You are on page 1of 6

Effect of Lithium on Mood, Cognition, and

Personality Function in Normal Subjects


Lewis L. Judd, MD

R ecently, we have initiated and completed a series of months' treatment with lithium carbonate in a double-
studies investigating the effects of various psycho- blind study on driving skills and on personality factors as
tropic medications in normal subjects, utilizing chronic assessed by the Beck depression scale and the Marke-
dosage regimens that approximate those used in treating Nyman personality scale. No consistent effects from
clinical populations.'-4 We believe that methodological lithium carbonate on driving skills or on the personality of
advantages of this research paradigm allow psychological these patients, as measured by the instruments, was found.
and behavioral effects from these drugs to be partialed out Only the side effects of tremor and increased thirst were
and identified more clearly in a normal sample than in noted.
patient populations responsive to a particular psychotropic One of the more intriguing findings emerging from
medication. We have hypothesized that normative data on controlled observations of lithium's effect on human
the effects of psychotropic medications will be helpful in psychological functions has been the identification of
delineating the behavioral and psychological mechanisms lithium-related performance deficits in a variety of senso-
that underlie and mediate desired clinical changes in ry, motor, and cognitive tasks. Linnoila et all reported a
patient populations. This experimental approach is best delay in choice reaction time in normal subjects main-
exemplified by the series of studies that we have conducted tained at therapeutic serum levels of lithium and hypothe-
over the past few years to identify the effects occurring in sized a potential information processing delay. Small et al,9
normal subjects maintained at therapeutic levels. in a controlled study of ten normal subjects, noted many
Certain studies are available that have investigated physical complaints, especially those related to the CNS,
what mood and personality effects occur in normal individ- during the second and third week of therapeutic lithium
uals maintained on therapeutic dosage levels of lithium administration. This was confirmed by items from the
carbonate. Schou5 and referenced authors reported that self-rating scales, which indicated a reduction in mental
normal subjects experienced only tiredness and muscular clarity, alertness, efficiency, and pep. In addition, there
heaviness after lithium carbonate treatment of approxi- was evidence of impairment in work and school perform-
mately 925 mg/day in a seven-day double-blind experi- ance. In a series of studies on manic-depressive patients
ment. These authors themselves took 925 mg/day for three receiving lithium, Demers and Heninger'°l'" have described
to six weeks and reported muscular weakness but no a lithium-related deficit in simple reaction time, a block-
subjective or objective evidence of mood or emotional turning task, the Digit Symbol subtest of the Wechsler
changes. When they took approximately 1,850 mg/day, Adult Intelligence Test (WAIS), and a small reduction in
they experienced a number of symptoms-muscular heavi- IQ scores. A study by Aminoff et al'2 of mentally subnor-
ness, increased irritability, emotional lability, increased mal patients receiving lithium reported a significant drop
mental effort in initiating physical tasks (inertia), indif- in IQ scores during lithium treatment. Finally, Marini and
ference, malaise, passivity, increased hypersensitivity Sheard," studying prisoners receiving lithium, were unable
alternating with a decreased response to environmental to find performance deficits in a series of perceptual motor
stimuli, being separated from environmental stimuli by a tests, although it was their impression that performance
"glass wall," etc. was "blunted" during the medication period.
Bech et al6 have studied the effects of lithium carbonate Our interests in the effects of lithium on normal person-
both in patients with affective disorder and in patients ality function were stimulated by a non-blind study in a
with Meniere's disease and found basically no long-term group of normal male subjects in whom we were assessing
effects on personality functions. The study of patients the effects of lithium pretreatment on pentobarbital-
with M6niere's disease focused on the effects of six induced euphoria. During the lithium maintenance period,
we were surprised at the large number of spontaneous
From the Psychiatric Service, San Diego Veterans Administration Hospi-
complaints from the normal subjects in regard to their
tal, and the Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of personal experiences on lithium. We had anecdotally noted
California at San Diego, La Jolla, Calif. during the study that there appeared to be general dulling

860 Arch Gen Psychiatry-Vol 36, July 20, 1979 Lithium in Normals-Judd

Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ by a Kings College London User on 05/16/2023


Table 1-Instruments and Experimental Tasks Used Table 2.-Self-Rated Effects of Lithium Carbonate:
in Studying Effects of Therapeutic Lithium Maintenance SHAS and POMS Combined*
on Normal Subjects
Lithium
Assessment of Affect and Mood Placebo Carbonate
Self-rated item Condition Condition t Pt
Profile of Mood States (POMS) Lethargic 1.61 2.13 2.41 .01
Subjective High Assessment Scale (Judd) Exhausted 0.48 0.87 1.68 .05
Subjective State Questionnaire Bewildered 0.17 0.52 2.91 <.01
Adverse Symptom Checklist
Independent rater observation Muddled 0.39 0.83 2.01 .03
Behavioral Observation Scale (Judd) Clearheaded 2.74 2.26 -2.42 .01
"Significant Other" Questionnaire (Judd) Ideas flow easily 2.00 1.48 -1.91 .03
Assessment of Personality Functions Good sexual feelings 1.47 1.22 -1.82 .04
California Psychological Inventory Wish it would last for
Holtzman Inkblot Technique days 1.78 1.26 -1.96 .03
Psychometric Assessment of Creative Functions Lonely 0.39 0.65 1.82 .04
Meier Aesthetic Perception Test
Meier Art Judgment Test Nauseous 1.22 1.48 2.02 .03
Christiansen-Guilford Battery Shaky 0.35 0.91 2.13 .02
Word Fluency
Ideational Fluency *SHAS, Subjective High Assessment Scale; possible range of scores is 1
Expressional Fluency (low) to 6 (high). POMS, Profile of Mood States; possible range of scores is
Associational Fluency 0 (low) to 4 (high).
Assessment of Cognitive and Sensory-Motor Functions tOne-tailed Student's t test.
Average Evoked Responses
Otis Quick-Scoring IQ Test lithium's effects on a wide spectrum of human functions. Listed in
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS)
Digit Symbol Table 1 are the dependent measures that have been used to assess
Block Design the effects of two weeks of lithium administration on these normal
Trail-Making Test, A and B subjects. These are grouped into five different general categories,
Speed of Closure Test representing subtests of a psychometric inventory, a complete
Minnesota Clerical Test
Number Learning Test psychometric inventory, and rating scales developed and stan-
Standardized Serial Seven Test dardized by others or by our own research group.
Porteus Mazes In the main, the statistical procedures used to analyze the data
Simple Reaction Time Procedure from these studies were correlated t tests or analysis of variance
Posner Information Processing Task
Backward Masking Procedure-Iconic Storage for a repeated measures design. No order effects for lithium and
Assessment of Other Functions placebo presentation were found across the studies. Scores for
Sleep Log (self-rated) those few tests (WAIS-Digit Symbol Substitution and Block
Stanford Sleepiness Scale (self-rated) Design; Trail Making A and B)'4'l in which learning effects were
noted were adjusted by a learning-correction factor calculated for
and blunting of various personality functions during each test.
lithium maintenance, which was confirmed by the data RESULTS
from the self-rating scales indicating that the subjects Lithium Effects on Affect and Mood (N = 23)
experienced generalized subjective dysphoria on lithium. The data from the self-rated measures of affect and
The data reported in this paper summarize the findings mood and other subjective feeling tones in these normal
derived from a series of controlled studies, now completed, subjects reflected a consistent pattern during lithium
which were designed to assess the effects of therapeutic administration. Data from our previous study' gave a basis
maintenance levels of lithium on a wide variety of mood from which to predict in advance the direction of change of
and psychological functions in normal subjects. affect and mood in subjects receiving lithium, enabling
SUBJECTS AND METHODS one-tailed levels of significance to be reported. The items
from the Profile of Mood States (POMS)16 and Subjective
Details of the research methods have been described else- High Assessment Scale (SHAS)2 that changed significant-
where2 l; a brief summary will be presented here for purposes of
orientation. We have now studied a total of 42 young healthy men ly during the period of lithium administration are included
ranging in age from 21 to 31 years (mean, 24 years) who were in Table 2. As can be seen, the subjects reported increased
screened and selected for study participation as previously levels of lethargy, exhaustion, confusion, bewilderment,
reported. All subjects in our studies gave informed consent after and reduced clearheadedness after 14 days of lithium
having the purposes of the study fully explained. In each study a administration. In addition, mean scores on both the
double-blind, randomized, split-half crossover procedure was used Tension and Fatigue scales from the POMS were elevated.
in the administration of lithium carbonate and an inactive placebo Listed in Tables 3 and 4 are the self-rated items from the
(14 days each). Serum lithium levels were monitored twice weekly, Subjective State Questionnaire (SSQ)' and the Adverse
with dosages during the active medication phase adjusted to Symptom Checklist (ASC)' that changed during lithium
achieve serum levels ranging from 0.7 to 1.4 mEq/L (mean, 0.9 administration. It is clear that these items represent a
mEq/L on testing days). A sham dosage adjustment procedure consistent pattern of the subjective changes these subjects
was utilized in the placebo condition. In this experimental design,
two identical testing sessions were administered, one at the end of experienced as a result of receiving lithium. Twenty-five of
the 14-day placebo period and the other after the lithium carbon- 74 items on the SSQ showed changes induced by lithium.
ate maintenance period. These items, when grouped logically into homogenous
A broad range of tasks and instruments were used to evaluate feeling tones, revealed an interesting lithium effect. The

Arch Gen Psychiatry-Vol 36, July 20, 1979 Lithium in Normals-Judd 861

Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ by a Kings College London User on 05/16/2023


Table 3.-Self-Rated Items From Subjective State Table 4.-Items From Adverse Symptom Checklist That
Questionnaire That Changed on Lithium Carbonate Changed on Lithium Carbonate Maintenance*
Maintenance*
Lithium
Lithium Placebo Carbonate
Placebo Carbonate Item Condition Condition t Pt
Item Condition Condition t Pt Twitching 1.95 6.05 -1.96 .04
Feelings of negativism and dysphoria Numb 1.33 2.33 -1.77 .05
Helpless 2.68 3.86 -2.08 .03 Headache 2.05 6.57 -3.10 <.01
Depressed 9.50 11.95 -2.03 .03 Pain 1.48 2.67 -2.18 .02
Miserable 7.41 12.45 -3.01 <.01 Itchy 3.24 4.76 -1.74 .05
Negative toward life 7.71 10.29 -2.46 .01 Dizzy 1.33 4.62 -2.00 .03
Interested 25.90 22.82 2.86 <.01 Faint 1.91 3.67 -2.07 .03
Active 25.27 21.36 3.22 <.01 Stuffy nose 6.38 10.76 -2.10 .03
Empty 10.50 14.14 -2.02 .03 Stuffy head 4.00 7.71 -2.14 .03
Sick 7.41 11.09 -2.38 .02 Hard breathing 1.43 4.04 -2.33 .02
Feelings of tension and agitation Heart pounding 1.86 4.14 -1.97 .04
Restless 14.68 17.36 -1.88 .04
Stomach ache 1.71 3.14 -3.14 <.01
Anxious 12.45 16.95 -2.65 <.01 Dry mouth 2.38 5.38 -1.73 .05
Agitated 11.95 15.91 -3.03 <.01 Nausea 1.33 4.00 -2.59 <.01
Out of control 2.54 4.09 -2.58 <.01
Tense 9.77 14.55 -2.68 <.01 *Scoring on Adverse Symptom Checklist ranges from 1 (low) to 36
Distress 8.14 11.18 -2.19 .02 (high).
Suspicious 9.67 11.71 -2.07 .03 tOne-tailed Student's t test.
Feelings of interpersonal detachment or placebo administration on the 18 individual scales. Of
Want excitement 24.50 22.27 1.95 .03
Be with people 24.55 20.68 2.71 <.01 the 22 possible scoring dimensions in the Holtzman Inkblot
Want attention 22.36 19.05 3.38 <.01 Technique,'8 only two were significantly changed during
Want physical contact 23.32 19.82 2.01 .03 lithium administration. Lithium induced a longer delay in
Feelings of cognitive inefficiency response to the stimulus inkblot (P = .022, two-tailed), and
Attentive 26.59 23.59 2.49 .01 the subjects receiving lithium were more likely to respond
Rapidly changing with a popular response (P = .048, two-tailed). Thus, only
thoughts 7.59 12.05 -2.61 <.01
Easy to remember 25.05 20.68 2.24 .02 two of a possible 40 scoring variables were influenced by
Confusion 7.27 10.27 -1.72 .05 lithium. Since these effects could have occurred by chance
Can't function 2.73 4.68 -2.75 <.01 alone, we have concluded that there were no significant
Drug effect 7.14 13.29 2.89 <.01 alterations on these personality inventories due to
*Scoring on Subjective State Questionnaire ranges from 1 (low) to 3fi lithium.
{high). Effects of Lithium on Psychometric Assessment of
tOne-tailed Student's t test. Creative Functions (N = 22, N = 14)
first grouping of items indicated increased negativism and Due to varying opinions expressed as to lithium's poten-
depression. The second grouping indicated increased feel- tial influence on creativity, we attempted to assess this
ings of agitation, anxiety, restlessness, and tension. The area using standardized tests that tap aspects of one's
third grouping reflected feelings of withdrawal from capacity in this area of function. No differences from the
contact with others and from interpersonal and environ- subjects' normal performances were noted during lithium
mental demands. The last group was composed of items administration on either of the Meier Art Tests."' Further,
indicating feelings of inefficiency and disrupted cognition. there were no differences between the scores obtained on
A significant number of subjective side effects, as rated on the four Christensen-Guilford Verbal Fluency Tests during
the ASC (Table 4), were also found. Fourteen of the lithium and placebo administration.2"' Thus, lithium did not
possible 32 items on this scale were experienced as being affect performance in semantic creativity or the capacity
changed during or after lithium administration. While a to judge the artistic efforts of others.
number of these items have been commonly reported by Effects of Lithium on Cognitive and Sensory-Motor
patients receiving lithium, several new effects were Performance (N = 22)
observed. It was interesting that trained independent
observers were unable to detect any behavioral changes in Table 5 lists the mean performance scores from the
the subjects induced by lithium, whereas individuals close various cognitive motor tasks contrasting the results
to the subjects (friends, roommates, girlfriends, etc) were obtained during lithium and placebo administration. In
able to identify lithium-related behavioral changes on a general, these results indicate performance deficits seen
questionnaire directed to them. These observed effects after 14 days of lithium carbonate administration. The one
included increased levels of drowsiness and lowered ability exception is the Speed of Closure test,2' on which the
to work hard and to think clearly. subjects receiving lithium scanned fewer letters but made
fewer errors than they did while receiving placebo. It
Effects of Lithium on Psychometric Tests of Personality should be noted that although lithium-related decrements
Function (N = 23) in performance were small, they nonetheless were consist-
No differences were found on the California Psychologi- ent enough to reach statistical significance. In examining
cal Inventory"' in the mean scores achieved during lithium this data more closely, it appeared to us that the deficit

862 Arch Gen Psychiatry-Vol 36, July 20, 1979 Lithium in Normals-Judd

Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ by a Kings College London User on 05/16/2023


Table 5.-Comparison of Scores From Cognitive and
Motor Performance Tests in Lithium Carbonate and
850
Placebo Conditions
Lithium
Testing Instrument Placebo Carbonate P
t 8
Otis 10 Test (10 Score) 120.75 118.54 2.25 .018 800
WAIS Block Design Test 14.38 14.42 -0.05 NS
WAIS Digit Symbol Test 15.09 12.91 2.02 .028 5
Halstead-Reitan Trail Making
A (time, s) 20.15 22.31 -1.82 .041 .S 750
Halstead-Reitan Trail Making
B (time, s) 49.04 52.28 -1.14 NS
Porteus Maze
Time, s 71.00 67.60 -0.21 NS * 700
Errors 4.85 5.35 0.20 NS
Serial 7
No. completed 38.80 37.75 -0.59 NS
No. correct 37.50 37.10 -0.21 NS
650
Speed of closure
No. of letters scanned 358.65 346.45 -0.68 NS
No. of errors 3.60 2.70 -2.07 .025
% Errors 14.73 11.50 -1.87 .034
Minnesota Clerical 600
No. completed 156.50 142.45 -4.02 .001 Simple (AA) Complex (Aa)
No. correct 153.30 137.40 -4.83 <.0001
Cognitive Processing Task Analysis: (N = 22)
'One-tailed Student's t test.
Task Complexity (Simple vs Complex) F= 50.619; df= 1,21; P<.001
Lithium Effect (Overall) F= 5.524; df= 1,21; P= .029
produced by lithium is due to a lithium-related slowing of Lithium x Task Complexity Interaction F= 6.765; df= 1,21; P= .017
performance rather than a reduction of accuracy levels. Simple Task (M)-
The data from these performance tests did not allow us to Lithium vs Placebo NS
partial out whether lithium's effect was due to a simple Complex Task (Aa)- F=9.815; df= 1,17; P= .006
motor slowing or if it was related to an actual slowing of Mean response times in milliseconds for two conditions of
cerebral processing. information processing task comparing lithium and placebo main-
To answer this question, another study was designed to tenance periods in normal subjects.
identify the locus of lithium's action on cognition by using
a simple reaction time procedure and an information
processing task. The information processing task, de- cating that the lithium-induced delay in response time is
scribed by Posner,22 is a letter-matching task in which present during the more complex and higher level of
tachistoscopically presented letter pairs are presented and cognitive processing. This was confirmed by a post hoc
the subjects are asked to indicate whether the pairs are the examination of the lithium factor using the test on simple
same or different. Evidence is now available that suggests main effects, which revealed' that a significant lithium
that a systematic manipulation of the letter pair stimuli effect occurred only during the complex condition of the
can tap two different types and levels of cognitive process- information processing task.
ing. A simpler, swifter type of cognitive processing occurs
when the letter pairs are physically identical, but when Effects of Lithium on Circadian Rhythm of Sleep
non-physically identical pairs are presented, a more (N = 20)
complete, higher level and significantly slower rate of Each morning during the four-week study, subjects
processing occurs. recorded, on awakening, the precise times they had slept
There were no significant differences between the during the previous 24 hours. A standardized sleep log
lithium maintenance and placebo conditions in the simple questionnaire was used that divided the full 24-hour period
reaction time data. The subjects had slightly slower mean into 15-minute intervals. In addition, they filled out the
reaction times after 14 days of lithium administration Stanford Sleepiness Scale,24 which asks specific questions
(192.55 ms) than when receiving placebo (180.61 ms), but about the quantity and quality of sleep and level of
the difference was not significant (t = 1.59, NS). Data alertness on awakening. Complete and usable data was
from the information processing task were analyzed using obtained in 20 of the 22 subjects.25
a within-subjects, two-way analysis of variance with drug Data from the self-report sleep log were analyzed by two
condition (lithium vs placebo) as one factor and the cogni- statistical methods. First, the median point of nocturnal
tive processing task-complexity (physically identical vs sleep was calculated for each of the last seven nights
non-physically identical) as a second factor. The results are during the lithium condition and during the placebo condi-
graphed in the Figure. There is a task complexity effect, tion. These median points were averaged for each subject
which confirms Posner's original observations.22 An overall to obtain the mean midsleep points during lithium and
effect for lithium was found in which the mean processing placebo administration. Secondly, a best-fitting 24-hour
rate is slower during lithium administration. In addition, cosine curve was computed from the sleep log data for the
there was a lithium-by-task complexity interaction, indi- last seven days of lithium and placebo, using the least-

Arch Gen Psychiatry-Vol 36, July 20, 1979 Lithium in Normals-Judd 863

Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ by a Kings College London User on 05/16/2023


squares method. The peak of the fitted curve (the acro- ings of Bech et al,6 who reported no lithium effect on the
phase) was selected as an estimate of the peak of the Neiman-Mark scale. In addition, lithium did not affect
24-hour sleep-wake rhythm. Both methods agreed closely, performances on either of the psychometric instruments
although nocturnal awakenings were weighted somewhat used to assess the subjects' capacity for aesthetic apprecia-
differently. Daytime naps were included in the cosine fits tion and verbal fluency and creativity."20
but were ignored in the analysis of the nocturnal sleep Whereas these instruments difinitely are limited and do
medians. not evaluate the spectrum of creative efforts, nonetheless
During lithium administration, the subjects' mean the performance scores indicate that those capacities and
median point of sleep was approximately 14.2 minutes capabilities tapped by these instruments were not
later than the median point obtained during placebo influenced by lithium maintenance.
administration (correlated t test, t = 2.39, df = 19, Both the subjects themselves and their "significant
P = .027, two-tailed). Similarly, the 24-hour acrophases others" did identify a lithium-related impairment in cogni-
were 3.640 later during lithium as contrasted to placebo tive functions. These subjective impressions were con-
administration (correlated t test, t = 2.37, df = 19, firmed, since a deficit in performance behavior on a
P = .029, two-tailed), where 3.64° corresponded to 14.6 variety of cognitive sensory motor tasks was found. This is
minutes. Approximately identical significance values were in agreement with the reports of Heninger," Linnoila et
obtained using the nonparametric Wilcoxon Signed-Rank al,8 and Aminoff et al,12 in which similar deficits were also
test. Since all sleep midpoints and acrophases were noted. A closer examination of our data revealed that speed
grouped in the early morning hours, a linear model was of performance was affected by lithium, whereas accuracy
justified, and the hypothetically circular distribution of levels were not. Another study was initiated in an attempt
these data was ignored. Nine of 20 subjects reported some to identify where lithium's effect on performance was
daytime naps that averaged a total of 19 minutes a day. primarily being manifested. The data from the study of
Nap duration was not significantly different during the information processing suggest that lithium does affect
lithium and placebo conditions. No differences between the rate at which information is processed. This is
lithium and placebo were found on the following dependent supported by the fact that lithium did not alter perform-
measures: the amplitudes of the fitted 24-hour cosines (a ance on the simple reaction time test or the simple task of
measure of the peak-trough magnitude of the rhythms); the information processing procedure. Since the motor
the mesors of the cosines (a measure of total 24-hour sleep component of the response is essentially identical for each
duration); and the level of sleepiness or alertness on of the tasks and the lithium-related slowing was primarily
awakening in the morning. In summary, there was a small manifested during the more complex cerebral processing
but consistent delay (14.6 minutes and 3.64°) in the sleep- component, this would indicate that lithium exerts a
wake circadian rhythm of these normal subjects during the central effect by slowing the rate of cognitive process-
lithium condition. ing.
It is our feeling that this slowing of central cognitive
DISCUSSION processing could be one of the behavioral mechanisms by
The various possible caveats that this particular research which lithium exerts a therapeutic effect during manic and
design raises have been discussed in detail in other publi- hypomanic states in patients with bipolar affective illness.
cations2 3 and therefore will not be included in this discus- Since one of the more distinctive characteristics of the
sion. The data that have been derived from the group of state is very rapid, pressured, highly distractible speech
studies summarized and reported in this paper strongly and cognition, it is our hypothesis that lithium, by slowing
suggest that maintenance at therapeutic lithium levels cognitive processes, may specifically correct the cognitive
induces subtle but distinct alterations in certain psycholog- disordering that is characteristic of the manic patient. This
ical and cognitive functions of normal subjects. Specifical- will remain speculative until data are available in manic
ly, there is evidence that lithium produces consistent patients who are tested under these experimental condi-
changes in the subjective state of healthy normal male tions.
subjects. The feeling tones induced by lithium are those of The data from the daily self-report questionnaires on
lethargy and lassitude, anxiety and tension, and feelings of sleep are interesting but should be considered preliminary
loss of clearheadedness with an inability to concentrate. in nature. Specifically, it is clear that information of this
These findings agree with the anecdotal report of Schoul nature, derived from self-report rather than from direct
and with the study reported by Small et al.9 It is empirical and electrophysiological monitoring, warrants
interesting that these subjective changes were not observ- only the most tentative conclusions. Self-report data are
able to trained independent observers; they were notice- always subject to inadvertent errors of recall and are
able to individuals, blind to the drug condition, who were vulnerable to a variety of other influences that could not be
more intimately acquainted with the subjects. The impres- identified or controlled in this particular design. Despite
sions of the "significant other" were completely consonant these limitations, lithium did appear to induce a small but
with the self-rated changes as reported by the subjects significant delay in the sleep-wake circadian rhythm of
themselves. these normal subjects. Given the fact that environmental
No pattern of lithium-related changes were noted in the and social synchronizers should be equivalent in both the
two psychometric personality instruments used in our lithium and placebo conditions, it tentatively suggests that
studies (the California Psychological Inventory and Holtz- the sleep-wake phase delay may be related to lithium's
man Inkblot Technique). This is consistent with the find- effect on an endogenous biological oscillator. This would

864 Arch Gen Psychiatry-Vol 36, July 20, 1979 Lithium in Normals-Judd

Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ by a Kings College London User on 05/16/2023


appear to be more likely, since neither the quantity nor The author wishes to acknowledge the important contributions made by
the following collaborators to each of the individual studies that are
quality of sleep and napping were affected. Basic research summarized and reported in this communication: Bruce Hubbard, MD,
in Meriones, a rodent, and kalanchoe, a plant with a Leighton Huey, MD, David S. Janowsky, MD, and Daniel F. Kripke, MD.
prominent circadian rhythm phenomenon, has demon-
strated a lithium-related slowing of circadian oscillators.2' Nonproprietary Name and Tradmarks of Drug
Further, it has been suggested from studies in the few
bipolar depressive patients in a manic state that desyn- Lithium carbonate-Eskalith, Lithane, Lithonate, Lithotabs, PFI-
chronization of circadian rhythms may be a feature of this Lithium.
illness, in which free-running circadian rhythms are more References
rapid (eg, one cycle per 22 hours vs the normal 24-hour 1. Judd LL, Hubbard RB, Attewell PA: The effects of lithium carbonate
cycle).27 In addition, it is postulated that one of the upon subjective state changes induced by sodium pentobarbital. Psycho-
effects of lithium carbonate may be to slow the circadian pharmacol Commun 1:631-639, 1975.
oscillators in patients with bipolar affective illness.2' These 2. Judd LL, Hubbard RB, Janowsky DS, et al: The effect of lithium
carbonate on affect, mood, and personality of normal subjects. Arch Gen
preliminary self-report data on the sleep-wake circadian Psychiatry 34:346-351, 1977.
rhythm are consistent with these hypotheses and findings, 3. Judd LL, Hubbard RB, Janowsky DS, et al: The effect of lithium
carbonate on the cognitive functions of normal subjects. Arch Gen Psychia-
and if true, may delineate another behavioral mechanism try 34:355-357, 1977.
underlying lithium's clinical effectiveness. It is our feeling 4. Judd LL, Hubbard RB, Huey LY, et al: Lithium carbonate and ethanol
that the initial results are promising enough to warrant induced "highs" in normal subjects. Arch Gen Psychiatry 34:463-467,
1977.
further investigation using more traditionally objective 5. Schou M: Lithium in psychiatric therapy and prophylaxis. J Psychiatr
methods for the study of biological rhythms. Res 6:67-95, 1967.
These studies of lithium's effects in normal subjects 6. Bech P, Vendsborg PB, Rafaelsen OJ: Lithium maintenance treatment
of manic-melancholic patients: Its role in the daily routine. Acta Psychiatr
have enabled us to hypothesize two specific behavioral Scand 53:70-81, 1976.
mechanisms by which clinical changes are mediated in 7. Bech P, Thomsen J, Rafaelsen OJ: Long-term lithium treatment:
Effect on simulated driving and other psychological tests. Eur J Clin
patients with lithium-responsive disorders. It is our Pharmacol 10:331-335, 1976.
impression that lithium's effect on cognition and biological 8. Linnoila M, Saario 1, Maki M: Effect of treatment with diazepam or
rhythms, observed in normal subjects, would not have been lithium and alcohol on psychomotor skills related to driving. Eur J Clin
Pharmacol 7:337-342, 1974.
as easily identified in a patient subject sample as it was in 9. Small JG, Milstein V, Perez HC, et al: EEG and neurophysiological
a normal subject sample. Invariably patients afflicted with studies of lithium in normal volunteers. Biol Psychiatry 5:65-76, 1972.
major psychiatric disorders are so discomforted and 10. Demers RG, Heninger GR: Visual-motor performance during lithium
treatment: a preliminary report. J Clin Pharmacol 11:274-279, 1971.
distracted by the disease process itself that it often 11. Heninger GR: Lithium carbonate and brain function. Arch Gen
obfuscates the identification of subtle psychological mech- Psychiatry 35:228-233, 1978.
12. Aminoff M, Marshall J, Smith E, et al: Cognitive function in patients
anisms altered by the various psychotropic drugs used in on lithium therapy. Br J Psychiatry 125:109-112, 1974.
treating mental illness. We feel relatively certain that the 13. Marini JL, Sheard MH: Anti-aggressive effect of lithium ion in man.
lithium-related effects reported here could only have been Acta Psychiatr Scand 55:269-286, 1977.
14. Wechsler D: The Manual for the Uechsler Adult Intelligence Scale.
identified in the relative psychological quietude and stabil- New York, Psychological Corp, 1958.
ity of a normal subject who is not experiencing the 15. Reitan RM: Manual for the Administration of Neuropsychological
Test Batteriesfor Adults and Children. Seattle, Wash, The Neuropsycholo-
dysphoria and disruption of a serious mental disorder. We gy Laboratory, University of Washington.
are convinced that the study of the effects of each class of 16. McNair D, Lorr M, Droppleman L: EITS Manual for the Profile of
psychotropic drugs, administered in a dosage range and Mood States. San Diego, Calif, Educational and Industrial Testing Service,
1971.
schedule that approximates their clinical use, in normal 17. Gough H: The California Psychological Inventory Manual. Palo Alto,
subject samples, is a potentially rewarding and interesting Calif, Consulting Psychologists Press Inc, 1969.
direction for future clinical psychopharmacological re- 18. Holtzman WH: The Holtzman Ink Blot Technique. New York, Psycho-
logical Corp, 1958.
search. 19. Meier NC: The Meier Art Tests: Examiner's Manual. Iowa City,
Interpretation of the clinical relevance of Dr Judd's very interest- Bureau of Education Research and Service, State University of Iowa,
1942.
ing results in normal subjects requires some consideration of 20. Christensen PR, Guilford JP: Manual for the Christensen-Guilford
time-frames. The data in normals were collected during two weeks Fluency Tests. Beverly Hills, Calif, Sheriden Supply Co, 1957.
of lithium administration. Clinical experience with lithium 21. Moran LJ, Mefford RB: Repetitive psychometric measures. Psychol
suggests that subjective awareness ofcognitive effects is greatest at Rep 5:269-275, 1959.
22. Posner MI, Boies SJ, Eichelman WH, et al: Retention of visual and
the initiation of treatment, and diminishes with time. Thus, the name codes of single letters. J Exp Psychology Monograph 79, No. 1, pt 2,
marked functional effects of lithium noted by Dr Judd may not be 1969.
predictive of what patients experience. on long-term maintenance 23. Beatty J, Wagoner BL: Pupillometric signs of brain activation vary
treatment. Dr Judd makes the interesting point that the cognitive with level of cognitive processing. Science 199:1216-1218, 1978.
24. Hoddes E, Zarconi V, Smythe H, et al: Quantification of sleepiness: A
slowing seen in his study may provide a way of understanding the new approach. Psychophysiology 10:431-436, 1973.
beneficial effect of lithium in hypomania. However, a cognitive 25. Kripke DF, Judd LL, Hubbard RB, et al: The effect of lithium
slowing mechanism seems more difficult to invoke in understand- carbonate on the circadian rhythm of sleep in normal human subjects. Biol
ing the demonstrated antidepressant effect of lithium. (See the Psychiatry 14:545-548, 1979.
26. Engelman W: A slowing down of circadian rhythms by lithium ions.
report by Mendels et al, p 845 this issue.) Here again, attention to Z Naturforsch 28:733-736, 1973.
the time-frame may be important. The acute effect of lithium on 27. Tupin JP: Certain circadian rhythms in manic-depressives and their
hypomania is often rapid, that is, within afew days. On the other response to lithium. Int Pharmacopsychiatry 5:227-232, 1970.
hand, the acute antidepressant effects of lithium are often more 28. Moody JP, Allsopp MNE: Circadian rhythms of water and electrolyte
excretion in manic-depressive psychosis. Br J Psychiatry 115:923-928,
gradual, with maximal improvement often not occurring until the 1969.
fourth week. It is possible that the initial effects observed by Dr 29. Atkinson M, Kripke D, Wolf SR: Autorhythmometry in manic-
Judd attenuate, or even rebound at later times.-ED. depressives. Chronobiologia 2:325-335, 1975.

Arch Gen Psychiatry-Vol 36, July 20, 1979 Lithium in Normals-Judd 865

Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ by a Kings College London User on 05/16/2023

You might also like