You are on page 1of 9

Brain Injury

ISSN: 0269-9052 (Print) 1362-301X (Online) Journal homepage: www.tandfonline.com/journals/ibij20

Rehabilitation of verbal working memory after left


hemisphere stroke

C. Vallat, P. Azouvi, H. Hardisson, R. Meffert, C. Tessier & P. Pradat-Diehl

To cite this article: C. Vallat, P. Azouvi, H. Hardisson, R. Meffert, C. Tessier & P. Pradat-Diehl
(2005) Rehabilitation of verbal working memory after left hemisphere stroke, Brain Injury,
19:13, 1157-1164, DOI: 10.1080/02699050500110595

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/02699050500110595

Published online: 22 Nov 2010.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 1066

View related articles

Citing articles: 8 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=ibij20
Brain Injury, December 2005; 19(13): 1157–1164

CASE STUDY

Rehabilitation of verbal working memory after left hemisphere stroke

C. VALLAT1, P. AZOUVI2, H. HARDISSON3, R. MEFFERT3,


C. TESSIER3, & P. PRADAT-DIEHL3
1
Antenne UEROS-UGECAN, Hôpital Raymond Poincare´, Garches, France, 2Service de Re´e´ducation Neurologique
Hôpital Raymond Poincare´, Universite´ de Versaille Saint Quentin, Garches, France, and 3Service de Me´decine Physique
et Re´adaptation, Hoˆpital de la Salpe´trie`re, Paris, France

(Received 20 July 2004; accepted 14 February 2005)

Abstract
Primary objective: To assess a rehabilitation programme of working memory.
Research design: Single case multiple-baseline-across-behaviour design in a patient with chronic stroke suffering from a
specific impairment of working memory.
Methods: Rehabilitation included training of storage and processing components of verbal working memory. Outcome was
assessed with specific working memory tests, non-specific tasks requiring working memory, non-target tasks not requiring
working memory and questionnaires addressing daily-life functioning. This assessment was performed twice before the
experimental therapy and once after.
Results: Cognitive measures were stable before therapy. A statistically significant improvement was found for target measures
and for daily-life skills. In contrast, no change was found on non-target tasks.
Conclusion: Improvement was not apparently related to spontaneous recovery, since performance was stable before therapy.
This study suggests that specific cognitive training may improve working memory in patients with brain injury.

Keywords: Working memory, rehabilitation, stroke

Introduction The phonological loop is responsible for the proces-


sing and maintenance of verbal information.
Working memory is used as a system for temporary
This system comprises two components: the phono-
maintenance and manipulation of information.
logical store in which representations of verbal
Baddeley and Hitch [1] and Baddeley [2] described
material are held and a sub-vocal rehearsal mechan-
a working memory model divided into three compo-
ism which serves to refresh the contents of the
nents. The central executive is an attentional control phonological store. The visuo-spatial sketchpad is
system, while the phonological loop and the devoted to the processing of visual and spatial
visuo-spatial sketchpad are two modality-specific information. Although this component has received
slave systems. The central executive supervises and less attention, it is currently assumed to com-
co-ordinates the regulation of information flow and prise two components: a ‘passive’ visual store and
controls strategy switching. The central executive is an active mechanism to refresh the contents of the
more involved in processing than in temporary visuo-spatial store.
storage [3] and has a limited capacity. Furthermore, Working memory is involved in many cognitive
it also serves as an interface between the two slave processes. The central executive is assumed to be
systems. This component is assumed to be similar involved in higher order mental functions, such as
to the supervisor attentional system (SAS) of logical reasoning, mental calculation, text compre-
Norman and Shallice’s [4] attentional model. hension [5] and in the ability to understand and

Correspondence: Claire Vallat, Antenne UEROS-UGECAN, Service de Rééducation Neurologique, Hôpital Raymond Poincaré, 92380 Garches, France.
Tel: þ 331 47 10 76 47. Fax: þ33 1 47 10 70 73. E-mail: claire.vallat@rpc.ap.hp.fr
ISSN 0269–9052 print/ISSN 1362–301X online # 2005 Taylor & Francis
DOI: 10.1080/02699050500110595
1158 C. Vallat et al.

manipulate both spoken and written language. It Case report


is also implicated in control-type functions and
CPP is a 53 year-old right-handed, high school
mainly in non-routine processes [6]. The main func-
graduate male. He was a computer scientist and
tions of the phonological loop are to provide support
managing director of a computer business. In
for the acquisition of new vocabulary, long sentences
November 1997, he suffered a left parietal lobe
comprehension [7] and repetition in order to main-
infarction, responsible for an isolated conduction
tain and refresh information in a short-term store.
aphasia, without any other neurological deficit.
The function of the visuo-spatial sketchpad is less
Conventional speech therapy was undertaken and
well known. his language abilities progressively and dramatically
The more frequently reported causes of working
improved. However, CPP still complained of diffi-
memory deficits in adults are traumatic brain injury culties in everyday life, suggesting an impairment of
(TBI) and Alzheimer’s disease, both associated working memory. He reported a difficulty to hold a
with central executive impairments [8, 9]. A few conversation involving several persons or to take
cases of phonological loop impairments have been notes during professional meetings. It was also
reported after left hemisphere stroke [7, 10]. difficult for him to do shopping because he tended
However, the incidence of working memory deficits to forget some of the items he was supposed to buy
after stroke is probably under-estimated and has and to make mistakes in evaluating the total cost of
not been systematically studied. Furthermore, resi- his purchases. These difficulties contrasted with his
dual impairments frequently reported by recovered apparently recovered language functions and his
aphasic patients could be related to a working ability to solve complex cognitive tasks, such as
memory deficit: difficulty to hold a conversation for difficult arithmetic problems. In March and April
more than a few minutes, particularly with a high 1998, 5 months post-onset, a first neuropsychologi-
speech flow, to understand complex sentences or cal assessment was carried out (which will be
long written texts, to participate in a conversation presented in detail later) which showed a working
between a number of people or to do two things memory deficit, mainly involving the verbal sub-
simultaneously (talking while walking for example). system and the central executive. He then continued
However, there has been, to date, very little to receive conventional speech therapy for a further
research on rehabilitation of working memory. 9 months. In January 1999, 14 months post-onset,
Cicerone [11] has recently reported successful work- CPP intended to return to work, but still had the
ing memory rehabilitation in a study of five patients same complaints and a second neuropsychological
with TBI. In addition, a few rehabilitation studies assessment (that will be presented in detail later) was
of attention have included training tasks addressing carried out, which showed that working memory
components of working memory, mainly the central impairments had not improved. This deficit was
executive. For example, Sturm et al. [12] proposed quite isolated, without any accompanying major
a rehabilitation programme focused on specific deficit of other cognitive functions, including lan-
attentional modules, including divided attention, guage, long-term memory and attention. Indeed,
which is currently thought to rely on the central CPP obtained a normal performance on a widely
executive of working memory. However, it is difficult used French aphasia battery [13], he performed at
to disentangle in these studies the effect on atten- ceiling on a naming test (score 80/80 on the test de
tional processes and on working memory itself. dénomination orale 80 [14]) and his oral compre-
Considering the dramatic consequences of hension, as assessed with the Token test (score:
working memory deficits on higher level cognitive 36/36) was normal as well. He also performed within
processes and its impact on daily-life skills, a rehabili- normal limits on two tests of verbal long-
tation programme of working memory was developed. term memory, the paired associates sub-test of the
This programme was designed within Baddeley’s Wechsler Memory Scale-revised (WMS-R)
[2, 6] theoretical framework and mainly addressed [15] (score: 23/24) and the Rey Auditory Verbal
the central executive and the phonological loop. A Learning Test [16] (score, free recall: 54/60).
single case design was used, with multiple baselines Attention was assessed with a computerized atten-
across behaviours, including an assessment of the tional assessment battery [17], using the alertness
generalization to daily life activities. The objective of and selective attention sub-tests, which were both
the present study was to assess both the efficacy and performed within normal limits (standard notes for
the specificity of this working memory therapy in a reaction time, respectively, 61 and 51). The only test
patient with a chronic stroke suffering from a selective that was found to be slightly impaired was visual
impairment of the central executive and the long-term memory, as assessed with Rey complex
phonological loop. figure recall [18] (score: 20/36). It was then decided
Rehabilitation of verbal working memory 1159

to start an experimental therapy, with special at level 1 with a three syllable concrete word:
emphasis on working memory. therapist spelled [ta/pã/lõ], ‘ta-pan-lon’ and the
subject have to answer [pãtalõ], ‘pantalon’.
. Alphabetic way: The therapist presented a letter
Rehabilitation method and a calculation (addition or subtraction) and the
A specific cognitive retraining of the central execu- patient had to find the target letter following the
tive and the phonological loop was used. Eight increasing or decreasing alphabetical order (for
different tasks were used, each one arranged in a example, B þ 6 ¼ H; E  2 ¼ C). There were four
difficulty hierarchy order organized following two levels of difficulty, depending on the numbers to
criteria, capacity and level of processing. Capacity be added or subtracted (either short numbers,
was related to the amount of information to be stored below 5 or long numbers, from 6–9).
(e.g. number of words, syllables or phonemes). The . Word sorting in alphabetic order: The patient was
level of cognitive processing was related to stimulus required to sort a series of concrete words into
complexity (e.g. frequency of usage, concreteness). alphabetic order. There were nine levels of
Stimuli were presented auditorily, only once, and at difficulty, depending on the number of words
a regular speed (1 item per second) and responses (three, four or five) and of syllables (one, two or
were given orally. three). For example, at level 1 with three one-
CPP received three 1-hour training sessions per syllable-words: [s"l/vwal/mul] ‘sel,voile,moule’
week for 6 months. In each session each one of was heard and the answer was: [mul/s"l/vwal].
the eight tasks was trained in the same order. The . Acronyms: The task was to find the word made
difficulty level was progressively adapted to CPP’s from the initial phonemes of a list of words. Five
performance, starting at a 3-item level (i.e. CPP’s levels of difficulty depending on the number of
n-1 backward digit span). Each level was trained words (two to five) and the number of syllables
until the patient succeeded in 90% of the trials. (one or two) were designed. For example, at level 1
There were eight types of tasks. For the first five with two words of one syllable: [bal/wa], ‘bal, oie’
tasks described below there were six levels of diffi- was heard and the answer was [bwa], ‘bois’.
culty depending on the number of syllables (one,
two or three syllables) and the degree of imagery
(concrete or abstract). In each one of these six Experimental design and outcome measures
levels, words were presented in increasing order of
A single-case methodology with a multiple-baseline-
number of letters and decreasing order of frequency.
across-behaviour design was used [19]. As previously
Before starting the therapy, the feasibility of these
stated, the therapy was started 14 months post-onset.
tasks was assessed in 10 control subjects, matched
At that time, spontaneous recovery was unlikely.
for age, sex and educational level to CPP. All tasks
Moreover, the working memory deficit had not
were completed at ceiling by control subjects.
improved during the previous 9 months, despite
Following is a description of the eight tasks:
conventional speech therapy. The outcome measures
. Reconstitution of words from oral spelling: The were similar to those used on baseline but, to limit
patient had to reconstruct and pronounce a word retest effects, parallel versions of tests were used. In
that was spelled by the therapist. addition, to control for a non-specific effect of
. Reconstitution of words from oral spelling with a letter training, non-target measures were also taken.
omitted: This is the same task as the previous one, These measures were assumed not to require work-
but with a letter omitted, replaced by a ‘bip’. For ing memory and were not specifically trained.
example, at level 3 with a two syllable concrete Hence, they were not expected to improve after
word: the item spelt is p-a.‘bip’-i-e-r- and the therapy. The outcome measures that will be pre-
subject had to find: ‘papier’. sented hereafter were obtained just after the end
. Oral spelling. of the therapy. A 3-month follow-up was initially
. Odd or even number of letters in a word: The task was planned, but could not be done, because CPP had
to find whether the number of letters in a word returned to work at that time.
heard was odd or even. For example, at level 5 Three sets of tasks were used: (i) cognitive tasks
with a three syllable concrete word: [kanape], specifically designed to assess the different compo-
‘canapé’ is heard and the answer must be ‘even’. nents of working memory; (ii) related verbal complex
. Reconstitution of words from syllables: The therapist tasks, i.e. tasks that were not specifically designed to
presented cluttered syllable words that the subject assess working memory, but that nevertheless
had to reorder and pronounce. There were three, included a high working memory load (text compre-
four or five syllables per word. For example, hension and arithmetic problem solving); and
1160 C. Vallat et al.

Table I. Digit spans. Correct responses (%) (out of five trials) at each span length for controls (n ¼ 10) and for patient CPP. For control
subjects, results are the mean (SD).

Span length

2 3 4 5 6 7

Forward digit span


Controls 100 (0.0) 100 (0.0) 100 (0.0) 96.6 (7.0) 86.6 (17.2) 64.9 (31.8)
Baseline 1 100 80 80 40 60 0
Baseline 2 100 100 80 60 60 0
Post-treatment 100 100 100 100 100 80
Backward digit span
Controls 100 (0.0) 100 (0.0) 94.9 (8.1) 53.3 (39.9) 38.3 (46.5) 9.9 (21.1)
Baseline 1 100 100 40 0 0 0
Baseline 2 100 100 60 0 0 0
Post-treatment 100 100 80 80 60 0

(iii) ecological questionnaires, designed to assess dramatically reduced, in comparison to matched


the consequences of working memory deficit in controls (Table I). This impairment was particularly
everyday life. For all cognitive tasks, three parallel marked for backward digit span. Chi-square statistics
versions were designed in order to minimize re-test were used to compare the percentage of correct
effects. For specific working memory tasks, CPP’s responses between the first and the second baseline
performance was compared to a group of 10 matched assessments and between the second baseline and
control subjects, of similar age and education post-therapy. Forward and backward digit spans
background (mean age ¼ 48.2, SD ¼ 6.2, range: were stable before the therapy (corrected 2
40–56, all subjects having 12 years or more of (df ¼ 1) ¼ 0.07 and 0.07, respectively, both p > 0.1).
education). Forward span improved significantly after training
Statistical analysis used corrected chi-square tests (corrected 2 ¼ 7.1, p < 0.01), while backward span
on the total number of correct responses. For each improvement fell just short of significance (corrected
measure, two separate tests were performed, one to 2 ¼ 3.3, p ¼ 0.06).
compare performance on the two baseline sessions
and one to compare performance on the second Assessment of the phonological loop. This was only
baseline and the post-therapy assessment. performed on the second baseline assessment. As
previously stated (see Introduction), the phonologi-
cal loop includes two sub-components, the phono-
Results
logical store and the sub-vocal rehearsal process. The
As previously stated, two baseline assessments of phonological store was tested using the phonological
working memory were obtained, respectively, 5 and similarity effect [2]. According to Baddeley, short-
14 months post-onset, to control for re-test effects term recall of phonologically close letters (such as t,
and/or spontaneous recovery. Outcome assess- p, b, v), is lower than phonologically dissimilar letters
ment was completed after the end of the 6-month (such as r, m, l, s). This effect is usually attributed to
therapy. the phonological store. Again, this was assessed with
five trials at each span length. As shown in Table II,
CPP’s letter span was impaired, as compared to
Cognitive tasks specifically designed to assess
controls, for dissimilar letters, but lay within normal
the different components of working memory
limits for phonologically close letters. He did not
Digit and visuo-spatial spans. Both forward and show any phonological similarity effect. Indeed,
backward digit and visuo-spatial spans were assessed. while, as expected, control subjects performed
Five trials were given for each span length, starting better for phonologically dissimilar letters, CPP
with two items. The test ended when the patient presented the reverse pattern of performance
failed the five consecutive trials for a given length. (Table II). This suggested an impairment of the
The measures were the percentage of correct phonological store [6]. Post-therapy assessment
responses out of five trials for each span length. showed a slight non-significant improvement of
Visuo-spatial spans were within the normal range both phonologically close and dissimilar letter
(maximal level achieved: 5 for both forward and spans (corrected 2 ¼ 0.9 and 0.05, respec-
backward visuo-spatial spans) and were not signifi- tively, both p > 0.1). CPP continued to show a
cantly modified post-therapy. In contrast, CPP’s better performance for phonologically close letters
forward and backward digit spans were both (Table II).
Rehabilitation of verbal working memory 1161

Table II. Letter spans. Correct responses (%) (out of five trials) at each span length for controls (n ¼ 10) and for patient CPP. For control
subjects, results are the mean (SD).

Span length

2 3 4 5 6 7 8

Phonologically close letters


Controls 100 (0.0) 100 (0.0) 100 (0.0) 85.0 (26.8) 55.0 (38.7)
Baseline 2 100 100 100 60 40 0
Post-treatment 100 100 100 100 60 40 0
Phonologically dissimilar letters
Controls 100 (0.0) 100 (0.0) 100 (0.0) 95.0 (10.5) 77.5 (32.2) 42.5 (44.2) 20.0 (34.9)
Baseline 2 100 100 100 40 20 0
Post-treatment 100 100 100 60 40 0

Table III. Word spans. Correct responses (%) (out of five trials) at each span length for controls (n ¼ 10) and for patient CPP. For control
subjects, results are the mean (SD).

Span length

2 3 4 5 6 7 8

Short words
Controls 100 (0.0) 100 (0.0) 100 (0.0) 77.5 (14.2) 35.0 (33.7) 12.5 (21.2) 0.0 (0.0)
Baseline 2 100 100 100 100 40 0
Post-treatment 100 100 100 100 100 0
Long words
Controls 100 (0.0) 100 (0.0) 67.5 (26.5) 10.0 (17.5) 0.0 (0.0)
Baseline 2 100 100 0 0
Post-treatment 100 100 80 0

The sub-vocal rehearsal process was assessed Table IV. Brown–Peterson task. Correct responses (%) at three
using the word-length effect, with monosyllabic and recall delays for controls (n ¼ 10) and for patient CPP. For control
quadri-syllabic words. As can be seen in Table III, subjects, results are the mean (SD).
CPPs short-word span was normal, but his long- Recall delay
word span was impaired comparatively to controls.
5 seconds 10 seconds 20 seconds
However, he showed a preserved word-length
effect, as indicated by a higher span for short than Articulatory suppression
for long words (higher level achieved, respectively, Controls 100 (0.0) 100 (0.0) 100 (0.0)
6 and 3), similarly to controls, suggesting that the Baseline 1 57 66 16
Baseline 2 83 50 33
sub-vocal rehearsal process was preserved. A slight Post-treatment 66 66 66
post-therapy improvement occurred for both short Digit addition
and long word spans, but that again did not reach Controls 92.0 (13.9) 86.0 (16.5) 76.0 (27.9)
statistical significance (corrected 2 ¼ 0.4 and 0.9, Baseline 1 33 0 33
both p > 0.1) (Table III). Baseline 2 16 33 0
Post-treatment 66 83 100

Assessment of the central executive system. The central


executive was tested using the Brown Peterson
paradigm [20]. Subjects were asked to recall con- first to the second baseline sessions. In contrast, after
sonant trigrams after three delays (5, 10 or rehabilitation, performance improved dramatically,
20 seconds) with or without an interfering task particularly under the most demanding condition,
(motor task, articulatory suppression and digit addi- with digit addition (Table IV). This effect was
tion). While CPPs performance without interference statistically highly significant (corrected 2 ¼ 13.4,
or with a motor task was at ceiling, it dropped p < 0.001). In this most difficult condition, CPPs
dramatically, as compared to controls, with more post-therapy performance was close to ceiling and did
demanding interfering tasks (articulation or digit not significantly differ from that of controls. In
addition) and at all recall delays (5 to 20 seconds) contrast, although there was also a mild improv-
(Table IV). The scores were not modified from the ement under the easier condition (articulatory
1162 C. Vallat et al.

suppression), this effect was not statistically signi- and shopping including money use (Table V),
ficant (corrected 2 ¼ 0.1, p > 0.1). contrasting with CPPs good performance on tradi-
In summary, these results suggested that CPP tional aphasia testing.
suffered from an impairment of working memory, In addition, a specific questionnaire was designed,
mainly involving the central executive and the to assess working memory functioning in various
phonological store, that was stable before treatment daily-life situations. This questionnaire included 16
and that significantly improved after rehabilitation. questions addressing three different domains:
storage/processing aspects of working memory
Related complex tasks (eight questions, e.g. ‘do you have difficulty in recal-
ling a series of digits, such as a telephone number?’
Text oral comprehension was assessed with 10 or ‘do you have difficulty in understanding what
sentences of different length and syntactic complex- you read?’); attentional aspects of working memory
ity, designed following the methodology of a widely (six questions, e.g. ‘do you have difficulty in doing
used French aphasia battery [13]. Performance something with background noise?’ or ‘does fatigue
was slightly impaired (correct responses ¼ 80%). affect your concentration?’); executive aspects of
Arithmetic problem solving was assessed with 15 working memory (two questions, such as ‘do you
problems of increasing complexity, close to those have difficulty in doing two things simultaneously?’).
used in the arithmetic sub-test of the Wechsler Adult A dichotomous scale was used (0 ¼ difficulty present,
Intelligence Scale–revised [21]. Performance was 1 ¼ no difficulty). A total score (maximum ¼ 16) and
severely impaired (correct responses on the two three sub-scores (storage/processing; attention;
successive baselines ¼ 20% and 33%). These two executive) were computed. Again, for clarity of
tasks were performed without difficulty (100%
presentation, all scores were recoded to obtain a
correct responses) by matched control subjects.
maximum score of 100, higher scores indicating
Post-treatment, there was a trend for a mild
better functioning. As shown in Table VI, CPP
improvement on both tasks (correct responses,
reported difficulties for the three sub-scales.
respectively, 100% and 66%) that did not reach
An improvement was found on both question-
statistical significance, presumably due to a ceiling
naires (verbal communication questionnaire, total
effect for text comprehension.
score: 2 ¼ 6.2; p ¼ 0.01; working memory question-
naire: 2 ¼ 4.800; p < 0.05) (Tables V and VI). CPP’s
Ecological questionnaire complaints in everyday life were significantly reduced
after treatment. In addition, after rehabilitation,
Two questionnaires were used to assess the con-
sequences of working memory impairments on daily- CPP returned to full-time work at his previous
life activities. A verbal communication questionnaire level. He was contacted by phone 3 months post-
(L’Echelle de communication verbale de Bordeaux: return to work. Unfortunately, he was too busy to
ECVB) [22] was used, because CPPs difficulties complete a follow-up neuropsychological assess-
were mainly related to the verbal components of ment. Nevertheless, he was working full-time and
working memory. This questionnaire includes 38 did not further mention any significant difficulty
questions addressing eight domains of verbal com- either at home or in his job.
munication, such as shopping, social interaction or
phone conversation. For each item, a 4-level scale
was used to assess the frequency of occurrence of the Table VI. Working memory questionnaire. Mean score for each
trouble (0 ¼ always, 3 ¼ never). For clarity of pre- category (maximum score ¼ 100, higher scores indicating better
sentation, scores in each domain were recoded to functioning).
obtain a maximum score of 100, higher scores Total Storage Attention Executive
indicating better functioning. Difficulties were
Baseline 43.7 50 33.3 50
found in all daily-life situations addressed by the
Post-treatment 81.2 100 50 100
questionnaire, mainly in conversation, phone use

Table V. Verbal communication questionnaire. Mean score (maximum score ¼ 100, higher scores indicating better functioning).

Intention Phone Social


Total expression Conversation conversation Shopping interaction Reading Writing Calculation

Baseline 69 89 54 57 67 81 92 83 0
Post-treatment 83 100 63 81 83 95 100 100 0
Rehabilitation of verbal working memory 1163

Non-target measures Post-therapy assessment showed a significant


improvement of the main target measures, particu-
Non-target measures included verbal fluency and
larly forward and backward digit spans, which were
long-term memory tests. Two conditions were used
close to the tasks used during training, but also of
to assess verbal fluency [23]. For each one, the
the Brown–Peterson task, using a procedure that
patient was asked to spell out as many words as
was not trained during therapy. There was also a
possible within 2 minutes, first animals then words
trend for an improvement of other related but
starting with a ‘p’. Scores were, respectively, 24
untrained tasks (arithmetic problem-solving). More
and 16. Visual long-term memory was assessed
importantly, an improvement was also found in
with Complex Figure recall [18, 24] (score: 20/36).
everyday life situations requiring working memory,
Verbal long-term memory was assessed with the Rey
as rated by the ecological questionnaires. In addition,
Auditory Verbal Learning Test [16], including 12
after this experimental rehabilitation, CPP returned
words repeated five times for a maximum score of 60
to full-time work at his previous level. By contrast,
(free recall score 54/60). None of these non-target
there was no improvement of non-target measures
measures changed after rehabilitation.
that were not trained and were not expected to
improve, such as visuo-spatial spans (visuo-spatial
Discussion working memory was not directly trained) and
other tasks addressing different cognitive domains,
The present study suggested an effectiveness of a such as long-term memory or verbal fluency.
specific therapy focusing on verbal and attentional Hence, CPP’s improvement could not readily be
aspects of working memory and its generalization to attributed to spontaneous recovery, non-specific
daily-life activities in a patient with a chronic stroke cognitive stimulation or to re-test effects. Indeed,
suffering from a selective impairment of the central the deficits had been stable for many months before
executive and the phonological loop. CPP presented, the experimental rehabilitation started, despite
1 year after a left hemisphere stroke, an isolated conventional speech therapy and, moreover, the
impairment of working memory. This impairment improvement was limited to the domain that was
mainly involved verbal aspects of working memory, specifically addressed by the treatment. In addition,
while the visuo-spatial sketchpad seemed to be to control for any re-test effect, parallel forms of
preserved. The patient’s spontaneous complaints tests were used in the different testing sessions and
mainly concerned complex resource-demanding the outcome measures were always different from
verbal tasks. Neuropsychological testing showed the tasks used for the therapy. These results suggest
reduced forward and backward digit spans, an that the training resulted in a specific improvement
impairment of the phonological store (as shown by of verbal and attentional aspects of working
a lack of the phonological similarity effect) and a memory, with a generalization to untrained tasks
poor performance in the Brown–Peterson task with and a transfer to everyday life skills.
demanding interference tasks. Visuo-spatial spans To the authors’ knowledge, there has been only
were within normal limits. These results suggested little research on the rehabilitation of working
an impairment of the phonological store and of the memory. Cicerone [11] reported a remediation study
central executive system. This deficit had serious of the central executive after mild TBI. Four patients
consequences on his everyday life skills. On formal were included and the outcome measures were the
testing, deficits were found on complex tasks requir- Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT) and
ing working memory, such as text comprehension an attentional questionnaire. The present study has
and arithmetic problem-solving. When interviewed several similarities with Cicerone’s study, the same
with standardized ecological questionnaires, CPP theoretical model was used and the training
reported difficulties in a wide range of everyday method was also derived from previous studies on
life situations, such as shopping, conversations or rehabilitation of attention. However, the training
phone use. tasks that were used by Cicerone [11] were different
This deficit did not improve despite 1 year of and consisted of n-back, random generation and
conventional speech therapy. Indeed, two baseline dual tasks, with special emphasis on allocation of
measures of working memory were taken 5 and 14 attentional resources and management of informa-
months post-stroke onset and did not show any tion processing speed, similar to time pressure
improvement. At that time, when spontaneous management [25]. By contrast, the training strategy
recovery seemed very unlikely, a specific rehabilita- was based on a direct restoration strategy of impaired
tion programme was undertaken. The training tasks working memory.
were arranged in a progressive hierarchical order of There has been more research on the rehabilitation
difficulty, regarding both storage and processing of attention, which is a concept closely related to
aspects of working memory. working memory. However, in most of these studies,
1164 C. Vallat et al.

attention was trained as a whole, without clearly deli- 8. Azouvi P, Couillet J, Leclercq M, Martin Y, Asloun S,
neating effects on the different attentional domains. Rousseaux M. Divided attention and mental effort after
severe traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychologia 2004;42:
In a recent review, Park and Ingles [26] concluded 1260–1268.
that there is only limited evidence of the effectiveness 9. Baddeley A, Della Sala S, Spinnler H. The two-component
of such rehabilitation programmes. Sturm et al. [12] hypothesis of memory deficit in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal
recently emphasized that different attentional deficits of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 1991;13:
should be treated with specific training methods, 372–380.
10. Waters G, Caplan D, Hidebrandt N. On the structure of
focusing on the particular impairment exhibited by verbal short-term memory and its functional role, in sentence
an individual patient. Accordingly, in the present comprehension: Evidence from neuropsychology. Cognitive
study it was also found that improvement was limited Neuropsychology 1991;8:81–126.
to the sub-components of working memory which 11. Cicerone K. Remediation of ‘working attention’ in mild
had been specifically trained (verbal and executive), traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury 2002;16:185–195.
12. Sturm W, Willmes K, Orgass B, et al. Do specific attention
while visual working memory, which was not addres- deficits need specific training? Neuropsychological
sed by the treatment, remained unchanged. From a Rehabilitation 1997;7: 81–103.
theoretical point a view, such results also support 13. Ducarne B. Examen de l’aphasie. Paris: Edition du Centre de
the modular approaches of working memory and Psychologie Appliquée; 1989.
attention. 14. Deloche G, Metz-Lutz MN, Kremin H, Hannequin D,
Ferrand I, Perrier D, Dordain M, Quint S, Cardebat D,
In conclusion, the present study suggested an
Lota AM, van der Linden M, Larroque C, Bunel G,
effectiveness of a specific therapy focusing on verbal Pichard B, Naud E. Test de dénomination orale de 80
and central executive aspects of working memory images: Do 80. Paris: ECPA; 1997.
and its generalization to daily-life activities. Such 15. Wechsler D. Wechsler memory scale-revised manual.
therapy can easily be used in a conventional rehabili- San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 1987.
16. Rey A. Examen clinique en psychologie. Paris: Presse
tation setting. In the present case, a long-lasting
Universitaire de France; 1964.
rehabilitation programme was given. The effective- 17. Zimmermann P, Fimm B. Batterie de tests neuropsychologi-
ness of a shorter training duration is currently ques pour l’évaluation des troubles de l’attention. Freiburg:
under study. In addition, further study could address Vera Fimm, Psychologische Testsysteme; 1992.
the specificity of therapy on different components of 18. Osterrieth PA. Le test de copie d’une figure complexe.
Archives de Psychologie 1944;30:206–356.
working memory.
19. Wilson B. Single-case experimental designs in neuropsycho-
logical rehabilitation. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Neuropsychology 1987;9:527–544.
20. Van der Linden M, Coyette F, Seron X. Selective impairment
References
of the ‘central executive’ component of working memory:
1. Baddeley A, Hitch G. Working memory. In: Bower GA, editor. A single case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology 1992;9:
Recent advances in learning and motivation. New York: 301–326.
Academic Press; 1974. pp 47–90. 21. Wechsler D. Wais-R manual. New York: The Psychological
2. Baddeley AD. Working memory. New York: Oxford University Corporation; 1981.
Press; 1986. 22. Darrigrand B, Mazaux J. Echelle de communication verbale
3. Baddeley A, Logie RH. Models of working memory: de bordeaux. Isbergues: Ortho Edition; 2000.
Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. In: 23. Cardebat D, Doyon B, Puel M, Goulet P, Joanette Y.
Miyake A, Shah P, editors. Working memory: The multiple- Evocation lexicale formelle et sémantique chez des sujets
component model. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University normaux. Performances et dynamiques de production en
Pres; 1999. pp 28–61. fonction du sexe, de l’âge et du niveau d’étude. Acta
4. Norman DA, Shallice T. Attention to action. Willed and Neurologica Belgica 1990;90:207–217.
automatic control of behavior. University of California, San 24. Taylor E. Psychological appraisal of children with cerebral
Diego, CHIP report N 99; 1980. deficits. Cambridge: Harvard University Press; 1959.
5. Kintsch W, Van Dijk T. Strategies of discours comprehension. 25. Fassoti L, Kovacs F, Eling P, Brouwer W. Time pressure
New York: Academic Press; 1983. management as a compensatory strategy training after
6. Baddeley A. Recent developments in working memory. closed head injury. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation 2000;
Current Opinion in Neurobiology 1998;8:234–238. 10:47–65.
7. Vallar G, Baddeley A. Phonological short-term store and 26. Park NW, Ingles JL. Effectiveness of attention rehabilita-
sentence processing. Cognitive Neuropsychology 1987;4: tion after an acquired brain injury: A meta-analysis.
479–491. Neuropsychology 2001;15:199–210.

You might also like