You are on page 1of 21

Society for Music Theory

Using Drumbeats to Theorize Meter in Quintuple and Septuple Grooves


Author(s): SCOTT J. HANENBERG
Source: Music Theory Spectrum , FALL 2020, Vol. 42, No. 2 (FALL 2020), pp. 227-246
Published by: Oxford University Press on behalf of the Society for Music Theory

Stable URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/10.2307/27141599

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms

Oxford University Press and Society for Music Theory are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize,
preserve and extend access to Music Theory Spectrum

This content downloaded from


123.113.229.61 on Sun, 10 Mar 2024 01:45:31 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Using Drumbeats to Theorize Meter in Quintuple and Septuple Grooves
scott j. hanenberg

This article explores common approaches taken by drummers when playing music with a quintuple
or septuple groove. Based on original analysis from a corpus of 350 songs released during the half-
century between 1967 and 2017, I show that these grooves fall into three categories: undifferentiated,
in which the drum/s and/or cymbal/s that mark each attack do not change in the course of the
groove; backbeat variants, based on the alternation of kick and snare attacks, as in the common-time
backbeat; and polymetric grooves comprising two distinct metric cues, often pitting the drums
against the rest of the band.

Keywords: drums, rhythm, meter, rock music, corpus, analysis, undifferentiated, backbeat,
polymeter, polymetric grooves.

I
rregular-meter grooves define the sonic palettes of much musically literate performers and listeners almost uniformly
rock music. While recent scholarship has demonstrated represent the beat level using quarter notes, and I agree that
the analytical value of studying metrically irregular rock the identification of a salient tactus with a quarter-note beat
music, common subdivision patterns and the instrumental level is useful in representing rock meter.2 As such, I employ
cues that express them remain undertheorized. Irregular time signatures as descriptive tools, acknowledging that meter
grooves may create relatively unfamiliar metric structures, com- is always more complex than the information conveyed by a
plicating widely held assumptions about meter. To better un- time signature.3 When I identify or transcribe a groove in, for
derstand irregular rock grooves, I examine drumbeat example, 54 time, I mean to say two things. First, that there is a
articulations, which project recurring rhythmic patterns. salient cyclic repetition with a cardinality of five. Second, that
However, the resulting groupings and sub-groupings can be when I count along with the five-pulse cardinality of this cycle,
metrically ambiguous. Thus, a central concern in this article is I am counting the quarter-note beat that corresponds to the
theorizing how to interpret drumbeat practice. I focus this tactus level at which, in a 44 groove, I would expect to find a
work by restricting my analysis to the two most common irreg- snare backbeat.
ular cardinalities—quintuple and septuple. After outlining my There are often several plausible transcriptions of a groove,
research method and defining a corpus of 350 songs released and one aspect that differentiates these interpretations is the
during the half-century between 1967 and 2017, I discuss the choice of meter. Consider, for instance, two transcriptions of
broadest trends within the corpus and explain which drumbeat the same groove, one in 74 , the other alternating measures of 44
features I prioritize in my analysis. The main part of this article and 34 . For my purposes, there is no difference between these
explores three strategies for drumbeat patterns—undifferenti- situations; both represent the same cardinality (a seven-beat
ated articulations, backbeat variants, and polymetric interac- cycle) and the same beat level (the quarter note). In this article,
tions. Before considering the makeup of the corpus, some I often represent the internal structure of a cyclic pattern using
explication of each strategy and relevant terminology is in integers enclosed within parentheses rather than a time signa-
order; but first, I share my conception of meter and groove in ture. For example, the 74 case just described is represented as
this article. (4,3) (whole note followed by a dotted-half note) or (2,2,3)
(two half notes followed by a dotted-half note). Likewise, to
assumptions and definitions
draw attention to rhythmic cues at a lower level, I might make
a distinction between (2,2,2,1) (three half notes followed by a
Recent scholarship and popular-press transcriptions alike
quarter note) and (4,4,3,3) (at the eighth-note level, two half
evince a high degree of intersubjective agreement on the basics
of how meter works in rock music, but in the context of less
conventional grooves this agreement quickly evaporates. One 1 See Tamlyn (1998, 54–60), Mauch and Dixon (2012, 1 and 4–5), and
uncontroversial point is the pervasiveness of the standard rock Biamonte (2014 [6.1]).
backbeat—defined by snare-drum articulations on beats 2 and 2 The prevalence of the quarter note as the standard measure of a beat in
rock grooves is supported by countless published transcriptions, e.g., The
4—which occurs in an overwhelming majority of rock songs
Beatles Complete Scores (1993). In general, when I speak of hierarchical lev-
(readers who are unfamiliar with this archetypal drumbeat may els, I follow Lerdahl and Jackendoff (1983, 18ff.).
wish to look ahead to Ex. 5).1 Even though most rock songs 3 On the independence of meter and time signature, see Hasty (1997, 5–6)
are not written down during their composition or production, and Cohn (2019).

227

This content downloaded from


123.113.229.61 on Sun, 10 Mar 2024 01:45:31 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
228 MUSIC THEORY SPECTRUM 42 (2020)

notes followed by two dotted-quarter notes).4 A second source closing with definitions for the three drumbeat strategies at the
of ambiguity concerns grooves that can be represented by ei- heart of this study.
ther a 74 or a 78 time signature. The issue here arises from the Isochrony: Isochrony describes the spacing in time of suc-
imprecise distinction between a fast groove and a “double- cessive rhythmic articulations. When attacks are evenly spaced,
time” groove, or between a slow groove and a “half-time” the resulting succession is isochronous. When attacks are un-
groove.5 My assessments of tactus level are principally influ- evenly spaced, the succession is non-isochronous. Because the
enced by two factors. First, tempo is always a guiding factor, beat is the most salient metric level in a listener’s embodied
indicated in each transcription. In this article, most grooves connection to a meter, isochronous beats require less
have a tactus in the range of 80–200 beats per minute, and this “metering” than their non-isochronous counterparts and thus
accords with extant scholarship on rock meter.6 Second, trends afford more immediate, automatic entrainment.9
in drumbeat patterns are often decisive in my assessments of Platonic Successions: Scott Murphy describes two catego-
tactus level, especially in the context of backbeat variants, ries of “temporally ordered succession[s] of contiguous
where comparisons to the standard rock backbeat shape my lis- durations” that “either [begin] with a series of one or more
tening habits. I offer further discussion of the role of drum threes and [end] with a series of one or more twos, or vice
articulations below. versa.”10 In Platonic-trochaic successions, the duration of the
We can now turn to the term groove as I employ it in this initial series (the “run”) exceeds that of the second series (the
article. For inclusion in the corpus, I require that a groove con- “comma”), whereas Platonic-iambic successions invert this re-
tains at least two consecutive measures that can be parsed in lationship.11 Murphy demonstrates that Platonic-trochaic suc-
the same quintuple or septuple meter.7 This limitation cessions are more common than Platonic-iambic ones across a
excludes isolated irregular measures (e.g., a 74 measure arising variety of meters and rhythmic categories.
from a deleted beat at the end of a 44 passage), in which the Continuation: Christopher Hasty employs this term (in
drum pattern is more likely to modify the surrounding meter tandem with its counterpart, “beginning”) to denote “a feeling
than to represent a conventional approach to playing in the ir- that [a given] articulation continues a process initiated with
regular meter. Most grooves in the corpus maintain their the beginning of the [prior] sound, [which] is still present, ac-
meters for more than two measures, but even those with only tive, and expanding.”12 The feeling Hasty describes aligns with
my own experience of listening to snare-drum backbeats. I ex-
two consecutive measures can establish the stability (or poten-
tend Hasty’s terminology, identifying double continuations—
tial stability) of a groove.8 Within longer passages in an irregu-
the second and third articulations within three-part groups,
lar meter, the meter is often articulated in two or more related
typically marked by the snare drum. Double continuations
ways. I count such cases as comprising multiple distinct
should not be confused with Hasty’s “deferred continuation”:
grooves, provided that each groove spans at least two measures.
in the former, the feeling of continuation is established imme-
One unfortunate consequence of the requirement for consecu-
diately by the first snare hit, then extended or repeated by the
tive measures is the omission of many irregular grooves based
second; in the latter, the first continuation is retrospectively
on alternating meters (e.g., 68 þ 58 or 44 þ 78 ). While such exam-
undermined by the second.
ples do form grooves, they are not properly quintuple or septu-
Undifferentiated Articulations: Here, the drum/s and/or
ple for the purposes of this study.
cymbal/s that mark each attack do not change in the course of
In view of the considerable amount of extant metric theory
the groove. There are two types of undifferentiated articula-
relevant to this article, I offer a brief glossary of relevant terms,
tions. In isochronous undifferentiated articulations, the
4 This and related types of Arabic-numeral and parenthesis notation are
attacks are evenly spaced (e.g., attacks on beats 1, 2, 3, 4, and
discussed in Gotham (2015, [2.5–2.6]). 5 of each measure in 54 ). In non-isochronous undifferentiated
5 A typical 44 half-time groove in rock is most often indicated by a drumbeat articulations, the attacks are unevenly spaced (e.g., attacks on
with a single snare-drum articulation on beat 3. Double-time grooves in 44
time involve snare articulations on the second halves of all four beats. For 9 Edward Large has written extensively on entrainment. See Large and
more on analyzing half- and double-time grooves, see de Clercq (2016). Jones (1999), Large (2001, 174), Large and Palmer (2002), and Large
6 See Biamonte’s (2014, [6.1]) depiction of the standard backbeat in the (2008, 192–94).
range of 80–160 bpm; Attas (2015), with examples in the range of 104– 10 Murphy (2016, [1.3]).
163 bpm; and Capuzzo (2018), with 80–160 bpm indications for the 11 Murphy’s work builds on Cohn (2016). Cohn focuses on commas of two-
quarter note and dotted-quarter note. For a more general discussion of pulse units, which reconcile runs of three-pulse units with an underlying
tempo and tactus, see London (2012, 25–47). duple framework. Murphy generalizes the logic of such successions by ad-
7 I use “irregular groove” to refer to the quintuple and septuple examples mitting cases in which the two- and three-pulse aspects of comma and
under consideration and, on occasion, to a broader collection of grooves run are inverted. Below I analyze many successions that contain durations
with uncommon cardinalities. of four pulses at the same level as durations of three (e.g., [3,3,4]), whereas
8 A well-known example is The Beatles’ “All You Need Is Love” (1967, Murphy deals only with twos and threes. Because four-pulse groups nec-
0:08), in which two measures of 74 give way to 44 . The two 74 measures are essarily divide as (2,2) under Murphy’s system, our analyses do not
sufficient to establish the irregular groove, which I can imagine continuing conflict.
further. 12 Hasty (1997, 105).

This content downloaded from


123.113.229.61 on Sun, 10 Mar 2024 01:45:31 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
using drumbeats to theorize meter in quintuple and septuple grooves 229

beats 1 and 4 of each measure in 54 ).13 “Undifferentiated” refers I have focused here on rock, broadly conceived.17 The songs
to the instrument/s that articulate the pattern, not to the collected for this article come from various sources and include
resulting metric interpretation (i.e., a 58 groove with a hi-hat at- several additions not found in the literature I consulted.18
tack on every eighth note does not imply a metric subdivision
of [1,1,1,1,1]). broad trends in the corpus
Backbeat Variants: These patterns are based on the alterna-
tion of kick and snare articulations, as observed in the common- The 350 songs of the corpus represent the work of 157 bands
time backbeat.14 In irregular meters, the resulting patterns are and artists and include 638 distinct quintuple or septuple
either non-isochronous (e.g., [3,3,2,2]) or include double- grooves. Well over half of the songs analyzed (206 of 350)
continuation structures in the snare drum (i.e., kick–snare– contain only a single quintuple or septuple groove; about an-
snare). The presence of a double continuation may form part of other fifth (seventy-one) contain two. Songs by metal and pro-
a larger pattern (e.g., the first three beats in a measure of 74 ) or gressive rock bands like Dream Theater, Genesis, and Tool
may itself fill the entire repeating pattern through non- contain more distinct grooves on average. In one case, I ana-
isochrony (e.g., as a [3,3,4] expression of a quintuple meter). lyzed twelve quintuple or septuple grooves in a single song—
Polymetric Grooves: Most often, backbeat variants and Tool’s “The Grudge.” The analysis that follows primarily con-
undifferentiated articulations express the same subdivisional cerns the individual grooves and not entire songs. In this sec-
structure as the rest of the band, or the two correspond closely tion, I comment on some general aspects of the corpus,
enough that any discrepancies are heard as syncopations. considering chronological trends and the prevalence of differ-
Sometimes, however, we encounter situations in which one or ent meters and drumbeat archetypes. It should be borne in
more instrumental parts suggest a meter entirely distinct from mind that the figures that follow are not necessarily representa-
that of the drum kit; hence my identification of this third cate- tive of all rock music.
gory.15 The grooves in this category exhibit various degrees of Example 1 shows the chronology of grooves in the corpus,
metric conflict—some listeners may feel a strong preference for from 1967 to 2017.19 The late 1970s and the 2000s are best
a particular metric interpretation of a given example. For my represented in the corpus, whereas I found relatively few
purposes it is sufficient that a groove affords entrainment to two examples from the late 1980s. It is also worth considering
interpretations, regardless of the relative strength of each. which bands and artists are most represented in the corpus;
Example 2 lists those with ten or more distinct grooves.
methodological considerations The two most robust parameters I employ to uncover
trends in drumbeat patterns are metric interpretation
This article is a corpus study. The grooves considered here (expressed by time signatures) and drumbeat archetype—
share several features and are bounded by certain restrictions. undifferentiated articulations, backbeat variants, and polymet-
Necessarily, each song in the corpus contains at least one
ric grooves. Example 3 shows the percentage breakdown of the
groove that involves both a repeating pattern on the drums
corpus according to each parameter. The chart in Example
and a quintuple or septuple meter; in most cases, these two
3(a) groups quintuple meters on the right-hand side and sep-
elements support one another.16 My analysis focuses on
tuple on the left. In the broadest sense, the two cardinalities
grooves in which at least one instrumental layer can be tran-
receive nearly even representation, but significant distinctions
scribed in 54 , 58 , 165 , 74 , 78 , or 167 . Passages with a quintuple or sep-
emerge when we consider hierarchical levels.20 Grooves
tuple cycle at the half-note level (or higher) were generally not
considered because of drum parts that employ the familiar 17 My understanding of rock follows Gracyk (1996) and aligns with much
backbeat, but there are some exceptions. For example, I in- music-theoretical writing on the subject—e.g., Moore (2001, 3–4),
clude passages with an ambiguous tactus (i.e., a groove that Stephenson (2002, xiv), Biamonte (2010, 95), Temperley (2011, [1.5]),
affords either a slow irregular quarter-note cycle or a half-time and Attas (2015, 275). For further discussion, see Fabbri (1982), Negus
interpretation with the irregular cardinality at the half-note (1999), McLeod (2001), and Holt (2007).
level) and those that involve a polymeter (e.g., a five-beat osti- 18 My sources include Rosenberg (2011), Biamonte (2014), Hanenberg
(2018), Temperley (2018), several Billboard magazine lists, the Rolling
nato and/or harmonic rhythm against a two-and-a-half-
Stone “500 Greatest Songs of All Time,” the Wikipedia pages on
measure 44 drumbeat). I found no examples in either cardinality “Quintuple Meter” and “Septuple Meter,” and several informal online lists
that implied a thirty-second-note pulse level. Another consid- and discussion boards. Due to the large number of songs considered, a full
eration in selecting examples concerns the issue of genre. listing in this article is impractical. Interested readers are invited to contact
the author for the details of the corpus.
13 I use the term “undifferentiated” in Hanenberg (2018, 108). 19 I use a rolling three-year average in representing this data for ease of
14 The term “backbeat variant” is synonymous with Osborn’s (2017, 57) legibility.
“backbeat deformation.” 20 In some grooves the tactus level is ambiguous. My judgments of tactus are
15 My use of “polymeter” and “polymetric” follows London (2012, 66–67). based on tempo, harmonic rhythm, and standards of drumbeat practice in
16 I make no distinction between the acoustic drum kit and electronic drums. regular meters. In Ex. 3, ambiguous entries count as a half point toward
Given the role of technology in recent record production, any such dis- each potential category, so as not to skew the data by overrepresenting
tinction is difficult to draw. these examples.

This content downloaded from


123.113.229.61 on Sun, 10 Mar 2024 01:45:31 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
230 MUSIC THEORY SPECTRUM 42 (2020)

25

20

15

10

0
1967
1968
1969
1970
1971
1972
1973
1974
1975
1976
1977
1978
1979
1980
1981
1982
1983
1984
1985
1986
1987
1988
1989
1990
1991
1992
1993
1994
1995
1996
1997
1998
1999
2000
2001
2002
2003
2004
2005
2006
2007
2008
2009
2010
2011
2012
2013
2014
2015
2016
2017
example 1. Chronology of irregular grooves in the corpus; three-year rolling average

is

ad
pa

K
d
h

l
r

e
oo
te

re

ea
us

es

U
he
ap
ea

T
T

D
en
R
Z

io
Th

ne

ul

ad
k

ef
i
an

up
am

R
t
Fr

ra
rc
re

G
Po
D

Grooves Songs

example 2. Bands and artists with ten or more grooves in the corpus

analyzed in 54 account for over two thirds of all quintuple septuple on the right. Within this corpus, drummers prefer
examples, and nearly one third of the entire corpus. Example backbeat-variant patterns for septuple-meter grooves, whereas
3(b) shows the prevalence of each archetype. Backbeat variants undifferentiated drumbeats are nearly as common as backbeat
are the most common by a substantial margin, followed by variants in quintuple meters.
undifferentiated patterns.21
By cross-referencing meter and drumbeat archetype, we
find further connections between the two modes of categoriza-
a concise model of drum-kit functions
tion. Example 4 shows the correlations as a 100 percent
stacked bar graph with quintuple meters on the left and
Before detailing the prevalence of different drumbeat patterns,
I offer thoughts of a more general nature on the functions of
21 As with examples with an ambiguous tactus, complex patterns are some- the drum kit and the roles typically played in rock grooves by
times counted in more than one category. each drum, cymbal, or group thereof.

This content downloaded from


123.113.229.61 on Sun, 10 Mar 2024 01:45:31 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
using drumbeats to theorize meter in quintuple and septuple grooves 231

35%
Undifferentiated
Polymetric
55% Backbeat

10%

example 3. Breakdown of grooves in the corpus according to meter and drumbeat archetype. (a) quintuple and septuple meters.
(b) prevalence of each archetype

100% of the drum kit is expressed in virtuosic drum solos, in which


meter can be highly destabilized and even abandoned alto-
90%
gether. At a deeper level than the meter, the third function of
80% the drums is to articulate musical form. Two essential strate-
gies are changes of groove (whether subtle, as in the variation
70%
of a kick-drum pattern, or striking, as in a shift from straight
60% to swung time) and drum fills; both techniques typically align
with the boundaries of formal sections or phrases. The fourth
50%
function of the drum kit is to highlight the rhythm of another
40% part of the ensemble. For instance, big-band jazz drummers
often play along with horn shots on the snare drum and cym-
30%
bals, while rock and pop drummers frequently support the bass
20% part of a groove with the kick drum.
Within the first function—meter articulation—different
10% drums, cymbals, and groups thereof function at particular hier-
0%
archical levels.24 The standard organization of these elements
5/4 5/8 5/16 7/4 7/8 7/16 varies between genres. For example, the articulation of the
Undifferentiated Polymetric Backbeat
backbeat by the snare drum is a defining feature of rock music,
whereas in jazz the same role is traditionally assigned to the
example 4. Correlation of each drumbeat archetype with each closing of the hi-hat cymbals with the foot pedal. My remarks
meter within the corpus in what follows primarily concern rock, in keeping with the
corpus at hand. Furthermore, these remarks concern multi-
The drum kit has at least four distinct rhythmic-metric drum textures. Any element of the drum kit can articulate an
undifferentiated pattern.
functions, each of which directs listener attention in a particu-
If the snare backbeat in 44 is understood to occupy the half-
lar way.22 The first of these functions, and the most relevant
measure level, lower hierarchical levels are typically articulated
for our purposes, is to convey the meter. This function is in-
by the hi-hats or ride cymbal (whether in quarter notes, eighth
voked when one speaks of the drums as a “timekeeper.”
notes, or sixteenth notes).25 Occasionally, the toms can assume
Within an established meter, the second function is to articu-
late a rhythmic paradigm (tresillo, bossa nova, etc.) or to stimu-
and Toussaint (2013, 146). On the interactions of syncopated rhythms
late rhythmic interest, such as through syncopated against an underlying meter in rock, see Temperley (1999).
embellishments.23 At its most extreme, this rhythmic function 24 The discussion that follows is informed by Iyer (2002), Osborn (2010),
and Biamonte (2014).
22 This is to say nothing of the extensive timbral functions of the drum kit, 25 As a marker of hierarchical structure, the backbeat is unusual for marking
its potential to contribute to the pitch content of a song via tuned drums, the midpoint of each cycle of its particular level (that of the half note).
and hermeneutic uses such as musical onomatopoeia or referencing the The backbeat thus not only articulates half-note regularity but also
drum part of another song. expresses the underlying quarter-note beat. It is perhaps because of this
23 On rhythmic paradigms, including the tresillo, see Biamonte (2014, [3.3– dual function that the backbeat has come to be so central to 44 grooves in
3.4] and [6.4]), Chor (2010, 37–39), Cohn (2016, [0.3]), Traut (2005), rock and other genres.

This content downloaded from


123.113.229.61 on Sun, 10 Mar 2024 01:45:31 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
232 MUSIC THEORY SPECTRUM 42 (2020)

rhythmic succession is articulated? and (3) what drum, cymbal,


or combination thereof is responsible for this articulation?
Study of the different successions (question 2) proved the most
fruitful; I found no meaningful correlation between meter and
instrument, but each parameter correlates with specific succes-
sions. The most significant division categorizes successions as
example 5. A characteristic backbeat, showing the articulation of isochronous or non-isochronous. Different meters show a
different hierarchical levels by distinct elements of the drum kit preference for one type or the other based on measure length:
meters with more beats (and sub-beats) per measure often con-
the role of beat or pulse articulation. Example 5 shows a typi- tain isochronous successions, while meters with fewer beats of-
cal snare-drum backbeat alongside eighth notes on the hi-hat. ten contain non-isochronous patterns. To be more specific,
In rock music, the kick drum can articulate any number of lev- isochronous successions are more common in 74 grooves by a
els. Several depictions of the backbeat pattern in music- factor of 5:3; both types receive about equal representation in
5 7 5 7 5
theoretical literature identify a kick articulation on beats 1 and 4 ; and non-isochronous successions dominate 8 , 8 , 16 , and 16

3, implying that the kick drum works with the snare at the by a factor of at least 2:1.
Example 6 summarizes the various isochronous undifferen-
half-measure level.26 Likewise, examples of “four-to-the-floor”
tiated successions, cross-listed according to meter.28 The
kick-drum patterns are common in rock, with the kick drum
entries highlighted in dark gray indicate the most common
articulating the beat level. Less clear, from a functional per-
successions among the best represented meters, all of which
spective, is what to make of grooves in which the kick drum
show a strong preference for articulating the metric level that
marks each downbeat but supplies a syncopated rhythm in the
houses the irregular cardinality (e.g., the quarter note in 74 ).
intervening beats (as in Ex. 5). I propose that only the initial
While it is not surprising that many drumbeats articulate this
downbeat of such patterns should be associated with meter ar-
level, it is not necessarily intuitive that the articulation of met-
ticulation, whereas the majority of the work performed by the
ric levels below the irregular cardinality should not be more
kick drum expresses a rhythmic function.27 Thus, in grooves
common (e.g., the eighth-note level is articulated in only two
of this type, the meter-articulating role of the kick drum is to
of twenty-five 74 grooves). One reason might be that drummers
reinforce the measure level. As we will see, kick-drum synco-
feel the need to draw attention to the irregular level, either to
pation is a common strategy among irregular grooves. Finally,
solidify their own counting or to aid other band members in
note that the crash cymbal on the first downbeat in Example 5
navigating the groove. Not shown in the example are the
is not repeated in the second measure. Crash, splash, and
drums and cymbals most commonly associated with each pat-
china cymbals often designate larger spans of music (e.g., one,
tern. In keeping with their typical roles within metrically regu-
two, or four measures, etc.), or occur at irregular intervals
lar grooves, the snare drum more often articulates deeper levels
through the course of a song, marking select formal boundaries
(the quarter note in particular), while cymbals or toms are
(e.g., the start of each chorus).
common at shallower levels (eighths and sixteenths, though
three drumbeat archetypes and their metric quarters are by no means rare). A few well-known examples of
interpretations isochronous undifferentiated drumbeats include the following:
The Beatles, “All You Need Is Love” (0:08): snare-drum
Each drumbeat archetype can accommodate a variety of metric quarter notes in 74 time.
structures. I begin by discussing the undifferentiated and Peter Gabriel, “Solsbury Hill” (0:00): kick-drum quarter
backbeat-variant types, followed by the rarer and more com- notes in 74 time.
plex polymetric category. Radiohead, “Everything in Its Right Place” (1:14): kick-
drum quarter notes in 54 time.
Undifferentiated Grooves Radiohead, “2 þ 2 ¼ 5” (0:14): processed or electronic per-
cussion eighth notes in 78 time.
Of the 638 grooves in the corpus, 303 exhibit undifferentiated Led Zeppelin, “Four Sticks” (0:00): hi-hat and floor-tom
drumbeat patterns. My analysis focused on three questions: eighth notes in 58 time.29
(1) what meter (or meters) best fits the groove? (2) what
28 Not included here are passages in which a backbeat-variant drumbeat
26 Examples include Butterfield (2006, 40–42), Hennessy (2008, 146 [Ex. employs an undifferentiated articulation in one part of the drum kit (usu-
5-2] and 161 [Ex. 5-4]), and Biamonte (2014, [6.1]). ally the hi-hat or ride cymbal). I comment on the potential intra-
27 The distinction between meter and rhythm implied here follows London drumbeat polyrhythms that result from such situations below.
(2012, 4): rhythmic durations are determined based “on the interonset in- 29 Some of these examples are ambiguous with regard to tactus level. Osborn
terval . . . between the attack points of successive events,” whereas meter is (2017) transcribes “2 þ 2 ¼ 5” in 74 (43), 78 (55), and 78 with a “non-metrical
contingent on “our perception and cognition of such stimuli,” on our cul- beat layer” (58) and discusses the issue at length, also referencing “15
tural upbringing, and on our musical education (italics in original). Step” (56–58). Rosenberg (2011, 68) analyzes “Four Sticks” in 54 with a

This content downloaded from


123.113.229.61 on Sun, 10 Mar 2024 01:45:31 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
using drumbeats to theorize meter in quintuple and septuple grooves 233

Quarter Notes Eighth Notes Sixteenth Notes Downbeat Only Other


Tally Pct. Tally Pct. Tally Pct. Tally Pct. Tally Pct.
7/4 20 80% 2 8% 0 0% 2 8% 1 4%
5/4 27 69% 3 8% 0 0% 5 13% 4 10%
7/8 1 5% 11 52% 3 14% 3 14% 3 14%
5/8 0 0% 9 69% 0 0% 4 31% 0 0%
7/16 0 – 0 – 0 – 0 – 0 –
5/16 0 0% 0 0% 2 33% 4 67% 0 0%

example 6. Prevalence of isochronous undifferentiated successions within each meter in the corpus

Example 6 also reveals which isochronous undifferentiated


possibilities are never employed and draws attention to several
rarities. Given the small number of sixteenth-note grooves in
the corpus, it is to be expected that some possibilities are not
represented; nevertheless, the absence of even a single isochro-
nous undifferentiated example in 167 is striking, especially in
light of the six grooves in 165 . Among these six, two articulate
every sixteenth note while four mark only the downbeat of
each cycle. The additional two pulses in 167 seem to encourage
drummers to employ non-isochronous patterns with greater
frequency. Indeed, such patterns may be necessary to clarify
the metric structure within the larger cardinality.
example 7. Two unusual non-isochronous undifferentiated
As for rarities, two examples are unique for their articula-
drumbeats. (a) King Crimson, “ Red” (1974), 5:37. (b) Pete
tion of a particular succession within a given meter: Example 7
Townshend, “ Jools and Jim” (1980), 0:16
offers transcriptions of the relevant grooves from King
Crimson’s “Red” and Pete Townshend’s “Jools and Jim.” The
7 is what places this groove in my “Other” category in Example
8 passage in “Red” occurs several times, underpinned by a
6 (the groove is also listed under “Quarter Notes” to account
straightforward sixteenth-note pulse on the china cymbal
for the kick part). Here the snare drum clarifies the (3,4) sub-
(transcribed in the example where the ride is normally shown
division of the 74 groove—a Platonic-iambic structure. Similar
because of its contextual use). Later in the song, this groove is
incomplete undifferentiated articulations account for several
sometimes complicated by a second isochronous undifferenti-
entries in the “Other” column. Eighth-note triplets are another
ated pulse stream—a regular snare-drum quarter note. Because
option seen exceedingly rarely.
the listener is accustomed to hearing this passage in 78 , led pri-
Unlike isochronous undifferentiated grooves, non-
marily by the guitar riff, the snare hits are likely heard as shift-
isochronous patterns are difficult to classify across meters be-
ing across the bar line from on-beat to off-beat, but the
cause each pattern is unique to its parent meter. Moreover,
emphatic quarter-note articulation also affords entrainment to
trends toward Platonic-trochaic successions or toward begin-
a 74 meter. The three layers of the groove (sixteenth-note china
ning with a run of twos or threes are mixed among the meters.
cymbal, quarter-note snare drum, and 78 guitar riff) thus create
However, for each meter, one succession or class of successions
a polymeter. Without the other elements of the groove, and
stands apart as a clear first choice. Example 8 summarizes the
without the context offered by earlier instances of the same
most common patterns in each meter and the prevalence of
riff, the snare-drum pattern would likely not convey a 78 meter. each option as a percentage of all non-isochronous undifferen-
Naturally, undifferentiated quarter notes seldom supply the tiated grooves in that meter. In 74 , nine of fifteen grooves ex-
only drum articulation among meters with an eighth-note tac- hibit a (3,4) division at the quarter-note level and subdivide
tus because of their destabilizing effect.30 the initial three beats as (3,3), sometimes suggesting a 68 mea-
A recurring passage in Townshend’s “Jools and Jim” like- sure, sometimes sounding more like a syncopation against 34 .
wise combines undifferentiated elements at two levels with a In 54 , successions of (3,3,2,2) at the eighth-note level predomi-
consistent quarter-note kick drum and snare-drum eighth nate (eighteen out of forty-eight). Although they are less prev-
notes in the first half of the cycle. The intermittent snare part alent than the dominant successions in other meters, several
related patterns imply the same metric subdivision without ar-
shift to 34 , whereas I hear the quintuple groove as an abbreviated 68 , alter-
ticulating it overtly. For example, five grooves incorporate
nating with sections in that more common meter.
30 Less rare are quarter-note pulse streams that complement a 58 or 78 backbeat sixteenth-note subdivisions below the same overriding pattern.
variant, where the meter is articulated more securely. See the discussion of The prevalence among quarter-note meters of successions that
backbeat-variant patterns below. begin with a pair of threes (even, as in 74 , if this promotes

This content downloaded from


123.113.229.61 on Sun, 10 Mar 2024 01:45:31 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
234 MUSIC THEORY SPECTRUM 42 (2020)

unarticulated eighth notes. The result is an uninterrupted (iso-


Most Common Succession Pct. of Same
chronous) articulation of the eighth-note background, within
7/4 (3,3 3,3,2) or (3,3 2,2,2,2) 60.0% which a non-isochronous succession is foregrounded by the
5/4 (3,3 2,2) 37.5% snare drum. A similar situation occurs in Dream Theater’s “In
the Presence of Enemies,” in which isochronous eighth notes
7/8 (2,2,3) 60.0%
in the toms establish a background and the subdivisional struc-
5/8 (2,3) 62.1% ture is foregrounded through dynamic accents and use of flams
7/16 (2,2,3) 62.5% (notated as grace notes in the transcription). This passage
demonstrates a similar treatment of 58 and 78 meters, ending
5/16 (2,3) 90.0%
each measure with a three-pulse unit. The last example, Traci
Lords’s “Good-N-Evil,” also establishes a foreground-
example 8. The most prevalent non-isochronous undifferenti- background relationship between the kick drum and hi-hat
ated successions for each meter in the corpus cymbals. Because these two elements function here as they
might in a standard backbeat, this undifferentiated pattern
approximates a common 78 backbeat variant. Indeed, such a
Platonic-iambic successions) may be related to the abundance groove is soon realized in this song with the addition of the
of tresillo (3,3,2) patterns in the work of several of the artists snare drum.
analyzed here and in rock music generally.31
Patterns at sub-tactus levels are more consistent within Backbeat Variants
each cardinality. For septuple meters, twenty-four out of forty
7 Over sixty percent of the grooves in the corpus (389 of 638)
8 instances express a (2,2,3) subdivision at the eighth-note
level; an additional two articulate the simplified (4,3) at the modify the 44 backbeat by articulating the subdivisional struc-
eighth-note level and six embellish the pattern with sporadic ture of a meter with the snare drum (usually alternating with
sixteenth notes. The analogous (2,2,3) and (4,3) sixteenth- the kick drum). Common irregular-meter variants of the back-
note successions account for all eight 167 grooves. In 58 , succes- beat can be modeled through the expansion or contraction of
sions of (2,3) are more than twice as common as (3,2) beats or sub-tactus pulses in the cycle. Two common strategies
(eighteen and eight instances of each, respectively, out of distance themselves from the traditional backbeat further by
twenty-nine). This is somewhat surprising, given the general eschewing strict alternations between kick and snare articula-
prevalence of Platonic-trochaic successions among irregular tions. The first strategy involves omitting a mid-measure kick-
meters. The Platonic-iambic option is even more pervasive drum articulation. Antecedents for this pattern occur in 44
among 165 passages, with nine of ten employing the succession. grooves, where the mid-measure kick-drum attack may be sup-
The prevalence below the quarter-note level of successions that planted by a syncopated kick-drum pattern (marking the “and”
of beats 2 and 3, for instance). In irregular grooves, it is more
begin with twos (in both quintuple and septuple meters) sug-
difficult to distinguish between structural and syncopated kick
gests that the articulation of a clear eighth- or quarter-note
articulations. The second strategy involves a double continua-
pulse helps listeners and performers to stabilize both the non-
tion (kick–snare–snare). Models for this pattern include triple
isochronous beats and the starting point of each new cycle.
meters and tresillo grooves. In irregular meters, single and dou-
Four representative passages are transcribed in Example 9.
ble continuations are often juxtaposed within larger repeating
In Genesis’s “The Battle of Epping Forest,” the kick drum
cycles. As with non-isochronous undifferentiated patterns,
and ride cymbal articulate the same initial syncopation that
each meter gives rise to characteristic grooves. In reviewing
characterizes many undifferentiated 74 grooves. The looser
trends within each meter, several notable distinctions between
rhythmic relationship between the two elements in the second undifferentiated and backbeat-variant grooves emerge. Most
half of the measure combines the two most common options; meters exhibit different subdivisional structures for each type
however, because the rivets in the ride cymbal somewhat mask of articulation. The clearest trend across all meters is that
the cymbal articulations, the kick-drum pattern comes forth as backbeat variants correlate with Platonic-trochaic successions
the more prominent succession. The same initial syncopation more consistently than do non-isochronous undifferentiated
characterizes certain 54 measures in Queen’s “Innuendo,” with grooves. I begin with septuple meters—which are more consis-
the snare drum articulating the common (3,3,2,2) succession. tent than quintuple—working from the shortest, most con-
The kick drum is subordinate to the snare, filling in the strained cycles (i.e., sixteenth-note level) to the longest, most
flexible (i.e., quarter-note level).
31 Tool is especially fond of tresillo and double-tresillo patterns as well as lon-
Among septuple backbeat variants, the potential discrep-
ger Platonic-trochaic successions. On Tool’s use of the tresillo, see
Hanenberg (2018, 96–99 and 159–61). On Tool’s use of metric disso-
ancy between meter and drumbeat may be felt most acutely.
nance, see Biamonte (2014, [8.7]). On the prevalence of tresillo patterns Listeners who follow traditional models in parsing irregular
in rock and pop more broadly, see Biamonte (2014, [3.3]–[3.4] and meters only in twos and threes may feel that many drum-kit
[6.4]). articulations deviate from their metric intuitions. Meanwhile,

This content downloaded from


123.113.229.61 on Sun, 10 Mar 2024 01:45:31 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
using drumbeats to theorize meter in quintuple and septuple grooves 235

example 9. Archetypal examples of non-isochronous undifferentiated grooves. (a) Genesis, “ The Battle of Epping Forest” (1973), 5:05.
(b) Queen, “ Innuendo” (1991) 4:49. (c) Dream Theater, “ In the Presence of Enemies” (2007), 2:05. (d) Traci Lords, “ Good-N-Evil”
(1995), 0:45

listeners who prioritize the drums in making sense of a Dying Soul.” In the latter, the final sixteenth-note articulation
groove’s structure will often have to contend with striking met- in the snare drum is not structural—the snare attack is a softer
ric deletions. In longer passages, the cyclical nature of groove- “ghost note” and the absence of the hi-hat further detracts
based music mitigates these difficulties by encouraging en- from its salience.
trainment through familiarity.32 As we consider other septuple meters, the same features
Backbeat grooves in 167 time, while underrepresented due to that typify 167 grooves remain significant. The strategy of metric
the general rarity of irregular sixteenth-note cardinalities, are deletion toward the end of the cycle is common at all hierar-
consistent. Of sixteen instances, all but one exhibit a (4,3) or chical levels. Note also that, throughout my discussion of 167 ,
(2,2,3) Platonic-trochaic succession (the lone exception is the pairing of fours and threes has generally been more com-
complicated by polymeter; see Ex. 20[c] below). Eleven of mon than the pairing of twos and threes (the more common
these exhibit the (4,3) approach, where we usually find strict case in many discussions of metric subdivision). Prioritizing
alternations between downbeat kick and backbeat snare articu- the snare drum as a marker of metric continuation yields anal-
lations. This groove thus imitates the 44 backbeat as closely as yses in which structures that are ill modeled by twos and threes
possible within a 167 cycle: through the omission of the final six- are common in all septuple meters.
teenth note of every two-beat span, the (4,4,4,4) sixteenth- In particular, the predominance of cycle-ending deletion is
note level of a 44 backbeat becomes two measures of 167 , articu- as evident among 78 grooves as it is in 167 . Example 11 lists the
lating a (4,3,4,3) pattern with alternating kick and snare hits. various patterns encountered according to the placement of
Less common 167 possibilities feature a snare articulation on the structural snare-drum articulations, and the prevalence of each
“and” of beat 1, either through a double-time imitation of the pattern. The bracketed numbers under the heading
most common 78 pattern or as the first of two continuations in “Subdivision” show the metric structure of each category at the
a (2,2,3) structure. Example 10 illustrates a straightforward lowest isochronous pulse level (eighth notes in all but one case;
(4,3) option in Dreadnought’s “To Luminous Scale” and the sixteenth notes are required for the third entry). The
double-continuation (2,2,3) pattern in Dream Theater’s “This “Articulation” column shows the placement of structural kick-
and snare-drum attacks, following the pattern given in the
“Subdivision” column. “K” denotes a kick-drum articulation
32 My assertion that such entrainment occurs is based on my own listening
consistent across all grooves sharing a particular snare pattern;
experience. Large (2008, 222) suggests that neural resonance explains the
cognitive work through which we entrain to regular meters, but notes that “S” indicates the structural snare articulations; “—” does not
“the relationship between complex (nonperiodic) musical rhythms, pulse, imply that a beat is empty, but that a variety of articulations
and meter remains a mystery.” occur across different grooves. In the most represented

This content downloaded from


123.113.229.61 on Sun, 10 Mar 2024 01:45:31 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
236 MUSIC THEORY SPECTRUM 42 (2020)

example 10. Two backbeat-variant possibilities in 167 time. (a) Dreadnought, “ To Luminous Scale” (2017), 2:42. (b) Dream Theater,
“ This Dying Soul” (2003), 10:39

Category Subdivision Articulation Tally Pct. Group Pct.


(K,S)(—,S) 39 36.4%
(2,2,2,1)
Variant of above* 18 16.8%
Quadruple Division (4,4,3,3) (K,S)(—,S) 14 13.1% 72.9%
(2,2,1,2) (K,S)(—,S) 4 3.7%
(2,1,2,2) (K,S)(K,S) 3 2.8%
(4,3) (K,S) 14 13.1%
Half-time groove 14.0%
(3,4) (K,S) 1 0.9%
(K,S,S) 2 1.9%
(3,2,2)
Triple Division (K,—,S) 2 1.9% 5.6%
(2,3,2) (K,S,S) 2 1.9%
Other — — 8 7.5% 7.5%

example 11. Backbeat-variant 78 grooves in the corpus, categorized by subdivisional structure

categories, further sub-categorization could be based on differ- Beautiful Smile” (0:03), and “I Was Brought to My Senses”
ent patterns of kick-drum articulation. (2:51; at a tempo slow enough to suggest a half-time feel).34 If
The most common pattern by far is a quadruple division of we add to this category instances that vary the two-snare-hits-
the measure with snare articulations on the third and seventh per-measure archetype, for instance by articulating the back-
eighth notes of each cycle—mimicking the articulation we find beat with toms or hi-hats, by embellishing the basic drumbeat
in the 44 backbeat up to the final eighth note, which is deleted. with fills, by omitting the second snare articulation, or by dis-
Over one third of the grooves in the corpus do this, as for ex- placing the backbeat by a sixteenth note, the share of grooves
ample in Sting’s “Love Is Stronger than Justice,” transcribed in represented by a (2,2,2,1) structure is over half of all 78 backbeat
Example 12(a), which employs two sub-variants of the pat- variants.
tern.33 Note the differences in cymbal accompaniment (abbre- There are still other ways that rock drummers articulate a
viated quarters on the ride cymbal in the first drumbeat and quadruple structure in 78 time, and all are clear variants of the
hi-hat eighths in the second) and the different patterns in the quadruple backbeat. The next most common option (account-
kick drum. The kick-drum syncopation in the second groove ing for about one eighth of all cases) closes the cycle with two
dotted-eighth notes, suggesting an alternation of 24 and 166
is the most common pattern among 78 backbeat variants, per-
measures. Examples include Alice in Chains’ “Them Bones”
haps because the resulting bisection of the measure affords en-
(0:00) and Yes’ “Changes”; the latter groove is transcribed in
trainment to (or at least flirts with) 167 time. Sting and
Example 12(b).35 Less common are patterns built on
drummer Vinnie Colaiuta are especially fond of this variant,
which also occurs in “St. Augustine in Hell” (0:07), “Like a
34 Colaiuta discusses Sting’s involvement in determining the drumbeats of
33 The tempo of this example (171 bpm) leads to ambiguity concerning the his songs in O’Shea (2016). Other well-known examples of the (2,2,2,1)
tactus level. John Covach teaches the groove in 74 , with a quick quarter- structure include the synth and guitar solos in Rush’s “Tom Sawyer”
note tactus and a half-time drumbeat (Whirty 2007). Because this groove (1:33) and the main groove of The National’s “Demons” (0:00).
affords entrainment in a comfortable range (with a quarter note equal to 35 “Changes” satisfies my definition of groove only minimally, as the tran-
about 86 bpm), I prefer to hear a slow groove with a recurring half-beat scribed measure of 78 is extended to 10
8 upon repetition (i.e., the full cycle
metric deletion. has a cardinality of seventeen). However, the internal consistency of the

This content downloaded from


123.113.229.61 on Sun, 10 Mar 2024 01:45:31 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
using drumbeats to theorize meter in quintuple and septuple grooves 237

example 12. Common drumbeats in 87 time. (a) Sting, “ Love Is Stronger than Justice” (1993), 0:01, 0:22. (b) Yes, “ Changes” (1983),
0:30

isochronous eighth-note structures in which the single-eighth- In 74 meters we continue to observe a prevalence of
note member of the quadruple structure is not in the final po- Platonic-trochaic successions. However, the absence of struc-
sition. Radiohead’s “Paranoid Android” (2:52), for example, tures of twos and threes that I noted for 167 and 78 grooves is
exhibits a kick-snare alternation of (4,3,3,4) in sixteenth notes. If complicated by the attenuation of our metric intuitions at
the mid-measure kick articulation is understood as syncopated, deeper hierarchical levels.40 On the one hand, almost every
anticipating the third quarter-note beat, we find a (2,2,1,2) subdi- groove in the corpus can be transcribed as a measure of 44 plus
visional structure at the eighth-note level. Another common op- one of 34 (in either order), and rock listeners have learned to
tion is the half-time groove, with only a single snare attack near equate the 44 measure with the standard unit of time at that
the midpoint of each cycle.36 In such grooves, as with 167 patterns, level of the metric hierarchy.41 The alternation of the expected
we see that drummers favor Platonic-trochaic (4,3) structures. I 4 3
4 with 4 may thus lead one to interpret a recurring metric dele-
found only a single example of a Platonic-iambic (3,4) subdivi- tion within those triple measures. On the other hand, the
sion—Tori Amos’s “Barons of Suburbia” (0:40), which is more drumbeats through many triple measures follow a double-
an expansion of a 68 groove than a compressed half-time 44 . continuation pattern, supporting an interpretation based on
The most striking trend among 78 backbeat variants is the expansion and marking the triple group as being on par with a
scarcity of triple subdivisions, especially given that much writ- half-measure of 44 (a duple group). At a high level, then, the
ing about 78 time identifies (3,2,2) and (2,2,3) as the default same 74 groove may sound duple (i.e., [4,3]) to a listener who
structures for this meter.37 Backbeat variants that support tri- prioritizes the measure level and triple (i.e., [2,2,3]) to one
ple divisions of the 78 measure are found in only six grooves in who attends to the snare drum.
the corpus: four with a (3,2,2) structure and two that suggest Example 13 shows the prevalence of different options
(2,3,2).38 The absence of (2,2,3) structures is explained by the
alongside potential subdivisional interpretations. Example 14
fact that the most common drum articulation of that subdivi-
illustrates some of the most common patterns. No single
sion is kick–kick–snare, suggesting a half-time interpretation
drumbeat pattern is as pervasive among 74 grooves as what we
of the groove. Other potential instances of this subdivisional
saw in 78 and 167 , but well over half of the grooves begin with
pattern are counted in the quadruple category since, given the
four beats of standard backbeat. The three most common pat-
prevalence of quadruple 78 grooves, this may be the more likely
terns all begin this way, one continuing with a single snare at-
association when a listener encounters an ambiguous pattern.
The absence of (2,2,3) patterns is therefore specifically an ab- tack on beat 6, one with a double continuation marking beats
sence of kick–snare–snare articulations, which would convey 6 and 7, and one with two snare articulations marking beat 6
the triple nature of the subdivision unambiguously.39 and the “and” of beat 7. The former two patterns evoke the
subdivisional ambiguity described above and together consti-
tute the most common backbeat-variant approach to 74 time.
Examples of the single-snare option include Pink Floyd’s
drumbeat through each potential pair of 78 measures is sufficient to estab- “Money” (Ex. 14[a]) and Heart’s “Barracuda” (2:23).
lish the groove. Examples of the double-continuation pattern occur in three
36 A typical tempo for the half-time grooves I identify falls in the range of
Rush singles—“The Spirit of Radio” (2:52), “Limelight”
120–52 bpm. Faster examples require strong contextual cues, such as a
shift from a more normative backbeat variant to the half-time groove in (0:12), and “Time Stand Still” (3:07)—and in Foo Fighters’
question. “Times Like These” (0:13). Example 14(b) shows Blondie’s
37 See Lerdahl and Jackendoff (1983, 69), Hasty (1997, 130–47), Temperley
(2001, 37), and Gotham (2015, [2.5]). 40 On the attenuation of listeners’ metrical acuity, see Cone (1968, 26),
38 The latter two grooves come from Kate Bush’s “Egypt” (2:13) and could Lerdahl and Jackendoff (1983, 99), and Hasty (1997, 175–82).
also be analyzed as (4,3,3,4) at the sixteenth-note level, given the strong Although London (2012, 24) holds that “there is no substantive distinc-
ride-cymbal articulations that bisect each measure. tion between meters and so-called hypermeters,” his discussion of entrain-
39 Coldplay’s “Glass of Water” (1:07) contains measures that follow this pat- ment limitations (e.g., 1–47) suggests the same phenomenon of metric
tern, but they are subsumed within a four-measure cycle involving other attenuation.
articulations. I count the song in my “Other” category and not as a (2,2,3) 41 The relevance of the measure as a span of musical time is affirmed by Iyer
groove. (1998), London (2012), and de Clercq (2016).

This content downloaded from


123.113.229.61 on Sun, 10 Mar 2024 01:45:31 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
238 MUSIC THEORY SPECTRUM 42 (2020)

Category Subdivision Articulation Tally Pct. Group Pct.


(K,S,—,S)(—,S,—) 16 16.0%
(K,S,—,S)(—,S,S) 14 14.0%
Begins with (2,2,3) or (4,3) (K,S,—,S)(—,—,S) 6 6.0%
standard 4/4 59.0%
measure (K,S,—,S)(fill) 2 2.0%
Other 6 6.0%
(4,4,4,2) (K–,S–,—,S–)(K–,S–,K,S) 15 15.0%
(3,4) or (3,2,2) (K,S,—)(—,S,—,S) 9 9.0%
Ends with 4/4
(3,3,3,3,2) (K-k,S--)(K-k,S--,S-) 4 4.0% 16.0%
measure
(3,3,2,2,2,2) (K-k,S--)(K-,S-,—,S-) 3 3.0%
(4,3) (K,—,S,—)(—,—,S) 9 9.0%
Half-time groove 11.0%
(3,4) (K,—,S)(K,—,S,—) 2 2.0%
half- (K,—,S,—)(—,S,—) 5 5.0%
(4,3) 8.0%
time groove (K,—,S,—)(—,–s,—) 3 3.0%
Other — — 6 6.0% 6.0%

example 13. Backbeat-variant 47 grooves in the corpus, categorized by subdivisional structure

example 14. Common drumbeats in 47 time. (a) Pink Floyd, “ Money” (1973), 0:26. (b) Blondie, “ Heart of Glass” (1979), 2:14. (c)
Tori Amos, “ God” (1994), 0:21. (d) Grateful Dead, “ Estimated Prophet” (1977), 0:00

“Heart of Glass,” an example of the third related beat, which Soundgarden’s “Spoonman” (0:34) to Motion City
raises a structural question. Many may argue that the final Soundtrack’s “Boxelder” (0:14), and more than one in seven 74
snare articulation should not be understood as structural since backbeat variants in the corpus adopt this drumbeat.
it destabilizes the otherwise steady quarter-note beat, and the Outside of the pervasive (4,3) category, 74 grooves are likely
interpretations of the two 74 drumbeats just discussed remain to follow one of two approaches: a (3,4) structure, or a half-
available here. However, it is worth acknowledging a less con- time (or partially half-time) groove. About half of the (3,4)
ventional possibility: that the final kick-snare alternation repre- grooves simply reorder the double-continuation (4,3) pattern,
sents the final two beats of a 44 backbeat model, compressed to leading with a 34 measure in alternation with a standard back-
accommodate a 74 meter (hence my subdivisional interpretation beat. Example 14(c), a transcription of Tori Amos’s “God,”
of [4,4,4,2] in Ex. 13). This interpretation follows the trend exemplifies this pattern. Other (3,4) options begin with dotted
among septuple meters toward cycle-ending deletion. rhythms, denoting either syncopation or an initial 68 feel. In
Moreover, at least one other interpretation, in which the final several passages, the dotted rhythm persists into the second
snare attack is understood as a recurring fill, seems dubious in half of the measure rather than yielding to the 44 backbeat.
light of the rarity of other such patterns (only two in the cor- These grooves relate to non-isochronous undifferentiated pat-
pus). The “Heart of Glass” groove is a far more robust option: terns, among which (3,3,3,3,2) was one of the more common
examples range from David Bowie’s “Soul Love” (0:13) to successions.

This content downloaded from


123.113.229.61 on Sun, 10 Mar 2024 01:45:31 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
using drumbeats to theorize meter in quintuple and septuple grooves 239

Subdivision Articulation Tally Pct. Group Pct. default backbeat-variant for 58 . The two obvious options for
(3,2) (K,S) 18 46.2% 46.2% kick-snare alternation—the Platonic-trochaic (3,2) and the
Platonic-iambic (2,3)—are about equally well represented.
(K,S) 13 33.3%
(2,3) 41.0% Example 16 reproduces one drumbeat of each type. The first
embelished 3 7.7% moments of Rush’s “Losing It” pass through several variants
Other — 5 12.8% 12.8% before reaching the transcribed drumbeat. Drummer Neil
Peart enters with a non-isochronous undifferentiated ride-
example 15. Backbeat-variant 85 grooves in the corpus catego- cymbal articulation (3,2), shifts to the transcribed ride part,
rized by subdivisional structure and then adds kick-drum articulations to the initial non-
isochronous pattern. Peart does not settle into the cycle given
in Example 16(a) until over a minute into the song. The sec-
ond drumbeat, from Tool’s “Rosetta Stoned,” is also less con-
sistent than it appears, not because of the sort of buildup
variation seen in “Losing It,” but rather because of variance in
its cycle-to-cycle articulation. The snare drum is sometimes
replaced with a tom; either drum may be echoed on beat 5;
and the kick drum sometimes adds syncopated rhythms be-
tween its downbeat articulations. Despite these local modifica-
example 16. Illustrations of the two principal 85 drumbeats. (a) tions, the structure of the groove remains unaltered.
Rush, “ Losing It” (1982), 1:23. (b) Tool, “ Rosetta Stoned” I observed many distinct drumbeat patterns among the 54
(2006), 0:34 grooves in the corpus. The tactus-level quintuple meter
describes far more grooves than any other irregular meter con-
sidered, and I found considerable support for various subdivi-
Half-time patterns largely follow their 78 relatives, especially sional structures. The clearest trend among these grooves is a
in the prevalence of (4,3) structures over (3,4) ones (only two preference for Platonic-trochaic structures over Platonic-
grooves in nineteen take the latter approach). The most com- iambic ones at the beat level: a (3,2) division of the measure is
mon option simply mimics the most common 78 drumbeat at a more than four times as common as (2,3). Beyond this general
slower tempo. This is the case in much of Grateful Dead’s subdivisional preference, however, drumbeat practice is far less
“Estimated Prophet”; an incipit based on the 28 May 1977 consistent across 54 grooves than in other meters. In my over-
live recording from Hartford, Connecticut is shown in views of 78 and 74 meters, I identified eleven and fourteen drum-
Example 14(d). Also common is a class of grooves where half- beat archetypes, respectively, which were not only specific with
time drum articulations account for part of the cycle: an initial regard to drum articulation, but also consistent for all grooves
half-time 44 measure (half-note kick and half-note snare) is fol- of that type. Conversely, Example 17 (showing 54 grooves) lists
lowed by a triple measure with the snare articulation either on twenty distinct possibilities, many of them variants of more
beat 6, recalling a normative 34 pattern, or the “and” of beat 6, stable patterns. Convincing categorization often proves elusive,
suggesting 68 . In either case, the listener may experience a sense leading to five different categories (at various levels of specific-
of shifting tactus within the groove cycle. ity) that accommodate “Other” possibilities. Together, these
We now turn to quintuple backbeat variants. These gener- miscellaneous categories account for over a quarter of all
ally modify the 44 model via expansion, rather than deletion, backbeat-variant 54 grooves.
and such modifications lead to more disparate options. As a The striking diversity of 54 drumbeats notwithstanding,
result, the quintuple data do not offer the clear default strate- many stable patterns of articulation are well represented in the
gies that characterize septuple meters. Furthermore, the first corpus; Example 18 illustrates seven representative grooves.
category—165 meters—contains only three plausible backbeat Example 18(a), from Jethro Tull’s “Thick as a Brick,” displays
variants (all by Frank Zappa), two of which are so unconven- several elements that can thwart easy classification in the first
tional that their inclusions are debatable.42 large category of Example 17. The first snare attack in the cy-
Because of the shorter measure-length involved, 58 grooves cle consistently falls on beat 2, giving strong support for the
admit fewer distinct patterns than those encountered in 78 . quarter-note beat, but the absence of an articulation on beat 3
Nevertheless, the relative prevalence of each pattern, summa- creates ambiguity regarding both subdivision at the half-
rized in Example 15, makes it clear that there is no single measure level and syncopation against the presumed quarter-
note beat. The former issue is clarified in this groove by a shift
42 The two debatable grooves are from “Zomby Woof” (0:02 and 1:45),
from hi-hat eighth notes to crash-cymbal quarters (supported
where loose backbeat relatives are established through the alternation of
the snare drum (or toms) and cymbals. The clearest 165 backbeat candidate
by the kick-drum rhythm and the guitar riff): the measure is
is a (2,3) alternation of kick and snare drums in “The Radio Is Broken” subdivided as (3,2). We will see in other grooves that subdivi-
(1:01). All three grooves are unconventional, even among irregular meters, sion at this level is not always so clear. Moreover, in the
because they imply a double-time feel. “Thick as a Brick” groove, the dotted rhythm in the kick drum

This content downloaded from


123.113.229.61 on Sun, 10 Mar 2024 01:45:31 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
240 MUSIC THEORY SPECTRUM 42 (2020)

(again coordinated with the guitar riff) encourages a (3,3,2,2) unambiguously, most of these grooves consistently articulate
non-isochronous hearing at the eighth-note level. This possi- beats 2, 4, and 5 with the snare drum. Shown in Example 18(e),
bility is emphasized by the mix, as the kick drum is relatively UK’s “Caesar’s Palace Blues” follows this approach. The slow
prominent and the snare understated. Finally, note that the tempo of this example encourages drummer Terry Bozzio to
snare attack on beat 5 is bracketed, indicating that it is not embellish the basic drumbeat often, sometimes overwriting en-
consistently articulated in the cycle. This is one of many fea- tire measures with fills. Nevertheless, the regular placement of
tures that can preclude a groove’s status as an archetypal drum- the three snare articulations maintains a consistent subdivisional
beat pattern. Several other features of this groove are likewise structure through the first minute of the song.
varied, though none destabilizes the metric structure. As evidence of the importance of all three snare articulations
The same ambiguity encountered in “Thick as a Brick” be- in articulating a (2,3) structure, consider the drumbeat given un-
tween (3,3,2,2) and (2,2,2,2,2) subdivisions at the eighth-note der the “Ambiguous” category in Example 17, with snare hits
level is characteristic of many 54 grooves. Example 18(b), from only on beats 2 and 4 (not on beat 5). This drumbeat is capable
Radiohead’s “Morning Bell,” gives the clearest possible expres- of supporting the same (2,3) structure, as shown in Example
sion of (3,3,2,2), articulating the non-isochronous subdivision 18(f), PJ Harvey’s “Water.” The first four beats of the groove
with alternating kick and snare hits and minimal embellish- follow what we might expect of a square instance of 44 time: a
ment. Continuous eighth notes on the hi-hat fill in the sub- riff divides the time symmetrically into two two-beat halves,
metric pulse layer and are likely interpreted as supporting the supported by simple kick-snare alternation and a regular
same non-isochronous pattern articulated by the drums. The 54 quarter-note hi-hat (with some eighth-note embellishment).
chorus of Porcupine Tree’s “The Start of Something The final beat is seldom articulated by more than the kick
Beautiful,” shown in Example 18(c), presents the same snare- drum. Indeed, one could interpret the structure as (2,2,1). In ei-
drum pattern as “Morning Bell” and a similarly unobtrusive ther interpretation, the first group comprises two beats.
kick-drum part. However, the quarter-note ride cymbal in the Compare this to the situation midway through Underoath’s
former groove suggests a different interpretation than the hi- “We Are the Involuntary” (Ex. 18[g]), where the same snare-
hat eighth notes of the latter. Where the eighth notes assume and-cymbal pattern occurs alongside a riff that suggests a (3,2)
a supporting role, the quarter notes challenge the kick-snare division of the measure with a recurring vocal entry on beat 4.
pattern, potentially even imposing the regular quarter as the This groove follows an unambiguous (3,2) drumbeat with snare
dominant tactus and relegating the snare succession to the sta- hits on beats 3 and 5. In this context, the (3,2) interpretation is
tus of a syncopation (especially for a listener who does not en- likely to prevail, with the beat-four snare attack assuming the
train easily to non-isochronous beats). The importance of this role of a half-time backbeat variant and the beat-two articulation
cue in the cymbal part is such that I have subcategorized playing a non-structural role (stabilizing the quarter-note pulse).
grooves with structural dotted-quarter-note elements in Although this two-snare drumbeat is relatively common (with
Example 17: “Isochronous (q)” denotes the presence of the ten instances in the corpus), its structural implications are incon-
contentious quarter-note pulse; “Non-isochronous” grooves are sistent, unlike most other patterns studied in this article.
those in which the cymbals support the kick-snare structure. Even among half-time 54 drumbeats, there is no clear default
A close relative of the (3,3,2,2) structure with alternating pattern of articulation. While the straightforward alternation
kick and snare articulations places the second snare attack a of kick and snare hits with a (3,2) subdivision is most com-
beat earlier. The resulting double snare-drum continuation mon, only three of the seven passages employ this groove.
implies a triple (3,3,4) structure, though some listeners will Other possibilities include the Platonic-iambic (2,3) subdivi-
likely subdivide the final four-pulse span. Example 18(d) illus- sion (one instance); a (3,3,4) succession led by two kick-drum
trates a clear articulation of this groove in “Spin the Bottle” by articulations, affording either half-time (3,2) or triple entrain-
the Juliana Hatfield Three. This triple drumbeat is less com- ment to the dotted quarter (one instance); and grooves in
mon than its duple (or quadruple at the beat-level) relative, ac- which the snare articulation is delayed until beat 5 (two instan-
counting for over one in nine 54 grooves (11.7 percent). Rarer ces). The designation “(K, S)” under the “Articulation” column
still is the quarter-note ride pattern of “Spin the Bottle.” in Example 17 reflects the one commonality among these
Triple 54 grooves typically involve cymbals that support (or at grooves, the presence of only one snare articulation following a
least do not contradict) the non-isochronous snare articulation. downbeat kick-drum attack. But the absence of an entry show-
This may be related to the already tenuous status of the triple ing the typical subdivision of such cases reinforces the fact that
subdivision. Examples without isochronous cymbal quarter they constitute a microcosm of 54 grooves more generally—a di-
notes are found in Tool’s “Hooker with a Penis” (3:22) and verse collection of drumbeats yielding equally diverse subdivi-
Hozier’s “From Eden” (0:08). sional possibilities.
Grooves with a Platonic-iambic organization at the half-
measure level (2,3) are about as common as the triple structures Polymetric Grooves
just discussed. Unlike the more common Platonic-trochaic cate-
gory, Platonic-iambic grooves are consistent in their drumbeat Because polymetric approaches to irregular grooves are the
patterns. In order to convey the less familiar subdivision least common (with only sixty-nine instances in the corpus),

This content downloaded from


123.113.229.61 on Sun, 10 Mar 2024 01:45:31 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
using drumbeats to theorize meter in quintuple and septuple grooves 241

Category Subdivision Articulation Cymbal accomp. Tally Pct. Group Pct.


(K,—,S)(—,S) 5 4.2%
7.5%
Variant of ^ 4 3.3%
Consistent (K,S,—)(—,S) 2 1.7%
6.7%
trochaic duple (3+2)
High-level Platonic-

quarter-note (2,2,2,2,2) Variant of ^ — 6 5.0% 29.2%


beat (K,S,S)(—,S) 4 3.3%
4.2%
Variant of ^ 1 0.8% 54.2%
Other 13 10.8% 10.8%
Non-isochronous 13 15.3%
Syncopated first (K,S)(—,S) 28.2%
Isochronous (q) 11 12.9%
three beats (3,3,2,2) 25.0%
("10/8" time) Non-isochronous 4 4.7%
Other 7.1%
Isochronous (q) 2 2.4%
Non-isochronous 11 10.1%
High-level triple (3,3,4) (K,S,S) 11.7% 11.7% 11.7%
Isochronous (q) 3 2.8%
(K,S)(—,S,S) 11 9.2%
High-level Platonic- (2,2,2,2,2) — 10.8% 10.8%
Other 2 1.7% 12.5%
iambic duple (2+3)
(2,2,3,3) (K,S)(—,S) Isochronous (q) 2 1.7% 1.7% 1.7%
(K,S,—,S,—) 10 8.3%
Ambiguous — — 9.2% 9.2% 9.2%
Other 1 0.8%
Half-time — (K,S) — 7 5.8% 5.8% 5.8% 5.8%
Other — — — 8 6.7% 6.7% 6.7% 6.7%

example 17. Backbeat-variant 45 grooves in the corpus, categorized by subdivisional structure

example 18. Seven 45 drumbeats. (a) Jethro Tull, “ Thick as a Brick” (1972), 3:06. (b) Radiohead, “ Morning Bell” (2000), 0:00. (c)
Porcupine Tree, “ The Start of Something Beautiful” (2005), 2:09. (d) Juliana Hatfield Three, “ Spin the Bottle” (1993), 0:36. (e) UK,
“ Caesar’s Palace Blues” (1979), 0:00. (f) PJ Harvey, “ Water” (1991), 0:00. (g) Underoath, “ We Are the Involuntary” (2008), 1:48

the observations that we can make are not nearly as robust as elements thereof). The most common situation involves a reg-
those above. Example 19 compares the prevalence of four types ular 44 backbeat in the drums against an irregular groove played
of polymetric interaction in connection with the meters under by the rest of the band. This strategy accounts for nearly half
consideration. The first three types involve a conflict between of the polymetric grooves in the corpus and five-eighths of
the drumbeat and the rest of the band (or one or more those with a quarter-note tactus (54 or 74 ). The second type

This content downloaded from


123.113.229.61 on Sun, 10 Mar 2024 01:45:31 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
242 MUSIC THEORY SPECTRUM 42 (2020)

4/4 backbeat vs. band in Irregular backbeat variant Undifferentiated irreg. Conflicting metric cues
given meter vs. something drums vs. something within the drumbeat
Tally Pct. Tally Pct. Tally Pct. Tally Pct.
7/4 7 58% 3 25% 2 17% 0 0%
5/4 17 65% 3 12% 4 15% 2 8%
7/8 6 35% 2 12% 3 18% 6 35%
5/8 2 18% 5 45% 3 27% 1 9%
7/16 1 50% 1 50% 0 0% 0 0%
5/16 1 100% 0 0% 0 0% 0 0%
Total 34 49% 14 20% 12 17% 9 13%

example 19. Four different polymetric situations in the corpus, categorized by meter

inverts this relationship, expressing the irregular meter in the notes make it easy to hear the meter as oriented around a
drums via a backbeat variant against a conflicting cue in the quarter-note beat—as I have notated it—this layer is under-
band (whether metrically regular or, rarely, in a second irregu- mined by a kick- and snare-drum pattern of seven sixteenths
lar meter). This option is most prevalent in 58 , perhaps because grouped as (3,2,2). It is therefore not difficult to instead hear
the small cardinality of such cycles (and the relatively quick the eighth- or sixteenth-note pulse as primary.
tempo range of the quintuple pulse) makes them easier to Two final examples involve more complex polymetric situa-
maintain when rotating against another meter, but the sample tions. The first comes from a live performance by Frank
is too small to be sure. The third type is similar to the second, Zappa’s band, The Mothers of Invention, spliced onto a studio
differing only in the drum kit’s articulation of the irregular me- recording and released as “Toads of the Short Forest” on the
ter—an undifferentiated pattern instead of a backbeat variant. 1970 album Weasels Ripped My Flesh. Over the course of
This approach does not show a strong correlation with any nearly a minute and a half, the band explores several subtle
specific meter. The fourth and final type of polymeter concerns variations of a multi-layered polymetric texture, growing out of
metric conflict within the drumbeat itself, though it is likely a full-ensemble 58 , as the drummers articulate conflicting
that one or both meters will be supported by another instru- meters against a continuing organ pulse. The first drummer
ment. Examples of this type are subtle, and it is not surprising performs a quick septuple groove, articulating a non-
that their numbers are so few. Those that appeared in the cor- isochronous undifferentiated (2,2,3) succession on the snare
pus were most often in 78 time. drum with the floor tom filling in an eighth-note background.
Example 20 gives incipits of three straightforward polymet- The second drummer plays an isochronous undifferentiated
ric grooves, highlighting the drums and a salient melodic ex- quarter note on the ride cymbal, clarified as 34 through a bass
pression of the other metric layer. Example 20(a), the Bee ostinato in the same meter. After a little over a minute, Zappa
provides the following spoken explanation: “At this very mo-
Gees’ “Jive Talkin’,” shows a melody in 74 with a prominent
ment on stage we have drummer A playing in 78 , drummer B
dotted-quarter-note (3,3,3,3,2) subdivision against a normative
4 playing in 34 , the bass playing in 34 , the organ playing in 58 , the
4 backbeat. All other pitch elements in the groove, including
tambourine playing in 34 , and the alto sax blowing his nose.”
the bass part and harmonic rhythm, support the septuple cycle
Example 21 shows the simplest versions of each potentially
of the melody. The groove continues beyond the transcribed
metric pattern with their initial downbeats aligned. The effect
music for two more 74 measures, realigning on the following
of the recording is less clear than my transcription due to var-
downbeat in both parts. Example 20(b), “Dance on a Volcano”
iations in the articulation of each groove, the quality of the live
by Genesis, demonstrates the third polymetric type in
production, and the fact that the alto saxophone solo does not
Example 19, setting a 44 guitar ostinato against an undifferenti- articulate any meter in particular.44 Indeed, were it not for
ated 78 hi-hat pattern that would be isochronous but for a re- Zappa’s explanation, it would be difficult to discern the careful
curring syncopation late in the septuple cycle. This minimal metric organization of the passage.
interruption of the otherwise steady quarter-note pulse is likely
sufficient to draw the listener’s attention to the odd-cardinality 43 This groove is counted only once in Ex. 19, as an example of the second
pattern. Two cycles of the septuple hi-hat part introduce the type in 167 . It could also be cataloged in the fourth category, in either 74 or
7
section before the guitar entry establishes the polymeter. The 16 . My choice reflects my preference to show the lowest hierarchical level
7:8 ratio between the two layers is fully realized through seven present and to emphasize the conflict between the plausible backbeat vari-
ant and the rest of the band.
cycles of the guitar ostinato and eight of the hi-hat pattern.
44 Issues involving production may go beyond the expected loss of fidelity
Example 20(c) shows both an irregular backbeat variant and a endemic to live recording. The organ is inaudible for much of the passage,
drumbeat with conflicting metric cues in Porcupine Tree’s but Zappa describes its contribution in the present tense, raising the possi-
“The Sound of Muzak.”43 Though the steady hi-hat quarter bility of a faulty transmission of sound from stage to recording equipment.

This content downloaded from


123.113.229.61 on Sun, 10 Mar 2024 01:45:31 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
using drumbeats to theorize meter in quintuple and septuple grooves 243

example 20. Three irregular polymetric grooves. (a) Bee Gees, “ Jive Talkin’” (1975), 1:17. (b) Genesis, “ Dance on a Volcano” (1976),
2:14. (c) Porcupine Tree, “ The Sound of Muzak” (2002), 0:00

first seven beats (up a perfect fourth at first, then a perfect


fifth) suggests two measures of 74 . The septuple pattern gives
way to unclear grouping at the quarter-note level in subsequent
measures, allowing the drumbeat to assume prominence.
However, the same three-measure 58 cycle remains unchanged
throughout the verse (note the subtle variance in snare-drum
and hi-hat placement toward the end of each measure).
Moreover, a hypermetric downbeat in the 58 cycle aligns with
the beginning of the next vocal phrase, whereas the 74 meter
suggested by the opening measures does not. The total repeat-
ing span is thirty quarter notes: four three-measure groups in
example 21. A polymetric groove with three competing metric 5 7
8 , or four measures of 4 with a two-beat comma (or two meas-
possibilities: The Mothers of Invention, “ Toads of the Short
ures of 4 and four of 44 ). The drums might therefore express
7
Forest” (1970), 1:32
the more appropriate choice of meter for the passage, even
A cleaner studio production aesthetic makes it easier to though the vocal grouping initially articulates the more phe-
hear the polymetric elements of the verse groove of Battles’ nomenally salient option through its prominence in the mix
“Ddiamondd,” shown in Example 22. The passage clearly and the comfortable tempo range it occupies.
articulates two irregular meters—74 in the voice, supported by
most of the band, and 58 in the drums. I find it easiest to gravi- summary and conclusion
tate to the cues in the vocal part. The grouping of sixteenth
notes in fours, differentiated by changes of pitch, establishes a Three parameters have guided my analysis of quintuple and
clear quarter-note beat. The approximate transposition of the septuple grooves: (1) the available metric interpretation(s); (2)

This content downloaded from


123.113.229.61 on Sun, 10 Mar 2024 01:45:31 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
244 MUSIC THEORY SPECTRUM 42 (2020)

example 22. A polymetric groove with two strongly articulated irregular elements: Battles, “ Ddiamondd” (2007), 0:00

the implied subdivisional structure or succession, including in considering other irregular meters, one might investigate
categorization as isochronous or non-isochronous; and (3) the whether the patterns I have identified here remain prevalent
nature of the drumbeat or other pattern, where undifferenti- in grooves based on alternating meters (e.g., 44 þ 78 ), or
ated successions and backbeat variants form the default whether subtle differences in metric context correspond to
options. These irregular grooves follow the general preference different drumbeat approaches.45 My findings could also be
for Platonic-trochaic successions observed in other metrically extended by comparing the patterns I have attributed to the
irregular musics, though this tendency is weaker among undif- rock drum kit to the practices of jazz and metal drummers.
ferentiated patterns, where Platonic-iambic successions are Analogous patterns may even be found in musical traditions
more common in 58 and 165 meters. Backbeat variants are the
that do not employ the drum kit, as in classical and various
most common type of drumbeat, accounting for over half of
world musics. Further research might consider the roles of
the grooves surveyed. Isochronous and non-isochronous
other instruments in shaping irregular grooves as well, fol-
structures are about equally common in undifferentiated
lowing the notion of positional listening.46 Study of bass
articulations, while a majority of backbeat variants have some
isochronous element (be it a consistent pulse or beat on the parts would likely dovetail most directly with the perspective
ride or hi-hat cymbals, the deployment of kick and snare I have provided on the drums; moreover, as a typically mono-
drums, or both). Non-isochronous backbeat variants are often phonic instrument, the study of bass parts would pose rela-
ambiguous, as they afford interpretations in which the non- tively few methodological hurdles. As we continue to probe
isochrony is felt as metric as well as syncopated interpreta- the details of metric irregularity and rock drumbeat conven-
tions in which the non-isochrony is felt as a rhythmic layer tions, this study offers tools with which such questions might
that contrasts with an underlying isochronous beat. Finally, be more easily answered.
polymetric grooves show the same preference for the 44 back-
beat characteristic of rock music. If the drums do not articu-
late a regular backbeat, they supply the irregular layer, which 45 Hanenberg (2018, Chapters 5 and 6) addresses drumbeat patterns in
will be undifferentiated or a backbeat variant with about larger irregular cardinalities and isolated irregular measures, within a more
modest corpus than this one.
equal frequency. 46 Positional listening and positional analysis (Spicer and Marvin 2016)
My research suggests several future avenues of inquiry. study the musical realities of different performing musicians within an en-
Each feature that delimited the corpus (meter, genre, instru- semble as distinct from one another and from the audience perspective
ment) could be modified to suit new research. For example, that has traditionally served as an analyst’s point of departure.

This content downloaded from


123.113.229.61 on Sun, 10 Mar 2024 01:45:31 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
using drumbeats to theorize meter in quintuple and septuple grooves 245

works cited Hasty, Christopher. 1997. Meter as Rhythm. New York:


Oxford University Press.
Attas, Robin. 2015. “Form as Process: The Buildup Hennessy, Jeffrey James. 2008. “Fiddle Grooves: Identity,
Introduction in Popular Music.” Music Theory Spectrum 37 Representation, and the Sound of Cape Breton Fiddle
(2): 275–96. Music in Popular Culture.” Ph.D. diss., University of
Beatles Complete Scores, The. 1993. Milwaukee, WI: Hal Toronto.
Leonard. Holt, Fabian. 2007. Genre in Popular Music. Chicago:
Biamonte, Nicole. 2010. “Triadic Modal and Pentatonic University of Chicago Press.
Patterns in Rock Music.” Music Theory Spectrum 32 (2): Iyer, Vijay. 1998. “Microstructures of Feel, Macrostructures of
95–110. Sound: Embodied Cognition in West African and African-
———. 2014. “Formal Functions of Metric Dissonance in American Musics.” Ph.D. diss., University of California,
Rock Music.” Music Theory Online 20 (2). http://www. Berkeley.
mtosmt.org/issues/mto.14.20.2/mto.14.20.2.biamonte.html ———. 2002. “Embodied Mind, Situated Cognition, and
(accessed 22 October 2019). Expressive Microtiming in African-American Music.”
Butterfield, Matthew W. 2006. “The Power of Anacrusis: Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal 19 (3):
Engendered Feeling in Groove-Based Musics.” Music 387–414.
Theory Online 12 (4). http://www.mtosmt.org/issues/mto. Large, Edward W. 2001. “Periodicity, Pattern Formation, and
06.12.4/mto.06.12.4.butterfield.html (accessed 21 October Metric Structure.” Journal of New Music Research 30 (2):
2019). 173–85.
Capuzzo, Guy. 2018. “Rhythmic Deviance in the Music of ———. 2008. “Resonating to Musical Rhythm: Theory and
Meshuggah.” Music Theory Spectrum 40 (1): 121–37. Experiment.” In The Psychology of Time. Edited by Simon
Chor, Ives. 2010. “Microtiming and Rhythmic Structure in Grondin. 189–231. Bingley, UK: Emerald Group
Clave-Based Music: A Quantitative Study.” In Musical
Publishing Ltd.
Rhythm in the Age of Digital Reproduction. Edited by Anne
Large, Edward W. and Mari Riess Jones. 1999. “The
Danielsen. 37–50. Burlington, VT: Ashgate Publishing.
Dynamics of Attending: How We Track Time-Varying
Cohn, Richard. 2016. “A Platonic Model of Funky Rhythms.”
Events.” Psychological Review 106 (1): 119–59.
Music Theory Online 22 (2). http://mtosmt.org/issues/mto.
Large, Edward W. and Caroline Palmer. 2002. “Perceiving
16.22.2/mto.16.22.2.cohn.html (accessed 21 October 2019).
Temporal Regularity in Music.” Cognitive Science 26 (1):
———. 2019. “Meter.” In The Oxford Handbook of Critical
1–37.
Concepts in Music Theory. Edited by Alexander Rehding
Lerdahl, Fred and Ray Jackendoff. 1983. A Generative Theory
and Steven Rings. 207–33. New York: Oxford University
of Tonal Music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Press.
London, Justin. 2012. Hearing in Time: Psychological Aspects of
Cone, Edward T. 1968. Musical Form and Musical
Performance. New York: W. W. Norton. Musical Meter, 2nd ed. New York: Oxford University Press.
de Clercq, Trevor. 2016. “Measuring a Measure: Absolute Mauch, Matthias and Simon Dixon. 2012. “A Corpus-Based
Time as a Factor for Determining Bar Lengths and Meter Study of Rhythm Patterns.” 13th International Society for
in Pop/Rock Music.” Music Theory Online 22 (3). http:// Music Information Retrieval Conference (ISMIR). 163–68.
mtosmt.org/issues/mto.16.22.3/mto.16.22.3.declercq.html Centre for Digital Music, Queen Mary University of
(accessed 21 October 2019). London.
Fabbri, Franco. 1982. “A Theory of Musical Genres: Two McLeod, Kembrew. 2001. “Genres, Subgenres, Sub-
Applications.” In Popular Music Perspectives: Papers from the Subgenres and More: Musical and Social Differentiation
First International Conference on Popular Music Research, within Electronic/Dance Music Communities.” Journal of
Amsterdam, June 1981. Edited by David Horn and Philip Popular Music Studies 13: 59–75.
Tagg. 52–81. Göteborg and Exeter: International Moore, Allan F. 2001. Rock: The Primary Text: Developing a
Association for the Study of Popular Music. Musicology of Rock, 2nd ed. Burlington, VT: Ashgate.
Gotham, Mark. 2015. “Meter Metrics: Characterizing Murphy, Scott. 2016. “Cohn’s Platonic Model and the
Relationships among (Mixed) Metrical Structures.” Music Regular Irregularities of Recent Popular Multimedia.”
Theory Online 21 (2). http://www.mtosmt.org/issues/mto. Music Theory Online 22 (3). http://mtosmt.org/issues/mto.
15.21.2/mto.15.21.2.gotham.html (accessed 25 October 16.22.3/mto.16.22.3.murphy.html (accessed 21 October
2019). 2019).
Gracyk, Theodore. 1996. Rhythm and Noise: An Aesthetics of Negus, Keith. 1999. Music Genres and Corporate Cultures. New
Rock. Durham, NC: Duke University Press. York: Routledge.
Hanenberg, Scott. 2018. “Unpopular Meters: Irregular Osborn, Brad. 2010. “Beats that Commute: Algebraic and
Grooves and Drumbeats in the Songs of Tori Amos, Kinesthetic Models for Math-Rock Grooves.” Gamut 3 (1):
Radiohead, and Tool.” Ph.D. diss., University of Toronto. 43–68.

This content downloaded from


123.113.229.61 on Sun, 10 Mar 2024 01:45:31 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
246 MUSIC THEORY SPECTRUM 42 (2020)

———. 2017. Everything in Its Right Place: Analyzing Temperley, David. 1999. “Syncopation in Rock: A Perceptual
Radiohead. New York: Oxford University Press. Perspective.” Popular Music 18 (1): 19–40.
O’Shea, Kev. 2016. “Vinnie Colaiuta Explains Sting’s ‘Seven ———. 2001. The Cognition of Basic Musical Structures.
Days.’” YouTube video, 04:35. https://www.youtube.com/ Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
watch?v¼GwmCeon6aPo (accessed 1 September 2018). ———. 2011. “The Cadential IV in Rock.” Music Theory
Rosenberg, Nancy. 2011. “Popular Music in the College Online 17 (1). http://www.mtosmt.org/issues/mto.11.17.1/
Music Theory Class: Rhythm and Meter.” In Pop-Culture mto.11.17.1.temperley.php (accessed 21 October 2019).
Pedagogy in the Music Classroom: Teaching Tools from ———. 2018. The Musical Language of Rock. New York:
American Idol to YouTube. Edited by Nicole Biamonte. Oxford University Press.
47–71. Toronto: Scarecrow Press. Toussaint, Godfried T. 2013. The Geometry of Musical
Spicer, Mark and Elizabeth Marvin. 2016. “Positional Rhythm: What Makes a “ Good” Rhythm Good? Boca Raton,
Listening/Positional Analysis.” Special session presented at FL: Taylor and Francis Group.
the Annual Meeting of the Society for Music Theory, 3–6 Traut, Don. 2005. “‘Simply Irresistible’: Recurring Accent
November, Vancouver, BC. Patterns as Hooks in Mainstream 1980s Music.” Popular
Stephenson, Ken. 2002. What to Listen for in Rock. New Music 24 (1): 57–77.
Haven: Yale University Press. Whirty, Ryan. 2007. “Rock ‘n’ Roll Professor.” Rochester
Tamlyn, Garry. 1998. “The Big Beat: Origins and Development Review 69 (5). https://www.rochester.edu/pr/Review/
of Snare Backbeat and Other Accompanimental Rhythms in V69N5/feature3.html (accessed 24 October 2019).
Rock’n’Roll.” Ph.D. diss., University of Liverpool.

Music Theory Spectrum, Vol. 42, Issue 2, pp. 227–46, ISSN 0195-6167,
electronic ISSN 1533-8339. V C The Author(s) 2020. Published by Oxford

University Press on behalf of The Society for Music Theory. All rights
reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com.
DOI: 10.1093/mts/mtaa005

This content downloaded from


123.113.229.61 on Sun, 10 Mar 2024 01:45:31 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

You might also like