You are on page 1of 6

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/8255302

The risks of eating and drinking

Article in EMBO Reports · November 2004


DOI: 10.1038/sj.embor.7400225 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

20 233

3 authors:

Susan Wilkinson Gene Rowe


University of East Anglia Gene Rowe Evaluations
3 PUBLICATIONS 55 CITATIONS 120 PUBLICATIONS 13,596 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Nigel Lambert
University of East Anglia
93 PUBLICATIONS 3,287 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Gene Rowe on 28 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


science
science&& society
society

The risks of eating and drinking


Consumer perceptions and ‘reality’
Susan B.T. Wilkinson, Gene Rowe & Nigel Lambert

L A
ife is full of risk—everyday we ccording to the UK Food Standards
encounter all sorts of hazards, some Agency (2000), most cases of micro-
of which are unavoidable. Eating and bial food contamination are caused
drinking are paramount to maintaining by five types of bacteria: Salmonella,
life, but unfortunately, are also coupled Campylobacter, Escherichia coli O157,
with a multitude of potential and some- Listeria monocytogenes and Clostridium
times fatal health risks. These risks are not perfringens. There were at least 81,000
to be taken lightly, which is highlighted by cases of microbial food poisoning in the
the current increase in obesity and related UK in 2000, with Campylobacter and
diseases in the developed world, a trend Salmonella responsible for 63,000 and
that has the World Health Organization 17,000 cases, respectively. Symptoms usu-
worrying about a growing epidemic of ally develop within 12–36 hours and com-
obesity (WHO/FAO, 2003). Similarly, pub- monly include abdominal pain, diarrhoea,
lic health officials are increasingly con- fever and vomiting. Microbial contamina-
cerned about a dramatic increase in dia- tion typically causes 100–200 fatalities per
betes and coronary heart diseases that are year, mostly in young children and the
caused by an inappropriate and ultimately elderly. However, the real number of food-
hazardous diet. But food holds many more borne illnesses may be higher as many cases
health risks, and recognizing these would go unnoticed and/or unreported. One of the
greatly benefit human health. most serious food contaminations is the
and that scientists and policy makers infection of cattle with bovine spongiform
would do well to consider the basis for encephalopathy (BSE) and the resulting risks
…obesity has become a leading consumer beliefs in order to preempt of developing variant Creutzfeldt–Jakob
cause of death in the USA, future food- and drink-related health and disease (vCJD), a fatal human illness with no
second only to smoking policy crises. known cure that has caused more than 100
A comprehensive list of all hazards asso- deaths in the UK so far.
ciated with eating and drinking would Recent examples of chemical contami-
The extent to which consumers identify clearly be beyond the scope of this article. nations of food include high amounts of
and appreciate food-related risks, and Furthermore, the nature and degree of the mercury in fish and elevated levels of
allow this knowledge to direct their eating different food and drink hazards is often the known carcinogen acrylamide, intro-
and drinking behaviour, is therefore of specific for both time and culture. For duced through the cooking process into
great research interest. In this article, we instance, the lack of clean drinking water some popular foods such as chips and
first detail the most significant sources of without microbial contamination is no crisps. Similarly, barbecuing certain foods
food and drink health risks and discuss longer a serious public health problem in creates carcinogenic dioxins and nitroso-
the implications of consumers’ percep- the UK. But it was a problem as recently as compounds. Finally, the contamination of
tions of these hazards. This discussion 150 years ago and it continues to be so foods with physical objects, such as glass,
touches on generic research on how peo- today for large parts of the developing metal, wood, insects and other objects,
ple perceive risks and considers the rea- world. We therefore concentrate on the remains a major concern especially for
sons behind apparent ‘misperceptions’ of most prevalent hazards in today’s food sup- food processors and packagers.
risks. We also want to emphasize that, in ply, focusing on UK examples, although Many foods contain colourants, flavour-
the face of limited scientific knowledge, they are liable to be pertinent to other ings, preservatives and antioxidants, some
consumer concerns may be well grounded, developed countries too. of which have been associated with

©2004 EUROPEAN MOLECULAR BIOLOGY ORGANIZATION EMBO reports VOL 5 | SPECIAL ISSUE | 2004 S 2 7
science & society special issue

P
adverse health consequences. The com- ublic health policies linked to food or delayed from a day or two (vomiting
mon sweetener aspartame has been are increasingly focusing on the haz- after microbial food poisoning) to years or
linked to such conditions as brain ards from eating a nutritionally sub- even decades (CHD from the continual
tumours, multiple sclerosis, Parkinson’s standard diet (Department of Health, consumption of an inappropriate diet).
disease, Alzheimer’s disease, arthritis and 2000). Current ‘Western diets’ with their One important source of variance is the
attention deficit disorder (Chivers, 2000). relatively high amounts of fat, sugar and extent to which people themselves can
The preservative sulphite can cause asth- salt and low amounts of fruits and vegeta- control their level of exposure. It is proba-
mas and urticaria (Allergy Clinic, 2003). bles, are risk factors for the development bly fair to say that at this moment in histo-
Another preservative, butylated hydroxy- of numerous cancers and heart diseases; ry, the food and drink in the developed
toluene (BHT), although not toxic itself, as many as 1 in 3 of all cancers may be world is as safe as it has ever been, due to
may interact with other substances to diet-related. Furthermore, such diets have improved food production practices and
increase cancer risk (Magnuson, 1997). contributed to the astonishing threefold public hygiene standards. However, in
rise in obesity over the past 20 years. At some cases, inappropriate behaviour con-
present, nearly half of the UK population tinues to put consumers at significant risk
…barbecuing certain foods is either overweight or obese, which is a from a variety of the hazards indicated. To
creates carcinogenic dioxins and major risk factor for both CHD and dia- improve consumer health and reduce
nitroso-compounds betes. In March this year, Secretary Tommy food and drink risks, it is therefore impor-
G. Thompson of the US Department of tant to understand consumers, particular-
Health and Human Services warned that ly why they act as they do.
A food allergy is an immune response obesity has become a leading cause of
to a food that the body mistakenly death in the USA, second only to smoking
believes to be harmful. Symptoms are (The New York Times, 2004). This situation …any ingested substance, even
usually quick to develop and can range is chronic in that it is the continual con- water, is potentially harmful if
from mild itching and rashes to more sumption of an inappropriate diet over consumed in large enough
severe swelling of the mouth and throat decades that leads to these health hazards. quantities
through to anaphylaxis, circulatory col- There are further risks, perhaps of less
lapse and death within a few minutes. In immediate severity, associated with con-
theory, any food can cause an allergy, but suming such a diet; for example, an While some food-related behaviours
in the UK 90% of all allergic reactions are increased level of tooth decay. are simply habitual, and others are driven
due to milk, eggs, peanuts, tree nuts, fish, This list is by no means comprehen- by a lack of knowledge, such as inappro-
shellfish, soy and wheat. Although rela- sive; it does not include risks from drink- priate storage and cooking of foods, the
tively uncommon, the incidence of food ing alcoholic beverages for instance. way people perceive and judge the risks
allergies is increasing; hospital admis- Indeed, any ingested substance, even of specific hazards may lead them to
sions due to anaphylaxis have increased water, is potentially harmful if consumed behave seemingly perversely in some situ-
sevenfold over the past decade and food in large enough quantities. Furthermore, ations. Many people voluntarily engage in
allergy is one of the most common causes. there are about 16,000 cases of choking hazards with considerable levels of risk,
At present, there is no cure for food allergies per year in the UK that are serious enough while they may go to great lengths to
other than avoidance. for a hospital visit. These most commonly avoid hazards that pose only little risk.
Food intolerances are due to enzyme involve children (under four years of Still, many people choose to smoke,
deficiencies and other mechanisms that age) and the elderly, and are knowing that smoking
mimic allergic reactions. These reactions caused mostly by causes cancer, and
are rarely life-threatening. Intolerances to food. Nevertheless, others indulge in
wheat (gluten intolerance or coeliac dis- what the list
ease) and milk (intolerances to lactose or does reveal is
milk proteins) are the most common in the how diverse
UK. In Asia and Africa, most adults are the potential
lactose intolerant. Sufferers of various risks from
conditions also need to avoid specific eating and
foods. For example, individuals with dia- drinking are,
betes must closely regulate their glucose with conse-
intake to avoid hypo- and hyper- quences
glycaemia, which can have both severe varying from
acute and chronic adverse effects. the mild (nau-
Sufferers of the genetic disorder phenyl- sea) to the
ketonuria must avoid phenylalanine in extreme (death
their food, and individuals with coronary from anaphylactic
heart disease (CHD) are strongly advised shock), and manifested
to limit their cholesterol intake to reduce either immediately (for
the risk of heart attacks. example choking on a fish bone),

S 2 8 EMBO reports VOL 5 | SPECIAL ISSUE | 2004 ©2004 EUROPEAN MOLECULAR BIOLOGY ORGANIZATION
special issue science & society
high-risk sports knowing the danger of
breaking bones. The UK has seen a
substantial decline in parents inoculat-
ing their children with the combined
MMR (measles, mumps, rubella) vaccine
for fear of inducing autism in spite of
evidence to the contrary, and thereby
increasing the risk of a serious national
measles epidemic (Murch, 2003).
Evidence suggests that in some cases
consumer responses to food and
drink hazards are similarly ‘illogical’—
although perhaps not to the extent they at
first seem.

O
f the list of potential hazards dis-
cussed in the previous section,
only a relative few have been
studied from a consumer perspective. This
choice perhaps reflects contemporary
concerns, rather than any attempt to mea-
sure and compare different risks and haz-
ards systematically. Nevertheless, these
studies show that consumers’ behaviour
and attitudes towards these hazards indi-
cate a different ranking of risks in their
minds. If we take contaminants as an
example, consumers can be highly sensi-
tive to information about relatively minor
incidences of physical contamination in
foods—such as epoxidized soya bean oil,
used to seal jars of baby food, that was
found to seep into the product (BBC,
1999)—and consequently avoid the rele-
vant products. However, it is likely that a
high proportion of these same people do
not wash their hands after going to the
lavatory—in spite of having some knowl-
edge of the risks involved in this activity—
thereby increasing the chance of

O
microbiological food contamination. intake of fruit and vegetables—are appar- ther research has taken a compar-
Although determining an exact figure is ent in inappropriate nutrition, which ative approach, looking at rela-
difficult, it is estimated that 17% of occurs in spite of people knowing, at tive perceptions of a variety of
women and 31% of men do not regularly least at a qualitative level, what a food hazards, which also reveals instances
wash their hands after using the toilet ‘healthy’ diet is. of apparent perceptual discrepancies. For
(Food Standards Agency, 2000). In other cases, such as with choking example, Fife-Schaw & Rowe (1996)
Similar cases of apparent overreaction and food intolerance, there is only limited found that people were more concerned
have been found regarding food allergies. information about consumer perceptions. about botulism than about saturated fats
Some sources have suggested that as One interesting example of apparent mis- and cholesterol, in spite of the risks of the
many as 20%–30% of adults in the UK perception concerns an issue that does latter being greater. Kirk et al (2002) found
believe they have a food allergy or intoler- not even appear in our list of food haz- a similar pattern, with saturated fats being
ance, but official figures are 1%–2%. ards, namely, genetically modified (GM) the least dreaded hazard and Salmonella
Likewise, some parents readily attribute foods. European consumers remain among the most dreaded.
hyperactivity in their children to various deeply concerned about GM foods, as These studies show that consumers’
additives in confectionery, despite only shown time and again by various surveys perceptions of risk do not necessarily
tenuous evidence for such a link (Food (Shaw, 2002). This is despite an absence correspond with scientific assessments.
Commission, 2002). Conversely, cases of of scientific evidence that anyone has or Consequently, the risk communication field
‘under-reaction’—that is, under-con- will be adversely affected (GM Science has traditionally assumed that consumers
sumption of the recommended daily Review, 2004). are ignorant, in some cases hysterical, and

©2004 EUROPEAN MOLECULAR BIOLOGY ORGANIZATION EMBO reports VOL 5 | SPECIAL ISSUE | 2004 S 2 9
science & society special issue

Certainly, past encounters with is a good rule to use. However, recall can
food scares have left consumers be influenced by many factors, one of
which is how vivid is an event. Images of
with an understandable level of
personal suffering from vCJD are highly
caution… dramatic, perhaps more so than images
of people suffering from heart disease as
for the most part misinterpret statistical like- a possible consequence of poor diet.
lihood. From this perspective, the so-called Consequently, using the availability heuris-
‘deficit model’, the appropriate presenta- tic alone may lead people to overestimate
tion of ‘the truth’ is all that is needed to dis- some risks and underestimate others.
pel ‘faulty’ beliefs and induce ‘correct’ Optimistic bias is another phenomenon
behaviour (Hansen et al, 2003). However, associated with apparent misperception of
reality is more complex than this: although risks (Weinstein, 1980). Research has found
in some cases apparent misperceptions that people tend to believe that they are less
appear to be related to cognitive limita- likely to experience negative events and
tions, in other cases consumers’ beliefs more likely to experience positive events
seem to be based on alternative, sensibly when compared with other people. For ‘number of people exposed’, which subse-
informed frameworks. example, people may believe themselves quent research has largely replicated (Fife-
less at risk from the effects of a diet rich with Schaw & Rowe, 1996).

P
eople frequently use a number of high-fat foods than similar others, therefore In real terms, this means that con-
quick decision-making rules when reducing the need to change their behav- sumers are frequently interpreting risk in a
dealing with uncertain situations. In iour. The underlying causes of this are still non-statistical manner, which is arguably
many cases these so-called heuristics, or in dispute, and range from the cognitive to quite sensible in incorporating uncertainty.
‘rules of thumb’, allow people to function the motivational or some combination of Taking GM foods as an example, while it
successfully in everyday life by making the the two (Miles & Scaife, 2003). may be true that scientific evidence sug-
most efficient use of limited cognitive abili- gests that the actual risk is minimal, con-
ties and time, and their inability to cope sumers may be considering the novelty of
consumers can be highly
with vast amounts of information. Cognitive the technology and hence assume that
research suggests five situations in which sensitive to information about there may well be risks that have not yet
people may be prone to using such rules: relatively minor incidences of been identified. When there are familiar
when they are overloaded with informa- physical contamination in foods and relatively risk-free alternatives to GM
tion; when they do not have enough time; foods available, who is to say that con-
when the issues are not overly important to sumer avoidance of GM foods is illogical?
them; when they have little knowledge or Although human cognitive limitations Certainly, past encounters with food
information on the topic; and when a may at times lead to faulty judgments about scares have left consumers with an under-
specific shortcut comes to mind easily risk, at other times—or perhaps even simul- standable level of caution; for example,
(Pratkanis & Aronson, 1992). Tversky & taneously—differences between consumer when UK authorities maintained in the
Kahneman (1974) identified several of estimations and expert-derived statistics early days of the BSE crisis that eating beef
these heuristics including availability, rep- may be a consequence of alternative fram- was safe, which then turned out to be
resentativeness, and anchoring and adjust- ings of, or emphasis on, the issue. In otherwise. This example highlights the
ment. If we take the availability heuristic as essence, consumers do not understand important role of risk communicators as
an example, this leads people to judge the ‘risk’ in the same way as do scientists. This sources of information, and indicates
likelihood of an event based on how easily discovery of Paul Slovic and colleagues has how consumer perceptions can mediate
they can recall similar past cases. Often this been replicated in different ways by other reactions to their messages.
researchers and demonstrates that, when

P
judging risk, consumers take into account ublic reactions to risk communica-
other qualitative aspects beyond the likeli- tions are frequently determined by fac-
hood of ensuing harm. For example, Slovic tors other than just information, such
et al (1980) reported that two dimensions as the credibility and trustworthiness of the
were important when laypeople judge risks: information source. Indeed, trust in industry
the first aspect being the extent to which a and government can be such an important
hazard is ‘dreaded’ (severe, likely, uncon- factor in risk perception in some cases that it
trollable, involuntary, catastrophic) and the may supersede the inherent characteristics
second being how ‘known’ the hazard is of the specific hazard (Senauer, 1992).
(known to science, new, has delayed Clearly, trust has a crucial influence on risk
effects). Sparks & Shepherd (1994), using a perception, although the exact relationship
similar methodology to consider food haz- between trust, perceived risk and hazard
ards, found three perceptual dimensions acceptability is less clear (Eiser et al, 2002).
that they termed ‘severity’, ‘unknown’ and Its relevance to the food domain was shown

S 3 0 EMBO reports VOL 5 | SPECIAL ISSUE | 2004 ©2004 EUROPEAN MOLECULAR BIOLOGY ORGANIZATION
special issue science & society
in a large US study, which found that trust in ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Shaw A (2002) “It just goes against the grain”. Public
the regulation of pesticides was positively The authors would like to acknowledge funding from understandings of genetically modified (GM) food
the Core Strategic Grant of the UK Biotechnology in the UK. Public Understanding Sci 11: 273–291
correlated with the perceived benefits of Senauer B (1992) Consumer food safety concerns.
and Biological Sciences Research Council.
such products (Dittus et al, 1993). Bord & Cereal Foods World 37: 298–303
O’Connor (1992) reported similar results Slovic P, Fischhoff B, Lichtenstein S (1980) in
REFERENCES
with food irradiation. Societal Risk Assessment: How Safe is Safe
Allergy Clinic (2004) Food allergy and additive
Enough? (eds Schwing RC, Albers WA)
Unfortunately, the main communicators intolerance. www.allergy-clinic.co.uk/
pp 181–216. New York, USA: Plenum
of food hazard information are not particu- food_allergy_for_doctors.htm
Sparks P, Shepherd R (1994) Public perceptions of
larly well trusted: in general, medical Bord RJ, O’Connor RE (1992) Determinants of risk
the potential hazards associated with food
perceptions of a hazardous waste site. Risk Anal
sources are the most trusted and govern- 12: 411–416
production and food consumption: an empirical
ment and industry the least. However, con- study. Risk Anal 14: 799–806
BBC News Online (1999) Baby food toxic chemical
The New York Times (2004) Death rate from obesity
sumers frequently expect food hazard alert. 15 Sept. http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/health/
gains fast on smoking. 10 Mar p A16
information to come from government 448122.stm
Tversky A, Kahneman D (1974) Judgement under
BBC News Online (2003) Anti-allergy foods ‘more
sources (Frewer et al, 2001), and also uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science 185:
popular’. 13 Mar. http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/
expect the government to make risk deci- 1124–1131
health/2842611.stm
Weinstein ND (1980) Unrealistic optimism about
sions on their behalf when needed (Earle & Chivers L (2000) Can we tell if aspartame is safe?
future life events. J Personality Social Psychol 39:
Cvetkovich, 1995). But if these sources are 7 Jan. http://serendip.brynmawr.edu/bb/neuro/
806–820
not trusted, this creates both a communica- neuro00/web2/Chivers.html
WHO/FAO (2003) Diet, nutrition and the
Department of Health (2000) The NHS Plan: a plan
tion and policy dilemma. Lack of con- for investment, a plan for reform. London, UK: The
prevention of chronic diseases. Joint report of the
sumer trust thus has important implications WHO/FAO Expert Consultation. Geneva,
Stationery Office
Switzerland: World Health Organization
for the communication of food hazard Dittus KL, Hillers VN, Beerman KA (1993) Attitudes
information and subsequent health policy. and behaviours about pesticide residues,
susceptibility to cancer, and consumption of fruits
and vegetables. J Nutr Edu 25: 245–250
European consumers remain Earle TC, Cvetkovich GT (1995) Social trust: towards
a cosmopolitan society. Westport, CT, USA:
deeply concerned about GM Praeger
foods… despite an absence of Eiser JR, Miles S, Frewer LJ (2002) Trust, perceived
risk and attitudes toward food technologies.
scientific evidence that anyone J Appl Soc Psychol 32: 2423–2433
has or will be adversely affected Fife-Schaw C, Rowe G (1996) Public perceptions of
everyday hazards: a psychometric study. Risk Anal
16: 487–500
Food Commission (2002) Additives do cause temper
In summary, while consumers are
tantrums. 25 Oct. www.foodcomm.org.uk/
faced with numerous different food haz- additive_2002_full.htm
ards, their perceptions of the risks associ- Food Standards Agency (2000) Handwashing. 11
ated with them do not always correspond Feb. www.foodstandards.gov.uk/hygcampaign/
to scientific risk estimates, although the keepingfoodsafe/handwashing?
Frewer LJ, Salter B Lambert N (2001) Understanding
levels of risk associated with many of patients’ preference for treatment: the need for
these hazards are scientifically uncertain. innovative methodologies. Quality in Healthcare
Past risk communication strategies 13 Mar, i50–54
assumed that these differences were GM Science Review (2) (2004) Final Report.
www.gmsciencedebate.org.uk/report/pdf/
essentially a consequence of consumer
gmsci-report2-pt1.pdf
ignorance of ‘the facts’ and that presenta- Hansen J, Holm L, Frewer L, Robinson P, Sandoe P
tion of ‘the truth’ would cure misjudge- (2003) Beyond the knowledge deficit: recent
ments. However, although cognitive limi- research into lay and expert attitudes to food risks.
tations occasionally do hamper consumer Appetite 41: 111–121
Kirk SFL, Greenwood D, Cade JE, Pearman AD
judgements of risks, at other times, con- (2002) Public perception of a range of potential
sumers’ judgements are often understand- food risks in the United Kingdom. Appetite 38:
able and consider many other qualitative 189–197
factors than are accounted for in a stan- Magnuson B (1997) What is the evidence for a link
between preservatives and cancer and other toxic
dard risk assessment. It is therefore impor-
effects? 13 Mar. http://ace.orst.edu/info/extoxnet/
tant for scientists, communicators and faqs/additive/preserca.htm
policy makers to consider the basis of Miles S, Scaife V (2003) Optimistic bias and food.
these qualitative consumer beliefs, both Nutr Res Rev 16: 3–19 Susan B.T. Wilkinson, Gene Rowe and Nigel
to preempt future food-related health and Murch S (2003) Separating inflammation from
Lambert are at the Institute of Food Research in
speculation in autism. Lancet 362: 1498–1499
policy crises and to communicate Norwich, Norfolk, UK.
Pratkanis AR, Aronson E (1992) The Age of
more effectively before, during and after E-mail: susan.wilkinson@bbsrc.ac.uk
Propaganda: The Everyday Use and Abuse of
such events. Persuasion. New York, USA: WH Freeman doi:10.1038/sj.embor.7400225

©2004 EUROPEAN MOLECULAR BIOLOGY ORGANIZATION EMBO reports VOL 5 | SPECIAL ISSUE | 2004 S 3 1

View publication stats

You might also like