You are on page 1of 3

3001

curred when scent was used at the direction traveled by regular foragers
o. 2S0 x0
a
experimental sites (Fig. 4), but did not in the field, the presence of this infor-
x
occur at the control site when foragers mation in the hive does not appear to
z
x collected unscented sucrose at the ex- contribute to the ecology of foraging
200-
=
VI
0
0.
0 perimental sites (Fig. 3). or recruitment (3).
0
Neither the odor of feeding bees nor ADRIAN M. WENNER
X 150--
the odor from the scent gland provided Department of Biological Sciences,
the problems anticipated (10). No site University of California,
co'0 100 had odor in the food on days 4, 9, and Santa Barbara 93106
14; and recruitment of bees was lowest PATRICK H. WELLS
so. on each of these days. This indicates Department of Biology, Occidental
that searching bees had to be very close College, Los Angeles, California 90041
lo 20 30 40
-0-
s0 to feeding and landing bees before they DENNIS L. JOHNSON
Clove oil (drops/liter) could use either the odor or the visual Department of Life Sciences,
Fig. 5. Data obtained from preliminary ex- pattern of flying or feeding bees. Ap- U.S. Air Force Academy,
periments testing the effect of the amount parently, the attraction afforded by for- Colorado Springs, Colorado 80840
of scent in the food on the incidence aging bees (13) was used only after the
of Nassanoff gland exposure (days 18 recruits had chemotactically oriented to References and Notes
through 22). Station No. 1 (0) had 50, the food odor (or distinctive location 1. K. von Frisch, Osterr. Zool. Z. 1, 1 (1946);
6, 0, 20, and 50 drops of oil of clove per translation, Bull. Anim. Behav. 5, 1 (1947).
liter of solution; and station No. 3 (X) odor) at that site. 2. K. von Frisch, Tanzsprache und Orientierung
had 0, 20, 50, 6, and 0 drops per liter, The degree of exposure of the scent der Bienen (Springer, New York, 1965),
respectively, on the 5 days of odor varia- gland varied inversely with recruitment translation, The Dance Language and Orien-
tation of Bees (Harvard Univ. Press, Cam-
tion. (Table 1), and it appeared that the use bridge, Mass., 1967).
3. A. M. Wenner, P. H. Wells, F. J. Rohlf,
of unscented sucrose solution contrib- Physiol. Zool. 40, 317 (1967).
uted to a high rate of gland exposure. 4. A. M. Wenner and D. L. Johnson, Anim.
Behav. 14, 149 (1966); D. L. Johnson and
landed only reluctantly at the control To determine whether there is a rela- A. M. Wenner, ibid., p. 261; D. L. John-
station. According to Kalmus (13), this tion between amount of odor in the son, ibid. 15, 487 (1967).
5. N. G. Lopatina, Pchelovodstvo 84, 34 (1964).
is due to a lack of adequate visual and food and rate of gland exposure, we 6. K. von Frisch, Z. Vergl. Physiol. 21, 1 (1934).
olfactory stimuli generated by the flight varied the amount of odor in the solu- -7. P. H. Wells and J. Giacchino, J. Apicult. Res.
7, 77 (1968).
activity and odors of foraging bees. To tion at the two experimental sites dur- 8. K. R. Popper, in British Philosophy In the
prevent bees from inspecting and reject- ing a 5-day period after our 17-day Mid-Century, C. A. Mace, Ed. (MacMillan,
New York, 1957).
ing the middle station because of the sequence. The results (Fig. 5) indicate 9. D. L. Johnson, Science 155, 844 (1967); A.
lack of a necessary "landing factor," M. Wenner, ibid., p. 847.
that the level of exposure of the scent
10. A. M. Wenner and D. L. Johnson, ibid. 158,
we lowered an insect net over the re- 1076 (1967).
gland can be adjusted by altering the
11. K. von Frisch, ibid., p. 1072.
luctant recruits as they hovered near amount of odor in the food. This may
12. M. Renner, Z. Vergl. Physiol. 43, 411 (1960);
the dish. This prevented them from ar- D. A. Shearer and R. Boch, J. Insect Phy-
also explain why bees do not expose
siol. 12, 1513 (1966).
riving at the control station and pro- their scent glands when visiting natural
13. H. Kalmus, Brit. J. Anim. Behav. 2, 63 (1954).
ceeding upwind to one of the experi- 14. R. A. Morse and A. W. Benton, Bee World
food sources such as flowers (16).
45, 141 (1964).
mental sites (usually No. 1). However, Three concepts have been examined
15. L. J. Friesen and M. Iacaboni, unpublished
most recruits landed at the dish, at- results.
in the above experiments: odor accu-
16. C. R. Ribbands, The Behaviour and Social
tracted in part by the visual stimulus mulation in the hive, attractiveness of
Life of Honeybees (Dover, New York, 1964).
of bee-sized pieces of cellulose sponges 17. E. M. Schweiger, Z. Vergl. Physlol. 41, 272
Nassanoff secretion, and the usefulness
(1958); A. M. Wenner, Anim. Behav. 10,
placed around the inside circumference of the olfaction hypothesis in predicting
79 (1962).
of the dish. Care used in transferring 18. Supported by NSF grant GB-6448. We thank
the field distribution of recruited bees.
P. Craig, J. Fawcett, L. Friesen, and M.
bees to the alcohol bottle prevented the Our results show that, although ele-
Iacaboni for technical assistane and D.
release of alarm odor (14). Davenport and D. Smith for reviewing the
ments of the dance maneuver in the
manuscript.
Our results (Table 1) support the hive do correlate with the distance and
22 October 1968; revised 31 December 1968 a
olfaction hypothesis and contradict the
dance language hypothesis (Table 1 and
Fig. 3). Recruits came to the site
marked by the food odor but not neces- Pan-Cultural Elements in Facial Displays of Emotion
sarily to the sites presumably indicated
in the hive by the dance maneuvers of Abstract. Observers in both literate and preliterate cultures chose the predicted
returning foragers. This was true, even emotion for photographs of the face, although agreement was higher in the
when the odor had not been used since literate samples. These findings suggest that the pan-cultural element in facial
the previous day. Other experiments displays of emotion is the association between facial muscular movements and
with a different hive in another location, discrete primary emotions, although cultures may still differ in what evokes
in which experimental and control sites an emotion, in rules for controlling the display of emotion, and in behavioral
were at different distances (370 and 150 consequences.
m, respectively), yielded comparable
results (15). In studies in New Guinea, Borneo, these cultures recognize some of the
Our results also support the odor the United States, Brazil, and Japan we same emotions when they are shown a
accumulation hypothesis. The linear in- found evidence of pan-cultural elements standard set of facial photographs. This
crease in recruitment per unit time oc- in facial displays of affect. Observers in finding contradicts (i) the theory (1) that
86 SCIENCE, VOL. 164
facial displays of emotion are socially to obtain data also from visually iso- word from a list of six affects for each
learned and therefore culturally vari- lated cultures, preferably preliterate picture. In the United States, Brazil, and
able; and (ii) the findings from studies cultures. Japan, slides were projected one at a
within a single culture that observers Photographs were selected from over time for 20 seconds each to groups of
of the face alone do not achieve 3000 pictures to obtain those which freshmen college students from whom
either accuracy or high agreement in showed only the pure display of a sin- the foreign-born had been eliminated.
recognizing different emotional states gle affect. The selection was guided The photographic prints (13 by 18 cm)
(2). by a study in which Ekman, Friesen, were shown one at a time to each ob-
Bruner and Taguiri (3) said: "The and Tomkins (7) developed a procedure server in New Guinea and Borneo. The
best evidence available [from 30 years for scoring facial affects that was based affect words were translated into the
of research] seems to indicate that on a compilation of lists of cues par- locally understood languages (Japanese,
there is no invariable pattern (or at ticular to each primary affect. The scor- Portuguese, Neo-Melanesian Pidgin,
least no innate invariable pattern of ing procedure had not been completed Fore, and Bidayuh). There were no
expression) accompanying specific emo- when the photographs were selected Neo-Melanesian Pidgin equivalents for
tions." In contrast, our findings support for this cross-cultural study, but the disgust-contempt or surprise, and in
Darwin's (4) suggestion that facial ex- partial lists provided the basis for these cases a phrase was submitted
pressions of emotion are similar among choosing pictures which contained cues (looking at something which stinks,
humans, regardless of culture, because distinctive for happiness, surprise, fear, looking at something new).
of their evolutionary origin. anger, disgust-contempt, and sadness. For our isolated, non-Western pre-
Our study was based in part on Tom- This list of affects includes all of Tom- literate samples we attempted to find
kins' (S) theory of personality, which kins' primary affect categories except those least affected by the modern tech-
emphasized the importance of affect for interest and shame; it also includes nological, commercial, and ideological
and which postulated innate subcortical almost all of the affect states, dis- currents. The New Guinea sample was
programs linking certain evokers to criminable within any one culture. the Fore linguistic-cultural group (8)
distinguishable, universal facial displays The most common reasons for reject- who until 12 years ago were an iso-
for each of the primary affects-inter- ing photographs were that they showed lated Neolithic material culture. We
est, joy, surprise, fear, anger, distress, the influence of display rules or blends studied the Fore most influenced by
disgust-contempt, and shame. Ekman of the cues of one affect with those of contacts with Westerners (government,
and Friesen (6) reasoned that past im- one or more other affects rather than missionaries, and others) as well as
pressions of cultural differences in single-affect pictures. Thirty photo- those least influenced by these recent
facial displays of affect may represent graphs met our criteria; they showed contacts who have preferred to remain
a failure to distinguish what is pan- male and female Caucasians, adults in their isolated hamlets in the moun-
cultural (the association of facial mus- and children, professional and amateur tains.
cular movements with each primary actors, and mental patients. The stim- We report in detail only on the most
affect) from what is culturally variable uli were reproduced as 35-mm slides Westernized Fore; we summarize the
(learned affect evokers, behavioral con- and photographs (13 by 18 cm) cropped results on the less Westernized Fore,
sequences of an affect display, and the to include only the face and neck. whose unfamiliarity with certain tasks
operation of display rules). T'he observers' task was to select a required development of specialized
Display rules were defined as proce-
dures learned early in life for the man-
agement of affect displays and include Table 1. Rates of recognition of six affects among samples from the United States, Brazil,
deintensifying, intensifying, neutraliz- Japan, New Guinea, and Borneo.
ing, or masking an affect display. These New Guinea*
rules prescribe what to do about the Affect category United Brazil Japan Pidgin Fore Borneo*
display of each affect in different social responses responses
settings; they vary with the social role Happy (H) 97 H 97 H 87 H 99 H 82 H 92 H
and demographic characteristics, and Fear (F) 88 F 77F 71 F 46F 54F 40F
should vary across cultures. 26Su 31 A 25A 33Su
Disgust-contempt 82 D 86 D 82 D 29 D 44 D 26 Sa
To uncover the pan-cultural elements (D) 23 A 30 A 23 H
in facial displays of affect, the investi- Anger (A) 69 A 82 A 63 A 56 A 50 A 64 A
gator must obtain samples (photo- 29D 14D 22F 25F
Surprise (SU) 91 Su 82 Su 87 Su 38 Su 45 F 36Su
graphs) of facial expression that are 30F 19A 23F
free of cultural differences because of Sadness (SA) 73 Sa 82 Sa 74 Sa 55 Sa 56 A 52 Sa
learned evokers, display rules, and con- 23 A
sequences. We attempted to select such Number of observers
99 40 29 18 14 15
photographs and to prove that observers Number of stimuli for which most frequent response was predicted response
from different cultures recognize the 30/30 30/30 29/30 11/24 12/24 18/23
same affect from the same photograph. Number of stimuli for which 70 percent of the observers agreed,
Because similarities in the recognition 25/30 26/30 23/30 7/24 6/24 6/23
of emotion among literate cultures Chi-squaref
10,393 3818 2347 532 261 427
might be attributed to learning their Chi-square excluding happy stimulit
own or each other's facial affect cues 5718 2119 1241 188 92 211
from a shared visual source (television, * A few photographs, mostly happy pictures, were eliminated in work with preliterate observers in
movies, or magazines), it was necessary order to make the task shorter. t All chi-squares were significant beyond P=.01.
4 APRIL 1969 87
judgment procedures and conducting a of observers in more than one of the 8. D. C. Gaidusek, Trans. Roy. Soc. Trop. Med.
Hyg. 57 (No. 3), 151 (1963); B. R. Sorenson
number of additional experiments. preliterate samples. Our studies of and D. C. Gajdusek, Pediatrics 37 (No. 1), 149
There were two subsamples in the most other much less Westernized Fore ob- (1966).
9. C. E. Izard, "The emotions and emotion con-
Westernized Fore; one subsample per- servers yielded similar results, with the structs in personality and culture research," in
formed the judgment task by using exception of the sadness category, and Handbook of Modern Personality Theory, R.
D. Cattell, Ed. (Aldine, Chicago, in press).
Pidgin translations of the affect terms, we also obtained additional support in 17 October 1968; revised 16 January 1969
and the other subsample used the af- studies in progress on how these affects
fect terms of their own Fore language. are expressed in the Fore. The possi-
The Borneo sample was the Sadong, bility that the data on 'the preliterate
a Bidayuh-speaking group of Hill samples might have been biased by the
Dyaks in southwest Sarawak. These use of Caucasoid faces as stimuli was Retrograde Amnesia in Free Recall
people still lived in their traditional negated by additional studies in which
long houses and maintained their tradi- Melanesian (South Fore) faces were Abstract. Supervention of high-pri-
tional agrarian way of life. Only one shown to the South Fore observers and ority events in a series of events con-
man spoke English, most men spoke results similar to those reported here stituting a free-recall task interferes
some Malay, and many had seen a were obtained. The proposition that with postexposure processing of mne-
few movies in a commercial center there are pan-cultural elements in hu- monic information about immediately
located about a day's walk from their man affect displays appears to be preceding events, with the result that
village. largely supported, both in the literate recall of these preceding events is im-
The distribution of six responses to cultures that we and Izard have studied, paired. Recall of immediately follow-
each category (affect) of photographs and for the most part in the preliterate ing events is not affected. This retro-
was' tallied, and the most frequent cultures that we have investigated. grade interference is time dependent.
judgment response for each affect cate- Those who deem it important to have
gory was converted into a percentage maximum control for shared visual in- Retrograde amnesia refers to selec-
of the total responses to the stimuli put to limit the opportunity to learn tive impairment of memory for events
which represented that category (Table common affect recognitions might still preceding a critical "amnestic" event.
1). The data from the three literate want the further evidence on the less The magnitude or the extent of such
samples support our contention of a Westerized samples of Fore to be re- impairment varies directly with the
pan-cultural element in facial affect dis- ported later. temporal proximity between the amnes-
play. Agreement and accuracy were far PAUL EKMAN tic event and the events whose retention
higher in each group than had been Langley Porter Neuropsychiatric is measured. Known and putative am-
reported for recognition of emotions Institute, San Francisco, nestic events or treatments include
within cultures, and the same affect California 94122 concussion, electroconvulsive shock,
term was the most frequent response E. RICHARD SORENSON local brain stimulation, anesthesia, an-
in the United States and Brazil for all National Institute of Neurological oxia, and administration of various
of the stimuli and for 29 out of the 30 Diseases and Blindness, drugs (1). The action of amnestic
stimuli when Japan is compared. Three Bethesda, Maryland 20014 events is usually interpreted in terms of
literate cultures are not a sufficient WALLACE V. FRIESEN the disruption of consolidation of the
sample to proclaim universality; how- Langley Porter Neuropsychiatric engrams or memory traces of events
ever, Izard (9), who worked indepen- Institute preceding the amnestic event, but al-
dently at the same time as we, but with References and Notes ternative interpretations have also been
his own set of facial photographs ob- 1. For example, 0. Klineberg, Social Psychology offered (2).
(Holt, New York, 1940); W. La Barre, J. Per-
tained results for eight other literate sonality 16, 49 (1947). Understandng of retrograde amne-
cultures that are substantially the same 2. Although the semantic dimensions which may sia is of considerable theoretical im-
underlie the judgment of emotions are similar
as ours. across cultures, it has not been demonstrated portance. The advancement of such
When exposure to common visual that the face displays the same emotion in understanding depends on availability
the same way across cultures. H. Schlosberg,
input is controlled (to answer the argu- Psychol. Rev. 61, 81 (1954); C. E. Osgood, of appropriate methods for the pro-
ment that such similarities among lit- Scand. J. Psychol. 7, 1 (1966); H. C. Triandis duction of retrograde amnesia under
and W. W. Lamber, J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol.
erate cultures only reflect learned rec- 56, 321 (1958). the laboratory conditions and for ac-
ognitions from mass media) the agree- 3. J. S. Bruner and R. Taguiri, "The perception curate specification of its characteris-
of people," in Handbook of Social Psychology,
ment and accuracy were lower in the G. Lindzey, Ed. (Addison-Wesley, Cambridge, tics. I describe here two experiments
Mass., 1954), vol. 2, pp. 634-654.
preliterate cultures than in the literate 4. C. Darwin, The Expression of the Emotions with use of a new method of demon-
ones. We believe that this is because in Man and Animals (Murray, London, 1872). strating a phenomenon that resembles
5. S. S. Tomkins, "The positive affects," Af-
of the enormous obstacles imposed by fect, Imagery, Consciousness (Springer, New retrograde amnesia. The method has
language barriers and task unfamiliar- York, 1962), vol. 1; "The negative affects," Af- certain advantages over the existing
fect, Imagery, Consciousness (Springer, New
ity in preliterate cultures (even with York, 1963), vol. 2; and R. McCarter, ones (3), although its applicability is
the more Westernized observers). De- Percept. Motor Skills 18 (Monogr. Suppl. No. limited to human subjects.
l-V18), 119 (1964).
spite such handicaps, there were similar 6. P. Ekman and W. V. Friesen, "Origins, usage In the experiments, the events to be
and coding of nonverbal behavior, in Com- remembered were common words pre-
recognitions of happiness, anger, and munication Theory and Linguistic Models in
fear in all samples, and for disgust, sur- the Social Sciences, E. Vernon, Ed. (Di Tella,
Buenos Aires, 1968); "The repertoire of non-
sented to the subjects sequentially, one
prise, and sadness in two out of three verbal behavior," Semiotica, in press. word at a time, with the instructions to
samples (Table 1). An affect category 7. P. Ekman, W. V. Friesen, S. S. Tomkins, "A remember as many of the words in a
facial affect scoring technique; and initial
was never misidentified by the majority validity study," in preparation. given list as possible and to recall them
88 SCIENCE, VOL. 164

You might also like