You are on page 1of 15

Accelerat ing t he world's research.

Student motivation to learn English


as a foreign language
Kassim Shaaban

Foreign Language Annals

Cite this paper Downloaded from Academia.edu 

Get the citation in MLA, APA, or Chicago styles

Related papers Download a PDF Pack of t he best relat ed papers 

Mot ivat ion and gender in t he Japanese EFL classroom


Pet er Gobel

T he Effect of Mot ivat ion


Yaşar E . Kürüm

Saudi Undergraduat es' Mot ivat ional Orient at ions t owards English Language Learning along Gender an…
Muhammad U Farooq, Prof. Dr. Choudhary Z A H I D Javid
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/229732738

Student Motivation to Learn English as a


Foreign Language

Article in Foreign Language Annals · December 2008


DOI: 10.1111/j.1944-9720.2000.tb00932.x

CITATIONS READS

30 4,307

2 authors:

Kassim A. Shaaban Ghazi Ghaith


American University of Beirut American University of Beirut
17 PUBLICATIONS 223 CITATIONS 29 PUBLICATIONS 621 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Kassim A. Shaaban on 20 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. All in-text references underlined in blue are added to the original document
and are linked to publications on ResearchGate, letting you access and read them immediately.
632 zyx
zyxw
zyxwvutsrqpon NOVE MR E IUD EC E MR E R 2000

Student Motivation to Learn


English as a Foreign Language
Kassim A. Shaaban
American University of Beirut
Ghazi Ghaith
American Univevsity of Beirut zyxwvu
Abstract: This study examined the motivation of 180 university-bound Lebanese students to
learn English as aforeign language (EFL). Data were gathered through administering a modified
version of the motivation scale developed by Wen (1997). Thefindings revealed that integrative

zy
motivation, effort, valence, expectancy, and self-estimation of ability were internally related deter-
minants of motivation for learning EFL. Instrumental motivation was found to be related to inte-
grative motivation and valence only. The findings also revealed that female students were more
motivated than their male counterparts. Similarly, level 11 proficiency students were more moti-
vated than were level III students. Howevel; thefindings did not show a significant effect on moti-
vation related to either students’first foreign language or university majol:

Introduction
The role of learners’ motivation in the acquisition of languages other than their own has been a
subject of controversy in applied linguistics. Typically, second language acquisition (SLA) theo-
rists tend to group motivation together with various aspects of personality and emotion as “affec-
tive” factors that play a role in language acquisition (Dulay et al. 1982; Stern 1983; Ellis 1985).
One corollary of this orientation is a focus on order of acquisition, developmental sequences,
and the role of biologically specified determinants of acquisition (universal grammar) over
which learners have no control.
However, the SLA theorists’ understanding of the role of motivation is limited, and our
understanding of motivation is likely to change, given widespread calls for helping students
become autonomous learners, that is, students who are involved in and responsible for their own
learning. Furthermore, many aspects of language learning are subject to learners’ active choices,
such as taking a course or not, communicating with native speakers, allocating attention, and so
forth. These considerations, coupled with the fact that learners’ cognitive strategies constitute a
strong determinant of acquisition (O’Malley et al. 1985), suggest that it is important to re-exam-
ine the role of motivation in the light of contemporary conceptualizations of language learning.
In their classic study of the role of motivation in second language acquisition, Gardner and
Lambert (1959) identified two kinds of motivation: integrative and instrumental. These
researchers maintained that integrative motivation signifies the learners’ desire to identify with
the target culture, whereas instrumental motivation refers to the need to fulfill a practical objec-
tive - such as obtaining employment. Later on, Gardner and Lambert (1972) and Gardner
(1980, 1985, 1988) proposed a socioeducational model of motivation that emphasized the influ-

Kassim A. Shaaban (Ph.D., University of Texas) is Associate Professor ofEnglish at the American
University of Beirut, Beirut, Lebanon.
Ghazi Ghaith (Ph.D., Indiana University) is Associate Professor of Language Education at the
American University of Beirut, Beirut, Lebanon.
zyxwvut
zyxwvutsrqp
zyxwvutsr
zyxwvutsrqp
FOKElGN LANGUAGE ANNALS * VOI,. 33, NO.6 633

ence of cultural beliefs on the development of the integra- instrumental motivation subsystems; (2) learner level,
tive motive and suggested the presence of a positive causal which comprises need for achievement and self-confi-
link between integrative motivation and second language dence (as determined by language use anxiety, perceived
achievement, as opposed to learning a second language for second language competence, causal attributions, and
instrumental purposes only where no such link exists. A self-efficacy); and (c) learning situation level, which
basic premise in Gardner’s model is that successful learners includes course-specific, teacher-specific, and group-spe-
are active and integratively motivated and that integrative cific components. Similarly, Wen (1997) incorporated
motivation is independent of aptitude. In addition, expectancy-value theories in his investigation of the
Gardner et al. (1979) and Gardner (1985) developed the motivation of Asian and Asian-American students learn-
Attitudehlotivation Test Battery (AMTB), thus “setting ing Chinese at American universities. Wen identified four
high research standards and bringing L2 motivation motivational factors that had a bearing on the enrollment
research to maturity” (Dornyei 1994, p. 277). and continuation of students in Chinese language classes;
Research evidence synthesized by Oller (1981), these were motivation of instrumentality, intrinsic moti-
Gardner (1985), and Au (1988) yielded mixed results vation, expected learning strategies and efforts, and pas-
regarding some key assumptions in Gardner’s model. This sivity towards requirements.
is especially so with respect to the integrative motive and In the present study, we used Gardner and Lambert’s
causality hypothesis. These studies did not clearly sup- conceptualization of integrative and instrumental motiva-
port the superiority of integrative motivation in different tions as well as expectancy-value theories (suggested by
contexts; Kruidenier and Clement (1986) and Belmechri Wen, 1997) to investigate the motivation of university-
and Hummel (1998) even maintain that instrumental bound Lebanese students to learn EFL. The expectancy-
motivation is generally more prominent in EFL contexts value theory was originally proposed by Lewin (1951).
than integrative motivation. These findings have led to This theory was further explicated by Vroom (1964),who
the conclusion that the relationship between motivation, postulated that the effort exerted toward any action is
as conceptualized by Gardner, and language learning is determined by the valence and expectancy that the action
an “unstable non-linear function that varies greatly across would lead to the desired outcomes. Vroom (1964)
individuals, contexts, and learning tasks” (Oller 1981, p. defined valence as “an affective orientation toward partic-
15). Consequently, researchers began to deal with moti- ular outcomes” (p. 14), and Lewin referred to it as “the
vation in the second language classroom from the per- psychological value of particular outcomes” (cited in
spective of motivation structures and a broad yet context- Wen 1997, p. 236). Expectancy is defined as the “the

zyxwvuts
specific approach (Crookes and Schmidt 1991; Dornyei probability of attaining successful performance.” (Oxford
1990; Wen 1997). and Shearin 1994, p. 21). Thus, according to the
Dornyei (1990), in a study of adult learners of English expectancy value theories, learners’ motivation to acquire
in Hungary, postulated a motivational construct “consist- a second language is determined by their effort, percep-
ing of (1) an Instrumental Motivational Subsystem, (2) an tion of the degree of attractiveness of the goals (valence),
Integrative Motivational Subsystem, ( 3 ) Need for perception of the probability of attaining the goals
Achievement, and (4) Attribution about Past Failures” (p. (expectancy), and appraisal of their ability to achieve the
45). The second and third components were seen as essen- goals.
tial for an intermediate level of proficiency in the target The present study addressed the following questions.
language, whereas the desire for going further was associ- 1. To what extent are the determinants of learners’ moti-
ated with integrative motivation. Furthermore, Dornyei vation to acquire a foreign language (integrative moti-
identified three loosely related aspects of the integrative vation, instrumental motivation, effort, valence,
motivational subsystem, namely, (1) interest in foreign lan- expectancy, ability) internally related?
guages, cultures, and people; (2) desire to broaden one’s 2. Is there a difference between male and female learners
view and avoid provincialism; and ( 3 ) desire for new stim- in their motivation to learn EFL?
uli and challenges. Along similar lines, researchers such as 3. Is there a difference among students with different lev-
Crookes and Schmidt (1991) and Oxford and Shearin els of proficiency in their motivation to learn EFL?
(1994) questioned the validity of existing motivational 4. Is there a difference in the motivation to learn English
models and suggested that the notion of second language between students whose first foreign language is
learning motivation should be broadened to include the French and those whose first foreign language is
general psychology theories of integrative motivation. English?
In 1994, Dornyei elaborated on his earlier model of 5. Is there a difference among students in various majors
motivation and suggested that it functions at three levels: at the university (Arts, Sciences, Engineering, and
(1) language level, which comprises integrative and other fields) in their motivation to learn English?
634 zy
zyxwvutsrqpon NOVEMRER/DECEMRER 2000

The first question was prompted by our interest in first foreign language and those who studied French as a
investigating the degree to which the motivational con- first foreign language.
structs identified in the literature (integrative and instru- H04. There is no significant difference in motivation
mental motivations, effort, valence, expectancy, ability) are to learn EFL among students with various university

zyxw
internally related. The second purpose was to determine majors.
the specific motivational constructs on which students
may differ across the variables of gender, proficiency levels,
first foreign language, and university major.
Method
Our review of the literature yielded mixed results Participants
regarding the effect of gender on learners’ motivation to One-hundred-eighty students (n = 180) enrolled in the
learn a language other than their own. Several studies have University Orientation Program (UOP), which offers
reported that females are more motivated and determined intensive English classes at the American University of
to do academic work than males (e.g., Karsenti and Beirut (AUB), participated in the study. This program is
Thibert 1994; Zammit 1993; Zughoul and Taminian intended to help upgrade students’ English language profi-
1984). Conversely, Coleman (1995) maintained that the ciency so that they may pursue their studies in the majors
gender-based differences in motivation are rather margin- that they have been accepted into at the University, where
al, and Suleiman (1993) reported negative motivation atti- English is the medium of instruction. The acceptance of
tudes to studymg English as a foreign language among students into various majors at the University is normally
female Arab students. Furthermore, Sung and Padilla determined on the basis of their scholastic record and their
(1998) concluded that there is need for further research to scores on SAT I and SAT 11. However, those students who
determine gender differences in motivation. do not get the required score on English language admis-
Similarly, the literature includes conflicting evidence sion tests (TOEFL, SAT Verbal, or AUB-EN) are asked to
with regard to the effect of proficiency level on motivation join UOP
to learn a foreign language. For example, Coleman (1995) All the participants were native speakers of Arabic.
reported a slight but measurable relationship between inte- Of these, 41 students (23.8%) were enrolled in level I1
grative motivation and higher levels of foreign language (Intermediate) and the remaining 139 (77.2%) were in
proficiency These findings are corroborated by those of Level I11 (High Intermediate). There were 108 males
Boykin and Tmngamphai (1987), who maintained that (60%) and 72 females (40%). Ninety-six students
motivation and language proficiency are positively corre- (53.3%) had studied in French-medium schools where
lated. Moreover, Sung and Padilla (1998) reported that French was the first foreign language and English was
“advanced level students [studying Asian languages] taught as a second foreign language to make it possible
scored significantly higher in instrumentauintegrative for them to attend English-medium universities. The
motivation to learn a foreign language than did beginning- remaining 82 students (45.6%) had studied in English-
level students” (p. 215). But can these findings be general- medium schools where English was the first foreign lan-
ized to university-bound students studymg EFL? This guage; only very few of these schools offer a second for-
question seems pertinent, especially in light of Tweles’ eign language, normally French. Although students in
(1995) report that students’ level of motivation was not the latter group had studied English for over 10 years,
shown to correlate highly with proficiency their proficiency remained low. This low proficiency may
The study also attempted to provide empirical evi- be explained by the fact that they came from lower-mid-
dence regarding the intuitive assumptions in the Lebanese dle-class families where the parents did not necessarily
multilingual context that students who study French as a speak any foreign language fluently; moreover, their
first foreign language and those who major in the “fields of schools tend to stress “science” subjects at the expense of
sciences” tend to be better motivated than those who study foreign language programs.
English as a first foreign language and those who major in Fifty-four students had been accepted into a major in
the arts. arts, 47 in sciences, 21 in engineering, and 54 in other
In light of the preceding discussion of the literature, fields (education, health sciences, and agriculture); 4 stu-
the present study proposed the following null hypotheses: dents did not report the major. The age of the participants
H01. There is no significant difference in motivation ranged from 17 to 24 years, with a mean average of 18.51
to learn EFL between male and female students. and a standard deviation of 1.15. However, as will be seen
HO2. There is no significant difference in motivation later in the study, the number of participants was reduced
to learn EFL among students with different proficiency. as a result of the introduction of pair-wise deletion of miss-
H03. There is no significant difference in motivation ing cases when applied to the statistical analyses adopted
to learn EFL between students who studied English as a in the study
zyxwvutsrqp
zyxwvuts
FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANNALS * VOL. 33, N O . 6 635

Instruments expectations and that no answer would reflect better on the


The participants’ level of motivation to study English as a respondent than another answer. The participants were
foreign language was measured by a modified version of also informed that the findings would be used for research
the Wen (1997) scale (see Appendix). This version con- purposes only and that their individual responses would
sisted of 40 items divided into three parts and had a gener- remain anonymous.
al internal consistency (alpha reliability) of .83 based on The questionnaire was read aloud to the participants
estimations from the present study. Part 1 included demo- before they recorded their responses to ensure that they
graphic questions (6 items) regarding the participants’ age, understood each item on the scale. For example, the items
gender, intended field of study, level of English proficiency, that made reference to English people, culture, and cus-
native language, and first foreign language. Part 2 consist- toms were explained as referring to people from all
ed of three subscales (16 items) measuring the factors of English-speaking countries and not just to the British. The
integrative motivation, instrumental motivation, and reference to “Western culture” was explained as referring

zyxw
effort. Part 3 consisted of three subscales (18 items) meas- not just to English-speaking people but to Europeans as
uring the factors of valence, expectancy, and ability. well. These concepts were not hard for the students to
The integrative motivation subscale consisted of five grasp, as they are familiar with the West and the outside
Likert-type, 7-point items that focused on the appreciation world as a result of the multilingual and multicultural

zyxwvutsrq
of the culture, art, and literature of English and had an nature of Lebanese society and its openness to western
internal consistency of 3 9 . Similarly, the instrumental ideas and cultures.
motivation subscale consisted of five Likert-type, 7-point
items that focused on the importance of English for obtain- Data Analysis
ing employment or pursuing further education and had an Six composite scores of integrative motivation, instrumen-
internal consistency of .60. Meanwhile, the effort subscale tal motivation, effort, expectancy, valence, and perception
consisted of six multiple-choice items that focused on the of ability were computed for each respondent by adding
degree of effort exerted in learning English and had an the scores on the subscale items that measure these vari-
internal consistency of .63. Responses ranged from “a” for ables. A total motivation score was computed by adding
no effort to “d” for very high effort. Furthermore, the the scores on the 34 items of the scale for each respondent.
valence subscale consisted of five Likert-type, 7-point Descriptive statistics and product-moment correlation

zyxwvu
items that focused on the participants’ views of the attrac- coefficients were then computed for all variables in order
tiveness of English for communication, completing school to determine the degree of interrelatedness among vari-
assignments, and understanding English culture and cus- ables. In addition, we ran four multivariate analysis of vari-
toms and had an internal consistency of .72. ance (MANOVA) tests with the determinants of motivation
Finally, the expectancy and ability subscales consisted (integrative motivation, instrumental motivation, effort,
of six items each and had internal consistencies of .78 and valence, expectancy, ability) as dependent variables in each
3 0 , respectively. The expectancy subscale measured the analysis and the variables of gender, proficiency level, first
participants’ perceptions of the probability of achieving the foreign language, and university major as independent
objectives of speaking English fluently, developing reading variables (factors), to address the questions raised in the
comprehension, achieving good grades, and learning about study.
English culture and customs. Responses ranged from “0”
for no probability to “100” for very high probability. The Results
ability subscale measured students’ perceptions of their All statistical tests used to address the questions in the pres-
ability to achieve the above objectives. Responses ranged ent study used .05 as the minimum alpha level. This part of
from “0” for very low ability to “100” for very high ability. the study presents descriptive statistics about the variables
The internal consistencies of the six subscales were all as well as highlights from the intercorrelation matrix and
based on estimations from the present study. the results of the MANOVA analyses run in the study. The
mean scores and standard deviations on the dependent vari-
Instrument Administration ables across the variables of gender, level, first foreign lan-
The researchers obtained the permission of the UOP guage, and university major are presented in Table 1, Table
administration to use a whole class period (40 minutes) for 2, Table 3, and Table 4, respectively.
the administration of the instrument. The purpose of the
questionnaire, its structure and content, and how it was to Correlational Analysis
be filled out were explained to the students. The The results of the correlational analysis (summarized in
researchers asked the participants to be honest in their Table 5) reveal the following aspects of interest.
answers and emphasized that they had no predetermined First, there is a very high, positive correlation between
zyxwvutsrqpon
zy
zyxwvuts
636 N O V E M RER/DECEM KER 2000

zyxwvuts
learners’ expectancy and total motivation ( r = .91, p < . O l )
and between learners’ estimation of ability and total moti-
vation ( r = .90, p < .01). The results also show a moderate
positive correlation between expectancy and estimation of
ability (r = .65, p < . O l ) .
tion ( r = .17, p < .Ol).
Finally, the results reveal that integrative motivation
has a low, positive correlation with instrumental motiva-
tion ( r = .36, p < . O l ) , with expectancy ( r = .37, p < . O l ) ,
and with total motivation (r =.37,p r: .Ol). The results also

zyxwvuts
Second, the results reveal that valence has a low posi- reveal little if any correlation between integrative motiva-

zyx
tive but measurable correlation with integrative motiva-
tion ( r = .46, p < . O l ) , with instrumental motivation ( r =
.30, p < . O l ) , with effort ( r = .36, p < . O l ) , with expectan-
cy (r = .49, p < . O l ) , with estimation of ability ( r = .34, p <
. O l ) , and with total motivation ( u = .49, p < . O l > .
Third, the results reveal that effort has little if any cor-
relation with estimation of ability ( r = .24, p < . O l ) , with
tion and estimation of ability ( r = .25, p < . O l ) . Meanwhile,
instrumental motivation was found to be unrelated to any
of the determinants of motivation except to integrative
motivation ( u = .36, p < . O l ) and to valence ( r = .30, p <
.Ol). This suggests that instrumental motivation may not
be perceived by learners as a strong motivating factor for
exerting exert more effort and developing high expectancy
expectancy ( r = .25, p < .Ol), and with integrative motiva- and the ability to acquire languages other than their own.

Variable
Instrumental
zyxwv
zyxw
DESCRlPTlVE STATISTICS FOR RATINGS OF MOTIVATION DETERMINANTS BY GENDER

M
30.21
Male
(n = 100)
SD
4.06
M
31.26
Female
(n = 65)
SD
3.16
Integrative 21.77 4.84 22.36 4.96
Effort 17.11 3.15 18.44 2.84
Expectancy 363.80 82.70 371.84 77.31
Valence 30.80 5.96 32.47 4.96
Ability 396.60 83.82 384.76 72.88
Total 860.29 155.29 861.16 144.74

zyxwvu
___________

DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS FOR RATINGS OF MOTIVATION DETERMINANTS BY LEVEL

Level 11 Level 111


(n = 38) (n = 127)
Variable M SD M SD
Instrumental 31.34 4.10 30.40 3.64
Integrative 23.78 4.36 2 1.47 4.92
Effort 18.57 2.67 17.35 3.16
Expectancy 361.84 87.63 368.50 78.51
Valence 33.18 4.99 30.94 5.73
Ability 382.10 78.71 394.88 80.03
Total 850.84 154.05 863.56 150.82
zyxwvutsr
zyxwvutsrqp
zyxwvut
zyxwvutsrqponmlkjihgfedc
FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANNALS * VOL.33, N O . 6 637

MANOVA Analyses perception of the valence of learning English, F(1, 166) =

zyxwvu
Gender Scores. The results of the MANOVA scores for 3.67, p = .05. The mean effort score for femaleswas 18.44
male and female participants showed that the first hypoth- (SD = 2.84), whereas the mean effort score for males was
esis is rejected (see Table 6). 17.11 (SD = 3.15). Similarly, the mean valence score for
The MANOVA analysis revealed a significant differ- females was 32.47 (SD = 4.96), and the mean valence score
ence between males and females in their motivation to for males was 30.80 (SD = 5.96).
acquire English as a foreign language, F(6, 161) = .91, p = Level Scores. The results of the MANOVA scores for

zyxwvut
.01. Second, the univariate analysis of variance showed no level 11 and level I11 participants showed that the second
significant differences between males and females in their hypothesis is also rejected (see Table 7).
integrative motivation, instrumental motivation, expectan- The MANOVA analysis showed a significant difference
cy, and estimation of ability. However, there was a signifi- between level 11 and level 111 participants in their motiva-
cant difference in their effort, F(1, 166) = 7.75, p = .OO, and tion to learn English, F (6, 161) = .90, p = .01. Second, the

zyxwvu
DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS FOR RATINGS OF MOTIVATION DETERMINANTS
BY FIRST FOREIGN LANGUAGE

French English
(n = 90) (n = 73)
Variable M SD M SD
lnstrumental 30.72 3.89 30.43 3.64
Integrative 21.74 4.89 22.93 4.89
Effort 17.42 3.13 17.82 3.04
Expectancy 369.55 76.71 363.15 86.24
Valence 31.58 5.95 31.24 5.31
Ability 394.22 80.14 386.57 77.74
Total 865.25 147.16 851.63 155.92

Variable
Instrumental
M
Arts
(n = 50)

31.20
SD
3.38
M
30.02
(n = 45)
zy
zyxwvu
DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS FOR RATINGS OF MOTIVATION DETERMINANTS BY MAJOR AT UNIVERSITY
Sciences

SD
3.76
Engineering

M
29.36
(n = 19)
SD
5.09
~~ ~~

M
31.05
Other
(n = 51)
~~~~~~~

SD
3.46
Integrative 22.14 4.62 22.26 5.57 19.73 5.33 22.49 4.19

Effort 17.90 3.23 17.35 3.39 18.31 2.31 17.37 2.95


Expectancy 374.40 74.64 370.66 86.11 354.73 75.59 360.98 83.91
~~~~~~~~~

Valence 31.46 4.71 31.35 6.07 31.94 5.40 31.37 6.26


Ability '388.40 72.29 388.66 101.59 394.73 74.86 397.25 67.44
Total 865.50 141.25 860.33 185.98 848.84 130.83 860.52 135.41
638 zy
zyxwvutsrqpon NOVEMBEWDECEMBER 2000

zyxwvutsr
univariate analysis showed no significant differences
between level I1 and level 111 participants in their instru-
mental motivation, expectancy, or estimation of ability.
However, there were significant differences with regard to

Variable
1. Integrative
2. Instrumental
INTERCORRELATIONS AMONG DETERMINANTS OF MOTIVATION

.36**
2 3 4 5 zyx
zyxw 6 7

3. Effort .17** .09


4. Valence .46** .30** .36**
5. Expectancy .37** .ll .25** .49**
6. Ability .25** .03 .24** .34** .65**
7. Total .37 ** .12 .31** .49** .91** .90**
n 176 180 176 179 179 177 168
Mean 21.91 30.61 17.72 31.45 368.04 390.22 860.82
SD 4.89 3.73 3.03 5.66 80.06 80.96 152.08
** Significant ai p c . 01

MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE SUMMARY OF DETERMINANTS OF MOTIVATION BY GENDER

Multivariate ANOVAa Univariate AN OVA^


Source F Integrative Instrumental Effort Valence Expectation Ability
Gender .91** F .23 2.24 7.75** 3.67* .78 1.38
a

zyxwvutsrqponm
dk = (6, 161)
bdfs = (1,166)
*p c .05.
**p c .01

MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE SUMMARY OF DETERMINANTS OF MOTIVATION BY LEVEL

Multivariate ANOVAa Univariate AN OVA^


Source F Integrative Instrumental Effort Valence Expectation Ability
Level .90** F 7.16** 1.82 4.25* 4.27* .20 .73
1dfs = (6. 161)
hdfs = (1, 66)
*p < .05.
**p c .01
zyxwvutsrqp
zyxwvutsrq
zyxwvutsr
FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANNALS * VOI,. 33, NO. 6 639

zyxwvuts
zyxwvuts z zyxwvu
integrative motivation, F(1, 166) = 7.16, p = .OO; the inte-
grative motivation mean score for level I1 was 23.78 (SD =
4.36) and for level 111was 21.47 (SD = 4.92). Furthermore,
there were significant differences with regard to effort, F (1,
166) = 4.25, p = .04; the effort mean score for level I1 was
Sciences, Engineering, and other fields of study, F(18,474)
= .89,p = .42. Therefore, the fourth hypothesis is accepted.

Discussion
This study set out to determine the interrelatedness of the
18.75 (SD = 2.67) and for level I11 was 17.35 (SD = 3.16). motivational factors identified in the literature, as well as
Finally, there were significant differences with regard to to examine the effects of gender, level of proficiency in
valence of the outcomes of learning English, F(1,166) = English, first foreign language learned at school, and major
4.27, p = .04; the valence mean score for level I1 was 33.18 of study on students’ motivation to learn EFL. The first
(SD = 4.99) and for level I11 was 30.94 (SD = 5.37). These question was about whether the determinants of motiva-
results indicate that students in this study with higher lev- tion (integrative motivation, instrumental motivation,
els of proficiency in English have lower levels of integrative effort, valence, expectancy, and ability) are internally relat-
motivation. ed. The results of the correlational analysis (see Table 5)
First Foreign Language Scores. The results of the indicate that students’ expectancy to achieve their goals
MANOVA scores for first foreign language (presented in and their perception of their ability to achieve those goals
Table 8) show no significant differences between partici- are strong determinants of their motivation. This finding
pants who had studied French as a first foreign language suggests that these two factors (estimation of ability and
and those who had studied English, F(6, 159) = .97, p = expectancy) should be used as variables in future research
.71. Therefore, the third hypothesis is accepted. on motivation and second/foreign language learning.
University Major Scores. The MANOVA scores by Furthermore, these results indicate that learners need to
university major (see Table 9) show no significant differ- feel that their study program goals are attainable and that
ences in the motivations of students majoring in the Arts, they are capable of achieving them in order to increase

MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE SUMMARY OF DETERMINANTS OF MOTIVATION


BY FIRST FOREIGN LANGUAGE

Multivariate ANOVAO Univariate AN OVA^


Source F Integrative Instrumental Effort Valence Expectation Ability
First Foreign
Language .97 F 1.12 .06 .67 .14 .06 .12
adfs = (6, 159)
bdfs = (1,164)
* p c .05.
**p < .01

I
MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE SUMMARY OF DETERMINANTS OF MOTIVATION
BY MAJOR AT UNIVERSITY
I
~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~~

Multivariate ANOVAa Univariate AN OVA^


Source F Integrative Instrumental Effort Valence Expectation Ability

zyxwvutsrqpon
Major .89 F 1.60 1.73 .66 .05 .40 .13
a dfs = (18,474)
bdk = (3,161)
*p c .05.
**p c .01
640 zyxwvutsrqpo
zy NOVEMKEIUDECEMBER 2000

their motivation. Along similar lines, the study goals


should be made attractive to students in order to motivate
them to learn a language other than their own. The peda-
gogical implications of these results underscore the impor-
tance of affective factors in language acquisition and the
need for setting up developmentally appropriate programs
and specifying interesting and relevant goals for these pro-
grams.
zyx
zyxw
and Taminian (1984), who conducted their study in a sim-
ilar context, found that Arab university students in gener-
al tended to have negative attitudes towards English,
which they perceived as a threat to Arabic - their native
language and the language of the Koran. In their study,
there were no statistically significant differences between
males and females in their attitudes towards the use of
English as a medium of instruction, towards the effect of
On the other hand, the results revealed a low but using English on Arab identity, and towards the utility of
measurable correlation among all the determinants of English compared with Arabic. However, when comparing
motivation, except for instrumental motivation. This find- means of responses, the authors concluded that females
ing sheds some light on the traditional dichotomy between tended to have a more open attitude towards English than
integrative and instrumental motivation and the role of did males.
each one in learning a foreign language. It is clear, for The third question raised in the present study dealt
example, that integrative motivation correlates positively with whether level of proficiency in English made a differ-
with all the other variables, including instrumental moti- ence in motivation towards learning the language. Results
vation, and with total motivation, a finding that could of the MANOVA analysis showed that level I1 students
explain why it is identified in the literature (Gardner and were more motivated than those in level 111. This finding
Lambert 1959; Gardner 1985; Dornyei 1990) as a major contradicts those of Sung and Padilla (1998) and is in
indicator of positive orientation towards the target lan- agreement with the findings of Tweles (1995), who main-
guage that could lead to better achievement. tained that the more proficient students are not necessari-
Instrumental motivation was found to correlate only ly more motivated than their less-proficient counterparts.
with integrative motivation and valence; it did not corre-
late with effort, expectancy, perception of ability, or total
motivation. In other words, the presence of instrumental
motivation in students may not mean that they will in fact
exert more effort or develop high expectancy or high abil-
ity with regard to EFL learning; however, instrumental
motivation may contribute to the enhancement of integra-
tive motivation and the development of positive affective
orientation towards particular learning outcomes. This
zyxw
The finding might be explained by the eagerness of low-
proficiency students to work hard so that they could enter
the university as soon as possible. Another pertinent
observation in the context of our study is that the majori-
ty of level I1 students came from French-medium schools
where English is a third language or from average schools
where the level of English is not up to the desired level.
This observation explains their low proficiency as well as
their desire to move fast into regular university studies,
finding of the limited effect of instrumental motivation in that is, to save money by not staymg long in the intensive
the Lebanese EFL context is in contrast to the findings of English program.
Kruidenier and Clement (1986) and Belmechri and The last two questions in the present study focused on
Hummel (1998), who found instrumental motivation the effect of the variables of university major and first for-
determined by an instrumental orientation to be generally eign language on learners’ motivation to learn EFL. The
prominent in foreign language contexts. findings showed no significant differences in motivation
The second question related to gender differences in based on these two variables. These results contradict the
motivation. Results of the MANOVA analysis indicated prevailing impression in Lebanon that students who
that there were differences between males and females in choose to join science majors and professional schools
their motivation to learn English. More specifically, the (e.g. Engineering and Agriculture) and those whose first
univariate analysis indicated that females were more likely foreign language is French tend to be more disciplined and
than males to report that they would exert effort in their better motivated to learn English than students whose first
learning and to perceive the goals of learning English in a foreign language is English and those who choose to major
positive manner. This corroborates the findings of Sung in humanities and social sciences.
and Padilla (1998), Karsenti and Thibert (1994), and
Zammit (1993), who reported similar results; however, Conclusion
they contradict the findings of Suleiman (1993), who In conclusion, the results of the study underscore the
reported negative attitudes to learning English among importance of learner characteristics and affective orienta-
female Arab EFL learners. One possible explanation for the tion toward program of study in the development of moti-
negative attitudes of females in Suleiman’s study towards vation to acquire another language. More specifically, if
English is that these attitudes could be context-specific students show integrative orientation toward the foreign
and may vary due to sociocultural factors. In fact, Zughoul language and culture, work hard, have a positive orienta-
zyxwvuts
zyxwvutsrq
zyxwvut
FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANNALS * VOL.33, NO. 6 641

tion towards the outcomes of learning, and develop the Second Language Acquisition: Conceptual, Contextual, and
sense that the program goals are attainable, they will devel- Statistical Considerations.” Language Learning 30:255-70.
op the needed motivation to learn the language. In addi- Gardner, R. C. 1985. Social Psychology and Second Language
tion, the findings of the present study indicate that, con- Learning. Baltimore, MD: Edward Arnold.
trary to the perceived prominence of instrumental motiva- Gardner, R. C. 1988. “The Socio-EducationalModel of Second
tion in foreign language contexts, learners do not perceive Language Learning: Assumptions, Findings, and Issues.”
it as a factor that will lead them to exert more effort and Language Learning 28:lOl-26.

zyxwvuts
develop the needed expectations and ability for learning Gardener, R. C., R. Clement, F! C. Smythe, and C. L. Smythe.
EFL. Finally, the results suggest that low-proficiency stu- 1979. AttitudelMotivation Test Battery-Revised Manual.
dents and females tend to be more willing to exert effort in (Research Bulletin No. 15). London, Ontario: University of
Western Ontario, Language Research Group.
learning English. They also tend to perceive the goals of
learning English as more attractive than do their male and Karsenti, T.,
and G. Thibert. 1994. “The Influence of Gender
higher-proficiency counterparts. Differences on Within-Term Changes in Junior-College
Student Motivation” (ED 373537).
Lewin, K. 1951. Field Theory in Social Sciences. New York:
Harper and Row Publications
References Oller, J. W 1981. “Can Affect be Measured?” International
Au, S.Y. 1988. “A Critical Appraisal of Gardner’s Social- Review of Applied Linguistics 19:227-35.
Psychological Theory of Second Language (L2) Learning.”
Kruidenier, B., and R. Clement. 1986. The Effect of Context on
Language Learning 38:75-100.
the Composition and the Role of Orientations in Second Language
Belmechri, E, and K. Hummel. 1998. “Orientation and Acquisition. Quebec City: International Center for Research on
Motivation in the Acquisition of English as a Second Language Bilingualism.
Among High School Students in Quebec City” Language
Learning 48:2 19-44. O’Malley, J. M., A. U. Chamot, G. Stewner-Manzaneres, R.
Russo, and L. Kupper. 1985. “Learning Strategy Applications
Boykin, A,, and A.Trungamphai. 1982. “Attitude and Attained with Students of English as a Second Language.” TESOL
English Language Proficiency of University Students in Quarterly 19557-84.
Thailand: A Sociolinguistic Study” (ED 219421).
Oxford, R., and J. Shearin. 1994. “Language Learning
Coleman, T. A. 1995. “Progress, Proficiency, and Motivation Motivation: Expanding the Theoretical Framework.” The
among British University Language Learners” (ED 383221). Modern LanguageJournal 78:12-28.
Crookes, G., and R. W. Schmidt. 1991. “Motivation: Stern, H. H. 1983. Fundamental Concepts of Language Teaching.
Reopening the Research Agenda.” Language Learning Oxford: Oxford University Press.

zyxwvutsr
41:469-5 12.
Suleiman, M. E 1993. “A Study of Arab Students’ Motivation
Dornyei, Z. 1990. “Conceptualizing Motivation in Foreign and Attitudes for Learning English as a Foreign Language”
Language Learning.” Language Learning 40:45-78. (ED 392279).
. 1994. “Motivation and Motivating in the Foreign Sung, H. and A. M. Padilla. 1998. “Student Motivation,
Language Classroom.” Modern LanguageJournal3:273-84. Parental Attitudes, and Involvement in Learning of Asian
Dulay, H., M. Burt, and S. Krashen. 1982. Language Two. New Languages in Elementary and Secondary Schools.” The Modern
York: Oxford University Press. Language Journal 82:205- 16.
Ellis, R. 1985. Understanding Second Language Acquisition. Tweles, B. 1995. “Motivation as a Two-sided Coin:
Oxford: Oxford University Press. Motivational Differences Between College-Level Chinese and
Japanese Learners of EFL” (ED 416704).
Gardner, R. C., and W. E. Lambert. 1959. “Motivational
Variables in Second-Language Acquisition.” Canadian Journal Vroom, V. H. 1964. Work and Motivation. New York: Wiley
of Psychology 13:24-44. Wen, X. 1997. “Motivation and Language Learning with
Gardner, R. C., and W. E. Lambert. 1972. Attitudes and Chinese.” Foreign Language Annals 30:234-51.
Motivation in Second-Language Learning. Rowley, MA: New- Zammit, S. 1993. Motivation, Test Results, Gender Dgerences
bury House. and Foreign Languages (ED 362007).
Gardner, R. C. 1979. “Social Psychological Aspects of Second Zughoul, M. R., and L. Taminian. 1984. “The Linguistic
Language Acquisition,” 193-220 in H. Giles and R. St. Clair, Attitudes of Arab University Students: Factorial Structure and
eds., Language and Social Psychology. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. Intervening Variables.” International Journal of the Sociology of
Gardner, R. C. 1980. “On the Validity of Affective Variables in Language 50:155-79.
642 zy
zyxwvutsrqpon
zy NOVEMB ER'DECEMBER 2000

zyxwvutsrq
Appendix: Student Questionnaire

Student Name:
I. General Infomation
1. Give your age in years:
2. Indicate your sex: M F
3. What is your major?
4. Intensive English level: Level I Level 11 Level 111 Level IV
5. Native language
6. First foreign language

The following are statements with which some people will agree and others will disagree. There are no right or wrong

zyxwvuts
answers, since many people have different opinions. Please give your immediate reactions to each of the items. On the
other hand, please do not be careless, as it is important that we obtain your true feelings. Circle the number of the alter-
native below the statement that best indicates your feelings about that statement.
II. Motivation Infomation
Studymg English will help me.. .
1. better understand and appreciate English art and literature.

zyxwvutsrq
zyxwvuts
Strongly Strongly
disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 agree
2. meet and converse with more and different people.
Strongly Strongly
disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 agree
3. learn about other cultures and understand the world better.
Strongly Strongly
disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 agree
4. understand the Western cultural heritage
Strongly Strongly
disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 agree
5. because I feel English is an important language in the economic development of the world.
Strongly Strongly
disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 agree
6. better understand the problems that English speakers face.
Strongly Strongly
disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 agree
7. in getting a job.
Strongly Strongly
disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 agree
8. converse and communicate with English-speaking friends.
Strongly Strongly
disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 agree
9. communicate in English when I travel to English-speaking countries.
Strongly Strongly
disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 agree
10. because I need it for my university
Strongly Strongly
disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 agree
FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANNALS. VOL. 33, N O . 6 zyxwvutsrq 643

zyxwvutsr
When I learn a foreign language, I expect that:
11. 1will ...
a. pass on the basis of sheer luck and intelligence
b. do just enough work to get along
c. try hard to learn the language
d. enjoy doing all the work
12. I will think about the words and ideas that 1 have learned in my classes.. .
a. hardly ever.
b. once or twice per week
c. several times during the week
d. daily
13. I will spend about the following amount of time to practice the language after class:
a. zero hours
b. one hour per week
c. four hours per week
d. more than six hours per week
14. I will.. .
a. not necessarily be active in speaking the language in class
b. answer the questions when I am called upon
c. volunteer answers to the questions that are easy
d. volunteer answers as much as possible
15. After I get my English assignments back, I will.. .
a. just throw them in my desk and forget them
b. look them over but won’t bother correcting mistakes
c. correct mistakes when I have time
d. always rewrite them, correcting my mistakes
16. I will try to speak English after class:
a. never
b. when I have to
c. when I am offered the opportunity to do so
d. in a wide variety of situations and as much as possible zyxwvu
III. Infomation on Learning Outcomes: Valency, Expectancy, and Ability
How significant are these outcomes of your English class to you?
Circle the number that best indicates your feelings about each statement.
1. To speak English fairly fluently

zyxwvut
very very
insig. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 sig.
2. To be able to communicate with English speakers in basic English
very very
insig. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 sig.
3 . To develop comprehension of reading assignments
Very very
insig. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 sig.
4. To receive the grade of “A” in course
Very very
insig. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 sig.
644 zy
zyxwvutsrqpo NOVEMBEWDECEMBER 2000

5. To better understand English people and their way of thinking


very very
insig. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 sig.

6. To learn about English culture and customs


very Very
insig. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 sig.

zyxwvu
How probable is it that you will achieve the above outcomes from the English class that you are taking now? Circle the

zyxwvu
expected probability for each outcome.
1. To speak English fairly fluently.

zyxwvu
no probability 100% probability
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100

2 . To communicate with English speakers in basic English .


no probability 100% probability
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100

3. To develop reading comprehension of English.


no probability 100% probability
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100

4. To receive the grade of “A” from the class.


no probability 100% probability
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100

5. To better understand English people and their way of thinking.


no probability 100% probability
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100

6. To learn more about English culture and customs.


no probability 100%probability
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100

What do you think of your own ability to achieve the above outcomes? Circle your estimated ability for each outcome.
1. To speak English fairly fluently
very low ability very high ability
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100

2. To be able to communicate with English speakers in basic English.


very low ability very high ability
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100

3. To develop comprehension of reading assignments.


very low ability very high ability
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100

4. To receive the grade of “A” in course.


very low ability very high ability
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100

5. To better understand English people and their way of thinking.


very low ability very high ability
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100

6. To learn more about English culture and customs.


very low ability very high ability
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100

View publication stats

You might also like