You are on page 1of 6

Available online at www.sciencedirect.

com

Personality and Individual Differences 45 (2008) 213–218


www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Need for belonging, relationship satisfaction, loneliness,


and life satisfaction
David Mellor a,*, Mark Stokes a, Lucy Firth b, Yoko Hayashi a, Robert Cummins a
a
School of Psychology, Deakin University, Burwood, Victoria 3125, Australia
b
Department of Information Systems, University of Melbourne, Parkville 8000, Australia

Received 2 December 2007; received in revised form 18 March 2008; accepted 27 March 2008
Available online 9 May 2008

Abstract

Loneliness and the need to belong are two subjective states that, on the basis of prior research and theory, would appear to be related
both to one another and to wellbeing. This study explored these relationships with a sample of 436 volunteer participants drawn from the
Australian Unity Wellbeing database. Participants completed a survey that included a measure of satisfaction with personal relationships
embedded in the Personal Wellbeing Index, the UCLA Loneliness scale, a measure of life satisfaction, and the Need to Belong Scale.
While loneliness was weakly related to need to belong, it was strongly associated with the discrepancy between need to belong and sat-
isfaction with personal relationships, which we used to measure unmet need for belonging. People living alone reported a lower need to
belong and less satisfaction with personal relationships than those living with others. However, the discrepancy scores, life satisfaction
scores and loneliness scores did not differ between these groups. Loneliness mediated the relationship between unmet need for belonging
and wellbeing (life satisfaction). These findings support Baumeister and Leary’s ‘‘belongingness hypothesis” and clarify the relationship
between these variables.
Ó 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Loneliness; Need to belong; Personal relationships; Life satisfaction

1. Introduction paper, we investigate the relationship between these two


factors and life satisfaction.
As social beings, most humans live in a matrix of rela-
tionships that, to a large extent, define their identity (I 1.1. Belongingness
am a daughter, wife, mother, student, etc.), and our per-
sonality (I am extraverted, friendly, and kind). Moreover, In their defining article on the importance of belonging-
the importance of such connections transcend cultural dif- ness to wellbeing, Baumeister and Leary (1995) proposed
ferences (for reviews, see Heine, Lehman, Markus, & the ‘‘belongingness hypothesis”, which suggested that
Kitayama, 1999; Kitayama & Markus, 1994; Silvera & ‘‘human beings have a pervasive drive to form and main-
Seger, 2004). Given such dependency on relationships with tain at least a minimum quantity of lasting, positive, and
others, it is not surprising that factors such as belonging- significant interpersonal relationships” (p. 497). Failure to
ness and loneliness are important predictors of psycholog- have belongingness needs met may lead to feelings of social
ical health (e.g., Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Ernst & isolation, alienation, and loneliness. Thus, a sense of
Cacioppo, 1999; Townsend & McWhirter, 2005). In this belongingness is not only a precursor to social connected-
ness but also a buffer against loneliness.
In their detailed analysis of the relevant research, these
*
Corresponding author. Tel.: +61 3 9244 3742; fax: +61 3 9244 6858. authors argued that the need for belongingness is more
E-mail address: mellor@deakin.edu.au (D. Mellor). than the need for social contact. It is the need for positive,

0191-8869/$ - see front matter Ó 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.03.020
214 D. Mellor et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 45 (2008) 213–218

and pleasant social contacts within the context of desired erature suggests that higher levels of loneliness are linked
relationships with people other than strangers. That is, to higher levels of psychological distress and lower levels
the need for belongingness is satisfied by an interpersonal of psychological wellness.
bond marked by ‘‘stability, affective concern, and continu-
ation into the foreseeable future” (p. 500). It is this rela- 1.3. Loneliness and need for belonging
tional context of interactions with other people that is
essential for satisfying the need to belong. Loneliness and belongingness share the subjective per-
They also propose that, through satiation, people who ception of connectedness to others. Thus, a considerable
are well-enmeshed in social relationships should have less body of literature has considered aspects of belonging
need to seek and form additional bonds than people who and loneliness together. For example, Hagerty, Williams,
are socially deprived. As their need for belonging has been Coyne, and Early (1996) found both to be related to social
met, and is no longer such a significant drive, they do not and psychological functioning while Tomaka, Thompson,
express or display the need for belonging as strongly as and Palacios (2006) found both to be associated with
those for whom this need has not been met. Importantly, health outcomes. However, these studies and the many oth-
however, individuals differ in the strength of their need to ers that have considered constructs related to belonging-
belong. As Kelly (2001) points out, some people with lower ness have failed to measure the need for belongingness.
need to belong may be satisfied by few contacts, while oth- This represents an important omission since it may be the
ers with greater need to belong may need many such con- unmet need for belongingness that is a risk factor for lone-
tacts. It is the lack of satisfaction with personal liness, and that loneliness may then be the risk factor for
relationships relative to their need to belong that puts the reduced wellbeing. If this were to be the case, then the rela-
individual at risk of loneliness. tionship between need for belongingness and wellbe-
ing should be mediated, or at least moderated by
1.2. Loneliness loneliness.
Thus, the major aims of the present study are firstly to
Loneliness is characterised by unpleasant feelings that explore whether the most important relationship between
arise when an individual perceives a discrepancy between loneliness, belonging and life satisfaction is the degree to
their desired and existing social relationships (Perlman, which the need for belongingness is satisfied. That is, rather
2004). It is therefore a subjective experience, is distinct than need to belong being the primary variable, as assumed
from the objective condition of aloneness (Rokach, 2004), by previous authors, it is the unsatisfied need for belong-
and cannot be simply predicted by objective indicators ingness that is associated with loneliness. We therefore
(de Jong Gierveld & Havens, 2004; Perlman, 2004). An expect that the relationship between need for belongingness
individual may have a small social network and yet experi- and loneliness will be weak, and that an examination of the
ence no loneliness. Conversely, an individual may have a relationship between loneliness and the degree to which
large social network yet still feel lonely. This discrepancy need for belongingness is unmet will be more informative.
may be subjective in relation to the level of felt intimacy, In order to investigate the relationship between unmet
and/or objective, in relation to the number of social con- need for belongingness and loneliness, we propose to calcu-
tacts (de Jong Gierveld & Havens, 2004). Thus, the com- late a difference score between self-reported need to belong
mon consensus is that the subjective and objective and self-reported reported satisfaction with personal rela-
indicators should be separately measured (Andersson, tionships. This estimate of unmet need for belongingness
1998; de Jong Gierveld & Havens, 2004; McWhirter, will allow us to more directly test the ‘belongingness
1990; Perlman, 2004; Rokach, 2004). While the strongest hypothesis’.
predictors of loneliness are subjective, certain objective Our second aim is to explore whether people who live
indicators, such as living alone, are also strong predictors alone differ from people who live with others in regard to
of loneliness (Andersson, 1998). the variables under investigation. Single person households
In individualistic Western countries the prevalence of now comprises from one third to one half of the total
loneliness is relatively high, with (Andersson (1998) esti- households in most Western cities (Fleming, 2007). Flem-
mating that about one in four people report regularly expe- ing, using figures from the Australian Bureau of Statistics,
riencing loneliness. Researchers have found loneliness to be reports that in Australia there are now more lone-person
implicated in negative aspects of mental health. For exam- households (1,962,100) than there are households made
ple, it has been found related to depression (Eisses et al., up of couples living with children (1,798,400). This social
2004; Nangle, Erdley, Newman, Mason, & Carpenter, phenomenon is an important part of our social fabric.
2003), and suicidal ideation (Kidd, 2004; Stravynski & While this lone-person demographic would appear to be
Boyer, 2001). Likewise, loneliness has been found to be at most obvious risk of social isolation and alienation, we
negatively related to life satisfaction (Goodwin, Cook, & do not know whether they chose to live alone because they
Yung, 2001; Schumaker, Shea, Monfries, & Groth-Marnat, have a low need for belonging, whether they are satisfied
1993) and subjective wellbeing (Bramston, Pretty, & Chi- with their personal relationships, or whether they are
puer, 2002; Chipuer, Bramston, & Pretty, 2003). Thus, lit- lonely.
D. Mellor et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 45 (2008) 213–218 215

Our final aim is to investigate the relationship between an 11-point scale ranging from ‘‘Strongly Agree” (0) to
unmet need for belongingness, loneliness and life satisfac- ‘‘Strongly Disagree” (10). Three items are reverse scored,
tion. In order to do this, we will conduct mediation and before a total score is derived by adding the responses.
moderation analyses. These will determine whether the Higher scores indicate a greater need to belong. Leary
effect of unmet need for belongingness on life satisfaction et al. (2006) have reported that the Need to Belong scale
is mediated by loneliness. correlates with, but is distinct from, other variables that
involve a desire for social contact, such as extraversion,
2. Method sociability, and need for affiliation. Pickett, Gardner, and
Knowles (2004) used the Need to Belong scale in a study
2.1. Participants of sensitivity to social cues, and reported that it demon-
strated adequate reliability, with Cronbach’s alpha being
The participants were a sample of Australian adults 0.83. In this study, Cronbach’s alpha was 0.78.
drawn from the Australian Unity Wellbeing project. Orig- Loneliness was measured using the UCLA Loneliness
inally, a cross-sectional sample was selected on a national Scale (Version 3) (Russell, 1996) which assesses subjective
geographical distributional basis. All in the current survey feelings of loneliness and social isolation. Russell (1996)
were members of that cross-sectional sample who had vol- reported Cronbach’s alphas ranging from 0.89 to 0.94 for
unteered for further contact and were enrolled in our lon- this 20-item scale across student, nurse, teacher and elderly
gitudinal study. Of the 896 questionnaires mailed out to samples. An item example is ‘‘How often do you feel close
these volunteers, 487 completed questionnaires were to people?” Participants responded on an 11-point scale
returned (54.4% response rate). Of these, 51 surveys had ranging from ‘‘Never” (0) to ‘‘Always” (10). In this sample
missing data on at least one of the three variables under Cronbach’s alpha was 0.95.
investigation, so these cases were deleted. This left a sample Satisfaction with personal relationships was measured
of 436 participants, of whom 244 were females and 192 through one of the items in the Personal Wellbeing Index
males. Their ages ranged from 20 to 86 years, with a mean (International Wellbeing Group, 2006). The item asks
age of 59.07 years (SD = 14.00). Seventy nine participants ‘How satisfied are you with your personal relationships?
reported that they lived alone. Table 1 describes the sample Participants responded on an 11-point scale ranging from
by age, gender and living arrangement. ‘‘Completely dissatisfied” (0) to ‘‘Completely satisfied” (10).
Life satisfaction was measured using the single item
2.2. Measures ‘‘How satisfied are you with your life as a whole?” Partic-
ipants responded on an 11-point scale ranging from ‘‘Com-
The following measures were contained in a 97-item pletely dissatisfied” (0) to ‘‘Completely satisfied” (10). This
questionnaire that constituted the Australian Unity Longi- single item has been commonly used in surveys since being
tudinal Wellbeing follow-up survey conducted in March devised by Andrews and Withey (1976). It has the desirable
during 2007. characteristic of being both highly personal and abstract,
Need to Belong was assessed using the Need to Belong which is the essence of the subjective wellbeing construct
Scale developed by Schreindorfer and Leary (1996) and (Cummins, Eckersley, Pallant, Van Vugt, & Misajon,
modified by Kelly (1999, cited by Leary, Kelly, Cottrell, 2003) and closely related to Core Affect (Davern, Cum-
& Schreindorfer, 2006). The modified version consists of mins, & Stokes, 2007).
10 items that assess the degree to which respondents desire
to be accepted by other people, seek opportunities to 3. Results
belong to social groups, and react negatively when they
were shunned, rejected, or ostracized. Item examples Data were analysed with SPSS for Windows statistical
include ‘‘If other people don’t seem to accept me, I don’t package (SPSS Inc., 2003 – SPSS for Windows: Release
let it bother me”, and ‘‘I need to feel that there are people 12.01, Chicago, IL: SPSS Inc.). Preliminary assumption
I can turn to in times of need”. Participants responded on testing was conducted prior to all analyses being

Table 1
Age, gender and living arrangements of participants (N = 436)
Gender Living arrangements Age group Total
18–25 26–35 36–45 46–55 56–65 66–75 >76
Male Living alone 0 0 8 1 7 7 5 28
Living with others 2 2 11 30 45 47 27 164
Total 2 2 19 31 52 54 32 192
Female Living alone 0 0 3 4 17 16 11 51
Living with others 4 16 37 43 52 31 10 193
Total 4 16 40 47 69 47 21 244
216 D. Mellor et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 45 (2008) 213–218

conducted. Both Need to Belong and Loneliness demon- Table 3


strated skewness and kurtosis within the acceptable ranges Correlations between need to belong, loneliness, and satisfaction with
personal relationships (n = 436)
described by Neter, Kutner, Nachtscheim, and Wasserman
(1996), but satisfaction with relationships was slightly Need to Satisfaction with Loneliness
belong personal relationships
skewed ( 1.284). However, the data were analysed in their
original form since the sample size was large enough to Need to Belong 1
Satisfaction with 0.17* 1
reduce the impact of any skewness/kurtosis (Tabachnick Personal
& Fidel, 2001). After the initial screening that deleted those Relationships
participants who supplied incomplete data on one of the Loneliness 0.28* 0.61* 1
three dependent variables (see Participants, above), 436 Difference score 0.66* 0.86* 0.62*
cases were retained for further analyses. *
p < 0.001.
A difference score between Need to Belong and Satisfac-
tion with Personal Relationships was calculated for each
participant. We used this score to estimate unmet need As the variables are continuous, the use of a technique like
for belongingness. The means and standard deviations ANOVA is inappropriate (Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken,
for these variables are shown in Table 2 for the entire sam- 2003). Variables were centred prior to multiplication
ple, and also for the two groups, those living alone and (Cohen et al., 2003). A hierarchical regression was under-
those living with others. Independent samples t-tests indi- taken, entering the main effects first (Loneliness, Discrep-
cated people living alone scored lower on Need to Belong ancy scores). As in ANOVA, the contributions of main
(t(433) = 2.68, p < 0.01), and Satisfaction with Relation- effects are assessed first to remove their contribution.
ships (t(434) = 4.13, p < 0.001). The groups did not differ Thereafter, the interaction term is assessed to check if it
in Loneliness, Life Satisfaction or discrepancy scores. The adds anything beyond the main effects themselves. If the
life satisfaction mean scores of 75.9 points (live alone) interaction adds little, then there is no reason to increase
and 76.5 points (live with others) lie just within the normal the complexity of the statistical model. The interaction of
range for the Australian population (75.8–79.2 points: loneliness and unmet Need to belong was entered at the
Cummins et al., 2007). second step. No significant moderation was detected (see
Table 3 shows the relationships between variables. As
can be seen, Need to Belong and Loneliness are signifi- Sobel’s Z=--5.01, p<0.001
Goodman’s Z=--5.02, p<001
cantly but weakly positively correlated suggesting that
those with a higher need to belong tend to be more lonely, Loneliness
0.415 -0.211
as might be expected. Satisfaction with Personal Relation- (0.026) (0.040)
ships is significantly negatively correlated with both need to β=0.615 β=-0.242
belong and loneliness. The difference score between Need -0.278
to Belong and Satisfaction with Personal Relationships is (0.027)
Unmet β=-0.473
strongly related to loneliness. Life
belongingness
satisfaction
need
3.1. Mediation and moderation analyses
-0.366
An analysis was undertaken to assess whether loneliness (0.022)
Unmet β=--622
mediated the relationship between unmet Need to Belong- Life
belongingness
ing and Life Satisfaction. As evident in Fig. 1, partial medi- satisfaction
need
ation was evident (Z = 5.01, p < 0.001).
A moderation analysis was undertaken to assess Fig. 1. Mediation model of unmet need for belonging through loneliness
whether unmet Need to Belong and Loneliness interact. to life satisfaction.

Table 2
Descriptives by living arrangements
Living Arrangement Living alone Living with others Total
N 79 357 436
M SD M SD MD SD
Life Satisfaction 7.59 1.91 7.65 1.57 7.64 1.64
Need to Belong* 4.47 1.62 4.95 1.42 4.86 1.47
Loneliness 3.52 2.08 3.13 1.83 3.20 1.88
Satisfaction with Personal Relationships** 6.68 2.61 7.75 1.95 7.56 2.12
Needs to Belong–Satisfaction 2.22 3.12 2.80 2.69 2.70 2.78
*
Significantly different, p < 0.01.
**
Significantly different, p < 0.001.
D. Mellor et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 45 (2008) 213–218 217

Table 4
Hierarchical regression analysis of the moderation of Loneliness and unmet Need to Belong (Discrepancy scores) upon Life Satisfaction
Step B SE b t p Adj R2 p
Constant 1 7.57 1.93 39.20 0.000
Discrepancy 1 0.28 0.027 0.47 10.20 0.000
Loneliness 1 0.21 0.040 0.24 5.23 0.000
0.420 <0.001
Constant 2 7.59 0.20 38.78 0.000
Discrepancy 2 0.279 0.03 0.47 9.78 0.000
Loneliness 2 0.21 0.043 0.24 5.11 0.000
Loneliness BY Discrepancy 2 0.01 0.01 0.03 0.63 0.529
0.419 0.529

Table 4). The variables Loneliness and unmet Need to ness needs met may lead to feelings of social isolation,
Belong alone accounted for 42% of the variance in Life Sat- alienation, and loneliness. Because of this, a discrepancy
isfaction (R2 = 0.420), while the addition of the centred between need to belong and satisfaction with personal rela-
interaction added only another 0.1% of explained variance. tionships should be associated with loneliness. Indeed, we
When assessed by itself, establishing the maximum possible found the level of association to be r = 0.62, thereby con-
interaction of these two variables, it was found that the firming the hypothesis that people who are lonely have
interaction of Loneliness and unmet Need to Belonging unmet need to belong.
accounted for 7.1% of variance (R2 = 0.071), which while The important implication of this finding is that it is not
significant (F(1, 434) = 34.314, p < 0.001), was a moderate simply a matter of ‘‘one size fits all” – the psychological sit-
result compared to the effect of Loneliness and Discrep- uation cannot be simply described by the use of these vari-
ancy scores individually. We probed the simple slopes of ables as discrete entities. Rather, the discrepancy between
the interaction term by adding or subtracting 1SD to each the need to belong and the degree to which this need is sat-
centred main effect term successively before multiplying isfied is the crucial variable. People with many friends and
against the second centred main effect term (Cohen et al., acquaintances may still be lonely, while people with few
2003). The interaction remained robust in these analyses. friends and acquaintances may not be lonely. This was
reflected in our investigation of those living alone and those
4. Discussion living with others. While we do not know the reasons why
some of our participants were living alone and whether it
This study investigated the relationships between loneli- was by choice, they reported a lower need to belong and a
ness, need to belong and satisfaction with personal relation- lower level of satisfaction with personal relationships than
ships. We also investigated whether living arrangement, those living with others. It may be that their dissatisfaction
alone or with others, was associated with these variables. with personal relationships had led them to live alone, or
We found that people who report a higher need to belong perhaps living alone was a consequence of their lower need
also report higher levels of loneliness. However, although to belong, and in turn had led to lower satisfaction with per-
the association was weak (r = 0.28), it contrasts with Leary sonal relationships. However, importantly, living alone or
et al.’s (2006) finding of no correlation in two university with others was not associated with discrepancy scores,
samples (N = 205, r = 0.02, and N = 325, r = 0.03). Sim- nor with loneliness, suggesting that people living with others
ilarly, the association between need to belong and satisfac- have just as many unmet belonging needs, and are just as
tion with personal relationships was significant but weak lonely as people living alone. Clearly, this needs further
(r = 0.17), suggesting a weak negative relationship investigation with more specific categories of living arrange-
between them. This is consistent with Kelly’s (2001) suppo- ments, and more information as to why people live alone.
sition that individuals vary in their need to belong, and that Our findings regarding subjective wellbeing (as mea-
lower needs are not necessarily more easily satisfied. sured by satisfaction with life as a whole) and loneliness
Somewhat more expected was the finding that satisfac- and unmet need for belonging suggest that loneliness medi-
tion with personal relationships was moderately negatively ates the relationship between unmet need for belonging and
correlated (r = 0.61) with loneliness.Thus, the less satis- wellbeing, rather than moderates the relationship. Thus,
fied one is with their personal relationships, independent while unmet need for belongingness exerts an influence
of need to belong, the more lonely one will feel. on subjective wellbeing, this is partially through feelings
Of greater interest is the behavior of the discrepancy of loneliness that arise as a result of the unmet need.
score between need to belong and relationship satisfaction. Despite these findings, our study was limited by a single
Baumeister and Leary (1995) argue that individuals ‘‘have item measure of satisfaction with personal relationships,
a pervasive drive to form and maintain at least a minimum and the possibility of a biased sample of people who had ini-
quantity of lasting, positive, and significant interpersonal tially agreed to participate in a telephone interview, agreed to
relationships” (p. 497). Thus, a failure to have belonging- participants in a future survey, and then did so. However, it is
218 D. Mellor et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 45 (2008) 213–218

not clear whether or not this self-selection process would Hagerty, B. M., Williams, R. A., Coyne, J. C., & Early, M. R. (1996).
have any influence on the variables assessed in the study. Sense of belonging and indicators of social and psychological
functioning. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing, X, 235–244.
In summary, our study provides support for Baumeister Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1999). Is
and Leary’s ‘‘belongingness hypothesis” to the extent that there a universal need for positive self-regard? Psychological Review,
the discrepancy between need to belong and satisfaction 108, 766–794.
with personal relationships is associated with loneliness. International Wellbeing Group (2006). Personal Wellbeing Index, 5th ed.
The relationship between unmet belongingness needs and Melbourne: Australian Centre on Quality of Life, Deakin University.
Kelly, K. M. (2001). Individual differences in reactions to rejection. In M.
wellbeing is mediated by loneliness. Future studies could R. Leary (Ed.), Interpersonal rejection (pp. 291–315). New York:
investigate further how this discrepancy varies with other Oxford University Press.
psychosocial variables and sociotropic traits. Kidd, S. A. (2004). ‘‘The walls are closing in, and we were trapped”: A
qualitative analysis of street youth suicide. Youth and Society, 36,
30–55.
Acknowledgements Kitayama, S., & Markus, H. R. (1994). Introduction to cultural
psychology and emotion research. In S. Kitayama & H. R. Markus
The research reported in this paper was supported by a (Eds.), Emotion and culture: empirical studies of mutual influence.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Linkage Grant from the Australian Research Council and Leary, M. R., Kelly, K. M., Cottrell, C. A., & Schreindorfer, L. S. (2006).
the industry partner Australian Unity. Individual differences in the need to belong: Mapping the nomological
network. Unpublished manuscript, Wake Forest University.
McWhirter, B. T. (1990). Loneliness: A review of current literature, with
References implications for counselling and research. Journal of Counselling and
Development, 68, 417–422.
Andersson, L. (1998). Loneliness research and interventions: A review of Nangle, D. W., Erdley, C. A., Newman, J. E., Mason, C. A., &
the literature. Aging and Mental Health, 2, 264–274. Carpenter, E. M. (2003). Popularity, friendship quantity, and
Andrews, F. M., & Withey, S. B. (1976). Social indicators of well-being: friendship quality: Interactive influences on children’s loneliness
American’s perceptions of life quality. New York: Plenum Press. and depression. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology,
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for 32, 546–555.
interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Neter, J., Kutner, M. H., Nachtscheim, C. J., & Wasserman, W. (1996).
Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529. Applied linear statistical models. Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill.
Bramston, P., Pretty, G., & Chipuer, H. (2002). Unravelling subjective Perlman, D. (2004). European and Canadian studies of loneliness among
quality of life: An investigation of individual and community deter- seniors. Canadian Journal on Aging, 23, 181–188.
minants. Social Indicator Research, 59, 261–274. Pickett, C., Gardner, W. L., & Knowles, M. L. (2004). Getting a cue: The
Chipuer, H. M., Bramston, P., & Pretty, G. (2003). Determinants of need to belong influences attention to subtle social cues. Personality
subjective quality of life among rural adolescents: A developmental and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1095–1107.
perspective. Social Indicator Research, 61, 79–95. Rokach, A. (2004). Loneliness then and now: Reflections on social and
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, L. S., & Aiken, S. G. (2003). Applied multiple emotional alienation in everyday life. Current Psychology: Develop-
regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). mental, Learning, Personality, Social, 23, 24–40.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. Russell, D. W. (1996). UCLA Loneliness Scale (version 3): Reliability,
Cummins, R. A., Eckersley, R., Pallant, J., Van Vugt, J., & Misajon, R. validity, and factor structure. Journal of Personality Assessment, 66,
(2003). Developing a national index of subjective wellbeing: The 20–40.
Australian Unity Wellbeing Index. Social Indicators Research, 64, Schreindorfer, L., Leary, M. (1996). Seeking acceptance versus avoiding
159–190. rejection: Differential effects on emotion and behavior. Paper presented
Cummins, R. A., Woerner, J., Tomyn, A., Gibson, A., Lai, L., Collard, J. at the meeting of the Southeastern Psychological Association, Norfolk,
(2007). Australian Unity Wellbeing Index: Report 18.0. The Wellbeing VA.
of Australians – Changing conditions to make life better. Melbourne: Schumaker, J. F., Shea, J. D., Monfries, M. M., & Groth-Marnat, G.
Australian Centre on Quality of Life, School of Psychology, Deakin (1993). Loneliness and life satisfaction in Japan and Australia. The
University. ISBN 978-1-74156-101-2. Journal of Psychology, 127, 65–71.
Davern, M., Cummins, R. A., & Stokes, M. (2007). Subjective wellbeing Silvera, D. H., & Seger, C. R. (2004). Feeling good about ourselves:
as an affective/cognitive construct. Journal of Happiness Studies, 8, Unrealistic self-evaluations and their relation to self-esteem in the
429–449. United States and Norway. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35,
de Jong Gierveld, J., & Havens, B. (2004). Cross-national comparisons of 571–585.
social isolation and loneliness: Introduction and overview. Canadian Stravynski, A., & Boyer, R. (2001). Loneliness in relation to suicide
Journal on Aging, 23, 109–113. ideation and parasuicide: A population-wide study. Suicide and Life-
Eisses, A. M. H., Kluiter, H., Jongenelis, K., Pot, A. M., Beekman, A. T. Threatening Behaviour, 31, 32–40.
F., & Ormel, J. (2004). Risk indicators of depression in residential Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidel, L. S. (2001). Using Multivariate Statistics (4th
homes. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 19, 634–640. ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Ernst, J. M., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1999). Lonely hearts: Psychological Tomaka, J., Thompson, S., & Palacios, R. (2006). The relation of social
perspectives on loneliness. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 8, 1–22. isolation, loneliness, and social support to disease outcomes among the
Fleming, K. (2007). The power of one. The Bulletin, 10, 28–34. elderly. Journal of Aging and Health, 18, 359–384.
Goodwin, R., Cook, O., & Yung, Y. (2001). Loneliness and life Townsend, K. C., & McWhirter, B. T. (2005). Connectedness: A review of
satisfaction among three cultural groups. Personal Relationships, 8, the literature with implications for counselling, assessment, and
225–230. research. Journal of Counselling and Development, 83, 191–201.

You might also like