You are on page 1of 6

International Journal of Behavioral Medicine

https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-019-09772-9

BRIEF REPORT

Moderation of Mood in the Transfer of Self-Regulation From an Exercise


to an Eating Context: Short- and Long-Term Effects on Dietary Change
and Obesity in Women
James J. Annesi 1,2

# International Society of Behavioral Medicine 2019

Abstract
Background Behavioral obesity treatments require an improved understanding of the dynamics of associated psychological
changes. This study aimed to clarify previous research on self-regulatory skills’ transfer from an exercise to eating context,
effects of mood on self-regulatory strength, and related effects on a targeted eating behavior.
Methods Women with obesity participated in a yearlong community-based cognitive-behavioral treatment that first focused on
self-regulatory skills development for exercise maintenance, then use of similar self-regulatory skills and improved mood to
facilitate short- and long-term increases in fruit/vegetable intake and reduction in weight. Groups were based on high (≥ 5% of
baseline weight; n = 51) and low (< 5%; n = 49) weight reduction 2 years post-initiation.
Results Improvements in eating self-regulation and fruit/vegetable intake were greatest in the high weight-reduction group.
Using lagged variable analyses to assess directionality, mood significantly moderated the prediction of eating self-regulation
change by exercise self-regulation change. The effect of increased exercise self-regulation on fruit/vegetable intake change over
6 months was significantly mediated by eating self-regulation change. Participants’ initial weight moderated the effect of eating
self-regulation change on fruit/vegetable intake. Change in eating self-regulation over 6 months predicted self-regulation at
24 months. Short-term change in fruit/vegetable intake predicted weight change over 2 years through its association with
long-term fruit/vegetable consumption.
Conclusions Findings supported the expected carry-over of self-regulation from an exercise to eating context, mood effects on
self-regulatory strength, and associations of exercise with eating and weight changes via effects on psychological variables.
Results have implications for sustained effects associated with behavioral treatments.

Keywords Self-regulation . Mood . Exercise . Obesity . Treatment . Behavioral

Introduction first several months [1]. A poor understanding of, and


accounting for, theory-based psychosocial changes asso-
Behavioral obesity treatments have generally been un- ciated with eating might be a reason [2]. Research be-
successful at maintaining enough weight loss to reduce ginning to address this shortcoming has often been
health risks (e.g., ≥ 5% of original weight) beyond the guided by social cognitive theory [3], emphasizing indi-
viduals’ control over their internal and environmental
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article barriers to behavioral changes. When driven by social
(https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-019-09772-9) contains supplementary cognitive theory, the development of self-regulatory
material, which is available to authorized users. skills such as cognitive restructuring and distal and
proximal goal setting, and consideration of emotional
* James J. Annesi states, is of concern within treatments. To define clear
jamesa@ymcaatlanta.org terminal behaviors, behavioral obesity treatments have
1 also sometimes focused on measurable targets such as
YMCA of Metro Atlanta, 101 Marietta Street, Atlanta, GA 30303,
USA a daily caloric limit, fat and/or sugar intake, or fruit and
2 vegetable consumption. Fruit/vegetable intake has alone
Department of Health Promotion and Physical Education, Kennesaw
State University, Kennesaw, GA, USA been associated with the health of the overall diet [4].
Int.J. Behav. Med.

Exercise is the strongest correlate of long-term weight loss and relationships among variables, were tested over 6
[5]. However, this association might be more from its effects and 24 months. Hypotheses were as follows: (a) there will
on psychosocial predictors of improved eating than associated be significantly greater improvements on all measured
energy expenditures [6]. As few as 2–3 moderate exercise variables in participants successful with weight loss, (b)
sessions per week have been associated with improved mood change in SR-eating will significantly mediate the fruit/
[6]. Although minimally addressed in applied weight-loss set- vegetable change–SR-exercise relationship, (c) mood will
tings, and contrary to laboratory research indicating that self- significantly moderate the SR-exercise–SR-eating change
regulation applied to one task (e.g., exercise) can deplete self- relationship, and baseline weight will significantly mod-
regulation applied to second task (e.g., healthy eating), some erate the self-regulation–eating change relationships, (d)
research posits a carry-over of self-regulatory skills learned in change in SR-eating from baseline to month 6 will signif-
an exercise-support context, to an eating context [7, 8]. More icantly predict SR-eating at month 24, and (e) change in
robust support of such a relationship could justify the need for fruit/vegetable intake over 24 months will significantly
exercise early in behavioral intervention processes for reasons mediate the prediction of weight change over 24 months
well beyond the minor energy expenditures expected in by fruit/vegetable intake over the initial 6 months. Fig. 1a,
deconditioned individuals. Other research suggests that mood b, and c depicts the proposed relationships.
affects self-regulation, especially through its influence on the
volition required to apply such means [9]. An individual’s
weight might also influence self-regulatory effects when seek- Methods
ing a healthier diet for weight loss [10]. For example, individ-
uals with a higher degree of excess weight might require great- Participants
er advances in self-regulation to overcome especially poor
habits [10]. Under conditions of enhanced self-regulation, Participants were adult females recruited through written and
short-term (e.g., 6-month) improvements in the diet might electronic media into community health-promotion centers in
lead to longer-term (e.g., 24-month) improvements through the southeast USA. Those currently pregnant/planning a preg-
sustained abilities to address barriers. nancy, in another weight-loss treatment, completing an aver-
If individuals successful with weight loss are more likely to age of 2 or more exercise sessions per week, or taking psy-
improve on the aforementioned behavioral variables than chotropic or weight-loss medications were ineligible.
those who are not, clarification of their interrelationships Volunteers meeting the established criteria were progressively
might be particularly useful for improving intervention archi- accepted into the study until the planned sample size of 100
tectures. Treatment implications include whether self- was obtained (which took approximately 10 weeks). This en-
regulation should be focused first on exercise, if mood change rollment goal was based on the need for a minimum of 97 total
should be targeted to enhance self-regulation transfer from an participants to detect a moderate effect size (f2 = 0.15) at the
exercise to an eating emphasis, and whether short-term eating statistical power of 0.80 (α ≤ 0.05) within the primary analy-
changes transfer to long-term changes and weight loss under ses [11]. Groups were established after the data-collection
conditions of increased self-regulation. phase based on whether at least 5% of a participant’s original
Thus, a yearlong cognitive-behavioral treatment aimed weight (a marker of health-risk reduction [6]) was lost at
at increasing self-regulation skills for both exercise (SR- month 24 (high weight-loss group; n = 51), or 5% of weight
exercise) and eating (SR-eating), improving mood, and was not lost (low weight-loss group; n = 49). There was no
increasing fruit/vegetable intake was administered in a significant mean difference in age (48.18 ± 7.61 years), ethnic
field setting to women with obesity. Treatment effects, make-up (75% white, 21% black, 4% other), yearly household

ΔSR-eatingb-6 ΔFruit/vegetableb-24
ΔSR-eatingb-6
a b c
Moodb-3/mean Weightb
a b a b a b

c c c
ΔSR-exerciseb-3 ΔFruit/vegetableb-6 ΔSR-exerciseb-3 ΔFruit/vegetableb-6 ΔFruit/vegetableb-6 ΔWeightb-24
Fig. 1 a Mediation model predicting change in fruit/vegetable intake change in score during indicated times. Subscripts b-3 = baseline–
from baseline to month 6. b Moderated mediation model predicting month 3, b-6 = baseline–month 6, b-24 = baseline–month 24. Letters a,
change in fruit/vegetable intake from baseline to month 6. c Mediation b, and c′ indicate path a, path b, and path c′
model predicting change in weight from baseline to month 24. Δ =
Int.J. Behav. Med.

income (US$50,000–$100,000), or educational level (24% 6.5 months. The primary emphasis was enabling participants’
high school, 32% bachelor’s degree, 44% beyond bachelor’s) use of the self-regulatory skills of cognitive restructuring,
between the groups. Institutional review board-approved in- stimulus control, relapse prevention (e.g., planning for viola-
formed consent was obtained in writing from each participant. tions in agreed-upon behaviors), dissociation from exercise-
induced discomfort, contingency-based self-reward,
Measures recruiting social supports, and behavioral contracting to over-
come their barriers to regular exercise. Although exercise
SR-exercise and SR-eating were separately measured using 10 types were chosen by each participant, durations and intensi-
items each. The previously developed and validated surveys ties were checked by instructors for palatability using a previ-
[12] assessed respondents’ present use of self-regulatory ously validated method of assessing pre- to post-exercise
skills. Sample items for SR-exercise were “I make formal changes in feeling states [6]. Long-term goals were also self-
agreements with myself to be physically active,” “I set phys- set by participants, with instructors actively helping to docu-
ical activity goals,” and “I keep a record of my physical ment and track proximal (i.e., next session) goals. A minimum
activities.” of 3 exercise sessions per week was assigned, and tracked by
Sample items for SR-eating were “I say positive things to both electronic and self-report methods.
myself about eating well,” “I set eating goals,” and “I schedule Based on study entry time, either 3 or 4 exercise-support
my times to eat.” Response options were from 1 (never) to 4 sessions were completed prior to (a) being trained how to log
(often) and were summed. Internal consistencies were energy and fruit/vegetable intake each day, (b) learning her
Cronbach’s α = 0.81 and 0.79, respectively [12]; and α = 0.79 daily caloric intake limit (e.g., 1500 kcal/day for a weight of
and 0.77 in the present sample. Test-retest reliabilities were 0.74 80–100 kg), and (c) initiating the group nutrition-change com-
and 0.78 [12]. ponent that met for 60 min every 2 weeks. Nearly all of the
Mood was measured using the 30-item total mood distur- nutrition session time was spent adapting the previously
bance scale of the Profile of Mood States brief form [13]. It learned self-regulatory skills from an exercise to a controlled
aggregated feelings related to depression, anxiety, fatigue, an- eating context using brief lectures and structured activities.
ger, confusion, and vigor (e.g., “sad,” “tense,” “energetic”) oc- The primary nutrition target was increasing fruit and vegetable
curring “over the past two weeks, including today.” Response intake. The combined exercise support and nutrition-change
options were from 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely) and were intervention terminated 56 weeks post-initiation.
summed after first reversing the scores on the vigor-related Fidelity checks on approximately 20% of sessions indicat-
items. Internal consistencies across factors were Cronbach ed strong protocol compliance, requiring only minor adjust-
α = 0.84–0.95 [13] and α = 0.77–0.87 in the present sample. ments by study staff. Manipulation checks indicated compli-
Test-retest reliabilities averaged 0.69 [13]. ance with the mandated exercise amount (i.e., estimated mean
Combined servings of fruits and vegetables “consumed in a of 4.4 exercise sessions per week).
typical day” (fruit/vegetable intake) were measured through 2
items corresponding to portion sizes indicated by the U.S. Data Analyses
Department of Agriculture (e.g., 118 mL canned peaches or
carrots) [14]. Adequate validity was indicated through signif- Because there was no systematic bias in the 10% of missing
icant (p values < 0.001) correspondences with the Block Food data [17], the expectation-maximization algorithm [18] was
Frequency Questionnaire, other comprehensive food frequen- used for imputation in the intention-to-treat analytic approach.
cy recalls, and energy consumption [15, 16]. Tolerance scores ranged from 0.50 to 0.99, which indicated
Weight was measured in kg using a recently calibrated acceptable collinearity in the data. Because directionality in
digital scale (Health-o-meter model 800KL, Buffalo Grove, the relationships between the variables being tested was pre-
IL, USA). Heavy clothing was removed prior to recording viously indicated [6], statistical significance was set at α ≤
the mean of 2 consecutive measurements. 0.05 (1-tailed) for those analyses, and α ≤ 0.05 (2-tailed) for
all others. SPSS version 22.0 (IBM, Armonk, NY), incorpo-
Procedure rating the PROCESS macro-instructional software version 2,
models 4 and 28 [11], was used for the statistical analyses. The
All treatment processes were administered by existing staff of bias-corrected bootstrapping procedure incorporated 20,000
community health-promotion centers. They had appropriate resamples of the data.
national certifications and 2.5 days of training in the study’s Dependent t tests assessed within-group, and overall,
protocol components. Participants were initially placed into changes in study variables over the incorporated temporal
the 6-session exercise support component [6]. The first 4 of intervals. Mixed-model repeated measures ANOVAs, with
these 45-min, 1-on-1 sessions were held within the first Bonferroni-adjusted follow-up t tests, assessed group differ-
3 months, then were spaced by 2 months to conclude at ences in those changes. Effect sizes were calculated as
Int.J. Behav. Med.

Cohen’s d and partial eta squared (η2partial), respectively, prediction of 24-month change in weight by change in fruit/
where small–large effects are, by convention, 0.20–0.80 and vegetable intake from baseline to month 6 (Fig. 1c). Results of
0.01–0.14. the overall model (R2), the unstandardized beta (B) and its
Using aggregated data, regression models incorporated a associated standard error (SEB), and the standardized beta
lagged variable approach [19] where change in a predictor (β) are reported appropriate. Because bootstrap resampling
from a preceding interval (e.g., baseline–month 3) was entered was incorporated, 95% confidence intervals (CI) were
to predict longer-term change (e.g., baseline–month 6). assessed as the significance of mediation [11].
Following recent suggestions [11], total effects between inde-
pendent and dependent variables (c paths) were not part of the
mediation analyses. Within the first mediation analysis Results
(Fig. 1a), whether change in SR-eating significantly mediated
the prediction of change in fruit/vegetable intake from SR- There was no significant difference at baseline in any study
exercise change was assessed. Within a moderated mediation variable (p values > 0.20). Significant improvements in SR-
analysis (Fig. 1b), it was next assessed whether mood change exercise, SR-eating, mood, and fruit/vegetable intake were
(and mean mood score) significantly moderated the prediction found in each group, and overall (Table 1). Changes were
of SR-eating change by change in SR-exercise and whether significantly greater in the high weight-loss group on changes
baseline weight significantly moderated the prediction of fruit/ in SR-eating (F1, 98 = 6.77, p = 0.010, η2partial = 0.07) and fruit/
vegetable intake change by SR-exercise and SR-eating chang- vegetable intake (F1, 98 = 7.99, p = 0.006, η2partial = 0.08).
es. Significance of moderation is determined by the indepen- Weight loss was significant not only in groups over 6 months
dent variable × moderator interaction term’s prediction of the but also over 24 months in only the high weight-loss group
dependent variable. The prediction of SR-eating at month 24 (Table 1).
by change in SR-eating from baseline to month 6 was next Intercorrelations of aggregated data indicated bivariate re-
evaluated. Finally, it was assessed whether change in fruit/ lationships between changes in self-regulation and mood, and
vegetable intake over 24 months significantly mediated the fruit/vegetable intake and weight, were each in the expected

Table 1 Descriptive statistics and changes in study measures, by group

Baseline Time 2 ΔBaseline-time 2 Month 24 ΔBaseline-month 24

Self-regulation-exercise
High weight loss 21.14 ± 5.97 33.69 ± 4.41 12.55 ± 7.04*(2.10)
Low weight loss 21.45 ± 5.74 32.04 ± 4.23 10.59 ± 6.51*(1.84)
Overall 21.29 ± 5.83 32.88 ± 4.38 11.59 ± 6.82*(1.99)
Self-regulation-eating
High weight loss 22.37 ± 5.93 33.59 ± 3.78 11.22 ± 6.69*(1.89) 31.82 ± 6.00
Low weight loss 23.67 ± 4.91 31.50 ± 4.51 7.83 ± 6.32*(1.59) 28.18 ± 5.16
Overall 23.01 ± 5.47 32.57 ± 4.26 9.56 ± 6.70*(1.76) 30.04 ± 5.87
Mood
High weight loss 25.61 ± 15.66 6.50 ± 12.47 − 19.11 ± 16.58*(1.22)
Low weight loss 25.14 ± 15.51 9.06 ± 14.63 − 16.08 ± 14.62*(1.04)
Overall 25.38 ± 15.51 7.76 ± 13.57 − 17.63 ± 15.64*(1.14)
Fruit/vegetable serving/day
High weight loss 3.85 ± 2.00 6.56 ± 2.04 2.71 ± 2.12*(1.36) 6.22 ± 1.92 2.36 ± 2.09*(1.18)
Low weight loss 3.84 ± 1.63 5.91 ± 2.11 2.07 ± 2.21*(1.27) 5.08 ± 1.69 1.24 ± 1.86*(0.76)
Overall 3.85 ± 1.82 6.24 ± 2.09 2.40 ± 2.17*(1.32) 5.66 ± 1.89 1.82 ± 2.05*(1.00)
Weight (kg)
High weight loss 95.16 ± 12.59 87.09 ± 12.36 − 8.07 ± 4.64*(0.64) 85.15 ± 12.52 − 10.01 ± 7.13*(0.80)
Low weight loss 93.89 ± 11.18 90.35 ± 11.34 − 3.54 ± 3.06*(0.32) 93.78 ± 11.29 − 0.10 ± 3.17(0.09)
Overall 94.54 ± 11.88 88.69 ± 11.92 − 5.85 ± 4.54*(0.49) 89.38 ± 12.64 − 5.16 ± 7.44*(0.43)

High weight loss group n = 51. Low weight loss group n = 49. Overall group N = 100
Time 2 = month 3 for self-regulation-exercise and mood, and month 6 for self-regulation-eating, fruit/vegetable intake, and weight. Values are given as
mean ± standard deviation. Δ = change in the score during indicated times. Cohen’s d values for within-group changes are given in parentheses
*p ≤ 0.001
Int.J. Behav. Med.

Moderation of path c′

baseline-month 6, b-24 = baseline-month 24. Path a = independent variable→mediator, path b = mediator→dependent variable, path c′ = independent variable→dependent variable, controlling for the
Results are based on a bootstrapping method incorporating 20,000 resamples [11]. Δ = change in the score during indicated times. Moodmean = baseline+month 3/2. b-3 = Δbaseline-month 3, b-6 =
direction and significant (see supplementary data). Change in
SR-eating significantly mediated the relationship between

Weightbaseline

Weightbaseline
changes in SR-exercise and fruit/vegetable intake (B = 0.05,

0.01 ± 0.01

0.01 ± 0.01
Beta±SE
SEB = 0.03, 95% CI = 0.01, 0.09) (Table 2). That overall
model was significant (R2 = 0.11, F2, 97 = 6.23, p = 0.003).
When entered into the previous equation, both mood change
(B = − 0.01, SEB = 0.01, p = 0.029) and mean mood score

−0.42 ± 0.42

−0.42 ± 0.42
0.03 ± 0.04

0.34 ± 0.88
(B = −0.01, SEB = 0.01, p = 0.019) significantly moderated

Beta±SE
Path c′
the SR-exercise–SR-eating change relationship; and weight
at baseline significantly moderated the relationship between
changes in SR-eating and fruit/vegetable intake (B = − 0.02,

Moderation of path b
SEB = 0.01, p = 0.043) (Table 2). Those overall models were
significant (R2 = 0.15, F5, 94 = 3.25, p = 0.010). Change

− 0.02 ± 0.01*

− 0.02 ± 0.01*
Weightbaseline

Weightbaseline
in SR-eating from baseline to month 6 significantly predicted
SR-eating at month 24 (B = 0.17, SEB = 0.09, β = .20, p =

Beta±SE
0.025, 95% CI = 0.03, 0.32). Change in fruit/vegetable intake
from baseline to month 24 significantly mediated the relation-
ship between changes in fruit/vegetable intake from baseline

− 3.84 ± 0.93***
to month 6 and change in weight from baseline to month 24

0.09 ± 0.04*

0.88 ± 0.46*

0.88 ± 0.46*
(B = − 2.27, SEB = 1.10, 95% CI = − 4.73, − 0.95) (Table 2).

Beta±SE
That overall model was significant (R2 = 0.21, F2, 97 = 12.53,

Path b
p < 0.001).
Moderation of path a

Discussion

− 0.01 ± 0.01**
− 0.01 ± 0.01*
ΔMoodb-3

Moodmean
The finding that change in SR-eating was significantly
Beta±SE

greater in participants successful with sustained weight


loss confirmed the need for further evaluation of the dy-
namic processes related to this important variable within
0.58 ± 0.08***

0.43 ± 0.12***

0.64 ± 0.09***

0.59 ± 0.07***
treatments. Through the present attention given to that
quest, findings suggested that improved exercise-related
Beta±SE
Path a

self-regulation carries over to better self-regulation related


Results of mediation and moderated mediation analyses (N = 100)

to healthy eating, which is largely sustained over the long


term (here, over 2 years). Although considerable replica-
ΔFruit/vegetableb-6

ΔFruit/vegetableb-6

ΔFruit/vegetableb-6
Dependent variable

tion across sample types is required to increase confidence


in findings, the current results support a strong self-
ΔWeightb-24

regulatory focus within behavioral treatments beginning


with an aim toward increasing exercise behaviors. This
appeared increasingly relevant as initial weights were
higher. The present finding that mood significantly affects
ΔFruit/vegetableb-24

self-regulatory progress, while previously posited [9],


should be further tested in field settings for both its im-
ΔSR-eatingb-6

ΔSR-eatingb-6

ΔSR-eatingb-6

portant theoretical and treatment implications. The finding


*p ≤ 0.05, **p ≤ 0.01, **p ≤ 0.001
Mediator

that short-term improvements in fruit/vegetable intake (a


proxy for the health of the overall diet) predicted long-
term changes suggests the present treatment paradigm can
have unusually favorable long-term effects [1]. The treat-
Independent variable

ΔFruit/vegetableb-6

ment’s foci of self-regulation, and a seemingly manage-


ΔSR-exerciseb-3

ΔSR-exerciseb-3

ΔSR-exerciseb-3

able 3 exercise sessions per week to induce mood im-


provements, also purposefully addressed adherence—a
mediator
Table 2

consistent problem in real-world settings that use primar-


ily educational (typically atheoretical) methods [6].
Int.J. Behav. Med.

Beyond the aforementioned need for replication, limita- 3. Bandura A. Social foundations of thought and action: a social cog-
nitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall; 1986.
tions of this research include incorporation of a volunteer
4. Aljadani HM, Patterson A, Sibbritt D, Hutchesson MJ, Jensen ME,
sample that might have been highly motivated, incomplete Collins CE. Diet quality, measured by fruit and vegetable intake,
consideration of constructs related to social cognitive theory predicts weight change in young women. J Obes. 2013:525161.
[3] (and other accepted theories), and lack of control over 5. Svetkey LP, Stevens VJ, Brantley PJ, Appel LJ, Hollis JF, Loria
CM, et al. Comparison of strategies for sustaining weight loss.
social support and expectation effects. Additionally, carry-
JAMA. 2008;299:1139–48.
over effects to eating-related self-regulation when only 6. Annesi JJ. Supported exercise improves controlled eating and
exercise-focused self-regulation is addressed remain un- weight through its effects on psychosocial factors: extending a sys-
known. A strength of the study, however, was its field setting tematic research program toward treatment development. Perm J.
2012;16(1):7–18.
and longitudinal design that increased external validity and
7. Oaten M, Cheng K. Longitudinal gains in self-regulation from reg-
rapid generalization of findings to treatments. Further ular physical exercise. Br J Health Psychol. 2006;11:717–33.
theory-based research remains needed so that an improved 8. Mata J, Silva MN, Vieira PN, Carraça EV, Andrade AM, Coutinho
understanding of the dynamics of psychosocial changes with- SR, et al. Motivational “spill-over” during weight control: increased
in the weight-loss treatment process can positively affect fu- self-determination and exercise intrinsic motivation predict eating
self-regulation. Health Psychol. 2009;28:709–16.
ture behavioral interventions—especially those applicable for 9. Gendolla GHE, Brinkman K. The role of mood states in self-regu-
large-scale dissemination. lation: effects on action preferences and resource mobilization. Eur
Psychol. 2005;10:187–98.
Compliance with Ethical Standards 10. Wilson GT, Brownell KD. Behavioral treatment for obesity. In:
Fairburn CG, Brownell KD. Eating disorders and obesity. 2nd ed.
All study procedures were in accordance with the ethical standards of the New York, NY: Guilford; 2002. pp. 524–528.
institutional and/or national research committee and the 1964 Helsinki 11. Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional
declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. process analysis: a regression-based approach. New York, NY:
Guilford; 2013.
12. Annesi JJ, Marti CN. Path analysis of cognitive-behavioral exercise
Conflict of Interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of treatment-induced changes in psychological factors leading to
interest. weight loss. Psychol Health. 2011;26:1081–98.
13. McNair DM, Heuchert JWP. Profile of Mood States technical up-
Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from all participants date. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems; 2009.
included in the study. 14. U.S. Department of Agriculture. MyPlate and historical Food
Pyramid resources. Beltsville, MD: National Agricultural Library;
Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdic- 2017.
tional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. 15. Block G, Hartman AM, Dresser CM, Carroll MD, Gannon J,
Gardner L. A data-based approach to diet questionnaire design
and testing. Am J Epidemiol. 1986;124:453–69.
16. Mares-Perlman JA, Klein BEK, Klein R, Ritter LL, Fisher MR,
References Freudenheim JL. A diet history questionnaire ranks nutrient intakes
in middle-aged and older men and women similarly to multiple
1. Dombrowski SU, Knittle K, Avenell A, Araújo-Soares V, Sniehotta food records. J Nutr. 1993;123:489–501.
FF. Long term maintenance of weight loss with non-surgical inter- 17. White IR, Horton NJ, Carpenter J, Pocock SJ. Strategy for intention
ventions in obese adults: systematic review of meta-analyses of to treat data in randomized trials with missing outcome data. BMJ.
randomised controlled trials. BMJ. 2014;348:g2646. 2011;342:d40.
2. Baranowski T, Lin LS, Wetter DW, Resnicow K, Hearn MD. 18. Schafer JL, Graham JW. Missing data: our view of the state of the
Theory as mediating variables: why aren’t community interven- art. Psychol Methods. 2002;7:147–77.
tions working as desired? Ann Epidemiol. 1997;7(suppl):S89– 19. Cromwell JB, Hannan MJ, Labys WC, Terraza M. Multivariate
S95, 7. tests for time series models. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1994.

You might also like