You are on page 1of 71

University of South Florida

Digital Commons @ University of


South Florida

USF Tampa Graduate Theses and Dissertations USF Graduate Theses and Dissertations

October 2022

An Object for Sexual Pleasure: Does Viewing Sexualized Media


Predict Increases in Self and Partner Objectification Impacting
Feelings of Sexual and Romantic Closeness?
Kaitlyn Ligman
University of South Florida

Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.usf.edu/etd

Part of the Social Psychology Commons

Scholar Commons Citation


Ligman, Kaitlyn, "An Object for Sexual Pleasure: Does Viewing Sexualized Media Predict Increases in Self
and Partner Objectification Impacting Feelings of Sexual and Romantic Closeness?" (2022). USF Tampa
Graduate Theses and Dissertations.
https://digitalcommons.usf.edu/etd/9790

This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the USF Graduate Theses and Dissertations at Digital
Commons @ University of South Florida. It has been accepted for inclusion in USF Tampa Graduate Theses and
Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Digital Commons @ University of South Florida. For more
information, please contact digitalcommons@usf.edu.
An Object for Sexual Pleasure: Does Viewing Sexualized Media Predict Increases in Self and

Partner Objectification Impacting Feelings of Sexual and Romantic Closeness?

by

Kaitlyn Ligman

A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment


of the requirements for the degree of
Master of Arts
Department of Psychology
College of Arts and Sciences
University of South Florida

Major Professor: Jamie Goldenberg, Ph.D.


Jennifer Bosson, Ph.D.
Tiffany Chenneville, Ph.D.

Date of Approval:
October 13, 2022

Keywords: Relationships, Couples, Intimacy, Social media

Copyright © 2022, Kaitlyn Ligman


TABLE OF CONTENTS
List of Tables ................................................................................................................................. iii

Abstract .......................................................................................................................................... iv
Introduction ......................................................................................................................................1
Objectifying Media ..............................................................................................................2
Implications for Media and Objectification in Relationships ..............................................4
Self-objectification and Sexual Intimacy ...................................................................4
Partner-objectification and Relationship Satisfaction ................................................6
The Current Study ...............................................................................................................8

Method ...........................................................................................................................................11
Participants .........................................................................................................................11
Sexualized Media ...............................................................................................................11
Sexualized Media Content Exposure .....................................................................11
Measures ............................................................................................................................11
Self and Partner Objectification .............................................................................11
Relationship Satisfaction and Closeness ................................................................12
Sexual Closeness ....................................................................................................13
Relationship Length/Status ....................................................................................13
Social Media Use ...................................................................................................14
Demographics ........................................................................................................14
Procedure ...........................................................................................................................15

Results ............................................................................................................................................17
Social Media and Objectification.......................................................................................18
Social Media, Gender, and Objectification ........................................................................19
Social Media, Objectification, Sexual and Relational Closeness ......................................20
Social Media, Objectification, Gender, Sexual and Relational Closeness ........................24

Supplemental Analyses ..................................................................................................................35

Discussion ......................................................................................................................................43
Hypotheses and Anticipated Effects ..................................................................................43
Individuating Factors of Social Media Use and Relationship Status/Length ....................46
Self and Partner Objectification and Self and Partner Comparison ...................................47
Limitations and Future Directions .....................................................................................49
Conclusion .........................................................................................................................51

References ......................................................................................................................................52

i
Appendix A: Scripts .......................................................................................................................59

Appendix B: Measures ...................................................................................................................60

ii
LIST OF TABLES

Table 1: Linear Regression Analysis Assessing Self-Objectification. ......................................28

Table 2: Linear Regression Analysis Assessing Partner-Objectification. .................................28

Table 3: Moderation Analysis Examining Self-Objectification ................................................29

Table 4: Moderation Analysis Examining Partner-Objectification ...........................................29

Table 5: Mediation Analyses Examining Sexual Closeness ......................................................30

Table 6: Mediation Analysis Examining Relationship Satisfaction and Closeness ..................31

Table 7: Moderated Mediation Analyses Examining Sexual Closeness ...................................32

Table 8: Moderated Mediation Analyses Examining Relationship Satisfaction and


Closeness......................................................................................................................34

Table 9: Moderation Analysis Examining Self-Objectification ................................................36

Table 10: Moderation Analysis Examining Partner-Objectification ...........................................37

Table 11: Mediation Analyses Examining Sexual Closeness ......................................................38

Table 12: Mediation Analysis Examining Relationship Satisfaction and Closeness ..................39

Table 13: Moderated Mediation Analyses Examining Sexual Closeness ...................................40

Table 14: Moderated Mediation Analyses Examining Relationship Satisfaction and


Closeness......................................................................................................................42

iii
ABSTRACT

Exposure to sexually objectifying media has been linked to the objectification of the self and of

one’s romantic partner (e.g., partner-objectification); yet the implications of this for romantic

relationships have remained relatively unexamined. There is, however, reason to suspect that

exposure to sexually objectifying media and engaging in objectification may have implications

for romantic couples. When a woman frequently monitors her appearance this may undermine

her ability to sexually connect with her partner and when a man views his partner as an object for

sexual pleasure it may impede his ability to develop intimate feelings of relational closeness to

his partner. In the current study, 410 individuals who were in or had recently been in a romantic

relationship were randomly shown either a sexualized or non-sexualized clip from social media,

and then evaluated on rates of self and partner objectification and feelings of sexual closeness

and relationship satisfaction and closeness. As hypothesized, greater self-objectification was

related to decreases in aspects of sexual closeness to one’s partner and greater partner-

objectification was related to decreases in feelings of relationship satisfaction and closeness to

one’s partner. However, contrary to the hypotheses, these pathways were not impacted by

exposure to sexualized social media. Collectively the current study illuminates the harm

objectification has upon romantic relationships and speaks to the need to further investigate the

role exposure to media may play in this dynamic.

iv
INTRODUCTION

The average American uses three to eight social media platforms, with 71% of young

adults logging on to photo or video-based sharing apps regularly (Auxier & Anderson, 2021;

Khattab, 2019). These platforms disseminate the message that women are objects valued for their

sexual features and physical appearance as opposed to their personal qualities (Halton, 2011;

Ramsey & Horan, 2018; Trekels et al., 2018). Not surprisingly, exposure to objectifying media,

particularly of women, has been linked to the objectification of the self (Harper & Tiggemann,

2008; Linder & Daniels, 2018) and of one’s romantic partner (Zurbriggen et al., 2011); yet the

implications of this in the context of romantic relationships has received little attention. There is,

however, reason to suspect that self and partner objectification may have implications for

heterosexual relationships. Frequent appearance monitoring that occurs when a woman views

herself as an object of sexual pleasure for a romantic partner may reduce a woman's capacity to

sexually connect or feel sexually close to her partner. Additionally, viewing one’s partner as an

object of sexual pleasure may reduce a man’s ability to foster intimate emotional connections

with his partner, thus diminishing feelings of relational satisfaction and closeness. The current

study has been designed to experimentally test how exposure to sexually objectifying media

impacts feelings of sexual and relational closeness to one’s romantic partner through self and

partner objectification while also exploring how these variables may differ depending upon

gender.

1
Objectifying Media

For decades a ubiquitous facet of mainstream media has been the sexual objectification of

women. Media sexualization may take many forms, from verbal references to women’s sexual

body parts to depictions of highly exposed women whose sole purpose is to look desirable (Ward

et al., 2016). Western media specifically is rampant with depictions of females as sexual objects,

serving as a proponent of objectification by publicly propagating the message to audiences that a

woman’s physical attractiveness and sexual features are prioritized above all other qualities

(Hatton & Trautner, 2011; Ward, 2016; Wright & Tokunaga, 2016). Recently, social media has

become a predominant platform for sharing and creating sexualized content, in particular of

young women (Trekels et al., 2018). This is especially concerning considering that 88% of

individuals aged 18-to-29 report the use of some form of social media (Auxier & Anderson,

2021), the most popular being photo and video-based sharing apps (e.g., Tik Tok, Instagram, and

Snapchat) (Auxier & Anderson, 2021). The high usage of photo and video-based sharing apps

however becomes concerning when evaluating the kind of content that is most popular within

these apps, which is sexualized young women.

Popular photo and video-based sharing apps central function are to view, like, share, and

comment on content that is most often relevant to the body’s physical appearance and

sexualization (Drenten et al., 2020; Ramsey & Horan, 2018). Indeed, Instagram accounts that

post sexualized images of women receive a significantly higher number of likes and followers

than accounts containing non-sexualized content (Ramsey & Horan, 2018). Evaluations of

TikTok reveal similar findings, where trending videos on the platform are commonly

characterized by heterosexual performances of feminine desirability in which females engage in

sexy poses to seduce the audience (Khattab, 2019).

2
Living in a culture that treats women as sexual objects may socialize women to self-

objectify, whereby women internalize an observer’s perspective on their bodily selves, viewing

their body as an object existing for the pleasure of others rather than as an entity to be

subjectively experienced (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). Indeed, exposure to sexualized mass

media (e.g., television, movies, and music) and social media is predictive of increased rates of

self-objectification (Aubrey, 2007; Fardouly et al., 2015; Linder & Daniels, 2018). Specifically,

exposure to sexually objectifying media leads women to develop an objectified self-concept

(Vandenbosch et al., 2015), self-sexualize (Ramsey & Horan, 2018; Ward et al., 2016), and to

increase body surveillance (Aubrey, 2007; Aubrey & Gerding, 2015; Feltman & Syzmanski,

2018). Additionally, women who use popular photo-based sharing apps such as Instagram

demonstrate increased body image concerns (Fardouly et al., 2018) and report a greater

likelihood to utilize social media to appear sexually appealing to others (Ramsey & Horan,

2018). Collectively these findings become concerning as each of these behaviors may then be

associated with detrimental outcomes such as declines in mental health, disruptions in bodily

awareness, drives for thinness, and increases in disordered eating (Butkowski et al., 2019;

Dakanalis & Riva, 2013; Ward et al., 2016).

A culture that treats women as sexual objects not only contributes to women’s tendency

to objectify their selves but also to men’s belief that the female body is an object meant to

provide them with sexual pleasure (Zurbriggen et al., 2011). It follows that men who are exposed

to sexualized media may be more likely to objectify women. Consistent with this, research

demonstrates that men who consume sexually objectifying content show an increase in the

importance they assign to women’s sexual body parts (Ward et al., 2016). Specifically, men who

consume objectifying content of women are more likely to expect sexual relations from women

3
(Moreno et al., 2011), demonstrate increases in objectified cognitions and attitudes of violence

towards women (Wright & Tokunga, 2016), and possess higher endorsements of sexist attitudes

with increased hostility towards women (Rollero, 2013). Thus, there is evidence that the

consumption of mass media propagates men’s treatment of women as sexual objects (Karsay et

al., 2018; Seabrook et al., 2019; Wright & Tokunga, 2016; Zurbriggen et al., 2011).

Implications for Media and Objectification in Relationships

Exposure to sexualized media content of women has been linked to increases in self-

objectification for women (Fardouly et al., 2015; Linder & Daniels, 2018) and the objectification

of women by men (Karsay et al., 2018; Ward et al., 2016; Wright & Tokunga, 2016), but to date,

only one study (Zurbriggen et al., 2011) has investigated the relationship between consumption

of objectifying media and self and partner objectification in the context of romantic relationships.

Theorizing from research on objectification suggests that objectification of the self and one’s

partner may have implications for sexual and relationship satisfaction, respectively.

Self-Objectification and Sexual Intimacy.

From the perspective of objectification theory, self-objectification may interfere with the

ability to experience sexual intimacy, to the extent that monitoring one’s physical appearance

creates a barrier to internalizing the experience of interconnectedness during sex (Fredrickson &

Roberts, 1997). When women self-objectify, they monitor their external appearance rather than

their internal experiences. Frequent appearance monitoring results in increases in appearance

anxiety (Lustig, 2012; Robbins & Reissing, 2018). Moreover, increases in both body surveillance

and appearance anxiety are negatively correlated with sexual well-being and positively

correlated with disruptions in connecting with the surrounding environment (Claudat & Warren,

2014; Vencill et al., 2015). Therefore, as women vigilantly monitor their appearance, they

4
become hyper-focused on how they look and less attuned to bodily states and mindfully

interacting with the surrounding environment, two key aspects in establishing feelings of

interconnectedness during sex (Claudat & Warren, 2014; Ramsey & Hoyt, 2015; Vencill et al.,

2015). Further, females in romantic relationships report greater instances of self-objectification

than men (Shinne, 2017), with women high in the trait of self-objectification showing an increase

in the enjoyment of sexualization by one’s partner, which later results in diminished feelings of

intimate interconnectedness (Ramsey et al., 2017). This demonstrates that for women specifically

objectification through body surveillance and appearance anxiety may inhibit the ability to

establish feelings of sexual connectedness.

The implications of self-objectification may then be further informed by the regular use

of social media by individuals in romantic relationships. As discussed previously, social media’s

portrayal of women is often objectifying (Ramsey & Horan, 2018) and greater exposure to

objectifying mass media is associated with higher levels of self-objectification among women

(Fardouly et al., 2015). More specifically, social media may provide a script for young women to

more eagerly self-sexualize for their partners and to self-objectify by continually monitoring and

comparing their appearance to current beauty standards (Butkoski et al., 2019; Fardouly et al.,

2015; Feltman & Szymanski, 2018). Lastly, studies on media, virtual environments, and

sexualization have found that women exposed to sexually objectifying images in a potential

romantic partner’s virtual apartment increased body consciousness (Overstreet et al., 2015). Thus

exposure to sexually objectifying social media may increase the likelihood to sexualize oneself

and monitor one’s appearance, decreasing the ability to possess or desire feelings of sexual

closeness in one’s romantic relationship for females.

5
To date, little research has considered the impact of consuming sexually objectifying

media on objectification as it relates to romantic couples and relationship outcomes. However,

Zurbriggen et al. (2011) posited that heterosexual relationships offer a rich environment to

understand objectification of the female body as it relates to the consumption of sexually

objectifying media and relationship and sexual satisfaction. To examine self-objectification in

the context of heterosexual relationships, Zurbriggen et al. (2011) recruited men and women who

were currently in or who had recently been in a relationship and asked them to report how often

they viewed various genres of media (e.g., television, film, magazines, and internet sites).

Participants then completed measures of self and partner objectification, relationship satisfaction,

and sexual satisfaction. Results indicated self-objectification was positively associated with

viewing objectifying magazines. In addition, a statistically reliable negative correlation between

self-objectification and sexual satisfaction for men in relationships was observed. The direction

of the association between self-objectification and sexual satisfaction was the same for women in

the study however the correlation was not statistically reliable. The authors methodology for

sexual satisfaction may partially explain why significant results were not attributed to women as

it relates to self-objectification. Zurbriggen et al. (2011) measured sexual satisfaction for all

participants with one single item (e.g., “How satisfied are you with the sexual part of your

current relationship?”) which may not have strongly resonated with female participants due to

its broad scope and lack of consideration for intimate feelings of interconnectedness.

Partner-Objectification and Relationship Satisfaction.

Whereas the act of self-objectification may reduce the ability to feel sexually connected

and synched with one’s partner, the act of partner-objectification may reduce the ability for

couples to develop an intimate emotional closeness that comprises relationship satisfaction.

6
Relationship satisfaction is defined as the total and complete experience of intimacy that

encompasses physical and emotional feelings of connectedness, closeness, and bondedness

(Sternberg, 1986), which objectifying one’s partner should undermine. When a woman is valued

for her external attributes, it results in her being perceived as lacking certain human qualities,

such as warmth, competence, (Heflick et al., 2011; Heflick & Goldenberg, 2014) and mind

(Loughnan et al., 2013). Thus, women’s sexual objectification leads to less human attribution on

behalf of the perceiver (Heflick & Goldenberg, 2009). These changes in perception due to

objectification then also influence how men interact with women by decreasing men’s empathy

towards women (Linz et al., 1988) and increasing proclivity toward sexually harassing behaviors

(Galdi et al., 2014). Therefore, objectifying one’s romantic partner places value upon a woman’s

body and appearance without consideration for human attributes, reducing the ability to cultivate

intimate emotional connections; as partner-objectification increases, feelings of relational

closeness that comprise relationship satisfaction decreases (Mahar et al., 2020; Ramsey et al.,

2017).

Limited research has been conducted on the impact of social media on partner-

objectification. However, it can be observed that consistent exposure to objectifying mass-media

predicts men’s belief of sexual reductionism (i.e., thinking of women in terms of their bodies and

evaluating them in terms of their physical appearance) (Wright & Tokunaga, 2015), which is

related to difficulties establishing intimate relationships with women (Wright et al., 2017). Thus

by viewing objectifying content of women it encourages men to view women they interact with

in a similar manner. Romantic relationships classified by instances of partner-objectification

show a negative association between dehumanization and intimate relational closeness to one’s

7
partner (Saez et al., 2019). The role social media may play in this environment is to repeatedly

expose men to the depiction of women as sexual objects. Social media exposure will perpetuate

the script of viewing one’s partner as an entity for male sexual pleasure therefore decreasing

ascriptions of warmth and humanness to one’s partner, ultimately impeding feelings of relational

closeness and the overall development of relationship satisfaction for men in particular.

Zurbriggen et al. (2011) theorized that objectification would impede relationship

development amongst couples due to male romantic partner’s internalizing the message from

media that women are sexual objects whose worth should be based upon their external

appearance. Their findings supported this; men in relationships reported greater instances of

partner-objectification than females, and further, consuming objectifying mass media was related

to lower relationship satisfaction through partner-objectification, for men only.

The Current Study

The aim of the current study is to expand on the findings of Zurbriggen et al. (2011) by

testing a causal impact of exposure to sexually objectifying social media on feelings of sexual

and relationship closeness. Zurbriggen et al.’s (2011) research was correlational and examined

traditional media, leaving modern media platforms (e.g., social media) unaccounted for in the

current implications. Further, individuating information such as levels of relationship length

(e.g., how long a couple has been together) and status (e.g., the title a couple assigns to their

relationship) were not assessed by Zurbriggen et al. (2011), a factor that may impact results as

feelings of sexual and relational closeness to one’s partner is affected by feelings of commitment

(Mahar et al., 2020). Relationships that are newly formed (e.g., friends with benefits, newly

dating) may foster an environment where feelings of lust and physical appearances are more

relevant and strongly valued in a relationship due to low levels of trust and comfort.

8
Comparatively, relationships that are not newly formed (e.g., engaged, married) are often long-

term partnerships where trust and comfort have been established amongst partners over time

(Mahar et al., 2020).

The current study aims to address these gaps in the literature by assessing the relationship

of the independent variable social media (sexualized vs. non-sexualized) on the outcomes of

objectification (self and partner), feelings of sexual closeness, and relational satisfaction and

closeness. Further, I will examine if the relationships between social media and feelings of

sexual closeness and relationship satisfaction and closeness are mediated by self and partner

objectification and further moderated by gender while controlling for relationship length/status

and social media use. The current design will serve three main purposes: (1) continue to expand

objectification research as it relates to romantic relationships, (2) examine the impact sexually

objectifying media content from social media has upon objectification in relationships, and (3)

evaluate how individuating factors such as gender may moderate the relationship amongst media

exposure, objectification, and feelings of closeness. To examine these three purposes, the

following hypotheses have been devised to independently evaluate individuals in romantic

relationships:

Hypothesis 1a: Individuals who are exposed to sexualized social media content will

experience higher levels of self-objectification than those who are exposed to non-sexualizing

social media content.

Hypothesis 1b: Individuals who are exposed to sexualized social media content will

experience higher levels of partner-objectification than those who are exposed to non-sexualized

social media content.

9
Hypothesis 2a: The relationship between exposure to sexualized social media content and

self-objectification will be moderated by gender, where females will report engaging in self-

objectification to a greater extent than males.

Hypothesis 2b: The relationship between exposure to sexualized social media content and

partner-objectification will be moderated by gender, where males will report engaging in partner-

objectification to a greater extent than females.

Hypothesis 3a: The relationship between social media content and feelings of sexual

closeness will be mediated by self-objectification, such that exposure to sexualized social media

will reduce feelings of sexual closeness through increases in self-objectification.

Hypothesis 3b: The relationship between social media content and feelings of

relationship satisfaction and closeness will be mediated by partner-objectification, such that

exposure to sexualized social media will reduce feelings of relationship satisfaction and

closeness through increases in partner-objectification.

Hypothesis 4a: Higher rates of self-objectification induced by exposure to sexualized

social media content will result in decreased feelings of sexual closeness, particularly for

women.

Hypothesis 4b: Higher rates of partner-objectification induced by exposure to sexualized

social media content will result in decreased feelings of relationship satisfaction and closeness,

particularly for men.

10
METHOD

Participants

Four hundred and ten individuals (49.5% [n =203] women) ages 18-24 were recruited

through the University participant pool system and received partial course credit for participating

in this study. Participants were on average 19.52 years old (SD = 1.43) and were primarily

(54.8%) White, with 19.3% Hispanic, 8.6 % Black, 7.1 % Asian, and 6.4 % biracial or

multiracial.

Sexualized Media

Sexualized Media Content Exposure.

Participants were randomly assigned to one of two conditions, exposure to non-

sexualized social media content or exposure to sexualized social media content. In both

conditions, participants were provided popularized videos from the social media site TikTok to

browse for two minutes and thirty seconds. Participants in the non-sexualized social media

content condition viewed brief snippets of videos that showed nature scenes whereas participants

in the sexualized social media content condition viewed targets presented in a sexually

objectifying manner dressed in minimal clothing while completing sexualized dances, poses, and

singing along to sexualized songs. Videos were rated by a panel of expert psychologists and

graduate students to ensure each condition provided accurate sexualized and non-sexualized

content that mimics the experience of scrolling through a modern social media feed.

Measures

Self and Partner Objectification.

11
To assess self-objectification, participants completed a modified version of the

surveillance subscale of McKinley and Hyde’s (1996) Objectified Body Consciousness Scale.

Participants responded to 7 items with 5 items reverse coded (from 1, strongly disagree to 7,

strongly agree) that analyzed the extent to which they take a third-person perspective of their

own body (e.g., “I am more concerned with what my body can do than how it looks”). An

average of the seven items was taken for the current study with higher scores reflecting greater

engagement in self-objectification. The average showed acceptable reliability (α = 0.78).

Following the methodology of Zurbriggen et al. (2011) to assess partner-objectification,

participants completed a modified version of the surveillance subscale (McKinley & Hyde,

1996). In this case, all instances of the words “I” and “my body” were replaced with “my partner”

or “my partner’s body.” Participants responded to the same 7 items with 5 reverse coded (from 1,

strongly disagree to 7, strongly agree) that analyzed the extent to which they take a third-person

perspective of their partner’s bodies (e.g., “I am more concerned with what my partner’s body can

do than how it looks”). An average of the seven items was taken for the current study with

higher scores reflecting greater engagement in partner-objectification. The average showed low

reliability (α = 0.58).

Relationship Satisfaction and Closeness.

To assess relationship satisfaction and closeness two measures were employed. First, to

assess relationship closeness as defined by feelings of connectedness with one’s partner, the

Inclusion of the Other in the Self (IOS) scale by Aron et al. (1992) was used. Participants

selected one of seven pairs of increasingly overlapping circles. In each pair of circles, one circle

refers to the participant and the other circle to the participant’s romantic partner. Participants

12
were asked to select the pair of circles (from 1, not very connected, to 7, very connected) that

most accurately depicts how emotionally close they feel to their partner in the current moment,

ranging from an almost completely overlapping to a mostly non-overlapping pair of circles.

Second, relationship satisfaction was assessed utilizing the satisfaction subscale of the

Investment Model Scale (Rusbult et al., 1998), as a core component of relationship satisfaction is

bondedness (Sternberg, 1986). The subscale consisted of seven items that measured (from 1, do

not agree at all, to 7, agree completely) an individual’s satisfaction with their partner, where

higher scores indicated greater satisfaction with the relationship (e.g., “I feel satisfied with my

relationship”). The two measures were averaged to create a composite measure of relationship

satisfaction and closeness and showed great reliability (α = .95).

Sexual Closeness.

Feelings of sexual closeness to one’s romantic partner were measured by a vignette

created by the PI. The vignette defined what it means to feel sexually close/connected to one’s

romantic partner (e.g., “when you feel wholly intertwined and fully lost in the being of your

partner, as though you do not know where they end and you begin.”). Participants were then

asked two questions assessing how sexually connected they would feel to their romantic partner

(from 5, I would definitely not feel sexually connected to 1, I definitely would feel sexually

connected) and how self-conscious they would feel about their physical appearance if they were

to have sex with their partner right now (from 1, I would definitely feel self-conscious to 5, I

definitely would not feel self-conscious). The first item was reverse scored such that high scores

would reflect high sexual closeness. Further, the two items showed poor reliability when

averaged together to form one scale of sexual closeness (α = 0.44) and thus were evaluated

13
independently for the current study.

Relationship Length/Status.

Relationship length was measured by the duration (e.g., months, years) of romantic

partnerships while relationship status of participants was measured by the legality of ties a

couple shares (e.g., newly dating, married). Thus, participants were asked to select what type of

relationship they are in (e.g., relationship status) when completing the demographics measure,

ranging from a variety of partnership options (e.g.,“ friends with benefits”, “engaged”) and how

long they have been dating their partner (e.g., relationship length; “less than one month”, “one

year”). These two items (e.g., relationship length and status) were then standardized and

averaged together to form a composite measure with acceptable reliability (α = 0.72). A higher

score on this measure reflects relationships that are longer in length and more bound by legal

agreements (i.e., marriages) in contrast to those short in length and not bound by legalities (e.g.,

newly dating).

Social Media Use.

The variable social media use was measured by four items (from 1, never to 7, multiple

times a day) assessing how frequently an individual uses social media (e.g., “How frequently do

you use social media?”) and its features (e.g., “How frequently do you post photos to social

media?”). The items showed acceptable reliability when averaged together (α = 0.64).

Demographics.

A standard demographics questionnaire was administered assessing the gender, age, race

and ethnicity, and academic year of all participants.

14
Procedure

Young adults, aged 18-24, who indicated that they were currently in or had been in a

relationship where they dated or had intimate sexual relations with an individual of the opposite

sex were recruited to participate in an online study in Qualtrics through SONA. Participants that

indicated they were not currently in a romantic relationship but had previously been in a

romantic relationship were asked to think of their most recent romantic partner for the duration

of the study. Upon clicking the link to participate in the Qualtrics survey through SONA,

participants were informed that we were conducting a study about the type of genres and media

content adults and couples currently like to watch that is a part of a larger study evaluating

relationship experiences overall (Appendix A). Participants were then randomly assigned to one

of two media content conditions to watch for just under three minutes. These served as our two

conditions (non-sexualized social media content and sexualized social media content). After the

manipulation, participants completed the modified Objectified Body Consciousness surveillance

subscale (McKinley & Hyde, 1996) for the partner and the self, the Inclusion of the Other in the

Self (IOS) scale (Aron et al., 1992), the sexual closeness items, the Investment Model

satisfaction subscale (Rusbult et al., 1998), a short demographic questionnaire inclusive of

relationship status/length questions, and a social media use questionnaire. The Objectified Body

Consciousness surveillance subscale (McKinley & Hyde, 1996) for the self and partner was

presented in a counterbalanced order. Further, scales were distributed with an attention check in

addition to filler questions pertaining to the media content watched (e.g., “my partner and I

would watch the previous media clips”) on a 5-point type Likert scale (from 1, “strongly

disagree” to 5, “strongly agree”) in order to keep participants from actively devising that we

were solely interested in objectification and satisfaction (see Appendix B). Upon completion,

15
participants were debriefed and thanked for their time. All procedures were approved by the

university’s Institutional Review Board.

16
RESULTS

Recall that we hypothesized that in response to the sexualized social media condition

individuals will experience higher levels of self and partner objectification (Hypotheses 1a and

1b), in response to the sexualized social media content women will engage in greater self-

objectification and men greater partner-objectification (Hypotheses 2a and 2b), exposure to

sexualized social media will reduce feelings of sexual closeness through increases in self-

objectification and reduce feelings of relationship closeness and satisfaction through increases in

partner-objectification (Hypotheses 3a and 3b), and exposure to sexualized social media will

reduce feelings of sexual closeness through increases in self-objectification specifically for

women and reduce feelings of relationship closeness and satisfaction through increases in

partner-objectification specifically for men (Hypotheses 4a and 4b).

To test these hypotheses, we first employed two linear regressions to examine the direct

effects of social media content on self and partner objectification. Second, we employed two

separate moderation analyses utilizing PROCESS Model 1 (Hayes, 2012) with bias-accelerated

bootstrap confidence intervals based on 10000 draws to examine the direct effects of social

media content on self and partner objectification as moderated by gender. Third, we utilized

three PROCESS Model 4s (Hayes, 2012) with bias-accelerated bootstrap confidence intervals

based on 10000 draws to assess the relationship between exposure to sexualized social media

content and self-objectification as it relates to the two items assessing sexual closeness and

partner-objectification as it relates to the one item assessing relationship closeness and

satisfaction. Lastly, we employed two PROCESS Model 7s (Hayes, 2012) with bias-accelerated

17
bootstrap confidence intervals based on 10000 draws to assess if gender moderates the

relationship between exposure to sexualized social media content and self and partner

objectification as it relates to the outcomes of sexual closeness and relationship closeness and

satisfaction. In all analyses, apart from the linear regressions, the variables of relationship

length/status and social media use were entered as covariates to control for their effects. All

results and analyses are reported below.

Social Media and Objectification

In order to test Hypothesis1a, that individuals who are exposed to sexualized social media

content will experience higher levels of self-objectification than those who are exposed to non-

sexualizing social media content, a simple linear regression was employed to test if social media

condition (0 = non-sexualized, 1 = sexualized) significantly predicted self-objectification. The

model was not significant, F (1,408) = 1.667, p = 0.197. Therefore, contrary to the hypothesis,

social media condition did not significantly predict self-objectification. The model explained

0.04% of the variance in self-objectification (R2 = .004). See Table 1.

To assess Hypothesis 1b, that individuals who are exposed to sexualized social media

content will experience higher levels of partner-objectification than those who are exposed to

non-sexualized social media content, a simple linear regression was carried out to test if social

media condition significantly predicted partner-objectification. The model was not significant, F

(1,408) = 0.02, p = 0.871. Therefore, contrary to the hypothesis, social media condition did not

significantly predict partner-objectification. The model explained less than 1% of the variance in

partner-objectification (R2 = .000). See Table 2.

18
Social Media, Gender, and Objectification

To assess Hypothesis 2a, which predicts that the relationship between exposure to

sexualized social media content and self-objectification will be moderated by gender, where

females will report engaging in self-objectification to a greater extent than males, a moderation

analysis using Model 1 of the PROCESS macro developed by Hayes (2012) was employed. For

this analysis, self-objectification served as the dependent variable and social media condition (0

= non-sexualized, 1 = sexualized) was the predictor variable. Gender served as a moderator of

the condition/self-objectification relationship.

Consistent with the results of the linear regression, the results of the regression analysis

revealed that social media condition was not associated with self-objectification, β = 0.17, SE =

0.16, t (388) = 1.04, p = 0.29, 95% CI [-0.15, 0.49]. Gender was also not associated with self-

objectification, β = -0.24, SE = 0.16, t (388) = -1.45, p = 0.14, 95% CI [-0.58, 0.08]. Moreover,

contrary to the hypothesis, the interaction between social media condition and gender was not

statistically significant, β = -0.10, SE = 0.23, t (388) = -0.46, p = 0.64, 95% CI [-0.57, 0.35]. The

covariate relationship length/status was also not statistically significant, β = 0.02, SE = 0.06, t

(388) = 0.31, p = 0.75, 95% CI [-0.11, 0.15]. Social media use, however, was associated with

self-objectification, β = 0.20, SE = 0.06, t (388) = 3.22, p = .001; 95% CI [0.07, 0.32]. Greater

social media use was associated with greater self-objectification. See Table 3.

To assess Hypothesis 2b, which examines whether the relationship between exposure to

sexualized social media content and partner-objectification will be moderated by gender, where

males will report engaging in partner-objectification to a greater extent than females. A

moderation analysis was employed, a moderation analysis was conducted using Model 1 of the

PROCESS macro developed by Hayes. For this analysis, partner-objectification was the

19
dependent variable and social media condition was the predictor variable. Gender served as a

moderator of the condition/partner-objectification relationship.

In-line with the previous linear regression analysis, the results of the regression analysis

revealed that social media condition was not associated with partner-objectification, β = -0.02,

SE = 0.12, t (388) = -0.16, p = 0.87, 95% CI [-0.26, 0.22]. Gender was associated with partner-

objectification β = 0.26, SE = 0.13, t (388) = 2.00, p = 0.04, 95% CI [0.005, 0.52]. Males

engaged in greater partner-objectification than females. However, contrary to the hypothesis, the

interaction between social media condition and gender was not statistically significant, β = 0.03,

SE = 0.18, t (388) = 0.17, p = .85, 95% CI [-0.32, 0.38]. Further, the covariate relationship

length/ status was not statistically significant, β = -0.08, SE = 0.05, t (388) = -1.58, p = 0.11,

95% CI [-0.18, 0.01]. Social media use, however, was associated with partner-objectification, β =

0.10, SE = 0.04, t (388) = 2.24, p = 0.02, 95% CI [0.01, 0.20]. Greater social media use was

associated with greater partner-objectification. See Table 4.

Social Media, Objectification, Sexual and Relational Closeness

To assess Hypothesis 3a, which states the relationship between social media content and

feelings of sexual closeness will be mediated by self-objectification, such that exposure to

sexualized social media will reduce feelings of sexual closeness through increases in self-

objectification, analyses of mediation were conducted with each item of sexual closeness:

feelings of sexual connectedness and feelings of self-consciousness about physical appearance

during sex. The outcome of sexual closeness was evaluated using the two separate items due to

poor reliability between the items. Both analyses used Model 4 of the PROCESS macro

developed by Hayes.

20
In the first analysis, social media condition (0 = non-sexualized, 1 = sexualized) served as

the independent variable, self-objectification was the mediator and feelings of sexual

connectedness was the outcome. In concordance with prior analyses, the effect of social media

condition on self-objectification was not statistically significant, β = 0.13, SE = 0.11, t (396) =

1.18, p = 0.23, 95% CI [-0.09, 0.36]. Once again, the covariate social media use did significantly

affect rates of self-objectification, β = 0.21, SE = 0.06, t (396) = 3.52, p <.001, 95% CI [0.09,

0.33]. Greater social media use was related to greater rates of self-objectification. Relationship

length/status however was again not significantly related to self-objectification, β = 0.02, SE =

0.06, t (396) = 0.40, p = 0.68, 95% CI [-0.10, 0.15].

Further, self-objectification did not significantly predict feelings of sexual connectedness,

β = -0.06, SE = 0.04, t (395) = -1.42, p = 0.15, 95% CI [-0.15, 0.02]. Social media condition also

did not predict feelings of sexual connectedness, β = 0.02, SE = 0.10, t (395) = 0.19, p = 0.84,

95% CI [-0.19, 0.23]. The covariate social media use did, however, significantly affect feelings

of sexual connectedness, β = 0.13, SE = 0.05, t (395) = 2.44, p = 0.01, 95% CI [0.02, 0.25].

Greater social media use was related to greater feelings of sexual connectedness. Relationship

length/status was also significantly related to feelings of sexual connectedness, β = 0.30, SE =

0.06, t (395) = 4.98, p < .001, 95% CI [0.18, 0.42]. Individuals who have been in a relationship

for a long period of time and who are seriously dating their partner feel greater feelings of sexual

connectedness towards their partner. However, contrary to the hypothesis, there was not an

indirect effect of condition on feelings of sexual connectedness mediated by self-objectification,

B = -0.009, SE = 0.01, 95% CI [-0.03, 0.01]. See Table 5.

The analysis was then repeated with self-consciousness about physical appearance during

sex as the outcome. Again, the effect of social media condition on self-objectification was not

21
statistically significant, β = 0.13, SE = 0.11, t (396) = 1.18, p = 0.23, 95% CI [-0.09, 0.36]. The

covariate social media use continued to significantly affect rates of self-objectification, β = 0.21,

SE = 0.06, t (396) = 3.57, p < .001, 95% CI [0.09, 0.33]. Greater social media use was related to

greater rates of self-objectification. Relationship length/status was not significantly related to

self-objectification, β = 0.02, SE = 0.06, t (396) = 0.40, p = 0.68, 95% CI [-0.10, 0.15].

In contrast to the other measure of sexual closeness, self-objectification significantly

predicted feelings of self-consciousness about one’s physical appearance during sex, β = -0.35,

SE = 0.05, t (395) = -6.40, p<.001, 95% CI [-0.45, -0.24]. Greater self-objectification was also

related to greater feelings of self-consciousness about one’s appearance and body during sex.

Social media condition did not predict feelings of self-consciousness about one's physical

appearance during sex, β = -0.08, SE = 0.12, t (395) = -0.67, p = 0.50, 95% CI [-0.33, 0.16].

Further, in this analysis, the covariate social media use did not significantly affect feelings of

self-consciousness about one's physical appearance during sex, β = 0.08, SE = 0.06, t (395) =

1.30, p = 0.19, 95% CI [-0.04, 0.22]. Relationship length/status was significantly related to

feelings of self-consciousness about one's physical appearance during sex, β = 0.29, SE = 0.07, t

(395) = 4.00, p < .001, 95% CI [0.14, 0.43]. Individuals who had been in a relationship for a

long period of time and who were seriously dating their partner reported feeling less self-

conscious about their physical appearance during sex. Lastly, contrary to the hypothesis, there

was not an indirect effect of social media condition on feelings of self-consciousness about one's

physical appearance during sex mediated by self-objectification, B = -0.04, SE = 0.04, 95% CI [-

0.14, 0.03]. See Table 5.

To test Hypothesis 3b, which examines whether the relationship between social media

content and feelings of relationship satisfaction is mediated by partner-objectification, such that

22
exposure to sexualized social media will reduce feelings of relationship satisfaction through

increases in partner-objectification a mediation analysis was conducted using Model 4 of the

PROCESS macro developed by Hayes. In this analysis, social media condition (0 = non-

sexualized, 1 = sexualized) served as the independent variable, partner-objectification was the

mediator and feelings of relationship satisfaction and closeness was the outcome. Relationship

length/status and social media use were entered in the model as covariates.

As observed in previous analyses, the effect of social media condition on partner-

objectification was not statistically significant, β = 0.01, SE = 0.09, t (396) = 0.15, p = 0.88, 95%

CI [-0.16, 0.19]. Further, in this analysis, the covariate social media use in this current model did

not significantly affect rates of partner-objectification, β = 0.07, SE = 0.04, t (396) = 1.58, p =

0.11, 95% CI [-0.01, 0.16]. Relationship length/status was also not significantly related to

partner-objectification, β = -0.09, SE = 0.05, t(396) = -1.87, p = 0.06, 95% CI [-0.19, 0.01].

Moreover, partner-objectification significantly predicted relationship satisfaction and

feelings of closeness to one’s partner, β = -0.19, SE = 0.07, t (395) = -2.43, p = .015, 95% CI [-

0.34, -0.03]. Greater partner-objectification was related to less relationship satisfaction and

feeling distant from one’s romantic partner. Further, social media condition did not predict

feelings of relationship satisfaction and closeness, β = 0.06, SE = 0.14, t (395) = 0.48, p = 0.62,

95% CI [-0.21, 0.34]. The covariate social media use did not significantly affect feelings of

relationship satisfaction and closeness, β = 0.006, SE = 0.07, t (395) = 0.08, p = 0.93, 95% CI [-

0.14, 0.15]. Relationship length/status was significantly related to feelings of relationship

satisfaction and closeness to one’s partner, β = 0.93, SE = 0.08, t (395) = 11.60, p < .001, 95% CI

[0.77, 1.09]. Individuals who have been in a relationship for a long period of time and who are

seriously dating their partner, feel greater relationship satisfaction and closeness to their romantic

23
partner. However, contrary to the hypotheses there was not an indirect effect of social media

condition on feelings of relationship satisfaction and closeness to one’s partner mediated by

partner-objectification, B = -0.002, SE = 0.01, 95%CI [-0.04, 0.03]. See Table 6.

Social Media, Objectification, Gender, Sexual and Relational Closeness

To assess Hypothesis 4a, which assesses if higher rates of self-objectification induced by

exposure to sexualized social media content will result in decreased feelings of sexual closeness,

particularly for women, two analyses of moderated mediation were conducted, one on each

measure of sexual closeness. The first analysis used Model 7 of the PROCESS macro developed

by Hayes. In this analysis, social media condition (0 = non-sexualized, 1 = sexualized) served as

the independent variable, self-objectification was the mediator, feelings of sexual connectedness

was the outcome, and gender was the moderator.

As observed in the previous analyses, the effect of social media condition on self-

objectification was not significant, β = 0.17, SE = 0.16, t (388) = 1.04, p = 0.29, 95% CI [-0.15,

0.49]. Gender did not significantly affect self-objectification in this analysis, β = -0.24, SE =

0.16, t (388) = -1.45, p = 0.14, 95% CI [-0.58, -0.08]. Gender also did not interact with condition,

β = -0.10, SE = 0.23, t (388) = -0.46, p = 0.64, CI [-0.57, 0.35]. Further, the covariate social

media use once again significantly affected rates of self-objectification, β = 0.20, SE = 0.06, t

(388) = 3.22, p = .001, 95% CI [0.07, 0.32]. Greater social media use was related to greater rates

of self-objectification. Relationship length/status was not significantly related to self-

objectification, β = 0.02, SE = 0.06, t (388) = 0.31, p = 0.75, 95% CI [-0.11, 0.15].

Further, as in the previous analysis self-objectification did not significantly predict

feelings of sexual connectedness, β = -0.06, SE = 0.04, t (389) = -1.41, p = 0.15, 95% CI [-0.15,

0.02]. Social media condition also did not predict feelings of sexual connectedness, β = 0.02, SE

24
= 0.10, t (389) = 0.26, p = 0.79, 95% CI [-0.18, 0.24]. The covariate social media use did again

significantly affect feelings of sexual connectedness, β = 0.13, SE = 0.05, t (389) = 2.36, p =

0.01, 95% CI [0.02, 0.25]. Greater social media use was related to greater feelings of sexual

connectedness. Relationship length/status was also significantly related to feelings of sexual

connectedness, β = 0.30, SE = 0.06, t (389) = 4.97, p < .001, 95% CI [0.18, 0.42]. Individuals

who have been in a relationship for a long period of time and who are seriously dating their

partner, feel greater feelings of sexual connectedness to their partner. Contrary to the hypothesis,

however, there was not an indirect effect of social media condition on sexual connectedness

mediated by self-objectification, for males, B = -0.004, SE = 0.01, 95% CI [-0.03, 0.02], or

females, β = -0.011, SE = 0.01, 95% CI [-0.05, 0.01]. Lastly, gender did not moderate this effect,

β = 0.007, SE = 0.02, 95% CI [-0.02, 0.05]. See Table 7.

The analyses were then repeated with feelings of self-consciousness about physical

appearance during sex as the outcome. In concordance with the prior analysis, the effect of social

media condition on self-objectification was not significant, β = 0.17, SE = 0.16, t (388) = 1.04, p

= 0.29, 95% CI [-0.15, 0.49]. Gender also did not significantly affect self-objectification in this

analysis, β = -0.24, SE = 0.16, t (388) = -1.45, p = 0.14, 95% CI [-0.58, -0.08]. Gender also did

not interact with condition, β = -0.10, SE = 0.23, t (388) = -0.46, p = 0.64, 95% CI [-0.57, 0.35].

Further, the covariate social media use did significantly affect rates of self-objectification, β =

0.20, SE = 0.06, t (388) = 3.22, p = .001, 95% CI [0.07, 0.32]. Greater social media use was

related to greater rates of self-objectification. Relationship length/status was not significantly

related to self-objectification, β = 0.02, SE = 0.06, t (388) = 0.31, p = 0.75, 95% CI [-0.11, 0.15].

Further, in contrast to the analysis on feelings of sexual connectedness, self-objectification

significantly predicted feelings of self-consciousness about one’s physical appearance during sex,

25
β = -0.36, SE = 0.05, t (389) = -6.64, p < .001, 95% CI [-0.47, -0.25]. Greater self-objectification

was related to greater feelings of self-consciousness about one’s appearance and body during sex.

However, social media condition did not predict feelings of self-consciousness about one's

physical appearance during sex, β = -0.10, SE = 0.12, t (389) = -0.85, p = 0.39, 95% CI [-0.36,

0.14]. The covariate social media use in this analysis once again did not significantly affect

feelings of self-consciousness about one's physical appearance during sex, β = 0.10, SE = 0.06, t

(389) = 1.52, p = 0.12, 95% CI [-0.02, 0.23]. Relationship length/status was significantly related

to feelings of self-consciousness about one's physical appearance during sex, β = 0.30, SE =

0.07, t (389) = 4.16, p < .001, 95% CI [0.15, 0.44]. Individuals who had been in a relationship

for a long period of time and who were seriously dating their partner, reported feeling less self-

conscious about their physical appearance during sex. Contrary to the hypothesis, there was not

an indirect effect of social media condition on feelings of self-consciousness about one's physical

appearance during sex mediated by self-objectification, for either males, B = -0.02, SE = 0.06,

95% CI [-0.14, 0.09], or females B = -0.06, SE = 0.06, 95% CI [-0.19, 0.05]. Gender did not

moderate this effect, B = 0.03, SE = 0.08, 95% CI [-0.12, 0.21]. See Table 7.

To test Hypothesis 4b, which states that higher rates of partner-objectification induced by

exposure to sexualized social media content will result in decreased relationship satisfaction,

particularly for men, a moderated mediation analysis was conducted using Model 7 of the

PROCESS macro developed by Hayes. In this analysis, social media condition (0 = non-

sexualized, 1 = sexualized) served as the independent variable, partner-objectification was the

mediator, feelings of relationship satisfaction and closeness was the outcome, and gender was the

moderator.

26
As observed in all previous analyses, the effect of condition on partner-objectification was

not statistically significant, β = -0.02, SE = 0.12, t (388) = -0.16, p = 0.87, 95% CI [-0.28, 0.21].

Gender was related to partner-objectification, β = 0.26, SE = 0.13, t (388) = 2.00, p = 0.04, 95%

CI [0.005, 0.52]. Males engaged in greater partner-objectification than females. Gender,

however, did not interact with social media condition, β = 0.03, SE = 0.18, t (388) = 0.17, p =

0.85, 95% CI [-0.32, 0.38]. In contrast to the mediation analysis but in alignment with the prior

moderation analysis, the covariate social media use did significantly affect rates of partner-

objectification, β = 0.10, SE = 0.04, t (388) = 2.24, p = 0.02, 95% CI [0.01, 0.20]. Greater social

media use was associated with greater partner objectification. Relationship length/status was not

significantly related to partner-objectification, β = -0.08, SE = 0.05, t (388) = -1.58, p = 0.11,

95% CI [-0.18, 0.01].

Further, partner-objectification once again significantly predicted relationship satisfaction

and feelings of closeness to one’s partner, β = -0.20, SE = 0.07, t (389) = -2.52, p = .01, 95% CI

[-0.35, -0.04]. Greater partner-objectification was related to less relationship satisfaction and

feeling distant from one’s romantic partner. Social media condition, however, did not predict

feelings of relationship satisfaction and closeness, β = 0.08, SE = 0.14, t (389) = 0.59, p = 0.54,

95% CI [-0.19, 0.36]. The covariate social media use did not significantly affect feelings of

relationship satisfaction and closeness, β = 0.009, SE = 0.07, t (389) = 0.01, p = 0.99, 95% CI [-

0.14, 0.14]. Relationship length/status was significantly related to feelings of relationship

satisfaction and closeness to one’s partner, β = 0.93, SE = 0.08, t (389) = 11.52, p < .001, 95% CI

[0.77, 1.09]. Individuals who had been in a relationship for a long period of time and who were

seriously dating their partner, reported greater relationship satisfaction and closeness to their

romantic partner. Contrary to the hypothesis, however, there was not an indirect effect of social

27
media condition on feelings of relationship satisfaction and closeness mediated by partner-

objectification, for either males B = -0.002, SE = 0.02, 95% CI [-0.06, 0.04] or females B =

0.004, SE = 0.02, 95% CI [-0.05, 0.06]. Lastly, gender did not moderate this effect, B = -0.006,

SE = 0.03, 95% CI [-0.08, 0.07]. See Table 8.

Table 1. Linear Regression Analysis Assessing Self-Objectification.


df SS MS F p

Regression 1 2.34 2.34 1.66 0.19

Residual 408 572.50 1.40

Total 409 574.84

Coefficient SE LLCI ULCI t p

Constant 4.00 0.08 3.83 4.16 47.78 0.00***

Condition 0.15 0.11 -0.07 0.38 1.29 0.19


Note. In the model, condition is coded such that non-sexualized is 0 and sexualized is 1. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Table 2. Linear Regression Analysis Assessing Partner-Objectification.


df SS MS F p

Regression 1 0.22 0.02 0.02 0.87

Residual 408 338.51 0.83

Total 409 338.53

Coefficient SE LLCI ULCI t p

Constant 3.31 0.06 3.19 3.44 51.52 0.00***

Condition -0.01 0.09 -0.19 0.16 -0.16 0.87


Note. In the model, condition is coded such that non-sexualized is 0 and sexualized is. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

28
Table 3. Moderation Analysis Examining Self-Objectification
Model 1 (n=394) Effect SE LLCI ULCI t p

Predictor

Condition 0.17 0.16 -0.15 0.49 1.04 0.29

Moderator

Gender -0.24 0.16 -0.58 0.08 -1.45 0.14

Interaction

Condition→ Gender -0.10 0.23 -0.57 0.35 -0.46 0.64

Covariate

Relationship length 0.02 0.06 -0.11 0.15 0.31 0.75

SNS Use 0.20 0.06 0.07 0.32 3.22** 0.00

Note. These analyses treat relationship length and label as well as SNS use as a covariate. In the model, condition is coded such
that non-sexualized is 0 and sexualized is 1 and gender is coded such that female is 0 and male is 1. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p <
.001.

Table 4. Moderation Analysis Examining Partner-Objectification


Model 1 (n=394) Effect SE LLCI ULCI t p

Predictor

Condition -0.02 0.12 -0.15 0.49 1.04 0.29

Moderator

Gender 0.26 0.13 0.005 0.52 2.00* 0.04

Interaction

Condition→ Gender 0.03 0.18 -0.32 0.38 0.17 0.85

Covariate

Relationship length -0.08 0.05 -0.18 0.01 -1.58 0.11

SNS Use 0.10 0.04 0.01 0.20 2.24* 0.02

Note. These analyses treat relationship length and label as well as SNS use as a covariate. In the model, condition is coded such
that non-sexualized is 0 and sexualized is 1 and gender is coded such that female is 0 and male is 1.*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p <
.001

29
Table 5. Mediation Analyses Examining Sexual Closeness
Model 1 (n=400) Effect SE LLCI ULCI t p

Path

a: C→SO 0.13 0.11 -0.09 0.36 1.18 0.23

b: SO→ SC -0.06 0.04 -0.15 0.02 -1.42 0.15

c’: C→SC 0.02 0.10 -0.19 0.23 0.19 0.84

Covariate

SNS→ SO 0.21 0.06 0.09 0.33 3.57*** 0.00

RLL→ SO 0.02 0.06 -0.10 0.15 0.40 .68

SNS→SC 0.13 0.05 0.02 0.25 2.44* 0.01

RLL→SC 0.30 0.06 0.18 0.42 4.98*** 0.00

Indirect effect

c: C→SO→SC -0.009 0.01 -0.03 0.007

Model 2 (n=400) Effect SE LLCI ULCI t p

Path

a: C→SO 0.13 0.11 -0.09 0.36 1.18 0.23

b: SO→ SFC -0.35 0.05 -0.45 -0.24 -6.40*** 0.00

c’: C→SFC -0.08 0.12 -0.33 0.16 -0.67 0.50

Covariate

SNS→ SO 0.21 0.06 0.09 0.33 3.57*** 0.00

RLL→ SO 0.02 0.06 -0.10 0.15 0.40 0.68

SNS→SFC 0.08 0.06 -0.04 0.22 1.30 0.19

RLL→SFC 0.29 0.07 0.14 0.43 4.00*** 0.00

Indirect effect

c: C→SO→SFC -0.04 0.04 -0.14 0.03

Note. These analyses treat relationship length and label as well as SNS use as a covariate. Model 1 refers to the analysis
evaluating feelings of sexual connectedness. Model 2 refers to the analysis of evaluating self-consciousness about one’s
appearance during sex. In both models, condition is coded such that non-sexualized is 0 and sexualized is 1. “C” refers to
Condition, “SO” refers to self-objectification, “SNS” refers to SNS Use, “RLL” refers to relationship length/status, “SC” refers to
sexual connectedness, and “SFC” refers to self-consciousness about appearance. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

30
Table 6. Mediation Analysis Examining Relationship Satisfaction and Closeness
Model 1 (n=400) Effect SE LLCI ULCI t p

Path

a: C→PO 0.01 0.09 -0.16 0.19 0.15 0.88

b: PO→ RSC -0.19 0.07 -0.34 -0.03 -2.43* 0.01

c’: C→RSC 0.06 0.14 -0.21 0.34 0.48 0.62

Covariate

SNS→ PO 0.07 0.04 -0.01 0.16 1.58 0.11

RLL→ PO -0.09 0.05 -0.19 0.004 -1.87 0.06

SNS→RSC 0.006 0.07 -0.14 0.15 0.08 0.93

RLL →RSC 0.93 0.08 0.77 1.09 11.60*** 0.00

Indirect effect

c: C→PO→RSC -0.002 0.01 -0.04 0.03

Note. These analyses treat relationship length and label as well as SNS use as a covariate. In this model, condition is coded such
that non-sexualized is 0 and sexualized is 1. “C” refers to Condition, “PO” refers to partner-objectification, “SNS” refers to SNS
Use, “RLL” refers to relationship length and status, “RSC” refers to relationship satisfaction and closeness . *p < .05, **p < .01,
***p < .001.

31
Table 7. Moderated Mediation Analyses Examining Sexual Closeness
Model 1 (n=394) Effect SE LLCI ULCI t p

Path

a: C→SO 0.17 0.16 -0.15 0.49 1.04 0.29

a2: G→ SO -0.24 0.16 -0.58 0.08 -1.45 0.14

b: SO→ SC -0.06 0.04 -0.15 0.02 -1.41 0.15

c’: C→SC 0.02 0.10 -0.18 0.24 0.26 0.79

Covariate

SNS→ SO 0.20 0.06 0.07 0.32 3.22** 0.00

RLL→ SO 0.02 0.06 -0.11 0.15 0.31 0.75

SNS→SC 0.13 0.05 0.02 0.25 2.36* 0.01

RLL→SC 0.30 0.06 0.18 0.42 4.97*** 0.00

Interaction effect

c: C x G -0.10 0.23 -0.57 0.35 -0.46 0.64

Indirect effect

c: C→SO→SC -0.01 0.01 -0.05 0.01

0.01 -0.03 0.02


-0.004

Moderated
mediation

0.007 0.02 -0.02 0.05

Model 2 (n=394) Effect SE LLCI ULCI t p

Path

a: C→SO 0.17 0.16 -0.15 0.49 1.04 0.29

a2: G→ SO -0.24 0.16 -0.58 0.08 -1.45 0.14

b: SO→ SFC -0.36 0.05 -0.47 -0.25 -6.64*** 0.00

c’: C→SFC -0.10 0.12 -0.36 0.14 -0.85 0.39

Covariate

SNS→ SO 0.20 0.06 0.07 0.32 3.22** 0.00

32
Table 7. Cont.
RLL→ SO 0.02 0.06 -0.11 0.15 0.31 0.75

SNS→SFC 0.10 0.06 -0.02 0.23 1.52 0.12

RLL→SFC 0.30 0.07 0.15 0.44 4.16*** 0.00

Interaction effect

c: C x G -0.10 0.23 -0.57 0.35 -0.46 0.64

Indirect effect

c: C→SO→SFC -0.06 0.06 -0.19 0.05

-0.02 0.06 -0.14 0.09

Moderated
mediation

0.03 0.08 -0.12 0.21

Note. These analyses treat relationship length and label as well as SNS use as a covariate. Model 1 refers to the analysis
evaluating feelings of sexual connectedness. Model 2 refers to the analysis of evaluating self-consciousness about one’s
appearance during sex. In both models, condition is coded such that non-sexualized is 0 and sexualized is 1 and gender is coded
such that female is 0 and male is 1. “C” refers to Condition, “SO” refers to self-objectification, “G” refers to gender, “SNS” refers
to SNS Use, “RLL” refers to relationship length and status, “SC” refers to sexual connectedness, and “SFC” refers to self-
consciousness about appearance. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

33
Table 8. Moderated Mediation Analyses Examining Relationship Satisfaction and Closeness
Model 1 (n=394) Effect SE LLCI ULCI t p

Path

a: C→PO -0.02 0.12 -0.26 0.22 -0.16 0.87

a2: G→ PO 0.26 0.13 0.005 0.52 2.00* 0.04

b: PO→ RSC -0.20 0.07 -0.35 -0.04 -2.52* 0.01

c’: C→RSC 0.08 0.14 -0.19 0.36 0.59 0.54

Covariate

SNS→ PO 0.10 0.04 0.01 0.20 2.24* 0.02

RLL→ PO -0.08 0.05 -0.18 0.01 -1.58 0.11

SNS→RSC 0.0009 0.07 -0.14 0.14 0.01 0.99

RLL→RSC 0.93 0.08 0.77 1.09 11.52*** 0.00

Interaction effect

c: C x G -0.10 0.23 -0.57 0.35 -0.46 0.64

Indirect effect

c: C→PO→RSC 0.004 0.02 -0.05 0.06

0.02 -0.06 0.04


-0.002

Moderated
mediation

-0.006 0.03 -0.08 0.07

Note. These analyses treat relationship length and label as well as SNS use as a covariate. In this model, condition is coded such
that non-sexualized is 0 and sexualized is 1 and gender is coded such that female is 0 and male is 1. “C” refers to Condition, “G”
refers to gender, “PO” refers to partner-objectification, “SNS” refers to SNS Use, “RLL” refers to relationship length and status,
“RSC” refers to relationship satisfaction and closeness . *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

34
SUPPLEMENTAL ANALYSES
In order to demonstrate the above effects were unique to the variables of self and partner

objectification four items, two assessing self-comparison (e.g., “I am inclined to compare my

physical appearance to the appearance of others”) and two assessing partner-comparison (e.g.,

“I am inclined to compare my partner’s physical appearance to the appearance of others”) were

included in the current study on a 7-point type Likert scale (from 1, “strongly disagree” to 7,

“strongly agree”. The two sets of comparison items, one for the self and one for the partner, were

independently averaged together to create a composite of self-comparison (α = .73) and partner-

comparison items (α = .63). The comparison items were then entered as a covariate into each

model listed above (e.g., self-comparison was entered when evaluating models inclusive of self-

objectification and partner-comparison was entered when evaluating models inclusive of partner-

objectification).

When examining the results, all models of self-objectification inclusive of self-

comparison remain unchanged. However, when evaluating the models of partner-objectification

inclusive of partner-comparison, the results do not remain consistent. For one when including

partner-comparison into the moderation analysis of social media condition on partner-

objectification, gender becomes not significant, B = 0.19, t (388) = 1.73, p = 0.08, SE = 0.11,

95% CI [-0.02, 0.42]. Secondly, in the mediation analysis of social media on relationship

satisfaction and closeness as mediated by partner-objectification the path of partner-

objectification on relationship satisfaction and closeness becomes not significant, B = -0.05, t

(388) = -0.63, p = 0.52, SE = 0.08, 95% CI [-0.22, 0.11]. Then lastly, in line with the prior

35
analysis listed, when examining the moderated mediation analysis of social media condition to

relationship satisfaction and closeness, the path of gender to partner-objectification remains not

significant, B = 0.19, t (388) = 1.73, p = 0.08, SE = 0.11, 95% CI [-0.02, 0.42], and the path of

partner-objectification to relationship satisfaction closeness also remains not significant, B = -

0.06, t (388) = -0.68, p = 0.49, SE = 0.09, 95% CI [-0.223, 0.11]. This illuminates that partner-

objectification is impacted by the inclusion of the covariate partner-comparison and as a result

partner-objectification may not fully explain the relationships observed amongst the variables of

interest in the current study. All results and analyses inclusive of self and partner comparison as

a covariate are reported in Tables 9-14. The new pathways inclusive of the comparison items and

any pathways that change as a result of the comparison items for the self or partner being entered

into the model have been highlighted below.

Table 9. Moderation Analysis Examining Self-Objectification


Model 1 (n=394) Effect SE LLCI ULCI t p

Predictor

Condition 0.21 0.13 -0.03 0.47 1.67 0.09

Moderator

Gender 0.06 0.13 -0.20 0.33 0.46 0.64

Interaction

Condition→ Gender -0.16 0.18 -0.53 0.20 -0.87 0.38

Covariate

Relationship length -0.03 0.05 -0.13 0.07 -0.62 0.52

SNS Use 0.13 0.05 0.03 0.23 2.74** 0.00

Self-Comparison 0.43 0.02 0.37 0.49 14.84*** 0.00

Note. These analyses treat relationship length and label, SNS use, as well as self-comparison as a covariate. In the model, the
condition is coded such that non-sexualized is 0 and sexualized is 1 and gender is coded such that female is 0 and male is 1.
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

36
Table 10. Moderation Analysis Examining Partner-Objectification
Model 1 (n=394) Effect SE LLCI ULCI t p

Predictor

Condition 0.003 0.11 -0.21 0.22 0.02 0.97

Moderator

Gender 0.19 0.11 -0.02 0.42 1.73 0.08

Interaction

Condition→ Gender -0.06 0.15 -0.37 0.25 -0.39 0.69

Covariate

Relationship length -0.02 0.04 -0.11 0.06 -0.48 0.62

SNS Use 0.10 0.04 0.01 0.18 2.43* 0.01

Partner-Comparison 0.28 0.02 0.23 0.33 10.75* 0.02

Note. These analyses treat relationship length and label, SNS use, as well as partner-comparison as a covariate. In the model,
condition is coded such that non-sexualized is 0 and sexualized is 1 and gender is coded such that female is 0 and male is 1.*p <
.05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

37
Table 11. Mediation Analyses Examining Sexual Closeness
Model 1 (n=400) Effect SE LLCI ULCI t p

Path

a: C→SO 0.16 0.09 -0.01 0.34 1.77 0.07

b: SO→ SC -0.01 0.05 -0.12 0.09 -0.27 0.78

c’: C→SC 0.01 0.10 -0.20 0.22 0.09 0.92

Covariate

SNS→ SO 0.12 0.04 0.03 0.22 2.63** 0.00

RLL→ SO -0.03 0.05 -0.14 0.06 -0.74 0.45

SNS→SC 0.14 0.05 0.02 0.25 2.46* 0.01

RLL→SC 0.31 0.06 0.19 0.42 5.09*** 0.00

SSC→SO 0.43 0.02 0.37 0.49 15.15*** 0.00

SSC→SC -0.05 0.04 -0.14 0.02 -1.41 0.15

Indirect effect

c: C→SO→SC -0.002 0.01 -0.02 0.02

Model 2 (n=400) Effect SE LLCI ULCI t p

Path

a: C→SO 0.13 0.11 -0.09 0.36 1.18 0.23

b: SO→ SFC -0.35 0.05 -0.45 -0.24 -6.40*** 0.00

c’: C→SFC -0.08 0.12 -0.33 0.16 -0.67 0.50

Covariate

SNS→ SO 0.12 0.04 0.03 0.22 2.63** 0.00

RLL→ SO -0.03 0.05 -0.14 0.06 -0.74 0.45

SNS→SFC 0.09 0.06 -0.04 0.22 1.37 0.17

RLL→SFC 0.31 0.07 0.16 0.45 4.31*** 0.00

SSC→SO 0.43 0.02 0.37 0.49 15.15*** 0.00

SSC→SFC -0.15 0.04 -0.25 -0.06 -3.19** 0.00

38
Table 11. Cont
Indirect effect

c: C→SO→SFC -0.03 0.02 -0.10 0.003

Note. These analyses treat relationship length and label, SNS use, as well as self-comparison as a covariate. Model 1 refers to the
analysis evaluating feelings of sexual connectedness. Model 2 refers to the analysis of evaluating self-consciousness about one’s
appearance during sex. In both models, condition is coded such that non-sexualized is 0 and sexualized is 1. “C” refers to
Condition, “SO” refers to self-objectification, “SNS” refers to SNS Use, “RLL” refers to relationship length and status, “SSC”
refers to self-comparison, “SC” refers to sexual connectedness, and “SFC” refers to self-consciousness about appearance. *p <
.05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Table 12. Mediation Analysis Examining Relationship Satisfaction and Closeness


Model 1 (n=400) Effect SE LLCI ULCI t p

Path

a: C→PO -0.005 0.08 -0.16 0.15 -0.07 0.94

b: PO→ RSC -0.05 0.08 -0.22 0.11 -0.63 0.52

c’: C→RSC 0.07 0.14 -0.19 0.35 0.56 0.57

Covariate

SNS→ PO 0.08 0.04 -0.001 0.16 1.93 0.05

RLL→ PO -0.03 0.04 -0.12 0.05 -0.69 0.48

SNS→RSC -0.007 0.07 -0.15 0.13 -0.09 0.92

RLL →RSC 0.91 0.08 0.75 1.07 11.36*** 0.00

PC→PO 0.28 0.02 0.23 0.33 10.76*** 0.00

PC→RSC -0.16 0.05 -0.27 -0.06 -3.21** 0.00

Indirect effect

c: C→PO→RSC 0.003 0.008 -0.01 0.01

Note. These analyses treat relationship length and label, SNS use, as well as partner-comparison as a covariate. In this model,
condition is coded such that non-sexualized is 0 and sexualized is 1. “C” refers to Condition, “PO” refers to partner-
objectification, “SNS” refers to SNS Use, “RLL” refers to relationship length and status, “PC” refers to partner comparison,
“RSC” refers to relationship satisfaction and closeness . *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

39
Table 13. Moderated Mediation Analyses Examining Sexual Closeness
Model 1 (n=394) Effect SE LLCI ULCI t p

Path

a: C→SO 0.21 0.13 -0.03 0.47 1.67 0.09

a2: G→ SO 0.06 0.13 -0.20 0.33 0.46 0.64

b: SO→ SC -0.01 0.05 -0.13 0.09 -0.27 0.78

c’: C→SC 0.01 0.10 -0.19 0.23 0.17 0.85

Covariate

SNS→ SO 0.13 0.05 0.03 0.23 2.74** 0.00

RLL→ SO -0.03 0.05 -0.13 0.07 -0.62 0.52

SNS→SC 0.13 0.05 0.02 0.25 2.37* 0.01

RLL→SC 0.31 0.06 0.19 0.43 5.08*** 0.00

SSC→SO 0.43 0.02 0.37 0.49 14.84*** 0.00

SSC→SC -0.05 0.04 -0.14 0.02 -1.39 0.16

Interaction effect

c: C x G -0.16 0.18 -0.53 0.20 -0.87 0.38

Indirect effect

c: C→SO→SC -0.003 0.01 -0.03 0.02

0.009 -0.01 0.02


-0.0009

Moderated
mediation

0.002 0.01 -0.02 0.04

Model 2 (n=394) Effect SE LLCI ULCI t p

Path

a: C→SO 0.21 0.13 -0.03 0.47 1.67 0.09

a2: G→ SO 0.06 0.13 -0.20 0.33 0.46 0.64

b: SO→ SFC -0.23 0.06 -0.37 -0.10 -3.47*** 0.00

c’: C→SFC -0.13 0.12 -0.38 0.11 -1.04 0.29

40
Covariate

SNS→ SO 0.13 0.05 0.03 0.23 2.74** 0.00

RLL→ SO -0.03 0.05 -0.13 0.07 -0.62 0.52

SNS→SFC 0.10 0.06 -0.02 0.23 1.54 0.12

RLL→SFC 0.31 0.07 0.17 0.45 4.44*** 0.00

SSC→SO 0.43 0.02 0.37 0.49 14.84*** 0.00

SSC→SFC -0.14 0.04 -0.24 -0.05 -3.02** 0.00

Interaction effect

c: C x G -0.16 0.18 -0.53 0.20 -0.87 0.38

Indirect effect

c: C→SO→SFC -0.05 0.03 -0.14 0.008

-0.01 0.03 -0.09 0.04

Moderated
mediation

0.03 0.04 -0.05 0.14

Note. These analyses treat relationship length and label,SNS use, as well as self-comparison as a covariate. Model 1 refers to the
analysis evaluating feelings of sexual connectedness. Model 2 refers to the analysis of evaluating self-consciousness about one’s
appearance during sex. In both models, condition is coded such that non-sexualized is 0 and sexualized is 1 and gender is coded
such that female is 0 and male is 1. “C” refers to Condition, “SO” refers to self-objectification, “G” refers to gender, “SNS” refers
to SNS Use, “RLL” refers to relationship length and status, “SSC” refers to self-comparison, “SC” refers to sexual connectedness,
and “SFC” refers to self-consciousness about appearance. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

41
Table 14 Moderated Mediation Analyses Examining Relationship Satisfaction and Closeness
Model 1 (n=394) Effect SE LLCI ULCI t p

Path

a: C→PO 0.003 0.11 -0.21 0.22 0.02 0.97

a2: G→ PO 0.19 0.11 -0.02 0.42 1.73 0.08

b: PO→ RSC -0.06 0.09 -0.23 0.11 -0.68 0.49

c’: C→RSC 0.10 0.14 -0.17 0.37 0.70 0.47

Covariate

SNS→ PO 0.10 0.04 0.01 0.18 2.43* 0.01

RLL→ PO -0.02 0.04 -0.11 0.06 -0.48 0.62

SNS→RSC -0.01 0.07 -0.16 0.13 -0.19 0.84

RLL→RSC 0.91 0.08 0.75 1.07 11.29*** 0.00

PC→PO 0.28 0.02 0.23 0.33 10.75*** 0.00

PC→RSC -0.16 0.05 -0.27 -0.06 -3.14** 0.00

Interaction effect

c: C x G -0.06 0.15 -0.37 0.25 -0.39 0.69

Indirect effect

c: C→PO→RSC -0.0002 0.01 -0.02 0.02

0.01 -0.02 0.03


0.003

Moderated
mediation

0.003 0.01 -0.03 0.04

Note. These analyses treat relationship length and label, SNS use, as well as partner-comparison as a covariate. In this model,
condition is coded such that non-sexualized is 0 and sexualized is 1 and gender is coded such that female is 0 and male is 1. “C”
refers to Condition, “G” refers to gender, “PO” refers to partner-objectification, “PC” refers to partner-comparison, “SNS” refers
to SNS Use, “RLL” refers to relationship length and status, “RSC” refers to relationship satisfaction and closeness . *p < .05, **p
< .01, ***p < .001.

42
DISCUSSION

Sexually objectifying content in media has evolved to encompass social media

applications, where the sexualization of the female body has become a popularized trend

observed by mass audiences online (Drenten et al., 2020). The present study aimed to establish a

causal relationship between exposure to sexualized social media content and increases in self and

partner objectification as it relates to the outcomes of sexual closeness and relationship

satisfaction and closeness within a population scarcely studied within these contexts: young

people in romantic relationships.

Hypotheses and Anticipated Effects

The results of the current study reveal that the relationship amongst the variables of

interest are far more dynamic than originally hypothesized, as several pathways deviate from the

hypothesized effects. For one, the relationship between self and partner objectification and

exposure to sexualized social media was not statistically significant across all models, which

indicates that exposure to sexualized social media is not the driving force of self and partner

objectification effects in the current study. However, the lack of relationship between the

manipulation (e.g., social media condition) and objectification may be the result of the short

length (e.g., roughly three minutes) and environment (e.g., remote and unmonitored online) in

which the manipulation took place. Prior research examining objectification and sexualized

media has found significant findings for self-objectification when examining consumption of

sexually objectifying media over sustained periods of time (Zurbriggen et al., 2011) and when

asking participants to actively interact with the sexualized media content (Fox et al., 2015). Due

43
to the manipulation taking place remotely, it cannot be verified how consistently or closely

participants watched or engaged with the virtual content (e.g., checking their phone,

communicating with another person in the room). Although participants were not permitted to

proceed through the online survey until the entire video manipulation had been completed, this

does not ensure active participant engagement or sustained attention with the content. In

addition, this does not account for a participant's overall consumption of media over time. Prior

research has proposed that media may impact objectification through the scripts it provides to its

audiences that a woman is a sexual object (Wright et al., 2017). As such, greater and repeated

exposure to scripts from media may have a greater impact on participant engagement in

objectification as compared to the brief clips from the current manipulation. Thus, the lack of

effects for condition may be the result of the brevity of the exposure to the media content as well

as a lack of participant engagement and attention toward the online environment and content.

Next, when examining the main effects of gender, partner-objectification but not self-

objectification was significantly related to participant gender, such that male participants

engaged in greater partner-objectification than female participants. The direction of self-

objectification as it relates to gender are in the direction researchers would anticipate, however it

was not significant. This illuminates an inconsistent relationship amongst the variables when

detecting main effects. Further, when examining the predicted interaction effects of the current

study, no significant results emerge. Partner-objectification has been scarcely studied within

these contexts, however, these results collectively deviate from prior research that has

continually established a significant relationship between rates of self-objectification in women

after consumption or exposure to sexually objectifying media (Aubrey, 2007; Fardouly et al.,

2015; Linder & Daniels, 2018). The lack of significant results may again be a consequence of the

44
manipulation being too brief as well as not engaging nor sustaining participant attention and as a

result not being powerful enough to consistently elicit effects within participants.

Additionally, the path of self-objectification to feelings of sexual connectedness was

found to be not significant which is in direct contrast to the path examining self-objectification to

feelings of self-consciousness about one’s appearance during sex. However, recall the dependent

variable of sexual closeness for the current study was meant to be comprised of both of these

items as one scale of sexual closeness, but due to poor reliability they were evaluated

independently. Therefore, the nonsignificant results of self-objectification as it relates to the item

of feelings of sexual connectedness is not entirely surprising as the variable of sexual

connectedness for the current study was exploratory, constructed by the PI, and was originally

intended to be a scale comprised of two items. Therefore, when examining the single item

assessing feelings of sexual connectedness for the current study, it may not have been sensitive

nor varied enough to assess the construct of interest, which would support similar nonsignificant

findings within this domain when evaluating feelings of sexual satisfaction as a broad one-item

measure (Zurbriggen et al., 2011).

With regards to the second dimension of sexual closeness (e.g., how self-conscious would

you feel if you were to imagine having sex with your partner right now), some of the

hypothesized effects did in fact emerge. For one, greater self-objectification is related to greater

feelings of self-consciousness about one’s appearance when imagining having sex with a

romantic partner. These results support the position that to the extent an individual engages in

appearance monitoring, it creates a barrier to the experience of intimacy during sex (Fredrickson

& Roberts, 1997) and builds on prior research demonstrating appearance anxiety negatively

impacts sexual well-being and positive relates to an inability to connect with the surrounding

45
environment (Claudat & Warren, 2014; Vencill et al., 2015). In particular, the current results

highlight the inability of those engaging in self-objectification to connect with one’s romantic

partner during sex, implicating self-objectification undermines aspects of sexual relations for

young couples, a factor that assists in the formation of enduring bonds for couples (Birnbaum &

Reis, 2019).

Moreover, the results of the current study also support the hypothesized effect of partner-

objectification negatively impacting feelings of relationship satisfaction and closeness.

Collectively, these results demonstrate that when a partner is valued for their external attributes it

results in viewing one’s partner more like an object than a human-being (e.g., partner-

objectification) and as a result of this objectifying perspective, feelings of relationship

satisfaction and closeness towards one’s partner are compromised. Specifically, the current study

finds that men in particular are more likely to engage in partner-objectification and, as a result,

feelings of relationship satisfaction and closeness to their female partner diminishes. This

supports an empirical line of research that when a partner places value upon a woman’s

appearance it severely undermines the ability to cultivate intimate emotional connections (Mahar

et al., 2020; Ramsey et al., 2017).

Individuating Factors of Social Media Use and Relationship Status/Length

Furthermore, when evaluating the covariates of the current study, the results speak to the

impact individuating information has upon young adults in romantic relationships. For example,

contrary to the hypotheses, for individuals who are in, or have been in, a romantic relationship

with a member of the opposite sex, exposure to sexualized content from social media does not

result in increased rates of self and partner objectification. Rather, social media use, a covariate

in the current study, predicted greater self-objectification and partner-objectification. In addition,

46
greater social media use was also related to feeling greater sexual connectedness to a romantic

partner, a finding which does not align with the results observed when examining social media

use to feeling self-conscious about one’s appearance. This indicates that the two items which

were originally intended to comprise one measure of sexual closeness may be tapping into

different constructs, and may also partially explain why the two items share low reliability with

one another. Additionally, it is possible the sexual connectedness item asks participants too

directly about their experiences of connected sex with their partner, and as a result, may lead to

bias when responding as young individuals may not be open to expressing having non-connected

(e.g., bad) sex with their partner nor may they have enough sexual experience to identify what

sexual connectedness feels like for them. As a whole, the pattern of results continues to suggest

that exposure to brief sexualized video clips from social media may not be powerful enough to

impact individuals; rather, it is perhaps extended consumption and use of social media which

may be the predominant factor impacting self and partner objectification and related outcomes.

Further, couples who indicate that they have been together for a long period of time and

define their relationship in formal terms (e.g., seriously dating, married) are less likely to be

impacted by detrimental outcomes. Young adults who were high in relationship length/status

were less self-conscious about their appearance when imagining sex with a partner, were higher

in terms of feeling relationship satisfaction and closeness to one’s partner, and engaged in less

partner-objectification. Indicating that relationships that are not newly formed are more robust to

detrimental outcomes.

Self and Partner Objectification and Self and Partner Comparison

To support the position that the effects of self and partner objectification are unique to the

current study, the variables of self and partner comparison were entered into the model as a

47
covariate to control for the effects of self and partner comparison, respectively. Results largely

remain unchanged when entering the variables of self and partner comparison into the models as

a covariate. For instance, when entering self-comparison into the models with self-

objectification, the effects of self-objectification upon the variables of interest remain

unchanged. However, the effects of partner-objectification are somewhat impacted by partner-

comparison. For instance, upon entering partner-comparison into the model the effects of

partner-objectification on relationship satisfaction and closeness becomes nonsignificant. In

addition, the effect of gender on partner-objectification also becomes nonsignificant. These

results indicate that some of the relationships observed when entering partner-objectification into

a model are also capable of being explained by partner-comparison. To some degree, this is

unsurprising as the measure of partner-objectification for the current study demonstrated low

reliability. Therefore, the current measure is not wholly reliable in measuring the construct of

interest and therefore may not entirely capture the phenomena and therefore reduce its

robustness. In addition, the variables of partner-objectification and partner comparison are

significantly related, indicating they share variance, which may compromise the reliability of

relationships when both are entered in a model together.

Collectively, the current study remains one of the few aimed at investigating a causal

relationship amongst factors and the detrimental impact objectification has on romantic

relationships and feelings of closeness and satisfaction amongst young couples. Although the

results of the current study do not entirely support the proposed hypotheses, the results of the

current study illuminate the valuable context of romantic relationships afford to understand

objectification. In addition, it may be seen that there is growing need for additional research in

this area to further understand how couples may be negatively impacted by the perspective that is

48
taken when objectification occurs and how precisely social media may contribute to eliciting this

perspective in individuals. Specifically, this research suggests that exposure to brief sexualized

clips from social media may not impact rates of self and partner objectification. Rather, the

current study implicates the variable of social media use as a larger factor of concern, however

this implication is dependent upon certifying that participants fully watched and engaged with

the video manipulation, which is something that cannot be fully concluded. Ultimately,

understanding how these factors develop and relate to individuals in romantic relationships is

crucial as objectifying oneself and one’s partner is associated with risks greater than the ones

assessed in the current study. For instance, self and partner objectification have been associated

with severely detrimental outcomes such as low self-esteem, depression, sexual assault,

harassment, and coercion (Berberick, 2010; Saez et al., 2019). Thus, the current study sets the

stage for beginning to understand how social media informs couples to view both their own and

their partner’s body and how objectification may impede the positive development of romantic

relationships for young couples.

Limitations and Future Directions

The current study is not without its limitations. First, the sample largely consisted of

college-aged students as participants who were recruited through a university research portal,

limiting the generalizability of the results. Second, the current research examined the perspective

of one relationship partner in a dyad, as recruitment was not centered on examining both partners

from a relationship simultaneously. Again, this limits the generalizability of the results as it does

not fully encompass the perspective of both relationship partners as it relates to the current

factors and their relationship. Third, the current research examined individuals who were in or

had been in a romantic relationship with a member of the opposite sex. Therefore, the current

49
results largely speak to heterosexual relationships which limits the generalization of the current

findings as it relates to considering non-heterosexual relationships and populations. In addition, it

can be observed that the variables of partner-objectification and the proposed 2-item measure of

sexual closeness showed low reliability as compared to a well-validated measure. However,

well-developed measures examining partner-objectification and sexual closeness currently

remain undeveloped. Thus, the current study may be considered a substantial step forward in

acknowledging and developing measures to assess the constructs of partner-objectification and

sexual closeness. Further, the researchers cannot verify that participants watched the video

manipulation with sustained attention, although precautionary measures were taken (e.g., the

video must be completed before moving forward in the survey and the use of an attention check

question pertaining to the social media platform used). This ultimately limits the current study’s

ability to potentially detect effects as it relates to one of the key variables of interest, exposure to

sexualized social media clips.

Future research may aim first aim to parse out the unique relationship between social

media use, exposure to sexualized social media, and objectification. Media content has

previously been theorized to induce feelings of self and partner objectification through the scripts

it provides to users. For example, it provides the script that a woman is an object to be used for

sexual pleasure and valued for her appearance, due to the way an icon in media is repeatedly

portrayed (Wright et al., 2017). Therefore, perhaps greater social media use in the current study

is more strongly related to self and partner objectification due to more frequent and repeated

exposure to the script that a woman is a sexualized object. Researchers should thus strive to

understand how various aspects of media such as exposure, engagement, length of time, and

content impact rates of self and partner objectification. It may be more critical that the findings

50
of previous research be directly replicated, such as the findings of Zurbriggen et al. (2011) which

points to sustained consumption of sexualized media content and Fox et al. (2015) which points

to active engagement with sexualized media content, to solidify the understanding of how the

various aspects of media impact objectification before progressing into research examining

nuanced extensions of media.

Conclusion

The current study ultimately contributes to the literature examining the impact of

sexualized media content on individuals and further illuminates the viable context media and

romantic relationships afford for evaluating objectification. The findings of this study extend

beyond previous research in order to encompass how facets of social media impact romantic

couples, as well as how objectification may impact feelings of sexual closeness and relationship

satisfaction and closeness for young adults in romantic relationships. Collectively, the current

study emphasizes the need to further examine facets of media and the domain of social media in

the current context and to continually develop and establish empirical models that are more

inclusive of examining romantic relationships, objectification, and gender.

51
REFERENCES

Aron, A., Melinat, E., Aron, E. N., Vallone, R. D., & Bator, R. J. (1997). The experimental

generation of interpersonal closeness: A procedure and some preliminary findings.

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23(4), 363-377.

Aubrey, J. S. (2007). The impact of sexually objectifying media exposure on negative body

emotions and sexual self-perceptions: Investigating the mediating role of body self-

consciousness. Mass Communications and Society, 10, 1-23.

Aubrey, J. S., & Gerding, A. (2015). The cognitive tax of self-objectification. Journal of Media

Psychology, 27(1), 22-32.

Auxier, B., & Anderson, M. (2021). Social media use in 2021. Pew Research Center.

Berberick, S. N. (2010). The objectification of women in mass media: Female self-image in

misogynist culture. The New York Sociologist, 5(2), 1-15.

Butkowski, C. P., Dixon, T. L., & Weeks, K. (2019). Body surveillance on Instagram: examining

the role of selfie feedback investment in young adult women’s body image concerns. Sex

Roles, 81(5), 385-397.

Claudat, K., & Warren, C. S. (2014). Self-objectification, body self-consciousness during sexual

activities, and sexual satisfaction in college women. Body Image, 11(4), 509-515.

Dakanalis, A., Riva, G., Timko, A. C., Volpato, C., Clerici, M., & Zanetti, A. M. (2013).The

association between body image issues and women's sexual functioning through the lens

of the objectification theory. European Psychiatry, 28(S1), 1-15.

52
Drenten, J., Gurrieri, L., & Tyler, M. (2020). Sexualized labour in digital culture: Instagram

influencers, porn chic and the monetization of attention. Gender, Work & Organization,

27(1), 41-66.

Fardouly, J., Diedrichs, P. C., Vartanian, L. R., & Halliwell, E. (2015). The mediating role of

appearance comparisons in the relationship between media usage and self-objectification

in young women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 39(4), 447-457.

Fardouly, J., Willburger, B. K., & Vartanian, L. R. (2018). Instagram use and young women’s

body image concerns and self-objectification: Testing mediational pathways. New Media

& Society, 20(4), 1380-1395.

Feltman, C. E., & Szymanski, D. M. (2018). Instagram use and self-objectification: The roles of

internalization, comparison, appearance commentary, and feminism. Sex Roles, 78(5),

311-324.

Fox, J., Ralston, R. A., Cooper, C. K., & Jones, K. A. (2015). Sexualized avatars lead to

women’s self-objectification and acceptance of rape myths. Psychology of Women

Quarterly, 39(3), 349-362.

Fredrickson, B. L., & Roberts, T. A. (1997). Objectification theory: Toward understanding

women’s lived experiences and mental health risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21,

173–206.

Galdi, S., Maass, A., & Cadinu, M. (2014). Objectifying media: Their effect on gender role

norms and sexual harassment of women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 38(3), 398-

413.

Harper, B., & Tiggemann, M. (2008). The effect of thin ideal media images on women’s self-

objectification, mood, and body image. Sex Roles, 58(9), 649-657.

53
Hatton, E., & Trautner, M. N. (2011). Equal opportunity objectification? The sexualization of

men and women on the cover of Rolling Stone. Sexuality & Culture, 15(3), 256-278.

Hayes, A. F. (2012). PROCESS: A versatile computational tool for observed variable mediation,

moderation, and conditional process modeling.

Heflick, N. A., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2009). Objectifying Sarah Palin: Evidence that

objectification causes women to be perceived as less competent and less fully human.

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(3), 598-601.

Heflick, N. A., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2014). Seeing eye to body: The literal objectification of

women. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(3), 225-229.

Heflick, N. A., Goldenberg, J. L., Cooper, D. P., & Puvia, E. (2011). From women to objects:

Appearance focus, target gender, and perceptions of warmth, morality and competence.

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(3), 572-581.

Karsay, K., Knoll, J., & Matthes, J. (2018). Sexualizing media use and self-objectification: A

meta-analysis. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 42(1), 9-28.

Khattab, M. (2019). Synching and performing: body (re)-presentation in the short video app

TikTok. WiderScreen, 21(1-2), 1-16.

Linder, J. R., & Daniels, E. A. (2018). Sexy vs. sporty: The effects of viewing media images of

athletes on self-objectification in college students. Sex Roles, 78(1-2), 27-39.

Linz, D. G., Donnerstein, E., & Penrod, S. (1988). Effects of long-term exposure to violent and

sexually degrading depictions of women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

55(5), 758-766.

54
Loughnan, S., Pina, A., Vasquez, E. A., & Puvia, E. (2013). Sexual objectification increases rape

victim blame and decreases perceived suffering. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 37(4),

455-461.

Lustig, K. B. (2012). Objectification theory and sexual health among women. ProQuest

Dissertations Publishing.

Mahar, E. A., Webster, G. D., & Markey, P. M. (2020). Partner–objectification in romantic

relationships: A dyadic approach. Personal Relationships, 27(1), 4-26.

McKinley, N. M., & Hyde, J. S. (1996). The objectified body consciousness scale: Development

and validation. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 20(2), 181-215.

Moreno, M. A., Swanson, M. J., Royer, H., & Roberts, L. J. (2011). Sexpectations: Male college

students’ views about displayed sexual references on females’ social networking web

sites. Journal of Pediatric and Adolescent Gynecology, 24(2), 85-89.

Overstreet, N. M., Quinn, D. M., & Marsh, K. L. (2015). Objectification in virtual romantic

contexts: Perceived discrepancies between self and partner ideals differentially affect

body consciousness in women and men. Sex Roles, 73(9), 442-452.

Ramsey, L. R., & Horan, A. L. (2018). Picture this: Women's self-sexualization in photos on

social media. Personality and Individual Differences, 133, 85-90.

Ramsey, L. R., & Hoyt, T. (2015). The object of desire: How being objectified creates sexual

pressure for women in heterosexual relationships. Psychology of Women Quarterly,

39(2), 151-170.

Ramsey, L. R., Marotta, J. A., & Hoyt, T. (2017). Sexualized, objectified, but not satisfied:

Enjoying sexualization relates to lower relationship satisfaction through perceived

partner-objectification. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 34(2), 258-278.

55
Robbins, A. R., & Reissing, E. D. (2018). Appearance dissatisfaction, body appreciation, and

sexual health in women across adulthood. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 47(3), 703-714.

Rollero, C. (2013). Men and women facing objectification: The effects of media models on well-

being, self-esteem and ambivalent sexism. Revista de Psicología Social, 28(3), 373-382.

Rusbult, C. E., Martz, J. M., & Agnew, C. R. (1998). The investment model scale: Measuring

commitment level, satisfaction level, quality of alternatives, and investment size.

Personal Relationships, 5(4), 357-382.

Sáez, G., Riemer, A. R., Brock, R. L., & Gervais, S. J. (2019). Objectification in heterosexual

romantic relationships: Examining relationship satisfaction of female objectification

recipients and male objectifying perpetrators. Sex Roles, 81(5), 370-384.

Seabrook, R. C., Ward, L. M., & Giaccardi, S. (2019). Less than human? Media use,

objectification of women, and men’s acceptance of sexual aggression. Psychology of

Violence, 9(5), 536-548.

Shinne, E. D. (2017). The relationship between sexual objectification and self-objectification,

adult romantic attachment, and relationship satisfaction. Proquest Dissertations

Publishing.

Sternberg, R. J. (1986). A triangular theory of love. Psychological Review, 93(2), 119-126.

Trekels, J., Ward, L. M., & Eggermont, S. (2018). I “like” the way you look: How appearance-

focused and overall Facebook use contribute to adolescents' self-sexualization.

Computers in Human Behavior, 81, 198-208.

56
Vandenbosch, L., & Eggermont, S. (2012). Understanding sexual objectification: A

comprehensive approach toward media exposure and girls' internalization of beauty

ideals, self-objectification, and body surveillance. Journal of Communication, 62(5), 869-

887.

Vencill, J. A., Tebbe, E. A., & Garos, S. (2015). It’s not the size of the boat or the motion of the

ocean: The role of self-objectification, appearance anxiety, and depression in female

sexual functioning. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 39(4), 471–483.

Ward, L. M. (2016). Media and sexualization: State of empirical research, 1995–2015. The

Journal of Sex Research, 53(4-5), 560-577.

Ward, L. M., Merriwether, A., & Caruthers, A. (2006). Breasts are for men: Media, masculinity

ideologies, and men’s beliefs about women’s bodies. Sex Roles, 55(10), 703-714.

Ward, L. M., Seabrook, R. C., Manago, A., & Reed, L. (2016). Contributions of diverse media to

self-sexualization among undergraduate women and men. Sex Roles, 74(1), 12-23.

Wright, P. J., & Tokunaga, R. S. (2015). Activating the centerfold syndrome: Recency of

exposure, sexual explicitness, past exposure to objectifying media. Communication

Research, 42(6), 864–897.

Wright, P. J., & Tokunaga, R. S. (2016). Men’s objectifying media consumption, objectification

of women, and attitudes supportive of violence against women. Archives of Sexual

Behavior, 45(4), 955-964.

Wright, P. J., Tokunaga, R. S., Kraus, A., & Klann, E. (2017). Pornography consumption and

satisfaction: A meta-analysis. Human Communication Research, 43(3), 315–343.

57
Zurbriggen, E. L., Ramsey, L. R., & Jaworski, B. K. (2011). Self-and partner-objectification in

romantic relationships: Associations with media consumption and relationship

satisfaction. Sex Roles, 64(7-8), 449-46.

58
APPENDIX A: INTRODUCTORY SCRIPT

“We are interested in gathering some feedback about the type of media adults currently enjoy

watching both independently and with their romantic partners. We are going to present you a

series of different media clips from the social media site TikTok and would like you to watch the

brief videos for the next few minutes. Further, we are part of a larger study overall that is

interested in learning about adult relationship experiences, therefore general relationship

questions will also be included.”

59
APPENDIX B: MEASURES

Sexual Closeness

For the following questions we'd like you to consider what it means to be SEXUALLY

CONNECTED to your partner. Being sexually connected to one’s romantic partner can be

defined as times when you feel wholly intertwined and fully lost in the being of your partner, as

though you do not know where they end and you begin. It is an intimate experience where you

can share and enjoy your body in its most raw form with another. You may feel free with little on

your mind in the current moment beyond the sensation of touch you are receiving from your

romantic partner.

Item 1: Sexual Connectedness

How much do you think you would experience sexual connectedness, as previously defined

above, if you were to have sex with your romantic partner right now?

1. I would definitely feel sexually connected to my partner if we had sex right now

2. I would likely feel sexually connected to my partner if we had sex right now

3. I might feel sexually connected to my partner if we had sex right now

4. I don’t think I would feel sexually connected to my partner if we had sex right now

5. I definitely would not feel sexually connected to my partner if we had sex right now.

*reverse scored such that 5 = 1; 1 = 5. High scores are indicative of high sexual

connectedness.

Item 2: Self-consciousness about appearance

60
People feel self-conscious about their body and how they look to their partner during sex,

especially as their romantic partner repeatedly looks at different parts of their body during sex.

How self-conscious would be if you were to have sex with your partner right now?

1. I would likely feel self-conscious if I had sex with my romantic partner right now

2. I might feel self-conscious if I had sex with my romantic partner right now

3. I don’t think I would feel self-conscious if I had sex with my romantic partner right

now

4. I definitely would not feel self-conscious if I had sex with my romantic partner right

now

Relationship Length

1. How long have you been with your current romantic partner?
a. Less than one month
b. Six months or less
c. Less than one year
d. One year
e. Five years
f. Ten years
g. Greater than ten years

Relationship Label

2. How would you define your current relationship with your romantic partner?

a. Friends with benefits


b. Newly dating, taking the relationship casually and seeing where it goes
c. Seriously dating, taking the relationship with the intent of making long-term
commitments
d. Engaged
e. Married

61
Demographics

1. Please select the option that best describes your gender.


a. Man
b. Woman
c. Gender neutral

2. What is your age?

3. How would you describe your race/ethnicity? Please select the one best descriptor, or use
the “Biracial/Multiracial” option to specify further.

a. African. African American/Black


b. American Indian/Native American
c. Arab American/Middle Eastern
d. Asian/Asian American
e. Hispanic/Latina/o American
f. Pacific Islander
g. White/European American/ Caucasian
h. Biracial/Multiracial (please specify)
i. Other (please specify)
4. What is your academic year?
a. Freshman
b. Sophomore
c. Junior
d. Senior

62
SNS use

1. Which of the following social media apps do you have an account on?

a. Instagram

b. Snapchat

c. TikTok

d. Facebook

e. Twitter

f. Reddit

g. Other

2. How frequently do you use social media?

a. Never

b. Yearly

c. Monthly

d. Weekly

e. Multiple times a week

f. Daily

g. Multiple times a day

3. On average, each time you visit a social media application how long do you spend on it?

a. 15 minutes or less

b. 15-30 minutes

c. 0.5-1 hour

d. 1 to 2 hours

e. 2 to 3 hours

63
f. 3 to 4 hours

g. More than 4 hours

4. How frequently do you send private or direct messages (DM) to other users on social

media?

a. Never

b. Yearly

c. Monthly

d. Weekly

e. Multiple times a week

f. Daily

g. Multiple times a day

5. How frequently do you update your status/story on social media?

a. Never

b. Yearly

c. Monthly

d. Weekly

e. Multiple times a week

f. Daily

g. Multiple times a day

6. How frequently do you post on social media?

a. Never

b. Yearly

c. Monthly

64
d. Weekly

e. Multiple times a week

f. Daily

g. Multiple times a day

7. Is the content you viewed today typical of what you view on social media?

a. Yes

b. Somewhat

c. No

Attention check

1. What social media site did the videos you watched today come from?

a. Instagram

b. TikTok

c. Snapchat

d. YouTube

65

You might also like