You are on page 1of 8

Available online at www.sciencedirect.

com

ScienceDirect
Procedia Economics and Finance 32 (2015) 200 – 207

Emerging Markets Quueries in Finance and Business

Behavioral biases of the invvestment decisions of Romanian


investorson the Buccharest Stock Exchange
Mihai Tomaa,*
Filip-M
a
The Bucharest University of Economic Studies, 6, Piaata Romana, 1stdistrict, Bucharest, postal code:010374, Romania

Abstract

Classical economics and the study of financial markets from f a normative point of view have their foundations laid in the
rationality of economic agents. The main hypothesis reevolves around decision making under rationality. Any(financial)
decision is taken as if a person(investor) is maximizingg a certain expected utility (welfare).This has been contradicted
repeatedly through experiments within the behavioral finnance field, whose development took place specifically in order to
integrate irrationality in economic decision making. Moreeover, there have also been theoretical studies that have shown that
investors do not act as if they are rational, but on the conttrary, exhibit many biases that lead to poor investment decisions in
specific contexts. This paper wishes to analyze the investtment decisions and behavior of investors from Bucharest’s Stock
Exchange, Romania. Using financial transaction data, we w wish to study some of the most prominent behavioral biases
investors have shown to be prone to. Thus, we wish too see if traders exhibit overconfidence in their trading positions,
whether they have a representativeness bias and a dispoosition effect. The paper is structured as follows: the first section
introduces the literature that has been done on similar stuudies. In section 2, the data is analyzed both from a normative and
behavioral point of view. In section 3, we undergo our em mpirical analysis and test the main hypotheses. Section 4 concludes.

©©2015
2015 The Authors.
Authors. Published
Published by Elsevier
by Elsevier B.V.isThis
B.V. This is anacce
an open open
essaccess
article article under
under the CC the CC BY-NC-ND
BY-NC-ND license license
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/).
Selection
Selection and peer-review under
and peer-review under responsibility of Asociatia Grupul
the Emerging M Roman
Markets de Cercetari
Queries in Finante
in Finance Corporatiste
and Business local organization.

Keywords: financial markets, biases, investment decisions, behavvioural finance, neurofinance, overconfidence, representativeness bias,
disposition effect, Romanian stock exchange

*
Corresponding author
E-mail address: toma.filip.mihai@gmail.com

2212-5671 © 2015 The Authors. Published by Elsevier B.V. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).
Selection and peer-review under responsibility of Asociatia Grupul Roman de Cercetari in Finante Corporatiste
doi:10.1016/S2212-5671(15)01383-0
Filip-Mihai Toma / Procedia Economics and Finance 32 (2015) 200 – 207 201

1. Introduction

Investing in financial markets from a normative point of view has been studied extensively over the past
decades, numerous theories being elaborated with regards to financial return, financial risk and both of the prior
taken together in the practice of investing. Most of the theoretical frameworks that have been proposed in the
literature are based on the main criterion of individual rationality – the investor acts as if he is maximizing a
certain expected utility (in the financial sense, he is maximizing his welfare), notion that was first approached
by von Neumann and Morgenstern, 1947.Markowitz, 1952, proposed a mathematical framework for investment
allocation on financial markets – proposal that was the foundation for modern portfolio theory. It represents a
cornerstone in modern financial theory as it provides a means for investors to model the allocation of assets
within a portfolio for a given required return and selected risk. Sharpe, 1964, develops the CAPM model which
can be used to model the return of an asset, given a certain level of risk the investor is willing to take. Of
course, both of these important theories, along with other major studies in the financial markets literature, are
coupled in the rationality mentioned above. This rationality, taken aggregately, comprises the basis for financial
markets efficiency, developed by Fama, 1970, which states that financial markets are able to publicly reveal all
information to market participants, the latter being able to take full informed decisions with regards to their
trading positions. However, human emotions and irrationality managed to play their role in financial markets
movements, both of these factors being studied from a psychological stance by researchers starting with the
1970s. The rather novel field of behavioral finance appears, under prospect theory, developed by Kahneman
and Tversky, 1979.The history of literature on financial markets, both from a traditional and a behavioral point
of view is extensive and leads to the current study on investing biases. As mentioned above, behavioral finance
is a field that captures the irrationality of investors, biases that investors are prone to. These cognitive errors are
due to investors’ inability to certainly know market movements for the next periods, which inclines them to
make biased decisions. Of course, these can prove to be both poor and beneficial for their welfare. The next
section describes three behavioral biases that investors are inclined to fall into. Upon this analysis, we start
developing our empirical framework to analyze investors’ decisions on the Romanian stock market.

2. Literature review and behavioral biases

People make irrational decisions in everyday events. Such non-optimal decisions are also taken in the
marketplace, where the pressure of losing money can build up intensively, leading to serious psychological
cognitive errors. These faulty decisions arise mainly because of human heuristic simplification under risk and
uncertainty. We will test for irrational trading decisions in section 3, by studying the following three behavioral
biases.

2.1 Overconfidence

Overconfidence is a state in which people tend to think they are better than they really are (Trivers, 1991).
Investors that exhibit overconfidence in their trading behavior are likely to expect larger returns during periods
of boom on financial markets and such investors also attribute their successes to their skills, while their failures
are attributed to “bad luck”. Both psychological and behavioral studies have been developed on overconfidence
(Campbell, Goodie and Foster, 2004, Lichtenstein et al., 1982). Another important sign of overconfidence is
given by trading frequency, these data being used as a proxy for measuring it. Under this hypothesis, Barber
and Odean, 2001, have found that US traders trade excessively, revealing a high degree of risk, while making
poor investment decisions after buying stocks. Furthermore, using data from a brokerage account in China,
Chen et al., 2007, have argued that the Chinese investors show both a disposition effect and a
representativeness bias, while also revealing overconfidence in their trading decisions. The overconfidence was
202 Filip-Mihai Toma / Procedia Economics and Finance 32 (2015) 200 – 207

also studied in comparison with US traders, which tend to hold on more to their stocks and trade less than their
Chinese counterparts. However, the frequency of trading decisions can be a noisy measure of overconfidence,
as investors can use both their skills and superior information of the marketplace to speed the process of
trading. Chen et al., 2007, argue that trading frequency can be measured by analyzing two main types of
investors: those with superior information, but who wrongly believe they have superior trading skills (therefore
exhibiting overconfidence) and those who are indeed superior to other, from a technical trading standpoint, but
with no information. On average, the trading frequency represents the overconfident behavior for investors.

2.2 Disposition effect

If we are to analyze the emotive factors that influence investing decisions, we would conclude that ex-post
feelings about investment behavior can influence traders’ behavior and lead to an important bias. The
disposition effect describes the tendency of investors to sell stocks that have subsequently performed better,
while keeping the stocks that have performed poor, believing that the latter would increase in value. This is also
consistent with the anchoring effect, present in prospect theory (Kahneman, Tversky, 1979). According to
prospect theory, the utility function is convex in losses and concave in the area of winning – thus implying risk
aversion in the area of winning. Under this note, Nofsinger, 2005, finds that investors have their pride
stimulated after having sold stocks which have increased in value (the winners), thus validating a good decision
to buy the stock in the first place. On the other hand, investors tend to hang on more to the “losers” (i.e. that
have declined in value post-purchase), as the selling decision of such a stock leads to regret. The same behavior
has been observed by Shefrin, Statman, 1985 – investing in financial markets creates the predisposition to keep
stocks that have decreased in value, in order to avoid regret. The finding is consistent with Odean, 1998a. This
type of behavior clearly indicates that stocks are regarded at an individual level, rather than at a portfolio level,
investors keeping track of individual stock performance. The disposition effect therefore combines the
application of mental accounting from prospect theory with the above findings.

2.3 Representativeness bias

One other major bias that investors have been prone to which violates traditional financial theory is the
representativeness bias. This state determines investors to take investment decisions that are based on
probability judgments of the outcomes that systematically violate Bayes’ rule (Kahneman, Tversky,
1973).Investors have been shown to constantly choose to invest in stocks emitted by “glamour” companies,
misattributing their good characteristics (quality products, managers and other fundamentals) to a good
investment decision. It has also been shown that investors were prone to invest in stocks with higher recent past
returns, the latter being consider as representative for future returns (DeBondt, 1993). This is consistent with
Dhar, Kumar (2001) who have investigated the price trends of stocks and have shown that stocks with positive
abnormal recent returns are preferred to others.

2.4 Investor characteristics

In order to analyze the above mentioned variables, we proceed with introducing three indicators as
characteristics of investors. Two indicators can be inferred from the transactions that have taken place, the end
account value for each investor and the frequent trading dummy. The end account value is calculated as the
total number of sales minus the total number of purchases and the frequent trading dummy is a dummy variable
that takes the value 1 when the account is in the first half with respect to trading frequency (i.e. the first 10
accounts) and the value 0 for the other 11 accounts. One indicator is given, the age of investors, in years.
Filip-Mihai Toma / Procedia Economics and Finance 32 (2015) 200 – 207 203

3. Empirical study of Romanian investors behavioral biases

The Bucharest Stock Exchange (“Bursa de Valori, BucureЮti”) has been the main financial market in
Romania since 1881. There are 197 companies listed with a total market capitalization of 133.7 billion RON
(approx.30.4 billion EUR). † Due to political and economic transitions over the last decades, investing in
financial markets by Romanian investors has remained a rather opaque subject, as there seems to be a national
risk aversion concerning such investments. This aversion has increased mainly after the fall of the communist
regime in 1990. Up until 2012, the main regulatory and supervisory body was CNVM (“Consiliul Naаional al
Valorilor Mobiliare”), its role being passed on to ASF (“Autoritatea de SupraveghereFinanciară”) in 2013.
The market benchmark index is the BET (listed in 1998), comprised of the 10 most liquid listed companies.

3.1 Data analysis

Our dataset contains 21 accounts of individual investors with a historical dataset from the beginning of the
year 2011 to its end. ‡ Due to the high unavailability and confidentiality clauses of brokerage firms from
Romania, only these observations will be taken into account in this study.The brokerage account data has been
received through an entity acting as an intermediary between individual investors and the Romanian Stock
Exchange. We therefore analyzed the behavior of individual investors throughout the year 2011. Summary
statistics can be found in Table 1.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the brokerage accounts

Individual investors Average Total

End account value (RON) 93000.51 1953010.73


Trading frequency (trades per month) 86 1813
Transaction volume (quantity) 5393648 113266609
Number of stocks (quantity) 29 611

3.2 The models

In the next section, we will adopt the methodologies proposed by Chen et al (2007), while also taking into
account certain indicators presented by Barber and Odean (2001) and Odean (1998a, 1999). In each of the
subsequent three models, we will use multiple linear regressions to estimate overconfidence, the disposition
effect and traders’ trading performance. The independent variables are given by the individual investors’
characteristics: investors’ age, frequent trading dummy and account value at the end of the year.

3.2.1 Overconfidence

We study overconfidence by assessing the following three variables as proxies: trading frequency, mean
monthly portfolio turnover and diversification of individual portfolios. Each of the previous indicators will be


Source: www.bvb.ro,Statistics section, 2014.

The author would like to thank Goldring society for providing the necessary information. In order to ensure full
confidentiality of the data, no names or other personal information were disclosed and none of information can be
transferred to any other third parties.
204 Filip-Mihai Toma / Procedia Economics and Finance 32 (2015) 200 – 207

introduced within a multiple linear regression. Trading frequency is computed for each investor by averaging
the number of traded stocks in each month, as follows:

ͳ
‫ ݆ܨ‬ൌ σ݊݅ൌͳ ܰ݅ , (1)
݊

where ‫ܨ‬௝ represents the trading frequency for investor ݆ and ܰ௜ is the number of trades (either sales or
purchases) in each month.We compute the monthly turnover for each investor according to the below formula,
after which we calculate the average for all investors.
݆ ݆
݆ ͳ ܰ ܰ
ܴ‫ ݐ‬ൌ ሺσ݅ൌͳ
‫ݐ‬
ܵ௧ ൅ σ݅ൌͳ
‫ݐ‬
ܲ௧ ሻ, (2)
ʹ

௝ ௝
where ܴ௧ is the monthly turnover for account ݆ at time ‫ݐ‬, ܰ௧ is the number of observations (i.e. months) for
each account ݆, ܵ௧ denotes the total value of sales from a certain month and ܲ௧ denotes the total number of
purchases in a certain month. This indicators are aggregated throughout all stocks. Diversification of individual
portfolios is calculated by counting the total number of stocks held by an investor throughout the period in
question.

3.2.2 The disposition effect

We assess the disposition effect by computing the relative size of the PGR indicator over the PLR indicator.
Proportion of gains realized (PGR) can be calculated as:

ோ௘௔௟௜௭௘ௗ௚௔௜௡௦
ܲ‫ ܴܩ‬ൌ , (3)
ሺோ௘௔௟௜௭௘ௗ௚௔௜௡௦ା௉௔௣௘௥௚௔௜௡௦ሻ

Here, the numerator indicates the return of the investor after having sold a stock that has done well after he
had initially bought it. The denominator contains paper gains as well, which are the returns the investor would
have made if he had sold the stock with a return, but did not.

The proportion of losses realized can be computed as:

ோ௘௔௟௜௭௘ௗ௟௢௦௦௘௦
ܲ‫ ܴܮ‬ൌ , (4)
ሺோ௘௔௟௜௭௘ௗ௟௢௦௦௘௦ା௉௔௣௘௥௟௢௦௦௘௦ሻ

The same interpretation applies: the numerator indicates the stocks the investor sold at a lower price than the
one he had bought them at, while the denominator includes the paper losses as well – the stocks that the
investor did not sell after they had fallen in value, but preferred to keep them in the portfolio. A large PGR
relative to PLR indicates that investors tend to sell their winners more than their losers. Calculations of the
above indicators for our dataset can be found in Table 2.

Table 2. The disposition effect


Variable Average Total
PGR 0.5173 10.8638
PLR 0.2108 4.4286
PGR/PLR 3.2856 68.9982
Filip-Mihai Toma / Procedia Economics and Finance 32 (2015) 200 – 207 205

We can see that there is a strong disposition effect present for our sample of investors. The average ratio
between the proportion of realized gains and the proportion of losses realized surpasses the value of one with
more than two units, this being a clear indicator of the disposition effect.

3.2.3 Representativeness bias

Finally, we analyze the investors ‘representativeness bias by estimating mean monthly abnormal returns of
the purchased stocks for each investor. We considered an abnormal return any return that surpassed a given
threshold, be it negative or positive. For simplicity and given the low number of observations, the chosen
threshold and the mean monthly abnormal returnee computed as:


‫ ݑ‬ൌ ͷ ή ሺܵ‫ ݐ‬൅ ܲ‫ ݐ‬ሻ, (5)

ೕ ೕ
ଵ ே ே
ߟ ൌ ൤൬σ௜ୀଵ


ܵ‫ݐ‬ȁܵ‫ ݐ‬൐ ‫ݑ‬൰ ൅ ൬ฬσ௜ୀଵ

ሺܲ‫ ݐ‬ȁܲ‫ ݐ‬൏ ‫ݑ‬ሻฬ൰൨, (6)

where ߟ is the mean monthly abnormal return, ‫ݑ‬is the threshold.

3.3 Results and interpretation

The following multiple linear regressions were estimated, having as dependent variable each of the above
described indicators:

‫ ݕ‬ൌ ߚ଴ ൅ ߚଵ ݅݊‫ݏݎ݋ݐݏ݁ݒ‬Ԣܽ݃݁ ൅ ߚଶ ݂‫ ݕ݉݉ݑ݀݃݊݅݀ܽݎݐݐ݊݁ݑݍ݁ݎ‬൅ ߚଷ ܽܿܿ‫ ݁ݑ݈ܽݒݐ݊ݑ݋‬൅ ߝ, (7)

where ‫ ݕ‬becomes each of the indicators computed above each time we run the regression. We therefore proceed
with the estimation of five linear regressions. Results are presented in Table 3.

Table 3. Linear regression results


Dependent variables
Mean Mean ܲ‫ܴܩ‬ Representativeness
Independent variables Trading frequency monthly monthly bias
turnover no. stocks ܲ‫ܴܮ‬
3.5913*** 10.8645*** 2.9656*** 4.2197*** 3.5841***
Intercept
(4.9792) (1.8365) (13.0327) (2.7575) (2.6859)
-0.0213** -0.0252** - -0.0196 -0.0091
Investors’ age
(-1.9232) (-2.0644) - (-0.6271) (-0.8118)
*** ***
0 0.0002 -0.0001 0 0.7765***
End account value
(-1.0850) (2.7178) (-2.9846) (-2.0006) (4.4981)
*** ***
2.4911 2.2727 1.1969 1.0813 2.2577***
Frequent trading dummy
(7.6153) (6.6720) (4.7429) (1.1339) (7.0990)
R - squared 75.02% 77.10% 61.54% 20% 82.37%
***, **
Note: Here, we display the coefficients for each of the independent variables, with the t-statistics in parentheses below. and * display
statistical significance at the 1, 5 and 10 percent levels, respectively.
206 Filip-Mihai Toma / Procedia Economics and Finance 32 (2015) 200 – 207

All of the significant regressions have been verified for stability and robustness, while also checking for
serial autocorrelation, heteroskedasticity and normality among the error terms. Neither serial autocorrelation,
nor heteroskedasticity are present amongst the innovations, while all of the latter series present normality.
Furthermore, all of regressions have a probability of zero for the F-stat, which indicates that the coefficients are
statistically significant. We also need to mention that all of the dependent variables have been switched into a
natural logarithm form, so as to reduce the values of the resulting coefficients.
Frequent trading dummy is a dummy variable that takes the value 1 when the account is in the first half with
respect to trading frequency (i.e. the first 10 accounts) and the value 0 for the other 11 accounts. End account
value is the account value at the end of the period, computed as total number of sales minus the total number of
purchases. Investor’s age is the age of the investor, in years. We also add an intercept for estimation robustness.
It is worth mentioning that in some equations, for the sake of the model, certain variables had to be removed.
This is the case for the investors’ age in the third regression, when estimating the impact of the mean monthly
number of stocks. These coefficients were not significant and influenced negatively the results of our
estimation.
We see that investors’ age influences negatively all of our dependent variables. We expect that the older the
investor is, the more experience he has accumulated and therefore proceeds with more caution when making
decisions to buy or sell. This is also consistent with mean monthly return of investors, as the lower the trading
frequency, the lower the returns. Implicitly, there is also a negative relationship between the investors’ age and
the propensity to have monthly abnormal returns (Chen et al. 2007, Barber and Odean, 2001, Odean, 1999).
The end account value influences only three of our selected dependent variables. Investors with a higher
account value at the end of the observation period have, on average, a higher monthly turnover. Of course,
those with higher account values have also exhibited more monthly abnormal returns.The frequency dummy
influences positively all of the dependent variables, this being in line with the results of similar studies. The
mean monthly turnover is influenced positively if the investor is trading more frequently and of course that the
higher the trading frequency, the higher the mean abnormal returns.
Unfortunately, some of the coefficients in our study were not statistically significant within the framework
of the present model. We refer mainly to the disposition effect, where only the intercept has a statistical effect,
but none of the investors’ characteristics determines it. Nonetheless, this effect from the behavioral finance
theory is present, as per Table 2. However, given the low number of observations, we believe the results are
consistent with the findings of similar ones (Chen et al., 2007, Barber and Odean, 2001, Odean, 1999). This
reduced number is motivated by confidential clauses and the difficulty of brokers to process the information.

4. Conclusions

In this paper, we have analyzed the behavior of investors on the Romanian stock exchange to see if they are
prone to classical behavior biases from the behavioral finance theory. We have used investors’ characteristics
to explain investors’ overconfidence, the disposition effect and the representativeness bias. We have used as
proxies for overconfidence the investors’ mean monthly trading frequency, the mean monthly turnover and the
number of stocks they hold in their portfolios. Investors’ age and the frequent trading variables influence most
these three indicators, while the end account value has a smaller influence only over the mean monthly turnover
and the mean monthly number of stocks. The disposition effect is present, given the high PGR to PLR ratio;
however, this proxy is not statistically significant within the multiple linear regression setting. We motivate this
lack of influence by the low number of observations. The representativeness bias is consistent with the
independent variables, younger investors having higher monthly abnormal returns. Also, those with a higher
end account value and who, on average, trade more stocks have higher abnormal returns. The results of our
regressions are mostly consistent with the studies of Chen et. al., 2007, Barber and Odean, 2001 and Odean,
1999.Chen at al., 2007, have developed this study on Chinese investors, which posits an even higher
Filip-Mihai Toma / Procedia Economics and Finance 32 (2015) 200 – 207 207

explanation for our study, as we believe that political regimes from the past might have had influence over the
behavior of domestic investors. In the future, we would like to develop this study further, by adding other
variables, such as gender and the time period from when the account was opened, as a proxy towards investing
experience. Also, this would be incorporated in a framework where the number of observations would be
significantly larger.

References

1.Neumann, J., Morgenstern, O., 1947. Theory of Games and Economic Behaviour, Princeton, NJ, Princeton University Press, 1944,
second ed. 1947, third ed. 1953;
2. Markowitz, H.M., 1952. Portfolio Selection, The Journal of Finance Vol. 7, No. 1;
3. Sharpe, W., 1964, A Theory of Market Equilibrium under Conditions of Risk, The Journal of Finance, Vol. 19, No. 3, pp. 425-442;
4. Fama, E. F., Efficient Capital Markets: A Review of Theory and Empirical Work, The Journal of Finance, 1970, Vol. 25, No. 2, pp.
383-417;
5. Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A., 1979. Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, Vol. 47, pp. 263–292;
6. Trivers, R., 1991. Deceit and self-deception, R. Robinson, & L. Tiger (Eds.), Man and beast revisited. Washington, DC: Smithsonian;
7. Campbell, W. K., Goodie, A. S., & Foster, J. D., 2004. Narcissism, confidence, and risk attitude. The Journal of BehavioralDecision
Making, Vol. 17, pp. 297–311;
8. Lichtenstein, S., B. Fischhoff, L. D. Philips, 1982. Calibration of probabilities: The state of art to 1980, Judgment under Uncertainty:
Heuristics and Biases, Ed. D. Kahneman and A. Tversky, Cambridge University Press;
9. Barber, B. M., & Odean, T., 2001. Boys will be boys: Gender, overconfidence, and common stock investment. Quarterly Journal of
Economics, Vol. 116, pp. 261–292.
10. Chen, G., Kim, K., Nofsinger, J., Rui, O., 2007, Trading performance, Disposition Effect, Overconfidence, Representativeness bias and
Experience of Emergent Market Investors, The Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, Vol. 20, pp. 425-451;
11. Nofsinger, J., 2005. The Psychology of Investing (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall;
12. Shefrin, H., Statman, M., 1985. The disposition to sell winners too early and ride losers too long: Theory and evidence. The Journal of
Finance, Vol. 40, pp. 777–790;
13. Odean, T., 1998a. Are investors reluctant to realize their losses? The Journal of Finance, Vol. 53, pp. 1775–1798;
14. Kahneman, D., Tversky, A., 1973. On the psychology of prediction, Psychological Review, Vol. 80, pp. 237–251;
15. DeBondt, W., 1993. Betting on trends: Intuitive forecasts of financial risk and return.International Journal ofForecasting, Vol. 9, pp.
355–371;
16. Dhar, R., Kumar, A., 2001. A non-random walk down the main street: Impact of price trends on trading decisions of individual
investors. Working paper (No. 00-45), International Center for Finance, Yale School of Management, New Haven, CT;
17. Odean, T., 1999. Do investors trade too much? American Economic Review, Vol. 89,pp. 1279–1298.

You might also like