You are on page 1of 10

Accepted: 27 July 2022

DOI: 10.1002/jsc.2521

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Bargaining our emotions: Exploring the lived experience


of purchasing luxury fashion counterfeit

Aurore C. Bardey1 | Rose Turner2 | Patrice Piccardi1

1
Burgundy School of Business, Université
Bourgogne Franche-Comté, CEREN EA 7477, Abstract
Dijon, France
The study identified contrasting experiences of purchasing and owning luxury fashion
2
University of the Arts of London, London
College of Fashion, London, UK counterfeit items: while some participants carefully considered and were ultimately
satisfied with their counterfeit purchase, others impulsively purchased and later reap-
Correspondence
Aurore C. Bardey, Burgundy School of
praised and regretted their decision. The findings underlined the importance of con-
Business, Université Bourgogne Franche- sumer perceptions of quality to luxury and luxury counterfeit fashion products. They
Comté, CEREN EA 7477, Dijon, France.
Email: aurore.bardey@bsb-education.com
showed how education about the unethical features of the counterfeit industry could
change consumer behavior.

1 | I N T RO DU CT I O N 2 | LI T E RA T U R E RE V I E W

Counterfeit is defined as the use of a trademark (a sign or symbol used Several factors involved in prompting an individual to make a counter-
by a particular brand to distinguish the brand from others, such as feit luxury purchase have been documented in the literature. Eisend
Nike's iconic swoosh logo) on an item, which is identical to a trade- and Schuchert-Güler (2006) identified four main factors involved in
mark registered to another party (Bian & Moutinho, 2009). Counter- counterfeit purchasing: (1) person (demographic and psychographic
feit occurs at all fashion market levels, from independent brands to characteristics), (2) product (price, product attributes, scarcity);
well-known iconic luxury brands, and has adverse economic effects (3) social and cultural context; (4) and situation (purchase situation,
(Bian et al., 2016). The counterfeit fashion market was valued at $464 consumer's mood). Thaichon and Quach (2016) refined and developed
billion in 2019 (Shoaib & Maguire, 2021), and fashion sales losses this initial classification by proposing a set of external and internal
from counterfeit goods reached 26.3 billion euros worldwide in 2020 motives for buying counterfeit. The authors articulated seven internal
(Statista, 2021). As a result, counterfeiting represents a pervasive motives: (1) sense of adventure (risk and adventure-seeking); (2) fash-
issue in the luxury fashion industry. ion/novelty seeker (need to be on-trend or seek fashion novelty);
The digital revolution has provided unprecedented access to (3) sense of morality; (4) perception of inequality (purchasing counter-
luxury fashion counterfeits (Bian & Veloutsou, 2017; Islam feit to fight against branding and inequality in society); (5) perception
et al., 2021; Vida, 2007), and the luxury industry has been of the actual product; (6) quality acceptance; (7) purchasing experi-
attempting to fight this key issue in collaboration with social ence; and nine external motives: (1) social acceptance (approval of
media channels; for example, through joint lawsuits against coun- one's action or behavior within the social group); (2) peer influence;
terfeiters (e.g., the Gucci-Facebook lawsuit; Luxury Daily, 2021). (3) sense of belonging/desired image (when a consumer would like to
In addition to digital access, technological innovations have be seen as part of a group, trend, fashion or social class); (4) perceived
increased luxury fashion counterfeit access and mis-spelling risks associated with purchase (sense of safety during the purchase);
(Hall, 2020). More than ever, developing knowledge of the con- (5) perceived risks related to usage (sense of embarrassment if others
sumer behavior involved in luxury fashion counterfeit purchasing identify the counterfeit nature of the item); (6) affordability of the
is needed to equip the luxury sector with the knowledge needed product; (7) accessibility (degree of freedom and access from the
to more effectively address this issue. Within this context, the consumer); (8) degree of justice; and (9) access via social networking
present research aims to tackle an unexplored aspect of counter- sites. These factors have received support from several studies
feit consumer behavior: the lived and emotional experience of (Bian et al., 2015; Bian et al., 2016; Bian & Moutinho, 2009; Bian &
purchasing luxury fashion counterfeit. Moutinho, 2011; Furnham & Valgeirsson, 2007; Jiang & Cova, 2012;

Strategic Change. 2022;1–10. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/jsc © 2022 John Wiley & Sons Ltd. 1
2 BARDEY ET AL.

Kim et al., 2012; Phau & Teah, 2009; Poddar et al., 2012; Tang Emotions and fashion are also known to be intertwined
et al., 2014; Wilcox et al., 2009). (Masuch & Hefferon, 2014; Moody et al., 2010; Smith & Yates, 2018),
While most counterfeit-related research had focused on better particularly in that purchasing can lead to a specific pattern of emo-
understanding pre-purchase factors, the last decade has seen an tions (based on dimensions of hostility, surprise and interest;
increasing interest in understanding the post-purchase factors Westbrook & Oliver, 1991). More recently, research has examined
involved in counterfeit purchases. Recent study has explained how purchase-related emotions in the context of counterfeit purchasing,
consumers attempt to reduce the unfavorable consequences of with several studies indicating feelings of guilt, anger and shame after
the counterfeit purchase by (1) twisting, that is, reframing, the having purchased counterfeit (Elsantil & Hamza, 2019; Kapferer &
actual scenario of purchasing; (2) hiding the counterfeit product or Valette-Florence, 2022; Liao et al., 2010; Sharma & Chan, 2016; Viot
not using it in front of “experts” (fashion experts and/or wealthy et al., 2014). Explanations for these negative emotions include an
people); (3) buying “limited edition” or “latest collection” counter- unpleasant post-counterfeit-purchase experience (e.g., inferior quality
feit in order to get a recognizable item; (4) buying an as-similar-as- of the item or regret), or cognitive dissonance arising from the con-
possible counterfeit version of a genuine product (Agarwal & sumer's ethical standards in contrast to the perceived unethical
Panwar, 2016; Amaral & Loken, 2016; Bian et al., 2016; Gistri dimensions in purchasing counterfeit (Baumeister et al., 1994;
et al., 2009; Pueschel et al., 2017; Tang et al., 2014). Li et al. Freedman et al., 1967; Gregory-Smith et al., 2013).
(2020) suggested that consumers use these strategies in order to Interestingly, Martinez and Jaeger (2016) showed that emotions
construct and maintain their social identity (“part of an individual's are not always negative when purchasing counterfeit. Indeed, while
self-concept which derives from his/her knowledge of his/her high-moral awareness participants indicated feelings of guilt, low
membership of a social group (or groups) together with the value moral awareness participants reported emotions of gratitude. More-
and emotional significance attached to that membership”; over, Chaudhry and Stumpf (2011) showed that positive hedonic
Tajfel, 1978, p. 63). This finding is consistent with Penz and Stot- emotions can be found in consumers willing to purchase counterfeit
tinger (2005) who suggested that social identity can play a role in movies or pharmaceuticals.
purchasing luxury counterfeits. The impact of social identity in Zampetakis (2014) quantified luxury counterfeit purchases' posi-
status-oriented consumption, along with value for money, is a criti- tive and negative emotions. In the study, participants were asked to
cal factor in luxury counterfeit consumerism (Ang et al., 2001; think carefully about their most recent luxury counterfeit purchase
Furnham & Valgeirsson, 2007). For example, consumers might (within the last 6 months) and then to indicate their emotions con-
spend an affordable amount to purchase a counterfeit lookalike of cerning their counterfeit purchase via a Likert scale. Where emotions
an authentic trending luxury product, generating a sense of were present, participants experienced both positive and negative
belonging to an affluent or high-status group (Harvey & feelings, with the highest average (albeit moderate) scores in happi-
Walls, 2003; Staake & Fleisch, 2008; Thaichon & Quach, 2016; ness and pride (2.14 and 2.44 out of 5, respectively), and distress, guilt
Wilcox et al., 2009). and shame (2.21, 2.43 and 2.48 out of 5, respectively). Scores of
Self-concept and fashion are known to be intertwined. Individ- 1 (“very slightly or not at all”) for a given emotion taken to indicate an
uals' self-concepts have been found to be central to fashion consump- emotion was not felt, and 72.2% of participants reported not having
tion, with clothing practice functioning as a mechanism for self- felt fear, 61.9% anger, 43.3% distress, 35.9% guilt, 42.3% shame,
expression and self-perception (Entwistle, 2000; Masuch & Hefferon, 13.8% interest, 25.7% happiness, and 59.6% pride. As a consequence,
2012; Moody et al., 2010; Nessim & Bardey, 2022). Materialistic pos- Zampetakis (2014) showed that consumers could feel both negative
session plays a key role in self-identity construction and can be and positive emotions when purchasing counterfeit, and several
viewed as an extension of the self (Belk, 1988). This phenomenon is researchers (Eisend & Schuchert-Güler, 2006; Kim et al., 2009;
reflected in purchasing behaviors: consumers with high self-esteem Zampetakis, 2014) have highlighted the importance of exploring the
have been shown to be more inclined to purchase authentic brands emotional aspects of purchasing counterfeit items.
that convey higher-status self-identity (Yoo & Lee, 2009) and to have Over the last decade, promising research exploring the impact of
a greater tendency to buy items with a logo (Wilcox et al., 2009). Fur- counterfeit on self-perception and emotions has begun to emerge,
thermore, purchasing counterfeit products can increase aspirational though gaps remain to be addressed. On the one hand, Gino et al.
consumers' willingness to pay for their genuine counterparts (2010) showed the impact of wearing counterfeit on the self, but the
(i.e., consumers will initially buy a counterfeit item and then buy the impact of counterfeit purchases on self-identity remains unclear. On
actual product when they have enough money) as well as helping to the other hand, Zampetakis (2014) quantified emotions related to
boost their self-image (Bekir et al., 2013; Romani et al., 2012). Gino counterfeit purchasing. However, the study description highlighted
et al. (2010) found a “counterfeit self” effect in which wearing coun- the unethical dimensions of counterfeit (described as “illegal, low-
terfeit items was found to lead to more cheating behaviors. This priced and often lower quality” as well as “other names such as rep-
research inquiry represents the only study to assess the impact of licas, imitation, bogus, fakes, copy, or knock-off”). It may have
counterfeit on the self to date and highlights an interesting avenue for impacted participants' recollections of their emotions. In light of previ-
future research. ous findings and remaining gaps in the literature, we aimed to explore
BARDEY ET AL. 3

T A B L E 1 Participant demographic characteristics, favorite brands, fashion involvement questionnaire scores (product development
involvement, purchase decision involvement, advertising involvement and consumption involvement; O'Cass, 2000)

Occupational Monthly spend


Pseudo Age Gender status on fashion (€) Favorite brands Counterfeit items purchased (price in €)
Anna 25 Female Employed 100 M&S Chanel small purse (30€)
Chanel
Jimmy Choo
Betty 23 Female Student 60–500 Zadig & Voltaire Louis Vuitton & Hermes bags, Burberry scarfs (n.k.)
Mango
Naf Naf
Elisa 34 Female Student 60 Beyond Retro Chanel cardigan (10€)
Chanel
Oxfam
James 21 Male Student 50–100 CK CK tee-shirt and boxers (100€ in total) & Ralf Lauren wallet
Hollister (20€)
Desigual
Jane 21 Female Student 60 Kate Spade Christian Dior saddlebag (30€)
Forever 21
Shein
Mary 22 Female Employed 50 SMR Clothes & Jewelry (n.k.)
The Kooples
Zara
Max 20 Male Student 200–250 Raf Simons Clothes from Dior, Off-White, Chanel, Balenciaga (20–60€ per
Kusikohc item)
Ader Error
Rachel 21 Female Employed 350 P. Mills Stella McCartney bag (40€)
Etro
A. McQueen
Sarah 24 Female Student 500 Miumiu Chanel and Louis Vuitton Jewelry (n.k.)
Chanel
Dior
Victoria 25 Female Student 150 Gina Tricot Clothes and bag (10–50€ per item)
Ralph Lauren
Lacoste

Abbreviation: n.k., not known.

the lived experience of purchasing luxury counterfeit by investigating 50 to 500€ per month on fashion items and were able to name at least
the impact on consumers' self-identity and post-purchase emotions. one favorite luxury brand, indicating an interest in authentic luxury
fashion. The study was granted ethics approval at the London College
of Fashion (UK) and adhered to the British Psychological Society's
3 | METHODS guidelines for treating human participants, and pseudonyms were
used to ensure anonymity.
3.1 | Participants

Ten participants, two men and eight women, aged between 20 and 3.2 | Procedure
34 (Table 1) were recruited using opportunity and snowball sampling.
This sample size was appropriate for our qualitative methodology and After providing informed consent, participants were asked to state
based on the sample's homogeneity (Boddy, 2016; Marshall their favorite brands and list their counterfeit purchases over the
et al., 2013; Smith et al., 2009), and in line with previous qualitative past 2 years. Then, participants took part in a 1:1 semi-structured
research in emotion and fashion (Carr & Mercer, 2017; Fleetwood- interview lasting approximately 40 min. We aimed to understand
Smith et al., 2019; Sadkowska et al., 2015; Smith & Yates, 2018; participants' experiences of non-deceptive practice (i.e., knowingly
Townsend & Sadkowska, 2017). Three participants were early-career purchasing counterfeits; Grossman & Shapiro, 1988) to assess the
employees, and seven were students living in France or England, who motivations and processes involved (in line with Bian &
had knowingly purchased at least one counterfeit luxury fashion prod- Moutinho, 2011). Open-ended questions such as “Can you tell me
uct in the past 2 years. Participants reported spending an average of about the emotions you felt when purchasing/wearing the
4 BARDEY ET AL.

counterfeit item?,” were used to elicit rich, exploratory data (Smith “Right after the purchase, I was using it very often. I
et al., 2009), and questions were constructed to probe the got compliments on it. The bag fitted everything in, it
emotions involved in purchasing counterfeit and genuine luxury was multipurpose.” (Rachel)
fashion product(s), participants' social identity and their extended
self-perception. Participants were verbally debriefed following the The positive emotional responses to finding a bargain were imme-
interview. diate and strong enough that they felt like a “serotonin release” (Jane).
However, two participants, Mary and Sarah, experienced a lack of
emotions in response to purchasing and then using counterfeit items.
3.3 | Data analysis The participants used everyday metaphors such as “stuff,” “things,”
and “object,” highlighting the insignificance of the item purchased.
Interpretive phenomenological analysis (IPA; a hermeneutic approach Both participants described purchasing their items to complete stylish
that aims to explore “how people make sense of their major life clothes: for them, the clothing was important, but the accessories
experiences,” Smith et al., 2009, p. 1, in this context, participants' were not. The aim was, therefore, to purchase appropriate accessories
emotions and thoughts about purchasing and wearing counterfeit lux- to complement an outfit without spending a high price.
ury items) was used to analyze the interview data due to its emphasis
on complex and subjective experience (as opposed to quantifying the
occurrence of patterns), following the procedure outlined by Smith 4.2 | Post-purchase negative emotions
et al. (2009) and Willig (2013). First, the interviews were transcribed,
and each transcript was read several times in order to identify rele- Participants showed two different post-purchase emotions follow-
vant and descriptive words and phrases. Next, systematic and critical ing the initial peak in positive emotions. For some participants, the
reading of the transcripts identified emergent themes, which were initial positive emotions were followed by negative emotions. Four
grouped, developed and summarized, and then consolidated across out of the 10 participants (Anna, Rachel, Elisa and James) reported
the transcripts to interpret shared themes. that negative emotions gradually outweighed the positive until
they only felt negative emotions toward their counterfeit pur-
chase. “Disappointment,” “guilt,” and “upset” were linked with the
4 | RESULTS item's diminishing superficial quality, which, they felt, made it
appear counterfeit rather than authentic.
The main outcomes of our data analysis are presented in Figure 1. We
found that most participants initially experienced positive emotions, “In the end, quality was definitely an issue. I kept it for
followed either by negative emotions, continued positive emotions or a little while but didn't wear a lot. The shape wasn't
a return to neutral emotions. Participants reflected on how counter- very good. It was a size too big. The colour was not
feit or luxury items reflected their self-identity. nice.” (Elisa)

“I am so disappointed by the quality of the bag, which


4.1 | Initial positive emotion of finding a bargain is now falling apart. The quality of this bad was good
for a year, but then it started to look a bit tatty. I don't
While Rachel, Victoria, James and Max bought their counterfeit items use it anymore.” (Rachel)
at a local market, the other six participants purchased theirs on the
internet (retail websites or social media channels). Victoria, Max and The inferior quality of the counterfeit item generated some early
Rachel bought their counterfeit items as part of a social event, such as negative emotions and ultimately, in some cases, rejection of the item.
going to the local market with family or mother-daughter shopping, Furthermore, subsequent negative associations emerged, experienced
whereas James went shopping on his own. Despite these differences as “guilt” and “shame.” These emotions were connected to the
in the purchase environment, most participants expressed initial posi- broader (non-item-specific) working conditions of the counterfeit
tive emotions during the moment of purchase and immediately after industry:
the purchase:
“But to be honest, soon after buying this purse, I've
“When I received it, I was happy with the purchase, I decided not to purchase a counterfeit item anymore
was happy with the quality. (…) When I opened the because it makes me feel guilty because of how
box, I felt very happy, it was a bargain pur- they've been made. The more I thought about it, the
chase.” (Anna) more I thought about what can happen in other coun-
tries that produce counterfeit, that does make me feel
These initial positive feelings translated into a high level of usage: guilty.” (Anna)
BARDEY ET AL. 5

FIGURE 1 Thematic analysis map Impulsive purchase

Negative feelings Faking the


- Guilt - self

Inferior quality
Purchase Positive feelings
OR
counterfeit “Bargain”

Positive feelings Embracing the


- Enjoyment - self

Excellent quality

Deliberate purchase

“By buying counterfeit, I'm feeding the wrong side of “Now, when I wear my authentic C* tee-shirts and
poverty. I feel like it's wrong to support unsafe work underwear, I feel so good with myself.” (James)
labour. I feel like I'm supporting illegal work by pur-
chasing counterfeit. (…) I felt guilty due to the impact All participants who purchased counterfeit experienced negative
of counterfeit, unsafe supply chain, child labour, etc.” emotions overwhelming the initial positive emotions soon after the
(Rachel) purchase. This emotion was associated with the disintegration of the
item's quality over time, alongside knowledge of unethical aspects of
The negative emotions experienced by participants were associ- the counterfeit market. Ultimately, these negative feelings were suffi-
ated with an understanding of sustainable fashion and ethics in the cient to instigate changes in behavior in favor of purchasing more
fashion industry, including knowledge about counterfeit supply chains, expensive authentic products, which would engender sustained, posi-
production and materials. The more participants learned about sus- tive emotions.
tainable fashion and the counterfeit industry, the more they regretted
their purchase. Education in sustainability and counterfeit processes
was the key source of this regret: 4.3 | Post-purchase positive emotions

“I've started asking myself questions when I was at uni- While Rachel, Anna, Elisa and James, described the development of
versity studying sustainability. The things I learned at negative emotions in arising from their counterfeit purchases, two
university had more of an impact on me feeling guilty participants (Victoria and Max) did not share this emotional
rather than the quality declining. (…) My attitude experience:
towards counterfeit changed completely with the
knowledge I gained.” (Anna) “I was happy about my purchase. For me, it was a good
deal. I'm not ashamed to say that I buy counterfeit
Although ethical concerns may have been present at the point of because I buy genuine brands like Supreme or what-
purchase, the unease was initially superseded by positive emotions ever. I'm not ashamed and I have no problem with that.
arising from the acquisition of a bargain. Subsequently, feelings of For me, the downside is finding a good quality, inex-
remorse led to decisions to behave differently in future; namely, by pensive item.” (Victoria)
purchasing better quality genuine fashion items. Rachel and James
highlighted how this behavior change led to more sustaining positive Victoria and Max carefully chose their counterfeit items; their
and validating emotions: purchases took time and were not impulsive. Both participants
explained that their counterfeit items remain high quality, and both
“Now I hate the fact that I bought it. By then, I was continue to wear items purchased up to 5 years ago. The differences
impatient and greedy; but with a bit more patience, in participants' emotional responses appear to be related to percep-
you can save money and buy the real thing. Now that I tions of item quality. While the four participants who expressed nega-
earn money, I'd rather work hard, buy myself the real tive emotions did not pay attention to the item's quality at the point
thing and be proud of it.” (Rachel) of purchase, Victoria and Max felt that they had sufficient expertise
6 BARDEY ET AL.

to detect quality and “go-to [online] addresses” wherein to locate them. “I think I have a lot of dissonance about it because it's
Victoria explained, “I know my way around. I know what I'm buying,” not usual for me. And I think this is where all the nega-
and both Victoria and Max described the excellent quality, style, and tivity comes from. What does it say about me when I
cut of the counterfeit items: wear something that is fake and not real? I think it
doesn't reflect nicely on myself.” (Elisa)
The quality is amazing. I'm still wearing items bought
three years ago. No one even noticed my items were Participants worried that the counterfeit could negatively affect how
counterfeit. Some of the items are just perfect. Like my others would perceive them; as Anna explained: “I'm scared that some-
counterfeit trousers, I tried to find the same cut and one will notice that it's counterfeit and they will think I'm a horrible per-
style in fashion stores, but I couldn't find it. The trou- son … which I'm not.”
sers are just perfect. These participants were attracted to the authenticity of genuine
luxury brands as luxury items were felt to reflect better who they are:
Participants who deliberately and carefully purchased luxury fash- “The negative feeling went from the lack of authenticity. I don't really
ion counterfeit did not express any post-purchase negative emotions. want to wear something fake. And I think this is why I didn't want to
On the contrary, the participants remained extremely content with wear that often.” (Elisa).
their purchase as the counterfeit items' quality did not diminish The participants who expressed positive emotions about purchas-
over time. ing counterfeit also valued finding products that suited their style and
An appreciation of ethical issues factored not only in feelings of identity. However, for them, this outcome could be achieved by pur-
regret post-purchase but also in Victoria and Max's positive appraisals. chasing counterfeit items. In this way, counterfeit functioned to
Here, however, the ethics of the luxury, rather than counterfeit, mar- enable them to embrace their fashion identity.
ket was meaningful; namely, unfair and inappropriate high prices for
poor-quality luxury items. Max felt manipulated by expensive, genuine “Buying things that I couldn't have been able to pur-
brands: chase as genuine items allowed me to really express
myself with my style, it allowed me to express who I
“Some (genuine) brands are not worth the money. The am.” (Max)
quality is not that good. They manipulate us. I don't
find it ethically right. I know what the product really “My fashion style represents my identity. For example,
costs, and the brands increased the price so much, it's the perfect trousers I mentioned, they are like my
too much. Doubling the price from factory to selling is ‘style signature’, my friends talked about it, they repre-
not fair, and not worth it.” (Max) sent my identity.” (Max)

Following the initial, in-the-moment positive response, two par- Max and Victoria understood that purchasing counterfeit can be
ticipants, Jane and Betty, felt neither particularly positive nor particu- illegal but reasoned that the high prices of some luxury products justi-
larly negative about their counterfeit purchases. Again, this sentiment fied this course of action. Rather than simply pitting counterfeit
was related to the item's quality: the participants lost interest in their against authentic products, the participants illustrated multiple dimen-
counterfeit items as quality decreased. A willingness to find good- sions through which an item can be appraised, such as quality and
quality counterfeit for a reasonable price can be attributed to the par- style. Therefore, their emotional associations were not based on
ticipants' lack the expertise or “go-to addresses” of Max and Victoria. whether or not the product was counterfeit.
Development of expertise might lead to more sustaining positive
emotions in the future. “Whether I'm wearing real brands or a fake item, I feel
the same. There is no difference.” (Victoria)

4.4 | Styling one's self with counterfeit “The style is what defined the item, which is why I
didn't say to my friends it was counterfeit when I gave
The participants who expressed negative emotions about purchas- them the counterfeit items. Counterfeit is just one
ing counterfeits explained that buying counterfeits did not repre- aspect of the items. Quality and style are more impor-
sent the person they were. For these participants, the counterfeit tant when considering these items.” (Max)
purchase became linked to their sense of self. As expressed by
Elisa below, there was a discrepancy between who they are and Participants who experienced negative post-purchase emotions
what they felt they represented while wearing counterfeit. There highlighted dissonance between the counterfeit items and their sense
were feelings of dissonance between their social self and their of self, and this emotional state led to a feeling of faking the self. In
inner self as though they were faking their actual self while wear- contrast, participants who experienced sustained positive post-
ing counterfeit: purchase emotions identified dimensions of uniqueness and quality
BARDEY ET AL. 7

that enabled them to express their sense of style and self-using coun- participants who regretted their purchases discussed ethical issues in
terfeit items. the counterfeit industry (including working conditions and sustainabil-
ity issues). These factors were considered significant enough to war-
rant participants to assert that they would no longer purchase
5 | DISCUSSION counterfeits; in other words, it changed their behavior. In contrast,
the participants who experienced no regret talked about the ethics of
The present research aimed to investigate the impact of purchasing the luxury industry in which high prices were considered not to reflect
luxury fashion counterfeits on consumers' post-purchase emotions quality. Their purchases were justified as good quality counterfeits
and self-concept. The results showed that participants initially felt were likely to last longer (and therefore be more sustainable) than
positive about attaining a financial bargain when purchasing their their overpriced luxury counterparts.
counterfeit luxury items (and product affordability has been identified These results are coherent with Martinez and Jaeger (2016)
as an internal motive for purchasing counterfeit; Thaichon & who found that high-moral awareness participants experienced
Quach, 2016). Following the initial positive response, participants' emotions of guilt while low moral awareness participants reported
experiences diverged along three dimensions. emotions of gratitude. In our study, some participants' moral
First, participants who impulsively purchased a counterfeit item awareness was focused on the ethics of the counterfeit market,
soon developed negative emotions of regret and guilt, consistent with while other participants' moral awareness was focused on the
previous research (Elsantil & Hamza, 2019; Liao et al., 2010; ethics of the luxury industry. While all participants intended to buy
Sharma & Chan, 2016; Viot et al., 2014). These participants had not an as-similar-as-possible counterfeit version of a genuine product,
paid attention to the item quality when purchasing and were more the participants did not attempt to reduce the unfavorable conse-
focused on an iconic style or brand identity. Subsequently, the dimin- quences of the counterfeit purchase by reframing the purchasing
ishing quality of the items impacted their post-purchase emotions, scenario, hiding the counterfeit product, not using it in front of
leading to feelings associated with regret. Negative perceptions of the “experts,” or buying “limited editions” to get a recognizable item
items were compounded by knowledge of the ethical implications of (Agarwal & Panwar, 2016; Amaral & Loken, 2016; Bian et al., 2016;
purchasing counterfeits. On the other hand, participants who carefully Gistri et al., 2009; Pueschel et al., 2017; Tang et al., 2014). Instead,
selected their counterfeit products, considering both style and quality, our results revealed two contrasting approaches: either reasoning
did not develop negative associations with their purchases. Instead, that counterfeit purchasing was ethical (because high-priced low-
the participants were satisfied with the quality and prices of their quality luxury products are unethical) or, after regretting their pur-
items. Finally, a minority of participants lost interest in their items as chase, committing to no longer purchase counterfeits due to gain-
item-quality was seen to diminish. These participants did not experi- ing new knowledge about sustainable fashion and the counterfeit
ence negative feelings but described an interest in seeking out quality industry.
items at reasonable prices. An explanation for this could be a lack of The participants who experienced post-purchase dissatisfac-
expertise to find good quality items at the desired price. tion and satisfaction differed in how they felt the counterfeits
The subsequent emotions toward purchasing counterfeit seemed expressed their identities. Whereas the former was concerned that
to be dependent on whether the purchase was experienced as the “fake” items would generate negative connotations about
impulsive—leading to reappraisal and resulting in negative emotions themselves, the latter viewed the counterfeits as supportive and
about the purchase—or carefully considered. Most importantly, the expressive of their unique styles. For all participants, therefore,
present study highlighted that item quality is the key factor involved the counterfeits were experienced as expressive, although only the
in post-purchase emotions and satisfaction in luxury fashion counter- participants who had deliberated over their purchases perceived
feit purchases. the purchases as reflective of their true selves. This outcome aligns
In addition to quality, item accessibility seems to be an important with previous studies which have indicated that clothing practices
factor in purchasing counterfeit, which impacts consumers' emotions represent self-expression mechanisms and convey one's inner self
and satisfaction post-purchase. Thaichon and Quach (2016) identified and self-perception (Entwistle, 2000; Masuch & Hefferon, 2012;
“accessibility” (the freedom and access of the consumer) as one of the Moody et al., 2010; Nessim & Bardey, 2022). Our findings contrib-
external motives for purchasing counterfeit. The present study sup- ute to this literature by exploring the impact of purchasing coun-
ported this finding, highlighting how the inaccessibility of genuine lux- terfeit on participants' self-perception. While participants
ury products (in terms of location or cost) can lead to purchasing dissatisfied with the quality of their items and the ethical problems
counterfeits instead. For the participants who regretted their pur- of the counterfeit market felt uncomfortable wearing them as they
chases, the accessibility of counterfeits led to an impulsive purchase did not represent their genuine self, those who were satisfied with
decision. In contrast, the participants who were satisfied with their their items used the unique style of the counterfeit as a mecha-
counterfeits made their purchases based on their style preferences nism for expressing their own fashion identity. This outcome ech-
which were not met at a suitable price in the luxury market. oes Tassel et al.'s (2022) finding that wearing unsustainable
In line with Thaichon and Quach's (2016) model, most participants clothing led to negative emotions while wearing sustainable dress
expressed the importance of ethics in purchasing counterfeits. The led to positive emotions.
8 BARDEY ET AL.

6 | CONCLUSIONS, MARKETING While the present study underlines interesting outcomes and routes
I M P L I C A T I O N S , A N D F U T U R E R E S EA R C H for marketing implications, future research would help to understand lux-
ury counterfeit consumer behavior further. First, our participants were all
This study identified contrasting experiences of purchasing and own- from Western countries, and a future study could replicate our study
ing luxury fashion counterfeit items. While some participants carefully with Eastern cultures where the counterfeit market represents a differ-
considered the purchasing process and were ultimately satisfied with ent stage of development. Second, we need to explore the generalizabil-
their counterfeit purchase, others impulsively purchased and later ity of our findings by quantifying the contrasting experiences of
reappraised and regretted their decision. These emotional responses purchasing and owning luxury fashion counterfeits and exploring the
were linked to the relationship between the purchasers' self-concept psychological factors that may underpin dichotomic post-purchase emo-
and their ownership of counterfeit. Participants chose either to stop tions. Finally, future research may shed light on the potential benefits of
purchasing luxury counterfeit because the fake items did not feel rep- educating consumers about ethical dimensions concerning the luxury
resentative of their true selves, or they chose to carry on purchasing fashion counterfeit market.
luxury counterfeit because these items fitted with their defined fash-
ion style thus representing who they are. Knowledge of sustainability RE FE RE NCE S
and the ethics of counterfeit fashion was central to the negative Agarwal, S., & Panwar, S. (2016). Consumer orientation towards counter-
appraisal of counterfeit purchases. feit fashion products: A qualitative analysis. IUP Journal of Brand Man-
agement, 13(3), 55–74.
In contrast, ethical issues in the luxury industry supported posi-
Amaral, N. B., & Loken, B. (2016). Viewing usage of counterfeit luxury
tive perceptions of purchasing a luxury counterfeit. Perceptions of
goods: Social identity and social hierarchy effects on dilution and
product quality and accessibility contributed to positive and nega- enhancement of genuine luxury brands. Journal of Consumer Psychol-
tive appraisals of counterfeit items. The study highlighted the ogy, 26(4), 483–495.
importance of consumer perceptions of quality to luxury and luxury Ang, S. H., Cheng, P. S., Lim, E. A., & Tambyah, S. K. (2001). Spot the differ-
ence: Consumer responses towards counterfeits. Journal of Consumer
counterfeit fashion products and indicated that education about
Marketing, 18(3), 219–235.
the unethical features of the counterfeit industry could change Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (1994). Guilt: An
consumer behavior. interpersonal approach. Psychological Bulletin, 115(2), 243–267.
Digital-technological innovation and counterfeit have been found Belk, R. W. (1988). Possessions and the extended self. Journal of Consumer
Research, 15(2), 139–168.
to be interlinked. On the one hand, digital and technology innovation
Bekir, I., El Harbi, S. & Grolleau, G. (2013). How a luxury monopolist might
has helped fight against counterfeit with tools such as holograms, benefit from the aspirational utility effect of counterfeiting? European
luminescent topcoats, blockchain technologies, and the creation of Journal of Law and Economics, 36(1), 169–182.
start-ups like Ennoventure, which allows consumers to check whether Bian, X., Haque, S., & Smith, A. (2015). Social power, product conspicuous-
their luxury product is genuine or not directly (Singh, 2021; ness, and the demand for luxury brand counterfeit products. British
Journal of Social Psychology, 54(1), 37–54.
Slater, 2020). On the other hand, the counterfeit market takes advan-
Bian, X., & Moutinho, L. (2009). An investigation of determinants of counter-
tage of digital and technological innovation to ease accessibility, feit purchase consideration. Journal of Business Research, 62(3), 368–378.
increase image capture and item reproduction speed and maintain the Bian, X., & Moutinho, L. (2011). Counterfeits and branded products:
“quality” of counterfeit items (Meraviglia, 2016; National Research Effects of counterfeit ownership. Journal of Product & Brand Manage-
ment, 20(5), 379–393.
Council, 2007). While the continued development of digital and tech-
Bian, X., Wang, K. Y., Smith, A., & Yannopoulou, N. (2016). New insights
nological barriers is required to combat counterfeiting, the present into unethical counterfeit consumption. Journal of Business Research,
study highlighted new approaches to addressing the issue. Our results 69(10), 4249–4258.
showed that educating consumers on the unethical aspects of the Bian, X., & Veloutsou, C. (2017). Consumers' attitudes regarding non-
deceptive counterfeit brands in the UK and China. In Advances in Chi-
counterfeit market may convince consumers to stop buying counter-
nese brand management (pp. 331–350). Palgrave Macmillan.
feit and that this approach may be particularly effective with partici- Boddy, C. R. (2016). Sample size for qualitative research. Qualitative Mar-
pants already experiencing dissatisfaction with their previous ket Research: An International Journal., 19(4), 426–443.
counterfeit purchases. High quality, uniqueness, rarity, and heritage Carr, D. J., & Mercer, J. (2017). Young people's experiences of fashion
modelling: An exploratory phenomenological study. International Jour-
are some traditional luxury codes (Dubois et al., 2001; Heine, 2012;
nal of Fashion Studies, 4(1), 51–68.
Keller, 2009) that may attract even more consumers to the genuine Chaudhry, P. E., & Stumpf, S. A. (2011). Consumer complicity with coun-
luxury market. Interestingly, however, the same luxury traditional terfeit products. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 28(2), 139–151.
codes, that is, uniqueness, rarity, and thus lack of accessibility, may Dubois, B., Laurent, G., & Czellar, S. (2001). Consumer rapport to luxury:
Analysing complex and ambivalent attitudes (Consumer research work-
convince the consumers satisfied with previous counterfeits to find
ing paper no. 736). HEC.
an as-similar-as possible quality item for a lower price via the counter-
Eisend, M., & Schuchert-Güler, P. (2006). Explaining counterfeit pur-
feit market. For these consumers, transparency about price policy for chases: A review and preview. Academy of Marketing Science Review,
genuine luxury items may support luxury purchasing, although this 12, 1–25.
openness does contradict traditional luxury codes (Dubois Elsantil, Y., & Hamza, E. A. (2019). The impact of self-conscious emotions
on willingness to pay for sustainable products. Humanities & Social Sci-
et al., 2001; Heine, 2012; Keller, 2009).
ences Reviews, 7(2), 77–90.
BARDEY ET AL. 9

Entwistle, J. (2000). Fashion and the fleshy body: Dress as embodied prac- Masuch, C. S., & Hefferon, K. (2014). Understanding the links between
tice. Fashion theory, 4(3), 323–347. positive psychology and fashion: A grounded theory analysis. Interna-
Fleetwood-Smith, R., Hefferon, K., & Mair, C. (2019). ‘It's like … it's me’: tional Journal of Fashion Studies, 1(2), 227–246.
Exploring the lived experience of clothing attachment during wear. Meraviglia, L. (2016). Technology and counterfeiting: Friends or foes? Global
International Journal of Fashion Studies, 6(1), 41–62. Fashion Conference.
Freedman, J. L., Wallington, S. A., & Bless, E. (1967). Compliance without Moody, W., Kinderman, P., & Sinha, P. (2010). An exploratory study: Rela-
pressure: The effect of guilt. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- tionships between trying on clothing, mood, emotion, personality and
ogy, 7(2), 117–124. clothing preference. Journal of Fashion Marketing and Management: An
Furnham, A., & Valgeirsson, H. (2007). The effect of life values and materi- International Journal, 14(1), 161–179.
alism on buying counterfeit products. The Journal of Socio-Economics, National Research Council. (2007). A path to the next generation of US
36(5), 677–685. banknotes: Keeping them real. National Academies Press.
Gino, F., Norton, M. I., & Ariely, D. (2010). The counterfeit self: The decep- Nessim, Y., & Bardey, A. (2022). The rise of female empowerment in Egypt:
tive costs of faking it. Psychological Science, 21(5), 712–720. The fashion psychology behind their attire & armour. Fashion marketing in
Gistri, G., Romani, S., Pace, S., Gabrielli, V., & Grappi, S. (2009). Consump- emerging economies—Strategies, tools, and insights for fashion brands.
tion practices of counterfeit luxury goods in the Italian context. Journal Palgrave Macmillan.
of Brand Management, 16(5), 364–374. O'Cass, A. (2000). An assessment of consumers product, purchase deci-
Gregory-Smith, D., Smith, A., & Winklhofer, H. (2013). Emotions and disso- sion, advertising and consumption involvement in fashion clothing.
nance in ‘ethical’ consumption choices. Journal of Marketing Manage- Journal of economic psychology, 21(5), 545–576.
ment, 29(11–12), 1201–1223. Penz, E., & Stottinger, B. (2005). Forget the Areal@ thingbtake the copy! An
Grossman, G. M., & Shapiro, C. (1988). Foreign counterfeiting of status explanatory model for the volitional purchase of counterfeit products.
goods. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 103(1), 79–100. ACR North American Advances.
Hall, C. (2020). The pandemic has created a ‘perfect storm’ for counterfeits. Phau, I., & Teah, M. (2009). Devil wears (counterfeit) Prada: A study of
Just ask Louis Vuitton. BoF Business of Fashion. antecedents and outcomes of attitudes towards counterfeits of luxury
Harvey, P. J., & Walls, W. D. (2003). Laboratory markets in counterfeit brands. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 26(1), 15–27.
goods: Hong Kong versus Las Vegas. Applied Economics Letters, 10(14), Poddar, A., Foreman, J., Banerjee, S. S., & Ellen, P. S. (2012). Exploring
883–887. the Robin Hood effect: Moral profiteering motives for purchasing
Heine, K. (2012). The concept of luxury brands. Bartek Goldmann & Kate counterfeit products. Journal of Business Research, 65(10), 1500–
Vredenburgh. 1506.
Islam, T., Pitafi, A. H., Akhtar, N., & Xiaobei, L. (2021). Determinants of pur- Pueschel, J., Chamaret, C., & Parguel, B. (2017). Coping with copies: The
chase luxury counterfeit products in social commerce: The mediating influence of risk perceptions in luxury counterfeit consumption in
role of compulsive internet use. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Ser- GCC countries. Journal of Business Research, 77, 184–194.
vices, 62, 102596. Romani, S., Gistri, G., & Pace, S. (2012). When counterfeits raise the appeal
Jiang, L., & Cova, V. (2012). Love for luxury, preference for counterfeits—A of luxury brands. Marketing Letters, 23(3), 807–824.
qualitative study in counterfeit luxury consumption in China. Interna- Sadkowska, A., Wilde, D., & Fisher, T. (2015). Third age men's experience
tional Journal of Marketing Studies, 4(6), 1–9. of fashion and clothing: An interpretative phenomenological analysis.
Kapferer, J. N., & Valette-Florence, P. (2022). The myth of the universal Age, Culture, Humanities: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2(1), 33–68.
millennial: comparing millennials' perceptions of luxury across six Sharma, P., & Chan, R. Y. (2016). Demystifying deliberate counterfeit pur-
countries. International Marketing Review, 39(2), 149–165. chase behaviour: Towards a unified conceptual framework. Marketing
Keller, K. L. (2009). Managing the growth tradeoff: Challenges and oppor- Intelligence & Planning, 34(3), 318–335.
tunities in luxury branding. Journal of Brand Management, 16(5), Shoaib, M., & Maguire, L. (2021). How independent brands are resisting
290–301. counterfeits. Vogue Business.
Kim, J. E., Cho, H. J., & Johnson, K. K. (2009). Influence of moral affect, Singh, A. (2021). This innovative start-up aims to crack the counterfeit conun-
judgment, and intensity on decision making concerning counterfeit, drum. SME Futures.
gray-market, and imitation products. Clothing and Textiles Research Slater, F. (2020). Digital vs physical—Which anti-counterfeiting technology is
Journal, 27(3), 211–226. best? Packaging Europe.
Kim, J., Kim, J. E., & Park, J. (2012). Effects of cognitive resource availabil- Smith, J., Flowers, P., & Larkin, M. (2009). Interpretative phenomenological
ity on consumer decisions involving counterfeit products: The role of analysis: Theory, method and research. Sage Publications.
perceived justification. Marketing Letters, 23(3), 869–881. Smith, R., & Yates, J. (2018). Flourishing fashion. Fashion Studies, 1(1),
Li, J., Ghaffari, M., & Su, L. (2020). Counterfeit luxury consumption strate- 5–44.
gies in a collectivistic culture: The case of China. Journal of Brand Man- Staake, T., & Fleisch, E. (2008). Countering counterfeit trade: Illicit market
agement, 27(5), 546–560. insights, best-practice strategies, and management toolbox. Springer Sci-
Liao, C., Lin, H. N., & Liu, Y. P. (2010). Predicting the use of pirated software: A ence & Business Media.
contingency model integrating perceived risk with the theory of planned Statista. (2021). Sales losses from counterfeit goods worldwide in 2020, by
behavior. Journal of Business Ethics, 91(2), 237–252. retail sector.
Luxury Daily. (2021, 27 April). Gucci, Facebook file joint lawsuit against Tang, F., Tian, V. I., & Zaichkowsky, J. (2014). Understanding counterfeit
alleged counterfeiter. consumption. Asia Pacific Journal of Marketing and Logistics, 26(1),
Martinez, L. F., & Jaeger, D. S. (2016). Ethical decision making in counter- 4–20.
feit purchase situations: The influence of moral awareness and moral Tajfel, H. E. (1978). Differentiation between social groups: Studies in the
emotions on moral judgment and purchase intentions. Journal of Con- social psychology of intergroup relations. Academic Press.
sumer Marketing, 33(3), 213–223. Tassell, C., Bardey, A., & Schat, A. (2022). How to wear happiness: Impact
Marshall, B., Cardon, P., Poddar, A., & Fontenot, R. (2013). Does sam- of wearing clothing labelled sustainable or fast fashion on subjective
ple size matter in qualitative research?: A review of qualitative well-being. International Journal of Sustainable Fashion & Textiles, 1(1),
interviews in IS research. Journal of Computer Information Systems, 63–80.
54(1), 11–22.
10 BARDEY ET AL.

Thaichon, P., & Quach, S. (2016). Dark motives-counterfeit purchase frame- AUTHOR BIOGRAPHIES
work: Internal and external motives behind counterfeit purchase via digi-
tal platforms. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 33, 82–91.
Townsend, K., & Sadkowska, A. (2017). Textiles as material Gestalt: Cloth Aurore C. Bardey is an Associate Professor in Marketing at the
as a catalyst in the co-designing process. Journal of Textile Design Burgundy School of Business, France.
Research and Practice, 5(2), 208–231.
Vida, I. (2007). Determinants of consumer willingness to purchase non- Rose Turner is a Lecturer in Psychology at the London College of
deceptive counterfeit products and the European Union. Managing Fashion, UK.
global transitions, 5(3), 253.
Viot, C., Le Roux, A., & Kremer, F. (2014). Attitude towards the purchase Patrice Piccardi is an Associate Professor in Marketing at the Bur-
of counterfeits: Antecedents and effect on intention to purchase. gundy School of Business, France. He is also the head of the Mar-
Recherche et Applications en Marketing, 29(2), 3–31. keting Department at the Burgundy School of Business and the
Westbrook, R. A., & Oliver, R. L. (1991). The dimensionality of consump-
MSc Luxury Management and Innovation.
tion emotion patterns and consumer satisfaction. Journal of Consumer
Research, 18(1), 84–91.
Wilcox, K., Kim, H. M., & Sen, S. (2009). Why do consumers buy counter-
feit luxury brands? Journal of Marketing Research, 46(2), 247–259.
Willig, C. (2013). Introducing qualitative research in psychology (3rd ed.).
How to cite this article: Bardey, A. C., Turner, R., & Piccardi, P.
Open University Press.
Yoo, B., & Lee, S. H. (2009). Buy genuine luxury fashion products or counter- (2022). Bargaining our emotions: Exploring the lived
feits? ACR North American Advances. experience of purchasing luxury fashion counterfeit. Strategic
Zampetakis, L. A. (2014). The emotional dimension of the consumption of Change, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1002/jsc.2521
luxury counterfeit goods: An empirical taxonomy. Marketing Intelli-
gence & Planning, 32(1), 21–40.

You might also like