You are on page 1of 27

ADHD Atten Def Hyp Disord

DOI 10.1007/s12402-014-0137-y

REVIEW ARTICLE

Sons and daughters beyond your control: episodes


in the prehistory of the attention deficit/hyperactivity syndrome
Paul Bernard Foley

Received: 5 February 2014 / Accepted: 24 March 2014


 Springer-Verlag Wien 2014

Abstract Hyperactive and inattentive children have been the interpretation of behavior. Further, the controversial
discussed in both the pedagogic and medical literature nature of the concept of ‘ADHD’ itself means that any
since the nineteenth century, and many controversies history will be viewed by both its proponents and oppo-
associated with attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder nents as providing evidence for their respective viewpoints.
(ADHD) have been repeatedly analyzed in different con- This brief overview will initially explore how problems
texts. The ‘prehistory’ of the ADHD concept—that is, up to of attention and hyperactivity were described prior to the
the definition of ADHD in DSM-III and of the corre- twentieth century. Historical accounts of difficult are
sponding ‘hyperkinetic disorder’ in ICD-9—is outlined, plentiful, but we are not interested here in simply rebellious
with an emphasis on the literature not previously discussed or naughty children, for instance, nor in those with profound
in English language reviews of the subject. intellectual problems or whose excessive motility is deter-
mined by psychosis; our review will be limited to children
Keywords Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder  of fairly normal intelligence who attracted adult attention
History of psychiatry  Classification of behavioral because of problems of attention and excessive voluntary
disorders motor activity. Further, the evolution of the ADHD concept
since 1902 has been explored many times (Helmerichs
2002; Sandberg and Barton 2002; Barkley 2006; Lange
Introduction et al. 2010; Taylor 2011; Baumeister et al. 2012; Warnke
and Riederer 2013). While the milestones installed by these
Reports of unruly and intransigent children have been previous reviews will not be ignored, a broader context for
transmitted from as long ago as the eighth century BC, this history will be provided by focusing upon sources lesser
when the author of Deuteronomy advised parents troubled known, in particular, to Anglophone readers. With regard to
by a stubborn and unruly son to denounce him to the city the German literature, this means, for example, considering
fathers, so that he might be stoned (21: 18–21). But iden- not only the psychiatric literature, but also the pedagogic
tifying accounts of children presenting what might now be journals in which the problem of inattentive, restless chil-
interpreted as attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder dren were discussed; the behavioral problems in post-
(ADHD) is more complicated, primarily because retro- encephalitis lethargica children during the 1920s will be
spective diagnosis is no more uncertain than when based on discussed, as will the hyperactivity disorder described by
Kramer and Pollnow at about the same time. The most
prominent proponents of the French ‘unstable child’ con-
P. B. Foley
Unit for History and Philosophy of Science, cept will also be discussed, as this model of behavior,
University of Sydney NSW 2006, Australia developed since the beginning of the twentieth century in
parallel with that of the ‘hyperactive child’ model, is even
P. B. Foley (&)
less familiar to English-speaking readers.
Neuroscience Research Australia,
Randwick (Sydney) NSW 2031, Australia A number of early descriptions of ‘ADHD’ have been
e-mail: p.foley@neura.edu.au discussed recently (Kast and Altschuler 2008; Bonazza

123
P. B. Foley

et al. 2011; Barkley and Peters 2012), but these generally It is a Contradiction to the natural State of Childhood
require significant willful interpretation in order to fit the for them to fix their fleeting Thoughts. Whether this
ADHD mold. The most prominent example in this respect is be owing to the Temper of their Brains, or the
undoubtedly the depiction by Frankfurt psychiatrist Hein- Quickness or Instability of their animal Spirits, over
rich Hoffmann (1809–1894) of the Zappelphilipp (‘Fidgety which the Mind has not yet got a full Command; this
Philip’) in his illustrated children’s book Struwwelpeter, is visible, that it is a Pain to Children to keep their
often regarded as an early depiction of ADHD (Thome and Thoughts steady to any thing (Sect. 167).
Jacobs 2004; Lange et al. 2010). Freiburg medical historian
No pedagogic success can be attained against this inclina-
and pediatrician Eduard Seidler (2004) has discussed in
tion with punishment or rebuke: Calmness is instead
detail why this is inappropriate, and it suffices here to note
required on the part of teacher. Nearly two hundred years
that rocking back and forth on a chair, sometimes with
later, the Toronto philosopher Frederick Tracy commented
catastrophic consequences, is normal for young boys, as I
in a similar vein:
know from personal experience as a father (another of
Hoffmann’s characters, the distracted ‘Johnny-Stare-in-the- The child’s attention is comparatively weak and
Air,’ is similarly familiar). Hoffmann’s book, cautionary intermittent. He cannot attend to the unimpressive,
tales composed for the amusement and mild admonition of the stimulus must be strong, must be on the motor
children, is no more a depiction of pathologic behavior than side, and must be frequently renewed. His attention is
Max und Moritz (1865) or the often gruesome tales col- very easy to obtain, but very hard to retain (Tracy
lected by the brothers Grimm from earlier in the nineteenth 1896, p. 118).
century. Philipp may even contain an autobiographical
A key feature of their discussions published between these
element: Hoffmann described himself as a child as dis-
two statements—primarily the subject not of medical
tracted, forgetful, flighty, as I was, nothing stuck with me,
literature, but of pedagogic journals and handbooks—was
everything was dispersed (Hoffmann 1926, p. 26).
that there was little perception of problem children, but
rather of a spectrum of distractibility in children, and
variation in the competence of teachers respecting these
The nineteenth century: Restlessness as an obstacle differences. One of the basic goals of education was, in
to education fact, to gradually channel and focus the quicksilver minds
of children: The distracted child was neither defective nor
It was not until the mid-nineteenth century that attention bad, simply untrained. Philosophical and medical thought
deficits and hyperactivity in children attracted a significant concerning consciousness and attention also contributed to
degree of published comment. This is no coincidence: It increased interest in disorders of these faculties during this
was at this time that evolution of the modern concept of period.
childhood gathered pace, as did recognition of the social The focus of educational discussions in Germany until
and political importance of compulsory schooling (not the early twentieth century generally concerned attention,
introduced in France and the United Kingdom until the with motor restlessness regarded more as its corollary than
final quarter of the century), both major evidence of social a separate feature. Zappeligkeit (‘fidgitiness’) was the term
progress. At the same time, socioeconomic and political usually applied to children presumed consumed by inner
changes associated with industrialization encouraged ideals restlessness; it was regarded as both normal and necessary
of orderliness, self-control and adaptation to the changing in infants, but inappropriate in older children, obliged to
political and economic circumstances that were necessarily self-control (Siegert 1910). It could be presented as
also inculcated in middle-class children, in particular, and uncontrolled motor activity (including chorea) or as Vie-
difficulties by children in accommodating these values lgeschäftigkeit: hectic, uncoordinated activity, where much
were increasingly regarded as problematic. as initiated, but little brought to a satisfactory conclusion
One constant in the concern regarding hyperactivity and (Siegert 1909).
inattention in children has, in fact, been their impact on Krankhafte Unruhe des Kindes (‘pathological restless-
learning by otherwise intellectually capable pupils. At the ness in children’) could also be observed in restless, hys-
same time, the natural ‘volatility’ of childhood attention terical, or neurasthenic children, as well as in more serious
had long been accepted both as prerequisite and hindrance psychiatric disorders. The movements of these children are
to their rapid capacity for learning, as noted by no less than restless and more hurried than in the healthy, but by no
the great English philosopher John Locke in Some thoughts means clumsy. They make always fiddling with objects
concerning education (included from the 4th enlarged unnecessarily, attacking everything, flicking through their
edition of 1699): books, opening drawers, playing with the pencil box,

123
Sons and daughters beyond your control

twisting scraps of paper, fumbling with their hair, etc. abandoned after a short time, no instruction is fol-
Striking remains the great tendency to fatigue and lowed without forgetting something. Critical parents
depression (Bergmann 1917). The psychomotor irritation and teachers, for whom such children with their
exhibited by these children was seen as lying on a con- careless, their hastily executed, carefree, playful
tinuum that ranged from that in healthy children to invol- behavior, with their restless sitting and demeanor, are
untary motor tics and even tremor (see also Fuchs 1896; a constant source of concern and sorrow, sometimes
Ziehen 1909). describe its essence as flightiness, sometimes as
Unstätigkeit der Kinder (‘instability of children’) was nonsense, at other times as negligence. All these
described by the Encyclopädie der Pädagokik as a deficit in different expressions can be summed up with the one
maintaining attention, especially in livelier children, there word: ‘distractibility’…. [This] volatility of spirit
is a restless, drifting bustle; the lad commences something does not arise periodically, … it is not limited to a
with passion, but soon abandons it and starts something particular activity, but … it gives lends all the doings
new. The author attributed the problem to parents or of the child a certain character that has become
teachers constantly switching between activities and noted habitual, or is close to becoming so (Klähr 1896).
that the situation was difficult to correct in those over
Klähr regarded the phenomenon as reflecting a failure of
9 years of age (Anonymus 1860).
will: it must be numbered among the moral defects, against
Attention was, not unexpectedly, addressed at length in
which upbringing is obliged to organize countermeasures-
pedagogic handbooks throughout the nineteenth century.
action. Indeed, where attention is lacking, not only is
Inattention was also specifically discussed: Zerstreutheit
achievement of the intellectual aspects of schooling
(‘distractedness’, ‘absentmindedness’) denoted the inabil-
threatened, but also the goals of moral education. While
ity to voluntarily focus attention, particularly upon the
congenital or acquired organic factors were probably
subject of the teacher’s interest. Such attention deficits
involved, as they were in the development of the person-
were not invariably associated with hyperactivity and could
ality in general, the roles played by the child’s social
refer to both dreamy lack of focus on school lessons, or to a
environment and by their parents and teachers were at least
general lack of sustained concentration. In his classic
as significant: Both those parents who spoiled their child
definition of the term, the Austrian philosopher–physician
with excessive indulgence and those whose discipline
Ernst von Feuchtersleben (1806–1849) described it as:
disregarded the child’s physical and mental nature were
… an indecision of attention … that corresponds in liable to encourage poor attention. Some of Klähr’s
mental life to the trembling of the muscles in the comments could, in fact, be drawn from the more recent
corporeal: an oscillation that indicates that the power sources: The craving for pleasure, the desire for the new
of the soul is not sufficient to act persistently in one and the exciting also draws the world of the children into
direction, so that rest, relaxation, constant change is their demonic circles. Can we then be surprised if children,
necessary (von Feuchtersleben 1860, p. 47). mentally poisoned by premature visits to theaters and all
kinds of exhibitions, by participation in adult amusements,
Th. Klähr provided a detailed sketch of the distracted child
and by stimulating reading, become incapable of collecting
in an 1886 essay for the Deutsche Blätter für erziehenden
and controlling their thoughts, of the regular strenuous
Unterricht (‘German pages for educational instruction’):
work required by their lessons? (Klähr 1896).
Even the external appearance of such a child reveals One author explained the situation by reference to the
the deficit requiring correction. The mercurial rest- ‘psychological mechanism,’ the constant changes in con-
lessness of the entire body, the wandering and scious perception and thought elicited by sensations that
uncertain gaze, the facial features that betray strange underlays all mental activity, but which occurs as uncon-
moods are the features of a distracted child that ini- sciously as does breathing or digestion. The child, as a still
tially catch one’s eye. Their mental life certainly only partially developed mental person, is still bound
shows no tendency to indolent inactivity or reverie, as tightly to the unsteady psychic activity at this lower level
seen in lazy children: on the contrary, the mind reg- … their mental life is controlled via thousand threads from
isters too many novel impressions without allowing the sensory world. It is the goal of the teacher to free the
them to persist, so that the new impression quickly child from this constant buffeting by their senses (Anon-
pushes its fleeting predecessor back below the ymus 1890; see also Strümpell 1892, pp. 63f.). Münch
threshold of consciousness. Thoughts spring quickly (1842), on the other hand, had earlier emphasized the
from one object to another, and even in interesting importance of home life: The higher classes tended
activities they dwell only a short time upon the same to provide their children with too many diversions, dis-
points. … No game is initiated without being couraging the development of focused attention and

123
P. B. Foley

discipline, while children from the lower classes were himself that he sometimes spoke of suicide, nevertheless
dulled by their poor environment or by their employment in improved after 9 years of age (Haslam 1809, pp. 198–206).
mindless jobs from an early age. The Breslau psychiatrist Heinrich Neumann (1814–1884)
Hermann Grünewald (Herborn, Nassau) published a dis- wrote particularly graphically of the ambiguous feelings
cussion of this aspect in 1902 that encapsulated much of what that the hectic child could arouse in their parents:
had been published over the past half century in Germany,
But one knows another kind of precocious develop-
linking pedagogic insights with philosophical consider-
ment that is more reminiscent of hypermetamorphosis
ations. Attention varied across the day, so Grünewald, even
… Such children have something restless about them,
in adults, but even more in younger children, where intrinsic
they are in perpetual motion, highly volatile in their
motor restlessness discouraged focused application; the
inclinations, unsteady in their movements (including
construction of the school day must recognize this by not, for
chorea!), difficult to get to sit down, slow in learning
example, switching rapidly between activities, or expecting
the positive, but often dazzling with rapid, brazen
children to immediately settle down to work after the
replies. This condition, which conceited mothers
excitement of playtime. The fantasy of the child, combined
describe as witty, anxious mothers as excited of
with their more immediate response to sensations, required
course, is certainly not itself a mental illness, but it
taming for structured learning to be possible. The home
must be regarded as a morbid tendency, the conse-
environment plays a similarly critical role: Grünewald
quences cannot be foreseen, and must be seriously
offered the case of a 10-year-old boy constantly exposed to
examined with regard to physical and moral educa-
the activities of his father’s tavern, so that he was uncon-
tion (Neumann 1859, pp. 143f).
sciously trained in distractedness. The discussion in this
paper mostly concerned ‘incidental distractedness’ in Neumann’s neologism, hypermetamorphosis, described the
otherwise healthy children, so that Grünewald comments inability to resist a shift in focus elicited by each. The term
that it is dubious to even regard it as a problem, but he did was later associated with Wernicke; according to his
note that in rare cases, it could become ‘habitual’ if steps concept of the ‘motility psychosis,’ hypermetamorphosis
were not undertaken to correct the situation; in the one induced an excessive urge to move (Bewegungsdrang)
serious case he had encountered it was combined with (Wernicke 1906, pp. 203f.), but neither Wernicke nor his
unmistakable symptoms of mania (Grünewald 1902). successors (Kleist, Leonhardt) appear to have addressed the
issue of hyperactivity in nonpsychotic children.
The German psychiatrist Wilhelm Griesinger, who
Inattention in the medical literature famously declared in 1845 that mental disease was brain
disease (1817–1868), described a spectrum of mania-like
The nineteenth German educational literature was thus well behavior in children, at the lower end of which scale was a
acquainted with problems of poor attention and excessive form that he distinguished from both folie raisonnante and
activity in pre-pubescent pupils, but they were largely moral insanity (to be discussed below):
regarded as developmental problems, as issues to be
Sometimes it is also a persistent state, but more
addressed in the classroom and the home, rather than as
intense: there is greater restlessness, a constant aim-
psychiatric conditions.
less roaming, confusion of the intelligence, perver-
Similar problems were also seeped gradually into the
sion of the emotions, with excitement which
medical literature. Scottish physician Alexander Crichton
sometimes passes (with greater impairment of mental
(1763–1856) provided one of the earliest references to
development) into profound mental debility. It is
attention problems in children in 1798, noting that it could
impossible to categorically distinguish this from the
be congenital or acquired; although poor concentration was
versatile form of infantile dementia: these are the
attributed to unnatural or morbid sensibility of the nerves, it
children who are never at rest for even a moment,
was not an insurmountable obstacle to education, even were
who chatter incessantly and in a confused manner,
it evident from an early age; further, what is fortunate, it is
exhibit no attentiveness, always wandering around,
generally diminished with age (Crichton 1798, p. 271; see
laughing, screaming, etc. (Griesinger 1867, 146f).
also Palmer and Finger 2001). The English physician John
Haslam (1764–1844) described a spoiled child who had These children were at the mercy of sensory impressions;
been raised to be a creature of volition and the terror of his the prognosis, however, was not bad. The general impres-
family, as he wanted the power of continued attention, and sion indeed remains throughout the nineteenth century that
was only attracted by fits and starts, it may be naturally hyperactivity and attention deficits could rarely be consid-
supposed he was not taught letters, and still les that he ered pathologic, and were expected, in any case, to regress
would copy them. His behavior, which so disturbed the boy with age, so long as the children were not indulged in their

123
Sons and daughters beyond your control

vices. This contrasts with moral insanity, a concept object of psychological analysis has generally been regarded
introduced by James Cowles Prichard (1786–1848) as an as the three Goulstonian Lectures presented by George
approximate equivalent to Pinel’s (1806) folie raisonnante: Frederic Still (1868–1941), the first British professor of
children’s diseases (1906, King’s College, London), to the
Moral Insanity, or madness consisting in a morbid
Royal College of Physicians in March 1902. Still reported his
perversion of the natural feelings, affections, incli-
systematic study of abnormal psychical conditions in chil-
nations, temper, habits, moral dispositions, and nat-
dren, principally disorders of moral conduct and increased
ural impulses, without any remarkable disorder or
emotional lability (passionateness), describing antisocial
defect of the intellect or knowing and reasoning
behavior in three groups of children: those with intellectual
faculties, and particularly without any insane illusion
handicaps, those who had suffered an acute febrile disease or
or hallucination (Prichard 1835, p. 6).
brain injury, and others who had suffered neither. Still
‘Moral’ originally referred to the ‘passions’ enjoying socially regarded all three groups as displaying similar deficits in
inappropriate free rein, not to a faculty of conscience, so that moral conduct, defined as immediate gratification of self in
‘moral insanity’ more closely translates as ‘conduct disorder’ opposition to the control of action in conformity with the idea
in modern parlance, although this distinction had been of the good of all, behavior marked by aggression, defiance,
gradually occluded by the late nineteenth century. Equally and incorrigibility. In his view, moral deficits were primarily
important was that intellectual defects were explicitly related to more general disorders of intellect, ‘moral control’
excluded by the definition. These children, in contrast to requiring the interaction between three factors—a cognitive
the inattentive and restless, did not grow out of their relationship with the environment (in order to develop a
condition. Many reviewers of the history of ADHD include ‘moral sense’); the ability to rationally forecast the conse-
the ‘morally insane’ and other ‘evil’ children (and, in some quences of an action; volition—and it was a morbid failure of
cases, hyperactivity in the mentally handicapped) as well as the last to which Still ascribed the moral deficiencies of
the hyperactive but morally competent child as earlier avatars children who retained a moral consciousness, this in turn
of the ADHD concept (for instance, Rafalovich 2001). perhaps related to exalted irritability of nervous centres,
Challenging children relevant to our discussion may indeed resulting from cell-modification dependent upon interfer-
have been included in this category, but discussions by major ence with cell-nutrition rather than morphological irregu-
authors, particularly in England (where the ‘moral insanity’ larities, for which reason childhood behavioral symptoms
concept was particularly strong), but this is not evident to any could presage psychiatric disease in the adult. He did,
significant degree in the European literature. however, note that physical stigmata suggested early,
It is significant that the Freiburg psychiatrist Hermann otherwise unapparent physical changes. Proposing a physi-
Emminghaus (1845–1904) did not depict an ADHD-like ological basis for behavior was nothing new, being consis-
condition in his Psychischen Störungen des Kindesalters, tent with the, in principle, linkage of psychological processes
the first handbook of pediatric psychiatry (1887), although with brain function that had commenced in the late nine-
he did include several pages on ‘moral insanity’ (Em- teenth century. Further, there is nothing Darwinistic about
minghaus 1887, pp. 231–243). In 1899, the Scottish psy- this thought, other than the observation that, as the highest
chiatrist Thomas Clouston described states of over- and latest product of mental evolution, moral control was
excitability, hypersensitiveness, and mental explosiveness also the most fragile (Still 1902).
in children, a state he located on the borderland of psy- Some caution is warranted in identifying Still’s
chiatry, and which he linked with an explosive tendency in observed conditions with ADHD. Firstly, he was primarily
cortical brain regions, similar to that of epilepsy and per- interested in moral deficits in behavior: His twenty patients
haps linked with asynchronous development, leading to without general impairment of intellect and without phys-
loss of inhibition by higher centers. The result was exces- ical disease were not so much hyperactive as inappropri-
sive responsiveness to internal and external stimuli. The ately active, so that it would be more apt to speak of
prognosis was good, but Clouston nonetheless advocated ‘conduct disorders’ or impulsiveness. Attention deficits
high-dosage bromide therapy to manage the situation, were indeed found to be fairly constant, but were only
together with good nutrition and exercise (Clouston 1899). mentioned in passing as a possible alternative to deficient
‘moral consciousness’ as an explanation for bad behavior.
He described one example of a quite abnormal incapacity
Early twentieth century: the rise of neurological models
for sustained attention, causing school failure even in the
of behavioral disorders
absence of intellectual retardation:
The major step in the direction of recognizing hyperactivity Another boy, aged six years, with marked moral
and attention deficits as a medical condition more than an defect was unable to keep his attention even to a

123
P. B. Foley

game for more than a very short time, and, as might normal intelligence. Like Still, he envisaged the possibil-
be expected, the failure of attention was very ity of both congenital (‘psychopathic’ or ‘neuropathic
noticeable at school, with the result that in some diathesis’) and acquired defects (in utero and perinatal
cases the child was backward in school attainments, injury, infection) and is therefore seen by some as a
although in manner and ordinary conversation he harbinger of the 1940s ‘minimal brain damage’ concept of
appeared as bright and intelligent as any child could ADHD-type disorders. While more influential than Still
be (Still 1902, p. 1166). (Amentia continued to appear in new editions until 1952),
his contribution to the prehistory of ADHD was similarly
The immediate impact of Still’s lectures is also dubious. minor.
He himself did not refer to them in his highly regarded In 1908, Adalbert Czerny (1963–1941), one of the pio-
Common disorders and diseases of childhood (five editions neers of modern pediatrics, published the first edition of his
between 1909 and 1927), where moral defect is only popular handbook on the role of the physician in raising
discussed in connection with epilepsy and convulsions, nor children; neither this nor the following editions refers
were they often cited during his lifetime, and then usually specifically to hyperactive or distracted children. The 6th
with reference to his description of post-infection character edition (1922), however, included the description of a
change. His major aim seems to have been to raise group of difficult to educate children with familiar char-
awareness of the fact that ‘bad behavior’ should not be acteristics: a great need for movement, lack of sustained
automatically condemned as willful or evidence of bad application in play and other activities, disobedience, and
character, nor should the intelligence of such children be deficient ability to concentrate during lessons (Czerny
underestimated; that is, he pleaded for a more differenti- 1922, pp. 107f.). Czerny discerned an interaction of con-
ated treatment of ‘problem children,’ in order to save those stitution and education in these cases, which he did not
who could be saved. While recognizing the value of his regard as psychiatric in nature.
observations, they must nonetheless be regarded as part of After the Great War, the Heidelberg psychiatrist August
the prehistory of ADHD, not as the beginning of its history. Homburger (1873–1930) and the Karlsruhe pediatrician
The work of Alfred Frank Tredgold (1870–1952), an Franz Lust (1880–1939) each similarly underscored inter-
English authority in mental disease, with respect to the actions between constitutional, psychological, and social
‘moral sense’ essentially expanded upon that of Still factors in the development of irritability, lack of concen-
(without citing it), although his more deeply biological tration, and behavioral restlessness in children; excessive
viewpoint saw less of an environmental influence on its sensory stimulation and poor teaching methods continued
development, consistent with his eugenicist stance. In the to be cited as contributing factors, so that antisocial
second edition of his much cited work Mental deficiency behavior need neither be purely inborn, nor the result in
(amentia) (1914), he introduced the following in his dis- inadequate parenting (Seidler 2004). In his Vorlesungen
cussion of the ‘feeble-minded’, the mildest form of mental über Psychopathologie des Kindesalters, Homburger also
retardation: discussed deficiencies and volition and self-control in
adolescents. The very basis of this instability was the
In the lethargic, inert type of feeble-mindedness there
inadequate emotional core of the personality, the result of
is a defect of spontaneous attention; but this is never
which is a lack of inhibition, cohesion, perseverance.
so marked in this degree as in the more serious grades
Instead of reasoning, interests, obligations, principles,
of amentia. The general stir and excitement aroused
views of the future are supplanted by stubbornness,
by a visitor is much more pronounced in the special
impulsiveness and vague desires, all at the mercy of fleet-
school than in the imbecile ward. On the other hand,
ing impressions (Homburger 1926). The solution for these
active or voluntary attention is commonly in defect,
children primarily involved appropriate training; as Franz
both with regard to its intensity and its duration. The
Kramer commented in 1925: the better our parents and
most trifling thing serves to distract these children
educators [Erzieher], the fewer psychopaths we will have
from their occupation, so that even where the atten-
(in discussion of von der Leyen 1926).
tion is readily gained, it is with difficulty held
Mikhail Gurewich (1878–1953), director of the Pediatric
(Tredgold 1914, p. 167).
Psychiatric Clinic of the Children’s Medical Institute in
The result was impulsiveness and impeded schooling, but Moscow, similarly described in 1925 a frontal motor
hyperactivity was not a feature of these children. Further, condition in infants (3–5 years) that lacked extrapyramidal
Tredgold specifically defined these children as typically signs, including excessive liveliness (excess of drive),
requiring life-long specialized care—they are unable to which can be explained by the disinhibition (the frontal
swim against the stream, or even to keep their heads systems subordinate) motor centrality, a crass deficiency of
above water (p. 172)—so that these were not children of active attention and a general intellectual insufficiency,

123
Sons and daughters beyond your control

elevated mood and moria-like conditions, as well as speech continuous irritation of all around them, none of which
production deficits (Gurewitsch 1925). Hypermotor were more than momentarily reduced by the harshest dis-
syndromes had been recognized as both medical and ped- cipline or the most passionate pleading. The severity of the
agogic problems during the 1920s, and a ‘psychoneuro- behavioral symptoms often exceeded anything that could
logical sanatorium’ school had provided special programs be dismissed as ‘youthful energy’ or simply ‘difficult
since 1919. The authors regarded the problem as having children,’ or even as defiance of social conventions. The
increased by the 1950s due to a series of factors, including spectrum of behaviors regarded as ‘abnormal’ may have
increased survivability of difficult births and therapeutic been broader in the 1920 than in 2014, and many cases
possibilities, as well as improved detection; further, were less criminal than irritating, but these behaviors were
hyperactivity is not only a school problem: it often becomes not simply disagreeable for parents and medical practitio-
the source of rapidly advancing maladjustment within ners, but also for the child who had never previously
the family, the school, and society, and thus is an impor- behaved in this manner and who even now often experi-
tant social problem as well (reviewed: Isaev and Kagan enced them as foreign to their ‘selves.’ Doctors also noted
1981). that although children suffering post-encephalitic psychi-
atric disturbances could exhibit the cruelest behavior—
removing their own eyes or killing a sibling—they exhib-
Infection and behavioral disorders: encephalitis ited genuine remorse after the fact, and claimed to have
lethargica been compelled to act as they did. The ultimate fate of such
children could hardly be more tragic: their behavioral
Awareness of the potential impact of specific external problems receded during their early 20s, only to be
factors upon behavior via neurologic changes increased replaced by rapidly advancing parkinsonism from which
during the 1920s following descriptions of conduct disor- there was no recovery (reviewed: Anderson 1923; Leyser
ders associated with post-vaccination and post-infection 1924; Homburger 1926, pp. 451–462; Thiele 1926;
encephalitis, as well as, most dramatically, following Borthwick 1927; Proby 1939; Fairweather 1947; Vilensky
encephalitis lethargica (EL). Between 1917 and the late et al. 2007).
1920s, EL was an epidemic, probably viral, neuropsychi- The behavioral syndrome was all the more frightening
atric infection. The acute disorder was characterized by for all concerned because it sometimes included features of
different combinations of cranial nerve palsies, sleep dis- congenital psychopathy, a term then supplanting ‘moral
turbances, and involuntary motor symptoms; mortality was insanity’: the combination of unimpaired intelligence with
about 15 %. In children (and some adults) who survived apparent ethical nihilism meant that the children pursued
the acute phase, the following pseudo-neurasthenic phase their anti-social activities with a certain refinement and
was characterized severe psychiatric and behavioral chan- sometimes managed to astutely conceal their misdeeds …
ges, including impaired voluntary attention, impulsiveness, such patients can become the worst of vexations with which
emotional lability, precocious sexuality, self mutilation, a one must contend. The onset of largest epidemic of the
tendency to wander, and delinquency, often to a degree disease which makes criminals (The Times: Medical cor-
requiring confinement in psychiatric or penal institutions, respondent 1924), in the United Kingdom in 1924, caused a
appeared in various constellations in children who had great deal of public and political concern, building on
previously not attracted attention in this way. Neither previous disquiet in nations as different as the United
intelligence nor memory were impaired, but sleep was Kingdom, Germany, and the Soviet Union that delinquency
commonly affected: The children tend to be overactive in general had increased since the War. EL was cited in the
through the night but slept through most of the day. In the defence of youths arraigned for various crimes during the
few cases that came to autopsy, neuropathology was con- 1920s, and its effect on adolescents was the subject of
fined to the brainstem and was not markedly different to parliamentary debate and government enquiries. Special
that associated with post-encephalitic parkinsonism in institutes for the care of such children, with varying suc-
adults; that is, the cerebral cortex was spared (reviewed: cess, were established in various countries, including
Stern 1936). There was also later some suggestion that Germany, France, the United Kingdom, and the United
involvement of the region implicated in ‘sham rage’ in States. On the other hand, EL-related character change of
animals might be implicated (Le Gros Clark 1938). degree that seriously threatened the safety of others was not
Disobedience, rudeness, constant silliness and lack of as common as some literature suggests. Even Arthur Hall
seriousness, brazenness: such symptoms might be dis- (1886–1951)—who coined the denomination ‘apache type’
missed as ‘children being children,’ but their significance to describe such children—noted that they were atypical,
in EL children was their combination with more serious but were all the more visible because of their potential for
conduct problems: erethism, amoral tendencies, the criminal activity (Hall 1925). The most common type,

123
P. B. Foley

however, consisted of his ‘super-difficult’ children, who activity, thereby shaping emerging the concept of the
could also engage in activities that brought them to the ‘hyperkinetic child.’
attention of the police, but were more likely to be patho-
logically unsocial creatures.
The EL behavioral syndrome was described wherever Charité, Berlin: The Kramer–Pollnow hyperkinetic
EL was encountered, but the most systematic investigations syndrome
were conducted in Germany, particularly at the Charité in
Berlin under Karl Bonhoeffer (1868–1948), who suggested The EL epidemic had largely ebbed by 1930, when Franz
that a discordance between cortical and subcortical func- Kramer (1878–1967) and Hans Pollnow (1902–1943),
tion underlay the unprecedented character change (Bon- also of the Psychiatric-Neurologic Clinic at the Charité,
hoeffer 1922), presaging more recent models of reduced described a ‘hyperkinetic syndrome’ in children, charac-
impulsive control, despite differences in the details. terized by persistent movement, climbing, handling of
Important studies were undertaken here by Rudolf Thiele objects, reduced concentration, and delayed speech
(1880–1960), who explained the problem in terms of development; epileptic fits were also common at the
Drang: onset. The disorder commenced between the ages of 3
and 4 years and then declined gradually from the age of
By Drang we mean a primarily utterly amorphous,
six, whereafter the previous intellectual and speech defi-
objective and non-directional discharge tendency that
cits were largely compensated. The authors presented
represents, with regard to its mental representation,
several examples, including a monozygotic twin whose
an impassionate restlessness and tension, and that
brother was free of the disorder, suggesting that it was
only in its impact through its manipulation of the
probably caused by infection or other external agent; the
object or as the result of interference with directed,
only autopsy had found evidence of EL-like brainstem
intentional actions assumes the form of a specific
changes (Kramer and Pollnow 1930). Nearly 2 years later,
action with content. … One can formulate this rela-
Kramer and Pollnow published their results in detail,
tionship briefly in this manner: The will selects its
including reports on 45 children seen between 1921 and
object, the drive [Trieb], due to its inherent direct-
1931, 15 of whom had been followed for several years.
edness, seeks its object, and the inherently goal- and
They noted that Wernicke had presented a similar case in
directionless, ‘‘blind’’ Drang finds its object, i.e. it
1901 as an example of his ‘motility psychosis’ (Kramer
interacts with whatever object is available. It lacks, at
and Pollnow 1932).
least primarily, an imagined or otherwise mentally
The symptoms of the syndrome were very similar to
represented motive. The most primitive manifestation
those of the EL syndrome, and the authors conceded that
of Drang presents itself as a general, chaotic motion
most differences were more quantitative than qualitative;
unrest (Thiele 1926, pp. 55f.; see also Thiele 1953).
some cases even included typical somatic symptoms, such
In any case, EL triggered behavioral changes in children as hypersalivation. But there were also subtle differences:
that are not duplicated by any other neurological condition, The restlessness of the Kramer–Pollnow syndrome was
with the possible exception of traumatic brain injury; in more chaotic, for example, and hyperactivity was similar
particular, the dark fissure that opened between the EL throughout the day rather than nocturnal. Critical, however,
sufferer and their urges was rarely again described, an was the absence of neurological signs in the Kramer–
puzzling separation of the person from their actions (which Pollnow children, particularly the absence of both oculo-
does not exclude the possibility of its being encountered motor and extrapyramidal phenomena. Their hyperactivity
but not acknowledged). It was this phenomenon that could give the impression of severe intellectual deficits, but
endowed the syndrome with its sinister eeriness, the baleful the observations of Kramer and Pollnow suggested to them
malignity that horrified the powerless parent who asked the that the intellect was less severely affected than it
marginally less helpless neurologist whether they would appeared; their perception and memory were, in fact,
ever see ‘their’ child again, with much the same trepidation excellent. The Kramer–Pollnow report thus further sup-
that parents once sought the services of an exorcist. ported the concept that antisocial behavior need not be
Therapeutic approaches ranged from special training in inborn, nor the result in inadequate parenting. While dis-
dedicated institutions to frontal leucotomy. The EL expe- tractibility, hyperkinesia, and impulsiveness were key
rience of the 1920s inspired a multifaceted discussion of features, other characteristics are not as easy to reconcile
the role played by the brainstem in psychological processes with more recent view of ADHD: the strong association
as well as in psychiatric disorders, including psychosis and with convulsions or epilepsy during epilepsy, early speech
hysteria in adults. as well as providing a large number of production deficits, the regression of symptoms by age
children and adolescents exhibiting excessive psychomotor seven.

123
Sons and daughters beyond your control

Immediate reaction to the Kramer–Pollnow report was any behavioral disorder that followed childhood infection,
mixed. The Zentralblatt review of the 1932 paper by Julius thereby pooling completely different types of neuro- and
Hallervorden was extensive and neutral (Hallervorden psychopathology (see, for example, Anderson and Mambar
1933), but Hans Heinze (1895–1983), Assistant Physician 1939; Levy 1959).
to Paul Schröder (1873–1941) in Leipzig—recently The sociologist Adam Rafalovich, amongst others, has
described as the centre of European child and adolescent argued that this period consolidated an increasing drive to
psychiatry (Angermeyer und Steinberg, cited by Rothen- medicalize unconventional childhood behavior (Rafalovich
berger and Neumärker 2005, p. 110)—argued that Kramer 2001); Lantéri-Laura and Gros (1982) argued that the
and Pollnow had seen nothing but a reaction elicited by any ‘hyperkinetic syndrome’ was essentially invented by child
of a number of organic brain processes (Heinze 1932), psychiatrists in the 1920s as a means for distinguishing
missing the point of the Kramer–Pollnow paper that there their nascent field from other areas of medicine. The social
existed a distinct, reversible childhood hyperactivity syn- aspects of both the origins and consequences of the EL
drome, the number of cases of which had remained fairly syndrome, in particular, were, however, far from neglected.
constant across the 1920s, uninfluenced by the rise and fall The suggestion that physicians never asked about the
of EL. Wilhelm Lange (1894–?) in Chemnitz was more social variables (Rafalovich 2001) affecting childhood
sympathetic, declaring that Kramer-Pollnow children were behavior ignores the pools of ink devoted to describing the
easier to identify when, as in his unit, their presentation social backgrounds of affected children and discussing the
could be directly compared with EL (Lange 1933). impact of crowded housing, the parents’ mental and social
The further reception of the concept was somewhat problems, the child’s prior diseases, and other factors.
confused: When it was cited over the next quarter century Further, consideration of social factors played critical roles
was often misinterpreted as a form of encephalitis, epi- in the medical and legal approaches to behavioral prob-
lepsy, or schizophrenia—Lay (1938) devoted two pages of lems. Although the children were diagnosed in institutions,
his paper on childhood psychosis-like disorders to their EL children were transferred to institutions specifically
‘hyperkinetic disease,’ while Parsons and Barling’s Dis- because they had proved intolerable in their homes; their
eases of infancy and childhood (Parsons et al. 1954) conduct often improved during institutional care, but
mentioned it as a form of childhood psychosis, their only deteriorated once discharged. Further, the examples cited
discussion of hyperactivity (see also Strömgren 1947; by Rafalovich also give the impression that merely naughty
Creak et al. 1952) or as a variant of Heller’s dementia children were being institutionalized. This ignores several
infantilis; there was even one report of possible Kramer– critical features of their behavior: that it represented a stark
Pollnow syndrome without hyperactivity (Everberg 1957). contrast with their pre-EL personality; that it was regarded
As late as 1961, however, the British Medical Journal as so extreme that parents sought official help in an era
recognized their contribution, although conflating it with when there were less restrictions on the punitive actions
‘moral insanity’: they could legally adopt; that the children themselves were
often distressed by their behavior and their lack of control
In the hyperkinetic syndrome, first described by F.
over it; that this behavior could escalate with age to include
Kramer and H. Pollnow, the child may be cold, cruel,
violent crimes, including rape and murder; that these
ruthless, destructive, and difficult to contact emo-
children ultimately succumbed to parkinsonism. To regard
tionally, but the condition is distinct from schizo-
these children as victims of a need to ‘medicalize’ deviant
phrenia: about 50 to 60% have epileptic fits or
behavior is to miss the point entirely: These were unhappy,
electroencephalographic evidence of focal lesions,
driven children who derived little or no pleasure from their
and many of the remainder probably have some more
misadventures. It may be unpalatable, but neurological
subtle cerebral anomaly. The disturbance often
disease can so radically modify both cognitive functions
becomes attenuated with advancing age and the
and personality that the pre-disease personality of the
prognosis is better than in autistic children (Anony-
sufferer can be difficult to recognize, or recover.
mus 1961).
This situation is partly explained by the vicissitudes visited
upon by the two Jewish authors after 1933, impeding their The hyperkinetic syndrome in older children
further research, and neither returned to their syndrome and adolescents
(see Neumärker 2005; Rothenberger and Neumärker 2005),
partly by the fact that the new syndrome was overshad- Shortly after Pollnow and Kramer reported their syndrome
owed by the seemingly more numerous EL cases. From the in detail, Emil von Lederer and Stephan Éderer (University
1930s, ‘post-encephalitic behavior disorder’ was indis- Pediatric Clinic, Budapest) described a hypermobility
criminately employed, particularly in the United States for neurosis of childhood that resembled in many features the

123
P. B. Foley

Kramer–Pollnow syndrome, but curiously affected those Interestingly, Kramer had previously reported a disorder
between 7 years of age and puberty; without apparent under the name haltlose Psychopathen (‘unstable psycho-
reason, the child was suddenly restless, their schoolwork paths’) characterized by indecision and impulsivity that
suffered, their mood changed, and their speech was was typically manifested as the person commenced work-
impaired, while intelligence was untouched; extrapyrami- ing life: The unstable person is drowned in the momentary
dal symptoms could also be presented: situation and he changes purpose with the slightest change
of external situation. Sufferers initiated plans of action, but
Apart from nighttime rest, which is usually undis-
could not lead them to completion, as automatic series
turbed, the child does remain quiet for even a second.
required constant and considerable voluntary effort, and
They scuttle back and forth, they don’t walk but run,
this could not be maintained. Internal restlessness also
instead of sitting quietly they shuffle around on their
caused a need of activity, but lacking in direction; impul-
chair, they kick against the table, chair etc. Even their
sivity and momentary decisions led to ill-considered
hands are never still, touching everything unneces-
actions. This was combined with internal sense of superi-
sarily and without thought. Facial expressions are
ority to others that was, however, combined with feelings
lively, their speech hasty and unconsidered. … the
of insufficiency, and together with their heightened sensi-
previously more or less industrious, obedient student
tivity, this typically resulted in failure in employment and
does not follow his lessons, they make no progress,
social withdrawal (Kramer 1927).
and their constant restlessness and talkativeness dis-
The ‘hyperkinetic child’ (and persistence of certain
turbs both teachers and classmates. The parents also
aspects of the syndrome into early adulthood) was thus a
notice that the child is unable to apply themselves to
recognized phenomenon in Europe by the early 1930s, but
any task for an extended period, that the usual street
was clearly not yet regarded as a major problem for psy-
noise suffices to lure him to the window, that his
chiatry (it rarely rated a mention in handbooks prior to the
attention is distracted by the slightest disturbance. …
1950s, while German neuropsychiatrist Theodor Ziehen
He likes to run around aimlessly, climbs on furniture,
(1862–1950), for example, discussed attention deficits in
fences, trees; sometimes he almost breaks his neck
his Outline of physiological psychology (Ziehen 1924)
because his awkwardness has increased. … Their
without referring to their occurrence in children, although
mood has also changed: The formerly friendly, easily
he had previously contributed a handbook article on
controlled child is ill-tempered, impatient, moody; it
‘pathological restlessness’: Ziehen 1909), but rather as an
has a tendency to tears, but can abruptly switch to a
issue that was resolved by puberty or, in more difficult
sunny mood (von Lederer and Éderer 1934).
cases, by specific educational, ‘character-forming’ mea-
The incidence of the syndrome was found in 424 urban sures. It is nonetheless somewhat surprising that it did not
school children to be 9 %, but in most instances, the attract more attention, given the great interest during early
symptoms disappeared by puberty at the latest; in others, twentieth century in psychomotor function and correlations
symptoms were so mild as to attract little attention, while, (see, for instance, Kleist 1908; Dupré and Merklen 1910;
in contrast, Lederer and Éderer warned that the complaints Kleist 1927).
of the parents are exaggerated, as they are irritated even In the United States, Eugen Kahn and Louis Cohen
by the natural mobility and exuberance of their healthy (Yale University School of Medicine) described the same
child. phenomenon in 1934 as organic drivenness, or a surplus of
Similar cases were described in Italy by Sante de Sanctis inner impulsion, and similarly saw it as a symptom that was
as ‘dysthymic children,’ including symptoms such as not pathognomic for any one disorder (two of his three
undirected behavior, slowness, apathy, inactivity, absence examples were children, 8 and 11 years). Its biological
of emotional delicacy, and also by positive signs: impul- basis was brainstem dysfunction, one of the two regions
siveness, psychomotor agitation and choreiform move- (the other being the cerebral cortex) they regarded as most
ments, general restlessness, excitability going up to important for superior mental functioning; extrapyramidal
compulsive talking, refusal to work, tears and laughs pro- symptoms were also presented (tics, choreiform move-
voked by the slightest stimulants, sleep troubles, erotism, ments) (Kahn and Cohen 1934). Wilmot Schneider
wandering (de Sanctis 1923, 1934). Both Pollnow/Kra- (Cleveland, Ohio) was also familiar with the hyperkinetic
mer’s and de Sanctis’ cases, which he interpreted as an at child in 1944:
least partially hereditary metabolic disorder, were cited by
Austrian psychiatrist Hans Hoff (1897–1969) in his article It matters not whether we are teacher, parent, school
on ‘hyperkinetic disorder’ in his 1956 textbook of psy- or private physician—we are all familiar with the
chiatry (Hoff 1956, p. 544). hyperkinetic child. Many of you may not recognize

123
Sons and daughters beyond your control

him by that name. Some of you may know him as the presentation of more primitive, uninhibited behavior, had
‘‘nervous’’ child, or as the one who ‘‘never sits still’’; been suggested by the EL behavioral syndrome, and applied
others call him hypomanic or the ‘‘fidgety child.’’ … to hyperactivity in children in general. Interest moved to the
restless, overactive, aggressive, distractible children cerebral cortex during the 1940s, with comparisons of
whose behavior includes inattention, flightiness of hyperkinetic behavior in children compared with that of
ideas, an explosiveness of motor output; those whose monkeys following frontal lobe ablation, or in children who
behavior in the school, home, and doctors’ examining had suffered frontal lobe trauma (Levin 1938). Further,
rooms includes unpredictable, impulsive behavior, Kramer and Pollnow were not alone in their association of
hair-raising ‘‘getting into everything’’ (Schneider the hyperkinetic behavior with epilepsy: abnormal or ‘epi-
1945, p. 559). leptoid’ type EEG patterns had been identified in more than
half the children with behavioral disorders but no other
Schneider emphasized the organic roots of this behavior,
evidence of CNS dysfunction or insult (Jasper et al. 1938;
but nonetheless recognized social and psychogenic
see also later publications by Bradley, reviewed by Bau-
contributors:
meister et al. 2012), a finding confirmed by later studies (for
We must know him or her as a psychobiologic unit; example, Ingram 1956; Knobel et al. 1959; Anderson 1963;
an integration of the psyche (emotions and intelli- Satterfield 1973; Loo and Barkley 2005).
gence) and soma (physicochemical components, During the 1940s, the American educationist Alfred
endocrine, allergy) being acted upon by the total Strauss (1897–1957) and colleagues explored personality
environment (home, school, society) and in turn and problems of mental processing in the brain-injured
having an effect, as a personality, on this environ- child, initially in patients with marked intellectual deficits:
ment (Schneider 1945, p. 560).
A brain-injured child is a child who before, during, or
While the focus was on hyperactivity, its association with after birth has received an injury to, or suffered an
poor attention was also noted by some authors: A. infection of the brain. As a result of such organic
T. Childers (Cincinnati, Ohio), for example, found that impairment, defects of the neuromotor system may be
neurological signs were not always present in hyperactive present or absent; however, such a child may show
children, but that difficulty to hold his attention, short disturbances in perception, thinking, and emotional
attention span or excessive divertibility was evident in 22 behavior, either separately or in combination (Strauss
of 30 cases; part of the child’s over-activity as he and Lehtinen 1947, p. 4).
constantly sought new experiences or gave himself up to
That is, ‘brain damage’ was determined on the basis of test
uninhibited curiosity. Other prominent features included
performance, not upon neurological evidence of an insult.
talkativeness, boastfulness, and poor sleep. Childers main-
Deficient attention and hyperactivity were only briefly
tained that social and environmental factors were insuffi-
touched upon in the volume generally cited as establishing
ciently considered in the discussion of the nature of
the ‘minimal brain damage’ concept—Strauss was more
childhood overactivity (Childers 1935).
interested in learning and perceptual deficits—but it none-
theless led to increased interest in ‘hidden’ brain damage as
underlying not only hyperactivity in children, but all
Minimal brain damage childhood learning, perceptual, and behavioral difficulties.
The major problem with this model was that Strauss and
Tredgold had noted that in milder cases [of asphyxia dur- colleagues proposed that all brain lesions, wherever
ing birth], the initial symptoms may rapidly pass off and it localized, are followed by a similar kind of disordered
is only when the child begins his schooling that deficiency behavior (Strauss and Lehtinen 1947, p. 20), and that the
is noted (Tredgold 1914, p. 229). This was largely over- various behavioral and learning difficulties they had
looked until the mid-1920s, when EL raised awareness of described were merely variants of this one response; con-
the potential impact of brainstem injury upon personality versely, another commentator opined that there was a
and behavior, and major publications provided diverse striking similarity in the behavior of all hyperactive chil-
examples of both specific and less specific impacts of dren regardless of the cause of the hyperkinesis (Russell
different types of head injury incurred during the First 1942). Gesell and Amatruda (1941) similarly commented
World War. A number of major examinations of perinatal that The concept of minimal injury has been gradually
brain injury were also published during this period (for forced upon us by our clinical experience with atypical
example, Ehrenfest 1922; Doll et al. 1932). infants. … In obscure or doubtful cases the following is a
The argument that brain damage released lower brain safe rule: Do not assume that there has certainly been a
regions from inhibition by higher control, leading to the cerebral injury, but assume that every child who is born

123
P. B. Foley

alive has run the universal risk of such injury (p. 231). psychoneurologic test findings that were interpreted in
Further, there was no neurophysiological explanation that favor of frontal lobe dysfunction, possibly of biochemical
provided a link between the presumed ‘brain damage’—it rather than neurological nature, resulting in release from
is notable that the term ‘brain damage’ in this context was inhibitory input (Knobel et al. 1959). Knobel (1959) argued
used primarily by psychologists, not neurologists—and that a purely diencephalic problem would produce a more
specific learning difficulties or behaviors. The concept of complicated symptomatology than seen in ‘acting out’
‘minimal brain damage’ was, however, not quite as (impulsive, aggressive hyperactivity), perhaps remember-
abstruse as it might first appear: It was, in principle, a ing the picture seen in EL children.
natural extension of the search for neuropathology under-
lying major psychoses and other psychiatric disorders,
combined with the evidence provided by encephalitis The surprising effects of amphetamines: the roots
lethargica, wartime injuries, and other phenomena causing of an unresolved controversy
brain injury are often associated with attention deficits and
motor activity. Further, the catastrophic psychiatric and The quasi-neurological approach was reinforced by the
neurological symptoms had occurred following ‘silent’ initially little heeded findings of Charles Bradley
infections with EL and epidemic polio; Ramsay and Young (1902–1979) regarding the seemingly paradoxic effects of
(1963)argued similarly that EEG changes following rubella amphetamines in children with behavior problems at the
and measles were more common than would be suspected. Emma Pendleton Bradley Home on Rhode Island (USA).
Even today, it has been suggested that most instances of The effects of Benzedrine (racemic amphetamine; 10 mg
encephalitis remain undiagnosed (Davison et al. 2003). In as single daily dose) were trialled in thirty children (5–14-
1959, Knobloch and Pasamanick wrote in compromise, on years old) over 3 weeks, presumably because of the
the basis of a study of 500 behaviorally challenging chil- recently reported effects on mood in performance in adults
dren that found a higher presentation of neurological (stimulation of choroid plexus function would be an unli-
abnormalities than in 350 control children, that a a con- kely motivation). The scholastic performance of 15 of the
tinuum of cerebral damage ranging from severe abnor- 29 school age children improved in spectacular manner
malities, such as cerebral palsy and mental deficiency, to while taking the drug, with a notable increase in drive; 15
minimal damage. This haziness was reflected by the children who had previously burdened their environment
absence from major child psychiatry texts of the hyperki- with noisy, aggressive, domineering behavior were now
netic syndrome as late as 1969. less moody (subdued) and more sociable, but of these only
In any case, the assumption that mental, emotional, eight were those whose schoolwork improved. Bradley
motor, and behavioral abnormalities in children were extended his study to 100 children over subsequent years,
attributable to undetected brain damage was widely most diagnosed with behavior disorders of psychogenic
explored during the 1950s, whereby it was not applied only origin but also including neurological cases, with the
to the hyperkinetic syndrome: It is protean but the form median dose raised to 20 mg/day; 54 children were sub-
that it most commonly takes is seen in the child who is dued and 19 stimulated; 50 of the 84 school children
clumsy, writes badly and slowly, lacks concentration and improved in scholastic performance. Bradley recom-
has slow reactions (Editorial 1964). The problem, indeed, mended the drug as useful for some children with behav-
arose that the specific ‘hyperkinetic syndrome’ was partly ioral problems for improving their sense of well-being and
subsumed into this ‘brain damage’ syndrome, leading to an voluntary control, but also commented that it could not
confused, undifferentiated view of such disorders replace alleviation of the child’s environment or psycho-
(reviewed: Kessler 1980). There were certainly reports that therapy (Bradley 1937; Bradley and Bowen 1941; recent
hyperkinetic behavior was often associated with preterm review: Strohl 2011).
births, and other complications of pregnancy were partic- This approach was little applied elsewhere until the
ularly common in the history of hyperkinetic children (for 1950s; while ethical reservations regarding the use of
example: Knobloch et al. 1956), but were not specific psychotropic agents in children were not unknown in the
indicators. As American child psychiatrist Leon Eisenberg 1940s, the general abuse potential of amphetamines in
(1922–2009) later commented (1964), one is tempted to adults had also caused sufficient alarm to provoke restric-
conclude that brain damaged is the expletive uttered by the tion of their availability (Staehelin 1941). The dominance
clinician vainly trying to restrain for examination a of psychoanalysis in post-1945 American psychiatry also
writhing whirling miniature dervish. played a role.
This is not to say that there was no evidence for brain The effectiveness of amphetamines in dampening
abnormalities in hyperkinetic children: Mauricio Knobel hyperkinetic behavior played a significant role in the
and colleagues (Kansas), for example, reported EEG and ongoing investigation of this symptom. For example, when

123
Sons and daughters beyond your control

Maurice Laufer and colleagues published their investiga- defined, but, as we have seen, both hyperactivity and
tion of the ‘hyperkinetic impulse disorder’ and the benefits attention deficits were objects of psychiatric and other
of amphetamine therapy (see below), it was at a time when medical discussion before stimulant therapy achieved
poor parenting was regarded as the major problem in widespread acceptance. Further, the long-held impression
poorly behaved children (Knobloch and Pasamanick 1959 that the benefits of stimulant therapy were restricted to
pointedly commented that Behavior problems in children those with non-neurological hyperactivity (Knobel et al.
have been ascribed to tension in their mothers, but the 1959) was long dubious, and was finally dismissed by the
likelihood that an infant with difficulties produces tension close of the 1980s (Rapoport et al. 1980). Their employ-
in his mother needs serious consideration). Laufer sus- ment remains controversial, some arguing that they repre-
pected that abnormal activity of the reticular activation sent the application of medical knowledge for purposes of
system might be involved, and applied Gastaut’s photo- social control; this argument also flows into doubts that
metrazol stimulation test to test this hypothesis: He found ADHD itself is nothing but a political construct for de-
that hyperkinetic children were more sensitive those with monizing inconvenient childhood energy. From Bradley
emotional disturbances to metrazol, and that this difference onwards, however, even advocates of stimulant use
was abolished by administration of amphetamine. The emphasized their roles as adjuncts to psychotherapy (see
authors concluded that poor sensory filtering at the level of Eisenberg 1971 for discussion at the beginning of the
the thalamus flooded resulted in overactivity (Laufer et al. ascendancy of methylphenidate). These issues cannot be
1957). further discussed here and can certainly not be settled by
A number of other agents had been recommended by appeal to history (review of amphetamine therapy until
this time, including methamphetamine, tranquilizers, and 1970s: Schmutz 2004).
several antihistamines, but none had proved as satisfactory
as amphetamine (Laufer and Denhoff 1957). One, how-
ever, would ultimately prove to be the most popular option: Renaissance of the hyperkinetic syndrome
methylphenidate, or Ritalin, first synthesized in 1944, but
commercially released in 1954 as an alternative to Ben- A return to the brainstem in different guise was proposed
zedrine with fewer untoward side effects (Panizzon 1944; by Maurice Laufer during the 1950s, whose investigations
Meier et al. 1954). Following a successful trial in 108 suggested that in the ‘hyperkinetic impulse disorder,’ as he
children and adults (mean age of 15 years) with emotional dubbed it—characterized by hyperactivity; short attention
problems, it was approved by the FDA for the treatment of span and poor powers of concentration; irritability;
childhood behavioral disorders in 1961. The first clinical impulsiveness; variability; and poor school work (Laufer
study restricted to hyperkinetic children (Knobel 1962: et al. 1957)—insufficient sensory filtering at the dience-
infantile hyperkinetic syndrome) and a double-blind, pla- phalic level led to overactivity in the reticular activation
cebo-controlled study in which parents and childcare system and hence to flooding of the cortex with
workers rated behavior (Conners and Eisenberg 1963) impressions:
provided further support for its employment as an adjunct
… hyperactivity is the most striking item. This may
to psychotherapy and family counseling. Most research in
be noted from early infancy on or not become
the following years was sponsored by the NIMH, as
prominent until five or six years of age. There are also
pharmaceutical firms were uneasy about perceptions of
a short attention span and poor powers of concen-
pediatric psychopharmacology; Ritalin was, indeed, one of
tration, which are particularly noticeable under
the first agents specifically examined in this age group. It
school conditions. Variability also is frequent, with
was not until the 1970s that it displaced amphetamine as
the child being described as quite unpredictable and
the first choice agent in this regard; Wender commented in
with wide fluctuations in performance. The child is
1971 that it was more expensive than Dexedrine, and that
impulsive and does things ‘‘on the spur of the
its duration of action was shorter; on the other hand, some
moment,’’ without apparent premeditation. Out-
children responded to Ritalin and not to Dexedrine, and
standingly also these children seem unable to tolerate
vice versa (Wender 1971, p. 105). Access to the agent,
any delay in gratification of their needs and demands.
however, was tightened at this point following reports of its
They are irritable and explosive, with low frustration
misuse as a recreational drug; this coincided with increased
tolerance (Laufer et al. 1957).
restrictions being imposed upon amphetamines for the
same reason (Diller 1996; Mayes and Rafalovich 2007). Laufer was aware that there was no evidence for brain
The effectiveness of stimulants on certain aspects of damage apart from the behavioral syndrome in many of
problem behavior contributed to focusing upon these these children, and emphasized that amphetamine therapy
aspects that became key features of ADHD as it is now was only appropriate in combination with psychotherapy.

123
P. B. Foley

In the same year, Leon Eisenberg (Johns Hopkins bring a youngster to a psychiatrist for diagnosis and
Hospital, Baltimore) singled out hyperkinesia and attention treatment. Chess explicitly placed more emphasis on
deficits among the most fundamental features of behavior behavior instead of brain damage in defining the physio-
in his description of the psychiatric implications of brain logical ‘hyperactive behavior syndrome’—where the child
damage in children (Eisenberg 1957), while in 1962, Heinz carries out activities at a higher than normal rate of speed
Prechtl (Groningen; b. 1927) described a choreiform syn- than the average child, or who is constantly in motion, or
drome consisting of specific muscle contractions, as well as both—and at the same regarded the prognosis as less dark
behavioral problems, including hyperactivity and specific than had been usual since the War; Chess associated the
learning difficulties (Prechtl and Stemmer 1962; see also syndrome with neither brain damage nor poor parenting.
Lemke 1953; Wieck 1957). Behavior modification techniques, special education, and
There were thus two separate but overlapping concepts psychotherapy, as well as stimulant medication as last
by the end of the 1950s: that of mild brain damage, and that resort, were appropriate, but the disorder could simplify
of the hyperkinetic child. Both Laufer and Eisenberg have rectify itself by puberty. She was not, however, specifically
been credited with the introduction of the concept of the referring to ADHD, but rather to a syndrome of hyperac-
‘hyperkinetic child’ or ‘hyperactive child’ (including tivity presented in a range of children with different
Barkley 2006), but even Laufer’s ‘hyperkinetic impulse underlying problems (Chess 1960).
disorder’ can only be seen as the provisional culmination of Concepts of ‘minimal brain damage’ were largely
developments underway in Europe and elsewhere since the introduced in an educational–psychological context and
1920s. Part of the confusion arises from the terminology: referred more to perceptual and performance deficits than a
‘hyperkinetic behavior’ itself was interpreted both as a specific neuropsychiatric syndrome. It would, however, to
symptom and as a specific (in the minds of those who used be mistaken to believe that the ‘hyperkinetic child’ was
it) syndrome, the former view having predominated in the overly vague: The picture gleaned from the literature
USA until the early 1960s, the latter reflecting more the changes little over 50 years, as exemplified by a descrip-
European viewpoint. Further, Laufer, for example, regar- tion in 1972:
ded his syndrome as typical for [all] those with mild brain
The ‘‘syndrome of the hyperkinetic child’’ may thus
damage, whereas Europeans regarded the hyperkinetic
be summarized as a youngster, usually male, who is
syndrome as more distinct. ‘Minimal brain dysfunction’
overly active for his age and whose actions are often
(MBD; the successor concept to minimal damage) and
impulsive, random, purposeless, and at times per-
hyperactivity apparently widely confounded as late as the
severative. His attention span is strikingly short; he
mid-1970s:
appears to flit from one task to another, completing
… despite the interchangeable use of MBD and few. He is easily distracted, seemingly responding to
[hyperactivity] in public discussions, the two cate- each and every stimulus in his environment. His
gories are not identical. For one, MBD children are emotional swings are great, so that he is overly
not always hyperactive, although most are. Secondly, affectionate one moment and biting and kicking the
hyperactive children do not always have a learning or next. He appears to test continually the limits of his
perceptual disability, although most do (Safer and environs and seems oblivious to punishment (Gro-
Allen 1976, p. 6). over 1972).
The very concept of ‘hyperactivity’ differed between North The issue of whether hyperactivity should be seen as the
American perceptions, where it referred to a unusually high cornerstone of a specific syndrome, or rather as a symptom
levels of motor activity that disturbed those around the encountered in a variety of contexts, remained nonetheless
child, while in Europe drivenness and extreme excessive- unresolved. The contours of a specific hyperactivity syn-
ness were required (see, for example, Barkley 2006). drome were hazed by the broader, not unreasonable concept
that a range of minor neurodevelopmental defects could
have consequences ranging from subtle sensorimotor and
A specific hyperactivity syndrome without etiologic behavioral consequences to profound mental deficits. This
assumptions lack of definition was perceived as increasingly unsatisfac-
tory during by the early 1960s, as the desire for definitive
In 1960, Stella Chess (1914–2007) noted that Hyperactivity catalogs of defined psychiatric conditions, analogous to the
is one of the common manifestations of disturbed child International Classification of Diseases (ICD) of the World
behavior. … Even when it is not mentioned initially by the Health Organization, became louder. The ICD had included
individuals offering complaints about the child, hypermo- the category ‘primary childhood behaviour disorders’(item
tility often is numbered among the prominent issues which 324) since 1948 (the 6th edition: the first to classify diseases,

123
Sons and daughters beyond your control

not causes of death; World Health Organization 1948, The 56 participants had been invited to submit their
p. 124), while the American Standard classified nomencla- understanding of the phrase ‘minimal brain damage’: 25 %
ture of disease (1933, 1935) had allowed the possibility of did not even approve of the term, while only one (London
including ‘over-activity’ as a specifier in the diagnosis of pediatric psychiatrist Guy Michell 1963) described the
‘primary behavioral disorders in children, neurotic traits,’ a ‘hyperkinetic syndrome’ as being associated with this term.
diagnosis applicable to children of any age to 19 years, More importantly, however, the group urged that this rather
alongside tics, stammering, nail-biting, and similar (cited in amorphous group of conditions should be analyzed into
Helmerichs 2002, p. 36). This latter formulation was specific clinical types, with Prechtl’s choreiform syndrome
essentially adopted in 1952 by the American Psychiatric nominated as one candidate form (Mac Keith 1963a, b; see
Association (APA) for the first edition of the Diagnostic and also pp. 95f).
statistical manual (DSM) in its definition of adjustment In the United States, however, the term was officially
reaction of childhood; attention deficits were not mentioned defined by a taskforce established to restrict its compass in
(Committee on Nomenclature and Statistics of the American such a manner that in practice it included all those
Psychiatric Association 1952, pp. 41f.). encompassed by the older concept, including all manner of
learning, cognitive, and conduct disorders, of whatever
etiology:
Minimal brain dysfunction
The term ‘‘minimal brain dysfunction’’ refers … to
children of near average, average or above average
On the other hand, the term ‘minimal brain damage’ was
general intelligence with certain learning or behav-
itself regarded with increasing scepticism, not merely
ioral disabilities ranging from mild to severe, which
because of the unsubstantiated supposition of ‘brain dam-
are associated with deviations of function of the
age’ on the basis of behavior (see, for instance, discussion
central nervous system. These deviations may mani-
in Birch 1964), but because even the assumption that all
fest themselves by various combinations of impair-
children with brain damage presented the ‘brain damaged
ment in perception, conceptualization, language,
child syndrome’ was dubious. Schulman and colleagues
memory, and control of attention, impulse, or motor
examined the issue in mild retarded children in 1965 and
function (Clements 1966, pp. 9f).
found that distractibility was the only sign consistently
associated with brain damage (regardless of scale or loca- The same author identified no less than 38 different
tion), and that hyperactivity certainly was not (Schulman currently employed clinical descriptors that fell within its
et al. 1965); further, the children were not more active than purview, ranging from Organic Brain Disease, Diffuse
other children per se, but rather unable to suppress activity Brain Damage, Neurophrenia, Organic Behavior Disorder,
when required to do so (Kaspar et al. 1971), foreshadowing Minimal Cerebral Palsy and Cerebral Dys-synchronization
a shift in focus from hyperactivity to attention in inter- Syndrome in the organic sphere to Hyperkinetic Behavior
pretation of such behavior. Syndrome, Character Impulse Disorder, Psychoneurologi-
The term ‘minimal brain dysfunction’ was introduced by cal Learning Disorders, Dyslexia, Clumsy Child Syndrome,
Sam Clements and John Peters in their 1962 discussion of a Hypokinetic Syndrome, Attention Disorders and Interjacent
syndrome including attention deficits and hyperkinesis in Child in the psychological, while no less than 99 symptoms
school children, explicitly avoiding the term ‘damage’ in were accommodated under its umbrella. This inevitably led
order to allow for the possibility of deviation on a genetic to criticism that nothing having been achieved (it was, for
basis or on the basis of a central nervous system matura- example, described by Ross and Ross (1976), p. 13, as an
tional lag (emphasis in original), while also noting that it umbrella term that failed to distinguish what it defined from
referred to the same phenomena as organic learning and other problems) and that the emphasis remained unduly
behavior disorders and minimal brain damage. They also restricted to neurological rather than environmental factors.
specifically rejected the false dichotomy [that] has placed It was important, however, that the definition applied to
most workers in the position of being able to think in terms children not classified as mentally defective.
of only psychogenesis or only organicity (Clements and The task force was also more optimistic: Of the 99
Peters 1962). symptoms, ten occurred more frequently (including
In Europe, the Oxford International Study Group of hyperactivity [1.], emotional lability [4.], disorders of
Child Neurology concluded in 1962 that one should refer to attention [5.], and impulsivity [6.]); moreover, Order is
‘minimal cerebral’ or ‘brain dysfunction’ rather than somewhat salvaged by the fact that certain symptoms do
‘damage’, and this semantic shift was widely adopted in the tend to cluster to form recognizable clinical entities,
literature (‘minimal brain/cerebral damage,’ it should be amongst which was the ‘hyperkinetic syndrome’ (Clements
noted, had not normally been employed as a clinical term). 1966, p. 13).

123
P. B. Foley

The term and the abbreviation ‘MBD’ was then adopted A year later, the ‘hyperkinetic reaction of childhood’ was
by Paul Wender in his influential 1971 handbook, explicitly included in the expanded DSM (DSM-II), defined as a
because he wished to accommodate a large number of disorder being characterized by overactivity, restlessness,
psychiatric conditions of children … under one conceptual distractibility, and short attention span, especially in young
roof (p. 2) and wished to avoid a name that elevated one children; the behavior usually diminishes by adolescence.
symptom or implied a particular etiology. The hyperkinetic The term ‘reaction’ reflected dynamic view of the disorder
form was in his concept perhaps the ‘classic form,’ but not held by Leo Kanner (1896–1981), founder of American
the only clinical type; indeed, hyperkinetic behavior was child psychiatry, and was included in all DSM-II childhood
typical but not necessary for the behavior pattern associ- condition names (Committee on Nomenclature and Statis-
ated with MBD (p. 3), a pattern he regarded as distinct and tics of the American Psychiatric Association 1968,
easy to recognize. Wender argued that the primary psy- pp. 49–51). It did not, however, immediately supplant
chological factors in MBD were decreased experience of ‘minimal brain dysfunction’, and Paul Wender’s 1971
pleasure and pain, high, poorly modulated activation levels, volume with precisely that title indicated that the nomen-
and extroversion; he further cautiously suggested that the clature problem had not yet been settled, even within the
first two factors, in turn, were related to altered catechol- United States, but also specifically established, by the aid
amine metabolism in critical brain regions (Wender 1971). of then new Venn diagrams, that MBD, brain damaged, and
Like ‘minor brain damage,’ minimal brain dysfunction hyperactive were not congruent but rather overlapping
primarily comprised the presentation of sensori-motor classes. In this comprehensive discussion of all aspects of
symptoms and a presumptive organic basis, sometimes the concept (except its history!), Wender also underscored
evidenced by ‘soft neurological signs,’ such as EEG the fact that while identification of MBD in the pre-
abnormalities, altered motor reflexes, speech defects, or adolescent child was relatively straightforward, its diagno-
gait disorders (Satterfield 1973), and was applied to chil- sis in older children was culturally dependent, as most
dren with various types of conduct disorder; hyperkinetic patients in this category were referred because of their
syndrome was a subclass that included around 60 % of antisocial behavior patterns:
cases (Stamm and Kreder 1979).
The archetypal all-American (or Australian) boy,
independent, stubborn, self-willed, other-directed,
would be a miserable failure as a traditional Japanese
The end of prehistory: new names for hyperkinetic child. If a child has MBD proclivities, he is well
disorders advised to be born as a male, and as the son of a
nineteenth century frontiersman with past prison
In 1967, the WHO Seminar on Psychiatric Disorders, sentences (Wender 1971, p. 35).
Classification and Statistics held in Paris as part of the
The DSM-II definition allowed maximum latitude to the
process, begun in 1965, for preparing the 1975 revision of
diagnosing physician. The new category of ‘hyperkinetic
the ICD (ICD-9). One of its findings was that there was a
syndrome of childhood’ (314) ultimately introduced into
need for a category such as ‘hyperkinetic syndrome,’ to be
the ICD-9 in 1975 was, in comparison, more restrictive:
provisionally included in the section on ‘developmental
disorders,’ as it was often encountered by child psychia- Disorders in which the essential features are short
trists. It was characterized by: attention-span and distractibility. In early childhood
the most striking symptom is disinhibited, poorly
extreme over-activity which was poorly organized
organized and poorly regulated extreme overactivity
and poorly regulated by the usual social controls,
but in adolescence this may be replaced by under-
distractibility, short attention span, impulsiveness,
activity. Impulsiveness, marked mood fluctuations
and often also, marked mood fluctuations and
and aggression are also common symptoms. Delays
aggression. Such disorders were much commoner in
in the development of specific skills are often present
boys than in girls; there was often a characteristic
and disturbed, poor relationships are common (World
response to drugs such as amphetamine, and fre-
Health Organization 1977, p. 210).
quently, too, there were associated perceptual diffi-
culties and problems at school. While these This definition reflected in part a shift in emphasis that had
conditions were sometimes associated with organic occurred in the meantime, a shift to the position that
brain pathology, it was generally agreed that this was attention, a psychological faculty, might be more central to
often not the case and that certainly this could not the problem than the observable behavior of hyperactivity,
constitute any part of the diagnostic criteria (Rutter and that hyperkinetic children were perhaps not innately
et al. 1969). more active than others, but less able to control this

123
Sons and daughters beyond your control

activity. From the psychological viewpoint, Roscoe Dyk- 1980, pp. 41–45; American Psychiatric Association 1987,
man and colleagues (Arkansas) had advanced the model of pp. 49–58).
an ‘attentional deficit syndrome’ with an developmental, The introduction to the definition in DSM-III conceded
organic basis that could be expressed as either an excess or that for most of the categories the diagnostic criteria are
deficit in motor activity (Dykman et al. 1971); Wender based on clinical judgment, and have not yet been fully
similarly noted that the most striking and constant validated by data (1980, p. 8) and that alterations were
perceptual-cognitive abnormality of the MBD child is inevitable. In the 1987 revision, however, the symptom
shortness of attention span and poor concentration ability lists were pooled, and the two subtypes effectively merged
(Wender 1971, p. 14). The Canadian Virginia Douglas into ‘Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD).’ It
(Montreal) not only regarded attention deficits and impul- still allowed leeway for interpretation, and this has been a
sivity as more central to the construction of the disorder, subject of criticism, as has the focus on attention deficits at
these were also the problems that responded best to the expense of other mental symptoms. Further clinical
stimulant therapy. Her research at McGill University research restored subtypes in DSM-IV (1994)—predomi-
indicated that hyperactive children were not especially nantly inattentive type, predominantly hyperactive-impul-
distractible under appropriate conditions, particularly if sive type, or a combination of both—and also elevated its
immediate and continuous reinforcement was provided; status by renaming the group to which ADHD belongs
this in turn suggests that reward processing is abnormal in ‘Attention Deficit and Disruptive Behavior Disorders.’
these children, going some way to explaining the benefits Further, the DSM-IV definition had broader support from
of amphetamine therapy. Further, it appears that it may be field trials than previous versions (American Psychiatric
just as important to consider the quality of the hyperactive Association 1994, pp. 78–94).
child’s behavior as its quantity. Because of his short Hyperactivity remains the dominant concept in the next
attention span, he tends to flit from one goal to another. As revision of ICD (ICD-10, 1992), with the category F90.0
a result his behavior is often fragmented and disorganized, ‘Hyperkinetic disorders: Disturbance of activity and
and these qualities may contribute to the impression of attention’ including ‘attention deficit disorder or syndrome
excessive activity (Douglas 1972). She thus identified with hyperactivity’ and ‘attention deficit hyperactivity
deficits in direction and maintenance of attention, elevated disorder.’ This phrasing has been preferred to ‘attention
need for reward, inadequate modulation of arousal levels, deficit disorder’ because the latter implies a knowledge of
and reduced capacity for inhibition of impulsivity as the psychological processes that is not yet available, and it
key features of the syndrome. Douglas also found, suggests the inclusion of anxious, preoccupied, or ‘dreamy’
however, that this impulsivity could persist into adoles- apathetic children whose problems are probably different.
cence and beyond, even if, as was typical, motor restless- However, it is clear that, from the point of view of
ness declined. behaviour, problems of inattention constitute a central
The circle had thus been completed, returning to atten- feature of these hyperkinetic syndromes (World Health
tion problems as the central focus. Douglas’ insights are Organization 1992, p. 206). ICD-10 and DSM-IV had
generally credited with influencing the revision of DSM significantly converged in the definition of the disorder,
title and criteria, pushing it towards a more testable and diverging primarily with respect to its title and, more
attention-focused definition. The new orientation was importantly, in the stringency of the guidelines stipulated
acknowledged in the change in name in DSM-III (1980) to for its diagnosis, particularly with respect to the presenta-
‘Attention Deficit Disorder(ADD) (with or without hyper- tion of all symptoms in all relevant life contexts. Never-
activity).’ DSM-III also established more specific symptom theless, key differences in their defined populations
lists for the three key symptoms (attention deficits, remained, with one study finding that only 11 % of those
hyperactivity, impulsiveness) and also demanded for the satisfying diagnosis of ADHD also met the criteria for
first time the exclusion of other childhood psychiatric hyperkinetic syndrome (Lee et al. 2008).
conditions. Further, there was no longer any explicit The ICD-10 criteria for hyperkinetic disorder remain
assumption of an underlying brain pathophysiology: stricter than those for ADHD in the latest DSM version,
Attention was a purely psychological concept, and the DSM-5 (2013), in that ICD-10 demands both hyperactivity
definition of ADHD descriptive and behavioral, not etio- and attention deficits; ‘hyperactivity’ in DSM-5 includes
logic (psychoanalytic explanations were also abandoned). behaviors not listed in ICD-10, such as excessive talking
‘Oppositional defiant disorder’ and ‘conduct disorder’, still and difficulties in waiting in turn; lack of persistence plays
part of ‘hyperkinetic reaction’ in DSM-II, were also a more prominent role in the ICD concept of the attention
removed from the ADHD group of disorders (although all deficit; the upper age limit for the presentation of symp-
were still clustered together in DSM-III-R under ‘disruptive toms is 7 years in ICD-10, but 12 years in DSM-5 (7 years
behavior disorders’) (American Psychiatric Association in DSM-IV); ICD requires observation of symptoms by the

123
P. B. Foley

clinician, whereas parental and teacher reports are accor- children might, however, also be the prelude to psychiatric
ded equal weight by DSM. disease in adulthood, particularly ‘manie raisonnante’
Nevertheless, the two syndromes share a common def- (here: hypomania), a view shared with Griesinger:
initional core in terms of symptoms; the major difference
Children intended by nature to suffer manie rai-
lies in the freedom of interpretation of these definitions.
sonnante attract attention through their great liveli-
The coming together of the two over the past generation is
ness. Wayward, distracted, mobile, unruly, they owe
expected to continue with the release of ICD-11 (forecast:
to their good memory the little progress they make in
2015): The draft proposal sees the current F90.0 replaced
schools or the lyceum is attributable to their good
by ‘Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder’:
memories. They are always at the bottom of their
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder is a disorder class; the efforts they sometimes undertake to escape
that is characterized by a variable mixture of persis- the lower ranks are usually in vain (Campagne 1869,
tent inattention, hyperactivity and impulsivity of a p. 184).
degree that significantly deviates from what would be
The term instabilité méntale—like ‘psychopathy’ at this
expected given the individual’s general develop-
point, settled somewhere between normalcy and psycho-
mental level, and that begins during childhood or
sis—was applied to these children in 1892 by Charles
adolescence. Inattentiveness is the inability to keep
Boulanger, student of neurologist and Charcot editor
one’s mind focussed on a task. Hyperactivity mani-
Désiré Bourneville (Bicêtre):
fests as excessive movements and difficulty to remain
still. Impulsivity is a tendency for rushed action They are distinguished by a lack of balance in the
without reflecting (World Health Organization 2012). faculties of the mind. They cannot fix their attention
for long, they have no attachment, they must do
Together with the new, currently provisional ‘Attention
otherwise than others. They are intelligent but their
deficit disorder without hyperactivity’ (persistent, signifi-
repeated inconsistencies, their constant eccentricities
cant difficulty sustaining attention on tasks that do not
soon attract attention. It seems that an unknown force
provide a high level of stimulation or frequent rewards), it
pushes them to periodic behavioral lapses that they
will constitute the group ‘Attention deficit disorders’.
cannot explain when asked (cited in Welniarz 2011).
Bourneville adopted the term himself, writing after his
French psychiatry and the ‘unstable child’
retirement that it was more common in the mildly feeble-
minded than in idiots or imbeciles, and described their lack
The French approach to ADHD-like conditions followed a
of impulse control:
somewhat different track, focused on the psychological
concept of ‘instability,’ and adopted in part from Kraepe- The unstable possess an exuberant physical mobility.
lin, rather than the more neurological ‘hyperkinesis.’ They never remain in one place, they repeatedly
French reviewers have attributed this divergence, at least in leave the table without a reason. If they play, they
part, to the fact that French psychiatrists have traditionally quickly pass from one game to another. Their intel-
been concerned with patients of all ages, whereas in other lectual mobility is similar. No sooner do they begin
countries children have been examined by pediatric spe- reading than they want to write or count, etc. They
cialists; further, a psychosocial approach to the organism as are disobedient. … They have sudden impulses, they
a whole has been preferred in France. The difference has run away from school, from their father’s house …
been epitomized as regarding childhood behavior as either They are lavish with their promises, often making
a matter of social adaptation (France) or physiological them with the best of intentions … but some hours
maturation (elsewhere), as the contrast between French or some minutes later do exactly what they had
psychodynamic holism and Anglo-Saxon pragmatism promised to no longer do (Bourneville 1897,
(reviewed: Micouin and Boucris 1988; Joly 1996; Welniarz p. LXXXVIIf).
2011).
Bourneville distinguished between the ‘unstable’ and the
This interpretation is somewhat, but it is true that French
merely undisciplined child in Les enfants anormaux au
interpretations pursued a separate path for much of the
point de vue intellectuel et moral:
twentieth century. The French literature includes numerous
descriptions of hyperkinetic behavior in children with The unstable child can dwell on nothing: the
intellectual disabilities from at least as far back as Edourd impressions that he receives via his senses follow one
Seguin’s 1846 Traitement moral, hygiène et éducation des another, through lack of attention, so fleetingly, that
idiots. There was some suspicion that hyperactivity in they do not give rise to a persistent idea. The

123
Sons and daughters beyond your control

dynamogenic power of his ideas manifests itself débilité motrice; it was characterized by instability of mood
abruptly, but its transience increases with its strength. and motor function, and difficulty in maintaining attention
… If he moves in class, it is because he cannot (Dupré and Merklen 1910); the term ‘debilité psychomot-
remain still; his physical instability is correlated with rice’ was still be used as the French pendant to the English
his mental instability. While the undisciplined child ‘minimal brain dysfunction’ during the 1960s (Bax and
can achieve something in the long term if it wants to, Mac Keith 1963, p. 97). Georges Heuyer (1884–1977)
especially something it has not been told to do, the pursued this psychomotor instability model further in his
unstable child achieves nothing. Before executing 1914 thesis Les enfants anormaux et les délinquants juve-
any decision, another emerges, which in turn will be niles, although his interest was more in those who would be
displaced by a third. described as ‘moral imbeciles’ in England, delineating
three key identifying symptoms in these children: attention
The unruly is dangerous.—The unstable is useless
deficits, hyperactivity, and perverse, incorrigible behavior
(Bourneville 1905, p. 18).
(Heuyer 1914).
Jean Demoor (1867–1941), physician at a school education Following the First World War, Guy Vermeylen was
school in Brussels, similarly reported a condition that has among the first to employ psychological testing in these
received insufficient attention from psychiatry that he children, and concluded that those with attention deficits,
termed mental chorea: paired with excellent memories and vivid imaginations,
were essentially the result of unharmonious development
Mental chorea is clearly a disease of attention, during
of different mental functions, retaining the same mobility of
which the process [attention] does not acquire suffi-
impressions and desires, the same scattering of attention,
cient strength to become an inhibiting factor. Under
the same discontinuity in thought and action, the same
these conditions, the sick child remains essentially
fickleness of feelings (Vermeylen 1922, p. 70) appropriate
reflexive, variable, unstable and unconscious. The
to preschoolers: His interest cannot be sustained in one
lack of attention causes … the lack of will and
direction for long, and requires immediate and repeated
therefore the fatal defect of pre-attention (Demoor
satisfaction to justify continuation of the action (p. 75).
1900).
This neurodevelopmental interpretation was pursued fur-
These children experienced a constant need for movement ther in the monumental 1925 thesis by Henri Wallon
and change as well as lability of mood. This disorder, (1879–1962), L’enfant turbulent, who defined several
uncommon in children with marked intellectual handicaps, anatomically based levels of instability in children, each
was linked by Demoor to deficient muscular proprioception affecting different aspects of psychomotor function, thus
(in more recent terms)—according to the Flechsig model of providing a neurophysiological basis for the disharmony of
volition, If sensations are defective in their intensity or Vermeylen, while at the same time emphasizing that this
their quality, attention phenomena are fleeting and con- was an evolving, dynamic brain situation, not a static
fused, and volition will be without energy and defective— condition (Wallon 1925). His model also emphasized the
and his therapeutic approach involved ‘progressive mus- close association of psychiatric function with abnormalities
cular discipline’. Jean Philippe and Georges Paul-Boncourt of both voluntary and involuntary movement that were
(Paris) noted in the same year that: widely explored in the context of psychomotor integration
during the 1920s, a direction that had arisen prior to the
The unstable schoolchild is a mentally abnormal
outbreak of EL, but was nonetheless heavily influenced by
child, who cannot fix his attention either to listen, or
the experience of this neuropsychiatric disorder.
to answer, or to understand. It is in vain that he is
The culmination of this line of thinking was the 1940
brought back to subject: forever and in spite of
monograph by Heuyer’s former student, Jadwiga Abram-
himself his mind turns elsewhere, and it should be
son, L’enfant et l’adolescent instables. Études cliniques et
noted that physical instability is often not less pro-
psychologiques, a volume seen as sealing the parting of the
nounced than mental instability (Philippe and Paul-
ways between French instabilité and the ‘hyperkinetic
Boncour 1905, p. 46; see also Paul-Boncour 1919).
child’ concept. Abramson was particularly interested in the
The authors excluded children with marked intelligence psychology of the unstable child as assessed by laboratory
deficits, hysteria, epilepsy, or developmental delays from testing. Abramson argued that disequilibrium between
this definition, and warned that it was not a diagnosis, but emotional, intellectual development can produce instability
rather a symptom encountered in a number of conditions. in the child (‘instability’ that was natural at certain stages
In 1909, French psychiatrist Ernest Dupré (1862–1921) of personality development, such as infancy, the com-
included instabilité psychomotrice as a form of his broader mencement of schooling, and puberty), and her testing of
integration of motor and mental function in his concept of these functions indicated that the unstable child possessed a

123
P. B. Foley

good memory and could complete tasks requiring only syndrome of ICD-10. The CFTMEA places greater
short-term attention but not those requiring more sustained emphasis upon social and psychological than neurobio-
application, temporal or spatial coordination, and speed; logical factors, with clear consequences for therapy.
their actions and reactions were impulsive and deficient in ‘Hyperactivity’ has, indeed, been criticized by many
continuity and connectedness, their swinging moods and French authors not primarily for its own sake, but because
heightened suggestibility were combined with an opposi- of its association with stimulant therapy (for example,
tion to the stable and organized. Voluntary direction of Diatkine and Fréjaville 1972), an approach still regarded
attention was poor, with the child being distracted by any with scepticism by French psychiatry. The rapprochement
external stimulus, or fixated upon a particular detail; ana- between the two concepts was nevertheless advanced by
lysis of complex situations or stimuli was also deficient. the revision of 2000, which listed Troubles hyperkinétique,
While organic factors could not be ignored, the dynamic with subtypes defined by the presence or absence of
nature of the instability, and its dependence upon social attention deficits; these syndromes had also been trans-
and other environmental factors were also emphasized ferred to the new class of ‘Conduct disorders’ (current
(Abramson 1940; see also similar by the Nijmegen author edition: Misès 2012, pp. 56–58). The rapprochement of the
Chorus 1942/1943). Heuyer himself later emphasized that two directions was thus undertaken by the beginning of the
these factors could, in fact, foster ‘conditioned psycho- new century, although a major 1997 French textbook on
motor instability’ (Heuyer and Lebovici 1951). pre-adult psychiatry omitted mention of either the instable
The ‘unstable child’ in French literature was thus or hyperactive child. Ritalin was approved for the treat-
broader than current ADHD, encompassing all those in ment of hyperactive children in France in 1995, but con-
whom age-appropriate psychomotor development was tinued opposition has constrained its adoption.
disturbed, regardless of etiology, and was thus similar to There is no space here to enter into discussions of
the Anglo-Saxon ‘brain-damaged child’ in the 1950s in its whether the divided path hereto was the result of the
scope, including the mythomaniac, the imaginative, the imperialist hegemony of Anglo-Saxon theories and retreat
impulsive, the simulator; but we also see him among the into protectionism by some French specialists in the
instinctive perverts, among the hysterical, the epileptics apparatus of the soul (Joly 2005), but much of this litera-
and the erratic (Abramson 1940, p. 12). The concept, ture is unfamiliar to most non-French readers (all the more
however, declined in use from the early 1960s, presumably striking given the positive reception of the work of Binet,
under the impression of discussions of hyperactivity in the Piaget, and others regarding child development). The main
English language literature. In 1968, Nicole Dopchie 1968 explanation is ultimately rather that French authors long
reviewed the international literature concerning the regarded hyperactivity/instabilité as a psychological–
‘hyperkinetic child,’ thereby introducing the term into developmental issue, whereas their non-French colleagues
French employment; in 1973, neuropsychiatrist Julian de were focused on the neurological aspects of the question.
Ajuriaguerra (1911–1993) published a detailed comparison The convergence of the two directions lies in the recog-
of the instable and hyperactive child models, wherein he nition that ADHD is indubitably a multifactorial and
criticized both the American ignorance of the French multidimensional disorder, where the impossible choice
model, and their assumption that response to stimulant between plague and cholera, between the rejection of any
medication was specific to hyperkinetic children: we still genuine psychopathologic option and the obliteration of
believe that the dual polarity as we described in psycho- the complexity of levels and systems of human functioning
motor instability remains valid as a heuristic point of view, (Joly 2005) is no longer necessary nor desired.
and is valid in the case of hyperkinetic syndromes. For us,
psychomotor instability is the psychomotor aspect of a
disorganized core (de Ajuriaguerra 1973). He averred that Overview of more recent developments
the French concept involved both motor and psychological
aspects, and that both were modified by interaction with an The essential nature of ADHD was by no means settled by
adult environment that did not tolerate instability. naming it in the DSM or ICD catalogs, and during the
In view of continued perceived shortcomings of both the 1970s, the explosion of literature provoked controversy as
DSM and ICD classifications of psychiatric conditions in much as it illuminated the subject. Only the major features
children and adolescents, Roger Misès and colleagues of this literature can be discussed here: While annual
introduced in 1988 the Classification française des troubles number of items concerning ‘attention deficit disorders’ or
mentaux de l’enfant et de l’adolescent (CFTMEA), ‘hyperactivity disorder’ listed by MedLine was less than
including under ‘Disorders of instrumental functions and 100 during the 1970s, it had grown to 300 annually in the
learning’ the clinical type hyperkinésies, instabilities mid-1990s, and nearly 2,000 today, a figure that does not
psychomotrices, corresponding to the hyperkinetic include most conference presentations or books.

123
Sons and daughters beyond your control

A catecholamine hypothesis of hyperactivity was first inherited appears to be a set of traits rather than an illness,
proposed in 1970—catecholamine hypotheses had been and a set of dispositions to react to the environmental
successfully introduced for Parkinson’s disease, schizo- associations of ADHD (Taylor 2011).
phrenia, and depression in the course of the 1960s—but did It is now recognized that so-called co-morbidity—the
not prove as fruitful as originally hoped. Nonetheless, the presentation of other behavioral, affective, or motor dis-
involvement of catecholamine systems is central to current orders by those suffering ADHD—is more common than
neurophysiological models of ADHD, although, as Bau- formerly thought, co-morbidities being identified in nearly
meister and colleagues have recently commented, the 70 % of cases in one large study. The developmental
models proposed are entirely consistent with ideas that relationships between these conditions are complex and
were put forward by researchers in the 1930s (Baumeister remain an important feature of ongoing pathophysiological,
et al. 2012). The 1990s witnessed an increase in research psychological, and therapeutic research. DSM-5 explicitly
consistent with older assumptions of localized brain dys- allows diagnosis of co-morbid ADHD and autism
function. A number of specific brain abnormalities have (reviewed: Taurines et al. 2010).
since been identified in the brains of ADHD children, In 1976, Borland and Heckman reported that half of the
including localized dopamine and noradrenaline abnor- men who had been hyperactive continued to show a num-
malities, altered fronto-cortical and striatal perfusion and ber of major symptoms of hyperactivity. … despite normal
metabolism, and EEG dysfunction, but a comprehensive IQ scores and levels of education, men who were hyper-
neurophysiological model of ADHD remains a hope for the active had never achieved a socioeconomic status equal
future. This contributes to the fact that only one major new their brothers. … Our findings suggest that emotional
medication has been introduced since methylphenidate, problems in everyday living may result from the persistence
atomoxetine (Strattera), a noradrenaline re-uptake inhib- of symptoms of hyperactivity (Borland and Heckman 1976;
itor that also hinders dopamine re-uptake and antagonizes see also Mendelson et al. 1971). This was among the ear-
NMDA receptors. Its absence or abuse potential is highly liest acknowledgements that the attention deficits and
regarded, but its precise mechanism of action in ADHD associated psychological features might persist into adult-
remains to be determined. hood, even if hyperactivity of childhood type diminished
Neuropsychological instruments has been developed for during puberty. This has been the subject of a burgeoning
the standardized assessment of behavior, allowing greater literature on this since 2000, with some finding evidence
comparability between studies in different laboratories. that ADHD may be a life-long condition. This is recog-
Further, investigations since the 1980s have suggested that nized in DSM-5, but remains controversial (reviewed:
motivation functions played a more fundamental role than Doyle 2004; Blondeau et al. 2009; Nylander 2011; Guld-
attention in the disorder; while this possibility was implicit berg-Kjär 2013).
in Douglas’s work, newer research investigating specific
aspects of attention (particularly with regard to perception
processing and filtering) indicated that deficits could not be Final comments
detected in these areas. Further, it had long been recog-
nized that hyperkinetic behavior was often manifested to The 1970s witnessed the first major upsurge of public
varying degrees in different environments, and this led to scepticism regarding the ‘reality’ of ADHD as a medical
both neurological (association with reduced cortico-limbic condition, with vigorous proposition of the viewpoint from
perfusion; effect of stimulant drugs) and psychological some quarters that hyperactivity was simply part of the
models of deficient stimulus response control (reviewed: normal range of childhood energy (for example, Conrad
Barkley 2006; review of motivation studies: Modesto- 1975: psychiatry and public health have always been
Lowe et al. 2013). concerned with social behavior and have traditionally
That mental disorders tend to cluster in certain families functioned as agents of social control; as well as popular
has been documented since the early twentieth century, and books such as The myth of the hyperactive child and other
that this also applies to hyperactivity and attention deficits means of child control: Schrag and Divoky 1975). Accu-
has been recognized since the 1970s (see, for example, sations that inadequate parents and teachers are demanding
Morrison and Stewart 1971), but only more recent genetic medical approval and support for the pharmacologic con-
investigations have enabled the disentangling of heredity trol of unruly children (at a time when overindulgence of
and environment in the development of ADHD (recent adults in pharmacologic solutions of their own problems
review: Caylak 2013). Although it has now been reported attracted less attention), as well of conspiracy theories
that the former may explain 75 % of the variance in its alleging that pharmaceutical profits are the driving force
incidence (Faraone 2005), it is recognized that genes alone behind the rapid increase in the diagnosis of hyperactivity
do not account for the phenomenon ADHD: What is disorders and the prescription of stimulant therapy over the

123
P. B. Foley

past half century have in the meantime, if anything, This naturally establishes the usefulness/validity of the
increased rather than diminished. Consensus statements on nosologic entity ‘ADHD’ in the context of current psy-
the nature of ADHD by medical experts (Taylor et al. chiatric thought: It does not mean that there is not more to
2004; appendix A to Barkley 2006) have not resolved these learn, nor does it validate the application of any particular
differences, and the divide between the camps comparable therapeutic approach; equally importantly, it does not
in stability with the trenches of the First World War (dis- avoid the careless or inappropriate extrapolation of the
cussed in Mayes and Rafalovich 2007). These arguments, diagnosis of ‘ADHD.’ It does not establish that ADHD is a
however, are generally rooted in concerns about the treat- syndrome with a single etiology (cf. Furman 2005), but nor
ment of children with stimulant medication, and can only does anybody seriously maintain that it is anything but a
be sensibly addressed by separating the two issues. behavioral syndrome that can result from any of a number
The figure of the ‘hyperkinetic’ or ‘hyperactive child’ of primary causes, the effects of which converge on pro-
has been remarkably constant for at more than a century. cesses of attention and motivation. Individualized care is
The incidence of hyperactivity in children, however, has therefore necessary. The tendency is towards viewing
long been a matter of controversy, and rates of hyperactivity ADHD as a multivariate entity, each component of which
in children as high as 10 % prior to the Second World War is distributed within the community, and only extreme
were noted above. In a review of several British studies, values are perceived as problematic, and then only where
Bax commented in 1972 that he believe[d] in the existence familial, social and medical contexts both register and
of the relatively rare hyperkinetic syndrome and do not assess these values. On the other hand, the evidence for
believe it is usually too difficult to distinguish between the underlying genetic and neurological bases for this behavior
hyperkinetic and the over-active child. … The child with is similarly clear. ADHD is ultimately a classic example of
true hyperkinesia has a short attention span and excess the interaction between genetic, environmental and social
motor activity, characteristics which are consistently factors in determining the development and assessment of
observed by all adults who come into contact with the child, personality and behavior; in the end treatment must,
and the presence of a familiar adult has little effect on his however, focus on the welfare of the child and not only on
behaviour (Bax 1972). In his view, there were numerous the convenience of those who interact with them, and, for
other reasons that a child might be especially active—other this reason, be specific for a given individual.
psychiatric disorder, unrecognized learning difficulties, Attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)
cultural or language differences, boredom, high energy may not be a psychiatric disorder of the same quality as, for
levels—that were not genuinely ‘hyperkinetic’ in sensu example, schizophrenia, and is not directly life threatening.
strictu. The latter point is important, and is reflected in Attention deficits nevertheless indubitably cause distress to
differences between North American and European of the child of normal or near-normal intelligence. Both
hyperactivity: The latter emphasizes the fact that unusually attention deficits and hyperactive behavior exert negative
high motor activity can be presented as part of a number of effects on their schooling, on their relationship with their
disorders (or simply as a character trait), but only qualifies family, and on their broader social development, and
as hyperkinetic disorder when combined with attention therefore requires remediation. This is particularly true
deficits. According to recent studies cited in DSM-5, the now that the chronic nature of the problem has been rec-
international incidence of childhood ADHD was about 5 %, ognized: Even if the hyperactivity becomes less external,
with no significant difference between North America and the internal restlessness and its attendant impact on mental
Europe, and that of adult ADHD is 2.5 % (American Psy- functions may represent a major impediment to a fulfilled
chiatric Association 2013, pp. 59–66). life, and needs to be addressed when it is most amenable to
The fact remains that ADHD is perhaps the best correction, in childhood. The assistance should as gentle as
investigated pediatric disorder of any kind. Faraone possible, with pharmacologic therapy, as in all areas of life,
reviewed the evidence for ADHD being a syndrome that a last resort to reduce frictions between the child and their
meets the standards set by Robins and Guze (1970), world. Decisions as to what constitutes appropriate support
essentially the criteria employed in the construction of in these cases will be a question that involves conscience
DSM-III, concluding that all six were clearly satisfied: and ethical considerations as much as it does medicine.
The diagnosis has well-defined clinical correlates and can It behooves us to remember why the concept of the
be distinguished from other diagnoses; the disorder has a ‘hyperkinetic child’ entered medicine: The concern has
characteristic course and outcome, shows evidence of always generally related to the impact of the condition on
heritability (family and genetic studies), and exhibits a the child’s scholastic and social development; it has indeed
characteristic response to treatment; laboratory studies been argued that the discovery of attention disorders and
have identified other neurobiological correlates of the hyperactivity was the direct result of the expansion of state-
disorder (Faraone 2005). sponsored mass education at the turn from the nineteenth to

123
Sons and daughters beyond your control

the twentieth centuries (Brancaccio 2000). This is inaccu- American Psychiatric Association (1980) DSM-III. Diagnostic and
rate, but it reminds us that the focus must be on helping the statistical manual of mental disorders, 3rd edn. American
Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC
child master their psychological difficulties for their own American Psychiatric Association (1987) DSM-III-R. Diagnostic and
sake, while not ignoring those around them: statistical manual of mental disorders (third edition—revised).
American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC
With forty-five to fifty children in a classroom, it can American Psychiatric Association (1994) DSM-IV. Diagnostic and
be unbearably disruptive to have among them a child statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th edn. American
who, despite seemingly good intelligence, cannot sit Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC
American Psychiatric Association (2013) DSM-5. Diagnostic and
still, cannot keep his mind on his work, minds other
statistical manual of mental disorders, 5th edn. American
pupils business instead of his own, hardly finishes the Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC
work assigned, and yet, unpredictably, may turn in a Anderson FN, Mambar R (1939) Post-encephalitic behavior. Social
perfect paper. As a result, the teacher tends to put Work Tech 4:67–72
Anderson GH (1923) The sequelae of epidemic encephalitis in
increased pressure upon the child to conform and to
childhood, with notes on the prognosis as regards complete
live up to apparent capabilities. Since he is unable to recovery. Q J Med 16:173–192
comply, strife and unhappiness result (Laufer and Anderson WW (1963) The hyperkinetic child: a neurological
Denhoff 1957). appraisal. Neurology 13:968–973
Anonymus (1860) Unstätigkeit der Kinder. In: Ein Verein praktischer
Many are uncomfortable describing attention deficits as Leher und Erzieher (ed) Encyklopädie der Pädagogik vom
constituting a ‘disorder,’ and it might be wise to avoid gegenwärtigen Standpunkte der Wissenschaft und nach den
Erfahrungen der gefeierten Pädagogen aller Zeiten. Zweiter
doing so, as advised in 1938 by Paul Schröder (1873–1941; Band. Ernst Schäfer, Leipzig, pp 705–706
Leipzig) with respect to what were then described as Anonymus (1890) Zerstreute Kinder. Allgemeine Deutsche Le-
‘psychopathies’: after noting that it was impossible to hrerzeitung 42:53–56
define ‘disease’ and ‘pathologic’ precisely, he argued that Anonymus (1961) Schizophrenic syndrome in childhood. Br Med J
2:944–947
the boundaries of character development were wide: Barkley RA (2006) History. In: Attention deficit-hyperactivity
disorder. A handbook for diagnosis and treatment. Guildford
It is essentially advisable not to regard all abnormal Press, New York, pp 3–75
children (apart from those few who, in contrast, are Barkley RA, Peters H (2012) The earliest reference to ADHD in the
clearly mentally or neurologically ill) as patients or medical literature? Melchior Adam Weikard’s description in
that they should be evaluated and treated as such 1775 of ‘‘attention deficit’’ (Mangel der Aufmerksamkeit,
attentio volubilis). J Atten Disord 16:623–630
(Schröder 1938). Baumeister AA, Henderson K, Pow JL, Advokat C (2012) The early
For the same reason, he noted, the term ‘child psychiatry,’ history of the neuroscience of attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder. J Hist Neurosci 21:263–279
he wrote, was at this point struggling to replace the older Bax M (1972) The active and the over-active school child. Dev Med
term ‘special education’ (Heilpädagogik). Child Neurol 14:83–86
Despite growing consensus on the definition of ADHD, Bax M, Mac Keith R (eds) (1963) Minimal cerebral dysfunction. In:
cultural differences, including those between modern Papers from the International Study Group held at Oxford,
September 1962. Little Club clinics in developmental medicine;
Western states, will determine how this definition is 10. National Spastics Society Medical Education and Informa-
interpreted, and will also influence which consequences, tion Unit; William Heinemann Medical Books, London
including choice of therapy, will follow this interpretation. Bergmann W (1917) Unruhe. In: Roloff EM (ed) Lexikon der
I hope that the prehistory I have presented in this paper has Pädagogik. Fünfter Band. Herdersche Verlagshandlung, Freiburg
im Breisgau, pp 339–343
illustrated how these questions have been addressed since Birch HG (1964) The problem of ‘‘brain damage’’ in children. In:
the birth of ‘scientific medicine’ in the early nineteenth Brain damage in children. The biological and social aspects.
century, but I would not presume to prescribe how a parent Williams and Wilkins, Baltimore, pp 1–12
or physician should respond to a child in 2013. The general Blondeau C, Rénéric J-P, Martin-Guehl C, Bouvard M (2009) Le
trouble déficit de l’attention/hyperactivité à l’âge adulte: con-
precept of medicine—above all, do no harm—remains the cept, tableau clinique, stratégies diagnostiques et thérapeutiques.
guiding principle, but this does not justify not providing Ann Médico-Psychol 167:234–242
assistance to a child when such assistance is required for Bonazza S, Scaglione C, Poppi M, Rizzo G (2011) Did Goethe
their personal security and social development. describe attention deficit hyperactivity disorder? Eur Neurol
65:70–71
Bonhoeffer K (1922) Psychische Residuärzustände nach Encephalitis
epidemica bei Kindern. Klin Wochenschr 1:1446–1449
Borland BL, Heckman HK (1976) Hyperactive boys and their brothers:
References a 25-year follow-up study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 33:669–675
Borthwick GA (1927) Some observations on the after-effects of
Abramson J (1940) L’enfant et l’adolescent instables. Études encephalitis lethargica in children. In: Metropolitan Asylums
cliniques et psychologiques. Alcan, Paris Board, Annual report for the year 1926–1927. 29th year of issue.

123
P. B. Foley

Adopted by the Board on 23 July, 1927. Harrison and Sons, de Sanctis S (1923) Les enfants dysthymiques. Encéphale 18:1–7,
London, pp 148–167 88–93, 156–163
Bourneville (1897) Recherches cliniques et thérapeutiques sur de Sanctis S (1934) La instabilità. Cervello 13:90–113
l’épilepsie, l’hystérie et l’idiotie. Compte-rendu du service des Demoor J (1900) La chorée mentale et son traitement. Bulletin de la
enfants idiots, épileptiques et arriérés de Bicêtre pendant l’année Société royale des sciences médicales et naturelles de Bruxelles
1896 (vol 17). Progrès médical; Félix Alacan, Paris 58:88–95
Bourneville (1905) Les enfants anormaux au point de vue intellectuel Diatkine R, Fréjaville JP (1972) Des amphétamines pour les enfants
et moral. Progrès Médical, Paris hyperkinétiques: danger de toxicomanie ou danger idéologique?
Bradley C (1937) The behavior of children receiving Benzedrine. Am Concours Méd 94:497–502
J Psychiatry 94:577–585 Diller LH (1996) The run on Ritalin: attention deficit disorder and
Bradley C, Bowen M (1941) Amphetamine (Benzedrine) sulfate stimulant treatment in the 1990s. Hastings Cent Rep 26:12–18
therapy of children’s behavior disorders. Am J Orthopsychiatr Doll EA, Phelps WM, Melcher RT (1932) Mental deficiency due to
11:92–103 birth injury. The Macmillan Company, New York
Brancaccio MT (2000) Educational hyperactivity: the historical Dopchie N (1968) Le syndrome hyperkinétique. Revue de la
emergence of a concept. Intercult Educ 11:165–177 littérature. Psychiatrie de l’Enfant 11:589–619
Campagne (1869) Traité de la manie raisonnante. Victor Masson et Douglas VI (1972) Stop, look and listen: The problem of sustained
fils, Paris attention and impulse control in hyperactive and normal
Caylak E (2013) Biochemical and genetic analyses of childhood children. Can J Behav Sci 4:259–282
attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Am J Med Genet Part B, Doyle R (2004) The history of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity
Neuropsychiatr Genet 159B:613–627 disorder. Psychiatr Clin North Am 27:203–214
Chess S (1960) Diagnosis and treatment of the hyperactive child. N Y Dupré EP, Merklen PJ (1910) La débilité motrice dans ses rapports
State J Med 60:2379–2385 avec la débilité mentale et l’insuffisance pyramidale physiolog-
Childers AT (1935) Hyperactivity in children having behavior ique du premier âge. Congrès des médecins aliénistes et
disorders. Am J Orthopsychiatr 5:227–243 neurologistes de France et des pays de langue française. XIXme
Chorus AMJ (1942/1943) Les enfants instables. J Psychiatr Infant Congrès, session de Nantes, août 1909. G. Masson, Paris
3:137–142, 161–173 Dykman RA, Ackerman PT, Clements SD, Peters JE (1971) Specific
Clements SD (1966) Minimal brain dysfunction in children. Termi- learning disabilities: An attentional deficit syndrome. Prog Learn
nology and identification. Phase one of a three phase project. Disabil 2:56–94
Public Health Service publication; 1415. US Department of Editorial (1964) ‘‘Minimal brain damage’’. Public Health 78:59–61
Health, Education and Welfare, Washington Ehrenfest H (1922) Birth injuries of the child. Gynecological and
Clements SD, Peters JE (1962) Minimal brain dysfunctions in the obstetrical monographs, vol 6. D. Appleton and Company, New
school-age child. Diagnosis and treatment. Arch Gen Psychiatry York
6:185–197 Eisenberg L (1957) Psychiatric implications of brain damage in
Clouston TS (1899) States of over-excitability, hypersensitiveness, children. Psychiatr Q 31:72–92
and mental explosiveness in children, and their treatment by the Eisenberg L (1964) Behavioral manifestations of cerebral damage in
bromides. Scott Med Surg J 4:481–490 childhood. In: Brain damage in children. The biological and
Committee on Nomenclature and Statistics of the American Psychi- social aspects. Williams and Wilkins, Baltimore, pp 61–76
atric Association (1952) Diagnostic and statistical manual. Eisenberg L (1971) Principles of drug therapy in child psychiatry with
Mental disorders. American Psychiatric Association Mental special reference to stimulant drugs. Am J Orthopsychiatr
Hospital Service, Washington 41:371–379
Committee on Nomenclature, Statistics of the American Psychiatric Emminghaus H (1887) Die psychischen Störungen des Kindesalters.
Association (1968) DSM-II. Diagnostic and statistical manual of Handbuch der Kinderkrankheiten. Nachtrag 2. H. Laupp’schen
mental disorders, 2nd edn. American Psychiatric Association, Buchhandlung, Tübingen
Washington Everberg G (1957) A family-study with otological, neurological, and
Conners CK, Eisenberg L (1963) The effects of methylphenidate on ophthalmological aspects (unilateral deafness, speech defect,
symptomatology and learning in disturbed children. Am J dyslexiua, petit mal, aphasia (Kramer–Pollnow syndrome?),
Psychiatry 120:458–464 astigmatism (amblyopia), disseminated sclerosis and goitre).
Conrad P (1975) The discovery of hyperkinesis: notes on the Acta Psychiatr Scand 32:307–324
medicalization of deviant behavior. Soc Probl 12:12–21 Fairweather DS (1947) Psychiatric aspects of the post-encephalitic
Creak M, O’Gorman G, Winnicott DW (1952) Discussion: psychoses syndrome. J Mental Sci 93:201–254
in childhood (abridged) [Section of Psychiatry, March 11, 1952]. Faraone SV (2005) The scientific foundation for understanding
Proc R Soc Med 45:797–802 attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder as a valid psychiatric
Crichton A (1798) An inquiry into the nature and origin of mental disorder. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 14:1–10
derangement. Comprehending a concise system of the physiol- Fuchs A (1896) Die Unruhe. Evang Schulbl
ogy and pathology of the human mind and a history of the 185–206(235–249):300–311
passions and their effects, vol 1. T. Cadell, junior, and W. Furman L (2005) What is attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder
Davies, London (ADHD)? J Child Neurol 20:994–1002
Czerny A (1922) Der Arzt als Erzieher des Kindes, 6th edn. Franz Gesell A, Amatruda CS (1941) Developmental diagnosis. Normal and
Deuticke, Leipzig abnormal child development. Clinical methods and pediatric
Davison KL, Crowcroft NS, Ramsay ME, Brown DWG, Andrews NJ applications. Paul P. Hoeber, New York, London
(2003) Viral encephalitis in England, 1989–1998: What did we Griesinger W (1867) Die Pathologie und Therapie der psychischen
miss? Emerg Infect Dis 9:234–240 Krankheiten, für Ärzte und Studirende, 2nd edn. Adolf Krabbe,
de Ajuriaguerra J (1973) Comparaisons entre les notions de syndrome Stuttgart
d’instabilité psychomotrice et de syndrome hyperkinétique. Groover RV (1972) The hyperkinetic child. Psychiatr Ann 2:36–41
Cinquantenaire de l’hôpital Henri Rousselle: 1922–1972. Edi- Grünewald H (1902) Die Zerstreutheit der Kinder. Z Kinderforsch
tions Sandoz, Paris, pp 129–137 7:16–24

123
Sons and daughters beyond your control

Guldberg-Kjär T (2013) ADHD in old age: Self-rated symptoms and Kleist K (1908) Untersuchungen zur Kenntnis der psychomotorischen
clinical information from a population-based Swedish sample Bewegungsstörungen bei Geisteskranken. Bibliothek medizini-
aged 65 and older. Thesis. University of Gothenburg scher Monographien, 8. Dr. Werner Klinkhardt, Leipzig
Gurewitsch M (1925) Über die Formen der motorischen Un- Kleist K (1927) Bewegungsstörungen und Bewegungsleistungen der
zulänglichkeit. Zeitschrift für die gesamte Neurologie und Stammganglien des Gehirns (Myostase und Psychomotorik).
Psychiatrie 98:510–517 Naturwissenschaften 15:973–977
Hall A (1925) Discussion on the mental sequelae of encephalitis Knobel M (1959) A syndromic approach to ‘‘acting-out’’ children. Dis
lethargica [Sections of Psychiatry, Neurology, Study of Disease Nerv Syst 20:80–87
in Children, and Epidemiology]. Proc R Soc Med 18(Joint Knobel M (1962) Psychopharmacology for the hyperkinetic child.
discussion):20–21 Dynamic considerations. Arch Gen Psychiatry 3:198–202
Hallervorden J (1933) Review of Kramer and Pollnow, ‘Über eine Knobel M, Wolman MB, Mason E (1959) Hyperkinesis and
hyperkinetische Erkrankung im Kindesalter’ (1932). Zentralblatt organicity in children. AMA Arch Gen Psychiatry 1:310–321
für die gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie 65:37–38 Knobloch H, Pasamanick B (1959) Syndrome of minimal cerebral
Haslam J (1809) Observations on madness and melancholy: including damage in infancy. J Am Med Assoc 170:1384–1387
practical remarks on those diseases; together with cases: and an Knobloch H, Rider R, Harper P, Pasamanick B (1956) Neuropsychi-
account of the morbid appearances on dissection, 2nd edn. atric sequelae in prematurity: a longitudinal study. J Am Med
J. Callow, London Assoc 161:581–585
Heinze H (1932) Das Syndrom der dranghaften Erethie im Kindes- Kramer F (1927) Haltlose Psychopathen. In: Deutscher Verein zur
alter. Zeitschrift für die gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie Fürsorge für Jugendliche Psychopathen e.V., Bericht über die
141:469–488 vierte Tagung über Psychopathenfürsorge Düsseldorf, 24–25
Helmerichs R (2002) Historical definitions and nomenclatures of the September 1926. Julius Springer, Berlin, pp 35–94
label ‘ADHD’: An investigation into attention-deficit and Kramer and Pollnow (1930) Hyperkinetische Zustandsbilder im
hyperactive behavior through time. Thesis: University of Wis- Kindesalter [Berliner Gesellschaft für Psychiatrie und Nervenk-
consin-Stout rankheiten. Sitzung vom 16. VI. 1930]. Zentralblatt für die
Heuyer G (1914) Enfants anormaux et délinquants juvéniles. Néces- gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie 57:844–845
sité de l’examen psychiatrique des écoliers. Thesis, Paris Kramer F, Pollnow H (1932) Über eine hyperkinetische Erkrankung
Heuyer G, Lebovici S (1951) Forme conditionée de l’instabilité im Kindesalter. Monatsschr Psychiatr Neurol 82:1–40
psychomotrice. Arch Fr Pédiatr 8:548–549 Lange KW, Reichl S, Lange KM, Tucha L, Tucha O (2010) The
Hoff H (1956) Lehrbuch der Psychiatrie. Verhütung, Prognostik und history of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. ADHD Attent
Behandlung der geistigen und seelischen Erkrankungen. Psy- Deficit Hyperact Disord 2:241–255
chohygiene, 10–11. Benno Schwabe, Basel, Stuttgart Lange W (1933) Die dementia infantilis (Heller) in ihrer klinischen
Hoffmann H (1926) ‘‘Struwwelpeter-Hoffmann’’ erzählt aus seinem Bedeutung und ihre differential-diagnostische Abgrenzung ge-
Leben. Lebenserinnerungen Dr. Heinrich Hoffmanns. Frank- gen die hyperkinetische Erkrankung (Kramer-Pollnow) sowie
furter Lebensbilder, 9. Englert und Schlosser, Frankfurt am Main psychische Folgezustände nach postvaccinaler Enzephalitis.
Homburger A (1926) Vorlesungen über Psychopathologie des Kin- Zeitschrift für Kinderforschung 41:455–484
desalters. Julius Springer, Berlin Lantéri-Laura G, Gros M (1982) Le syndrome hyperkinétique
Ingram TTS (1956) A characteristic form of overactive behaviour in d’enfant: Essai d’une élucidation épistémologique. Actual Psy-
brain damaged children. J Mental Sci 102:550–558 chiatr 3:19–25
Isaev DN, Kagan VE (1981) System of treatment and rehabilitation of Laufer MW, Denhoff E (1957) Hyperkinetic behavior syndrome in
hyperactive children. In: Gittelman M (ed) Strategic interven- children. J Pediatr 50:463–474
tions for hyperactive children. M.E. Sharpe, Armonk, pp 97–111 Laufer MW, Denhoff E, Solomons G (1957) Hyperkinetic impulse
Jasper HH, Solomon P, Bradley C (1938) Electroencephalographic disorder in children’s behavior problems. Psychosom Med 19:38–49
analyses of behavior problem children. Am J Psychiatry Lay RAQ (1938) Schizophrenia-like psychoses in young children.
95:641–658 J Mental Sci 84:105–133
Joly F (1996) Figures contemporaines et formes limites des instabil- Le Gros Clark WE (1938) Morphological aspects of the hypothal-
ités. In: Bourguignon O, Njiengwé F, Sztulman H (eds) Les amus. In: Le Gros Clark WE, Beattie J, Riddoch G, Dott NM
formes contemporaines du malaise dans la civilisation. Presses (ed) The hypothalamus. Morphological, functional, clinical and
Universitaires du Mirail, Toulouse, pp 73–101 surgical aspects. Oliver and Boyd for the William Ramsay
Joly F (2005) Introduction. In: F Joly (ed) L’hyperactivité en débat Henderson Trust, Edinburgh, London, pp 1–67
(Le Carnet psy), pp 15–19 Lee SI, Schachar RJ, Chen SX, Ornstein TJ, Charach A, Barr C,
Kahn E, Cohen LH (1934) Organic drivenness a brain-stem syndrome Ickowicz A (2008) Predictive validity of DSM-IV and ICD-10
and an experience. N Engl J Med 210:748–756 criteria for ADHD and hyperkinetic disorder. J Child Psychol
Kaspar JC, Millichap JG, Backus R, Child D, Schulman JL (1971) A Psychiatry 49:70–78
study of the relationship between neurological evidence of brain Lemke R (1953) Das enthemmte Kind mit choreiformer Symptoma-
damage in children and activity and distractibility. J Consult Clin tik. Psychiatr Neurol Med Psychol 5:290–294
Psychol 36:329–337 Levin PM (1938) Restlessness in children. Arch Neurol Psychiatry
Kast RE, Altschuler EL (2008) The earliest example of the 39:764–770
hyperactivity subtype of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder Levy S (1959) Post-encephalitic behavior disorder—a forgotten
(ADHD) in Jan Steen’s ‘The Village School’ (c. 1670). S Afr entity: a report of 100 cases. Am J Psychiatry 115:1062–1067
Med J 98:594–595 Leyser E (1924) Untersuchungen über die Charakterveränderungen
Kessler JW (1980) History of minimal brain dysfunctions. In: HE Rie, nach Encephalitis epidemica. Arch Psychiatr Nervenkrankh
Rie ED (eds) Handbook of minimal brain dysfunctions. A 72:552–609
critical view (Wiley series on personality processes. Wiley- Loo SK, Barkley RA (2005) Clinical utility of EEG in attention
Interscience, New York, pp 18–51 deficit hyperactivity disorder. Appl Neuropsychol 12:64–76
Klähr T (1896) Das Wesen und die Ursachen der Zerstreutheit der Mac Keith R (1963a) Defining the concept of ‘minimal brain
Kinder. Deutsche Blätter Erzieh Unterr 13(213–214):221–222 damage’. In: Bax M, Mac Keith R (eds) Minimal cerebral

123
P. B. Foley

dysfunction. Papers from the International Study Group held at Prichard JC (1835) A treatise on insanity and other disorders affecting
Oxford, September 1962. Little Club clinics in developmental the mind. Sherwood, Gilbert, and Piper, London
medicine, 10. National Spastics Society Medical Education and Proby R (1939) Contribution à l’étude de l’enfance coupable. Les
Information Unit; William Heinemann Medical Books, London, perversions morales postencéphalitiques. Janody, Lyon
pp 1–9 Rafalovich A (2001) The conceptual history of attention deficit
Mac Keith R (1963b) ‘Minimal brain damage’—a concept discarded. hyperactivity disorder: idiocy, imbecility, encephalitis and the
In: Bax M, Mac Keith R (eds) Minimal cerebral dysfunction. child deviant, 1877–1929. Deviant Behav 22:93–115
Papers from the International Study Group held at Oxford, Ramsay AM, Young SEJ (1963) Post infectious encephalitis with
September 1962 (Little Club clinics in developmental medicine; particular reference to rubella and measles. Public Health
10. National Spastics Society Medical Education and Informa- 78:100–107
tion Unit; William Heinemann Medical Books, London, unpag- Rapoport JL, Buchsbaum MS, Weingartner H, Zahn TP, Ludlow C,
inated foreword Mikkelsen EJ (1980) Dextroamphetamine. Its cognitive and
Mayes R, Rafalovich A (2007) Suffer the restless children: the behavioral effects in normal and hyperactive boys and normal
evolution of ADHD and paediatric stimulant use, 1900–80. Hist men. Arch Gen Psychiatry 37:933–943
Psychiatry 18:435–457 Robins E, Guze SB (1970) Establishment of diagnostic validity in
Medical correspondent (1924) ‘‘Sleepy sickness’’: A disease which psychiatric illness: Its application to schizophrenia. Am J
makes criminals. The Times (London) 20 August 1924, p 7D Psychiat 126:983–987
Meier R, Gross F, Tripod J (1954) Ritalin, eine neuartige synthetische Ross DM, Ross SA (1976) Hyperactivity: research, theory, and
Verbindung mit spezifischer zentralerregender Wirkungskompo- action. Wiley-Interscience/John Wiley & Sons, New York
nente. Klin Wochenschr 32:445–450 Rothenberger A, Neumärker K-J (2005) Wissensgeschichte der ADHS.
Mendelson W, Johnson N, Stewart MA (1971) Hyperactive children Kramer–Pollnow im Spiegel der Zeit. Steinkopff, Darmstadt
as teenagers: a follow-up study. J Nerv Mental Dis 153:273–279 Russell JA (1942) The hyperactive child. Am J Dis Child 63:94–101
Michell G (1963) Notes on the so-called ‘minimal brain damage’ Rutter M, Lebovici S, Eisenberg L, Sneznevskij AV, Sadoun R,
syndrome with particular reference to family dynamics. In: Bax Brooke E, Lin T-Y (1969) A tri-axial classification of mental
M, Mac Keith R (eds) Minimal cerebral dysfunction. Papers disorders in childhood. An international study. J Child Psychol
from the International Study Group held at Oxford, September Psychiatry 10:41–61
1962. Little Club clinics in developmental medicine, 10. Safer DJ, Allen RP (1976) Hyperactive children. Diagnosis and
National Spastics Society Medical Education and Information management. University Park Press, Baltimore
Unit; William Heinemann Medical Books, London, pp 46–50 Sandberg S, Barton J (2002) Historical development. In: Sandberg S
Micouin G, Boucris J-C (1988) L’enfant instable ou hyperkinétique. (ed) Hyperactivity and attention disorders of childhood. Cam-
Une étude comparée des concepts. Psychiatr Enfant 31:473–507 bridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 1–29
Misès R (ed) (2012) Classification française des troubles mentaux de Satterfield JH (1973) EEG issues in children with minimal brain
l’enfant et de l’adolescent—R-2012, 5e édition. Presses de dysfunction. Sem Psychiatry 5:35–46
l’école des Hautes Études en Santé Publique, Rennes Schmutz SD (2004) Die Amphetaminbehandlung verhaltensauffäl-
Modesto-Lowe V, Chaplin M, Soovajian V, Meyer A (2013) Are liger Kinder von 1937 bis in die 70er Jahre in Amerika unter
motivation deficits underestimated in patients with ADHD? A besonderer Berücksichtung der Substanz Methylphenidat (Rita-
review of the literature. Postgrad Med 125:47–52 lin). Thesis: Albert-Ludwigs-Universität, Freiburg i. Br
Morrison JR, Stewart MA (1971) A family study of the hyperactive Schneider WF (1945) Psychiatric evaluation of the hyperkinetic child.
child syndrome. Biol Psychiatry 3:189–195 J Pediatr 26:559–570
Münch MC (1842) ‘Zerstreutheit, Zerstreuung’. In: Universal-Lexi- Schrag P, Divoky D (1975) The myth of the hyperactive child and
con der Erziehungs- und Unterrichtslehre für ältere und jüngere other means of child control. Pantheon, New York
christliche Volksschullehrer. Band 3. J. A. Schlosser’s Buch- Schröder P (1938) Kinderpsychiatrie. Monatsschr Psychiatr Neurol
und Kunsthandlung, Augsburg, pp 415–417 99:269–293
Neumann H (1859) Lehrbuch der Psychiatrie. Ferdinand Enke, Schulman JL, Kaspar JC, Throne FM (1965) Brain damage and
Erlangen behavior. Charles C. Thomas, Springfield (ILL)
Neumärker K-J (2005) The Kramer–Pollnow syndrome: a contribu- Seidler E (2004) ‘‘Zappelphilipp’’ und ADHS. Von der Unart zur
tion on the life and work of Franz Kramer and Hans Pollnow. Krankheit. Deutsches Ärzteblatt 101:A239–A243
Hist Psychiatry 16:435–451 Siegert G (1909) ‘Vielgeschäftig’. In: W Rein (ed) Encyklopädisches
Nylander L (2011) Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism Handbuch der Pädagogik. 9. Band. Hermann Beyer & Söhne,
spectrum disorders in adult psychiatric patients. Thesis, Univer- Langensalza, pp 635–636
sity of Gothenburg Siegert G (1910) ‘Zappelig’. In: W Rein (ed) Encyklopädisches
Palmer ED, Finger S (2001) An early description of ADHD Handbuch der Pädagogik. 10. Band. Hermann Beyer & Söhne,
(inattentive subtype): Dr Alexander Crichton and ‘mental Langensalza, p 299
restlessness’ (1798). Child Psychol Psychiatry Rev 6:66–73 Staehelin JE (1941) Pervitin-Psychose. Zeitschrift für die gesamte
Panizzon L (1944) La preparazione di piridil- e piperidil-arilacetoni- Neurologie und Psychiatrie 173:598–621
trili e di alcuni prodotti di trasformazione (Parte Ia). Helv Chim Stamm JS, Kreder SV (1979) Minimal brain dysfunction: psycho-
Acta 27:1748–1756 logical and neurophysiological disorders in hyperkinetic chil-
Parsons L, Burling S, Parsons CG (eds) (1954) Diseases of infancy dren. In: Gazzaniga MS (ed) Neuropsychology. Handbook of
and childhood, vol 2, 2nd edn. Oxford University Press, London behavioral neurobiology, 2. Plenum Press, New York, London,
Paul-Boncour G (1919) La sélection morale des écoliers instables. pp 119–150
Progr Méd 361–363 Stern F (1936) Epidemische encephalitis (ECONOMOsche Krank-
Philippe J, Paul-Boncour G (1905) Les anomalies mentales chez les heit). In: Bumke O, Foerster O (eds) Handbuch der Neurologie.
écoliers. Étude médico-pédagogique. Félix Alcan, Paris Band XIII. Spezielle Neurologie V, Erkrankungen des Rücken-
Prechtl HFR, Stemmer CJ (1962) The choreiform syndrome in marks und Gehirns III. Infektionen und Intoxicationen II. Julius
children. Dev Med Child Neurol 4:119–127 Springer, Berlin, pp 307–500

123
Sons and daughters beyond your control

Still GF (1902) The Goulstonian lectures. On some abnormal von Feuchtersleben E (1860) Zur Diätetik der Seele, 6th edn. Carl
psychical conditions in children. Lancet 159:1008–1112, Gerold, Vienna
1077–1082, 1163–1168 von Lederer E, Éderer S (1934) Hypermotilitätsneurose im Kindes-
Strauss AA, Lehtinen LE (1947) Psychopathology and education of alter. Jahrb Kinderheilkd 143:257–268
the brain-injured child. Grune & Stratton, New York, London Wallon H (1925) L’enfant turbulent. Étude sur les retards et les
Strohl MP (2011) Bradley’s Benzedrine studies on children with anomalies du développement moteur et mental. Thesis, Paris
behavioral disorders. Yale J Biol Med 84:27–33 Warnke A, Riederer C (eds) (2013) ADHD. Attention deficit-
Strömgren E (1947) Psychoses in children. Acta Psychiatr Scand hyperactivity disorder. An illustrated historical overview. Self-
22(suppl. 47):245–258 published, s.l.
Strümpell L (1892) Die pädagogische Pathologie, oder Die Lehre von Welniarz B (2011) De l’instabilité mentale au trouble déficitaire de
den Fehlern der Kinder. E. Ungleich, Leipzig l’attention-hyperactivité: l’histoire d’un concept controversé.
Taurines R, Schmitt J, Renner T, Conner AC, Warnke A, Romanos M Perspect Psy 50:16–22
(2010) Developmental comorbidity in attention-deficit/hyperac- Wender Ph (1971) Minimal brain dysfunction in children. Wiley
tivity disorder. ADHD Attent Deficit Hyperact Disord 2:267–289 series on psychological disorders. Wiley-Interscience, New York
Taylor E (2011) Antecedents of ADHD: a historical account of Wernicke C (1906) Grundriss der Psychiatrie in klinischen Vorles-
diagnostic concepts. ADHD Atten Deficit Hyperact Disord ungen. Georg Thieme, Leipzig
3:69–75 Wieck C (1957) Die psychomotorische Unruhe im Schulkindalter
Taylor E, Döpfner M, Sergeant J, Asherson P, Banaschewski T, unter besonderer Berücksichtigung der Umweltfaktoren. Z Arztl
Buitelaar J, Coghill D, Danckaerts M, Rothenberger A, Sonuga- Fortbild 51:977–980
Barke E, Steinhausen H-C, Zuddas A (2004) European clinical World Health Organization (ed) (1948) Manual of the international
guidelines for hyperkinetic disorder—first upgrade. Eur Child statistical classification of diseases, injuries, and causes of death.
Adolesc Psychiatry 13(suppl. 1):I7–I30 Sixth revision of the International lists of diseases and causes of
Thiele R (1926) Zur Kenntnis der psychischen Residuärzustände nach death, adopted 1948, vol 1. World Health Organization, Geneva
Encephalitis epidemica bei Kindern und Jugendlichen, insbe- World Health Organization (ed) (1977) International classification of
sondere der Weiterentwicklung dieser Fälle. Abhandlungen aus diseases. Manual of the international statistical classification of
der Neurologie, Psychiatrie, Psychologie und ihren Grenzgebie- diseases, injuries, and causes of death. Based on the recommen-
ten. S. Karger, Berlin dations of the ninth revision conference, 1975, and adopted by
Thiele R (1953) Zum Begriff und zur Pathologie der Drangerschein- the twenty-ninth World Health Assembly, vol 1. World Health
ungen. Psychiatr Neurol Med Psychol 5:51–59 Organization, Geneva
Thome J, Jacobs KA (2004) Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder World Health Organization (1992) The ICD-10 classification of
(ADHD) in a 19th century children’s book. Eur Psychiatry mental and behavioural disorders. Clinical descriptions and
19:303–306 diagnostic guidelines. electronic version: www.who.int/classifi
Tracy F (1896) The psychology of childhood, 3rd edn. DC cations/icd/en/bluebook.pdf. World Health Organization,
Heath & Co., Boston Geneva
Tredgold AF (1914) Mental deficiency (amentia), 2nd edn. William World Health Organization (2012) Attention deficit hyperactivity
Wood, New York disorder. ICD-11 Beta Draft. http://id.who.int/icd/entity/
Vermeylen G (1922) Les débiles mentaux. Étude expérimentale et 821852937
clinique. Bull Inst Gén Psychol 22:4–6 Ziehen T (1909) ‘Unruhe, krankhafte’. In: W Rein (ed) Encyklopä-
Vilensky JA, Foley P, Gilman S (2007) Children and encephalitis disches Handbuch der Pädagogik. 9. Band. Hermann Beyer &
lethargica: a historical review. Pediatr Neurol 37:79–84 Söhne, Langensalza, p 438
von der Leyen R (1926) Sachverständigenkonferenz und Mitglieder- Ziehen T (1924) Leitfaden der physiologischen Psychologie in 16
versammlung des Deutschen Vereins zur Fürsorge für jugendli- Vorlesungen. Gustav Fischer, Jena
che Psychopathen e. V. am 13. und 14. November 1925, Berlin.
Z Kinderforsch 31:394–414

123

You might also like