You are on page 1of 20

Handbook of Clinical Neurology, Vol.

163 (3rd series)


The Frontal Lobes
M. D’Esposito and J.H. Grafman, Editors
https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-804281-6.00005-7
Copyright © 2019 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved

Chapter 5

Transcranial magnetic stimulation: Neurophysiological


and clinical applications

MATTHEW J. BURKE1, PETER J. FRIED1, AND ALVARO PASCUAL-LEONE1,2,3*


1
Berenson-Allen Center for Noninvasive Brain Stimulation and Division of Cognitive Neurology, Department of Neurology,
Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
2
Guttmann Brain Health Institute, Institut Guttmann de Neurorehabilitacio, Universitat Autonoma de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
3
Marcus Institute for Aging Research, Hebrew Senior Life, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States

Abstract
Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) is a safe and noninvasive means of electrically stimulating the brain
by electromagnetic induction. TMS is capable of probing intracortical circuits and modulating cortical activ-
ity in humans; as such it has been instrumental to studying the neurophysiology and functional neuroanatomy
of the frontal lobes. For example, using TMS to induce “virtual lesions”—transient disruption of function in
the targeted brain region—has yielded important insights into the functional organization of the prefrontal
cortex (PFC) with respect to working memory, language, and other core cognitive functions. Whereas neu-
roimaging is typically limited to observing correlations between brain function and behavior, TMS, by inter-
acting with neural circuits, can lead to causal inferences that bridge human, nonhuman primate, and other
model system studies. Applied repetitively in trains of stimuli, TMS is also capable of normalizing aberrant
patterns of cortical activity in the treatment of neurologic and psychiatric disorders. The earliest and most
well-established clinical use of repetitive TMS is in the treatment of medication-resistant depression with
high-frequency stimulation of the left dorsolateral PFC. Research efforts to identify other promising clinical
applications—such as for stroke and Alzheimer’s disease—are rapidly expanding; however, the majority of
these indications have yet to have devices cleared by the FDA for on-label use.

functions as well as its clinical applications for diagnos-


INTRODUCTION ing and treating disorders with frontal lobe dysfunction.
Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) is a noninva- We will begin by introducing the context and rationale
sive brain stimulation technique that generates brief, rap- for using TMS and provide an overview of its technolog-
idly changing magnetic fields capable of inducing ical principles. We will then present a wide range of
electric currents in the brain. It is safe, well tolerated, research that exemplifies the importance of TMS in the
and has a very favorable side-effect profile, provided that study of the frontal lobes.
safety recommendations are followed. TMS has a broad
range of capabilities that make it an optimal neurophys- PART I: BASIS AND TECHNOLOGICAL
iological tool for studying the function of brain regions PRINCIPLES OF TMS
and their associated networks, interrogating brain–
behavior relationships and identifying neurobiologic
Studying brain/behavior relationships
substrates of disease. In this chapter, we will focus on The ongoing effort to better understand the complex
the contribution of TMS to understanding frontal lobe role of the frontal lobes in cognition has a long and

*Correspondence to: Alvaro Pascual-Leone, M.D. Ph.D., Berenson-Allen Center for Noninvasive Brain Stimulation, Beth
Israel Deaconess Medical Center, KS-158, 330 Brookline Ave, Boston, MA 02215, United States. Tel: +1-617-667-0203,
Fax: +1-617-975-5322, E-mail: apleone@bidmc.harvard.edu
74 M.J. BURKE ET AL.
challenging history. As described in previous chapters, injury plus focal lesion(s). Finally, and perhaps most
the functions ascribed to the frontal lobes are extensive importantly, lesion studies are often limited to case
and range from fundamental constructs such as mem- studies or series and lack experimental control. For
ory and language to abstract phenomena such as example, one lacks a baseline (prelesion) evaluation
morality, expectation, and reasoning. In modern times, and cannot control for age, comorbidities, or other rel-
the most common approach employed by neuroscien- evant variables that may also influence brain function in
tists to study frontal lobe function has been to observe the context of a new lesion.
an individual completing a relevant neuropsychologic TMS overcomes many of the pitfalls described here.
task while recording a given metric of brain function. TMS can be used safely and noninvasively to induce
Metrics for assessing brain function range from per- “virtual lesions” in experimentally controlled settings.
formance on neurophysiological tests to changes in Virtual lesions are brief periods of stimulation-induced
activity on functional magnetic resonance imaging neurobehavioral dysfunction caused by transient disrup-
(fMRI), positron emission tomography (PET), or elec- tion of the targeted focal brain region (Pascual-Leone
troencephalography (EEG). Though these approaches et al., 2000). This approach allows causal inferences to
have led to many valuable insights, they are limited be made about the necessity of a given brain region or
by the fact that they are correlational in nature. For network in a cognitive or behavioral process. TMS can
example, one could correlate proficiency at a given also be combined with other modalities such as EEG
task with the magnitude of a recorded neural state- and/or fMRI to provide concurrent real-time, brain-wide
change, but this tells you relatively little about the neurophysiological data to support behavioral observa-
causal necessity of the studied brain region in such a tions and establish circuit-level causality. To fully appre-
relationship. ciate the concepts underpinning the “virtual lesion”
One approach to establish much-needed causal links model and its translational applications, we will briefly
has focused on investigation of patients with specific describe the fundamental principles of TMS.
focal brain lesions, commonly referred to as lesion
studies. For example, a frontal lobe lesion yielding a
TMS mechanisms
temporally documented pre/postchange in cognitive or
emotional functioning provides causal information that Based on the principles of electromagnetic induction
the damaged brain region may be involved (directly or first described by Michael Faraday in 1831, TMS
indirectly) in symptom generation. This technique has utilizes a coil made of wire to produce brief, rapidly
been used with systematic methods since the early changing current pulses capable of generating strong
1900s and was championed by many great neuroscien- (1–2 T) rapidly fluctuating magnetic fields. The first
tists such as Alexander Luria (Luria, 1972). Lesion demonstration of TMS in a human was performed by
studies have and continue to provide many important Barker et al. (1985). Since this time there has been a
findings, but they also have several limitations that rapid growth of this field and many technological adap-
may obscure appropriate interpretation of causal rela- tations; however, the core principles remain the same.
tionships (Pascual-Leone et al., 1999). As eloquently The coil is traditionally placed tangentially to the scalp
argued by von Monakow (1853–1930) in his 1910 clas- and induces a magnetic field pulse perpendicular to its
sic U€ ber Lokalisation der Hirnfunktion (on localization plane that penetrates skin, scalp, and skull, reaching
of brain function), a lesion is necessary for localization the brain. In the brain, the magnetic field pulse induces
of function, but it is not sufficient. First, a brain lesion an electric current perpendicular to the magnetic field
has an impact on the rest of the brain—an impact and thus parallel to the plane of the coil. The induced
mediated by functional connections that von Monakow currents are able to elicit action potentials in neurons
described as diaschisis and that today can be examined of the targeted brain region (Hallett, 2000). Recent
using resting state functional connectivity and network advances have been made in understanding which neu-
localization approaches (Boes et al., 2015). Second, fol- ral elements are activated by TMS. Using epidural cer-
lowing a brain lesion such as a stroke or trauma, the vical electrodes to record the descending volleys along
brain reorganizes itself to compensate for the damaged the corticospinal pathway elicited by single TMS pulses
structures. This can make it difficult to delineate to primary motor cortex, researchers have mapped out
changes due to the primary lesion vs effects of neuro- the “direct” and “indirect” (via mono- and polysynaptic
plastic adaptation. Third, lesions often involve larger interneuron circuits) activation of layer-V pyramidal
volumes than the specific region being evaluated. This neurons (Di Lazzaro et al., 2012). However, despite
could include adjacent cortical areas or fiber tracts pass- these advances, the exact mechanisms of TMS-induced
ing through a region. Fourth, lesions can often be asso- neural activation remain poorly understood and a source
ciated with multiple areas of injury or global/diffuse of ongoing debate.
TRANSCRANIAL MAGNETIC STIMULATION 75
TMS coils methodology can frequently miss the DLPFC target
due to individual anatomic differences (Pascual-Leone
The coil is the only part of the TMS apparatus that
et al., 1999; Herbsman et al., 2009). Other approaches
directly contacts the subject/patient. The rest of the
attempt to localize DLPFC using measurements related
TMS apparatus primarily consists of energy storage
to conventional 10–20 EEG recording sites (Homan
capacitors, a charging unit, circuits for controlling pulse
et al., 1987; Beam et al., 2009). They have found that
parameters, and a user-control interface. Starting with a
more anterior and lateral targets could more closely
simple annulus, there have been different shapes and
approximate DLPFC localization but similar issues
sizes of coils available for use. The most commonly used
regarding anatomic heterogeneity exist (Pascual-
coil in clinical and research settings is the figure-of-8
Leone et al., 1999; Herwig et al., 2003a, b; Herbsman
coil, which is usually favored due to the relatively high
et al., 2009).
focality of the induced current (Hallett, 2007). Newer
More recently, MRI-based neuronavigation equip-
coil designs, such as the H-coil, have been engineered
ment has been utilized to visualize and target the DLPFC
to try to allow deeper stimulation beyond the superficial
directly (Fitzgerald et al., 2009). This frameless stereo-
cortex (Zangen et al., 2005). This is relevant to many
taxic method uses standard brain-mapping coordinates
neuropsychiatric fields, as deep TMS techniques have
on either a template brain MRI or the subject’s own
shown the ability to stimulate deep frontal structures such
MRI that is then coregistered with the TMS coil in 3D
as the anterior cingulate (Hayward et al., 2007). How-
space. This technique is currently the gold standard in
ever, it is important to note that this type of stimulation
most research settings; however, this process takes time
is not selective for the deep target and there is a depth-
and requires access to costly, specialized equipment that
focality trade-off (Deng et al., 2013). A final coil that
may not be feasible in most clinical centers. It is impor-
should be discussed, to facilitate proper interpretation
tant to realize that neuronavigation systems are precise
of studies described later in this chapter, is the sham coil.
(i.e., they can assure that all TMS pulses induce a current
Sham devices attempt to mimic the sensations associated
of consistent focus); however, accurate targeting depends
with active TMS pulses (including an auditory “click”
on the actual goal of the stimulation and precision does
and somatosensory “tap”), while avoiding delivery of
not necessarily equal accuracy. For example, precise tar-
meaningful stimulation to the brain. A realistic sham
geting of all stimuli to a given cortical target defined by
device is critical for maintaining blinding integrity in
structural brain anatomy does not ensure that the same
comparative neurophysiological and treatment studies
functional brain region is stimulated across individuals.
of TMS. The current gold standard sham coil looks, feels,
Thus it is worth considering the use of functional, rather
and sounds indistinguishable from an active coil but is
than anatomic, defined targets. Recent clinical studies
engineered to shield the brain from the magnetic fields.
have shown promising results using resting state fMRI
Some sham coils also allow for superficial electrical
and functional connectivity to guide TMS targeting in
stimulation of the scalp that simulates TMS tactile sensa-
protocols of rTMS for medication-resistant depression
tions (Davis et al., 2013).
(Fox et al., 2012a, b). In a validation study of this
technique for left DLPFC targeted TMS in patients with
depression, the strength of the functional connectivity
TMS targeting
(negative correlation) between an individual’s TMS
Once an appropriate coil is selected, a variety of different cortical target and the subgenual cingulate predicted
approaches can be used to target the brain region of inter- antidepressant response (Weigand et al., 2018). Similar
est. Most relevant to this chapter is the approach for tar- functional connectivity-based approaches could be
geting the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), a adopted more widely for localizing TMS targets (Fox
common site of stimulation for neurophysiological, cog- et al., 2014).
nitive, and clinical protocols. Most studies start by iden- Obviously, the considerations made here regarding
tifying the motor cortex “hot spot” of a hand muscle, such targeting the DLPFC can be extrapolated and adapted
as the first dorsal interosseous. This process involves to targeting other regions within the frontal and prefron-
applying trials of TMS pulses and observing for muscle tal cortex (PFC). For anatomic precision, the use of
twitches in the target muscle or measuring motor evoked neuronavigation and, for target definition, functional
potentials (MEPs) using standard surface electromyo- (not just macroanatomic) criteria are desirable. Ideally,
gram (EMG). Once the motor cortex hot spot has been capturing the impact of the applied stimulation and relat-
established, the coil is then moved 5–6 cm anteriorly ing that impact to the measured effects should be
along the scalp to approximate the DLPFC location attempted. One way to do this is to use MRI not only
(Pascual-Leone et al., 1996). Though relatively quick to guide the TMS but also to capture its brain impact.
and practical, subsequent studies have found that this Real-time integration of TMS and EEG or the use of
76 M.J. BURKE ET AL.
other brain-mapping techniques concurrently with TMS of the motor cortex is nearly ubiquitous in all TMS stud-
are also worth considering and will be further ies as a way to identify the subject/patient’s resting motor
discussed later. threshold (RMT). The RMT is a measure of cortical
Finally, it should be noted that there are many other excitability typically defined as the minimal intensity
technological considerations, such as pulse shape needed to induce a visible muscle contraction or MEP
(monophasic, biphasic, or polyphasic), coil orientation, larger than 50 mV at least 50% of the time (Rossini
and current direction, that are critical factors to consider et al., 2015). The motor threshold is frequently used as
in planning and interpreting the results of TMS experi- a reference to set the intensity of stimulation (e.g., inten-
ments but are beyond the scope of this chapter and have sity of 80%–120% of RMT) for subsequent protocols,
been reviewed elsewhere (Rotenberg et al., 2014). and safety recommendations are often based on extrapo-
lations of applied TMS intensity in relation to the RMT
(Rossi et al., 2009). It is, however, not always necessary
TMS safety to set the intensity of TMS based on the RMT. Safe pro-
TMS has been studied in healthy volunteers and patients tocols can be developed inferring stimulation intensity
around the world for over 25 years. Meta-analyses asses- from other measures, for example the intensity to evoke
sing adverse effects collected in experimental and clini- a phosphene when targeting the occipital cortex (phos-
cal settings support the overall safety and tolerability of phene threshold) or the intensity to evoke a TMS-
TMS (Machii et al., 2006; Janicak et al., 2008). Risks induced electroencephalographic potential (transcranial
associated with TMS and comprehensive safety protocol evoked potential, TEP) or simply setting a given stimu-
guidelines have been formally outlined by the Safety of lator output intensity (Machii et al., 2006).
TMS Consensus Group of the International Federation of A single TMS pulse can also be delivered to the motor
Clinical Neurophysiology (Rossi et al., 2009; Rossini cortex to measure the cortical silent period (cSP). This
et al., 2015). Briefly, the most commonly reported silent period is a transient suppression of EMG activity
side-effect of TMS is headache or neck pain, which is following a TMS pulse during voluntary muscle contrac-
believed to be related to muscle tension due to repetitive tion and is believed to measure GABA B receptor-
muscle twitches and scalp tapping sensation, as well as mediated cortical inhibition (Ziemann et al., 2015).
from tightly fitted caps/headbands or positioning of the Beyond these two commonly used measures, single-
participant. This pain is generally short-lived and either pulse TMS has also been used extensively within and
resolves spontaneously or with over-the-counter analge- outside the motor cortex in TMS neurophysiological
sics. The main rare, but serious, potential adverse effect studies. Single TMS pulses can be timed to disrupt the
of TMS is the induction of a seizure. There have been less ongoing function of a target brain region, which will
than 20 reported cases of TMS-induced seizures since be discussed further in the section focusing on virtual
initial safety guidelines were published in 1998 lesions.
(Wassermann, 1998) and the commonly quoted risk is Paired-pulse TMS paradigms are primarily used to
less than 1 in 1000. This risk applies almost exclusively record measures of intracortical excitability within, or
to repetitive pulse TMS (rTMS), rather than single-pulse connectivity between, brain regions. Two TMS pulses,
TMS. In predisposed individuals, for example patients a conditioning stimulus and a test stimulus, are delivered
with epilepsy, the risk is higher, but still relatively low, in close succession with varying interstimulus intervals
around 1%–2% (Bae et al., 2007). TMS-induced seizures and intensities. The modulation of the test stimulus by
are considered provoked events and there has been no the conditioning stimulus is typically the primary mea-
associated risk of generation of epilepsy. Other rare surement of interest. Paired-pulse measures include short
events include potential hearing loss related to the interval cortical inhibition (SICI), long interval cortical
TMS clicking sounds (mitigated by wearing earplugs), inhibition (LICI), intracortical facilitation (ICF), interhe-
syncope (related to anxiety or anticipation of TMS), mispheric inhibition (IHI), and short-latency afferent
and occasional reports of transient cognitive, mood, or inhibition (SAI) (Kobayashi and Pascual-Leone, 2003;
neuropsychiatric symptoms. The general conclusions Valero-Cabre et al., 2017). Please see Table 5.1 for tech-
are that the risk of serious adverse effects is very low nical descriptions of these measures. Except for the latter
as long as safety guidelines and recommendations are two, the test and conditioning stimuli are applied to the
adhered to. same cortical region (typically in the motor cortex).
For IHI, interhemispheric connections are typically eval-
uated by delivering a conditioning stimulus to the ipsilat-
TMS protocols
eral M1 and a test stimulus applied to the contralateral
TMS can generally be applied in single pulses, pairs of M1 (Ferbert et al., 1992). However, targeting nonmotor
pulses, or trains of repetitive pulses. Single-pulse TMS areas and recording TEPs as a measure of IHI is also
TRANSCRANIAL MAGNETIC STIMULATION 77
Table 5.1
Paired-pulse paradigms—illustrated by applications to motor cortex.

Measure Stimulation parameters Response Proposed mechanism

SICI Intensity of CS ¼ subthreshold (e.g., 80% of RMT) Inhibitory GABA-A receptor


ISI ¼ 1–5 ms
LICI Intensity of CS ¼ suprathreshold (e.g., 120% of RMT) Inhibitory GABA B receptor
ISI ¼ 50–200 ms
ICF Intensity of CS ¼ subthreshold (e.g., 80% of RMT) Excitatory Glutamate
ISI ¼ 10–17 ms
IHI Intensity of CS ¼ subthreshold (e.g., 80% of RMT) Inhibitory Transcollosal inhibition
ISI ¼ 8–10 ms
SAI CS ¼ applied to peripheral nerve (Median nerve) Inhibitory Sensory-mediated inhibition of motor
ISI ¼ 20 ms cortex (Cholinergic activity)

When applied to the motor cortex, response is measured by changes in motor evoked potential amplitude (decrease for inhibition and increase for
excitation). Note, however, that these paradigms can be adapted for study of nonmotor cortical regions and TMS electroencephalographic evoked
potentials can be recorded as responses. Test stimuli used in paired-pulse studies is typically delivered at 100%–120% of resting motor threshold.
CS, conditioning stimulus; ISI, interstimulus interval; RMT, resting motor threshold; SICI, short interval cortical inhibition; LICI, long interval
cortical inhibition; ICF, intracortical facilitation; IHI, interhemispheric inhibition; SAI, short-latency afferent inhibition.

possible. For SAI, the conditioning stimulus is an electri- modulation via long-term depression and long-term
cal current applied to a peripheral nerve (typically the potentiation. Mimicking these respective processes,
median nerve) and the test stimulus is applied to the con- low-frequency trains (e.g., 1 Hz) tend to result in sup-
tralateral motor cortex (Tokimura et al., 2000). The pression of activity/excitability and high-frequency stim-
paired-pulse technique has been used to investigate the ulation (e.g., 10–20 Hz) tend to result in excitatory effects.
effects of CNS-active drugs (Ziemann et al., 1996) and Theta-burst stimulation is a more recent rTMS proto-
to study the pathophysiology of various neurologic and col that delivers bursts of three 50-Hz pulses repeated at
psychiatric diseases (Maeda and Pascual-Leone, 2003; 5 Hz, which is within the theta band of human neural
Chen et al., 2008). The primary goal for the latter oscillatory activity (Huang et al., 2005). The parameters
research has been to develop diagnostic biomarkers of for this methodology are based on coupling of theta and
disease, which will be discussed in the last section of this gamma wave patterns found to be powerful inducers of
chapter. neuroplasticity in animal models (Larson and Lynch,
Repetitive pulse TMS is an umbrella term that refers 1986). Similar to low-frequency rTMS, continuous
to any combination of three or more pulses of a set inten- theta-burst stimulation typically leads to cortical inhibi-
sity delivered at a frequency of at least 0.5 pulses/s; tion and, similar to high-frequency rTMS, intermittent
rTMS may be delivered across a wide range of frequen- theta-burst stimulation typically leads to cortical excita-
cies, train patterns, and durations. Unlike the transient tion. Theta burst has been shown to induce neuroplastic
effects of single- and paired-pulse TMS, longer trains changes very quickly and efficiently, which makes these
of rTMS have been shown to produce changes in activity protocols ideal indices of cortical plasticity (e.g., dura-
and metabolism that outlast the stimulation itself. tion of posttheta-burst response) (Suppa et al., 2016).
Accordingly, its effects can be divided into two main Please see Fig. 5.1 for an overview of the primary
categories: “online” effects (those that occur during stim- TMS protocols.
ulation and are typically disruptive in nature) and The neuromodulatory ability of rTMS, both theta-burst
“offline” effects (those that outlast the stimulation and and traditional protocols, has led to novel treatment strat-
are typically neuromodulatory in nature). First, rTMS egies for a variety of neurologic and psychiatric disorders,
trains can cause short-term “online” interference effects which will be discussed later in this chapter. Though
that can disrupt the function of a targeted brain area. This an extensive body of basic science and human studies sup-
concept and its relevant applications will be discussed ports the general mechanisms of rTMS proposed previ-
further in the section on virtual lesions. Second, ously (Hallett, 2007), it is important to stress that the
“offline” effects can be sustained long after the end of binary “excitatory”/“inhibitory” classification, commonly
the stimulation protocol and are believed to be mediated used in the TMS literature, likely represents an over-
by neuroplastic mechanisms related to synaptic simplification of complex, variable, and incompletely
78 M.J. BURKE ET AL.

Fig. 5.1. Overview of common TMS stimulation protocols. (A) Paired-pulse paradigm: dotted line, conditioning stimulus; black
line, test stimulus; time between stimuli, interstimulus interval and (B) gray underline in theta-burst protocol represents a burst of
three pulses at 50 Hz with an interval of 250 ms between bursts. A common protocol for intermittent theta burst would be 2 s of
bursts followed by 8 s off, repeated sequentially. As described in more detail in the text, binary labels of “excitatory” or “inhibitory”
are used for practical purposes, but interindividual and intraindividual variability exists.

understood effects. For example, rTMS can have very high unanswered. If studies do not acknowledge the implicit
interindividual variability and the same frequency that assumptions about such protocols, this can lead to spec-
results in excitatory effects in one individual could have ulative interpretation and misleading claims about the
inhibitory effects in another (Maeda et al., 2000a, b; structure–function relationships being investigated.
Hamada and Rothwell, 2016; Jannati et al., 2017). Further- Techniques that combine TMS with concurrent measure-
more, TMS also has “state-dependent” effects within the ment of neurophysiological data outside of the motor
same individual (Silvanto and Pascual-Leone, 2008). Sim- cortex, such as TMS-EEG and TMS-fMRI, are critical
ply put, the ongoing or basal brain state at the time of stim- tools to begin understanding these questions.
ulation strongly impacts the observed TMS effect. When
unaccounted for, this can lead to considerable heterogeneity
TMS-EEG and TMS-fMRI
and difficult-to-interpret results. However, when appropri-
ately controlled, state-dependency can be manipulated to TMS-EEG allows one to “perturb” the brain with pulses
purposively shape the direction and/or magnitude of of TMS and then use EEG to measure its effects. TMS
TMS effects. This topic has been explored at multiple pulses produce a series of EEG waveforms that are
levels, from behavioral (e.g., level of arousal) to neurophys- referred to as TEPs (Ilmoniemi et al., 1999). These TEPs
iological (e.g., type of EEG waveform present) and has are commonly defined according to time-point and the
been comprehensively reviewed elsewhere (Silvanto and direction of inflection (e.g., N15, P30) and last up to
Pascual-Leone, 2008). 300 ms after the TMS pulse (Komssi and K€ahk€onen,
Another important stipulation is that the vast majority 2006). TMS-EEG has the ability to map motor and
of studies assessing the effects of TMS are based on stud- nonmotor changes in cortical excitability, evaluate con-
ies stimulating and recording output from the motor cor- nectivity between brain regions, and measure frequency
tex. However, many TMS studies target regions outside bands of intrinsic oscillations generated after perturbation
of the motor cortex, such as the DLPFC. Whether or (Shafi et al., 2012, 2014; Chung et al., 2015). The combi-
not the same or similar excitatory/inhibitory changes nation of this rich output data with millisecond temporal
occur in such nonmotor brain regions remains largely resolution makes TMS-EEG an extremely powerful tool
TRANSCRANIAL MAGNETIC STIMULATION 79
to probe the neurophysiological effects of TMS. How- PART II: TMS AS A PROBE FOR FRONTAL
ever, it should be noted that this tool is still in develop- LOBE PHYSIOLOGY
ment. Ongoing efforts by research groups around the
world are trying to optimize approaches to clean artifacts
Virtual lesions, chronometry, and
produced by the TMS pulse and associated peripheral
neuromodulation
sensations (van de Laar et al., 2016) and to ensure that TMS applied as single pulses or trains of repetitive pulses
the data collected is robust and has good test–retest reli- can be used to transiently disrupt a targeted brain region,
ability (Kerwin et al., 2017). creating a “virtual lesion” (Pascual-Leone et al., 1999;
TMS has also been successfully paired with neuro- Valero-Cabre et al., 2017). The mechanisms underpin-
imaging modalities such as fMRI and PET. TMS-fMRI ning this technique are complex but will be summarized
provides a platform for conducting similar “perturb briefly. The experimental setup typically involves perfor-
and measure” studies and provides additional value mance of a cognitive/behavioral task while the subject is
toward better understanding brain-wide TMS effects simultaneously being stimulated by TMS. The TMS
(Fox et al., 2012a, b). TMS-fMRI offers superior spa- pulses are localized to a brain region believed to be impli-
tial resolution (but reduced temporal resolution) to cated in the function of interest (e.g., based on previous
TMS-EEG, which is particularly advantageous for neuroimaging or lesion studies). These pulses effectively
identifying activity in remote brain regions and connec- act as electrical interference by depolarizing and keeping
tivity patterns during TMS stimulation (Ruff et al., targeted neurons refractory such that they are unable to
2009). While this technique has been shown to be tech- contribute to the coding of a given process. Changes in
nically feasible, high-quality systematic methods to task performance can be assessed by comparing active
remove artifacts and improve signal-to-noise ratios TMS stimulation trials to either trials of no stimulation
remains an ongoing process (de Lara et al., 2017). or, ideally, a sham TMS trial. The latter helps control
Finally, from a logistical perspective, the specialized for factors involved in TMS delivery that are not specific
equipment required for TMS-fMRI studies could pose to the targeted stimulation (e.g., sound clicks, tactile
a barrier to its more widespread use as a clinical scalp sensations, and placebo/expectation-based effects).
research tool. See Fig. 5.2 for a schematic depiction of the virtual lesion

Fig. 5.2. General framework for TMS “virtual lesion” neurophysiological studies. In this example, TMS is delivered in single
pulses or repetitive trains to a target brain region concurrently with the selected cognitive task. TMS-induced disruption in task
performance is evaluated by comparison to either completing the task with no stimulation or, ideally, a sham coil control group.
EEG and fMRI can be added to the experimental setup to provide valuable additional neurophysiological data.
80 M.J. BURKE ET AL.
methodology. Additional chronometric methodological the precise definition of executive functioning is evolv-
components can be added to this design to determine ing, its core components are commonly acknowledged to
the time at which the function of a given brain region include inhibition and interference control (i.e., the abil-
becomes causally critical (Walsh and Pascual-Leone, ity to suppress certain behaviors in favor of others),
2003). This protocol harnesses the high temporal resolu- working memory (the ability to maintain and manipulate
tion of single pulses or brief TMS trains that can be information in the absence of persistent perceptual
sequentially delivered during specific time windows in input), and cognitive flexibility (the abilities to think cre-
relation to the behavioral task. The statistical analyses atively, respond quickly to changing circumstances, and
employed by these studies can be complicated but essen- choose between multiple strategies). Along with atten-
tially compare times that TMS stimulation impacted per- tion, executive functions underlie nearly all other higher
formance with times when it did not. Chronometric TMS cognitive abilities (Courtney, 2004).
studies have been invaluable in understanding the tempo- The earliest and most convincing evidence for the
ral sequence of complex cognitive/behavioral functions. role of the PFC in executive functions came from obser-
In some TMS neurophysiological studies, longer vations of patients with traumatic brain injuries or
trains of rTMS are applied prior to the task as a way of other naturally occurring lesions (including stroke and
modulating activity in the target brain region instead of tumors). Patients with damage to the PFC often exhibit
stimulating during the task to induce online disruption. changes in personality, mood, social behaviors, and cog-
Depending on the desired neuromodulatory effect, pre- nitive functions that are collectively referred to as dys-
task rTMS can be applied with different parameters executive syndrome (Thompson-Schill et al., 2002; du
aimed to induce an excitatory or inhibitory effect. This Boisgueheneuc et al., 2006; Kumar et al., 2013). Indeed,
technique is based on short-term plasticity mechanisms the original term for the constellation of symptoms was
rather than electrical interference and can also be used frontal lobe syndrome, acknowledging the common
to shed light on brain–behavior relationships. location of damage in the PFC. However, there are many
Finally, it is important to emphasize that TMS neuro- challenges inherent in using naturally occurring lesions
physiological methods do not solely implicate causality to infer structure–function relationships about the brain.
of the brain structure directly stimulated but also the neu- Studies in nonhuman animals (mainly rhesus macaque
ral network that the region/node may be connected to. and other primates) and neuroimaging studies in humans
The previously described TMS-EEG and TMS-fMRI have provided critical supporting evidence, but each
perturbation paradigms have provided the best evidence of these approaches in turn suffers from its own limita-
to support this critical principle. Many cognitive func- tions. While precise lesions and baseline testing can be
tions ascribed to the frontal lobes have been studied using performed in nonhuman animals, their cognitive abili-
TMS neurophysiological techniques. However, as per ties and brain structures are inarguably different from
the previous qualifier, it is perhaps best to conceptualize humans, which limits the generalizability of any infer-
such “frontal lobe functions” as network functions with ences that can be made. In the case of functional neuro-
critical nodes residing in frontal lobe structures. In this imaging in humans, fMRI and EEG can demonstrate the
section, we will focus on the core PFC topics of working spatial distribution of hemodynamic responses and the
memory and executive functions. We will also briefly temporal dynamics of summed postsynaptic activity,
discuss language processing and provide a few other respectively, that are associated with a given cognitive
more abstract examples that encapsulate the wide spec- process. However, the inferences that can be made are
trum of TMS’s utility in studying the frontal lobes. largely correlational in nature and influenced by choices
in data processing and analysis routines.
With the advent of noninvasive brain stimulation, it
Memory and executive functions
has been possible to directly interfere with the ongoing
With its location rostral to the primary motor and premo- activity of a specific brain area in a temporary, reversible
tor cortices, the PFC is chiefly responsible for integrating manner in awake, healthy humans. While the “virtual
information from the rest of the brain for the purpose of lesions” induced by rTMS do not replicate the complete
planning and executing complex goal-directed behavior. loss of function of an invasive lesion or reversible
In this role, the PFC is often conceptualized as the central deactivation, they nonetheless have the capacity to
executive of the brain (i.e., the pinnacle of top-down pro- change neural activity in a manner sufficient to draw
cesses) and its range of cognitive abilities are described causal inferences regarding brain–behavior relation-
as executive functions (also executive control or cogni- ships. Moreover, the possibility of inducing bidirectional
tive control) (Diamond, 2013). These functions encapsu- changes in activity and metabolism (i.e., decreasing or
late purposeful mental activity contrasted with automatic increasing activity depending on the frequency and pat-
or routine behaviors (i.e., running on “autopilot”). While tern of stimulation) provides for the relatively unique
TRANSCRANIAL MAGNETIC STIMULATION 81
observation of enhanced cognitive performance rather Researchers have also used rTMS to understand how
than only looking at diminished or lost functions. the PFC is segregated with respect to domain, or the
Indeed, some of the earliest reports of the impact of pre- type of information in WM. Mottaghy et al. (2002b)
frontal rTMS on cognition came from improvements in applied 1-Hz rTMS to temporarily suppress local activity
working memory and attention following treatment of in one of three regions of left PFC: dorsomedial PFC
medication-resistant major depression (Guse et al., (DMPFC), dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC), and ventrolateral
2010; Martin et al., 2017). PFC (VLPFC). The impact was measured as a pre-/
postchange in performance on a delayed response task
using either faces or dots in different locations. The
TMS and working memory
authors found that accuracy on the spatial dot WM task
Numerous studies have used rTMS, either in short trains was reduced following stimulation of either DMPFC or
to disrupt ongoing activity or longer regimens to mod- DLPFC, while the face WM task was impacted by stim-
ulate activity, to demonstrate the causal role of the PFC ulation of either DLPFC or VLPFC (Mottaghy et al.,
to WM (Pascual-Leone and Hallett, 1994; Mottaghy 2002b). These findings suggest a dorsal–ventral axis
et al., 2000, 2002a, 2003a), including distinguishing within the lateral PFC for spatial vs nonspatial informa-
contributions of PFC from posterior parietal cortex tion. This is consistent with a model of information pro-
(PPC) (Koch et al., 2005; Postle et al., 2006; Hamidi cessing in nonhuman primates, in which the dorsal and
et al., 2008, 2009), and/or functionally dissociate ventral segregation of visual information (Ungerleider
WM by process and/or type of information (Mottaghy and Mishkin, 1982) is maintained within the lateral
et al., 2002b; Sandrini et al., 2008; Zanto et al., 2011; PFC (Petrides and Pandya, 1984; Barbas, 1988).
Fried et al., 2014). As discussed in the following para- An alternative model suggests that human networks
graphs, the evidence gained from these studies is some- for WM are similar to language in displaying a left/right
what inconsistent, reflecting in part the wide range of hemispheric segregation depending on whether or not the
approaches (i.e., rTMS protocols, study populations, information in storage can be verbalized (Acheson et al.,
and the cognitive assessments employed) but also likely 2010). Consistent with this model, Fried et al. (2014)
the natural individual variation in the organization of used 1-Hz rTMS to separately modulate activity in the
the PFC. left and right DLPFC and assessed performance using
In one of the earliest studies to investigate the segre- verbal and spatial versions of a 3-back task. The authors
gation of short-term/working memory by process, found evidence of a subtle double dissociation: putatively
Pascual-Leone and Hallett (1994) were able to disrupt reducing left DLPFC activity tended to both decrease ver-
recall by applying short trains of high-frequency rTMS bal performance while increasing spatial accuracy, while
to either left or right lateral PFC, but not M1, during the opposite was observed when the right DLPFC was
the delay period of a delayed response task. Extending stimulated (Fried et al., 2014). Some TMS studies have
these findings, Koch et al. (2005) used a spatial sequence found similar evidence of a verbal/spatial hemispheric
matching task to compare right PFC and PPC and specialization (Mull and Seyal, 2001; Sandrini et al.,
observed PFC TMS impacted performance during both 2008), while others have failed to find such a distinction
the delay and decision periods, while PPC stimulation (Oliveri et al., 2001; Mottaghy et al., 2002a, 2003a; Rami
impacted only the delay. In a series of studies using a et al., 2003; Postle et al., 2006).
spatial delayed match-to-sample task, Hamidi et al. To reconcile these inconsistencies, it has been sug-
(2008, 2009) found TMS (to either hemisphere) affected gested that the PFC is organized by both domain and
the delay period when applied to PPC, but not PFC, the type of process (Sandrini et al., 2008; Zanto et al.,
while the decision period was selectively impacted by 2011). Accordingly, the left and right PFC process both
stimulation of the PFC, but not PPC. Several other stud- verbal and nonverbal information but are selectively
ies similarly failed to find effects on WM of PFC stim- engaged depending on the task demands. Sandrini
ulation during the delay period (Herwig et al., 2003a, b; et al. (2008) tested this theory by comparing a combined
Postle et al., 2006; Luber et al., 2007), implying the PFC verbal/spatial n-back task (letters appearing in different
is more associated with the executive aspects of WM spatial location and subjects had to attend to one aspect
(i.e., monitoring, manipulating, inhibiting) rather than while inhibiting the other) with traditional single-domain
simple maintenance or short-term storage over a delay. versions. Using a short train of 10-Hz rTMS, the authors
This is consistent with single unit recording studies reported a lateralized interference only for the combined
(Fuster and Alexander, 1971; Chafee and Goldman- stimuli version, suggesting the left and right DLPFC
Rakic, 1998) and lesions/deactivation studies (Bauer were responsible for inhibiting verbal and spatial aspects
and Fuster, 1976; Funahashi et al., 1993; Moore et al., of the stimuli, respectively (Sandrini et al., 2008). While
2012) in nonhuman primates. the organization of the PFC with respect to WM remains
82 M.J. BURKE ET AL.
a topic of debate, the “virtual lesion” approach offered by 1 Hz over the supersylvian region (homologous to pri-
TMS has and will continue to play an important role. mate intraparietal sulcus). In humans, attempts have
A detailed table of TMS neurophysiological studies of been made to better understand the impact of rTMS
memory has been previously completed and is available in the PFC by combining it with neuroimaging methods
elsewhere (Brem et al., 2013). such as fMRI (Herwig et al., 2003a, b), PET (Mottaghy
et al., 2000, 2003b), and EEG (Zanto et al., 2011;
TMS and other executive functions Shields et al., 2018). Herwig et al. (2003a, b) were
among the first to use task-based activation from fMRI
TMS has also been used to investigate the role of the
to guide TMS, an approach that is now widely used
PFC in attention and other executive functions such as
(Acheson et al., 2010; Zanto et al., 2011). Other studies
inhibitory/cognitive control and cognitive flexibility
have used fMRI to map the physiological changes
(e.g., set-shifting, verbal fluency) (Guse et al., 2010;
induced by rTMS: Andoh and Paus (2011) demon-
Lowe et al., 2018). In a series of experiments,
strated high-frequency rTMS reduced task-associated
Vanderhasselt et al. (2006a, b, 2007) investigated the role
activity on the homologous region of the contralateral
of the DLPFC in inhibitory control, set-shifting, and top-
hemisphere; Esslinger et al. (2012) observed 5-Hz
down attentional processes. The authors observed that
rTMS of the right DLPFC induced improvements in
10-Hz rTMS to the left DLPFC improved reaction times
reaction time on an n-back task that were accompanied
on the Stroop task for both congruent and incongruent tri-
by hemodynamic changes in a distributed network of
als (Vanderhasselt et al., 2006a, 2007), while targeting
regions functionally connected to the DLPFC (though
the right DLPFC improved reaction times for cued trials
local changes were not observed).
on a task-switching paradigm (Vanderhasselt et al.,
In a series of experiments using PET, Mottaghy et al.
2006b). By comparison, Huang et al. (2004) found no
(2000, 2003b) demonstrated that rTMS-induced modula-
change in go–no go task performance following 5-Hz
tion of WM performance resulted from decreasing
rTMS to the left DLPFC. Smirni et al. (2017) used
regional cerebral blood flow levels locally within the
1-Hz rTMS to both the right and left DLPFC and reported
DLPFC but also by disrupting the balance of activity
decreased verbal fluency after 1 Hz to the left but
across a frontoparietal WM network. Zanto et al. (2011)
increased after 1 Hz to the right. While these experiments
used fMRI to guide the selection of rTMS targets in the
were conducted in healthy individuals, many other
PFC and used EEG to understand its top-down effects
studies have used rTMS to try to improve memory and
on parietal and occipital cortices. They found that reduc-
executive functions in clinical populations. These appli-
tions in task accuracy were accompanied by attenuation of
cations will be discussed further in the section “Part III:
the P1 component associated with attentional modulation
Clinical Applications”.
of sensory features. Similarly, Shields et al. (2018) used
changes in task-evoked EEG activity to help explain
Validating behavioral results with
how 1-Hz rTMS to the DLPFC blunted practice effects
neuroimaging and EEG
for young, but not old, participants. Li et al. (2017) used
When interpreting reports of behavioral changes induced EEG to provide mechanistic insight into the impact of
by neuromodulatory regimens such as rTMS, it is critically multiple daily 10-Hz rTMS sessions targeting the left
important to be aware of the assumptions made regarding DLPFC on cognitive control. They found an improvement
the physiological mechanisms behind the observed in reaction time on the Stroop task that was associated with
changes. As discussed in the section “Part I: Basis and increased amplitudes of the N2 and N450 potentials dur-
Technological Principles of TMS”, the most direct evi- ing incongruent trials (Li et al., 2017).
dence for the modulatory effects of rTMS in humans come While the aforementioned studies demonstrate the
from measuring its effect on the amplitude of MEPs eli- added potential of combining rTMS with neuroimaging
cited by single-pulse TMS to the hand region of primary and EEG techniques, the reality is that the vast majority
motor cortex (Pascual-Leone et al., 1994; Maeda et al., of rTMS studies to date investigating the PFC do not
2000a, b). It is therefore enticing to assume that because have an independent neurophysiological measure of
low- and high-frequency rTMS protocols in the motor the impact of stimulation, which limits the cogency
cortex tend to decrease and increase average MEP ampli- of their conclusions.
tudes, respectively, that the same protocols would produce
a similar effect in nonmotor regions such as the PFC.
Language
Consistent with this assumption, Valero-Cabre et al.
(2007) used radiolabeled glucose injections in the Language has been one of the longest studied cognitive
cat to demonstrate that metabolism (glucose uptake) functions using the TMS virtual lesion method. Pascual-
increased following 20 Hz and decreased following Leone et al. (1991) conducted the first such study in six
TRANSCRANIAL MAGNETIC STIMULATION 83
presurgical epilepsy patients. The protocol applied 10-s (Knoch et al., 2006). Reciprocal fairness is the concept of
trains of high-frequency rTMS while patients were asked enforcing social fairness norms through the punishment
to count aloud. Stimulation was localized to 15 different of other’s unfair behaviors even if it hurts their own self-
targets in perisylvian regions of both hemispheres. When interest (Fehr and Fischbacher, 2003). The Ultimatum
stimulation was applied to the left but not right inferior Game can be used to assess reciprocal fairness by eval-
frontal gyrus (Broca’s area), speech arrest suggestive of uating the extent to which players reject their bargaining
transient motor aphasia was observed in all subjects. Intra- partner’s unfair offers despite an economic incentive to
carotid amobarbital testing was also conducted and con- accept such offers (G€uth et al., 1982). Knoch and col-
firmed left hemisphere language dominance in all leagues reported that 15 min of low-frequency rTMS
subjects. Subsequent TMS studies have largely corrobo- applied to the right, but not left, DLPFC reduced willing-
rated these findings and have attempted to optimize stim- ness to reject intentionally unfair offers (even though the
ulation protocols to improve interstudy variability (Jennum subjects still judged them as unfair). Put in other words,
et al., 1994; Epstein et al., 1996). This line of research has inhibition of the right DLPFC yielded subjects less able
provided a strong foundation for using TMS for presurgical to resist economic temptation and thus were weaker
mapping of language function. TMS devices for this indi- enforcers of principles of reciprocal fairness. This study
cation are FDA approved and will be discussed further in provides evidence for the causal involvement of the right
the section “Part III: Clinical Applications”. DLPFC in decision-making surrounding fairness-related
Since Pascual-Leone and colleagues’ landmark study behaviors.
over 25 years ago, there have been numerous efforts Another interesting example comes from TMS
to further probe the language system using TMS. This research investigating the role of the PFC in placebo
includes critical work identifying the regional func- responses. A variety of structural and functional neu-
tional anatomy of semantic and phonologic processes in roimaging studies in healthy volunteers have identified
Broca’s area (Devlin et al., 2003; Aziz-Zadeh et al., the DLPFC as a critical node in the placebo network.
2005; Sakreida et al., 2018), uncovering unexpected con- The majority of these studies have focused on placebo
nections between speech comprehension, language analgesia, but similar findings have been demonstrated
knowledge, and motor systems (Watkins et al., 2003; across many placebo paradigms (Stein et al., 2012;
Berent et al., 2015), and identifying a critical role for the Wager and Atlas, 2015). Further support for the impor-
left inferior frontal gyrus in processing grammar and syn- tant role of the DLPFC came from a longitudinal study
tax (Sakai et al., 2002). The latter study by Sakai and col- of placebo analgesia in patients with Alzheimer’s
leagues exemplifies the potential utility of chronometric disease (AD) (Benedetti et al., 2006). The rationale
TMS protocols. They eloquently showed that TMS- for this study was that cognitively impaired patients
induced effects on syntactic processing of sentences only may not have expectations about therapeutic benefits
occurred when stimulation was applied during a specific and thus may have diminished placebo responses. Indeed,
time window (150ms after the start of verb presentation). they found that the magnitude of placebo effects nega-
TMS studies have also contributed to our growing tively correlated with cognitive status but also with pre-
understanding of how the brain may functionally reorga- frontal functional connectivity. These studies provide
nize after damage (e.g., stroke) to left-sided language a strong signal for the role of the PFC in placebo mech-
areas. Most notably, this includes: (i) the potential benefit anisms but are correlative in nature. Krummenacher
of recruiting ipsilateral lesional and perilesional cortical and colleagues conducted a TMS placebo analgesia
regions and (ii) the potential detriment of increased right study aimed at adding direct causal inference to this
hemispheric activity leading to transcollosal interhemi- evidence base. In this study of 40 healthy volunteers,
spheric inhibition of the left-sided language areas half were given the expectation that the TMS device
(Hamilton et al., 2011). These neurophysiological princi- would yield analgesic effects and half were told that
ples have been translated to develop novel clinical treat- the device was simply recording data. Both groups
ment strategies of TMS for poststroke aphasia and will were then given either 15 min of 1-Hz rTMS to right
be discussed further in the section “Part III: Clinical and left DLPFC or sham stimulation. They found that
Applications”. active rTMS completely abolished strong placebo
responses (ratings of pain tolerance and pain thresh-
olds) in the group that was given analgesic effect expec-
Other examples
tation (Krummenacher et al., 2010). Such studies
More abstract social and moral based “frontal lobe” func- raise the intriguing possibility of using rTMS with dif-
tions have also been cleverly investigated using TMS ferent parameters to possibly enhance placebo response
methodologies. One of the best examples of this comes and thus improve the therapeutic effect of various
from Knoch and colleagues’ work on reciprocal fairness interventions.
84 M.J. BURKE ET AL.
PART III: CLINICAL APPLICATIONS treatments. The clinical applications of TMS biomarkers
are still in their infancy but recently there have been large
Presurgical mapping strides toward reaching this objective.
TMS virtual lesion methodologies have been clinically AD has been one focus of such efforts and implicates
translated as a novel noninvasive methodology for pre- many of the neurophysiological measures described in
surgical motor and language mapping. We will focus the section “Part I: Basis and Technological Principles
on the example of language mapping to build on themes of TMS” (Freitas et al., 2011). First, patients with mild
discussed earlier in this chapter. and moderate AD often present with lower RMTs, as evi-
In the surgical management of tumors in or near lan- dence of cortical hyperexcitability (Cantone et al., 2014).
guage “eloquent” brain regions (e.g., left-sided inferior The exact mechanism underpinning this increased excit-
frontal, supramarginal, and superior temporal gyri), neuro- ability is debated but some studies suggest that it may
surgeons must balance the extent of their resection with parallel early disease progression (Ferreri et al., 2011).
the preservation of critical language functions. To evaluate Interestingly, in end-stage AD, RMT can increase, creat-
and optimize considerations prior to the surgery, surgeons ing an overall bimodal distribution. This latter observa-
have traditionally used approaches that include fMRI, tion may be related to the burden of cortical neuronal
PET, EEG, and/or magnetoencephalography (MEG). loss and atrophy (Perretti et al., 1996). In an early study
More recently, MRI-navigated TMS has been investigated of SICI in AD patients, Liepert and colleagues found
as a means of directly interrogating language pathways reduced SICI compared with healthy controls, and the
and adding causal data to preoperative decision-making level of reduction correlated with dementia severity
algorithms. Similar to previously described neuropsycho- (Liepert et al., 2001). However, these findings have been
logic studies, TMS single pulses or rTMS trains can be inconsistently replicated (Di Lazzaro et al., 2004). Per-
delivered to a specific brain region during a language task haps a more robust TMS measure in AD is SAI, in which
(e.g., object naming). If stimulation of the targeted region multiple studies have shown more consistent alterations
results in consistent task impairment (e.g., naming errors) in cortical excitability (Freitas et al., 2011; Cantone et al.,
then one can infer that the region may be necessary for the 2014). As previously described, SAI mechanisms are
patient’s language function and should ideally not be believed to reflect central cholinergic activity and thus
included in the resection. This process is then repeated these findings support a model of cholinergic deficit in
multiple times with different cortical targets (typically AD patients. This is further strengthened by findings that
5–10mm apart) until a map of brain coordinates that are cholinesterase inhibitors increase SAI in AD patients
causally implicated and not implicated is produced (Di Lazzaro et al., 2002, 2005). SAI has also been found
(Lioumis et al., 2012; Tarapore et al., 2013). to be significantly reduced in amnestic MCI patients but
TMS presurgical mapping has been shown to correlate not in nonamnestic MCI patients (Nardone et al., 2012).
well with the gold standard of intraoperative direct cortical Accordingly, Nardone and colleagues suggest that SAI
stimulation and may offer superior accuracy to presurgical may predict the progression to AD, but this requires fur-
fMRI and MEG (Krieg et al., 2012; Picht et al., 2013; ther study and corroboration. Other single- or paired-
Tarapore et al., 2013). There is also evidence suggesting pulse TMS measures such as LICI, ICF, and cortical
that TMS mapping may improve patient outcomes by silent period have not yielded reliable abnormalities in
optimizing patient selection, allowing for larger safe resec- AD (Freitas et al., 2011; Cantone et al., 2014). Finally,
tions with smaller craniotomies and reducing long-term changes in rTMS-induced plasticity (i.e., ability of
neurologic deficits (Frey et al., 2014; Sollmann et al., LTP/LTD-like plasticity responses to be induced) have
2015). Currently, the Nexstim eXimia Navigated Brain also been demonstrated in AD patients. Studies in this
System TMS device has been cleared by the FDA area are relatively limited but preliminary findings sug-
for motor and language mapping and its clinical use is gest that AD patients may have impaired plasticity
rapidly expanding. responses to both traditional rTMS and theta-burst proto-
cols (Cantone et al., 2014; Fried et al., 2017).
Frontotemporal dementia (FTD) is another relevant
Diagnostic biomarkers
disorder that has been investigated for potential TMS
One of the primary goals of investigating TMS cortical markers of disease. In contrast to AD, the majority of cor-
excitability and plasticity paradigms has been to try to tical excitability measures, including RMT and SAI,
develop neurophysiological signatures of different brain have not yielded significant changes. In one study, SICI
diseases. Many disorders in neurology and psychiatry was shown to be depressed in FTD patients with the pro-
lack objective biomarkers, which can result in challeng- gressive nonfluent aphasia subtype but was normal in
ing clinical diagnoses, heterogeneity in clinical trial patients with behavioral variant FTD and semantic
enrollment, and difficulty quantifying response to dementia (Burrell et al., 2011). Patients with FTD do
TRANSCRANIAL MAGNETIC STIMULATION 85
show consistent depression of a variety of nonspecific depression (Brunoni et al., 2017). The main goal of this
TMS measures assessing the integrity of central motor TMS clinical application is to increase excitability of
circuits, such as reduced MEP amplitude, increased the left DLPFC as a means to modulate activity both
MEP latency, and increased corticomotor conduction locally and throughout its connected neural network. This
times (Burrell et al., 2011). This complements lines of is based on two fundamental principles: (i) in most patients
research suggesting that FTD may have subclinical with depression, the left DLPFC has been shown to be
motor dysfunction and shared pathophysiological mech- hypoactive and hypometabolic on functional neuroimag-
anisms with motor neuron diseases such as ALS. ing studies (Baxter et al., 1989; Fitzgerald et al., 2006) and
A recent study conducted by Benussi and colleagues (ii) clinical improvement with antidepressant pharmaco-
aimed to consolidate much of the data presented earlier to therapies has been associated with increased activity of
see if AD could be reliably distinguished from FTD the left DLPFC (Mayberg et al., 2000). The first controlled
based on a panel of TMS neurophysiological measures clinical trial on the use of rTMS for medication-resistant
(Benussi et al., 2017). They compared RMT, SICI, depression was published in 1996 (Pascual-Leone et al.,
ICF, LICI, and SAI in 79 patients with AD, 64 patients 1996) and revealed the laterality of the effects and the
with FTD (22 patients with primary progressive aphasia dependency on the specific rTMS protocol. The most
and 42 with behavioral variant), and 32 healthy controls. commonly used protocol involves high-frequency (e.g.,
They found significant impairment of SAI among 10-Hz) rTMS applied to the left DLPFC for 37.5 min,
AD patients and significant impairment of combined 5 days per week over 4–6 weeks (Lefaucheur et al.,
SICI/ICF in FTD patients. Receiver operating character- 2014). However, a recent randomized, multicenter, clini-
istic curve analyses revealed that TMS markers differen- cal noninferiority trial has shown that intermittent theta
tiated FTD from AD with a sensitivity of 91.8% and burst applied to the left DLPFC may achieve equivalent
specificity of 88.6%, AD from healthy controls with a efficacy to the traditional rTMS protocol and can be con-
sensitivity of 84.8% and specificity of 90.6%, and ducted in a fraction of the time (average 3 min/session)
FTD from healthy controls with a sensitivity of 90.2% (Blumberger et al., 2018). Though more work is needed
and specificity of 78.1%. These results highlight the to corroborate Blumberger and colleagues’ findings,
potential clinical utility of TMS as a noninvasive diag- these results are consistent with data discussed in the
nostic biomarker in cognitive neurology practice. section “Part I: Basis and Technological Principles of
Briefly, TMS neurophysiological measures are also TMS” regarding theta burst’s potential ability to more
being actively studied as biomarkers for a variety of other rapidly and efficiently induce synaptic long-term poten-
neurologic and psychiatric conditions outside of the tiation (Suppa et al., 2016). Other stimulation targets and
focus of this chapter and this book. Most notably, protocols exist, such as low-frequency rTMS to the right
single- and paired-pulse TMS “threshold tracking” has DLPFC; however, the evidence supporting their effi-
been shown to be a robust early marker of upper motor cacy is less robust (Lefaucheur et al., 2014).
neuron dysfunction in patients with ALS (Menon In addition to the clinical efficacy demonstrated in
et al., 2015). ALS can be a challenging diagnosis to make clinical trials, rTMS for depression has also been found
(particularly with respect to identifying upper motor neu- to have real-world clinical effectiveness (George et al.,
ron signs), and thus TMS techniques are rapidly being 2013) and cost effectiveness (Nguyen and Gordon,
adopted in ALS clinical settings and in research trials. 2015). To date, the FDA has cleared six different TMS
Other areas of ongoing TMS biomarker research include devices for medication-resistant depression and insur-
epilepsy (de Goede et al., 2016), autism (Uzunova et al., ance companies in many countries around the world
2016), multiple sclerosis (Simpson and Macdonell, now cover the cost of treatments.
2015), schizophrenia (Hasan et al., 2013), and a variety
of movement disorders (e.g., Parkinson’s disease, pro-
Treatment of AD
gressive supranuclear palsy, multiple system atrophy,
and Huntington’s disease) (Vucic and Kiernan, 2017). Large-scale research efforts are actively investigating
many other therapeutic applications of TMS with frontal
lobe targets. AD has been studied over the past 5–10
Treatment of medication-resistant
years and preliminary results are promising. In one ran-
depression
domized three-arm pilot study of patients with mild-to-
The ability of rTMS to noninvasively modulate target moderate AD, 20-Hz rTMS applied to the left and right
brain regions has been translated to provide new treat- DLPFC was found to improve cognitive function when
ment strategies for a variety of neurologic and psychiatric compared to low-frequency TMS and sham stimulation
disorders (Lefaucheur et al., 2014). Currently, the most- groups. Significant improvements were observed on all
recognized therapeutic indication is medication-resistant outcome measures (Mini Mental Status Examination,
86 M.J. BURKE ET AL.
Instrumental Daily Living Activity scale, and Geriatric Weiduschat et al., 2011) stimulation strategies have
Depression Scale) and were assessed at 1 and 3 months shown benefit in small pilot studies. The latter approach
(Ahmed et al., 2012). However, another sham-controlled has been further evaluated by a randomized sham-
study with a similar 20-Hz rTMS protocol (applied only controlled trial that aimed to relate clinical effects to
to the left DLPFC) did not find any significant improve- brain imaging activation patterns (Thiel et al., 2013).
ments in memory or executive functions but did report This study enrolled 24 right-handed, subacute ischemic
significant improvement in sentence comprehension stroke patients with left MCA infarctions and clinically
(Cotelli et al., 2011). documented aphasia. The treatment protocol consisted
There is also a line of TMS AD research that combines of 10 days of daily 20-min sessions of low-frequency
TMS with cognitive training (rTMS-COG). This proto- 1-Hz rTMS over the right posterior inferior frontal gyrus
col consists of 10-Hz rTMS that targets six different brain combined with 45 min of poststimulation speech and lan-
regions believed to be implicated in AD. During the stim- guage therapy. Comparing rTMS to the sham group, they
ulation, patients receive concurrent cognitive training reported significant improvements in language perfor-
targeting the function of the corresponding brain region. mance on their primary outcome of the Aachen Aphasia
The six regions include: right and left DLPFC (working Test score. They also conducted pre/post-PET scans and
memory, judgment, and executive functions), Broca’s found that patients in the rTMS group activated propor-
and Wernicke’s areas (language production and percep- tionally more voxels in left hemisphere language net-
tion, respectively), and right and left parietal somatosen- works after treatment compared with sham group
sory association cortex (spatial/topographical orientation patients. Based on these findings, a large multicenter trial
and praxis). The treatments occur for 1 h daily, five of this combined TMS and speech-language therapy pro-
sessions/week for 6 weeks, followed by biweekly ses- tocol is currently underway (NORTHSTAR trial). Given
sions for 3 months. After a positive open-label proof- the scope of this book, we have focused on poststroke
of-concept study (Bentwich et al., 2011), Rabey and aphasia; however, it should be noted that rTMS treatment
colleagues completed a small randomized, double-blind, trials of motor recovery after stroke have also been quite
sham-controlled study of the rTMS-COG intervention. promising (Hsu et al., 2012; Du et al., 2016) and trials are
They reported significant improvements on their primary ongoing.
outcome of Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale-
Cognitive Subscale (ADAS-Cog) and also found their Other neurologic and psychiatric treatments
protocol to be safe and well tolerated (Rabey et al.,
The list of potential TMS applications in neurology and
2013). A large, pivotal, premarket approval study of
psychiatry is lengthy and rapidly expanding month by
rTMS-COG has been completed but the results are cur-
month. A comprehensive review and evidence-based
rently awaiting publication.
guidelines of all major applications have been provided
in a consensus statement of European TMS experts
Treatment of stroke (Lefaucheur et al., 2014). Briefly, additional indications
with prefrontal targets include addiction (right/left
Stroke rehabilitation for aphasia and motor weakness is
DLPFC), obsessive–compulsive disorder (left orbito-
another exciting area of translational TMS research. After
frontal cortex), anorexia nervosa (left DLPFC), negative
stroke, a variety of neuroplastic changes occurs that may
symptoms of schizophrenia (right/left DLPFC), and
lead to decreased excitability of lesioned/perilesional
posttraumatic stress disorder (right DLPFC). Outside
brain regions and increased excitability of the homotopic
of the PFC, other major fields of TMS therapeutic
regions in the contralateral hemisphere (Cicinelli et al.,
research include chronic pain (motor cortex), movement
2003). This imbalance is believed to be maladaptive
disorders (motor cortex, premotor/supplementary motor
and primarily mediated by abnormally increased interhe-
cortices), auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia (left
mispheric inhibition from the unaffected to the affected
temporal–parietal cortex), epilepsy (targeted to epilepto-
side (Ferbert et al., 1992; Shimizu et al., 2002). TMS
genic focus), and migraine (occipital cortex). With
stroke rehabilitation strategies aim to restore this disequi-
regards to the latter, the eNeura single-pulse TMS device
librium via two main approaches: (i) increase cortical
has recently received FDA approval for migraine acute
excitability of the affected hemisphere (for example,
abortive therapy (Lipton et al., 2010) and as a daily pre-
using high-frequency rTMS) or (ii) decrease cortical
ventative treatment (Starling et al., 2018).
excitability of the contralateral hemisphere (for exam-
ple, using low-frequency TMS) (Fregni and Pascual-
Conclusions and future directions
Leone, 2007).
For poststroke aphasia, both ipsilateral (Holland TMS is a noninvasive brain stimulation technology
et al., 2011) and contralateral (Barwood et al., 2011; that has provided extensive contributions toward better
TRANSCRANIAL MAGNETIC STIMULATION 87
understanding frontal lobe functions and relevant that could dramatically improve future TMS-based
brain–behavior relationships. TMS is increasingly being research. These innovations can generally be split into
translated for use in clinical settings and represents a two major categories: (i) refinement of existing technol-
promising new treatment approach for many complex ogy, such as new TMS-EEG analysis techniques and
neurologic and psychiatric disorders that currently have new TMS navigation tools integrated with functional
limited treatment options. However, we need to empha- connectivity and (ii) new technologies that are currently
size that, aside from depression, most of the therapeutic in the process of being translated to humans from animal
indications discussed in this chapter have yet to have studies, such as TMS microcoils (Park et al., 2013)
devices cleared by the FDA for on-label use. As such, and noninvasive deep-brain stimulation techniques
their use remains restricted to off-label and experimental (Lozano, 2017). These and other advances will undoubt-
investigational research. edly accelerate the ongoing efforts investigating brain–
We also must point out that there are still many unan- behavior causality, diagnostic biomarkers, and clinical
swered questions regarding TMS mechanisms of action. treatments.
This includes mechanisms at the cellular/neuronal level
as discussed in the section “Part I: Basis and Technological REFERENCES
Principles of TMS”, but also relates to therapeutic mech- Acheson DJ, Hamidi M, Binder JR et al. (2010). A common
anisms at the clinical trial level. With respect to the latter, neural substrate for language production and verbal work-
there is a clear need to better understand the degree of indi- ing memory. J Cogn Neurosci 23: 1358–1367.
vidual variability in the physiological effects of TMS and Ahmed MA, Darwish ES, Khedr EM et al. (2012). Effects of
the nature and magnitude of TMS’s placebo effects. TMS low versus high frequencies of repetitive transcranial mag-
exemplifies an innovative and elaborate device technol- netic stimulation on cognitive function and cortical excit-
ogy that may induce high therapeutic expectations and ability in Alzheimer’s dementia. J Neurol 259: 83–92.
yield enhanced placebo effects (e.g., compared to oral pill Andoh J, Paus T (2011). Combining functional neuroimaging
treatments) (Kaptchuk et al., 2000). How to properly inter- with off-line brain stimulation: modulation of task-related
pret efficacy in this context and how to delineate the rel- activity in language areas. J Cogn Neurosci 23: 349–361.
Aziz-Zadeh L, Cattaneo L, Rochat M et al. (2005). Covert
ative importance of treatment-specific effects vs placebo
speech arrest induced by rTMS over both motor and non-
effects is challenging. This is particularly difficult given motor left hemisphere frontal sites. J Cogn Neurosci 17:
the growing body of literature providing strong evidence 928–938.
that placebos themselves are capable of meaningfully and Bae EH, Schrader LM, Machii K et al. (2007). Safety and tol-
relevantly modulating brain networks (Benedetti, 2014). erability of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in
Most of the placebo-focused work in the TMS field has patients with epilepsy: a review of the literature. Epilepsy
concentrated on developing optimal sham devices to Behav 10: 521–528.
ensure adequate blinding in placebo-controlled studies. Barbas H (1988). Anatomic organization of basoventral and
However, there is a relative paucity of research investigat- mediodorsal visual recipient prefrontal regions in the rhe-
ing the bigger-picture questions posed here, which may sus monkey. J Comp Neurol 276: 313–342.
need to be probed with specialized study designs such Barker AT, Jalinous R, Freeston IL (1985). Noninvasive
magnetic stimulation of human motor cortex. Lancet 2:
as additional “no treatment” control arms (Wager and
1106–1107.
Atlas, 2015). Barwood CH, Murdoch BE, Whelan BM et al. (2011). The
Another major issue that will need to be tackled going effects of low frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic
forward involves the potential ethical implications sur- stimulation (rTMS) and sham condition rTMS on beha-
rounding new therapeutic applications of TMS. Most rel- vioural language in chronic non-fluent aphasia: short term
evant to the readership of this book would be the example outcomes. NeuroRehabilitation 28: 113–128.
of TMS for cognitive enhancement. This topic is quite Bauer RH, Fuster JM (1976). Delayed-matching and delayed-
controversial and there is mixed evidence surrounding response deficit from cooling dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
the potential ability of TMS to meaningfully enhance in monkeys. J Comp Physiol Psychol 90: 293–302.
speed and/or accuracy of cognitive processes in healthy Baxter LR, Schwartz JM, Phelps ME et al. (1989).
individuals (Luber and Lisanby, 2014). However, regard- Reduction of prefrontal cortex glucose metabolism com-
mon to three types of depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry
less of the level of evidence, people are and will continue
46: 243–250.
to use these technologies for this purpose and this Beam W, Borckardt JJ, Reeves ST et al. (2009). An efficient
requires careful analysis from ethical and public policy and accurate new method for locating the F3 position for
perspectives. prefrontal TMS applications. Brain Stimul 2: 50–54.
Finally, as has been highlighted throughout this chap- Benedetti F (2014). Placebo effects: from the neurobiological
ter, the technological aspects of TMS are constantly and paradigm to translational implications. Neuron 84:
rapidly evolving. On the horizon are many innovations 623–637.
88 M.J. BURKE ET AL.
Benedetti F, Arduino C, Costa S et al. (2006). Loss of Davis NJ, Gold E, Pascual-Leone A (2013). Challenges of
expectation-related mechanisms in Alzheimer’s disease proper placebo control for non-invasive brain stimulation
makes analgesic therapies less effective. Pain 121 (1–2): in clinical and experimental applications. Eur J Neurosci
133–144. 38: 2973–2977.
Bentwich J, Dobronevsky E, Aichenbaum S et al. (2011). de Goede AA, ter Braack EM, van Putten MJ (2016). Single
Beneficial effect of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimu- and paired pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation in drug
lation combined with cognitive training for the treatment of naı̈ve epilepsy. Clin Neurophysiol 127: 3140–3155.
Alzheimer’s disease: a proof of concept study. J Neural de Lara LIN, Tik M, Woletz M et al. (2017). High-sensitivity
Transm 118: 463–471. TMS/fMRI of the human motor cortex using a dedicated
Benussi A, Di Lorenzo F, Dell’Era V et al. (2017). Transcranial multichannel MR coil. Neuroimage 150: 262–269.
magnetic stimulation distinguishes Alzheimer disease from Deng ZD, Lisanby SH, Peterchev AV (2013). Electric field
frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 89: 665–672. depth–focality tradeoff in transcranial magnetic stimula-
Berent I, Brem AK, Zhao X et al. (2015). Role of the motor tion: simulation comparison of 50 coil designs. Brain
system in language knowledge. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S Stimul 6: 1–13.
A 112: 1983–1988. Devlin JT, Matthews PM, Rushworth MF (2003). Semantic
Blumberger DM, Vila-Rodriguez F, Thorpe KE et al. (2018). processing in the left inferior prefrontal cortex: a combined
Effectiveness of theta burst versus high-frequency repeti- functional magnetic resonance imaging and transcranial
tive transcranial magnetic stimulation in patients with magnetic stimulation study. J Cogn Neurosci 15: 71–84.
depression (THREE-D): a randomised non-inferiority trial. Di Lazzaro V, Oliviero A, Tonali PA et al. (2002).
Lancet 391: 1683–1692. Noninvasive in vivo assessment of cholinergic cortical cir-
Boes AD, Prasad S, Liu H et al. (2015). Network localization cuits in AD using transcranial magnetic stimulation.
of neurological symptoms from focal brain lesions. Brain Neurology 59: 392–397.
138: 3061–3075. Di Lazzaro V, Oliviero A, Pilato F et al. (2004). Motor cortex
Brem AK, Ran K, Pascual-leone A (2013). Learning and hyperexcitability to transcranial magnetic stimulation in
memory. In: Handbook of clinical neurology, vol. 116. Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 75:
Elsevier, pp. 693–737. 555–559.
Brunoni AR, Chaimani A, Moffa AH et al. (2017). Repetitive Di Lazzaro V, Oliviero A, Pilato F et al. (2005).
transcranial magnetic stimulation for the acute treatment of Neurophysiological predictors of long term response to
major depressive episodes: a systematic review with net- AChE inhibitors in AD patients. J Neurol Neurosurg
work meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiat 74: 143–152. Psychiatry 76: 1064–1069.
Burrell JR, Kiernan MC, Vucic S et al. (2011). Motor neuron Di Lazzaro V, Profice P, Ranieri F et al. (2012). I-wave origin
dysfunction in frontotemporal dementia. Brain 134: and modulation. Brain Stimul 5: 512–525.
2582–2594. Diamond A (2013). Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol
Cantone M, Di Pino G, Capone F et al. (2014). The contribu- 64: 135–168.
tion of transcranial magnetic stimulation in the diagnosis du Boisgueheneuc F, Levy R, Volle E et al. (2006). Functions
and in the management of dementia. Clin Neurophysiol of the left superior frontal gyrus in humans: a lesion study.
125: 1509–1532. Brain 129: 3315–3328.
Chafee MV, Goldman-Rakic PS (1998). Matching patterns Du J, Tian L, Liu W et al. (2016). Effects of repetitive transcra-
of activity in primate prefrontal area 8a and parietal area nial magnetic stimulation on motor recovery and motor
7ip neurons during a spatial working memory task. cortex excitability in patients with stroke: a randomized
J Neurophysiol 79: 2919–2940. controlled trial. Eur J Neurol 23: 1666–1672.
Chen R, Cros D, Curra A et al. (2008). The clinical diagnostic Epstein CM, Lah JJ, Meador K et al. (1996). Optimum stimu-
utility of transcranial magnetic stimulation: report of an lus parameters for lateralized suppression of speech with
IFCN committee. Clin Neurophysiol 119: 504–532. magnetic brain stimulation. Neurology 47: 1590–1593.
Chung SW, Rogasch NC, Hoy KE et al. (2015). Measuring Esslinger C, Sch€ uler N, Sauer C et al. (2012). Induction and
brain stimulation induced changes in cortical properties quantification of prefrontal cortical network plasticity
using TMS-EEG. Brain Stimul 8: 1010–1020. using 5 Hz rTMS and fMRI. Hum Brain Mapp 35: 140–151.
Cicinelli P, Pasqualetti P, Zaccagnini M et al. (2003). Fehr E, Fischbacher U (2003). The nature of human altruism.
Interhemispheric asymmetries of motor cortex excitability Nature 425: 785.
in the postacute stroke stage: a paired-pulse transcranial Ferbert A, Priori A, Rothwell JC et al. (1992).
magnetic stimulation study. Stroke 34: 2653–2658. Interhemispheric inhibition of the human motor cortex.
Cotelli M, Calabria M, Manenti R et al. (2011). Improved J Physiol 453: 525–546.
language performance in Alzheimer disease following Ferreri F, Pasqualetti P, M€a€att€a S et al. (2011). Motor cortex
brain stimulation. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 82: excitability in Alzheimer’s disease: a transcranial magnetic
794–797. stimulation follow-up study. Neurosci Lett 492: 94–98.
Courtney SM (2004). Attention and cognitive control as emer- Fitzgerald PB, Oxley TJ, Laird AR et al. (2006). An analysis of
gent properties of information representation in working functional neuroimaging studies of dorsolateral prefrontal
memory. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 4: 501–516. cortical activity in depression. Psychiatry Res 148: 33–45.
TRANSCRANIAL MAGNETIC STIMULATION 89
Fitzgerald PB, Maller JJ, Hoy KE et al. (2009). Exploring the Hamada M, Rothwell JC (2016). Neurophysiology of rTMS:
optimal site for the localization of dorsolateral prefrontal important caveats when interpreting the results of therapeu-
cortex in brain stimulation experiments. Brain Stimul 2: tic interventions. In: Therapeutic rTMS in neurology,
234–237. Springer, Cham, pp. 1–10.
Fox MD, Buckner RL, White MP et al. (2012a). Efficacy of Hamidi M, Tononi G, Postle B (2008). Evaluating frontal and
transcranial magnetic stimulation targets for depression parietal contributions to spatial working memory with
is related to intrinsic functional connectivity with the sub- repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Brain Res
genual cingulate. Biol Psychiatry 72: 595–603. 1230: 202–210.
Fox MD, Halko MA, Eldaief MC et al. (2012b). Measuring Hamidi M, Tononi G, Postle BR (2009). Evaluating the role
and manipulating brain connectivity with resting state of prefrontal and parietal cortices in memory-guided
functional connectivity magnetic resonance imaging response with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation.
(fcMRI) and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). Neuropsychologia 47: 295–302.
Neuroimage 62: 2232–2243. Hamilton RH, Chrysikou EG, Coslett B (2011). Mechanisms
Fox MD, Buckner RL, Liu H et al. (2014). Resting-state net- of aphasia recovery after stroke and the role of noninvasive
works link invasive and noninvasive brain stimulation brain stimulation. Brain Lang 118: 40–50.
across diverse psychiatric and neurological diseases. Proc Hasan A, Falkai P, Wobrock T (2013). Transcranial brain
Natl Acad Sci U S A 111 (41): eE4367–eE4375. https:// stimulation in schizophrenia: targeting cortical excit-
doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1405003111. ability, connectivity and plasticity. Curr Med Chem 20:
Fregni F, Pascual-Leone A (2007). Technology insight: nonin- 405–413.
vasive brain stimulation in neurology—perspectives on Hayward G, Mehta MA, Harmer C et al. (2007). Exploring the
the therapeutic potential of rTMS and tDCS. Nat Rev physiological effects of double-cone coil TMS over the
Neurol 3: 383. medial frontal cortex on the anterior cingulate cortex: an
Freitas C, Mondragón-Llorca H, Pascual-Leone A (2011). H215O PET study. Eur J Neurosci 25: 2224–2233.
Noninvasive brain stimulation in Alzheimer’s disease: Herbsman T, Avery D, Ramsey D et al. (2009). More lateral
systematic review and perspectives for the future. Exp and anterior prefrontal coil location is associated with bet-
Gerontol 46: 611–627. ter repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation antidepres-
Frey D, Schilt S, Strack V et al. (2014). Navigated transcranial sant response. Biol Psychiatry 66: 509–515.
magnetic stimulation improves the treatment outcome in Herwig U, Abler B, Sch€ onfeldt-Lecuona C et al. (2003a).
patients with brain tumors in motor eloquent locations. Verbal storage in a premotor-parietal network: evidence
Neuro-Oncology 16: 1365–1372. from fMRI-guided magnetic stimulation. NeuroImage
Fried PJ, Rushmore 3rd RJ, Moss MB et al. (2014). Causal evi- 20: 1032–1041.
dence supporting functional dissociation of verbal and spa- Herwig U, Satrapi P, Schonfeldt-Lecuona C (2003b). Using
tial working memory in the human dorsolateral prefrontal the international 10-20 EEG system for positioning of tran-
cortex. Eur J Neurosci 39: 1973–1981. scranial magnetic stimulation. Brain Topogr 16: 95–99.
Fried PJ, Jannati A, Davila-Perez P et al. (2017). Holland R, Leff AP, Josephs O et al. (2011). Speech facilita-
Reproducibility of single-pulse, paired- pulse, and inter- tion by left inferior frontal cortex stimulation. Curr Biol 21:
mittent theta-burst TMS measures in healthy aging, 1403–1407.
type-2 diabetes, and Alzheimer’s disease. Front Aging Homan RW, Herman J, Purdy P (1987). Cerebral location of
Neurosci 9: 263. international 10–20 system electrode placement.
Funahashi S, Bruce CJ, Goldman-Rakic PS (1993). Dorsolateral Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 66: 376–382.
prefrontal lesions and oculomotor delayed-response perfor- Hsu WY, Cheng CH, Liao KK et al. (2012). Effects of repet-
mance: evidence for mnemonic “scotomas”. J Neurosci 13: itive transcranial magnetic stimulation on motor functions
1479–1497. in patients with stroke: a meta-analysis. Stroke 43:
Fuster JM, Alexander GE (1971). Neuron activity related to 1849–1857.
short-term memory. Science 173: 652–654. Huang C-C, Su T-P, Shan I-K et al. (2004). Effect of 5 Hz
George MS, Taylor JJ, Short EB (2013). The expanding evi- repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on cognition
dence base for rTMS treatment of depression. Curr Opin during a Go/NoGo task. J Psychiatr Res38: 513–520.
Psychiatry 26: 13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2004.01.006.
Guse B, Falkai P, Wobrock T (2010). Cognitive effects of high- Huang YZ, Edwards MJ, Rounis E et al. (2005). Theta burst
frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation: a stimulation of the human motor cortex. Neuron 45:
systematic review. J Neural Transm 117: 105–122. 201–206.
G€uth W, Schmittberger R, Schwarze B (1982). An experimen- Ilmoniemi RJ, Ruohonen J, Virtanen J et al. (1999). EEG
tal analysis of ultimatum bargaining. J Econ Behav Organ responses evoked by transcranial magnetic stimulation.
3: 367–388. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Suppl 51: 22–29.
Hallett M (2000). Transcranial magnetic stimulation and the Janicak PG, O’Reardon JP, Sampson SM et al. (2008).
human brain. Nature 406: 147. Transcranial magnetic stimulation in the treatment of
Hallett M (2007). Transcranial magnetic stimulation: a primer. major depressive disorder: a comprehensive summary of
Neuron 55: 187–199. safety experience from acute exposure, extended exposure,
90 M.J. BURKE ET AL.
and during reintroduction treatment. J Clin Psychiatry 69: Lowe CJ, Manocchio F, Safati AB et al. (2018). The effects of
222–232. theta burst stimulation (TBS) targeting the prefrontal cor-
Jannati A, Block G, Oberman LM et al. (2017). Interindividual tex on executive functioning: a systematic review and
variability in response to continuous theta-burst stimula- meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 111: 344–359.
tion in healthy adults. Clin Neurophysiol 128: 2268–2278. Lozano AM (2017). Waving hello to noninvasive deep-brain
Jennum P, Friberg L, Fuglsang-Frederiksen A et al. (1994). stimulation. N Engl J Med 377: 1096–1098.
Speech localization using repetitive transcranial magnetic Luber B, Lisanby SH (2014). Enhancement of human cogni-
stimulation. Neurology 44: 269. tive performance using transcranial magnetic stimulation
Kaptchuk TJ, Goldman P, Stone DA et al. (2000). Do medical (TMS). Neuroimage 85: 961–970.
devices have enhanced placebo effects? J Clin Epidemiol Luber B, Kinnunen LH, Rakitin BC et al. (2007). Facilitation
53: 786–792. of performance in a working memory task with rTMS stim-
Kerwin LJ, Keller CJ, Wu W et al. (2017). Test–retest reliabil- ulation of the precuneus: frequency- and time-dependent
ity of transcranial magnetic stimulation EEG evoked poten- effects. Brain Res 1128: 120–129.
tials. Brain Stimul 11: 536–544. Luria AR (1972). The man with a shattered world: the
Knoch D, Pascual-Leone A, Meyer K et al. (2006). history of a brain wound, Harvard University Press,
Diminishing reciprocal fairness by disrupting the right pre- Cambridge, MA.
frontal cortex. Science 314: 829–832. Machii K, Cohen D, Ramos-Estebanez C et al. (2006). Safety
Kobayashi M, Pascual-Leone A (2003). Transcranial magnetic of rTMS to non-motor cortical areas in healthy participants
stimulation in neurology. Lancet Neurol 2: 145–156. and patients. Clin Neurophysiol 117: 455–471.
Koch G, Oliveri M, Torriero S et al. (2005). rTMS evidence of Maeda F, Pascual-Leone A (2003). Transcranial magnetic
different delay and decision processes in a fronto-parietal stimulation: studying motor neurophysiology of psychiat-
neuronal network activated during spatial working mem- ric disorders. Psychopharmacology 168: 359–376.
ory. Neuroimage 24: 34–39. Maeda F, Keenan JP, Tormos JM et al. (2000a). Modulation of
Komssi S, K€ahk€onen S (2006). The novelty value of the corticospinal excitability by repetitive transcranial mag-
combined use of electroencephalography and transcranial netic stimulation. Clin Neurophysiol 111: 800–805.
magnetic stimulation for neuroscience research. Brain Maeda F, Keenan JP, Tormos JM (2000b). Interindividual var-
Res Rev 52: 183–192. iability of the modulatory effects of repetitive transcranial
Krieg SM, Shiban E, Buchmann N et al. (2012). Utility of pre- magnetic stimulation on cortical excitability. Exp Brain
surgical navigated transcranial magnetic brain stimulation Res 133: 425–430.
for the resection of tumors in eloquent motor areas. Martin DM, McClintock SM, Forster JJ et al. (2017).
J Neurosurg 116: 994–1001. Cognitive enhancing effects of rTMS administered to the
Krummenacher P, Candia V, Folkers G et al. (2010). Prefrontal prefrontal cortex in patients with depression: a systematic
cortex modulates placebo analgesia. Pain 148: 368–374. review and meta-analysis of individual task effects.
Kumar S, Rao SL, Chandramouli BA et al. (2013). Reduced Depress Anxiety 34: 1029–1039.
contribution of executive functions in impaired working Mayberg HS, Brannan SK, Tekell JL et al. (2000). Regional
memory performance in mild traumatic brain injury metabolic effects of fluoxetine in major depression: serial
patients. Clin Neurol Neurosurg 115: 1326–1332. changes and relationship to clinical response. Biol
Larson J, Lynch G (1986). Induction of synaptic potentiation in Psychiatry 48: 830–843.
hippocampus by patterned stimulation involves two events. Menon P, Geevasinga N, Yiannikas C et al. (2015). Sensitivity
Science 232: 985–988. and specificity of threshold tracking transcranial magnetic
Lefaucheur JP, Andre-Obadia N, Antal A et al. (2014). stimulation for diagnosis of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: a
Evidence-based guidelines on the therapeutic use of repet- prospective study. Lancet Neurol 14: 478–484.
itive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS). Clin Moore TL, Schettler SP, Killiany RJ et al. (2012). Impairment
Neurophysiol 125: 2150–2206. in delayed nonmatching to sample following lesions of dor-
Li Y, Wang L, Jia M et al. (2017). The effects of high- sal prefrontal cortex. Behav Neurosci 126: 772–780.
frequency rTMS over the left DLPFC on cognitive control Mottaghy FM, Krause BJ, Kemna LJ et al. (2000). Modulation
in young healthy participants. PloS One 12: e0179430. of the neuronal circuitry subserving working memory in
Liepert J, Bar KJ, Meske U et al. (2001). Motor cortex disin- healthy human subjects by repetitive transcranial magnetic
hibition in Alzheimer’s disease. Clin Neurophysiol 112: stimulation. Neurosci Lett 280: 167–170.
1436–1441. Mottaghy FM, D€ oring T, M€uller-G€artner H-W et al. (2002a).
Lioumis P, Zhdanov A, M€akel€a N et al. (2012). A novel Bilateral parieto-frontal network for verbal working
approach for documenting naming errors induced by nav- memory: an interference approach using repetitive tran-
igated transcranial magnetic stimulation. J Neurosci scranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS). Eur J Neurosci 16:
Methods 204: 349–354. 1627–1632.
Lipton RB, Dodick DW, Silberstein SD et al. (2010). Single- Mottaghy FM, Gangitano M, Sparing R et al. (2002b).
pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation for acute treatment Segregation of areas related to visual working memory in
of migraine with aura: a randomised, double-blind, parallel- the prefrontal cortex revealed by rTMS. Cereb Cortex
group, sham-controlled trial. Lancet Neurol 9: 373–380. 12: 369–375.
TRANSCRANIAL MAGNETIC STIMULATION 91
Mottaghy FM, Gangitano M, Krause BJ et al. (2003a). memory manipulation from retention functions in the pre-
Chronometry of parietal and prefrontal activations in ver- frontal, but not posterior parietal, cortex. J Cogn Neurosci
bal working memory revealed by transcranial magnetic 18: 1712–1722.
stimulation. Neuroimage 18: 565–575. Rabey JM, Dobronevsky E, Aichenbaum S et al. (2013).
Mottaghy FM, Pascual-Leone A, Kemna LJ et al. (2003b). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation combined
Modulation of a brain–behavior relationship in verbal with cognitive training is a safe and effective modality
working memory by rTMS. Cogn Brain Res 15: 241–249. for the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease: a randomized,
Mull BR, Seyal M (2001). Transcranial magnetic stimulation double-blind study. J Neural Transm 120: 813–819.
of left prefrontal cortex impairs working memory. Clin Rami L, Gironell A, Kulisevsky J et al. (2003). Effects of repet-
Neurophysiol 112: 1672–1675. itive transcranial magnetic stimulation on memory subtypes:
Nardone R, Bergmann J, Christova M et al. (2012). Short a controlled study. Neuropsychologia 41: 1877–1883.
latency afferent inhibition differs among the subtypes of Rossi S, Hallett M, Rossini PM et al. (2009). Safety, ethical
mild cognitive impairment. J Neural Transm 119: 463–471. considerations, and application guidelines for the use of
Nguyen KH, Gordon LG (2015). Cost-effectiveness of repet- transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and
itive transcranial magnetic stimulation versus antidepres- research. Clin Neurophysiol 120: 2008–2039.
sant therapy for treatment-resistant depression. Value Rossini PM, Burke D, Chen R et al. (2015). Non-invasive elec-
Health 18: 597–604. trical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord,
Oliveri M, Turriziani P, Carlesimo GA et al. (2001). Parieto- roots and peripheral nerves: basic principles and proce-
frontal interactions in visual-object and visual-spatial dures for routine clinical and research application. An
working memory: evidence from transcranial magnetic updated report from an I.F.C.N. Committee. Clin
stimulation. Cereb Cortex 11: 606–618. Neurophysiol 126: 1071–1107.
Park HJ, Bonmassar G, Kaltenbach JA et al. (2013). Activation Rotenberg A, Horvath JC, Pascual-Leone A (Eds.), (2014).
of the central nervous system induced by micro-magnetic Transcranial magnetic stimulation. Springer, New York.
stimulation. Nat Commun 4: 2463. Ruff CC, Driver J, Bestmann S (2009). Combining TMS and
Pascual-Leone A, Hallett M (1994). Induction of errors in a fMRI: from ‘virtual lesions’ to functional-network accounts
delayed response task by repetitive transcranial magnetic of cognition. Cortex 45: 1043–1049.
stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Sakai KL, Noguchi Y, Takeuchi T et al. (2002). Selective
NeuroReport 5: 2517–2520. priming of syntactic processing by event-related transcra-
Pascual-Leone A, Gates JR, Dhuna A (1991). Induction of nial magnetic stimulation of Broca’s area. Neuron 35:
speech arrest and counting errors with rapid-rate transcra- 1177–1182.
nial magnetic stimulation. Neurology 41: 697–702. Sakreida K, Lange I, Willmes K et al. (2018). High-resolution
Pascual-Leone A, Valls-Sole J, Wassermann EM et al. (1994). language mapping of Broca’s region with transcranial mag-
Responses to rapid-rate transcranial magnetic stimulation netic stimulation. Brain Struct Funct 223: 1297–1312.
of the human motor cortex. Brain 117: 847–858. Sandrini M, Rossini PM, Miniussi C (2008). Lateralized con-
Pascual-Leone A, Rubio B, Pallardo F et al. (1996). Rapid-rate tribution of prefrontal cortex in controlling task-irrelevant
transcranial magnetic stimulation of left dorsolateral pre- information during verbal and spatial working memory
frontal cortex in drug-resistant depression. Lancet 348: tasks: rTMS evidence. Neuropsychologia 46: 2056–2063.
233–237. Shafi MM, Westover MB, Fox MD et al. (2012). Exploration
Pascual-Leone A, Bartres-Faz D, Keenan JP (1999). and modulation of brain network interactions with nonin-
Transcranial magnetic stimulation: studying the brain– vasive brain stimulation in combination with neuroimag-
behaviour relationship by induction of “virtual lesions”. ing. Eur J Neurosci 35: 805–825.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 354: 1229–1238. Shafi MM, Westover MB, Oberman L et al. (2014).
Pascual-Leone A, Walsh V, Rothwell J (2000). Transcranial Modulation of EEG functional connectivity networks in
magnetic stimulation in cognitive neuroscience–virtual subjects undergoing repetitive transcranial magnetic stim-
lesion, chronometry, and functional connectivity. Curr ulation. Brain Topogr 27: 172–191.
Opin Neurobiol 10: 232–237. Shields J, Mock J, Devier D et al. (2018). Unilateral repetitive
Perretti A, Grossi D, Fragassi N et al. (1996). Evaluation of the transcranial magnetic stimulation differentially affects
motor cortex by magnetic stimulation in patients with younger and older adults completing a verbal working
Alzheimer disease. J Neurol Sci 135: 31–37. memory task. J Neurol Sci 384: 15–20.
Petrides M, Pandya DN (1984). Projections to the frontal cor- Shimizu T, Hosaki A, Hino T et al. (2002). Motor cortical dis-
tex from the posterior parietal region in the rhesus monkey. inhibition in the unaffected hemisphere after unilateral cor-
J Comp Neurol 228: 105–116. tical stroke. Brain 125: 1896–1907.
Picht T, Krieg SM, Sollmann N et al. (2013). A comparison of Silvanto J, Pascual-Leone A (2008). State-dependency of tran-
language mapping by preoperative navigated transcranial scranial magnetic stimulation. Brain Topogr 21: 1.
magnetic stimulation and direct cortical stimulation during Simpson M, Macdonell R (2015). The use of transcranial mag-
awake surgery. Neurosurgery 72: 808–819. netic stimulation in diagnosis, prognostication and treat-
Postle BR, Ferrarelli F, Hamidi M et al. (2006). Repetitive ment evaluation in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler Relat
transcranial magnetic stimulation dissociates working Disord 4: 430–436.
92 M.J. BURKE ET AL.
Smirni D, Turriziani P, Mangano GR et al. (2017). Modulating cortex on Stroop task performance. Exp Brain Res 169:
phonemic fluency performance in healthy subjects with 279–282.
transcranial magnetic stimulation over the left or right lat- Vanderhasselt M-A, De Raedt R, Baeken C et al. (2006b). The
eral frontal cortex. Neuropsychologia 102: 109–115. influence of rTMS over the right dorsolateral prefrontal
Sollmann N, Ille S, Hauck T et al. (2015). The impact of pre- cortex on intentional set switching. Exp Brain Res 172:
operative language mapping by repetitive navigated tran- 561–565.
scranial magnetic stimulation on the clinical course of Vanderhasselt M-A, De Raedt R, Baeken C et al. (2007). The
brain tumor patients. BMC Cancer 15: 261. influence of rTMS over the right dorsolateral prefrontal
Starling AJ, Tepper SJ, Marmura MJ et al. (2018). cortex on top-down attentional processes. Brain Res
A multicenter, prospective, single arm, open label, obser- 1137: 111–116.
vational study of sTMS for migraine prevention Vucic S, Kiernan MC (2017). Transcranial magnetic stimula-
(ESPOUSE study). Cephalalgia 38: 1038–1048. tion for the assessment of neurodegenerative disease.
Stein N, Sprenger C, Scholz J et al. (2012). White matter integ- Neurotherapeutics 14: 91–106.
rity of the descending pain modulatory system is associated Wager TD, Atlas LY (2015). The neuroscience of placebo
with interindividual differences in placebo analgesia. Pain effects: connecting context, learning and health. Nat Rev
153: 2210–2217. Neurosci 16: 403.
Suppa A, Huang YZ, Funke K et al. (2016). Ten years of theta Walsh A, Pascual-Leone A (2003). Neurochronometrics of mind:
burst stimulation in humans: established knowledge, TMS in cognitive science, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
unknowns and prospects. Brain Stimul 9 (3): 323–335. Wassermann EM (1998). Risk and safety of repetitive tran-
Tarapore PE, Findlay AM, Honma SM et al. (2013). Language scranial magnetic stimulation: report and suggested
mapping with navigated repetitive TMS: proof of tech- guidelines from the International Workshop on the
nique and validation. Neuroimage 82: 260–272. Safety of Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation,
Thiel A, Hartmann A, Rubi-Fessen I et al. (2013). Effects of June 5–7, 1996. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol
noninvasive brain stimulation on language networks and 108: 1–16.
recovery in early poststroke aphasia. Stroke 44: 2240–2246. Watkins KE, Strafella AP, Paus T (2003). Seeing and hearing
Thompson-Schill SL, Jonides J, Marshuetz C et al. (2002). speech excites the motor system involved in speech pro-
Effects of frontal lobe damage on interference effects in duction. Neuropsychologia 41: 989–994.
working memory. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 2: Weiduschat N, Thiel A, Rubi-Fessen I et al. (2011). Effects of
109–120. repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in aphasic
Tokimura H, Di Lazzaro V, Tokimura Y et al. (2000). Short stroke: a randomized controlled pilot study. Stroke 42:
latency inhibition of human hand motor cortex by somato- 409–415.
sensory input from the hand. J Physiol 523: 503–513. Weigand A, Horn A, Caballero R et al. (2018). Prospective
Ungerleider LG, Mishkin M (1982). Two cortical visual sys- validation that subgenual connectivity predicts antidepres-
tems. In: D Ingle (Ed.), Analysis of visual behavior, MIT sant efficacy of transcranial magnetic stimulation sites.
Press, Cambridge, MA. Biol Psychiatry 84(1): 28–37. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Uzunova G, Pallanti S, Hollander E (2016). Excitatory/inhib- j.biopsych.2017.10.028.
itory imbalance in autism spectrum disorders: implications Zangen A, Roth Y, Voller B (2005). Transcranial magnetic
for interventions and therapeutics. World J Biol Psychiatry stimulation of deep brain regions: evidence for efficacy
17: 174–186. of the H-coil. Clin Neurophysiol 116: 775–779.
Valero-Cabre A, Payne BR, Pascual-Leone A (2007). Zanto TP, Rubens MT, Thangavel A et al. (2011). Causal
Opposite impact on 14C-2-deoxyglucose brain metabolism role of the prefrontal cortex in top-down modulation of
following patterns of high and low frequency repetitive visual processing and working memory. Nat Neurosci
transcranial magnetic stimulation in the posterior parietal 14: 656.
cortex. Exp Brain Res 176: 603–615. Ziemann U, L€ onnecker S, Steinhoff BJ (1996). Effects of anti-
Valero-Cabre A, Amengual J, Stengel C et al. (2017). epileptic drugs on motor cortex excitability in humans: a
Transcranial magnetic stimulation in basic and clinical transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Ann Neurol 40:
neuroscience: a comprehensive review of fundamental 367–378.
principles and novel insights. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 83: Ziemann U, Reis J, Schwenkreis P et al. (2015). TMS and
381–404. drugs revisited 2014. Clin Neurophysiol 126: 1847–1868.
van de Laar B, de Bonis D, Zanow F (2016). Simultaneous
TMS & EEG: methodology and pitfalls. Clin FURTHER READING
Neurophysiol 46: 232.
Vanderhasselt M-A, De Raedt R, Baeken C et al. (2006a). The Ziemann U (2017). Thirty years of transcranial magnetic stim-
influence of rTMS over the left dorsolateral prefrontal ulation: where do we stand? Exp Brain Res 235: 973–984.

You might also like