You are on page 1of 5

Response Variability in the Visual Field: Comparison of

Optic Neuritis, Glaucoma, Ocular Hypertension,


and Normal Eyes
David B. Henson,1 Shaila Chaudry,1 Paul H. Artes,1 E. Brian Faragher,2 and Alec Ansons1

PURPOSE. To compare the relationship between sensitivity and response variability in the visual field
of normal eyes and eyes with optic neuritis (ON), glaucoma (POAG), and ocular hypertension
(OHT).
METHODS. Frequency-of-seeing (FOS) data were collected from four visual field locations in one eye
of 71 subjects (12 ON, 25 POAG, 11 OHT, and 23 normal), using a constant stimulus method on
an Henson 4000 perimeter (Tinsley Instruments, Croydon, UK). At each location, at least 20 stimuli
(subtending 0.5°) were presented for 200 ms at six or more intensities above and below the
estimated threshold. The mean and SD of the probit fitted cumulative Normal function were used
to estimate sensitivity and response variability. Cluster regression analysis was carried out to
determine whether there were differences in the sensitivity-log (variability) relationship between
the four groups.
RESULTS. Variability was found to increase with decreased sensitivity for all four groups. The
combined data from the four groups was well represented (R2 ⫽ 0.57) by the function loge(SD) ⫽
A䡠sensitivity (dB) ⫹ B, where the constants A and B were ⫺0.081 (SE, ⫾0.005) and 3.27 (SE,
⫾0.15), respectively. Including other statistically significant covariates (false-negative errors, P ⫽
0.004) and factors (diagnosis, P ⫽ 0.005) into the model increased the proportion of explained
variance to 62% (R2 ⫽ 0.62). Stimulus eccentricity (P ⫽ 0.34), patient age (P ⫽ 0.33), fixation loss
rate (P ⫽ 0.10), and false-positive rate (P ⫽ 0.66) did not reach statistical significance as additional
predictors of response variability.
CONCLUSIONS. The relationship between response variability and sensitivity is similar for ON, POAG,
OHT, and normal eyes. These results provide supporting evidence for the hypothesis that response
variability is dependent on functional ganglion cell density. (Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2000;41:
417– 421)

O ptic neuritis is a demyelinating condition which in its plied to single locations within the visual field, it has been
acute form gives rise to a series of symptoms, includ- established that variability increases as the sensitivity reduc-
ing abrupt loss of visual acuity, reduced contrast sen- es.12–15 Weber and Rau12 also investigated the relationship
sitivity, dyschromatopsia, ocular pain, and visual field loss. between sensitivity and variability in ocular hypertensive
Although most visual functions show substantial recovery (OHT) and normal eyes. They found that peripheral locations
within 6 months of the attack, some residual visual field loss is in the visual field of OHT and normal eyes, where sensitivity is
common.1 There is also evidence of retinal nerve fiber loss in lower, demonstrated more variability.
the majority of patients.2 Increased variability has been estab- Variability in the visual field of POAG eyes decreases with
lished both for the global visual field indices derived from increasing stimulus size.16 –22 This relationship has led to a
automated static threshold perimetry3 and at individual test hypothesis linking variability to functioning ganglion cell den-
locations using a frequency-of-seeing (FOS) technique.4 sity.22,23 The hypothesis is based on three assumptions: (1) that
An increase in visual field variability also occurs in primary individual ganglion cells give variable responses when repeat-
open angle glaucoma (POAG).5–11 Using FOS techniques, ap- edly stimulated, (2) that adjacent ganglion cells do not vary in
synchrony with each other, and (3) that there is pooling of
responses from ganglion cells. The hypothesis predicts that
From the 1Department of Ophthalmology, University of Manches-
ter; and 2Department of Organisational Health Psychology, University variability increases when the number of stimulated ganglion
of Manchester Institute of Science & Technology, Manchester, United cells is reduced, either by reduction of stimulus size or by a
Kingdom. reduction in the density of ganglion cells.
Supported by The Guide Dogs for the Blind Association, The
According to this hypothesis the variability versus sensi-
Wellcome Trust, and The Manchester Royal Eye Hospital Endowment
Funds. tivity relationship would be independent of the underlying
Submitted for publication May 21, 1999; revised September 21, cause of any ganglion cell loss. The variability versus sensitivity
1999; accepted October 5, 1999. relationship, therefore, would be similar in POAG and ON
Commercial relationships policy: N.
despite the significant differences in the mechanism of nerve
Corresponding author: David B. Henson, Department of Ophthal-
mology, University of Manchester, Royal Eye Hospital, Oxford Road, fiber damage and the nature (depth, location, and perma-
Manchester M13 9WH, UK. david.henson@man.ac.uk nency) of the visual field defects.

Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, February 2000, Vol. 41, No. 2
Copyright © Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology 417

Downloaded from iovs.arvojournals.org on 09/15/2023


418 Henson et al. IOVS, February 2000, Vol. 41, No. 2

The aim of this study was to establish whether there are


differences in the relationship between sensitivity and re-
sponse variability in ON, POAG, OHT, and normal eyes and
which other variables could be clinically important predictors
of response variability.

METHODS
Subjects
Data were collected from one eye of 12 patients with a history
of optic neuritis (5 men, 7 women), 25 patients with POAG (12
M, 13 F), 11 patients with OHT (5 M, 6 F), and 23 normal
subjects (13 M, 10 F).
The ON, POAG, and OHT patients were recruited from FIGURE 1. Typical frequency-of-seeing curves from a normal control
the outpatient clinics at the Manchester Royal Eye Hospital. eye (A) and an eye with POAG (B). The results from the POAG eye
The ON patients all had defective color vision (Ishihara), re- have a shallower slope (increased variability). The line drawn through
the data points is the probit fitted function along with its 95% confi-
duced visual acuity (VA; ⱖ 6/9, equivalent to 0.18 logMAR) and
dence limits (dotted lines).
a relative afferent pupillary defect. Nine patients had a history
of numbness. Data were collected from these patients once
their VA had returned to 6/18 or better. The interval between
continuous feedback on the selected intensities and current
diagnosis and data collection ranged from 1 to 169 weeks
responses, would interrupt the collection of data and adjust the
(median, 26 weeks). The POAG patients all had glaucomatous
intensities (minimum step size, 1 dB) and number of presen-
visual field loss (AGIS score: range, 1 to 19; median, 5) com-
tations to ensure that (1) the response range approached 0 and
bined with either (or both) glaucomatous changes the optic
100% seen; (2) data were collected for at least six intensities;
nerve head or a raised intraocular pressure (IOP; ⬎21 mm Hg).
and (3) there were a minimum of 20 presentations at each
OHT patients all had IOPs greater than 21 mm Hg on two
intensity. While the adjustments were being made, the subject
separate occasions, with normal disc appearance and no visual
was allowed a short rest. The presentation of stimuli was
field loss using program 24-2 and the Glaucoma Hemifield Test
randomized with respect to intensity and location. A typical
of the Humphrey Visual Field Analyzer (HFA; Humphrey Instru-
session lasted for approximately 30 minutes.
ments Inc., San Leandro, CA).
Normal subjects were recruited from hospital staff and Data Analysis
had no history of ophthalmic disease, a normal ophthalmic
examination, no systemic illness, and a visual acuity better than The FOS data from each test location were imported into the
6/9 (equivalent to 0.18 logMAR). The study was approved by statistical package for probit regression analysis (SPSS, Chi-
the Central Manchester Research Ethics Committee and fol- cago, IL). The mean and SD parameters of the fitted cumulative
lows the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. Informed con- normal function were used as estimates of sensitivity and
sent was obtained from each subject. All subjects underwent a response variability.
visual field test (HFA 24-2) before the collection of FOS data. Cluster regression analysis25 was used as observations
They were only included in the study if they fulfilled the HFA were independent between patients but not within patients.
reliability criteria (⬍20% fixation losses, ⬍33% false-positive Suitable adjusted (robust) standard errors were computed us-
errors, ⬍33% false-negative errors). ing STATA V5 (STATA Corporation, College STN, TX) to deter-
mine the following:
Test Locations 1. Whether there were differences in the relationship be-
FOS data were collected at four visual field locations during a tween sensitivity and variability for normal eyes and eyes
single experimental session. For the normal and OHT eyes, the with ON, POAG, and OHT.
locations were 12.7° from fixation along the 45, 135, 225, and 2. Whether the prediction of variability could be enhanced
315 meridians. For the ON and POAG eyes, one location was by the variables: diagnosis, eccentricity, patient age, fix-
chosen to lie in an area where sensitivity was within normal ation losses, and false-positive and -negative response
limits and, if possible, three locations in or adjacent to a rates. The fixation loss and false-positive and -negative
damaged area of the visual field. The damaged locations were response rates were estimated from the catch trial data
chosen on the basis of the HFA visual field test. of the 24-2 HFA visual field test.

Data Collection A backward elimination procedure was used in which insig-


nificant variables were removed successively from the model in
FOS data were collected using a modified program on a Henson
order of significance to identify those factors independently con-
4000 bowl perimeter (Tinsley Instruments, Croydon, UK).
tributing to the variance explained by the model.
Stimuli subtended 0.5° and were presented for 200 msec. After
input of the test locations, the program estimated the sensitiv-
ity at each location using the full threshold (4-2) strategy.24 For RESULTS
each location the program then selected five intensities that
straddled the estimated threshold in steps of 2 dB. At three Figure 1 gives typical FOS data along with the probit fit for a
occasions during each session the experimenter, who received normal and a POAG eye.

Downloaded from iovs.arvojournals.org on 09/15/2023


IOVS, February 2000, Vol. 41, No. 2 Visual Field Variability 419

FIGURE 2. Variability versus sensitiv-


ity for all four groups (ON, POAG,
OHT, and normal). The line drawn
through the data points is the best
fitting (least-squares) function.

Figure 2 gives a scatter plot of log response variability between the intercept of the POAG group and that of the other
versus sensitivity for all four groups (ON, POAG, OHT, and three diagnostic groups. Accounting for the rate of false-nega-
normal). As sensitivity decreases there is a dramatic increase in tive responses increased the proportion of explained variance
response variability. The data of all four groups were well by 2%. Neither stimulus eccentricity (P ⫽ 0.34), age (P ⫽
described by the simple model loge(SD) ⫽ Aⴱsensitivity ⫹ B, 0.33), fixation loss rate (P ⫽ 0.10), nor false-positive response
where parameters A and B are ⫺0.081 (robust SE, ⫾0.005) and rate (P ⫽ 0.66) had a significant effect on variability.
3.27 (SE, ⫾0.15), respectively. This model explains 57% of the Figure 3 gives the data from each group on separate axes
variance observed in our data (R2 ⫽ 0.57, P ⬍ 0.001). Separate along with the individual regression lines and the regression
regression analyses for each group produced the parameters A line fitted to the combined data. This figure highlights the
(slope) and B (intercept) shown in Table 1. agreement between the fit of the combined data and that of the
Multivariate regression analysis with backward elimina- individual groups.
tion was used to establish whether the data from the different
diagnostic groups could be represented by a single model and
to establish the contribution of covariates (eccentricity, age, DISCUSSION
false-positive and -negative rates, and fixation losses) to the
The results from this study agree with earlier work that reported
variance explained by that model. The slopes of the sensitivity–
an increase in the variability of visual field measures, where
variability relationship did not differ significantly between the
sensitivity had been reduced by either POAG or ON.3–15,26
four groups (P ⱖ 0.24). Table 2 shows the contributions of the
The relationship between sensitivity and response variabil-
different covariates and the diagnosis factor to the variance
ity was similar between the four groups of patients. Statistical
explained by the model.
analysis did not detect differences in the slopes of the sensi-
Inclusion of all covariates and the diagnosis factor in-
tivity ⫺log(variability) relationship between the four groups.
creased the coefficient of determination by only a modest
amount (R2 increased from 0.57 to 0.63). The significance of
the diagnosis factor (P ⫽ 0.005) is due to a small difference TABLE 2. Variables and Proportion of Explained Variance after
Inclusion into the Model

TABLE 1. Results of Cluster Regression by Diagnosis Variable R2 P (F test)

Group N A (Robust 95% CI) B (Robust 95% CI) Sensitivity 0.57 ⬍0.001
Diagnosis 0.60 0.005
Combined 71 ⫺0.081 (⫺0.091, ⫺0.071) 3.27 (2.98, 3.56) False-negative rate 0.62 0.004
Normal 23 ⫺0.066 (⫺0.101, ⫺0.031) 2.81 (1.64, 3.97) Fixation loss rate 0.63 0.10
OHT 11 ⫺0.078 (⫺0.109, ⫺0.047) 3.22 (2.33, 4.11) Age 0.63 0.33
POAG 25 ⫺0.098 (⫺0.112, ⫺0.085) 3.62 (3.24, 4.00) Eccentricity 0.63 0.34
ON 12 ⫺0.077 (⫺0.105, ⫺0.048) 3.28 (2.49, 4.07) False-positive rate 0.63 0.67

Downloaded from iovs.arvojournals.org on 09/15/2023


420 Henson et al. IOVS, February 2000, Vol. 41, No. 2

ty.28 –31 FOS curves for motion stimuli, using a line displace-
ment test, show an increase in response variability with in-
creasing motion threshold.32 An increase in variability with
loss in sensitivity also has been reported for frequency-dou-
bling perimetry.23 All these findings are in agreement with the
hypothesis relating variability to the density of functioning
ganglion cells. Some of the benefits resulting from targeting
sparse, vulnerable populations may be lost due to the increased
response variability associated with sparse populations.
Reliability parameters (fixation loss rate and false-positive
and -negative response rates), which were extracted from the
prior HFA 24-2 test, did not substantially increase the variance
explained by the model. Although the false-negative response
rate (P ⫽ 0.004) was a significant additional predictor of
variability, when included in the model, the explained variance
rose by only 2% (R2 increased from 0.60 to 0.62). The study’s
inclusion criteria of good patient reliability and the poor pre-
cision of estimates of patient reliability33 may account for why
these covariates did not have a larger effect on the total vari-
ance explained by the model.
In summary, the relationship between visual field sensitiv-
FIGURE 3. Variability versus sensitivity for all four groups (ON, POAG,
ity and response variability is similar in ON, POAG, OHT, and
OHT, and normal) separated on the basis of their diagnosis. The solid
normal subjects. This finding lends support to the hypothesis
lines drawn through the data points are the best fitting (least-squares)
function to the combined data set; the dashed lines are the best fitting that variability is dependent on functional ganglion cell den-
(least-squares) functions to the individual groups. sity. A similar relationship between sensitivity and response
variability may exist for other types of perimetric stimuli. The
increased variability makes it difficult to differentiate genuine
There was a statistically significant difference in the intercept changes in the visual field from noise and, therefore, has
between the normal, OHT, and ON groups and the POAG important clinical implications. Targeting sparse populations
group, which showed less variability (18%, P ⫽ 0.005). This will only be beneficial if it leads to an increase in the “signal-
difference might be explained by the greater perimetric expe- to-noise” ratio between defect and variability.
rience of the POAG group.
The pathophysiology of ON is very different from that of
References
POAG. In acute ON there is swelling in the area of demyelination
that affects the transmission of impulses along the ganglion cell 1. Beck RW, Cleary PA, Anderson MM, et al. A randomized, con-
axons. This can take the form of a total block, an attenuation, or trolled trial of corticosteroids in the treatment of acute optic
neuritis. N Engl J Med. 1992;326:581–588.
an extended refractory period. In certain fibers there is a break-
2. Steel DHW, Waldock A. Measurement of the retinal nerve fibre
down of the myelin sheath and a destruction of the ganglion cell layer with scanning laser polarimetry in patients with previous
axons, with subsequent proximal and distal degeneration. The demyelinating optic neuritis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry.
process can occur at any location within the optic nerve and 1998;64:505–509.
frequently involves the fibers that supply the fovea. In compari- 3. Wall M, Johnson CA, Kutzko KE, Nguyen R, Brito C, Keltner JL.
son, damage to ganglion cell axons in POAG is a slow chronic Long- and short-term variability of automated perimetry results in
patients with optic neuritis and healthy subjects. Arch Ophthal-
process that occurs at the optic nerve head and more frequently mol. 1998;116:53– 61.
involves the fibers at the superior and inferior poles. The similarity 4. Patterson VH, Foster DH, Heron JR. Variability of visual threshold
between the data from all groups suggests that there is a common in multiple sclerosis. Effect of background luminance on fre-
underlying process linking variability with sensitivity, which is quency of seeing. Brain. 1980;103:139 –147.
independent of the pathophysiology of the two diseases (POAG 5. Holmin C, Krakau CET. Variability of glaucomatous visual field
defects in computerised perimetry. Graefes Archiv Clin Exp Oph-
and ON). A common feature of these two pathologies is the loss thalmol. 1979;210:235–250.
of functional ganglion cell axons. 6. Werner E, Saheb N, Thomas D. Variability of static threshold
The results from this study support the hypothesis that a responses in patients with elevated IOPs. Arch Ophthalmol. 1982;
reduction in the number of stimulated functional ganglion cells 100:1627–1631.
is likely to lead to decreased sensitivity and a concurrent 7. Flammer J, Drance S, Zulauf M. Differential light threshold; short
increase in response variability.22,23 This reduction could come and long term fluctuation in patients with glaucoma, normal con-
trols, and patients with suspected glaucoma. Arch Ophthalmol.
about via a loss of ganglion cells, such as occurs in ON and 1984;102:704 –706.
POAG, or transfer of the stimulus to the peripheral visual 8. Wilensky JT, Joondeph BC. Variation in visual field measurements
field.12 This hypothesis also predicts the reported reduction in with an automated perimeter. Am J Ophthalmol. 1984;97:328 –331.
variability with an increase in the stimulus size.22 9. Katz J, Sommer A. Asymmetry and variation in the normal hill of
In short wavelength perimetry, blue stimuli are presented vision. Arch Ophthalmol. 1986;1986:104:65– 68.
on a yellow background. This type of perimetry was designed 10. Lewis RA, Johnson CA, Keltner JL, Labermeier PK. Variability of
quantitative automated perimetry in normal observers. Ophthal-
to isolate a sparse population of ganglion cells and, as a result mology. 1986;93:878 – 881.
of this, identify loss at an earlier stage.27 Short wavelength 11. Heijl A, Lindgren A, Lindgren G. Test-retest variability in glauco-
perimetry is associated with an increase in response variabili- matous visual field. Am J Ophthalmol. 1989;108:130 –135.

Downloaded from iovs.arvojournals.org on 09/15/2023


IOVS, February 2000, Vol. 41, No. 2 Visual Field Variability 421

12. Weber J, Rau S. The properties of perimetric thresholds in normal 23. Chauhan BC, Johnson CA. Test-retest variability of frequency-dou-
and glaucomatous eyes. Germ J Ophthalmol. 1992;1:79 – 85. bling perimetry and conventional perimetry in glaucoma patients
13. Olsson J, Heijl A, Bengtsson B, Rootzen H. Frequency-of-seeing in and normal subjects. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1999;40:648 –
computerised perimetry. In: Mills RP, ed. Perimetry Update 1992/ 656.
1993. Amsterdam: Kugler; 1993:551–556. 24. Bebie H, Fankhauser F, Spahr J. Static perimetry: strategies. Acta
14. Chauhan B, Tompkins J, LeBlanc R, McCormick T. Characteristics Ophthalmol. 1976;54:325–328.
of frequency-of-seeing curves in glaucoma in normal subjects, 25. Rogers W. Regression standard errors in clustered samples. In:
patients with suspected glaucoma, and patients with glaucoma. Stata Technical Bulletin Reprints Volume 3. Stata Corporation,
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1993;34:3534 –3541. Texas; 1994:88 –94.
15. Henson DB, Chaudry SJ, Artes PH. The relationship between sen- 26. Harms H. Role of perimetry in assessing of optic nerve dysfunc-
sitivity and variability in normal and glaucomatous visual fields. In: tion. Trans Ophthalmol Soc UK. 1976;96:363–376.
Wall M, ed. Perimetry Update 1997/1999. Amsterdam: Kugler; In
27. Johnson CA. Selective versus nonselective losses in glaucoma. J
press.
Glaucoma. 1994;3:S32–S44.
16. Wilson ME. Spatial and temporal summation in impaired regions of
28. Moss ID, Wild JM, Whitaker DJ. The influence of age-related cata-
the visual field. J Physiol. 1967;189:189 –208.
ract in blue-on-yellow perimetry. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1995;
17. Wilensky JT, Mermelstein JR, Siegel HG. The use of different sized
36:764 –773.
stimuli in automated perimetry. Am J Ophthalmol. 1986;101:710 –
713. 29. Wild JM, Moss ID, Whitaker D, O’Neal EC. The statistical interpre-
18. Choplin NT, Sherwood MB Spaeth GL. The effect of stimulus size tation of blue-on-yellow visual field loss. Invest Ophthalmol Vis
on the measured threshold values in automated perimetry. Oph- Sci. 1995;36:1398 –1410.
thalmology. 1990;97:371–374. 30. Wild JM, Cubbidge RP. Pacey IE, Robinson R. Statistical aspects of
19. Fellman RL, Lynn JR, Starita RJ, Swanson WH. Clinical importance the normal visual field in short-wavelength automated perimetry.
of spatial summation in glaucoma. In: Heijl A, ed. Perimetric Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1998;39:54 – 63.
Update 1988/1989. Amsterdam: Kugler; 1989:313–324. 31. Nelson–Quigg JM, Johnson CA, Casson EJ, Adams AJ. Long and
20. Zalta AH. Use of a central 10 degree field and size V stimulus to short term variability for perimetry of short wavelength sensitive
evaluate and monitor small central islands of vision in end stage (SWS) mechanisms [ARVO Abstract]. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci.
glaucoma. Br J Ophthalmol. 1991;75:151–154. 1990;31(4):S190. Abstract nr 937.
21. Takashima M, Nagata S, Kani K. Examination of receptive fields 32. Westcott MC, Fitzke FW, Crabb DP, Hitchins RA. Characteristics of
using automated perimeter. In Mills RP, ed. Perimetric Update frequency-of-seeing curves for motion stimulus in glaucoma eyes,
1992/1993. Amsterdam: Kugler; 1993:537–541. glaucoma suspect eyes and normal eyes. Vis Res. 1999;39:631–
22. Wall M, Kutzo KE, Chauhan BC. Variability in patients with glau- 639.
comatous visual field damage is reduced using size V stimuli. 33. Vingrys AJ, Demirel S. False-response monitoring during auto-
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1997;38:426 – 435. mated perimetry. Optom Vis Sci. 1998;75:513–517.

Downloaded from iovs.arvojournals.org on 09/15/2023

You might also like