You are on page 1of 6

14698749, 2017, 12, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/dmcn.13546 by CAPES, Wiley Online Library on [23/03/2023].

See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
DEVELOPMENTAL MEDICINE & CHILD NEUROLOGY REVIEW

Principles of plasticity in the developing brain


BRYAN KOLB 1,2 | ALLONNA HARKER1 | ROBBIN GIBB 1
1 Canadian Centre for Behavioural Neuroscience, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, AB; 2 Canadian Institute for Advanced Research, Toronto, ON, Canada.
Correspondence to Bryan Kolb, Department of Neuroscience, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Alberta T1K 3M4, Canada. E-mail: kolb@uleth.ca

An early version of this paper was presented at the Early Intervention in Light of the ICF-CY Meeting held in Groningen, the Netherlands, 7–9 April 2016.

The developing brain is especially sensitive to a wide range of experiences, showing a


PUBLICATION DATA remarkable capacity for plastic changes that influence behavioural outcomes throughout the
Accepted for publication 21st June 2017. lifetime. We review the principles that regulate this plasticity in development and consider
Published online 13th September 2017. the factors that modulate the developing brain. These include early sensory, motor, and lan-
guage experience, early stress, caregiver interactions, peer interactions, psychoactive drugs,
ABBREVIATIONS diet, microbiome, and the immune system. Emphasis is given to changes in behaviour, epi-
mPFC Medial prefrontal cortex genetics, and neuronal morphology.
OFC Orbital frontal cortex
SES Socioeconomic status

Studies examining the influence of experience on brain events. Neurons that are active together increase their con-
development originally assumed that it would require large nections, whereas those that are not coincidentally active
changes in experience, such as being raised in darkness, to weaken their connections. Experience-expectant plasticity
influence brain development. We now know that an unex- occurs mostly during early postnatal development. For
pectedly large range of experiences alter brain development instance, a child raised hearing Korean will thus be
and that even fairly innocuous-appearing experiences can exposed to different speech sounds than a child raised in
profoundly affect brain development. We will summarize an English-speaking environment. Early in life, infants can
some of the most extensively studied effects, with an discriminate the speech sounds of all languages, but over
emphasis on studies of laboratory animals, and also include the first year the auditory system begins to change such
effects on functions such as language that are uniquely that the infant becomes expert in discriminating sounds in
human. We will not review the effects of early brain injury, its language environment but loses the ability to discrimi-
which we have reviewed elsewhere.1 nate sounds that are not experienced.2 Experience-depen-
Four special features of developmental plasticity are dent plasticity, a process whereby the connections of
especially important. The first feature is found in the cells ensembles of neurons are modified by experience, begins
lining the subventricular zone of the lateral ventricles and in early postnatal life and continues for a lifetime. One
cells in the hilus of the dentate gyrus. Both regions contain common example is seen in the effects of so-called ‘en-
stem cells that remain active throughout life. The cells in riched experiences’.
the subventricular zone produce both glial and neural pro- A third special feature is the speed at which dendrites,
genitor cells that can migrate into cerebral grey or white and especially dendritic spines, can modify their structure
matter, even in adulthood. In humans, the subventricular to form or delete synapses in response to experience, possi-
zone cells appear mostly quiescent but can become acti- bly in a matter of minutes.
vated, largely in response to cerebral perturbations. Stem A fourth special feature is the presence of critical peri-
cells in the dentate gyrus generate new neurons at a slow ods, especially for experience-expectant plasticity. One of
but steady pace throughout life in both rodents and the best studied models was first described by Wiesel &
humans, although there is a decline with ageing. The func- Hubel3 in which they showed that if one eye of a kitten is
tional role of these cells is not totally understood, but they kept closed after birth, the open eye expands its territory
do integrate with the existing neurons and likely play a at the expense of the closed eye. When the closed eye is
role in the formation of new memories. eventually opened after a few months, its vision is compro-
A second special feature is that three types of plasticity mised, resulting in an enduring loss of spatial vision (am-
can be distinguished in the developing brain: experience- blyopia). Recent work has shown that the critical period
independent, experience-expectant, and experience-depen- results from an appropriate balance of excitatory and inhi-
dent. Experience-independent plasticity results from the bitory inputs (Fig. 1). The maturation of inhibitory c-ami-
fact that the genome generates a rough approximation of nobutryic acidergic circuits underlies the timing of onset
connectivity that is modified by both internal and external of the critical periods, which vary across brain regions.

1218 DOI: 10.1111/dmcn.13546 © 2017 Mac Keith Press


14698749, 2017, 12, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/dmcn.13546 by CAPES, Wiley Online Library on [23/03/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
The critical period closes as molecular brakes dampen What this paper adds
plasticity, and alter the balance between excitation and • A discussion of the surprising range of factors influencing brain
inhibition (for a review, see Takesian & Hensch4). A key development
point is that it is possible to reopen the critical period by • Life experiences interact resulting in a phenomenon called metaplasticity
manipulating this balance chemically.
Relatively minor modifications of visual experience can
SENSORY AND MOTOR EXPERIENCES also have the significant effects. Prusky et al.7 placed rats in
The simplest way to manipulate developmental sensory/ a virtual optokinetic system in which the animals stood on
motor experiences is either to have extreme deprivation, a small platform and vertical lines of differing spatial fre-
such as raising animals in darkness, or to provide quasi- quency moved past the animal. When the rats (or humans)
‘enriched’ environments where there is enhanced social, view the moving lines it is impossible to avoid tracking the
cognitive, sensory, and/or motor experience relative to ani- lines. The rats were exposed to the moving lines daily for 2
mals living in small social groups in laboratory cages. Rais- weeks beginning at eye opening (approximately postnatal
ing animals in deprived conditions hinders development day 15). Measures of visual acuity in adulthood showed a
and may lead to the permanent loss of function. Monkeys, chronic 25% enhancement in visual acuity relative to con-
cats, or rats raised in the light but without patterned visual trol animals. Thus, simply being exposed to enhanced visual
stimulation show poor visual guidance of behaviour, even input was sufficient to modify visual development.
after prolonged periods of recovery with patterned visual Although it is generally assumed that the effects of com-
input.5 The reduced vision is associated with reduced den- plex housing result from the animals physically interacting
dritic branching and length in both visual and posterior with the environment, a series of studies by our group have
cingulate cortices. suggested that the effects of complex housing can cross
Rearing animals in enriched environments has rather dif- generations. For example, when male or female rats were
ferent effects. In our studies on the effects of raising wean- housed in complex environments for 28 days prior to being
ling rats in large social groups (6–8 same-sex animals) in mated with animals housed in standard laboratory housing,
complex environments leads to increased brain weight in there were similar decreases in global methylation (thus,
the order of 7% to 10%, which is associated with increases increased gene expression) in the offspring of both male-
in the number and density of blood vessels, neuron soma and female-enriched parents, as well as changes in beha-
size, dendritic elements, synapses, gene expression, and glia vioural development.8
throughout the neocortex, hippocampus, and striatum. The effects of preconception enriched housing is also
There is also increased production of a variety of growth seen in the response of offspring to perinatal cerebral
factors known to alter brain development.6 And, in con- injury. When the offspring of females housed in complex
trast to the effects of deprivation where there is reduced environments during gestation were given perinatal medial
function, complex housing is generally associated with prefrontal lesions they showed remarkably better beha-
increased motor, sensory, and cognitive functions. vioural outcomes relative to those without the gestational

E–I balance
(PV circuits)

Molecular
brakes
Cortical maturation

PSA-NCAM

Critical period
Off

Age
Off

On

e.g. GABA e.g. Perineuronal Nets

Figure 1: Proposed mechanism for turning on and off critical periods. Plasticity is blocked initially by factors such as polysialic acid (PSA) on neural
cell adhesion molecule (NCAM), thus limiting the parvalbumin system (PV). Critical period closes as molecular brakes (e.g. perineuronal nets) emerge to
dampen plasticity (adapted from Takesian and Hensch).4 E-I balance, balance between excitation and inhibition; GABA, c-aminobutyric acid. [Colour
figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com].

Review 1219
14698749, 2017, 12, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/dmcn.13546 by CAPES, Wiley Online Library on [23/03/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
housing experience. The gestational experience increased and the prefrontal cortex. Studies of laboratory rodents
cortical thickness, thalamic volume, and spine density in have shown a decrease in the dendritic arborization and
cortical pyramidal neurons in both the control and lesion spine density in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) but
animals.9 an increase in these measures in orbital frontal cortex
(OFC). There are also changes in mRNA gene expression
LANGUAGE, COGNITIVE EXPERIENCE, AND POVERTY in the mPFC, OFC, and hippocampus, with each region
Infants can discriminate the speech sounds of all languages showing a different pattern of altered gene expression.
over the first year, but the auditory system begins to Early stress predisposes individuals to a range of mal-
change such that the infant becomes expert in discriminat- adaptive behaviours and psychopathologies, including
ing sounds in its language environment but loses the ability schizophrenia, depression, and drug addiction (e.g. Anda
to discriminate sounds that are not experienced. Learning et al.14). Perinatal stress in laboratory rodents produces a
more than one language in development adds another range of behavioural abnormalities, including an elevated
dimension, and especially in learning how to switch rou- and prolonged stress response, increased anxiety, impaired
tinely between languages. This mental switching has a sig- learning and memory, deficits in attention, altered explora-
nificant impact on cognitive abilities, and especially in tory behaviour, altered social and play behaviour, and
enhancing attentional and executive functions, and in increased preference for alcohol. These effects vary both
increasing cognitive reserve in ageing, presumably by some with precise timing and the intensity.
type of plastic change in the frontal lobe.10 Evidence has accumulated to show that preconception
Although virtually all children learn a language, there stress in either parent may influence the offspring by altering
are large differences in rate of vocabulary acquisition. In a behaviour, brain morphology, and epigenetics in the adult
powerful study, Hart and Risley followed children for 2 offspring.13 For example, in our studies of the effects of pre-
and a half years (from the age of 7–9mo to 36mo) by conception paternal stress the offspring showed increased
observing the families at home for 1 hour each month.11 anxiety and impairment in a skilled reaching task, even with
Children could be grouped into those with large vocabu- the stress being over a single sperm generation cycle. Epige-
laries at 3 years of age (~1200 words) versus those with netic analysis showed significantly decreased methylation in
smaller vocabularies (~400–600 words). This difference was frontal cortex (mPFC and adjacent motor cortex) in female
related to the number of words that the children had been but not male offspring, and increased methylation in the
exposed to in the home, which was directly related to hippocampus. Furthermore, there were abnormalities in
socioeconomic status (SES). Thus, over 1 year, children of dendritic complexity and spine density that changed signifi-
high SES would have been exposed to about 11 million cantly across development. A parallel study showed that
words and children of low SES to about 3.2 million words. preconception maternal stress led to altered dendritic
By 4 years of age the average child of lower SES would complexity and spine density in two mPFC regions.13
have been exposed to about 30 million fewer words than Studies on the effects of gestational stress have generally
the child of higher SES. When the children were measured found that the offspring exhibit increased anxiety, altered
again at 9 to 10 years, the SES-related difference actually play behaviour, impaired skilled reaching, and slower spa-
grew larger, suggesting that school had a negligible effect tial learning.13 Postmortem analyses reveal decreases in
in erasing this deficit. overall brain, but not body, weight, and decreased spine
The SES-related difference in cognitive abilities is density in OFC, but increased spine density in mPFC cor-
related to developmental differences in cerebral cortex. related with changes in gene expression in these regions.
Noble et al.12 examined the relationship between SES and Although the anatomical pattern is different both from
cortical surface area in over 1000 participants between the preconception stress, the behavioural changes are similar.
age of 3 years and 20 years. Lower family income, inde- The effects of gestational stress in humans are being
pendent of ethnicity or sex, was associated with decreased studied in a prospective study on the effects of the Quebec,
cortical surface area in widespread regions of frontal, tem- Canada ice storm of 1998. The storm hit in midwinter and
poral, and parietal cortex, and this was correlated with power was knocked out for up to 6 weeks, affecting
poorer performance on tests of attention, memory, vocabu- approximately 2 million people, during the coldest month
lary, and reading. Thus, lower SES is associated with smal- of the year. Many females were pregnant at different stages
ler cortical surface area and poorer test outcomes. of gestation and had varying levels of hardship, ranging
from severe to none. The offspring of pregnant females
PRENATAL AND POSTNATAL STRESS have now been studied from the age of 2 years to 11 years
The effects of stress are not only dependent upon the 6 months. To date, the children of stressed mothers show
hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis but involve a two-way cognitive, linguistic, motor, and play abnormalities, and at
communication between the brain and the cardiovascular, 11 years 6 months there are dramatic differences in gene
immune, and other systems via neural and endocrine expression (e.g. Cao-Le et al.15).
mechanisms (for an extensive review of early stress effects, Postnatal stress also changes brain development. Short
see Kolb et al.13). One effect of stress in adults is a change periods of maternal separation alter the hypothalamic–pitu-
in the structure of neurons in the hippocampus, amygdala, itary–adrenal axis, making it more efficient allowing better

1220 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2017, 59: 1218–1223


14698749, 2017, 12, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/dmcn.13546 by CAPES, Wiley Online Library on [23/03/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
recovery from stress.13 But there is a limit to the intensity the attacking rat to nuzzle the nape of the defending rat,
of maternal separation that is beneficial. Longer periods who, in turn, tries to avoid the nuzzle. The roles of defen-
(e.g. 3 hours daily) leads to increased anxiety in adult male der and attacker are reciprocal such that the rats take turns
offspring, disrupted play behaviour in both sexes, decreased in each role and breaking this rule tends to end the play
brain weight, and increased dendritic complexity and spine bout. Kittens have a similar play pattern. Because adult rats
density in both mPFC and OFC.13 Thus, it appears that do not play with juveniles it is possible to house individual
brief periods of maternal separation are beneficial to later juveniles with one or more adults, which provides social
adult behaviour, whereas more extended periods of separa- interactions, but the younger animals are deprived of play.
tion have the opposite effect. It is likely that these con- In contrast, juveniles can be housed with one or more juve-
trasting effects are related to differential effects on gene niles, which results in lots of play and social interaction.
expression. Play behaviour acts to prune pyramidal neurons in
In sum, preconceptual, gestational, and postnatal stress mPFC,19 which allows the neurons to be more plastic later
all change behaviour, epigenetics, and neuronal morphol- in life. Many developmental experiences, including gesta-
ogy, although in different ways, and very differently than tional stress, tactile stimulation, and psychoactive drugs,
stress in adulthood. alter various aspects of play behaviour, which presumably
alters mPFC development.
PARENT–CHILD RELATIONSHIPS
Young mammals are dependent upon their parents and PSYCHOACTIVE DRUGS
must learn to identify, remember, and prefer their care- All psychoactive drugs, including prescription drugs,
givers. Modifications of these relationships can significantly change the structure of neurons, especially in prefrontal
alter brain development and leave effects that remain into cortex and nucleus accumbens.20 Less is known about the
adulthood. For example, extensive studies on rats have effects of drug exposure during development, although it
shown that the time spent in contact between the mother has long been known that early exposure to alcohol is dele-
and pups, including both the amount of maternal licking terious to brain development. As many as 20% of North
and grooming correlate with a variety of somatic and beha- American females smoke for at least part of their preg-
vioural differences, and especially modify the development nancy and nicotine easily crosses the blood–brain barrier.
of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis. These changes Prenatal nicotine exposure increases the risk for various
are correlated with changes in gene expression in the off- neurodevelopmental disorders, including hyperactivity,
spring.16 Other studies have shown changes related to learning and memory deficits, and conduct disorders (e.g.
maternal–infant interactions in the mPFC, OFC, hypotha- Button et al.21). Low doses of nicotine administered
lamus, and amygdala. throughout pregnancy in rats changes the structure of neu-
The effects of reduced parent–child interactions in rons in prefrontal cortex, nucleus accumbens, and parietal
humans have been studied in children adopted from insti- cortex of adult offspring. As with the effects of nicotine
tutions and related to the age at adoption. For example, exposure in adulthood, prenatal nicotine exposure is associ-
12- to 14-year-old children who were adopted from insti- ated with epigenetic changes related to dendritic complex-
tutional care at a mean age of 12 months had reduced ity.22
grey-matter volumes and especially reduced prefrontal vol- Although less well studied than nicotine, there is grow-
ume, which was consistent with previous studies showing ing evidence that prenatal exposure to a wide range of
reduced prefrontal cortical thickness (e.g. Hodel et al.17). drugs, including amphetamine, fluoxetine, valproic acid,
Extensive studies of Romanian orphans adopted to various morphine, and marijuana alter behaviour, neuronal struc-
Western countries have shown similar results, although ture, and epigenetics in the adult offspring. Preliminary
one additional finding is that adoption after about 18 non-invasive imaging studies in children have shown paral-
months is associated with very poor behavioural and neu- lel results in which dopamine-rich cortical (e.g. prefrontal
rological outcomes, even after over 20 years of living in cortex) and subcortical (e.g. basal ganglia) reduction in
good and stable families. These children have smaller- grey matter volume.23 One difficulty with many of the ani-
than-normal brains, reduced cognitive and social functions, mal studies is that the doses are often much larger than
and abnormal brain electrical activity (e.g. Johnson typically experienced by pregnant human females, which
et al.18). The Romanian children are more affected than in may exaggerate the dangers of drug exposure.
the US study, and this is likely related to both the severity
of caregiver deprivation and the duration of institutional- DIET
ization in the Romanian children. Most research on nutrients in development have focused
on the effects of nutrient deficiencies, especially related to
PEER RELATIONSHIPS protein-energy, iron, zinc, copper, and choline. Such nutri-
Although the details differ, all mammals play and the tional deficiencies can have global effects on the developing
opportunity to play is rewarding. Play can be seen as a brain or brain circuit-specific effects, depending upon the
form of problem-solving and many forms of play have precise timing of the nutrient deficit. A more intriguing
innate rules. For example, when rats play the goal is for question is whether brain plasticity might be enhanced by

Review 1221
14698749, 2017, 12, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/dmcn.13546 by CAPES, Wiley Online Library on [23/03/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
vitamin and/or mineral supplements, and especially a com- Furthermore, there is evidence that immune system mole-
bination of nutrients that would work synergistically to cules, such as interleukin-17, cross the placenta and
enhance metabolic activity, and ultimately brain function- increase interleukin-17 receptor expression in the fetal
ing. One promising product is EmpowerPlus. This product brain, which, in turn, is suggested to be a risk factor in aut-
is a blend of 36 vitamins, minerals, and antioxidants, and ism. Finally, it appears that the Zika virus infects human
includes a proprietary blend of herbal supplements such as neural progenitor cells, leading to increased cell death and
gingko biloba and the amino-acid precursors for neuro- gene transcriptional dysregulation.
transmitters, choline, phenylalanine, glutamine, and
methionine. This product has been reported to improve CONCLUSIONS
mood and behaviour in children, and been shown to The developing brain is responsive to a wide range of fac-
decrease anger, activity levels, and social withdrawal in aut- tors that modulate its development beginning with precon-
ism while also increasing spontaneity.24 Rodent studies ception experiences of the parents, gestational experiences,
have shown that using this supplement during development and postnatal experiences. We have considered these fac-
leads to enhanced motor and cognitive functions and tors as though they are independent singular events, but as
increased dendritic arbour in mPFC. The mechanism of we go through life experiences interact to alter both beha-
dietary effects on neuronal structure may be epigenetic. viour and brain, a process often referred to as meta-plasti-
Dominguez-Salaz et al.25 studied gene methylation in the city. We have only just begun to understand how different
blood of infants conceived either in the Gambian dry or factors might interact with one another or how the effects
rainy season. The maternal diets are dramatically different of negative factors such as severe stress might be amelio-
in the two seasons and so was the pattern of gene methyla- rated by experiences such as tactile stimulation. We have
tion. focused here on changes in behaviour, neuronal morphol-
ogy, and epigenetics, but we certainly recognize that plas-
GUT BACTERIA tic changes in brain organization can be studied at many
Given that the microbiome interacts with the brain, Dinan other levels in both humans and laboratory animals.
et al.26 proposed that the use of bacteria to alter the micro- Finally, in our discussion we have given equal weight to
biome could be a novel class of psychotropic, which are each of the factors, but there are likely significant differ-
called psychobiotics. The idea is that manipulation of bac- ences in the magnitude of effects. The effects of early
teria in the gut could modify brain plasticity, a conclusion stress and psychoactive drugs are the most studied nega-
supported by several studies showing that normal gut tive effects and likely the most powerful. On the positive
microbiota can affect brain and behavioural development. side, early experiences such as tactile stimulation would
For example, manipulation of gut bacteria in newborn appear to be very influential and have the ability to reverse
mice influenced motor and anxiety behaviours.27 These some of the negative effects of stress and perhaps psy-
behavioural changes were correlated with changes in the choactive drugs.
turnover of noradrenaline, dopamine, and serotonin in the
striatum, as well as changes in the production of synaptic- A CK N O W L E D G E M E N T S
related proteins in cortex and striatum. We wish to thank both Natural Sciences and Engineering
Research Council of Canada and the Canadian Institute for
IMMUNE SYSTEM Advanced Research for their long-term support for the studies
Many proteins in the immune system are expressed in the related to our work discussed in this review. We also thank Cathy
developing brain and some appear essential for the develop- Carroll, Grazyna Gorny, Celeste Halliwell, Yilin Li, Fraser Mus-
ment and modifications of synapses. Although there is little tard, Richelle Mychasiuk, Arif Muhammad, and Kehe Xie for
direct evidence that the immune system can compromise their many contributions to the studies. An early version of this
brain development and plasticity, epidemiological evidence paper was presented at the Groningen Early Intervention Meet-
implicates maternal infection as risk factors in various ing, which was held in April 2016. The authors have stated that
neurodevelopmental disorders, including autism, attention- they had no interests which might be perceived as posing a con-
deficit–hyperactivity disorder, and schizophrenia.28 flict or bias.

REFERENCES
1. Kolb B, Mychasiuk R, Muhammad A, Gibb R. Brain 4. Takesian AE, Hensch TK. Balancing plasticity/stability 6. Kolb B, Forgie M, Gibb R, Gorny G, Rowntree S. Age,
plasticity in the developing brain. Prog Brain Res 2013; across brain development. Prog Brain Res 2013; 207: 3– experience, and the changing brain. Neurosci Biobehav
207: 35–64. 34. Rev 1998; 22: 143–59.
2. Werker JF, Hensch TK. Critical periods in speech percep- 5. Riesen AH, Zilbert DE. Behavioral consequences of 7. Prusky GT, Silver BD, Tschetter WW, Alam NM,
tion: new directions. Annu Rev Psychol 2015; 66: 173–96. variations in early sensory environments. In: Riesen Douglas RM. Experience-dependent plasticity from eye
3. Wiesel TN, Hubel DH. Comparison of the effects of AH, editor. The developmental neuropsychology of opening enables lasting, visual cortex-dpendent
unilateral and bilateral eye closure on cortical unit sensory deprivation. New York: Academic Press, 1975: enhancement of motion vision. J Neurosci 2008; 28:
responses in kittens. J Neurophysiol 1965; 28: 1029–40. 211–52. 9817–27.

1222 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2017, 59: 1218–1223


14698749, 2017, 12, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/dmcn.13546 by CAPES, Wiley Online Library on [23/03/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
8. Mychasiuk R, Zahir S, Schmold N, Ilnystskyy S, Koval- 15. Cao-Lei L, Elgbeili G, Massart R, Laplante DP, Szyf 21. Button T, Maughan B, McGuffin P. The relationship of
chuk O, Gibb R. Parental enrichment and offspring M, King S. Pregnant women’s cognitive appraisal of a maternal smoking to psychological problems in the off-
development: modifications to brain, behavior and the natural disaster affects DNA methylation in their chil- spring. Early Hum Dev 2007; 83: 727–32.
epigenome. Behav Brain Res 2012; 228: 294–8. dren 13 years later: Project Ice Storm. Transl Psychiatry 22. Jung Y, Hsieh LS, Lee AM, et al. An epigenetic mecha-
9. Gibb R, Gonzalez C, Kolb B. Prenatal enrichment and 2015; 5: e515. nism mediates developmental nicotine effects on neu-
recovery from perinatal cortical damage: effects of 16. Cameron N, Champagne F, Carine P, Fish E, Ozaki- ronal structure and behavior. Nat Neurosci 2016; 19:
maternal complex housing. Front Behav Neurosci 2014; 8: Kurodoa K, Meaney M. The programming of individual 905–14.
223. differences in defensive responses and reproductive 23. Derauf C, Kekatpure M, Neyzi N, Lester B, Kosofsky
10. Wiseheart M, Viswanathan M, Bialystok E. Flexibility in strategies in a rat through variations in maternal care. B. Neuroimaging of children following prenatal drug
task switching by monolinguals and bilinguals. Biling Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2005; 29: 843–65. exposure. Semin Cell Dev Biol 2009; 20: 441–54.
(Camb Engl) 2016; 19: 141–6. 17. Hodel AS, Hunt RH, Cowell RA, Van den Heuvel SE, 24. Mehl-Madrona L, Leung B, Kennedy C, Paul S, Kaplan
11. Hart B, Risley T. Meaningful differences in the everyday Gunnar MR, Thomas KM. Duration of early adversity BJ. Micronutrients versus standard medication manage-
experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: and structural brain development in post-institutiona- ment in autism: a naturalistic case-control study. J Child
Brookes, 1995. lized adolescents. NeuroImage 2015; 105: 112–9. Adolesc Psychopharmacol 2010; 20: 95–103.
12. Noble KG, Houston SM, Brito NH, et al. Family 18. Johnson DE, Guthrie D, Smyke AT, et al. Growth and 25. Dominguez-Salas P, Moore SE, Baker MS, et al. Mater-
income, parental education and brain structure in chil- associations between auxology, caregiving environment, nal nutrition at conception modulates DNA methylation
dren and adolescents. Nat Neurosci 2015; 18: 773–8. and cognition in socially deprived Romanian children of human metastable epialles. Nat Commun 2014; 5: 3746.
13. Kolb B, Harker L, de Melo S, Gibb R. Stress and pre- randomized to foster vs ongoing institutionalized care. 26. Dinan TG, Stanton C, Cryan JF. Psychobiotics: a novel
frontal cortical plasticity in the developing brain. Cogn Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2010; 164: 507–16. class of psychotropic. Biol Psychiat 2013; 74: 720–6.
Dev 2017; 42: 15–26. 19. Bell HC, Pellis SM, Kolb B. Juvenile peer play experi- 27. Diaz Heijtz R, Wang S, Anuar F, et al. Normal gut
14. Anda RF, Felitti VJ, Bremmer JD, et al. The enduring ence and the development of the orbitofrontal and med- microbiota modulates brain development and behavior.
effects of abuse and related adverse experiences in child- ial prefrontal cortex. Behav Brain Res 2010; 207: 7–13. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2011; 108: 3047–52.
hood. A convergence of evidence from neurobiology and 20. Robinson TE, Kolb B. Structural plasticity associated 28. Estes ML, McAllister AK. Maternal immune activation:
epidemiology. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2006; with exposure to drugs of abuse. Neuropharmacology implications for neuropsychiatric disorders. Science 2016;
256: 174–86. 2004; 47: 33–46. 353: 772–7.

Review 1223

You might also like