You are on page 1of 13

International Journal of Clinical and Experimental

Hypnosis

ISSN: 0020-7144 (Print) 1744-5183 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/nhyp20

PSYCHOLOGICAL MINDEDNESS, ATTITUDES


TOWARD HYPNOSIS, AND EXPECTANCY AS
CORRELATES OF HYPNOTIZABILITY

Lauren L. Koep, Mattie L. Biggs, Joshua R. Rhodes & Gary R. Elkins

To cite this article: Lauren L. Koep, Mattie L. Biggs, Joshua R. Rhodes & Gary R. Elkins (2020)
PSYCHOLOGICAL MINDEDNESS, ATTITUDES TOWARD HYPNOSIS, AND EXPECTANCY
AS CORRELATES OF HYPNOTIZABILITY, International Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Hypnosis, 68:1, 68-79, DOI: 10.1080/00207144.2020.1682255

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/00207144.2020.1682255

Published online: 08 Jan 2020.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 360

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Citing articles: 3 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=nhyp20
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF CLINICAL AND EXPERIMENTAL HYPNOSIS
2020, VOL. 68, NO. 1, 68–79
https://doi.org/10.1080/00207144.2020.1682255

PSYCHOLOGICAL MINDEDNESS, ATTITUDES TOWARD


HYPNOSIS, AND EXPECTANCY AS CORRELATES OF
HYPNOTIZABILITY
Lauren L. Koep, Mattie L. Biggs, Joshua R. Rhodes, and Gary R. Elkins
Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Baylor University, Waco, Texas, USA

ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY


This study aimed to understand how psychological mindedness, atti- Received April 01, 2019
tudes toward hypnosis, and expectancy of hypnotizability are related to Accepted June 24, 2019
hypnotizability. Ninety-one undergraduate students were given mea-
sures pertaining to attitudes toward hypnosis, psychological minded-
ness, and self-ratings of expectancy of hypnotizability. The subjects were
then administered the Elkins Hypnotizability Scale (EHS). Results demon-
strated a significant correlation between participant scores on the EHS
and the Attitudes Toward Hypnosis Scale (r = .401, p < .01) and self-
ratings of expectancy of hypnotizability (r = .391, p < .01). The results of
this study did not reveal any statistically significant correlations between
psychological mindedness and hypnotizability (r = .113, p > .29) or
expectancy of hypnotizability (r = .175, p > .10). Additional research is
needed to fully understand the relationship between psychological
mindedness and hypnotizability.

Hypnosis has been defined as “a state of consciousness involving focused attention and
reduced peripheral awareness characterized by an enhanced capacity for response to
suggestion” (Elkins, Barabasz, Council, & Spiegel, 2015, p. 382). The process of hypnosis
requires and emphasizes an inward reflection or inward awareness on the part of the
participant. Much of what is asked of a participant during hypnosis is to turn inward and
allow oneself to experience the imagery and feelings that arise throughout this personal
process. Research has indicated that hypnosis is useful in clinical treatment for a variety of
physical and psychological problems (Elkins, 2017; Moore & Tasso, 2008). Hypnotizability
of an individual is one factor that may moderate the experience and effectiveness of
hypnosis (Montgomery, Duhamel, & Redd, 2000). Hypnotizability is defined as “an
individual’s ability to experience suggested alterations in physiology, sensations, emotions,
thoughts, or behavior during hypnosis” (Elkins et al., 2015, p. 383).
The psychological components of hypnotizability are not yet fully known (McConkey,
2008). In the past, researchers have attempted to relate hypnotizability to various personality
factors, such as absorption (De Groot, Gwynn, & Spanos, 1988), locus of control (Austrin &
Pereira, 1978), and empathy (Wickramasekera & Szlyk, 2003). Absorption has been one of the
most consistently investigated potential correlates of hypnotizability. However, these studies
generally demonstrate a weak correlation ranging from an r value of .31 to .43. For example,
the study by De Groot et al. (1988) investigated absorption and hypnotizability, demonstrating

CONTACT Gary R. Elkins Gary_Elkins@baylor.edu Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Baylor University,
801 Washington Avenue, 2nd Floor, Waco, TX 76701, USA
© 2019 International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis
CORRELATES OF HYPNOTIZABILITY 69

that scores on measures of absorption correlated with hypnotizability (r = .32), but only in
women and only when they had been informed about hypnotic testing prior to completing
their absorption questionnaire. Also, Austrin and Pereira (1978) found a positive correlation
(r = .35) between external locus of control and hypnotizability. However, this small correlation
was only found among female participants. Wickramasekera and Szlyk (2003) examined the
relationship between empathy and hypnotizability. A positive correlation was found between
scores on a measure of empathy and scores on a hypnotizability scale (r = .41). Positive
correlations were also found between absorption and hypnotizability (r = .31) and absorption
and empathy (r = .43), suggesting that both absorption and empathy may contribute to
hypnotizability (Wickramasekera & Szlyk, 2003).
A consistently investigated correlate of hypnotizability has been attitudes toward
hypnosis. London, Cooper, and Johnson (1961) found a positive correlation (r = .40)
between attitudes toward hypnosis and hypnotizability. Melei and Hilgard (1964) found
that attitudes toward hypnosis correlated positively (r = .37) with hypnotizability in
participants with no previous hypnotic experience. Barber and Calverley (1964) also
found evidence that attitudes toward hypnosis correlate positively with hypnotizability.
Spanos and McPeake (1975) found a positive correlation (r = .29) between attitudes
toward hypnosis and hypnotizability. Yanchar and Johnson (1981) replicated these find-
ings, demonstrating that attitudes toward hypnosis were positively correlated with hyp-
notic susceptibility (r = .35). Therefore, the research has shown that attitudes toward
hypnosis is a promising correlate of hypnotizability.
In addition to attitudes toward hypnosis, the expectancy of being able to experience
hypnosis has been a focus of research. Cronin, Spanos, and Barber (1971) found that they
were able to alter subjects’ expectancy of their hypnotizability by providing them with
favorable information about hypnosis. This favorable information and subsequent increase
in expectancy resulted in increased responses to hypnotic suggestions (F = 35.15, p < .001;
Cronin et al., 1971). Silva and Kirsch (1992) and Kirsch, Silva, Comey, and Reed (1995)
both found a positive correlation (r = .38) between expectancy and hypnotizability. Later,
Braffman and Kirsch’s (2001) study also highlighted the role of expectancy in hypnotiz-
ability (r = .33). Benham, Woody, Wilson, and Nash (2006) found that an initial measure
of expectancy was positively correlated with hypnotizability (r = .39).
While research has shown that positive correlations between hypnotizability, absorp-
tion, locus of control, and empathy are present, each of these correlations is modest in
nature. In addition, while the literature does reveal that attitudes toward hypnosis and
expectancy of hypnotizability are positively correlated to hypnotizability, further research
is necessary to identify stronger correlates of hypnotizability in order for clinicians to
maximize clinical utility of hypnosis.

Psychological Mindedness
One particularly promising component that has not yet been thoroughly investigated is
the concept of psychological mindedness. Psychological mindedness as a correlate of
hypnotizability can have clinical and research implications and allow for more tailored
hypnosis interventions, and is, therefore, the focus of the current study. The definition of
psychological mindedness has evolved over the years. Appelbaum (1973, p. 36) defined
psychological mindedness as a combination of four components and as “A person’s ability
70 L. L. KOEP ET AL.

to see relationships among thoughts, feelings, and actions, with the goal of learning the
meanings and causes of his experiences and behavior.”
Hall (1992, p. 138) built upon this definition by proposing a model of psychological
mindedness that states that “accurate psychological-mindedness is displayed by an indi-
vidual to the extent that he or she displays both the interest in and ability for reflectivity
about psychological processes, relationships, and meanings, and across both affective and
intellectual dimensions.” Hall’s (1992, p. 138) model went on to claim that “accurate
psychological-mindedness is contributed to and limited by the existence of accurate
intellectual psychological-mindedness and accurate affective psychological-mindedness.”
Drawing upon Hall’s (1992) definition, Grant (2001, p. 12) proposed the following
revised definition: “Psychological mindedness is a form of metacognition: a predisposition
to engage in acts of affective and intellectual inquiry into how and why oneself and/or
others behave, think, and feel in the way they do.” Grant’s (2001) emphasis upon
metacognition arose from his assertion that the ability to think about one’s own thoughts
was an implicit component of all previous definitions of psychological mindedness and
merely had not been previously stated in those terms.
Finally, Denollet and Nyklicek (2004) conducted extensive reviews of the existing
literature regarding psychological mindedness and focused on the construct as it relates
to both mental and physical health. They highlighted how psychological mindedness has
come to be understood as a multifaceted construct, made up of several components.
However, they focused on how psychological mindedness can help individuals cope with
various negative emotional states, consequently impacting their emotional health as well
as their physical health and relaxation. Denollet and Nyklicek (2004) mentioned the
capacity for psychological mindedness to serve as a type of coping skill and stated that
“psychological mindedness is closely related to the willingness to accept the influence of
intrapsychic processes on behavior and on coping with the stress of everyday life.” In sum,
the researchers proposed the following definition of psychological mindedness:
“Psychological mindedness refers to the intrinsic motivation to be in touch with one’s
inner feelings and thoughts by monitoring and analyzing them in an adaptive way”
(Denollet & Nyklicek, 2004, p. 191). This definition greatly influenced the construction
of the Balanced Index of Psychological Mindedness (BIPM; Nyklicek & Denollet, 2009),
which has been utilized in the current study. Based on this definition, if someone is open
to psychological concepts, one could propose that the individual would also be open to
a psychological intervention such as hypnosis. Therefore, it is rational to hypothesize that
people who have more of this trait would be more hypnotizable.
There has been one previous study with regard to psychological mindedness and
hypnotizability. McKnight (1980) provided an early investigation of personality correlates
and hypnotizability. In his study, 100 undergraduate volunteers were administered the
Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility (Shor & Orne, 1962) in groups of 20 to 30
via a public address system in classrooms. Subsequently, the complete California
Psychological Inventory (Gough, 1956) was administered. However, the impersonal
group setting in which the hypnotizability scale was administered during this study
prevented researchers from closely observing participants’ responses to the items on the
scales and collecting qualitative data. Additionally, the personality inventory was adminis-
tered to participants after the hypnotic induction, potentially affecting their responses to
the California Psychological Inventory. Furthermore, the measure of psychological
CORRELATES OF HYPNOTIZABILITY 71

mindedness utilized by McKnight does not accurately represent current definitions of this
concept.
Although no specific relationship between one particular scale of the California
Psychological Inventory and hypnotizability was predicted, multiple regression was utilized
to predict hypnotizability from a combination of personality measures included in the
complete California Psychological Inventory. Initial results identified eleven subscales that
were significantly correlated with hypnotizability (McKnight, 1980). However, McKnight
(1980) found that after accounting for the interrelationship among the variables by utilizing
a multiple regression analysis, only the subscales of “Responsibility” (r = − .39) and
“Psychological Mindedness” (r = − .43) were significantly correlated with hypnotizability.
These results indicated that psychological mindedness was a correlate of hypnotizability;
however, since psychological mindedness was not investigated independently, this correlation
should be interpreted with caution. Therefore, in order to achieve more clarity regarding the
connection between the two constructs, it is necessary to further examine the relationship
between psychological mindedness and hypnotizability, as this current study aimed to do.
A review of the literature, as mentioned above, demonstrates an overarching theme that
psychological mindedness is a construct that involves introspection, intrapersonal insight,
understanding and acceptance, and openness to experience. These are personal character-
istics that may be relevant to the hypnotic experience. Furthermore, the ability to
experience hypnosis to the fullest extent varies between individuals. Therefore,
a reasonable hypothesis is that psychological mindedness and hypnotizability could be
related. While McKnight (1980) found a modest negative correlation between psycholo-
gical mindedness and hypnotizability, the current study aims to build on this by investi-
gating psychological mindedness independently as a correlate of hypnotizability as well as
utilizing a measure that reflects the current definition of psychological mindedness.
The primary objective of the current study was to examine the relationship between
hypnotizability and psychological mindedness to determine if there is a positive correla-
tion between the two constructs. The secondary objective was to replicate the previous
findings regarding the relationship between hypnotizability, attitudes toward hypnosis,
and expectancy of hypnotizability. In addition, the study sought to highlight the relation-
ship between psychological mindedness and expectancy of hypnosis.

Methods
Participants
Participants were 91 undergraduate volunteers drawn from Baylor University. Some
participants were offered course credit at the discretion of their professor for participating
in the study. Flyers were posted to advertise the study and request volunteers for the
investigation of “factors impacting hypnosis.” The Department of Psychology and
Neuroscience’s website for recruiting research participants (SONA systems) was also
utilized to recruit volunteers by providing a way to read about and enroll in the study
online.
Participants were required to be at least 18 years of age and have a minimal reading level to
ensure the participant’s ability to complete study measures. Individuals with a previous
diagnosis of borderline personality disorder or schizophrenia (or related psychiatric disorder
72 L. L. KOEP ET AL.

involving psychosis) were excluded from the study. As outlined by Walker (2016), these
exclusions were utilized since psychosis involves a distortion of reality and hypnosis blurs the
boundaries of the subject’s external reality. Individuals who had prior experience with clinical
hypnosis were also excluded from the study. Prior to recruitment of participants or initiation
of the study, approval for the study was attained by the Baylor University Institutional Review
Board (IRB).

Procedure
Participants were presented with basic information about the study and provided
informed consent by the investigator or a qualified research assistant. Upon completion
of informed consent, all participants were asked to complete a demographic questionnaire,
the BIPM (Nyklicek & Denollet, 2009), the Attitudes Toward Hypnosis Scale (Spanos,
Brett, Menary, & Cross, 1987), and the self-rating of estimated hypnotizability. The EHS
(Elkins, 2014), a hypnotic procedure and assessment of hypnotizability, was then admi-
nistered to participants individually by the investigator or a research assistant who
completed training in hypnosis and administration of hypnotizability scales. All partici-
pants were offered debriefing and, if necessary, provided with a referral and contact
information for professional mental health consultation.

Measures
Psychological Mindedness
Psychological mindedness was assessed using the BIPM as this was the most contemporary
and well-validated scale. The BIPM is a 14-item self-report questionnaire assessing the
different components of psychological mindedness. Items are answered on a 5-point Likert
scale with a score of “1” representing Not at all and a score of “5” representing Very much. The
psychometric properties of scores from this measure were examined in two community
samples and one mental health patient sample in the Netherlands. The total score from this
measure demonstrated good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = .85), test-retest reliability
(r = .75), and construct validity (r > .40 with related constructs such as private self-
consciousness and attention to feelings) (Nyklicek & Denollet, 2009). In the current study,
this measure demonstrated good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = .79).

Hypnotizability
Hypnotizability was measured using the Elkins Hypnotizability Scale (EHS). This scale is
a hypnotic procedure and is administered individually by a research assistant specifically
trained in hypnotic induction and assessment procedures. The scale begins with
a hypnotic induction that includes suggestions for relaxation. The participant is then
guided through a series of hypnotic suggestions for experiences ranging from simple
motor responses to more involved imagery and posthypnotic amnesia. Items are adminis-
tered in order of progressing difficulty within the context of a continuous hypnotic
procedure. A participant’s responses to items are scored numerically based upon extent
of response to the hypnotic suggestion given. A total score, ranging from 0 to 12, is then
calculated to indicate the participant’s level of hypnotic responsiveness. The scale takes
approximately 30 minutes to administer and provides a sample of hypnotic responsiveness
CORRELATES OF HYPNOTIZABILITY 73

(Elkins, 2014). The EHS was chosen for this study because it is a brief and safe measure to
use with clinical and research participants. Additionally, the EHS has demonstrated
adequate internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = .78), its items have shown good discrimi-
nating ability, and scores of the EHS and the Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale, Form
C (Weitzenhoffer & Higard, 1962) were highly correlated (r = .86), indicating that the EHS
is a reliable and valid tool to assess hypnotizability (Kekecs, Bowers, Johnson, Kendrick, &
Elkins, 2016).

Attitudes toward Hypnosis


Attitudes toward hypnosis were evaluated by scores on the Attitudes Toward Hypnosis
Scale (Spanos et al., 1987). The Attitudes Toward Hypnosis Scale is a 14-item self-report
questionnaire assessing the three factors that comprise the overall construct. These three
factors are positive beliefs about hypnosis, absence of beliefs that hypnosis is associated
with mental instability, and low fear of hypnosis. Items are answered on a 7-point Likert
scale with a score of “1” representing Not at All True, and a score of “7” representing Very
True. This measure was investigated in two samples of undergraduate volunteer partici-
pants and scores demonstrate good reliability as measured by internal consistency
(Cronbach’s α = .81). Factor analyses demonstrated that all three dimensions correlated
significantly with each other and with the total dimension score, in addition to demon-
strating significant relationships with measures of hypnotizability (Spanos et al., 1987). In
the current study, this measure demonstrated good internal consistency (Cronbach’s
α = .85). Additionally, this measure was chosen due to its brief nature and sound
psychometrics.

Expectancy of Hypnotizability
Expectancy of hypnotizability was self-rated by participants on a visual analogue scale with
a length of 100 millimeters entitled “Estimated Hypnotizability.” Scores were determined
by measuring the point that the participant marked on the scale, resulting in a score
between 0 and 100. The literature on visual analogue scale methods of measurement
suggests that scores demonstrate satisfactory test-retest reliability (for example, r = .73), as
well as criterion validity (for example, r = .71 – .78 with an established instrument of the
same construct). Visual analogue scales have been described as “providing a convenient,
easy and rapidly administered measurement strategy that is useful in a wide variety of
clinical and research settings to measure a number of subjective phenomena,” while also
demonstrating satisfactory psychometric properties (Wewers & Lowe, 1990, p. 233).

Results
The final sample comprised 91 undergraduate volunteers. Table 1 provides the frequencies
and percentages associated with gender and race. Of the participants, 20.9% (n = 19) were
male and 78.0% (n = 71) were female. In terms of race, 61.5% of the participants identified
as Caucasian, 11.0% identified as African American, 11.0% identified as Asian, 3.3%
identified as American Indian, and 13.2% identified as being of other ethnic backgrounds
(not specified). The average age of participants was 20 years of age. None of the partici-
pants endorsed having previous experience with hypnosis. Additionally, descriptive sta-
tistics of the measures utilized in the study were calculated and can be found in Table 2.
74 L. L. KOEP ET AL.

Table 1. Demographics of study participants.


Variable N = 91 Percent
Gender
Female 71 78.0
Male 19 20.9
Race
Asian 10 11.0
African American 10 11.0
American Indian 3 3.3
Caucasian 56 61.5
Other 12 13.2

Table 2. Descriptive statistics of study measures.


Minimum Maximum Mean Standard Deviation
Elkins hypnotizability scale 0 11 5.47 2.243
Balanced index of psychological mindedness 39 68 54.33 7.165
Attitude toward hypnosis scale 39 94 67.03 12.863
Expectancy of hypnotizability 0 97 47.86 22.595

Table 3. Correlations between hypnotizability, attitudes toward hypnosis, expectancy, and


psychological mindedness.
Attitudes Toward
Hypnosis Expectancy Psychological Mindedness
Hypnotizability .401** .391** .113
Expectancy - - .175
** p < .01.

Relationships between Hypnotizability, Attitudes toward Hypnosis, Expectancy, and


Psychological Mindedness
Results demonstrated no significant correlation between the EHS and the BIPM (r = .113,
p > .29). A significant, moderate, positive correlation was found between the EHS and the
Attitudes Toward Hypnosis Scale (r = .401, p < .01). In addition, a significant, moderate, positive
correlation was found between the EHS and self-ratings of expectancy of hypnotizability
(r = .391, p < .01). Lastly, no significant correlation was found between scores on the BIPM
and self-ratings of expectancy of hypnotizability (r = .175, p > .10). Pearson product-moment
correlations can be found in Table 3.

Discussion
The objectives of the current study were to understand the relationship between hypnotiz-
ability and psychological mindedness as well as replicate the previous findings regarding
the relationship between hypnotizability, attitudes toward hypnosis, and expectancy of
hypnotizability. The construct of psychological mindedness, defined by Denollet and
Nyklicek (2004) as “the intrinsic motivation to be in touch with one’s inner feelings and
thoughts by monitoring and analyzing them in an adaptive way,” was investigated in
relationship to hypnotizability. Despite findings by McKnight (1980) that revealed
a negative correlation between psychological mindedness and hypnotizability, the present
CORRELATES OF HYPNOTIZABILITY 75

study did not reveal any statistically significant correlations between psychological mind-
edness and hypnotizability or expectancy of hypnotizability.
While the correlation from the current study did not reach statistical significance, by
examining the 95% confidence intervals around the correlation of .113, there is a possible
range from −.09 to +.30. This suggests that while there is a small chance of again finding
a negative correlation between the two constructs as McKnight did, the most likely
correlation is actually small but positive. What can be concluded at this time is that no
significant correlation was found between psychological mindedness and hypnotizability
in this particular population.
It was hypothesized that attitudes toward hypnosis would be positively correlated with
hypnotizability, and the analysis revealed a significant, positive correlation between these two
constructs. This finding is highly consistent with previous literature (Barber & Calverley,
1964; Melei & Hilgard, 1964; Spanos & McPeake, 1975) and indicates that hypnotizability
and attitudes toward hypnosis are positively related to one another. This finding confirms
attitudes toward hypnosis as a consistent positive correlate of hypnotizability.
Furthermore, it was hypothesized that expectancy of hypnotizability would be posi-
tively correlated with hypnotizability. The analysis revealed a significant, positive correla-
tion between these two constructs. This finding is also highly consistent with previous
literature (Braffman & Kirsch, 2001; Silva & Kirsch, 1992) and indicates that hypnotiz-
ability and expectancy of hypnotizability are positively related to one another. This finding
confirms expectancy of hypnosis as another consistent positive correlate of hypnotizabil-
ity. These positive correlations between attitudes toward hypnosis and hypnotizability, as
well as between expectancy and hypnotizability, indicate that an individual’s thoughts and
beliefs about the benefit of hypnosis, including his or her prediction of how much he or
she will be impacted by hypnosis, are indeed related to that individual’s degree of
hypnotizability. As more positive beliefs and expectancies about hypnosis are held, an
individual is increasingly hypnotizable.

Limitations of the Study


One of the primary limitations of the study is that the sample consisted entirely of
undergraduate college students at a private, religiously affiliated university. Due to this
limitation, generalizability of results to a greater population demographic cannot be
assumed because of several demographic variables that are predominant in this population
(age, ethnicity, and level of education).
An additional limitation is that the majority of the study participants were enrolled in
a psychology class at the time in which they participated in the study. This experience of
being actively engaged in learning about psychological principles could have impacted
students’ perceptions of hypnosis as well as their responses to items on the BIPM. It may
be beneficial to investigate these same relationships in individuals who have had no recent
exposure to the study of psychology within an academic setting.
The current study may have also been limited by insufficient power. However, the
preliminary power calculation for the current study determined that, based upon
a predicted moderate effect size between psychological mindedness and hypnotizability
(similar in magnitude to the correlation between empathy and hypnotizability), a sample
size of approximately 70 participants would be sufficient to detect significant correlations.
76 L. L. KOEP ET AL.

Conclusions
While no significant correlation between psychological mindedness and hypnotizability was
observed within the current study’s population, other study populations may yield different
results. Expectancy and attitudes toward hypnosis have been consistently demonstrated to be
positive correlates of hypnotizability, therefore future research aimed at determining what
factors may predict positive expectancy and positive attitudes toward hypnosis may be
a productive avenue to identify which individuals may benefit most from hypnosis.
Finally, discovering ways of increasing self-ratings of expectancy of hypnotizability may be
a beneficial focus for future research as demonstrated by the significant correlation found
between self-ratings of expectancy of hypnotizability and the EHS. Additionally, as demonstrated
by the significant correlation between attitudes toward hypnosis and the EHS, discovering ways
of increasing individuals’ positive attitudes toward hypnosis may also be a productive focus of
research to increase the number of individuals experiencing maximum benefit from hypnosis in
clinical settings.

Disclosure statement
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

References
Appelbaum, S. A. (1973). Psychological-mindedness: Word, concept and essence. International
Journal of Psychoanalysis, 54(1), 35–46.
Austrin, H. R., & Pereira, M. J. (1978). Locus of control as a predictor of hypnotic susceptibility.
American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 20(3), 199–202. doi:10.1080/00029157.1978.10403930
Barber, T. X., & Calverly, D. S. (1964). Empirical evidence for a theory of “hypnotic” behavior:
Effects of pretest instructions on response to primary suggestions. Psychological Record, 14(4),
457–467. doi:10.1007/BF03396019
Benham, G., Woody, E. Z., Wilson, K. S., & Nash, M. R. (2006). Expect the unexpected: Ability,
attitude, and responsiveness to hypnosis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91,
342–350. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.2.342
Braffman, W., & Kirsch, I. (2001). Reaction time as a predictor of imaginative suggestibility and
hypnotizability. Contemporary Hypnosis, 18(3), 107–119. doi:10.1002/(ISSN)1557-0711
Cronin, D. M., Spanos, N. P., & Barber, T. X. (1971). Augmenting hypnotic suggestibility by
providing favorable information about hypnosis. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 13(4),
259–264. doi:10.1080/00029157.1971.10402122
De Groot, H. P., Gwynn, M. I., & Spanos, N. P. (1988). The effects of contextual information and
gender on the prediction of hypnotic susceptibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
54(6), 1049–1053. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1049
Denollet, J., & Nyklicek, I. (2004). Psychological mindedness: A new index to assess a major
emotion-focused coping style. In I. Nyklicek, L. Temoshok, & A. Vingerhoets (Eds.), Emotional
expression and health: Advances in theory, assessment and clinical applications (pp. 185–203).
New York, NY: Brunner- Routledge.
Elkins, G. R. (2014). Hypnotic relaxation therapy: Principles and applications. New York, NY: Springer.
Elkins, G. R. (2017). Handbook of medical and psychological hypnosis: Foundations, applications, and
professional issues. New York, NY: Springer.
Elkins, G. R., Barabasz, A. F., Council, J. R., & Spiegel, D. (2015). Advancing research and practice:
The revised APA division 30 definition of hypnosis. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 57(4),
378–385. doi:10.1080/00029157.2015.1011465
CORRELATES OF HYPNOTIZABILITY 77

Gough, H. G. (1956). California psychological inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Grant, A. M. (2001). Rethinking psychological mindedness: Metacognition, self-reflection, and
insight. Behaviour Change, 18(1), 8–17. doi:10.1375/bech.18.1.8
Hall, J. A. (1992). Psychological-mindedness: A conceptual model. American Journal of
Psychotherapy, 46(1), 131–140. doi:10.1176/appi.psychotherapy.1992.46.1.131
Kekecs, Z., Bowers, J., Johnson, A., Kendrick, C., & Elkins, G. (2016). The Elkins hypnotizability
scale: Assessment of reliability and validity. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Hypnosis, 64(3), 285–304. doi:10.1080/00207144.2016.1171089
Kirsch, I., Silva, C. E., Comey, G., & Reed, S. (1995). A spectral analysis of cognitive and personality
variables in hypnosis: Empirical disconfirmation of the two-factor model of hypnotic responding.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(1), 167–175. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.1.167
London, P., Cooper, L. M., & Johnson, H. J. (1961). Subject characteristics in hypnosis research.
International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 10, 13–21. doi:10.1080/
00207146208415861
McConkey, K. M. (2008). Generations and landscapes of hypnosis: Questions we’ve asked, ques-
tions we should ask. In A. J. Barnier & M. R. Nash (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of hypnosis (pp.
21–52). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
McKnight, R. T. (1980). Prediction of hypnotizability from personality variables of the California
psychological inventory: A multiple regression analysis. Psychological Reports, 47(3), 1319–1322.
doi:10.2466/pr0.1980.47.3f.1319
Melei, J. P., & Hilgard, E. R. (1964). Attitudes toward hypnosis, self-predictions and hypnotic
susceptibility. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 12(2), 99–108.
doi:10.1080/00207146408409265
Montgomery, G. H., Duhamel, K. N., & Redd, W. H. (2000). A metaanalysis of hypnotically induced
analgesia: How effective is hypnosis? International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis,
48(2), 138–153. doi:10.1080/00207140008410045
Moore, M., & Tasso, A. F. (2008). Clinical hypnosis: The empirical evidence. In A. J. Barnier &
M. R. Nash (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of hypnosis (pp. 697–725). New York, NY: Oxford
University Press.
Nyklicek, I., & Denollet, J. (2009). Development and evaluation of the balanced index of psycho-
logical mindedness (BIPM). Psychological Assessment, 21(1), 32–44. doi:10.1037/a0014418
Shor, R. E., & Orne, E. C. (1962). Harvard group scale of hypnotic susceptibility: Form A. Palo Alto,
California, USA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Silva, C. E., & Kirsch, I. (1992). Interpretive sets, expectancy, fantasy proneness, and dissociation as
predictors of hypnotic response. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(5), 847–856.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.63.5.847
Spanos, N. P., Brett, P. J., Menary, E. P., & Cross, W. P. (1987). A measure of attitudes toward
hypnosis: Relationships with absorption and hypnotic susceptibility. American Journal of Clinical
Hypnosis, 30(2), 139–150. doi:10.1080/00029157.1987.10404174
Spanos, N. P., & McPeake, J. D. (1975). Involvement in everyday imaginative activities, attitudes
toward hypnosis, and hypnotic suggestibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31(3),
594–598. doi:10.1037/h0076509
Walker, W. L. (2016). Guidelines for the use of hypnosis: When to use hypnosis and when not to
use. Australian Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 41(1), 41–53.
Weitzenhoffer, A. M., & Higard, E. R. (1962). Stanford hypnotic suggestibility scale: Form C. Palo
Axlto, California, USA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Wewers, M. E., & Lowe, N. K. (1990). A critical review of visual analogue scales in the measurement of
clinical phenomena. Research in Nursing & Health, 13(4), 227–236. doi:10.1002/(ISSN)1098-240X
Wickramasekera, I. E., & Szlyk, J. P. (2003). Could empathy be a predictor of hypnotic ability?
International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 51(4), 390–399. doi:10.1076/
iceh.51.4.390.16413
Yanchar, R. J., & Johnson, H. J. (1981). Absorption and attitude toward hypnosis: A moderator
analysis. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 29(4), 375–382.
doi:10.1080/00207148108409171
78 L. L. KOEP ET AL.

Psychologische Introspektionsfähigkeit, Einstellung gegenüber Hypnose und die


Erwartung als Korrelate der Hypnotisierbarkeit

LAUREN L. KOEP, MATTIE L. BIGGS, JOSHUA R. RHODES, UND GARY R. ELKINS


Abstract : Diese Studie zielt darauf ab zu verstehen, wie Psychologische
Introspektionsfähigkeit, die Einstellung gegenüber Hypnose und die Einschätzung
der Hypnotisierbarkeit mit Hypnotisierbarkeit verbunden sind. 91
Vordiplomstudenten wurden Meßgrößen für die Einstellung gegenüber Hypnose,
Psychologische Introspektionsfähigkeit und die eigene Einschätzung der
Hypnotisierbarkeit gegeben. Die Teilnehmer wurden dann mittels der Elkins
Hypnotizability Scale (EHS) eingeordnet. Die Ergebnisse zeigten eine signifikante
Korrelation zwischen den Teilnehmerpunktzahlen auf der EHS und der Attitudes
Toward Hypnosis Scale (r = 0,401, p < 0,01) und der Selbsteinschätzungen zur eigenen
Hypnotisierbarkeit (r = 0,391, p < 0,01). Die Resultate dieser Studie offenbarten keine
statistisch signifikanten Korrelationen zwischen der Psychologischen
Introspektionsfähigkeit und Hypnotisierbarkeit (r = 0,113, p > 0,29) oder der
Erwartung gegenüber der Hypnotisierbarkeit (r = 0,175, p > 0,10). Weitere
Forschung ist nötig, um das Verhältnis zwischen Psychologischer
Introspektionsfähigkeit und Hypnotisierbarkeit vollständig zu verstehen.
STEPHANIE RIEGEL, MD

INCLINAISON PSYCHOLOGIQUE, ATTITUDES VIS-À-VIS DE L’HYPNOSE ET ATTENTES EN TANT QUE


CORRÉLATS DE LA SUSCEPTIBILITÉ HYPNOTIQUE

Lauren L. Koep, Mattie L. Biggs, Joshua R. Rhodes et Gary R. Elkins


Résumé : Cette étude visait à comprendre en quoi l’inclinaison psychologique, les
attitudes à l’égard de l'hypnose et les attentes en matière de suggestibilité hypnotique
sont liées à la sensibilité hypnotique. Quatre-vingt-onze étudiants de premier cycle ont
reçu des données relatives aux attitudes à l’égard de l'hypnose, à l’inclinaison psycho-
logique et à l'auto-évaluation des attentes en matière de suggestibilité hypnotique. On
leur a ensuite administré l'échelle d'hypnotisabilité d'Elkins (EHS). Les résultats ont
montré une corrélation significative entre les scores des participants sur l'EHS et sur
l'échelle des attitudes à l’égard de l'hypnose (r = 0,401, p < 0,01) et l'auto-évaluation
des attentes en matière de suggestibilité hypnotique (r = 0,391, p < 0,01). Les résultats
de cette étude n'ont révélé aucune corrélation statistiquement significative entre
l’inclinaison psychologique et la suggestibilité hypnotique (r = 0,113, p > 0,29) ou les
attentes en matière de suggestibilité hypnotique (r = 0,175, p > 0,10). Des recherches
supplémentaires sont nécessaires pour bien comprendre la relation entre l'inclinaison
psychologique et la suggestibilité hypnotique.
JOHANNE RAYNAULT
C. Tr. (STIBC)
CORRELATES OF HYPNOTIZABILITY 79

Disposición mental hacia la psicología, actitudes hacia la hipnosis y expectativas como


correlatos de la hipnotizabilidad.

LAUREN L. KOEP, MATTIE L. BIGGS, JOSHUA R. RHODES Y GARY R. ELKINS


Resumen: El objetivo de este estudio era entender cómo se relacionan la disposición
mental hacia la psicología, las actitudes hacia la hipnosis, y las expectativas sobre la
hipnotizabilidad con la habilidad hipnótica. Se evaluó a 91 estudiantes de licenciatura
sobre sus actitudes hacia la hipnosis, su disposición mental hacia la psicología y sus
expectativas sobre su hipnotizabilidad. En seguida se les aplicó la Escala de
Hipnotizabilidad de Elkins (EHS por sus siglas en inglés). Los resultados muestran
una correlación significativa entre las puntuaciones en la EHS y la Escala de Actitudes
hacia la Hipnosis (r = .401, p < .01) y las expectativas autovaloradas sobre la propia
hipnotizabilidad (r = .391, p < .01). Los resultados de este estudio no revelaron
correlaciones estadísticamente significativas entre la disposición mental hacia la
psicología y la hipnotizabilidad.
OMAR SÁNCHEZ-ARMÁSS CAPPELLO
Autonomous University of San Luis Potosi, Mexico

You might also like