You are on page 1of 19

Psychological Bulletin

1080. Vol. 87, No. 1, 166-184

Analysis of Variance and the "Second Discipline"


of Scientific Psychology: A Historical Account
Anthony J. Rucci and Ryan D. Tweney
Bowling Green State University

A historical analysis of the period from 1925 to 1950 was conducted to inves-
tigate the incorporation of analysis of variance (ANOVA) techniques in psycho-
logical research. In addition to attempts to identify the earliest uses of ANOVA
in psychology, the gradual incorporation of the technique was examined by
counting its appearance in 6,457 articles appearing in major American psycho-
logical journals from 1935 through 1952. Expository articles and texts directed
at psychologists were also identified, and a questionnaire survey of graduate
psychology programs was conducted to establish how psychologists were intro-
duced to ANOVA. Finally, the phylogeny of major contributors to American psy-
chological statistics was established for the period. The data suggest a segmen-
tation into three historical periods: (a) an initial, expository phase lasting until
the onset of World War II, (b) a wartime interregnum during which use of
ANOVA declined, and (c) a postwar resurgence, characterized by the institu-
tionalization of ANOVA training. Although ANOVA certainly had a major impact
on experimental psychology, the data do not permit the conclusion that the
incorporation of ANOVA in psychology in itself constituted a revolutionary
"paradigm shift."

In 1957 Cronbach, in a presidential ad- Cronbach asserted that "Fisher made the
dress to the American Psychological Associa- experimentalist an expert puppeteer, able to
tion, described two disciplines of scientific keep untangled the strands to half-a-dozen
psychology: the correlational and the experi- independent variables (p. 675)."
mental. He characterized the experimental Although it may be superfluous to reassert
school by its reliance on analysis of variance that the ANOVA has been the workhorse sta-
(ANOVA) techniques for research design, in tistic of experimental psychology, it is not
contrast to the correlational techniques fa- superfluous to consider how this technique
vored by the other school. Referring to the carne to dominate experimental design in psy-
English statistician who developed ANOVA, chology. Not until the late 1930s did ANOVA
begin to appear in psychological journals, and
yet by 1952 it was fully established as the
The authors would like to acknowledge contribu- most frequently used technique in experi-
tions in the form of personal communications from
Harold Bechtoldt, Isadore Blumen, Alphonse Cha-
mental research. It took less than 15 years
panis, C. J. Cranny, Lee Cronbach, Allen L. Ed- for psychology to incorporate ANOVA.
wards, Nathaniel Gage, Bert Green, J. P. Guilford, The purpose of the present study was to
Harold Gulliksen, Lloyd Humphreys, E. F. Lind- develop a historical account of psychology's
quist, Quinn McNemar, Don Ragusa, T. A. Ryan,
Patricia C. Smith, and Ray Tucker.
incorporation of ANOVA from Fisher's initial
A brief version of this article was presented at presentation of the technique (1925b)
the annual convention of the American Psychologi- through the early 1950s. It became clear in
cal Association in Toronto, Ontario, Canada, August the course of this research that experimental
1978. design in psychology changed radically in the
Requests for reprints should be sent to Anthony
J. Rucci, who is now at the American Hospital
period in question: Single-variable designs
Supply Corporation, 1 American Plaza, Evanston, were being used less frequently from 1940
Illinois 60201. on, whereas factorial designs were used more

Copyright 1980 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0033-2909/80/8701-0166S00.75

166
ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE 167

frequently. The temptation, therefore, is to 1951; and in Walker, 1940). In 1876, an


conclude that variance techniques caused a astronomer named Helmert had mathemati-
profound change in the way psychologists cally derived the exact distribution of the
approached research. It is, of course, impossi- sum of squared deviations of a normal variate
ble to assess causal relations in this type of about the population mean (cited in Wilks,
historical investigation. Although it might 1932). Prior to 1950 developments in design
appear that variance techniques had sub- were overwhelmingly centered in agricultural
stantial impact on psychology, it is just as research. Boussingault began field experimen-
plausible that the discipline had developed in tation as early as 1834, and in 1841 Lawes
such a way that the time was ripe for the established in England what would become
adoption of the technique. Did ANOVA affect the nucleus of research on experimental de-
psychological design, or did psychological sign—the Rothamsted Experimental Station.
design affect the adoption of ANOVA? The Longitudinal research at Rothamsted from
question is unanswerable, given our data. It 1843 to the present day has had a profound
is clear, however, that changes in psychologi- influence on modern experimental design and
cal design were concomitant with the incorpo- analysis. It was there that Fisher developed
ration of variance techniques. A characteri- the ANOVA technique in the 1920s.
zation of this incorporation, rather than an Gossett, writing under the pseudonym of
assessment of causality, was the goal of this Student, was the first to suggest the form of
investigation. the distribution of the sum of squared devia-
Five approaches were taken to trace psy- tions about a sample mean (Gossett, 1908).
chology's incorporation of variance tech- Student's work was of immediate relevance to
niques: (a) identification of early published the development of ANOVA. Fisher (1915)
uses and expositions of the technique, (b) a rigorously developed the sampling distribu-
frequency count to trace the growth in use tion for the t statistic and showed its appli-
of the technique from 193S on, (c) a review cation to experimental data (Fisher, 1925a).
of early statistical research on variance tech- Fisher's work led to the development of the
niques done by psychologists, (d) identifica- r-to-2 statistic, which is equal to one half of
tion of the earliest behavioral science texts the difference between the natural logarithms
to include the technique, and (e) a question- of two estimated variances: z = -}|log (1 —
naire survey of graduate curricula. The results /•) — log (1 + > ' ) ] • When r in this equation is
are presented following a brief summary of equal to the intraclass correlation, z provides
the statistical development of ANOVA, a topic a test statistic for the difference between two
that has been thoroughly discussed by others independent variance estimates. In Fisher's
(e.g., Hotelling, 1951; Johnson, 1943; Ma- 1925(b) presentation of ANOVA technique,
halanobis, 1938; Stanley, 1966; Yates, 1951). this variance difference logarithm was tabled
for use in determining the significance of
Statistical Development of ANOVA difference between two variances. It was not
until 1934 that Snedecor tabled mean square
Fisher pointed out in 1935 that ratios in the now common F tables. Snedecor
chose to call his tabled values F ratios in
statistical procedure and experimental design are commemoration of Fisher. It is interesting to
only two different aspects of the same whole, and note that through all later editions of the
that whole is the logical requirements of the com-
plete process of adding to natural knowledge by
1925(b) text (the last edition was 1970)
experimentation, (p. 3) Fisher never included F tables but rather re-
tained only the tabled r-lo-z values.
Although Fisher's (1925b, 1935) two texts One must be cautious in citing the first
are the seminal works on ANOVA, statistical presentation of any concept in research litera-
developments leading to the technique go as ture. Although most writers cite Fisher's
far back as Gauss who, as early as 1826, de- 1925(b) text as the original reference to
scribed the concepts of degrees of freedom ANOVA, two articles referred to the technique
and least squares (cited in Hotelling, 1943, in 1923. Fisher and MacKenzie (1923) sub-
168 ANTHONY J. RUCCI AND RYAN D. TWENEY

divided the total variance of the results of an today, had its beginning with Sir Ronald A. Fisher
experiment to test whether different varieties and began in the second quarter of the twentieth
century. (Federer & Balaam, 1973, p. 40)
of potatoes responded differently to potash
fertilizers. The subcomponents of variance for Early Uses of ANOVA in Psychology
differences between manures and differences
between varieties were tested for significance Our first approach was to trace the devel-
against a variance due to variations between opment of variance techniques in psychology
plots. These data were reanalyzed in the by identifying early published articles in be-
1925(b) text because, according to Yates havioral research that used ANOVA or proposed
(1964), Fisher realized the need to partition it as a design and analysis alternative. An
the error term due to the arrangement of extensive review was made of all located
plots in the original design. articles that used ANOVA prior to 1940. In ad-
The second 1923 reference to ANOVA was an dition, a review was made of all located arti-
article by Student (Gossett, 1923), on testing cles through 1950 that recommended the use
varieties of cereals. Student quoted a letter of variance techniques in psychology or dem-
that he received from Fisher regarding the onstrated their application to psychological
breakdown of variances for trials and varie- research problems.
ties. Fisher presented a table in this letter Table 1 lists 17 published studies in psy-
that is nearly identical to the form in which chological research that used ANOVA prior to
ANOVA results are reported today. 1940. These studies were identified using
The 1925(b) text, Statistical Methods for personal communications from Cronbach
Research Workers, was the first didactic pre- (Note 1) and a systematic survey of approxi-
sentation of ANOVA, It was not a compre- mately 50 pre-1940 journals. Although some
hensive presentation, however, and in fact pre-1940 studies may not have been discov-
only two chapters out of eight were directly ered, Table 1 should contain the majority of
related to ANOVA. The remainder of the text published psychological research articles to
was devoted to a discussion of the scope and use ANOVA prior to 1940.
characteristics of statistics, the Poisson dis- A number of similar articles appear in
tribution, chi-square, and the t statistic. Be- Table 1. Three used covariance analysis
sides the presentation of ANOVA, the fourth (Dressel, 1939; Gaskill & Cox, 1937; Snede-
edition of this text (Fisher, 1932) included cor, 1935), 2 investigated memorization of
Fisher's first presentation of the analysis of piano music (Rubin-Rabson, 1937, 1939), 2
covariance (ANCOVA). studied differences in alpha rhythms between
Soon after the initial presentation of ANOVA, normal and abnormal individuals (Kreezer,
Fisher became acutely aware of the relation- 1939; Rubin, 1938), 3 applied ANOVA to
ship between design and analysis (Fisher, evaluating validity of test items and test
1926). The 1935 text, The Design oj Experi- reliability (Baker, 1939; Jackson, 1939; Lev,
ments, was the culmination of the interfacing 1938), and 4 were concerned with the quanti-
of design and analysis. This was the first tative study of trance personalities (Caring-
comprehensive presentation of factorial design ton, 1934, 1936, 1937; Thouless, 1937). Of
and analysis. The first 113 pages of this text the 17 articles listed, 16 involved research
are an elegant presentation of the concepts primarily relating to or using human subjects.
of null hypothesis testing, randomization, and Only 1 study (Crutchfield, 1939) used ANOVA
inductive inference. in research with animals. Twelve of the arti-
Fisher's contributions to statistics were cles were in psychological publications, 4
numerous, and although some colleagues appeared in educational journals, and 1 ap-
claimed that his work was not new in a revo- peared in a statistical journal.
lutionary sense, he clearly exerted a profound The earliest behavioral research to use
influence on contemporary experimental de- ANOVA was reported by Reitz (1934) in the
sign and analysis: Journal of Experimental Education. Reitz
presented the formulas for computing a z
It might be stated that experiment design as known value, noted the assumption of homogeneity
ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE 169

Table 1
Psychological Articles Using Analysis of Variance Prior to 1940

Author Source Description Design

Reitz (1934) Journal of Experimental Tested grade differences One-way ANOVA


Education between college classes
Carington Proceedings of the Society for Psychical research on One-way ANOVA
(1934) Psychical Research trances
Snedecor Journal of the American Covariance applied to One-way ANOVA
(1935) Statistical A ssociation college math grades
Carington Proceedings of the Society for Psychical research on One-way ANOVA
(1936) Psychical Research trances
Carington Proceedings of the Society for Psychical research on One-way ANOVA
(1937) Psychical Research trances
Gaskill & Cox Journal of General Covariance applied to One-way ANOVA
(1937) Psychology respiration rates
Rubin- Rabson Archives of Psychology Investigated memorization Factorial
(1937) of piano music (4X4X4X4)
Thouless Proceedings of the Society for Psychical research on Factorial
(1937) Psychical Research trances
Rubin (1938) Journal of Psychology Studied alpha rhythms, Factorial
normal vs. schizophrenic
Lev (1938) Journal of Educational Evaluated test-item One-way ANOVA
Psychology validity
Gottsdanker Journal of Experimental Studied student test Factorial (5 X 5)
(1939) Psychology response patterns
Baker (1939) Psychological Record Evaluated test-item One-way ANOVA
validity
Rubin-Rabson Journal of Educational Investigated memorization Factorial
(1939) Psychology of piano music (4X4X4X4)
Kreezer American Journal of Studied alpha rhythms, One-way ANOVA
(1939) Psychology normals vs. Mongoloid
Dressel Journal of Educational Covariance applied to One-way ANOVA
(1939) Psychology high school grades
Crutchfield Journal of Psychology Studied string pulling in Factorial
(1939) rats (3X3X3X3X3)
Jackson British Journal of Psychology Evaluated test reliability One-way ANOVA
(1939)

Note. ANOVA = analysis of variance.

of variance, and checked his computed z was a direct application of covariance analy-
value by outlining the relationship between sis to a behavioral research problem. This
the correlation ratio (rj-) and z. Given that article represents one of the earliest uses of
only Fisher's editions of the 1925(b) text applied variance techniques. Smith, the head
were available to Reitz, this was a remarkably of the mathematics department at Iowa State,
competent use of the technique. wanted to determine if different grading prac-
Four of the earliest articles to use ANOVA tices by teachers were responsible for dif-
were in research on trance personalities ferences between mathematics classes or if the
(Carington, 1934, 1936, 1937; Thouless, different grades reflected different levels of
1937) through the London-based Society for achievement. Snedecor applied covariance
Psychical Research. After these four very analysis to the problem and tested for class
early articles, however, use of variance tech- differences on a common exam.
niques completely disappeared from the jour- A wide range of statistical sophistication
nal. was demonstrated by these early articles. The
Although Snedecor's (1935) covariance Snedecor (193S) and Gaskill and Cox (1937)
analysis of grades appeared in a statistical articles were complex statistical applications,
journal, we have included it here since it particularly in view of the newness of the
170 ANTHONY J. RUCCI AND RYAN D. TWENEY

techniques. Even more striking, however, was of Peters and VanVoorhis (1940) in a chap-
the extent to which the authors of these ter entitled "The Technique of Controlled
early articles acknowledged the new technique Experimentation":
being used. Reitz (1934), Gaskill and Cox
( 1 9 3 7 ) , and Thouless (1937), for example, ll seems scarcely necessary for us to say here that
care must be exercised to keep all other conditions
made a point of telling the reader that the constant in the two situations except the one experi-
statistical technique being used was both new mental factor; this is the law of the single variable,
and particularly appropriate for research so fundamental to all scientific experimentation.
problems in psychology. On the other hand, (p. 44V)
Rubin-Rabson (1939) did not acknowledge
The pervasiveness of the single-variable law
the newness of variance analysis, and Rubin
in behavioral research was felt well into the
(1938) cited no statistical text or article rele-
late 1930s and early 1940s.
vant to the statistical treatment used!
Baxter (1941) noted that Fisher's methods
Although the pre-1940 articles were pio-
had only recently begun to appear in psycho-
neering applications of ANOVA, some of these
logical journals, but these methods frequently
authors displayed uncertainty regarding the
appeared in journals of agriculture and biol-
technique. Kreezer (1939) provides the best
ogy. Baxter's article outlined the analogy
example of this uneasiness. After pointing out
between the agricultural terminology associ-
that F was equal to t~, Kreezer analyzed his
ated with Fisher's method and psychological
data using both t and F, saying, "Both meth-
terminology. The fundamental analogy pro-
ods have, nevertheless, been used in this study
posed by Baxter was
so that they may provide a check on each
other'' (p. 5 2 2 ) . Treatment: Soil: :
A systematic survey of psychological jour- Experimental variable: Organism.
nals through 19SO turned up 66 articles that
were identified as expository, written with Further, he stated that "treatment corre-
the stated intention of presenting a new sta- sponds to the traditional term independent
tistical technique to be applied to psychologi- variable" (p. 270). An article by Garrett and
cal research. These included articles that en- Zubin (1943) gave extensive treatment to po-
dorsed ANOVA for psychological research and tential applications of ANOVA in psychological
also those that demonstrated its application research. Grant (1944) published a rejoinder
to psychological research problems. to this article, pointing out errors of omission
Three articles, Crutchfield (1938), Baxter in Garrett and Zubin's discussion of some
(1941), and Garrett and Zubin (1943), were early psychological articles using ANOVA.
major efforts toward "psychologizing" the Many other articles that need not be re-
ANOVA technique. In 1938, Crutchfield il- viewed here endorsed ANOVA for psychology
lustrated the application of factorial design to (Bloomers & Lindquist, 1942; Burt, 1938;
a study of the perseverative performance of Crutchfield & Tolman, 1940; Dunlap, 1938,
rats in a string-pulling apparatus. Only the 1940, 1941; Garrett, 1943; Grant, 1944,
layout of the research design, in accordance 19SO; Hotelling, 1935; Royce, 19SO; Thou-
with Fisher's technique, was reported, not the less, 1939).
results of the study. Crutchfield was un- A major portion of the 66 expository arti-
equivocal in his endorsement of factorial cles were applications of variance analysis.
designs: The titles of a number of these articles began
with the words "An application of" ANOVA or
Whenever, in experimental, comparative, or social factorial design to a particular research issue.
psychology, a systematic investigation of the pri- Jackson's (1940) research bulletin was fre-
mary effects and the interacting effects of a number quently cited by later researchers. Other arti-
of experimentally controllable factors is being con-
ducted, the principles of efficient factorial design cles on applications ranged from studies of
can be invoked with inestimable benefit, (p. 341) educational development (Johnson & Tsao,
1945) to galvanic skin response data (Hag-
Contrast this quote by Crutchfield with that gard, 1949), from liberalism scores (Schrader,
ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE 171

1940) to reaction times (Baxter, 1942), and fact, as late as 1944, Grant used the future
from lightness and saturation (Helson, 1942) tense to refer to variance techniques in psy-
to practice effects (Owens, 1942). chology: "Analysis of variance and kindred
The Latin square design received much techniques developed by Fisher and his co-
early attention from psychologists. No fewer workers will very likely become valuable
than seven articles were found that were statistical tools in psychological research" (p.
devoted wholly or partially to the Latin 158).
square in research design (Baxter, 1941;
Bugelski, 1949; Edwards, 19SOb; Garrett & ANOVA Growth Curve
Zubin, 1943; Grant, 1944; Thomson, 1941).
An issue that began to receive attention in We carried out a frequency count of articles
the late 1940s was that of post hoc tests. The appearing in major psychological journals
common practice was to compute t tests be- from 1935 through 1952 to determine the
tween individual comparisons following a sig- size of the impact of ANOVA. Six major Amer-
nificant overall F value. Webb and Lemmon ican psychological journals were selected for
(19SO) and Walker (1947), however, com- analysis: American Journal oj Psychology,
mented on this practice. Webb and Lemmon Journal oj Applied Psychology, Journal oj
maintained that it was too rigidly applied, Educational Psychology, Journal of Experi-
and Walker saw post hoc techniques as a yet mental Psychology, Journal oj General Psy-
unresolved problem in research. chology, and Journal oj Psychology.
An examination of publication dates showed Every article from 1935 to 1952 in each
that 45 out of the 66 expository articles journal was reviewed to determine what type
(687c) appeared between 1938 and 194S. of statistical analysis was employed. A total
This was a critical period with respect to the of 6,457 articles was included in the count.
development of ANOVA techniques in psychol- Editorial articles, notes and discussions, and
ogy. Nearly all of these articles were explicit articles reporting new apparatus were not
introductions to variance techniques. This included.
suggests that at the time of the publication Figure 1 shows the proportion of articles
of these articles, the ANOVA was still not that used ANOVA, t tests, critical ratio tests,
widely used by psychological researchers. In and correlational analysis for each year

CRITICAL RATIO • • • • •
CORRELATIONAL

1935 36 38 40 42 44 46 48 50 52
YEAR OF JOURNALS
Figure 1. Proportion of articles using selected statistical tests as a function of year.
172 AXTHOXY J. RUCCI AND RYAX D. TWENEY

from 1935 through 1952. These categories are proportion of articles that used correlation in
the most reflective of the transition from the 1952 is nearly equal to that in 1935. This
conventional method (critical ratio) to the indicates that the use of correlational meth-
Fisher techniques. Use of both t and ANOVA ods remained relatively constant, even after
increased gradually prior to World War II, the Fisher methods were incorporated. With
declined during the war, and increased im- the aid of hindsight, it becomes clear that
mediately thereafter. Use of the critical ratio variance techniques enabled researchers to
technique declined in exact proportion to the fill the void that existed in experimental
increase in ANOVA and t, which confirms the methodology. To use Cronbach's terminology,
idea that Fisherian procedures were supplant- the correlational school probably would have
ing the conventional technique for examining been sustained whether Fisher's methods ap-
group differences. Note the marked similarity peared or not.
in the profiles for ANOVA and t. Every in- Some aspects of the frequency distribution
crease or decrease in the use of ANOVA is par- for individual journals deserve brief com-
alleled by a concomitant increase or decrease ment: The American Journal of Psychology
in /-test usage. By f949 use of the / test sur- and the Journal oj Applied Psychology
passed that of the critical ratio, and in 1950 showed increases in the use of ANOVA and t
ANOVA usage surpassed the critical ratio. Sur- up to 1942. From 1942 to 1945, however,
prisingly, use of the t test did not precede there was a decrease in the use of these
that of ANOVA, although it had been developed statistics. From 1945 on, use of ANOVA and t
much earlier and was very similar to the increased, with a concomitant decrease in the
already established critical ratio technique. use of the critical ratio. The percentage of
Figure 1 shows a decline in the use of articles that used both ANOVA and the t test
Fisher's techniques during the war years, surpassed that of articles that used the criti-
1942-194S. Note also the general increase in cal ratio only after 1947. The Journal oj Ex-
the use of the critical ratio during the same perimental Psychology and the Journal oj
period. Those research psychologists young Psychology showed the same pattern, though
enough to be inducted into military service growth of ANOVA was faster in the former and
were also those most likely to have had slower in the latter. The Journal oj General
graduate training in variance techniques Psychology was late to include ANOVA and did
(Chapanis, Note 2). Therefore, those re- not use it at all in 1948, although it increased
searchers most likely to use ANOVA were taken rapidly thereafter.
out of the publishing ranks during the war The results for the Journal oj Educational
years, which could account for the decline. In
Psychology were anomalous compared with
addition, those who continued to publish dur-
ing the war may have been those too old to the results of the other five. It evidenced the
be inducted and who lacked variance training. largest initial increase in ANOVA through 1942.
Their published research, then, would rely This may be attributed to a text by Lind-
more heavily on the critical ratio technique, quist (1940) on ANOVA for educational re-
and this could account for the increase in its searchers, which was widely cited. Although
use from 1943 to 1945. This explanation of a decrease in the use of variance techniques
the war year effect is consistent with Kuhn's from 1942 to 1946 was also evident in this
( 1 9 7 0 ) account of paradigm shifts. Kuhn has journal, the depression lasted much longer
suggested that new ideas are adopted differ- than in the other journals. It was not until
entially by young and old scientists. 1950 that the use of ANOVA surpassed its
The use of correlational analysis (also prewar use. Thus the paradox—Although ed-
shown in Figure 1) from 1935 to 1952 sug- ucational researchers initially adopted the
gests a striking conclusion: The Fisher meth- technique more quick!)' than others, the im-
ods did not supplant the use of correlational mediate postwar upsurge never materialized.
analysis. The curve of the use of correlational It was not until 1952 that usage of the criti-
analysis runs parallel to the abscissa. The cal ratio was surpassed by either ANOVA or /.
ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE 173

Statistical Research on ANOVA by relationship between interaction in ANOVA and


Psychologists intercorrelation (Coombs, 1948; Peters, 1944;
Taylor, 1942), Edwards (19SOc) discussed
Our third approach traced statistical re- the use of the interaction mean square as the
search done by psychologists relevant to error term in random model factorial designs.
ANOVA. Table 2 lists the relevant located Other articles listed in Table 2 covered a
articles. variety of topics. Festinger (1943a, 1943b)
Kogan (19S3) reported that psychologists used an F test for determining significance of
had done much to adapt ANOVA techniques to data with exponential or skewed distributions.
repeated measures designs. Gilliland and D. The relationship between F and t- was shown
W. Humphreys (1943) acknowledged L. by at least three researchers: Engelhart
Humphreys for providing the computational (1941), Rulon (1943), and Brozek and Alex-
formulas for the repeated measures analysis ander (1950). A frequently cited article by
used in their study. Edwards (19SOa) and L. Walker (1940) gave a geometrical and con-
G. Humphreys (Note 3) cite Gilliland and ceptual presentation of the concept of degrees
D. W. Humphreys and L. G. Humphreys of freedom.
(1943) as the first to use repeated measures The majority of articles on statistical issues
ANOVA in psychology. Alexander (1946) and published by psychologists were highly utili-
Lindquist (1947) provided variance tests for tarian. Most were concerned with facilitating
trends of repeated trials data. Alexander the application of ANOVA to psychological
(1946) also gave tests for comparing trends research. The unique research problems en-
between two groups and tests for estimating countered in psychology (e.g., repeated mea-
reliability across repeated trials (Alexander, sures designs) necessitated extensions and
1947). revisions of variance analyses as developed by
One problem frequently faced by psycholo- statisticians. Dunlap (1938) foresaw the need
gists was that of unequal or disproportionate for psychologists to enlarge their mathemati-
subclass sizes, Tsao (1942, 1946), Tsao and cal and statistical armamentarium:
Johnson (1946) and Ansbacher and Mather
(194S) all gave formulas for ANOVA or ANCOVA I can only extend my sympathy to the psychologist
with disproportionate subclasses or unequal of the future, for it seems as if he must first be
a mathematician, then a statistician, . . . and, if he
subclass sizes. is not dead of old age bv then, a psychologist, (p.
The 1947 issue of Biometrics covered three 571)
extremely important issues related to ANOVA.
Eisenhart (1947) outlined the difference be- The New Textbooks
tween fixed and random models and cited the
need to devise ways of interpreting and test- The mark of establishment is incorporation
ing mixed models. The entire issue of fixed in textbooks. Accordingly, we reviewed text-
versus random models received little early books that appeared through 19SO that con-
attention from psychologists and is, in fact, tained coverage of ANOVA. So few were pub-
still a subject of debate (Clark, 1973; Clark, lished by psychological researchers prior to
Cohen, Smith, & Keppel, 1976; Wike & 1950, however, that it was necessary to in-
Church, 1976). In the same issue of Bio- clude books by statistical and mathematical
metrics, Cochran (1947) examined the effects researchers that were cited in early articles
of departures from the assumptions of ANOVA using variance analysis.
and Bartlett outlined transformations that Table 3 lists the authors, years of publica-
could be applied to eliminate nonhomo- tion, and affiliations of the located textbooks
geneity of variance. Other articles related to through 1950. There are two noteworthy
the same issue were published by Edwards facts in Table 3. First of all, many of the
(19SOb), Mueller (1949), Baxter (1941), names in this table have not appeared in
and Godard and Lindquist (1940). prior sections of this article, since only a
A number of articles dealt with the in- handful of the authors were psychologists.
terpretation of interaction effects and the Secondly, the affiliations are dominated by
174 ANTHONY J. RUCCI AND RYAN D. TWENEY

Table 2
.1 rticles Containing Statistical Treatments Related to Analysis of Variance.

Author Source Description

Godard & Psycliometrika Tested effect of violating homogeneity of


Lindquist (1940) variance assumption
Shen (1940) Journal of Experimental Education Outlined differences between critical ratio,
/ test, and AN'OVA and ANCOVA
\Valkcr (1940) Journal of Educational Psychology Conceptual and geometrical discussion of
degrees of freedom
Engelhart (1941) Psycliometrika Demonstrated that I- = F
Hoyt (1941) Psycliometrika Used AXOVA to estimate test reliability
Sattcrthwaitc Psycliometrika Extended z test to situations in which a
(1941) chi-square estimate would be inexact
Taylor (1942) British Journal of Psychology Relationship shown between interaction
effects and intercorrelation
Tsao (1942) Psyclwmelrika Formulas for AXOVA with unequal
subclass sixes
Humphreys (1943) Journal of Comparative Psychology Formulas for repeated measures AXOVA
Festinger (1943a) Psychonielrika Test of significance for means from an
exponential distribution
Festinger (1943b) Psycliometrika Test of significance for means from a
skewed distribution
Rulon (1943) Journal of Experimental Education Demonstrated that t'2 = F
Johnson & Tsao Psycliomelriku Factorial design and formulas for d e t e r m i n -
(1944) ing psychophysical thresholds
Kcllcy (1944) Psycliometrika A variance-ratio lest for principal-axis
components
Peters (1944) Psychological Bulletin Interaction in ANOVA related to
intercorrelation
Schoenfeld (1944) American Journal of Psychology Computational formulas for factorial
analysis
Ansbacher & Psyclioinelrika Formulas for AXOVA with unequal
Mather (1945) subclass sizes
Johnson & Tsao Psychonielrika Factorial design and formulas for applying
(1945) covariance analysis
Alexander (1946) Psychological Bulletin Test of trends and differences between
trends for repeated measures of d a t a
Tsao (1946) Psycliometrika. Formulas for AXOVA and ANCOVA with
unequal subclass sixes
Tsao & Johnson Journal of Experimental Education Formulas for ANOVA with unequal
(1946) subclass sizes
Alexander (1947) Psyclwmelrika Estimated reliability across repeated trials
Bartlett (1947) B-iometrics Transformations to eliminate heterogeneity
of variance
Brozck & American. Journal of Psychology Formulas for ANOVA with repeated
Alexander (1947) measures
Cochran (1947) Biometrics Evaluated the effect of departures from
AXOVA assumptions
Eisenhart (1947) Biometrics Discussion of fixed, mixed, and random
models
Lindquist (1947) Psycliometrika Test of trends and differences between
trends for repeated measures data
Coombs (1948) Psycliometrika Interaction in AXOVA related to inter-
correlation
Grant (1948) Psychological IIullelin Formulas for computing sums of squares
in a Latin square design
Kogan (1948) Psychological Bulletin Formulas for repeated measures AXOVA
Mueller (1949) Psychological Bulletin Transformations to use to meet assump-
tions of AXOVA
Rulon (1949) Psyclwmelrika Matrix models of ANOVA and ANCOVA
ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE 175

Table 2 (continued)

Author Source Description

Brozek & American Journal of Psychology Demonstrated that t- = F


Alexander (1950)
Edwards (1950c) Educational and Psychological Use of interaction sums of squares as
Measurement error terms
Edwards (1950b) Psychological Bulletin Applied homogeneity of variance tests to a
Latin square
Edwards & Psychomelrika Formulas for computing the highest
Horst (1950) interaction sum of squares in a factorial
design

Note. ANOVA = analysis of variance; ANCOVA = analysis of covariance.

researchers from Columbia University, Iowa articles using ANOVA. The two Snedecor
State College, the University of Iowa, and the (1934, 1937) texts, Fisher's (1935) text,
University of Minnesota. This is discussed in Goulden (1939), Rider (1939), Peters and
more detail later in this article. VanVoorhis (1940), and Walker (1943) ac-
A small subset of the textbooks listed in count for most of the citations through the
Table 3 was heavily cited by psychological early 1940s. In general, however, the texts by

Table 3
Statistical Textbooks Containing Analysis of Variance Coverage

Author Affiliation

Fisher (1925b) Rothamsted Experimental Station


Tippett (1931) British Cotton Industry
Snedecor (1934) Iowa State College
Fisher (1935) University of London
Davenport & Ekas (1936) Carnegie Institution and Northwest High School, Philadelphia
Treloar (1936) University of Minnesota
Yule & Kendall (1937) St. John's College
Snedecor (1937) Iowa State College
Fisher & Yates (1938) University of Cambridge and Rothamsted Experimental Station
Croxten & Crowden (1939) Columbia University
Goulden (1939) University of Manitoba
Rider (1939) Washington University
Peters & VanVoorhis (1940) Perm State College
Lindquist (1940) University of Iowa
Walker (1943) Columbia University
Smith & Duncan (1944) Princeton University
Edwards (1946) University of Washington
Wilks (1946) Princeton University
Eisenhart, Hastay, &
Wallis (1947) Columbia University Research Group
Garrett (1947) Columbia University
Kelley (1947) Harvard University
Lewis (1948) University of Iowa
Chapanis, Garner, & Morgan
(1949) Johns Hopkins University
Mann (1949) Ohio State University
McNemar (1949) Stanford University
Johnson (1949) University of Minnesota
Cochran & Cox (1950) Iowa State College
Edwards (1950) University of Washington
Guilford (1950) University of Southern California
Mood (1950) Iowa State College
176 ANTHONY J. RUCCI AND RYAN D. TWENEY

Snedecor (1937) and Lindquist (1940) had the ordering by doctorate with each of the
the largest impact on psychological research- three category orderings. The rank-order cor-
ers well into the late 1940s. The Lindquist relation (Spearman's rho corrected for ties)
book represents the first comprehensive text between doctoral year and year of first vari-
on ANOVA that was intended for behavioral ance training was .53; between doctoral year
researchers. Snedecor was a statistician at the and year of first psychology course in ANOVA,
Iowa State College agricultural station, and p = .63; between doctoral year and first year
his text was oriented to agricultural research. of program requirement, p = AS (all ps <
However, a number of the early psychology .01). The older the doctoral program, (a) the
programs offering variance training used this earlier variance training was started, (b) the
text. earlier the department offered a variance
The texts by Kelley (1947), McNemar course, and (c) the earlier variance training
(1949), Johnson (1949), Cochran and Cox became a graduate degree requirement.
(19SO), Edwards (T9SOa), and Guilford In the 5 years after World War II, there
( 1 9 S O ) proved to be important to psycho- was a dramatic increase in the number of
logical researchers through the 1950s. psychology departments in which graduate
Once again the effect of the war years on students were receiving training in ANOVA
the use of ANOVA is seen in Table 3. Only techniques. This result coincides with a similar
three texts appeared between 1940 and 1946. increase in journal and textbook use of and
From immediately after the war, however, attention to ANOVA immediately following the
through 1950, the production of texts on war. Again, the return of young psychologists
ANOVA averaged nearly three per year, not to academic positions following the war may
including later editions of earlier texts. have brought an increased emphasis on vari-
ance techniques.
ANOVA Training in Psychology
Phylogeny of Variance Training
Our fifth and final approach was to deter-
mine when ANOVA was incorporated into the Based on the results of the preceding
training of psychologists. It was expected analyses, the line of training was traced for
that graduate training in ANOVA would have those researchers identified as prominent con-
lagged behind incorporation into the research tributors to the development of variance tech-
literature. niques in psychology. (See Ben-David & Col-
A questionnaire survey was conducted of lins, 1966, and Boring & Boring, 1948, for
88 psychology departments that offered grad- examples of the method used.) Figure 2 was
uate degrees in 1940. The questionnaire was constructed using 1940 editions of the Ameri-
intended to determine when ANOVA training can Psychological Association membership
was introduced into the graduate program at directory, textbook prefaces and acknowledge-
each department. Of the 88 surveys mailed, ments, and personal communications (Cron-
41 ( 4 7 / J - ) were returned. Fourteen of the bach, Note 1; Chapanis, Note 2; Humph-
returned surveys were unusable due to incom- reys, Note 3; Bechtoldt, Note 4; Cronbach,
plete information. Therefore, Table 4 lists Note 5; Edwards, Note 6; Gage, Note 7;
the results of the 27 usable questionnaires. Green, Note 8; Gulliksen, Note 9; Lind-
The universities are listed in the order in quist, Note 10; McNemar, Note 11; Ryan,
which the}' began to offer the doctorate in Note 12; Smith, Note 13) from a number of
psychology degree. In all three categories, individuals.
those departments offering the doctorate in The phylogeny of the development of Fish-
psychology prior to 1940 incorporated ANOVA er's techniques in psychology and in Ameri-
training earlier than other departments. The can statistics as well was centered around
27 departments were rank ordered by the three statistical researchers: G. W. Snedecor
year in which they began to offer the doc- at Iowa State College, Harold Hotelling at
torate and by the year of the three categories. Columbia University, and Palmer Johnson at
Rank-order correlations were computed for the University of Minnesota. These three
ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE 177

Table 4
Summary of Questionnaire Results for Incorporation of Variance Training

Year degree first


offered
ANOVA
PhD MA
in psy- in psy- Require-
University chology chology Training" Course1' ment 0

Johns Hopkins 1886 1949 1952 1952


Cornell 1892 1950 1950 1954d
Stanford 1915 1935 1935 1935
of Kansas 1916 1916 1949 1949 1951
Indiana 1919d 1900d 1947 1951 1947
Northwestern 1924 1940 1940 1946
of Texas, Austin 1925d 1910d I947d 1949d 1950
Brown 1927« 1892 1946 1946 1946
of Michigan 1928' 1947d 1952 1948
of North Carolina, Chapel Hill 1920s 1954" 1954'1 1954d
Duke 1930 1930 1951 1951
1931 1923

of North Dakota 1958 1958 1958
Case Western Reserve 1932 1960d — —
Temple 1938 1938 1966 1966 1970
Purdue 1941 1935 1941 1941 1941
Loyola 1947 1930 1966 1966 1966
of Utah 1949 1931 1952 1956 1957
Vanderbilt 1950 1949* 1950 1950 1950
Tufts 1952 1940 1950 1950'1 1950
of Missouri, Columbia 1953 1920s 1960d 1968 1960d
of Georgia 1956 1930s 1956d 1956d 1971
of Arkansas 1959 1929 1956 1956 1959
of South Dakota 1963 1903 1964 1964 1964
Iowa State 1965 1920 d '>' 1934 — 1948
of Wyoming 1966 1914 1940s — 1970d
Howard 1969 1928 1950 1952 1952
of Nebraska, Omaha 1973 1937 1966 1966 1968

Note. ANOVA = analysis of variance; dash = not yet incorporated.


a
Year in which graduate students in psychology began receiving variance training, possibly through other
departments (i.e., biology, agriculture, or mathematics). For departments that offered a master's degree
prior to 1940, median year is 1950; for departments that offered a doctorate prior to 1940, median year is
1949.
b
Year in which psychology department first offered a course in ANOVA. For departments that offered a
master's degree prior to 1940, median year is 1952; for departments that offered a doctorate prior to 1940,
median year is 1951.
0
Year in which ANOVA training became a graduate program requirement. For departments that offered a
master's degree prior to 1940, median year is 1954; for departments that offered a doctorate prior to 1940,
median year is 1951.
d
Estimate provided by individual completing the questionnaire.
0
Previously offered through philosophy department.
' Offered through philosophy department 1906-1928.
e
Previously offered through philosophy and psychology departments.
h
Previously offered through history, philosophy, and psychology departments.

stand at the beginning of the entire line of 1931 he took a position as professor of Eco-
training in Fisherian statistics in the United nomics at Columbia University. Under Ho-
States. All three gained exposure to variance telling's influence Columbia University be-
techniques directly from Fisher himself. came one of the centers of statistical research
Hotelling visited Fisher's laboratory at the in the United States. Hotelling (1931) him-
Rothamsted Experiment Station in 1929. In self is responsible for T-, the multivariate
178 ANTHONY J. RUCCI AND RYAN D. TWENEY

1925 •1935- •1940 •1945 •1950

^,'Cox
0**
**''^, ..--Cochran • -Lindquist
[U. Iowa)
/ I Iowa St.) • Gaskill

,, Bartlett

S''\ Walker
[Wisconsin |
-Hotel lings:-!- MeNemar :*''
„' Edwards
I Columbia )"%>„ I Stanford I "**»
v\ *% N
3errett
s
" * Humphreys ''.
I Northwestern I
IU. Washington)

S
\v ""•Chapanis
Zubin (Johns Hopkins!

^Johnson-
Tsao
I Minnesota I^VV*«.S """ 1
""."^^ ~~" Broiek
\^> v
N "Baxter

x
Alexander

Figure 2. Phylogeny of major contributors to psychological statistics.

generalization of Student's t. His associate, chological terminology. Brozek, Tsao, and


S. S. Wilks (1932), developed the multivari- Alexander were also at Minnesota, and each
ate generalization of the ANOVA. Of those hav- of these researchers published several articles
ing a direct effect on psychology's adoption related to ANOVA.
of variance techniques, H. E. Garrett, J. Zu- Snedecor received exposure to ANOVA at
bin, and H. Walker were associated with Co- Iowa State College in the mid-1930s while
lumbia University. Garrett and Zubin's Fisher was a visiting professor there. Snede-
(1943) article can legitimately be cited as cor's work at Iowa State College was impor-
the initial comprehensive presentation of vari- tant to the development of variance tech-
ance techniques to psychology. niques in the United States. One or the other
Johnson visited the Gallon Laboratories in of Snedecor's textbooks (1934, 1937) was
London and received exposure to variance nearly always cited by early psychological
techniques from Fisher. After returning to research using variance analysis. His influence
Minnesota, Johnson had a profound influence was also felt through others at Iowa State
on the development of variance techniques in College. Gaskill and Cox (1937) published
behavioral research. He published numerous one of the earliest articles that dealt with
articles dealing with ANOVA, and Johnson and covariance analysis. Cochran and Cox (1950),
Neyman (1936) developed a covariance tech- who were also from Iowa State College, pub-
nique for testing linear hypotheses that came lished a text on experimental design that was
to be referred to as the Johnson-Neyman widely used by psychologists in the 1950s.
technique. The effects of Johnson's research At the University of Iowa, but through
and training were felt through other research- contacts with Snedecor and Cochran, E. F.
ers from Minnesota. Baxter's (1941) article Lindquist made a major contribution to the
on variance techniques was an excellent trans- incorporation of variance techniques into psy-
lation of agricultural terminology into psy- chology. His (1940) textbook on ANOVA for
ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE 179

educational researchers was the first bona fide began an uninterrupted increase immediately
statistics text devoted to the application of following the war. Psychologists became ac-
variance techniques in behavioral research. tively involved in statistical issues related to
This text was widely cited by researchers in the technique in the middle-to-late 1940s.
psychology who used ANOVA prior to 19SO. Textbooks in statistics written explicitly for
Of psychologists directly involved in the psychological researchers did not appear until
statistical development of ANOVA, the line of the late 1940s. Finally, graduate training in
training clearly traces back to Quinn McNe- ANOVA was the last in chronological order to
mar at Stanford University. McNemar, how- develop, the median year being 1951.
ever, received his exposure to statistical tech- Thus, introductory articles were followed
niques from Hotelling, who was an associate by a gradual increase in the application of
professor at Stanford University until 1931. the technique in the research literature. When
Through McNemar, at Stanford University, it became clear that the technique was es-
L. G. Humphreys was trained before going to tablishing itself as a primary method of de-
Northwestern University, where he was sta- sign and analysis, psychologists began refin-
tistical adviser for the research of Allen Ed- ing the technique to meet the unique
wards. Edwards became a prominent figure in requirements of behavioral research. Text-
the development of variance techniques in books written by psychologists for psycholo-
psychology, publishing numerous articles, an gists then appeared. When it was clear that
elementary textbook (Edwards, 1946), and in competent psychologists must receive ex-
1950 a comprehensive textbook of ANOVA. posure to variance techniques, graduate pro-
David Grant graduated from Stanford Uni- grams began offering and requiring courses in
versity in 1941 before going to the University ANOVA.
of Wisconsin. Grant was perhaps the most The history of psychology's incorporation
active of all psychologists both in introduc- of variance techniques can be logically di-
ing ANOVA to psychology and in applying it vided into three periods: (a) the expository
in his own research. From Stanford Univer- period from 1925 to 1942, (b) the war period
sity, therefore, came directly and indirectly from 1942 to 1945, and (c) the postwar
four of the most prominent names in the institutionalization period from 1946 to 1957.
development of variance techniques in psy- The expository period is characterized by
chology: McNemar, L. G. Humphreys, Ed- early attempts to use ANOVA, but even more
wards, and Grant. so by articles exhorting the use of variance
L. G. Humphreys may even have played an analysis by psychologists. The majority of
important role in introducing variance train- these articles appeared between 1936 and
ing at Yale University. While on a postdoc- 1945, with some of the most important (e.g.,
toral appointment at Yale in 1938, Humph- Baxter, 1941; Crutchfield, 1938; Crutchfield
reys reported on Fisher's techniques in a & Tolman, 1940; Garrett & Zubin, 1943)
seminar offered by Donald Marquis. In 1942, appearing between 1938 and early 1943.
at Yale, Chapanis received variance training The war years effect was pronounced. The
from Carl Hovland. Chapanis went on to gradual upsurge in the use of ANOVA between
introduce the ANOVA into applied experimental 1934 and 1942 was wiped out during World
research (Chapanis, 1951; Chapanis, Garner, War II. It was 1947 before use of the tech-
& Morgan, 1949). nique regained the high point it had reached
just before the war. The war, therefore, was
Discussion instrumental in how quickly the technique
was adopted and may have delayed its in-
Our results show that ANOVA was incorpo- corporation by some 5 years. This conclusion
rated into psychology in logical and orderly must be qualified, however. To establish such
steps. First came the expository articles from an effect conclusively would require extensive
1938 to 1945. The use of variance techniques review of the research conducted by psycholo-
in journals began to increase in the early gists for the military during the war. Many
1940s, was deterred during World War II, and psychologists may have received their initial
180 ANTHONY J. RUCCI AND RYAN D. TWENEY

exposure to variance analysis and experience service as psychologists. This period shows a
with the technique while in military service clear drop in the publication of articles using
(Chapanis, Note 2). If so, then the war may ANOVA from 1942 to 1946, whereas, at the
actually have accelerated use of the technique same time, ANOVA techniques were increas-
in the late 1940s. ingly used by military research psychologists
The postwar institutionalization period was (e.g., Chapanis & Schachter, Note 14;
dramatic. The use of ANOVA in the journals Schachter & Chapanis, Note 15). Following
began an uninterrupted increase immediately the war, these psychologists entered the aca-
following the war. By 19S2 nearly all psy- demic world and began to publish in regular
chology graduate programs were offering journals, using ANOVA. Further, there was
course work in ANOVA, clearly signaling its clearly resistance from older researchers.
institutionalization as an accepted analytic Peters (1943, 1944; Peters & VanVoorhis,
technique familiar to virtually all research 1940) was the most vociferous critic of Fish-
psychologists. er's techniques. He pointed out that Fisher
The proposed trichotomy of the history of presented only extensions of traditional sta-
psychology's adoption of variance techniques tistics, nothing fundamentally new. He re-
qualifies the usual claim (Hearnshaw, 1964; ferred to the ANOVA as "magic" on two occa-
Stanley, 1966) that the incorporation of vari- sions in print and warned,
ance techniques into psychology was a post-
World War II phenomenon. By 19S2 psychol- if educationalists and psychologists, out of some
ogy had completed a shift with respect to sort of inferiority complex, grab indiscriminately at
them [variance techniques] and employ them where
experimental design and analysis, a process they are unsuitable, education and psychology will
that began well before the war, in the 1930s. suffer another slump in prestige such as they have
Can this change be considered an instance often hitherto suffered in consequence of the pursuit
of a paradigm shift in Kuhn's (1970) sense? of fads. (Peters, 1943, p. 549)
The criteria that would have to be met to
substantiate such a claim are (a) that a major The last criterion for a paradigm—success-
change was produced in psychologists' way of ful incorporation of anomalies—does not seem
perceiving and/or approaching basic concep- to fit this case. Although examples can be
tual issues, (b) that the new techniques were found of pre-ANOVA research that could have
differentially adopted by younger psycholo- benefitted from use of ANOVA, it is not the
gists against the resistance of older workers, case that felt anomalies were present that
(c) that anomalies that could not be ac- needed to be attended to. ANOVA is, of course,
counted for with older techniques were in- a technique, not a theory, and one could ar-
corporated in the newer. gue that no methodological technique could
The first criterion can be supported by ever meet this criterion, by definition. If,
reference to Cronbach's (1957) characteri- however, one considers the concepts of
zation of the experimental discipline of psy- crossed classifications and interaction as being
chology. The approach to basic experimental anomalies prior to ANOVA, then, in that sense,
issues was radically different following the ANOVA did incorporate anomalies. Even so, we
adoption of Fisherian techniques. It is unde- feel that it is more reasonable to regard
niable that factorial design and analysis fa- ANOVA not as something fundamentally new,
cilitated the demise of the single-variable law in a revolutionary sense, but as an extension
in psychology. By 1957 it was standard ex- and explication of a set of design strategies.
perimental procedure to manipulate multiple Although ANOVA gave the experimenter a
levels of multiple independent variables. powerful set of analytic tools, the nature of
ANOVA was, therefore, the ideal analytic sta- psychological experimentation changed during
tistic for such designs. the period in question as psychologists moved
That the second condition held is suggested from single-variable studies to multiple-varia-
by the effect of the war on incorporation of ble studies. Thus, the history of ANOVA is
ANOVA. By and large, it was younger, less likely to be most interpretable as a component
established researchers who entered wartime of a broader, as yet unattempted, history of
ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE 181

the use of experimental research methods in Air Technical Service Command, Engineering
Division, Aero Medical Laboratory, October
psychology. 1945.
Thus, we feel that our study represents 15. Schachter, S., & Chapanis, A. Distortion in
only the beginning of a necessary but much glass and its effect on depth perception (Memo-
larger effort. We cannot legitimately assess randum Rep. TSEAL-695-48B). Wright Field,
the place of ANOVA in the history of psychol- Dayton, Ohio: Army Air Forces Air Technical
ogy without the context provided by knowl- Service Command, Engineering Division, Aero
Medical Laboratory, April 1945.
edge of the explicit ways in which experimen-
tation changed during the period in question.
Furthermore, neither statistical techniques nor References
design techniques can be assessed indepen- Alexander, H. W. A general test for trend. Psycho-
dently of the concomitant changes in the logical Bulletin, 1946, 43, 533-557.
empirical issues considered by psychologists Alexander, H. W. The estimation of reliability when
or independently of the changes in the theo- several trials are available. Psychometrika, 1947,
12, 79-99.
retical structures underlying psychological Ansbacher, H. L., & Mather, K. Group differences in
research. Our study is thus very much a size estimation. Psychometrika, 1945, 10, 37-56.
beginning effort only. By characterizing the Baker, K. H. Item validity by the analysis of vari-
growth and development of a single impor- ance. Psychological Record, 1939, 3, 242-248.
Bartlett, M. S. The use of transformations. Bio-
tant dimension of psychological method, we metrics, 1947, 3, 39-52.
hope it will contribute to the emergence of a Baxter, B. Problems in the planning of psychologi-
broader, more comprehensive history of scien- cal experiments. American Journal oj Psychology,
tific method in psychology. 1941, 54, 270-280.
Baxter, B. A study of reaction time using factorial
design. Journal oj Experimental Psychology, 1942,
Reference Notes 31, 430-437.
Ben-David, J., & Collins, R. Social factors in the
1. Cronbach, L. J. Personal communication, July origin of a new science: The case of psychology.
16, 1977. American Sociological Review, 1966, 31, 451-465.
Bloomers, P., & Lindquist, E. F. Experimental and
2. Chapanis, A. Personal communication, July 19,
statistical studies: Application of newer statistical
1977. techniques. Review of Educational Research, 1942,
3. Humphreys, L. G. Personal communication, July 12, 501-520.
18, 1977. Boring, M. D., & Boring, E. G. Masters and pupils
4. Bechtoldt, H. P. Personal communication, Au- among the American psychologists. American
gust 10, 1977. Journal oj Psychology, 1948, 61, 527-534.
5. Cronbach, L. J. Personal communication, July Brozek, J., & Alexander, H. A note on estimation of
25, 1977. the components of variation in a two-way table.
6. Edwards, A. L. Personal communication, Decem- American Journal oj Psychology, 1947, 60, 629-
ber 6, 1977. 637.
Brozek, J., & Alexander, H. The formula t~ = F.
7. Gage, N. L. Personal communication, July 21,
American Journal of Psychology, 1950, 63, 262-
1977. 269.
8. Green, B. F. Personal communication, August 10, Bugelski, B. R. A note on Grant's discussion of the
1977. Latin square principle in the design of experi-
9. Gulliksen, H. Personal communication, July 15, ments. Psychological Bulletin, 1949, 46, 49-50.
1977. Hurt, C. Recent developments of statistical methods
10. Lindquist, E. F. Personal communication, De- in psychology: 1. Occupational Psychology Land-
cember 4, 1977. marks, 1938, 12, 169-177.
11. McNcmar, Q. Personal communication, Decem- Carington, W. The quantitative study of trance
ber 20, 1977. personalities: 1. Proceedings oj the Society for
Psychical Research, 1934, 42, 173-240.
12. Ryan, T. A. Personal communication, July 27, Carington, W. The quantitative study of trance
1977. personalities: 2. Proceedings oj the Society for
13. Smith, P. C. Personal communication, May 10, Psychical Research, 1936, 43, 319-361.
1977. Carington, W. The quantitative study of trance
14. Chapanis, A., & Schachter, S. Depth perception personalities: 3. Proceedings of the Society for
through a P-80 canopy and through distorted Psychical Research, 1937, 44, 189-222.
glass (Memorandum Rep. TSEAL-69S-48N). Chapanis, A. Theory and methods for analyzing
Wright Field, Dayton, Ohio: Army Air Forces errors in man-machine systems. Annals of the
182 ANTHONY J. RUCCI AND RYAN D. TWENEY

.\eui York Academy of Sciences, 1951, 51, 1179- of squares for interactions in the analysis of vari-
1203. ance. Psychometrika, 1950, 14, 17-24.
Chapanis, A., Garner, VV. R., & Morgan, C. T. Eisenhart, C. The assumptions underlying the analy-
Applied experimental psychology. New York: sis of variance. Biometrics, 1947, 3, 1-21.
Wilej', 1949. Eisenhart, C., Hastay, M. W., & Wallis, W. A.
Clark, H. H. The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: Selected techniques of statistical analysis. New
A critique of language statistics in psychological York: McGraw-Hill, 1947.
research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Engelhart, M. D. The analysis of variance and
Behavior, 1973, 12, 335-359. covariance techniques in relation to the conven-
Clark, H. H., Cohen, J., Smith, J. E. K., & Keppel, tional formulas for the standard error of dif-
G. Discussion of Wike and Church's comments. ference. Psychometrika, 1941, 6, 221-234.
Journal oj Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, Federer, W. T., & Balaam, L. N. Bibliography on
1976, IS, 257-266. experiment and treatment design—pre 1968. New
Cochran, W. G. Some consequences when the as- York: Hafner, 1973.
sumptions for the analysis of variance are not Festinger, L. An exact test of significance for means
satisfied. Biometrics, 1947, 3, 22-28. of samples drawn from populations with expo-
Cochran, VV. G., & Cox, G. M. Experimental designs, nential frequency distribution. Psychometrika,
New York: Wiley, 1950. 1943, S, 153-160. (a)
Coombs, C. H. The role of correlation in analysis of Festinger, L. A statistical test for means of samples
variance. Psychometrika, 1948, 13, 233-243. from skew populations. Psychometrika, 1943, 8,
Cronbach, L. The two disciplines of scientific psy- 205-210. (b)
chology. American Psychologist, 1957, 12, 671- Fisher, R. A. On the distribution of the standard
684. deviation of small samples: Appendix 1. to papers
Croxten, F. E., & Crowden, D. J. Applied general by "Student" and R. A. Fisher. Biometrika, 1915,
statistics. New York: Prentice-Hall, 1939. W, 522-529.
Crutchfield, R. S. Efficient factorial design and Fisher, R. A. Applications of "Student's" distribu-
analysis of variance illustrated in psychological tion. Metron, 1925, 5, 90-104. (a)
experimentation. Journal oj Psychology, 1938, 5, Fisher, R. A. Statistical methods for research work-
339-346. ers. London: Oliver & Boyd, 1925. (b)
Crutchfield, R. S. The determiners of energy expendi- Fisher, R. A. The arrangement of field experiments.
ture in string pulling by the rat. Journal oj Psy- Journal of the Ministry of Agriculture, 1926, 33,
chology, 1939, 7, 163-178. 503-513.
Crutchfield, R. S., & Tolman, E. C. Multiple-varia- Fisher, R. A. Statistical methods for research work-
ble design for experiments involving interaction of ers (4th ed.). London: Oliver & Boyd, 1932.
behavior. Psychological Review, 1940, 47, 38-42. Fisher, R. A. The design of experiments. London:
Davenport, C. B., & Ekas, M. P. Statistical methods Oliver & Boyd, 1935.
in biology, medicine, and psychology. New York: Fisher, R. A. Statistical methods for research work-
Wiley, 1936. ers (14th ed.). London: Oliver & Boyd, 1970.
Dressel, P. The effect of high school grades on col- Fisher, R. A., & MacKenzie, W. A. Studies in crop
lege grades. Journal oj Educational Psychology, variation: 2. The manurial response of different
1939, JO, 611-616. potato varieties. Journal of Agricultural Science,
Dunlap, J. W. Recent advances in statistical theory 1923, 13, 311-320.
and applications. American Journal of Psychology, Fisher, R. A., & Yates, F. Statistical tables for bio-
1938, 51, 558-S71. logical, agricultural, and medical research. New
Dunlap, J. W. Applications of analysis of variance York: Hafner, 1938.
to educational problems. Journal oj Educational Garrett, H. E. AN OVA in psychological research.
Research, 1940, 33, 434-442. Journal of Educational Research, 1943, 36, 631-
Dunlap, J. W. Recent advances in statistical theory 632.
and applications. American Journal oj Psychology, Garrett, H. E. Statistics in psychology and educa-
1941, 54, 583-601. tion. New York: Longmans, Green, 1947.
Edwards, A. L. Statistical analysis for students in Garrett, H. E., & Zubin, J. The analysis of vari-
psychology and education. New York: Rinehart, ance in psychological research. Psychological Bul-
1946. letin, 1943, 40, 233-267.
Edwards, A. L. Experimental design in psychologi- Gaskill, H. V., & Cox, G. M. 1. Respiration: Use
cal research. New York: Rinehart, 1950. (a) of analysis of variance and covariance in psycho-
Edwards, A. L. Homogeneity of variance and the logical data. Journal of General Psychology, 1937,
Latin square design. Psychological Bulletin, 1950, 16, 21-38,
47, 118-129. (b) Gilliland, A. R., & Humphreys, D. W. Age, sex,
Edwards, A. L. On the use of interactions as "error method and interval as variables in time estima-
terms" in the analysis of variance. Educational tion. Journal oj Genetic Psychology, 1943, 63,
and Psychological Measurement, 1950, 10, 214- 123-130.
223. (c) Godard, R. H., & Lindquist, E. F. An empirical
Edwards, A. L., & Horst, P. The calculation of sums study of the effect of heterogeneous within-groups
ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE 183

variance upon certain /''-tests of significance in Johnson, P. 0., & Neyman, J. Tests of certain linear
analysis of variance. Psychometrika, 1940, S, 263- hypotheses and their application to some educa-
274. tional problems. Statistical Research Memoirs,
Gossett, W. S. (pseudonym, Student). The probable 1936, ;, 57-93.
error of a mean. Blomelriha, 1908, 6, 1-25. Johnson, P. O,, & Tsao, F. Factorial design in the
Gossett, W. S. (pseudonym, Student). On testing determination of differential limen values. Psycho-
varieties of cereals. Biometrika, 1923, 25, 271-293. metrika, 1944, 9, 107-146.
Gottsdanker, R. M. An experimental study of fixa- Johnson, P. 0., & Tsao, F. Factorial design and
tion of response by college students in a multiple covariance in the study of individual educational
choice situation. Journal oj Experimental Psy- development. Psychometrika, 1945, 10, 133-162.
chology, 1939, 25, 431-444. Kelley, T. L. A variance-ratio test of the uniqueness
Goulden, C. H. Methods of statistical analysis. of principal-axis components as they exist at any
Minneapolis, Minn.: Burgess, 1939. stage of the Kelley iterative process for their
Grant, D. A. On "The analysis of variance in psy- distribution. Psychometrika, 1944, 9, 199-200.
chological research." Psychological Bulletin, 1944, Kelley, T. L. Fundamentals of statistics. Cambridge,
41, 158-166. Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1947.
Grant, D. A. The Latin square principle in the Kogan, L. S. Analysis of variance—Repeated mea-
design and analysis of psychological experiments. surements. Psychological Bulletin, 1948, 45, 131-
Psychological Bulletin, 1948, 45, 427-442. 143.
Grant, D. A. Statistical theory and research design. Kogan, L. S. Variance designs in psychological re-
In C. P. Stone (Ed.), Annual review of psychol- search. Psychological Bulletin, 1953, 50, 1-40.
ogy (Vol. 1). Stanford, Calif.: Annual Reviews, Krcezcr, G. Intelligence level and occipital alpha
19SO. rhythm in the Mongolian type of mental defi-
Guilford, J. P. Fundamental statistics in psychology ciency. American Journal oj Psychology, 1939, 52,
and education. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1950. 503-532.
Haggard, E. A. On the application of analysis of Kuhn, T. S. The structure oj scientific revolutions.
variance to GSR data: 1. The selection of an Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1970.
appropriate measure. Journal of Experimental Lev, J. Evaluation of test items by the method of
Psychology, 1949, 39, 378-392. analysis of variance. Journal of Educational Psy-
Hearnshavv, L. S. A short history of British psy- chology, 1938, 29, 623-630.
chology. New York: Barnes & Noble, 1964. Lewis, D. Quantitative methods in psychology. Iowa
Helson, H. Multiple-variable analysis of factors City, Iowa: Bookshop, 1948.
affecting lightness and saturation. American Jour- Lindquist, E. F. Statistical analysis in educational
nal of Psychology, 1942, 55, 46-57. research. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin, 1940.
Hotelling, H. The generalization of "Student's" Lindquist, E. F. Goodness of fit and trend curves
ratio. Annals oj Mathematical Statistics, 1931, 2, and significance of trend differences. Psycho-
360-378. metrika, 1947, 12, 65-78.
Hotelling, H. Review of Snedecor, G. W.: Calcula- Mahalanobis, P. C. Professor Ronald Aylmer Fisher.
tion and interpretation of the analysis of variance Sankhya, 1938, 4, 265-272. •
and covariance. Journal oj the American Statisti- Mann, H. B. Analysis and design oj experiments.
cal Association, 1935, 30, 118. New York: Dover, 1949.
Hotelling, H. Dr. Peters' criticism of Fisher's statis- McNcmar, Q. Psychological statistics. New York;
tics. Journal oj Educational Research, 1943, 36, Wiley, 1949.
707-711. Mood, A. Introduction to the theory oj statistics.
Hotelling, H. The impact of R. A. Fisher on statis- New York: McGraw-Hill, 1950.
tics. Journal of the American Statistical Associa- Mueller, C. G. Numerical transformations in the
tion, 1951, 46, 35-46. analysis of experimental data. Psychological Bul-
Hoyt, C. Test reliability by analysis of variance. letin, 1949, 46, 198-223.
Psycttometrika, 1941, 6, 153-160. Owens, VV. A. A new technique in studying the
Humphreys, L. G. The strength of a Thorndikian effects of practice upon individual differences.
response as a function of the number of practice Journal oj Experimental Psychology, 1942, 30,
trials. Journal oj Comparative Psychology, 1943, 180-183.
35, 101-110. Peters, C. C. Misuses of the Fisher statistics. Jour-
Jackson, R. W. B. Reliability of mental tests. Brit- nal of Educational Research, 1943, 36, 546-549.
ish Journal oj Psychology, 1939, 29, 267-287. Peters, C. C. Interaction in analysis of variance in-
Jackson, R. W. B. Application of the analysis of terpreted as intercorrelation. Psychological Bulle-
variance and covariance methods to educational tin, 1944, 41, 287-299.
problems. Department of Educational Research, Peters, C. C., & VanVoorhis, W. R. Statistical pro-
University of Toronto Bulletin, 1940, 11, 67-74. cedures and their mathematical bases. New York:
Johnson, P. 0. Use of Fisherian statistics. Journal McGraw-Hill, 1940.
oj Educational Research, 1943, 36, 627-630. Reitz, W. Statistical techniques for the study of
Johnson, P. 0. Statistical methods in research. New institutional differences. Journal oj Experimental
York: Prentice-Hall, 1949. Education, 1934, 3, 11-24.
184 ANTHONY J. RUCCI AND RYAN D. TWENEY

Rider, P. R. An introduction to modern statistical Thouless, R. H. Review of Mr. Whatley Carington's


analysis. New York: Wiley, 1939. work on trance personalities. Proceedings of the
Royce, J. R. A synthesis of experimental designs in Society for Psychical Research, 1937, 44, 223-275.
program research. Journal of General Psychology, Thouless, R. H. Scientific method and the use of
1950, 43, 295-303. statistics. In F. C. Bartlett, F. Ginsberg, E. J.
Rubin, M. A. A variability study of the normal and Lindgren, & R. L. Thouless (Eds.), The study of
schizophrenic alpha rhythm: 1. Journal of Psy- society. London: Kegan Paul, 1939.
chology, 1938, 6, 325-334. Tippett, L. H. C. The methods of statistics. London:
Rubin.Rabson, G. The influence of analytical pre- Williams and Norgate, 1931.
study in memorizing piano music. Archives of Treloar, A. E. An outline oj biometric analysis.
Psychology, 1937, 31 (No. 220). Minneapolis, Minn.: Burgess, 1936.
Rubin-Rabson, G. Studies in the psychology of Tsao, F. Tests of statistical hypotheses in the case
memorizing piano music. Journal of Educational of unequal or disproportionate numbers of obser-
Psychology, 1939, 30, 321-34S. vations in the subclasses. Psychometrika, 1942, 7,
Rulon, P. J. Fisher's t-test as a special case of his 195-212.
s-test. Journal oj Experimental Education, 1943, Tsao, F. General solution of the analysis of vari-
11, 245-249. ance and covariance in the case of unequal or
Rulon, P. J. Matrix representation of models for the disproportionate numbers of observations in the
analysis of variance and covariance. Psychomet- subclasses. Psychometrika, 1946, 11, 107-128.
rika, 1949, 14, 259-278. Tsao, F., & Johnson, P. 0. A note on solutions of
Satterthwaite, F. E. The synthesis of variance. analysis of variance for the problem of unequal
Psyclwmetrika, 1941, 6, 309-316. or disproportionate subclass numbers. Journal oj
Schocnfeld, W. N. The treatment of multiple entries Experimental Education, 1946, 14, 253-255.
in analysis of variance with three criteria of Walker, H. M. Degrees of freedom. Journal of Edu-
classification. American Journal of Psychology, cational Psychology, 1940, 31, 253-269.
1944, 57, 500-508. Walker, H. M. Elementary statistical methods. New
Schrader, W. A. B. Analysis of variance applied to York: Holt, 1943.
liberalism scores. Journal of Experimental Educa- Walker, H. M. Certain unsolved statistical problems
tion, 1940, 8, 267-270. of importance to psychological research. Harvard
Shen, E. Experimental design and statistical treat- Educational Review, 1947, 17, 297-304.
ment in educational research. Journal of Experi- Webb, W. B., & Lemmon, V. W. A qualification in
mental Education, 1940, S, 346-353. the use of analysis of variance. Psychological
Smith, J. G., & Duncan, A. J. Elementary statistics Bulletin, 1950, 47, 130-136.
and applications. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1944. Wike, E. L., & Church, J. D. Comments on Clark's
Snedecor, G. W. Calculation and interpretation of "The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy." Journal of
the analysis of variance and covariance. Ames, Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1976, IS,
Iowa: Collegiate Press, 1934. 249-256.
Snedecor, G. W. Analysis of variance and covariance Wilks, S. S. Certain generalizations in ANOVA. Bio-
of statistically controlled grades. Journal of the metrika, 1932, 24, 471-474.
American Statistical Association, 1935, 30, 263-268. Wilks, S. S. Mathematical statistics. Princeton, N.J.:
Snedecor, G. W. Statistical methods. Ames, Iowa: Princeton University Press, 1946.
Collegiate Press, 1937. Yates, F. The influence of "Statistical methods for
Stanley, J. C, The influence of Fisher's "The design
research workers" on the development of the
of experiments" on educational research thirty science of statistics. Journal oj the American Sta-
years later. American Educational Research Jour-
tistical Association, 1951, 46, 19-34.
nal, 1966, 3, 223-229.
Taylor, W. S. The use of interactions in analyzing Yates, F. Sir Ronald Fisher and the design of ex-
psychological data. British Journal of Psychology, periments. Biometrics, 1964, 20, 307-321.
1942, 32, 248-258. Yule, G. U., & Kendall, M. G. An introduction to
Thomson, G. H. The use of the Latin square in the theory of statistics. London: Griffin, 1937.
designing educational experiments. British Journal
of Educational Psychology, 1941, 11, 135-137. Received October 23, 1978 •

You might also like