price, inventory level, and
that depleted the SKU's inventory and the dates of the scarcity message (defined above), over the 18-month
SKU's replenishment. Therefore, each SKU's daily sales period, resulting in 27,044 SKU-day observations.
measurements excluded all zero-unit “sales” observed dur- Among the statistics reported in Table 1, the average
ing these OOS days. Furthermore, our analyses used SKU of daily sales is 0.77 units with a standard deviation of
PARK ET AL. 7

4.80. The magnitude of the standard deviation provides SMid equals 1 if BT had the scarcity message for SKU i on
an indication of the variability that exists in daily sales display on day d. Otherwise, SMid equals zero. Observe
across SKUs. Moreover, the average and standard devia- that the scarcity message dummy variable, SM, enables
tion for price in Table 1 (383.50 and 330.83, respectively) us to distinguish the purchasing behavior “before” and
provide evidence of the existence of high variability “after” BT posted its scarcity messages. Thus, the
among the SKUs in the sample. Finally, Table 1 reports corresponding coefficient γ seeks to capture the demand
the values of the variables' Pearson correlation coeffi- effect as a function of the impact of scarcity messages on
cients. We find that the highest correlation coefficient daily sales. The term αi captures SKU-specific heteroge-
(in absolute value) is between scarcity message and neity in daily sales. Also, βm captures the month effect in
inventory level (−0.370). Thus, our analysis using these daily sales. In turn, εid corresponds to idiosyncratic ran-
variables is unlikely to be subject to multicollinearity dom shock variables with mean zero. This variable cap-
problems.1 tures unobserved factors influencing daily sales.
To estimate Equation (1), we first used a fixed effect
dummy least square estimator. It is well known that this
4 | S T A T I S T I C A L MO D E L S AN D estimation approach is consistent and efficient. It also
RESULTS uses fewer assumptions compared to the random effect
estimators approach (c.f., Chapter 10 in Wooldridge
To estimate the demand effect of the inventory scarcity (2010)). Moreover, it is consistent under potential selec-
messages on daily sales, we first used a “naïve” statistical tion or endogeneity problems caused by correlations
modeling approach that makes no attempt to control for between αi and other explanatory variables (i.e., Pid and
unobserved demand shocks that may make the disclosure SMid). In Equation (1), unobserved SKU-specific charac-
of scarcity messages endogenous with respect to the teristics are captured by αi. As we mentioned before, by
dependent variable. We then used a regression disconti- estimating this parameter using a fixed effect approach in
nuity design (RDD) approach to address the issue of our regression model, we circumvent potential empirical
endogeneity as well as to allow for heterogeneity in the problems due to selection issues.
demand effects of scarcity messages as a function of dif- Table 2 reports our estimation results of Equation (1).
ferences in inherent sales patterns across SKUs. We pre- We omit the estimates of αi's and βm's to conserve space.
sent the results obtained from the naïve statistical Please note that we evaluate the robustness of these
modeling approach in Section 4.1 and the results from results in the Appendix B using alternative modeling
the RDD approach in Sections 4.2 and 4.3. forms and specifications while still making no attempt to
control for unobserved demand shocks that may make
the disclosure of scarcity messages endogenous with
4.1 | Naïve modeling approach respect to the dependent variable.2 The results from these
alternative models are consistent with those presented in
Our initial analysis is based on the regression model as
stated in Equation (1) below
X TABLE 2 Baseline estimation results for Equation (1)
lnðSalesid + 1Þ = αi + β I ðd∈mÞ + γSMid
m m ð1Þ Fixed effect dummy least square
+ δlnðPid Þ + εid :
Dependent var.: ln(Sales+1)

Parameter Estimate SE t-val.


In our model, we index SKU i = 1, …, 199 and the
index d = 1, …, Di denote different calendar days. To cap- Price for sales (δ) −0.032** 0.014 −2.276
ture the effect associated with different months m = 1,…, Scarcity message (γ) −0.158*** 0.009 −17.671
M (including seasonal effects), we introduce an indicator SKU fixed effect (αi) Yes
variable, I, so that I (d ∈m) equals 1 if the day of the sales Month fixed effect (βm) Yes
d occurs in month m or 0, otherwise. Salesid represents F statistic 24.360***
the sales (in units) of SKU i on day d. Because the log of
R-squared 0.149
zero is undefined, we add a value of 1 to all Salesid values
Adj. R-squared 0.142
on the left-hand side of Equation (1).
On the right-hand side of Equation (1), Pid denotes Number of observations 27,044
the unit sales price of SKU i on day d. Next, the variable Note: Clustered standard errors are used.
SMid corresponds to a scarcity message dummy variable. **Significance at 5%, ***significance at 1%.
8 PARK ET AL.

Table 2. Observe from Table 2 that the estimated coeffi- estimate SMid's causal effects on Salesid through a com-
cient of price effects (δ) is negative and significant in daily parison of the values of this dependent variable on either
sales in Equation (1). As expected, this result confirms side of the threshold (Angrist & Pischke, 2008).
that decreases in price have overall favorable effects on Ho, Lim, Reza, and Xia (2017) provide a nice descrip-
daily sales, that is, lower prices will increase daily sales. tion of the RDD approach and they support RDD as a
Furthermore, the estimated coefficient of the scarcity method to tackle endogeneity and make causal infer-
message effect (γ) indicates that this message's disclosure ences when performing empirical research in
has a negative and significant effect on daily sales. Based OM. Recently, a few scholars have used RDD to address
on the estimated γ coefficient in Equation (1), γ = −.158 research questions in OM as well. For instance, Ander-
and p < .01, daily sales are lower by 15.80% “after” BT son, Dobkin, and Gross (2012) exploited a sharp change
posted the scarcity message than “before.” Hence, these in insurance coverage rates that results from young
results support Hypothesis 2. adults “aging out” of their parents' insurance plans and
used RDD to estimate the effect of insurance coverage on
the utilization of emergency and inpatient health care
4.2 | RDD modeling approach services. Following Anderson et al. (2012), we use a para-
metric linear spline estimator for our RDD analysis. This
Because BT posts the inventory message (“5 or less left in approach is preferable over nonparametric methods,
stock”) when inventory for an SKU drops below 6 units, which are sensitive to bandwidth selection and require a
SMid can be correlated with unobserved demand shocks, large number of observations. Additional evaluations of
which are included in the random shock variable εid. This our RDD analysis are included in Appendix C.
may generate endogeneity biases in our model estima- Our RDD analysis is based on the following model
tion. The demand shocks resulting in the correlation specification as stated in Equation (2).
between SMid and εid can be decomposed into (a) time- X
invariant shocks (e.g., general popularity of an SKU, lnðSalesid + 1Þ = αi + β I ðd∈mÞ + γSMid
m m
which does not vary over time) and (b) time-varying ð2Þ
+ δlnðPid Þ + θðInvid −τÞ
shocks (e.g., temporal popularity of an SKU due to sea-
+ ρðInvid −τÞSMid + εid ,
sonal needs or censoring effects caused by variations in
inventory levels over time). Time-invariant shocks can be
handled by using a “fixed effect” approach. Specifically, where Invid denotes the inventory level for SKU
this approach incorporates the time-invariant shocks into i observed on day d and τ represents the prespecified
αi and estimates αi by treating them as model parameters. threshold value (i.e., 5 units). Note that SMid equals 1 if
Note that we use a fixed effect approach in our model Invid ≤ τ or zero; otherwise.
estimations and thus we circumvent this problem. A An advantage of using the RDD analysis is that we
more challenging issue is the potential correlation can disentangle the demand effect attributed to SMid
between time-varying shocks and SMid. We tackle this from time-varying effects between Invid and Salesid
issue by using a RDD approach. induced by unobserved confounders, including temporal
RDD is a quasi-experimental design approach that variations in SKUs' demand due to consumers' seasonal
allows for the estimation of the causal effects of the inter- needs. In this case, while the parameter γ in Equation (2)
vention (the posting of a scarcity message in our case) measures the demand effect on Salesid by the disclosure
using the discrete nature of the threshold of the interven- of the scarcity message, the parameter θ captures the
tion. RDD allows for an estimation of the causal effect time-varying effects between Invid and Salesid, induced
when the treatment is assigned only at or below by these unobserved confounders. Finally, note that
(or above) a cutoff value of a known variable. The litera- Equation (2) allows SMid's causal effect on Salesid to vary
ture refers to this variable as the “forcing variable.” In our with respect to Invid by including an interaction term
case, this variable corresponds to the inventory level and between (Invid – τ) and SMid. The value of the ρ coeffi-
its cutoff value is 5 units, the threshold BT used to dis- cient estimated for this interaction term will capture the
close its scarcity message. Since BT kept this threshold magnitude of this variation, which can be attributed to
constant for all SKUs during our entire period of analysis, censoring effects caused by changes in inventory level
the value of our key predictor, SMid, changes abruptly at once scarcity messages are disclosed.
the threshold independently of time and SKU. Thus, Panel A in Table 3 reports the estimation results of
time-varying shocks lying closely on either side of the Equation (2). The statistically significant value estimated
threshold are comparable independently of the time and for the SMid coefficient (γ) indicates that the disclosure of
the SKU. By using RDD, we exploit this condition to the scarcity message decreases daily sales for an SKU by
PARK ET AL. 9

TABLE 3 Estimation results using regression discontinuity design

Panel A Panel B

Dependent var.: ln(Sales+1) Dependent var.: ln(Sales+1)

Parameter Estimate SE t-val. Estimate SE t-val.


Price for sales (δ) −0.032** 0.014 −2.284 — — —
Scarcity message (γ) −0.176*** 0.016 −11.208 −0.176*** 0.016 −11.174
θ 0.000 0.000 −1.471 0.000 0.000 −1.448
ρ −0.006 0.005 −1.057 −0.006 0.005 −1.070
SKU fixed effect (αi) Yes Yes
Month fixed effect (βm) Yes Yes
F statistic 22.300*** 23.070***
R-square 0.149 0.149
Adj. R-square 0.142 0.142
Number of observations 27,044 27,044
Panel C Panel D

Dependent car.: Sales Dependent car.: Sales

Parameter Estimate SE t-val. Estimate SE t-val.


Price for sales (δ) −0.247** 0.120 −2.051 — — —
Scarcity message (γ) −0.513*** 0.137 −3.753 −0.509*** 0.137 −3.722
θ −0.001 0.001 −1.519 −0.001 0.001 −1.499
ρ 0.025 0.045 0.558 0.025 0.045 0.547
SKU fixed effect (αi) Yes Yes
Month fixed effect (βm) Yes Yes
F statistic 7.978*** 7.995***
R-square 0.057 0.057
Adj. R-square 0.050 0.050
Number of observations 27,044 27,044

Note: Clustered std. errors are used.


**Significance at 5%, ***significance at 1%.

an average of 17.60%. Overall, the estimated value of this add robustness to our findings using alternative specifi-
coefficient echoes our findings using Equation (1), cations within the RDD approach. First, we drop price
reported in Table 2, and provides further support for (Pid) from the model (in Panel B). We then use in Panel
Hypothesis 2. In addition, the estimated coefficient of C an alternative dependent variable's specification,
price effect is significantly negative (δ= −0.030). This based on a level (i.e., direct measurement) variable
result is consistent with those in Section 4.1. They suggest instead of log-transformed one. Finally, in Panel D, we
that a 1% decrease in price leads to a 3.20% increase in use the same alternative dependent variable specifica-
daily sales. Therefore, daily sales are highly sensitive to tion as in Panel C and the same set of regressors in
variations in product prices. Panel B. The results from these alternative specifica-
Note also that the results we obtained are consistent tions are consistent with those in Panel A and with
with those obtained using the same specification and those in Table 2.
RDD modeling form but excluding all SKUs with In sum, the consistency in the results obtained across
extended OOS observations to account for biases due to the different RDD specifications in Table 3 as well as in
potentially unobserved heterogeneity in service levels the results in Table 2 provides support for Hypothesis
across the products (please refer to Appendix D). Fur- 2. These results point to significantly negative effects by
thermore, the results in Panels B, C, and D in Table 3 the disclosure of scarcity messages on daily sales.
10 PARK ET AL.

4.3 | Heterogeneity effects of scarcity T A B L E 4 Estimation results using regression discontinuity


message disclosures design interaction models

Dependent var.: ln(Sales+1)


We conclude our analysis with an evaluation of varia-
tions in demand effects induced by the disclosure of scar- Parameters Estimate SE t-val.
city messages across products based on their inherent Price for sales (δ) −0.029** 0.014 −2.065
daily sales (as a function of the amount of units sold per Scarcity message
day—high versus low quantities). For this analysis, we γ0 −0.031 0.021 −1.498
chose to use the same RDD discussed in Section 4.2 to
γS −0.159*** 0.015 −10.513
account for potential correlations between SMid and
θ −0.0003*** 0.0001 −3.193
unobserved demand shocks included in εid and to be able
to disentangle and estimate the demand effect. We pre- ρ −0.011** 0.005 −2.009
sent the specifications for this design in Equation (3) SKU fixed effect (αi) Yes
followed by the estimation results. Month fixed effect (βm) Yes
Equation (3) incorporates the scarcity message effect F statistic 23.512***
varying with SKU average daily sales (ASi):
R-square 0.152
X Adj. R-square 0.145
lnðSalesid + 1Þ = αi + β I ðd∈mÞ + ðγ 0
m m
+ γ S lnðASi ÞÞSMid
Number of observations 27,044
+ δlnðPid Þ + θðInvid − τÞ
+ ρ ðInvid − τÞSMid + εid , Note: Clustered standard errors are used.
**Significance at 5%, ***significance at 1%.
ð3Þ

where ASi denotes the average daily sales of SKU i. ASi the disclosure of scarcity messages will become more
represents, on average, the daily sales of SKU i (Among all acute. According to the 0.159 value estimated for the γ s
SKUs, the average daily sales is 2.69 units and the standard coefficient, a 1% increase in average daily sales expands by
deviation is 1.88). In Equation (3), we allow the effect of 15.90% the negative effect caused by the disclosure of scar-
scarcity message to vary with the logged value of ASi. The city messages on sales.
results we present in this section are based on this specifi- Finally, we consider the indirect effect of inventory
cation. However, note that we also verified that these levels associated with the impact of scarcity messages on
results were consistent with those obtained using similar daily sales. This indirect effect is evidenced when inven-
specifications to that in Equation (1) and can provide those tory is below τ so that (Invid − τ) < 0 and SMid = 1 in
results upon request. Moreover, in Appendix E, we present Equation (3). In this case, because the estimated θ and
supplementary results obtained from a robustness test ρ coefficients are −0.0003 and −0.011, the term θ 
using a specification with categorical values (high = 1 and (Invid − τ) + ρ  (Invid − τ) is positive. Hence daily sales
low = 0) for ln(ASi) based on median splits. These results increase for SKUs at increasingly lower inventory levels.
are consistent with those below. However, this increase is offset by the direct impact of
Table 4 reports the estimation results of Equation (3). scarcity messages on daily sales through γ S and γ 0, as dis-
The value estimated for the price coefficient (δ= −.029, cussed above, since γ S and γ 0 are much higher than θ and
p value <.01) is consistent with those estimated in Sections ρ. Therefore, the overall impact of scarcity messages is
4.1 and 4.2. Hence, we can conclude that daily sales tend negative: these messages' disclosure can cause daily sales
to increase with drops in prices: a 1% decrease in price can to decrease in general.
lead to an increase of almost 3% in daily sales. The esti-
mated values for the γ 0 and γ s coefficients indicate that the
demand effect induced by scarcity disclosures has a nega- 5 | S E N S I T I V I T Y AN A L Y S I S
tive effect on SKUs' daily sales contingent on the SKUs'
average number of units sold per day. As average daily The results in Table 4 provide evidence of a significant
sales increase, the demand effect will become increasingly SKU-wide heterogeneity in the demand effect induced by
more negative. For instance, if an SKU's average daily the disclosure of scarcity messages. In this section, we
sales is 2.69 units, its daily sales will decrease by 15.73% exploit this heterogeneity to conduct a sensitivity analysis
(=ln(ASi) • γ S) = ln(2.69) * (−0.159)) due to the demand regarding our results' support for Hypothesis 2 and iden-
effect induced by the disclosure of scarcity messages. As tify boundary conditions for the demand effect across
SKUs' average daily sales expand, the negative effect by SKUs' average daily sales (ASi).
PARK ET AL. 11

From the results in Table 4, we determined that all ratio will rise for SKUs with ASi values increasingly
SKUs with ASi values greater than 1.30 units/day will above their sample average, it will only match the ratio
experience decreases in daily sales due to the disclosure estimated for price elasticity in daily sales (−2.90) at very
of scarcity messages. In total, 91% of SKUs in our sample high levels of ASi (62.50). Thus, based on the estimated
have ASi values that exceed this lower bound. To obtain price elasticity for daily sales, price discounts are rela-
this benchmark, we let ln(Salesid + 1) = 0 and solve for tively more effective at offsetting the negative effect on
ASi in Equation (3), while controlling for SKU and time daily sales induced by scarcity messages' disclosure than
fixed effects. Note that as part of this estimation, we set reductions in ASi across a wide range of SKUs are.
to zero the γ 0 coefficient in Equation (3), estimated to be
nonstatistically different from zero in Table 4. We also
assumed Pid = $19.99 (the lowest price in the panel) and 6 | CONCLUSION
Invid = 1 (and thus Invid – τ = −4 and SMid = 1).
The value chosen for Invid generates conservative esti- In this article, we have examined the impact that posting
mates of ASi's lower-bound benchmark necessary for scarcity messages online has on daily sales. To that end,
SKUs to be susceptible to the negative effects described we focused on an online retailer selling durable goods to
in Hypothesis 2. Higher values of Invid will decrease this consumers over multiple inventory replenishment cycles,
lower bound, thereby increasing the percentage of sam- without restrictions as to the amount of time these prod-
pled SKUs susceptible to the effects in Hypothesis 2. On ucts are available for sale. By focusing on this particular
the other hand, higher Pid values will increase the ASi setting, we were able to obtain insights that are generaliz-
lower-bound value. Using the maximum value for Pid in able to markets in which retailers sell consumer goods for
the sample ($3,075) increases the ASi lower bound value which demand will vary depending on consumers' needs.
to 3.25 units. Therefore, low-value SKUs are susceptible Our results showed that the disclosure of inventory
to decreases in daily sales due to the disclosure of scarcity upper bound information via these scarcity messages
messages when daily sales are at least 1.30 units while decreases SKUs' daily sales quantities. On average, the dis-
high-value SKUs are susceptible to these effects when closure of scarcity messages decreases these amounts by
their daily sales are above 3.25 units. 17.60%, after controlling for SKU fixed effects. However,
One additional consideration is that, regardless of the when we partialed out moderation effects by SKUs' average
value of the SKUs, discounts can increase daily sales. daily sales quantities, we found that this effect is more
According to our results, a reduction of 1% in SKU price pronounced for items that are sold in greater quantities
will increase daily sales by almost 3%, on average (please per day. Furthermore, our analysis revealed that, price
refer to the value estimated for the price coefficient, discounts are particularly effective in offsetting the
δ = .029, in Table 4). Because this effect is independent of negative effect on daily sales induced by scarcity messages'
the disclosure of scarcity messages and of the SKUs' aver- disclosure.
age daily sales, it carries greater weight in increasing Together, these findings provide boundary conditions
daily sales for faster moving SKUs (with ASi greater than to the effects on sales caused by the disclosure of inven-
the minimum of 1.23 units/day), which are the items in tory information as well as price discounts to consumers
the assortment exposed to the most severe negative when selling durable goods online. It is evident from
effects by scarcity disclosure on daily sales. these conditions that sales reactions to stimuli conveyed
Moreover, the price effect estimated in our analysis sug- by the disclosure of scarcity messages are nontrivial and
gests that daily sales are highly  price elastic—daily
 sales' that these reactions are contingent on SKUs' daily sale
price elasticity equals −2.90 −2:90 = −0:01 0:029
, on average. amounts and prices. An indiscriminate implementation
This elasticity exceeds that observed for the effects caused of scarcity disclosure strategies across products will lead
by reductions of ASi, on the link between scarcity disclo- to suboptimal results. To be effective in increasing daily
sure and daily sales—as estimated based on the γ s coeffi- sales, these strategies must focus on SKUs that are
cient for ln(AS) • SM in Table 4 (γ s = −.159). For an SKU marketed by retailers at highly competitive prices as well
with an average ASi, (2.69 units), a unit decrease in ASi as on items that sell in small daily amounts. These find-
corresponds to a decrease of 37.17%. In turn, according to ings offer clear practical implications for retailers when
the value estimated for γ s, this reduction will ameliorate considering how to stimulate sales through a combina-
the adverse
 impact by scarcity  disclosure on daily sales by tion of SKU scarcity signals and prices.
4.80% eðγ S *lnð2:69Þ − γS *lnð1:69Þ Þ . When we compare this Our results also provide several implications for
effect against the percentage decrease in ASi responsible researchers who are interested in expanding on our
for it (37.17%), we see that it only amounts to a fraction
 4:80 study. Additional research may focus on product catego-
− 37:17 = −0:13 of the decrease in ASi. Although this ries different from those considered in our analysis.
12 PARK ET AL.

Studies involving home improvement products such as ENDN OTE S


those sold by retailers like Lowe's or Home Depot may 1
In the empirical analyses, we use log-transformed variables of
offer additional insights into the role of scarcity messages sales and price. When we use these transformed variables, the cor-
in shaping demand for SKUs typically purchased by con- relation coefficients show marginal changes and the highest cor-
sumers in larger sets. It is possible that the adverse relation coefficient is still −0.370.
2
impact by scarcity messages on daily sales we observed Specifically, we considered the following alternative model speci-
will be more prominent in these settings. fications for the robustness checks: (1) a linear model with an
unlogged dependent variable, (2) a Poisson regression model,
Another opportunity for future research is the evalua-
(3) a Negative binomial regression model, and (4) a Zero-inflated
tion of alternative inventory thresholds for the disclosure Negative binomial regression model.
of scarcity messages. This would provide retailers with an
understanding of the optimal threshold policy they
should use for the disclosure of scarcity messages. RE FER EN CES
Thresholds may vary as a function of SKUs' average daily Anderson, M., Dobkin, C., & Gross, T. (2012). The effect of health
sales and prices. An additional research avenue may insurance coverage on the use of medical services. American
involve an evaluation of the impact of price competition Economic Journal: Economic Policy, 4(1), 1–27.
among retailers on the effectiveness of scarcity disclosure Angrist, J. D., & Pischke, J. S. (2008). Mostly harmless econometrics: An
in influencing demand across SKUs. This is a particularly empiricist's companion. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Aviv, Y., Tang, C. S., & Yin, R. (2009). Mitigating the adverse
relevant issue for retailers operating online under slim
impact of strategic waiting in dynamic pricing settings: A study
price margins. It is possible that demand for SKUs under of two sales mechanisms. In S. Netessine & C. S. Tang (Eds.),
these conditions may not be as sensitive to the disclosure Consumer-driven demand and operations management models
of scarcity messages. Moreover, future work may con- (pp. 353–370). New York, NY: Springer.
sider the availability of SKUs in online and offline chan- Bakos, J. Y. (1997). Reducing buyer search costs: Implications for
nels operated by the same retailer as part of the electronic marketplaces. Management Science, 43(12), 1676–1692.
evaluation of scarcity disclosure strategies online. Our Balakrishnan, A., Pangburn, M. S., & Stavrulaki, E. (2004). Stack
them high and let them fly': Lot-sizing policies when invento-
research focused on a sample of SKUs offered only
ries stimulate demand. Management Science, 50(5), 630–644.
online. However, it is possible that expanding the sample
Balakrishnan, A., Pangburn, M. S., & Stavrulaki, E. (2008). Integrating
to include SKUs offered online as well as at brick and the promotional and service roles of retail inventories. Manufactur-
mortar stores will show variations in the effect of scarcity ing and Service Operations Management, 10(2), 218–235.
messages on demand. Boada-Collado, P., & Martinez-de-Albeniz, V. (2019). Estimating
Finally, future research may consider evaluating the and optimizing the impact of inventory on consumer choices in
effect of scarcity disclosure on SKUs' purchasing rates as a fashion retail setting. Manufacturing and Service Operations
measured by the time intervals between SKU purchases Management (Forthcoming). https://doi.org/10.1287/msom.
2018.0764
before versus after disclosure. Although our analysis of
Boyer, K. K., Hallowell, R., & Roth, A. V. (2002). E-services: Operating
the effect of scarcity disclosure on daily sales provides an strategy—A case study and a method for analyzing operational
account of this effect on overall demand, this proposed benefits. Journal of Operations Management, 20(2), 175–188.
research extension may show how variations in inter- Brock, T. C. (1968). Implications of commodity theory for value
purchase time contribute to the changes we observed in change. In A. G. Greenwald, T. C. Brock, & T. M. Ostrom
daily sales. Evaluating the effect of scarcity disclosure on (Eds.), Psychological foundations of attitudes (pp. 243–275).
purchasing rates requires a very high data collection fre- New York, NY: Academic Press.
quency to guarantee that no multiple purchases are mis- Brynjolfsson, E., & Smith, M. D. (2000). Frictionless commerce? A
comparison of internet and conventional retailers. Management
takenly bundled in the same record (such that two
Science, 46(4), 563–585.
purchases of one unit each are not mistaken by a single Cachon, G. P., Gallino, S., & Olivares, M. (2018). Does
purchase of 2 units, for example). The data collection fre- adding inventory increase sales? Evidence of a scarcity effect
quency in our study is not high enough to offer this guar- in U.S. automobile dealerships. Management Science, 65(4),
antee. We attempted to overcome this limitation by using 1469–1485.
simulated interpurchase times in instances when errone- Chevalier, J., & Goolsbee, A. (2003). Measuring prices and price
ous bundling of purchases was a possibility. The results competition online: Amazon.com and BarnesandNoble.com.
Quantitative Marketing and Economics, 1(2), 203–222.
are available in a prior version of this article at https://
Cui, R., & Shin, H. (2017). Sharing aggregate inventory information
doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3447624.
with customers: Strategic cross-selling and shortage reduction.
Management Science, 64(1), 381–400.
ORCID Cui, R., Zhang, D., & Bassamboo, A. (2018). Learning from inven-
Elliot Rabinovich https://orcid.org/0000-0001-7251- tory availability information: Evidence from field experiments
9571 on Amazon. Management Science, 65(3), 1216–1235.
PARK ET AL. 13

DeGraba, P. (1995). Buying frenzies and seller-induced excess affect online retail sales. SSRN Electronic Journal. https://doi.
demand. The Rand Journal of Economics, 26(2), 331–342. org/10.2139/ssrn.2872938
Ferreira, K., Lee, B. H., & Simchi-Levi, D. (2016). Analytics for an Urban, T. L. (2005). Inventory models with inventory-level-
online retailer: Demand forecasting and price optimization. dependent demand: A comprehensive review and unifying the-
Manufacturing & Service Operations Management, 18(1), 69–88. ory. European Journal of Operational Research, 162(3), 792–804.
Fisher, M., Gallino, S., & Li, J. (2017). Competition-based dynamic Wagner, L., Calvo, E., & Cui, R. (2018). The value of disclosing
pricing in online retailing: A methodology validated with field product availability on retail platforms. SSRN Electronic Jour-
experiments. Management Science, 64(6), 2496–2514. nal. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3195047
Greene, W. H. (2003). Econometric analysis. Pearson Education, Wolfe, H. B. (1968). A model for control of style merchandise.
Upper Saddle River, NJ. Industrial Management Review, 9(2), 69–82.
Ho, T.-H., Lim, N., Reza, S., & Xia, X. (2017). OM forum: Causal Wooldridge, J. M. (2010). Econometric analysis of cross section and
inference models in operations management. Manufacturing & panel data (2nd ed.). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Service Operations Management, 19(4), 509–525. Zhang, J., Fang, X., & Sheng, O. (2006). Online consumer search
Jerath, K., Netessine, S., & Veeraraghavan, S. K. (2010). Revenue depth: Theories and new findings. Journal of Management
management with strategic customers: Last minute selling and Information Systems, 23(3), 71–95.
opaque selling. Management Science, 56(3), 430–448.
Koschat, M. A. (2008). Store inventory can affect demand: Empiri-
cal evidence from magazine re-tailing. Journal of Retailing, 84
How to cite this article: Park S, Rabinovich E,
(2), 165–179.
Tang CS, Yin R. The impact of disclosing
Lee, D. S., & Lemieux, T. (2010). Regression discontinuity designs
in economics. Journal of Economic Literature, 48(2), 281–355. inventory-scarcity messages on sales in online
Lynn, M. (1991). Scarcity effects on value: A quantitative review of retailing. J Oper Manag. 2020;1–19. https://doi.org/
the commodity theory literature. Psychology & Marketing, 8(1), 10.1002/joom.1082
43–57.
Netessine, S., & Tang, C. S. (2009). Consumer-driven demand
and operations management models: A systematic study of
information-technology-enabled sales mechanisms. New York,
NY: Springer. APPENDIX A: METHODOLOGICAL
Olson, J. R., Belohlav, J. A., & Boyer, K. K. (2005). Operational, eco- VAL I DI TY
nomic and mission elements in not-for-profit organizations:
The case of the Chicago Symphony Orchestra. Journal of Oper-
ations Management, 23(2), 125–142.
To measure sales for each SKU, we estimated the daily
Olson, J. R., & Boyer, K. K. (2003). Factors influencing the utiliza- magnitudes (in units) associated with the inventory
tion of Internet purchasing in small organizations. Journal of reductions that Cartbound registered in the source code
Operations Management, 21(2), 225–245. on BT's SKU pages. To ensure the validity of this method-
Rabinovich, E., Sinha, R., & Laseter, T. (2011). Unlimited shelf ology in producing reliable sales measures, we first veri-
space in Internet supply chains: Treasure trove or wasteland? fied that BT's inventory information in its source code
Journal of Operations Management, 29(4), 305–317.
reflected the number of units of SKUs consumers pur-
Rao, S., Griffis, S. E., & Goldsby, T. J. (2011). Failure to deliver?
chased in real time. Our verification is based on the fol-
Linking online order fulfillment glitches with future purchase
behavior. Journal of Operations Management, 29(7–8), 692–703. lowing experiment.
Rao, S., Rabinovich, E., & Raju, D. (2014). The role of physical dis- First, we sampled five SKUs across the categories in
tribution services as determinants of product returns in Inter- Figure 2. We then visited the webpage of each SKU and
net retailing. Journal of Operations Management, 32(6), pretended to purchase the SKU by increasing the pur-
295–312. chase quantity in the shopping cart one unit at a time
Rosenzweig, E. D., Laseter, T. M., & Roth, A. V. (2011). Through until receiving notice that the amount of units in the cart
the service operations strategy looking glass: Influence of
exceeded the inventory level in BT's source code. We
industrial sector, ownership, and service offerings on B2B e-
marketplace failures. Journal of Operations Management, 29
obtained the same outcome after repeating this routine
(1–2), 33–48. for all SKUs in the sample. That is, once the total number
Smith, M. D., & Brynjolfsson, E. (2001). Customer decision making of units added to the cart exceeded the inventory level in
at an Internet shopbot: Brand still matters. Journal of Industrial the source code for each SKU, we received immediate
Economics, 49(4), 541–558. notification that BT did not have enough inventory to ful-
Sodero, A., & Rabinovich, E. (2017). Demand and revenue manage- fill the order. Based on this evidence, we infer that
ment of deteriorating inventory on the Internet: An empirical consumers' purchasing decisions and BT's inventory
study of flash sales markets. Journal of Business Logistics, 38(3),
information in its source code are linked in real time.
170–183.
Sodero, A., Rabinovich, E., Aydinliyim, T., & Pangburn, M. S.
Table A1 lists these SKUs' URLs and the inventory levels
(2017). An empirical analysis of how inventory levels and prices in the source code.
14 PARK ET AL.

TABLE A1 SKUs sampled to test for inventory updates following consumers' purchasing decisions

Inventory
SKU URL level
https://www.bonton.com/home/kitchen/small-appliances/nuwave-6qt-digital-air-fryer-black-652185370012/ 5 units
https://www.bonton.com/bed-bath/abyss-habidecor-medium-reversible-cotton-bath-rug-30309735 3 units
900000/
https://www.bonton.com/bed-bath/waterworks-sahara-cotton-linen-bath-rug-30309030560000/ 2 units
https://www.bonton.com/travel-luggage/suitcases/victorinox-swiss-army-avolve-20in-expandable-wheeled- 2 units
case-10409892920000/
https://www.bonton.com/home/storage-organization/baskets-hampers/neatnix-set-of-4-boxes-3010435728/ 4 units

In addition, we evaluated whether the frequency that did not differ statistically. While the daily average cra-
Cartbound used to crawl the source code for each SKU wling count among the group of 174 SKUs was 3.05,
page was sufficiently high to ensure that the inventory CI0.99 = [2.84, 3.27], the average among the other 25 SKUs
reductions registered were contemporaneous to the num- was 2.45, CI0.99 = [1.95, 2.94].
ber of units sold for the SKUs every day. To monitor the Although all available evidence suggests that the
SKU pages, Cartbound used Web crawlers that were avail- inventory reductions we registered for each SKU in the
able continuously and queried BT's SKU pages based on sample are contemporaneous to the number of units sold
the requests they received from Cartbound's users. From every day, we conducted two additional tests on the
these queries, we registered a daily average of 2.94 page robustness of the results reported in the article. We
crawls per SKU (SD = 1.33). Moreover, we registered an describe these tests as follows.
average of at least one page crawl per day for 99.14% of In the first test, we removed from the sample those
the intervals between consecutive changes in inventory or 25 SKUs that registered no inventory drops in intervals
price during our observation period. The number of units lower than or equal to 1 day and then re-estimated the
sold during these intervals represents 99.28% of the total article's RDD model. The goal of the test is to determine
in the entire observation period. Finally, we verified whether the removal of these items from the sample
whether the lengths of the intervals and the magnitude in uncovers biases in the article's estimation of the demand
inventory drops registered during the intervals were asso- effect by the disclosure of scarcity messages on daily sales.
ciated with low page crawl rates. To that end, we esti- The results obtained from the test failed to find statistical
mated the correlation coefficients between the average evidence of these biases. Specifically, the values of the γ
number of daily page crawls during each interval and the coefficient estimated in the article (as shown in Table 3) as
length and magnitude in the inventory drop during the well as in this robustness test under a logged or an
interval. Since the values we obtained for these coeffi- unlogged dependent variable (γ= −.195, p value < .01, γ=
cients were near zero (−0.048 and −0.046, respectively), −.576, p value < .01 in Table A2, Panel A) are all negative
low crawl rates do not appear to be linked significantly to and statistically different from zero, suggesting that daily
long interval durations or large inventory drops. sales decrease postscarcity disclosure.
Based on this crawling frequency, we were able to In the second test, we retained all inventory drop
record inventory reductions in intervals that were highly observations for all SKUs. However, we treated every
consistent with the daily unit of measurement we chose observation that led to a decrease in inventory from levels
for (daily) sales. This enabled us to register inventory above the scarcity message's disclosure threshold (of
drops in time intervals as low as 1 day or less for 174 out 5 units) to levels below the threshold as if the observation
of 199 SKUs in our sample. For the remaining 25 SKUs, had occurred “entirely after” scarcity disclosure. The goal
we registered inventory drops in intervals greater than or is to simulate a “shift” in the allocation of daily sales
equal to 2 days. The reason we did not register inventory from predisclosure to postdisclosure in order to challenge
drops at shorter time intervals for these 25 SKUs is likely directly the RDD estimation results in the article pointing
due to the fact that these SKUs are “slow moving items” to the existence of lower daily sales postdisclosure.
relative to other 174 SKUs in the sample: the average Table A2 (Panel B) presents the results from this test
daily sales for these 25 SKUs was 35% lower than the based on our simulated data. They show that the value
other 174 SKUs. Moreover, this difference in daily sales obtained for γ is negative and statistically different from
rates between the two groups of SKUs existed despite the zero when we use either logged or unlogged dependent
fact that the crawling frequency across both groups variables, which is consistent with the values obtained
PARK ET AL. 15

TABLE A2 Robustness checks for


Dependent var.: ln(Sales+1) Dependent var.: Sales
scarcity disclosure's demand effect on Panel A
daily sales Parameter Estimate SE t-val. Estimate SE t-val.
Ln(Price) (δ) −0.028* 0.014 −1.89 −0.190 0.128 −1.48
Scarcity message (γ) −0.195*** 0.017 −11.29 −0.576*** 0.147 −3.89
θ 0.000 0.000 −0.49 −0.001 0.000 −1.62
ρ −0.010* 0.005 −1.74 0.020 0.050 0.40
SKU fixed effect (αi) Yes Yes
Month fixed effect (βm) Yes Yes
F statistic 19.75*** 6.24***
R-square 0.133 0.046
Adj. R-square 0.126 0.038
Number of observations 23,553 23,553
Dependent var.: In(Sales + 1) Dependent var.: Sales
Panel B
Parameter Estimate SE t-val. Estimate SE t-val.
Ln(Price) (δ) −0.032** 0.014 −2.310 −0.248** 0.120 −2.06
Scarcity message (γ) −0.164*** 0.013 −13.05 −0.464*** 0.109 −4.26
θ −0.0002** 0.0001 −2.035 −0.001 0.001 −1.64
ρ −0.001 0.004 0.387 0.047 0.033 1.43
SKU fixed effect (αi) Yes Yes
Month fixed effect (βm) Yes Yes
F statistic 23.086*** 7.986***
R-square 0.149 0.057
Adj. R-square 0.143 0.050
Number of observations 27,044 27,044

Note: Clustered std. errors are used.


*Significance at 10%, **significance at 5%, ***significance at 1%.

for γ in the article (Table 3). Given that the results from Zero-Inflated Negative Binomial regression model. A
these two tests continue to support those presented in the Poisson model is a generalized linear model of regression
article, we can conclude that our findings are robust: analysis (Greene, 2003)—as shown in Equation (B1):
daily sales postscarcity disclosure are lower than daily
sales predisclosure. PrðSalesid = sjX id Þ = Poisson
 X  ðB1Þ
α+ m
β m  I ð d∈m Þ + γ  SM id + δ  ln ð P id Þ
A P P EN D I X B : R ES U L T S F R O M
AL TE RN AT IVE MO D EL IN G where s denotes the number of units sold for SKU i on
S PE CIF ICAT I O NS day d (count). Equation (B1) assumes that daily sales are
based on a Poisson random variable with a rate associ-
We first evaluate the robustness of the results in ated with different covariates in Xid (e.g., SMid, Pid). Thus,
Section 4 by estimating the same model in Equation (1) Equation (B1) states that the probability of observing s
using different specifications for the dependent variable, follows the Poisson probability density function associ-
daily sales, instead of using a log transformation. As the ated with those covariates in Xid (e.g., SMid, Pid). Here,
estimation results reported in Table B1 show, we find the estimated values exp(γ) and exp(δ) represent the
consistent results regarding the price and the scarcity impact of scarcity messages and price on expected daily
message effects, adding validity to our findings. sales, respectively.
Furthermore, we modeled daily sales as a count vari- The Poisson regression model has been criticized for
able using (1) a Poisson, (2) a Negative Binomial, and (3) a its restrictive property that the conditional variance
16 PARK ET AL.

TABLE B1 Robustness check using alternative model and ϑ is a vector of parameters. Since Φ(Zid  ϑ) should be
specification of Equation (1) a valid probability bounded between zero and one, we
specify a standard normal distribution function for Φ()
Fixed effect dummy least square
and use the same covariates as Xid for Zid.
Dependent var.: Sales We report maximum-likelihood estimation results
Parameter Estimate SE t-val. for the three count data models specified in Equa-
tions (B1)–(B3) in Table B2. From the Poisson regression
Price for sales (δ) −0.245** 0.120 −2.034
results, we find that both the effect of price and the effect
Scarcity message (γ) −0.531*** 0.078 −6.836
of scarcity message disclosure on daily sales are negative
SKU fixed effect (αi) Yes and statistically significant. This suggests that, relative to
Month Fixed Effect (βm) Yes baseline conditions with no scarcity messages (i.e., exp
F statistic 8.04*** (γ SMid) = exp (0) = 1), the disclosure of these messages
R-squared 0.06 can reduce expected daily sales shrink by 68% (exp
(γ SMid) = exp (−1.151) = 0.32). Similarly, when log-
Adj. R-squared 0.05
price (ln(Pid)) increases by a unit, daily sales decrease by
Number of observations 27,044
16% (exp (δ) = exp (−0.175) = 0.84).
Note: Clustered standard errors are used. From the negative binomial regression results, we find
**Significance at 5%, ***significance at 1%.
that both the effect of price and the effect of scarcity mes-
sage disclosure on daily sales are negative and statistically
equals the conditional mean (i.e., overdispersion prob- significant. This suggests that, relative to baseline condi-
lem). The negative binomial model tackles this problem tions with no scarcity messages, the disclosure of these
by introducing an unobserved heterogeneity term which messages can reduce expected daily sales by 69% (exp (γ
follows a gamma distribution. We estimate the following SMid) = exp (−1.173) = 0.31). Similarly, when log-price (ln
negative binomial regression model: (Pid)) increases by a unit, daily sales decrease by 11% (exp
(δ) = exp (−0.112) = 0.89). The gamma distribution param-
PrðSalesid 
= sjX id ,κ id Þ = κid eter η is highly significant and positive (8.541, p value
X 
 Poisson α + β  I ð d∈m Þ + γ  SM id + δ  ln ð P id Þ <.001). Note that when η converges to zero, the negative
m m
binomial regression model becomes a Poisson regression
ðB2Þ model. We also observe that, according to all model fit
  measures (log-likelihood, akaike's information criterion
where κid  gamma 1η , 1η distribution with E(κ id) = 1 (AIC), and bayesian information criterion (BIC)), the nega-
and V(κid) = η. Note κ id captures both cross-sectional and tive binomial model is preferable to the Poisson model. We
serial unobserved heterogeneity added to the Poisson can conclude that our daily sales data has substantial
regression part, adding flexibility to the model. unobserved heterogeneity and overdispersion and thus can
In addition to overdispersion and unobserved hetero- be better represented by a negative binomial model.
geneity, count data are often characterized by excess Next, from the zero-inflated negative binomial regres-
zeros. To additionally account for this, we consider the sion results, we find that both the effect of price and the
following zero-inflated negative binomial regression effect of scarcity message disclosure on daily sales are neg-
model: ative and statistically significant. When log-price (ln(Pid))

(
0 with probability ΦðZid  ϑÞ
Salesid  , ðB3Þ
Negative Binomial ðSalesid = sjXid Þ with probability 1 −ΦðZid  ϑÞ

where negative binomial(Salesid = s| Xid) represents the increases by a unit, daily sales decrease by 10% (exp
negative binomial regression model specified in Equa- (δ) = exp(−0.103) = 0.90). Relative to baseline conditions
tion (B3) and Φ(Zid  ϑ) is the zero inflation model which with no scarcity messages, the disclosure of these messages
captures the excessive zero observations. The zero inflation can reduce expected daily sales by 28% (exp(γ SMid) = exp
model is specified as a function of observed covariates Zid (−0.324) = 0.72). Compared to the two previous model
PARK ET AL. 17

TABLE B2 Robustness check using count data models

Negative binomial Zero-inflated negative


Poisson regression regression binomial regression

Dependent var.: Sales Dependent var.: Sales Dependent var.: Sales

Parameter Est. SE t-val. Est. SE t-val. Est. SE t-val.


Price (δ) −0.175*** 0.010 −18.30 −0.112*** 0.028 −3.90 −0.103*** 0.030 −3.50
Scarcity message (γ) −1.151*** 0.022 −52.30 −1.173*** 0.046 −25.50 −0.324*** 0.074 −4.40
Gamma dist. Parameter (η) 8.541*** 0.159 53.70 7.478*** 0.148 50.60
Zero-inflation model-constant (ϑ0) −6.955*** 0.142 −49.10
Zero-inflation model-Price (ϑ1) 0.048 0.050 1.00
Zero-inflation model-scarcity message (ϑ2) 6.853*** 0.142 48.40
Month fixed effect Yes Yes Yes
Log-likelihood −54,959 −24,533 −24,378
AIC 109,956 49,105 48,801
BIC 110,112 49,269 48,990
Number of observations 27,044 27,044 27,044

**Significance at 5%, ***significance at 1%.


Abbreviations: AIC, akaike's information criterion; BIC, bayesian information criterion.

results, this value looks rather small. This is because the


scarcity message can influence daily sales through another
route—the zero-inflation process. The coefficient of scar-
city message in the zero-inflation model (ϑ2) is highly sig-
nificant and positive, indicating that the probability of
observing zero versus the negative binomial process (which
has a mean greater than zero) significantly increases with
scarcity messages. More specifically, at the average log
price (=5.68), the estimated value of the zero inflation
model is 0.56 (= Φ(Zid  ϑ) = Φ(−6.955 + 0.047*5.68
+ 6.853)) when the scarcity message is on, while this num- F I G U R E C 1 Distribution of dependent variable [Color figure
ber shrinks to 0 when the message is off (0 = Φ(Zid  ϑ) = can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]
Φ(−6.955 + 0.047*5.68)). Again, our estimation result con-
firms that the scarcity message has a negative impact on on the right-hand side of the threshold. We tried first-,
daily sales through both a negative binomial process and a second-, and third-order polynomials and obtained rea-
zero inflation process. The gamma distribution parameter sonably consistent results from all three specifications.
η is highly significant and positive (7.478, p value <.001). Below, we provide all the details of the additional evalua-
We also observe that the zero-inflated negative binomial tions we performed to evaluate the rigor of this analysis.
model is the most preferred among three models in terms Figure C1 shows the distribution of our dependent
of three fit measures (log-likelihood, AIC, and BIC). variable (log daily sales) over inventory levels. We cannot
In summary, using three count data models, we re- visually detect any structural difference around the
examine the effects of scarcity messages on sales and reach threshold value of inventory level of 5 units from this fig-
the same conclusions as in Section 4. Specifically, we find ure. Lee and Lemieux (2010) point out that a graphical
that the disclosure of these messages decreases daily sales. presentation of an RDD should not be tilted toward
either finding an effect or finding no effect (pp. 283–284).
Discontinuity around the threshold can be masked if the
A P P EN D I X C : A D D I TI O N A L R D D effects of other control variables (e.g., price, SKU-specific
EVALUATIONS characteristics) are not properly accounted for. Using our
regression approach, we can properly control the effects
For the RDD analysis, we use the same polynomial for of other factors and then formally estimate the disconti-
observations on the left-hand side of the threshold and nuity around the threshold
18 PARK ET AL.

Next, we examine whether there exist abrupt changes (F-test) and find that the null hypothesis of no difference
near the threshold in the key control variable—log price. If cannot be rejected (F-value = 1.254, p value = .258). This
there exist such abrupt changes, the observed discontinuity finding implies that the observed discontinuity around the
in the dependent variable might be driven by the control threshold could be mainly due to the scarcity message and
variable and not by the scarcity message. Figure C2 shows not due to a discontinuity in the control variable, adding
the distribution of log price over inventory levels. We can validity to our results.
observe that the distribution of the control variable is com- Next, we investigate the robustness of our results to
parable over different levels of inventory near the threshold alternative specifications in RDD. Note that we use a para-
of 5 units. We formally test whether log price is different metric polynomial regression approach in RDD. Polyno-
across inventory levels using analysis of variance (ANOVA) mial regression has some advantages compared to
nonparametric local linear regression—appropriate when
9 sample size is relatively small and does not need to specify
ln(price) kernel and bandwidth parameter. However, researchers
8
7 have to determine the order of the polynomial function. In
6 the body of the article, we use the first-order polynomials
5 and estimate the model in Equation (2).
4 To show the robustness of our results to alternative
3 polynomial specifications, we estimate the following
2 second- and third-order polynomial regressions and
1 report the results in Table C1:
0
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 X
Inventory lnðSalesid + 1Þ = αi + β  I ðd∈mÞ + γ  SMid + δ
m m
 lnðPid Þ + θ1  ðInvid −τÞ +
F I G U R E C 2 Distribution of control variables. Note: Ends of
solid lines indicate maximum and minimum values. Boxes indicate
ρ1  ðInvid −τÞ  SMid + θ2  ðInvid −τÞ2 + ρ2  ðInvid −τÞ2
the 95% intervals
 SMid + εid ,
ðC1Þ

TABLE C1 Robustness check using alternative polynomial regression specifications

Second-order specification Third-order specification

Dependent var.: ln(Sales+1) Dependent var.: ln(Sales+1)

Parameter Estimate SE t-val. Estimate SE t-val.


Price for sales −0.031** 0.014 −2.208 −0.030** 0.014 −2.202
Scarcity message −0.006 0.020 −0.309 −0.118*** 0.023 −5.132
θ1 0.001** 0.000 3.125 0.001** 0.000 2.557
ρ1 0.235*** 0.020 11.638 −0.161*** 0.040 −4.036
θ2 0.000*** 0.000 −4.618 0.000** 0.000 −2.067
ρ2 0.054*** 0.004 12.367 −0.171*** 0.020 −8.535
θ3 — — — 0.000 0.000 0.941
ρ3 — — — −0.033*** 0.003 −11.489
SKU fixed effect Yes Yes
Month fixed effect Yes Yes
F statistic 23.75*** 24.27***
R-square 0.154 0.158
Adj. R-square 0.148 0.152
Number of observations 27,044 27,044

Note: Clustered standard errors are used.


*Significance at 10%, **significance at 5%, ***significance at 1%.
PARK ET AL. 19

sideration in our analyses. Moreover, we observed that


X
lnðSalesid + 1Þ = αi + β  I ðd∈mÞ + γ  SMid + δ
m m
most of these OOS observations involved only 20 SKUs
 lnðPid Þ + θ1  ðInvid −τÞ + for which the total days of OOS was above 10% of their
observation periods. It is possible that the reason most
OOS observations involved these SKUs is that they were
ρ1  ðInvid −τÞ  SMid + θ2  ðInvid − τÞ2 + ρ2  ðInvid −τÞ2
subject a priori to lower fill rate targets relative to those
 SMid + θ3  ðInvid −τÞ3
for the rest of the SKUs in the sample. Thus, because
demand measures may be “biased” by the inclusion of
+ ρ3  ðInvid − τÞ3  SMid + εid , ðC2Þ these SKUs, we constructed a new dataset that excluded
them from consideration and then used this dataset to re-
According to the results, the coefficient of scarcity estimate the RDD model in Equation (2). As we show in
message is negative in the second- and third-order speci- Table D1, the results we obtained from this revised
fication models. Moreover, although the polynomial dataset are consistent with those in Table 3. Thus, our
terms are highly correlated among each other by con- results are robust to noises or random shocks in demand
struction and this quickly increases the standard errors in due to potential differentials in fill rate policies across the
the estimation, the results show that the scarcity message SKUs in the sample.
effect is statistically significant (−0.118, p < .01) under
the third-order polynomial specification and that this
specification has the highest adj. R-square value of all A PP E N D IX E : R ES U L TS F R O M
specifications (adj. R-square = 0.152). A LT ER N A TI VE RD D SP E C IF I C A T IO N S F O R
HETEROGENEITY EFFECTS

A P P EN D I X D: R E S U L TS EX C L U D I N G S KU S We use HighAS as an indicator variable that takes a value


WI TH EXTE NDE D OOS O BS ER VAT IO NS of 1 if the average daily sales of an SKU i is greater than
the median of average daily sales of all sample products,
As we explained, there were 1,634 SKU-day observations or zero otherwise. Table E1 presents the results. These
with OOS (i.e., the available inventory was zero units). results are consistent to those in Table 4 (in Section 4).
Because consumers cannot purchase products during
these periods, we removed these observations from con- TABLE E1 Estimation results using RDD interaction models

Dependent var.: ln(Sales+1)


TABLE D1 Estimation results using RDD
Parameters Estimate SE t-val.
Dependent var.: ln(Sales+1) Price for sales (δ) −0.028** 0.014 −2.045
Parameter Estimate SE t-val. Scarcity message
Price for sales −0.067*** 0.015 −4.469 γ0 −0.109*** 0.017 −6.404
Scarcity message −0.155*** 0.017 −9.092 γS −0.189*** 0.018 −10.278
θ 0.000 0.000 −0.911 θ −0.0003** 0.0001 −3.124
ρ 0.009 0.006 1.623 ρ −0.010** 0.005 −2.004
SKU fixed effect Yes SKU fixed effect (αi) Yes
Month fixed effect Yes Month fixed effect (βm) Yes
F statistic 22.983*** F statistic 23.49***
R-square 0.154 R-square 0.152
Adj. R-square 0.147 Adj. R-square 0.146
Number of observations 23,439 Number of observations 27,044

Note: Clustered std. errors are used. Note: Clustered standard errors are used.
**Significance at 5%, ***significance at 1%. **Significance at 5%, ***significance at 1%.

You might also like