You are on page 1of 14

The Journal of Sex Research

ISSN: (Print) (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hjsr20

Sexual Orientation Beliefs and Identity


Development: A Person-Centered Analysis among
Sexual Minorities

David Tierney, Elliot S. Spengler, Elena Schuch & Patrick R. Grzanka

To cite this article: David Tierney, Elliot S. Spengler, Elena Schuch & Patrick R. Grzanka
(2021) Sexual Orientation Beliefs and Identity Development: A Person-Centered
Analysis among Sexual Minorities, The Journal of Sex Research, 58:5, 625-637, DOI:
10.1080/00224499.2021.1878344

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2021.1878344

Published online: 09 Feb 2021.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 878

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Citing articles: 6 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=hjsr20
THE JOURNAL OF SEX RESEARCH
2021, VOL. 58, NO. 5, 625–637
https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2021.1878344

Sexual Orientation Beliefs and Identity Development: A Person-Centered Analysis


among Sexual Minorities
David Tierney , Elliot S. Spengler , Elena Schuch, and Patrick R. Grzanka
Department of Psychology, University of Tennessee

ABSTRACT
We conducted person-centered analysis of sexual orientation beliefs among sexual minorities to explore
potential relationships between these beliefs and sexual orientation, developmental milestones, race and
ethnicity, and attitudes about sexual orientation. We recruited participants (N = 416) via Amazon
Mechanical Turk who completed measures of beliefs about sexual orientation and attitudes about their
own sexuality, as well as assessments of mental health and age of sexual identity development milestones.
We conducted a latent profile analysis and found three distinct sexual orientation belief profiles, similar to
those previously observed in predominantly straight samples. We found that sexual orientation and race-
ethnicity predicted belief profile membership, with the latter exhibiting the most substantial effect. Of the
four identity developmental milestones tested, only the age of first same-sex attraction predicted belief
profile membership. Across profiles, members differed significantly in their mean endorsement of both
positive and negative attitudes regarding sexual orientation and psychological distress. Thus, the current
research suggests that sexual orientation beliefs are powerful constructs in the lives of sexual minorities,
with relationships to sexual orientation, other social identities, and mental health.

Introduction attitudes have illuminated key connections between essential­


ism and heterosexuals’ sexual prejudice (Haslam & Levy, 2006;
Historically, beliefs about sexual orientation were classified
Hegarty & Pratto, 2001), but important questions remain about
into three philosophical schools of thought: essentialist, social
how these beliefs function in the lives of sexual minorities
constructionist, and interactionist approaches (DeLamater &
themselves. The current study investigates how these beliefs
Hyde, 1998). Popular discourses, most notably the “born this
are related to sexual identity development, other social iden­
way” explanation of sexual identity (e.g., Lady Gaga, 2011), fall
tities such as race, and attitudes about sexual orientation
within the philosophy of modern essentialism, which consists
among sexual minority groups.
of biological, evolutionary, and other scientific explanations of
individual group differences. In contrast, social constructionist
approaches hold that linguistic, social, cultural, and political
Sexual Orientation Beliefs and Attitudes
processes determine experience and identity of sexual orienta­
tion, with queer theorists falling almost entirely within this A significant body of empirical research suggests that straight-
framework (Butler, 1990; DeLamater & Hyde, 1998). Finally, identifying people’s beliefs regarding sexual orientation influence
interactionist schools hold that sexual orientation is both their attitudes toward sexual minorities. Many researchers report
essential and socially constructed (DeLamater & Hyde, 1998). that straight people who believe in the naturalness of sexual
It is important to note that beliefs (i.e., the cognitive represen­ orientation are less likely to hold prejudicial attitudes toward
tations and “primary convictions about events, causes, agency, sexual minorities (Aguero et al., 1984; Ernulf et al., 1989; Haider-
and objects” that people use to organize their understanding of Markel & Joslyn, 2008; Haslam et al., 2002; Hegarty, 2002;
reality; Connors & Halligan, 2015, p. 2) about sexual orienta­ Hegarty & Pratto, 2001; Horn & Heinze, 2011; Mitchell &
tion are related to, yet distinct from, attitudes (i.e., evaluative Dezarn, 2014; Overby, 2014; Sakalli, 2002; Whitley, 1990).
representations of a given construct; Haddock & Maio, 2004) However, other studies suggest that the impact of essentialist
regarding sexual orientation. In other words, beliefs about beliefs about sexual orientation is best understood beyond beliefs
sexual orientation refer to what one thinks sexual orientation of naturalness. Extending Hegarty’s (Hegarty & Pratto, 2001)
is (e.g., where sexual orientation comes from, if it can change, critical work on psychological essentialism, Grzanka, Arseneau
etc.), while attitudes about sexual orientation refer to indivi­ and colleagues synergized strands of queer theory (e.g., Butler,
duals’ negative, positive, or other ambivalent evaluations of 1990) and critical social psychology (e.g., Hegarty, 2002) to
various sexual orientations. Thus, the study of these beliefs assess various beliefs about sexuality beyond its perceived
remains separate yet intimately connected to the study of innateness (Arseneau et al., 2013). In an early study with the
attitudes toward sexual minorities. Decades of social scientific Sexual Orientation Beliefs Scale, Arseneau et al. (2013) found
inquiry into the intertwined nature of these beliefs and four factors underlying sexual orientation beliefs, including

CONTACT Patrick R. Grzanka patrick.grzanka@utk.edu Department of Psychology, University of Tennessee, 1404 Circle Drive, Knoxville, TN 37996.
© 2021 The Society for the Scientific Study of Sexuality
626 D. TIERNEY ET AL.

naturalness/immutability (i.e., beliefs that sexual orientation is Sexual Orientation Beliefs and Identity
innate or biological), discreteness (i.e., beliefs that there are clear
While little research has explicitly explored the role of sexual
distinctions between gay and straight identities), entitativity/
orientation beliefs in sexual minorities, sexual minority iden­
homogeneity (i.e., believing members of a sexual minority
tity development models have always positioned beliefs and
group all have similar characteristics), and personal and social
attitudes about sexual orientation as interconnected and situ­
importance/informativeness (i.e., believing that sexual orienta­
ated within developmental (Cass, 1979; Troiden, 1989) and
tion is a means of understanding someone). Subsequent studies
sociopolitical contexts (D’Augelli, 1994). This research grew
found the simultaneous endorsement of the other belief types –
into quantitative analysis that focuses on the age of develop­
particularly regarding discreteness and informativeness – pre­
mental milestones over the course of the lifespan, including the
dicted heterosexism better than the examination of naturalness
examination of age of (a) first same-sex attraction, (b) first
beliefs alone (Agadullina et al., 2018; Fry et al., 2020; Grzanka
identification as a sexual minority, (c) first sexual activity
et al., 2016). These findings are consistent with previous findings
with a member of the same sex, and (d) first self-disclosure of
that discreteness beliefs predict prejudice (Haslam & Levy, 2006;
self-identification as a sexual minority to others (Floyd & Stein,
Hubbard & de Visser, 2015) and that essentialist beliefs more
2002). Several studies suggest the order of these milestones is
broadly predict greater stereotyping (Bastian & Haslam, 2006).
relatively consistent: participants first experience same-sex
Given that these findings suggest that essentialist beliefs about
attraction prior to all other milestones, subsequently self-
sexual orientation may actually underlie prejudice, they likewise
identify as a sexual minority, and then either engage in first
provide empirical evidence that the rhetoric of immutability can
sexual activity with a member of the same sex or disclose their
enact systems of oppression, including monosexism, cissexism,
sexual minority status to others (Calzo et al., 2011; Floyd &
and White supremacy (Cole et al., 2012; Diamond & Rosky,
Stein, 2002; Rendina et al., 2019). However, other recent work
2016).
shows that the order of these milestones may be subject to
Overall, the research suggests a significant relationship cohort effects whereby older sexual minorities are more likely
between sexual orientation beliefs regarding sexual orientation to engage in sexual behavior with members of the same sex
and prejudice among straight populations, yet very little before identifying as a sexual minority (Bishop et al., 2020).
research has explicitly examined sexual orientation beliefs While this may seem to move toward a bio-behavioral
and attitudes regarding sexual minority status among sexual approach, research consistently shows these markers are reflec­
minorities. The Sexual Orientation Beliefs Scale (SOBS) was tive of identity-centered processes and contextual factors alike
designed in collaboration with sexual minority groups as (Bishop et al., 2020; Calzo et al., 2011; Floyd & Stein, 2002;
a means of assessing these beliefs and was initially validated Katz-Wise et al., 2017; Kaestle, 2019; Martos et al., 2015;
with these populations (Arseneau et al., 2013). However, sub­ Rosario et al., 2011), as well as social identities such as race,
sequent studies using this measure to explore the relationship ethnicity, and sexual orientation (Bishop et al., 2020; Grov
between sexual orientation beliefs and attitudes predominantly et al., 2018; Rosario et al., 2004). Likewise, studies consistently
worked with straight samples (Agadullina et al., 2018; Fry et al., show that these milestones are associated with negative atti­
2020; Grzanka et al., 2016, 2020). To date, only two studies tudes such as internalized homonegativity (Legate et al., 2012;
have used this measurement tool exclusively with sexual min­ Rendina et al., 2019; Savin-Williams & Cohen, 2015), positive
ority groups to explore how these beliefs impact attitudes attitudes regarding sexual orientation and identity formation
regarding sexual identity (Morandini et al., 2017, 2015). (Rosario et al., 2011), and psychosocial outcomes (Friedman
These results were mixed as naturalness beliefs predicted et al., 2008; Rosario et al., 2012). Thus, there is a series of
lower levels of internalized homonegativity in sexual minority empirical findings suggesting that identity development is
women (Morandini et al., 2017), but not in gay and bisexual related to social identity, psychosocial context, attitudes
men (Morandini et al., 2015). As observed in straight popula­ regarding sexual minority status, and mental health outcomes.
tions, discreteness beliefs were found to be associated with However, no research has quantitatively explored how identity
internalized stigma across both groups (Morandini et al., developmental milestones are related to beliefs about sexual
2017, 2015). Entitativity beliefs were associated with increased orientation.
identity uncertainty and decreased homonegativity in gay and A collection of findings suggests that social identities such as
bisexual men (Morandini et al., 2015) but not measured in race, ethnicity, and sexual orientation are connected to sexual
subsequent samples with sexual minority women (Morandini orientation beliefs. In her foundational research, Diamond
et al., 2017). None of these studies measured all four types of (2008) suggested that experiences of sexual fluidity, including
sexual orientation beliefs nor how they co-occurred within changing sexual identity, may influence how sexual minority
participants in these sexual minority groups. These mixed women understand sexual orientation. Galupo et al. (2016)
findings and this relative dearth of research is surprising found that beliefs about sexual orientation are embedded
given that sexual orientation beliefs are associated with atti­ within social identity and may be sites of resistance to systems
tudes about sexual orientation, which have serious implications of oppression; this is in line with other research that suggests
for mental health including depressive symptoms (Rosser et al., sexual minority people of color and bisexual people may rely
2008), suicidal ideation (D’Augelli et al., 2001), and attempts to on essentialist beliefs to combat the erasure of their sexual
seek “conversion therapy” (Tozer & Hayes, 2004). In short, orientation (Alimahomed, 2010; Gonzalez et al., 2017; Logie
these beliefs could have health implications for sexual minority & Rwigema, 2014). Morandini et al. (2017) found that experi­
groups. encing non-prototypical sexual attraction for one’s identity
THE JOURNAL OF SEX RESEARCH 627

(e.g., experiencing attraction to men while identifying as les­ explicitly tested. Finally, what are the relationships between
bian) predicted discreteness and immutability beliefs in sexual sexual orientation belief profiles, attitudes regarding sexual
minority women, but the authors did not examine differences identity, and psychological well-being among sexual minori­
in these beliefs across social identities. While these aforemen­ ties? While previous studies have demonstrated relationships
tioned findings seemingly suggest that identity impacts beliefs, between sexual orientation beliefs and attitudes in sexual
McCormack and Savin-Williams (2018) also found that beliefs minorities, no prior studies have examined how the co-
about sexual orientation (e.g., endorsement of queer theory) occurrence of these beliefs impacts these attitudes and well-
may influence how some sexual minorities come to self- being.
identify, thereby suggesting a reciprocal relationship as well.
While these findings implicate relationships between social
identities and sexual orientation beliefs, work in this area has Method
been piecemeal. To date, no work has quantitatively explored Participants
the relationship between sexual orientation beliefs and social
identity with an exclusively sexual minority population. We recruited participants (N = 416) through Amazon’s
In summary, very little psychological research has explicitly Mechanical Turk (MTurk) service. To be eligible, participants
explored how sexual orientation beliefs are related to attitudes were required to (1) have a valid MTurk worker account, (2)
regarding sexual orientation among sexual minority people. be at least 18 years old at the time of the first survey, (3)
The only two studies in this domain conducted with sexual identify as non-heterosexual at the time of the first survey
minority samples demonstrated mixed findings regarding attempt, (4) reside within the U.S. as verified per MTurk
these beliefs and negative attitudes, and there has been virtually settings, (5) use an I.P. address that had not already com­
no research on how sexual orientation beliefs impact positive pleted the survey, and (6) pass a series of three attention
attitudes regarding sexual orientation. Furthermore, findings checks and four validity checks. The original sample consisted
in the field of sexual minority identity development suggest of 1,000 participants who clicked on the survey. Of these
a relationship between these beliefs and sexual identity over the participants, 385 identified as straight and were immediately
lifespan. Finally, some findings indicate that there are relation­ terminated from the survey and directed to a page that again
ships between sexual orientation beliefs and self-identified stated they did not meet inclusion criteria. After accounting
sexual orientation. for those participants, 615 participants went past the initial
screening question. Ninety participants were excluded
because they initially identified as straight at the time of
The Current Study first completing the survey screener and had already been
Accordingly, we sought to explicitly study sexual orientation previously terminated. Fifty-two participants failed at least
beliefs and their implications in sexual minority groups one attention check. Fifty participants were excluded because
recruited via Amazon Mechanical Turk. We used latent profile they answered that they had completed a developmental mile­
analysis (LPA), a person-centered statistical approach that stone at an older age than they currently held, which is
allows observation of patterns of responses across multiple impossible and indicated that their responses were invalid.
continuous variables; LPA analytically prioritizes these pat­ Seven participants were excluded because their data could not
terns over individual relationships between variables, hence be located due to their failure to correctly transpose their
the term “person-centered” (Zeiders et al., 2013). In our Question Pro ID into MTurk. In total, 199 participants were
study, LPA helped us to foreground participants as the sites excluded, leaving 416 participants in the final sample.
of analysis and identify subgroups (i.e., “profiles”) of different
sexual orientation beliefs. This method was advantageous as it
Measures
allowed us to explore how these beliefs co-occur within mem­
bers of our samples (Grzanka, 2016; cf. Silva, 2019). Following Demographics Questionnaire
the identification of these profiles, we explored how sexual We asked participants for basic demographic information.
orientation beliefs related to identity (e.g., sexual orientation; Participants were allowed to select multiple options, and such
race and ethnicity), sexual identity developmental milestones, responses were coded as the endorsement of multiple identi­
attitudes about sexual orientation, and psychological distress in ties. Participants were asked about their age (e.g., “What is your
an exclusively sexual minority population. age?”), race and ethnicity (e.g., “What is your race?”), gender
The study was driven by three questions. First, what are the (e.g., “What is your gender?”), sexual orientation (e.g., “What is
sexual orientation belief profiles among sexual minorities? your sexual orientation?”), geographic location (e.g., “What is
While Grzanka et al. (2016, 2020), and Agadullina et al. your current zip code?”; “How would you describe your cur­
(2018) found three belief profiles among majority heterosexual rent zip code?”) and level of education (e.g., “What is your level
samples, these profiles were never examined among exclusively of education?”). We also asked participants about their sub­
sexual minority samples. Second, how do sexual orientation, jective socioeconomic status on a scale of 1 to 10 (e.g., “Think
race, and age of sexual identity development milestones predict of a ladder with 10 steps representing where people stand in the
sexual orientation belief profiles among sexual minorities? United States. At step 10 are people who are the best off – those
Extant research suggests that these beliefs are influenced by who have the most money, the most education, and the most
sexual orientation, developmental trajectory, and race and respected jobs. At step 1 are the people who are worst off –
ethnicity, although these have never been quantitatively and those who have the least money, least education, and the least
628 D. TIERNEY ET AL.

respected jobs or no job. Where would you place yourself on demonstrated the lowest but acceptable internal consistency
this ladder? (indicate 1–10)”; Adler et al., 2000). (α = .69) of all the subscales.
With regard to gender, participants were asked to select all
the gender identities that applied to them from the following Discreteness. The Discreteness subscale measures the degree to
options, or were asked to write in their own gender identity: which a participant believes sexual orientations have distinct and
246 (59.1%) participants self-identified as men, 151 (36.3%) clear boundaries existing between groups. The 6-item scale
self-identified as women, 10 (2.4%) identified as non-binary, demonstrated acceptable internal consistency in the current
and 9 (2.2%) identified as transgender. No participants chose study (α = .72).
to identify as a gender not listed. Participants were also asked
to select all of the sexual orientation labels that applied to Entitativity. The Entitativity subscale measures beliefs that
them; 236 (56.7%) participants self-identified solely as “bisex­ sexual orientation is informative about an individual, uniform,
ual,” 136 (32.7%) as “gay or lesbian/homosexual,” 12 (2.9%) as and shares a quality across people of a given sexual orientation.
“asexual,” 9 (2.2%) as “pansexual,” 4 (1.0%) as “demisexual,” 1 The subscale contains 10 items and demonstrated optimal
(0.2%) as “queer,” and 18 (4.3%) endorsed multiple orienta­ internal consistency (α = .92).
tion labels. Once again, no participants chose to write in any
additional sexual orientation that was not otherwise listed. Personal and Social Importance. The Personal and Social
Participants were asked to check all of the following racial- Importance subscale measures the relative salience and impor­
ethnic identities that applied to them: 231 participants tance of participants’ sexual minority identity to participants’
(55.5%) identified as “White or European American,” 109 overall identity. The subscale contains 7 items and demon­
(26.9%) identified as “Black/African American/African,” 30 strated acceptable internal consistency (α = .75) in our sample.
(7.2%) identified as “Latino/a/x or Hispanic,” 15 (3.6%) iden­
tified as “Asian/Asian American/Asian Pacific Islander,” 7 Retrospective Recall of Sexual Identity Developmental
(1.7%) identified as “Native American,” and 19 (4.6%) identi­ Milestones
fied with more than one race and/or ethnicity. With regards We asked participants to recall their age at the time of four
to subjective socioeconomic status on a scale of 1 (lower sexual identity developmental milestones. Similar to Calzo
income) to 10 (higher income) (Adler et al., 2000), the mean et al. (2011), we asked the age (a) at which participants experi­
response was 5.18 (SD = 2.10), with 99.3% of participants enced their first same-sex attraction, (b) had their first sexual
responding. In terms of geography, 183 (44.0%) identified as experience with a member of the same sex, (c) first identified as
currently residing in an “urban” area, 165 (39.7%) in non-heterosexual, and (d) at which they first came out to
a “suburban” area, 67 (16.1%) in a “rural” area, and 1 (0.2%) others. These items were also used as a validity check in con­
in an “other” type of area. None had less than a “high school junction with participants’ demographic information.
education or equivalent,” 33 (7.9%) had a “high school degree Participants who recalled milestones as occurring later than
or equivalent,” 74 (17.8%) had “some college,” 53 (12.7%) had their currently identified age had their responses deemed inva­
“completed a 2-year college degree,” 195 (46.9%) had “com­ lid, through a process described below.
pleted a 4-year college degree,” and 61 (14.7%) had “com­
pleted at least some post-graduate education.” Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual Identity Scale (LGBIS)
The Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual Identity Scale (LGBIS) is a 27-item
Sexual Orientation Beliefs Scale (SOBS) measure with eight distinct subscales: acceptance concerns, con­
We used the Sexual Orientation Beliefs Scale (SOBS) Form 1, cealment motivation, identity uncertainty, internalized homone­
which was found to be reliable for sexual minority groups gativity, difficult process, identity superiority, identity centrality,
(Arseneau et al., 2013). The scale consists of 35 items on and identity affirmation (Mohr & Kendra, 2011). As this instru­
a Likert-type scale ranging from strongly disagree (1) to ment is only validated among people who identify as lesbian, gay,
strongly agree (5) measuring four domains of beliefs about and bisexual, it was only administered to participants who iden­
sexual orientation comprising (a) naturalness (e.g., Sexual tified as holding at least one of these identities. In total this
orientation is innate) (b) discreteness (e.g., Sexual orientation instrument was administered to 389 participants.
is a category with clear boundaries: a person is either gay/
lesbian, bisexual, or heterosexual”), (c) entitativity (e.g., Acceptance Concerns. The Acceptance Concerns subscale mea­
“Individuals with the same sexual orientation seem to be con­ sures a person’s concern with stigmatization due to their sexual
nected to one another by some invisible link”), and (d) perso­ orientation (Mohr & Kendra, 2011). The scale contains three
nal and social importance of identity (e.g., “It is possible to items and demonstrated acceptable internal consistency (α = .75).
know about many aspects of a person once you know her or his
sexual orientation”). Grzanka et al. (2016) documented evi­ Concealment Motivation. The Concealment Motivation sub­
dence of construct validity via hypothesized relationships with scale contains three items and measures concern and motiva­
related constructs, including homonegativity. tion to protect one’s privacy with regards to sexual minority
status (Mohr & Kendra, 2011). The subscale demonstrated
Naturalness. The Naturalness subscale measures the degree to good internal consistency (α = .81).
which a participant believes sexual orientation to be a natural
category – i.e., innate, immutable, stable across cultures, and Identity Uncertainty. The Identity Uncertainty subscale mea­
fixed at an early age. The subscale contains 12 items and sures the degree to which one is uncertain about one’s sexual
THE JOURNAL OF SEX RESEARCH 629

identity (Mohr & Kendra, 2011). The subscale contains four could then click a link to visit Qualtrics, where the measures
items and demonstrated excellent internal consistency were housed. The first page of the Qualtrics survey was the
(α = .91). informed consent form approved by the IRB, again stating that
individuals must be 18 and identify as a sexual minority to
Internalized Homonegativity. The Internalized Homonegativity participate. Similarly, participants were told in the informed
subscale measures the degree to which one rejects one’s own consent that compensation depended on passing a series of
LGB identity (Mohr & Kendra, 2011). The subscale contains validity checks, ensuring the truthfulness of their answers.
three items and demonstrated excellent internal reliability in Participants who consented to participate were then immedi­
this study (α = .91). ately directed to the demographic questionnaire page. If parti­
cipants met basic inclusion criteria (i.e., age > 17, identified as
Difficult Process. The Difficult Process subscale measures the something other than straight) at this point, they were then
degree to which a person perceives their development as an presented with the rest of the survey measures in a randomized
LGB individual as difficult (Mohr & Kendra, 2011). The sub­ order. Only participants who identified as gay, lesbian, or
scale contains three items and demonstrated poor internal bisexual for at least one of their sexual orientations were
reliability (α = .56). given the LGBIS to complete since this measure has not been
validated with other sexual minority groups. Following the
Identity Superiority. The Identity Superiority subscale mea­ completion of the study, participants were provided with
sures views favoring LGB people over heterosexual people. The their unique Response ID provided by Qualtrics and instructed
subscale contains three items and demonstrated good internal to enter this into the MTurk Portal if they desired to receive
reliability (α = .89). compensation. Participants who failed to pass validity checks
(e.g., reported they had first identified as a sexual minority at
Identity Centrality. The Identity Centrality subscale measures an age later than they currently identified), attention checks
an individual’s view that their LGB identity is central to their (e.g., “select agree here”), or who were previously screened out
overall identity (Mohr & Kendra, 2011). The subscale contains for identifying as straight or under the age of 18, were removed
five items and demonstrated acceptable internal reliability from the data set and were not compensated. Participants who
(α = .75). successfully completed the survey received 4.00 USD each for
their participation.
Identity Affirmation. The Identity Affirmation subscale mea­
sures the degree to which participants affirm their own LGB
identity (Mohr & Kendra, 2011). The subscale contains three Data Analysis
items and showed good internal consistency (α = .82). Preliminary analyses in SPSS 26 found that the data were
normally distributed (no items skewed > ± 1.5) and did not
Kessler Psychological Distress Scale (KPDS) exhibit significant kurtosis (no items ± 1.5; Westfall &
The KPDS is a 10-item questionnaire designed to measure Henning, 2013). Less than 1% of all responses were missing
a person’s global level of psychological distress as manifested for any given variable and we conducted expectance maximi­
through anxiety and depressive symptoms over the last 30 days zation per best practices (Schlomer et al., 2010). Following this
(Kessler et al., 2002). Items consist of a 5-point Likert-type examination of the data, we calculated the means of the SOBS,
scale ranging from 1 (none of the time) to 5 (all of the time). LGBIS, and KPDS. We dichotomized the categorical predictor
This scale demonstrated excellent internal consistency in our variables (e.g., social identities) in SPSS before transferring the
study (α = .95). data to Mplus Version 8 (Collins & Lanza, 2010; Muthén &
Muthén, 1998–2017). Race and ethnicity were dichotomized
Attention Checks into (a) White Non-Hispanic and Non-Latino/a/x participants
Embedded within the other measures were three validity check and (b) people of color, which included participants who
questions. These questions were simply to check if participants identified as Latino/a/x and Hispanic. Sexual orientation was
were indeed reading the material (e.g., “select agree here”). dichotomized into monosexual participants, who identified
solely as gay and lesbian, and non-monosexual participants,
Procedure with this latter category including participants who identified
as queer, bisexual, ace, pan, demi, and/or identified with multi­
The University of Tennessee, Knoxville Institutional Review ple sexual orientations.
Board (IRB) approved all materials and procedures used in the
current study. We relied upon and adhered to the American
Psychological Association’s (2012) ethical guidelines for Results
research involving human participants. All participants were
Multiple Sexual Orientation Belief Profiles
recruited through MTurk in October and November of 2019.
Participants were informed in the description of the study Following the preparation of the data, we exported the data in
provided directly via MTurk that they must identify as Mplus to conduct LPA. LPA is a step-wise process, with each
a sexual minority to participate in the study and be at least successive step representing a solution of adding a profile (k+1)
18 years old. Participants interested in completing the study and probabilistically comparing the likelihood of the current
630 D. TIERNEY ET AL.

step to the previous step. We relied upon the Bayesian Three-Proile Solution of Sexual
Information Criteria (BIC), the Adjusted Bayesian Orientation Belief Proiles
Information Criteria (ABIC), and the Lo-Mendell-Ruben 4.5
Likelihood Ratio Test (LRT) to determine the number of pro­ 4

files. Lower values on the BIC and ABIC indicate a better 3.5

Subscale Mean Scores


Naturalness-Only (n =
solution, while higher values on the LRT are indicative of 3
107)
higher probabilities that the solution is a better fit than 2.5
Multidimensional
a model with one less profile (k-1). Consistent with recom­ 2
Essentialism (n=148)
mendations, we first conducted the LPA to determine the 1.5
High Discreteness,
number of profiles without predictor and outcome variables 1
Entitativity, Importance
(Lanza et al., 2013). The results of the LRT, BIC, and ABIC for 0.5 (High DEI) (n= 161)

1 through 5 profile solutions are provided in Table 1. The LRT 0


Discreteness Entitativity Naturalness Importance
suggested that both a two- and three-profile solution were Sexual Orientation Beliefs Subscale
statistically plausible, although it suggested that a two-profile
solution was more likely. According to the LRT, four-profile Figure 2. Latent mean sexual orientation beliefs subscale scores for a three-profile
solution.
solutions and above were unlikely (p > .05). The ABIC and BIC
levels suggested that solutions with more than two profiles
better fit the data up to a five-profile. Given the improbability
of a four or five-profile solution per the LRT, we removed these Comparison of means determined that the three profiles were
from further consideration and more closely examined the significantly different from each other with regards to natural­
two- and three-profile solutions. It was clear that the LRT ness, F(2, 413) = 8.19, p< .001, discreteness, F(2, 413) = 80.64,
suggested a two-profile solution but left open a significant p< .0001, importance, F(2, 413) = 83.85,p< .0001, and entita­
possibility of a three-profile solution. The BIC and ABIC tivity, F(2, 413) = 85.840, p< .0001. Consequently, we decided
clearly suggested a three-profile solution as these values were upon a three-profile solution.
significantly lower in the three-profile solution as opposed to This three-profile solution replicated the profiles previously
the two-profile solution. We examined both profiles as solu­ reported with predominantly straight samples (e.g., Grzanka
tions and found that the three-profile solution offered et al., 2016, 2020). In keeping with these findings, we named
a theoretically meaningful and distinct solution when com­ the profiles in this case (1) Naturalness-only (NO) belief profile,
pared with the two-profile solution (see Figures 1 and 2). (2) Multidimensional essentialism (ME) belief profile, and (3)
High discreteness, entitativity, and, importance (high DEI)
beliefs profile (see Figure 2). Post-hoc Tukey tests demon­
Table 1. Profile fit statistics for 1- through 5- profile solutions for latent profile
analysis (LPA).
strated that there was no significant difference in the natural­
ness-only scores between the multidimensional essentialism
LRT
Profile BIC ABIC Lo-Mendell-Rubin (LRT) pvalue and high DEI profiles (p = . 96), but significant differences
1 3782.595 3757.209 – – were found between all other profiles on all other subscales.
2 3301.63 3260.378 494.712 0.000 The only alteration in naming with our profiles occurred with
3 3217.623 3160.504 110.496 .0139 the high DEI profile, which mirrored the high DHI profile (i.e.,
4 3173.176 3100.191 72.205 0.1733
5 3149.67 3060.816 51.937 .4477 discreteness, homogeneity, and informativeness, which are
BIC = Bayesian Information Criterion; ABIC = Adjust Bayesian information criter­ three subscales in SOBS-Form 2) found by Grzanka et al.
ion; LRT = Likelihood Ratio Test. (2016). As we used the SOBS-Form 1, which is validated for
use with sexual minority-only samples, two of these subscales
differed by three items.
Two-Proile Solution of Sexual Orientation
Belief Proiles Predictor Variables
4

3.5
After determining the number of profiles, we proceeded to
test variables that may predict profile membership (i.e., predictor
3
variables) using the 3-Step Method (Asparouhov & Muthén,
Subscale Mean Scores

2.5 2014a). The 3-step method effectively functions as a multinomial


2 logistic regression, whereby the predictor variables are treated as
Pro ile 1 (n=217)
Pro ile 2 (n=199)
the independent variables, and the referent class or profile is
1.5
treated as the dependent variable, such that the predictor vari­
1
ables are regressed onto each profile using one of the other
0.5 profiles as a control group. In this case, race, sexual orientation,
0
and developmental milestones were regressed onto each profile.
Discreteness Entitativity Naturalness Importance Race and sexual orientation were dichotomized and dummy-
Sexual Orientation Beliefs Subscale
coded (i.e., White versus nonwhite, and monosexual versus non-
Figure 1. Latent mean sexual orientation beliefs subscale scores for a two-profile monosexual). Mplus output provided the log of the odds of
solution. given class membership and a significance test value. These log
THE JOURNAL OF SEX RESEARCH 631

odds were then transformed out of their log function into odds Table 2. Mean comparison between sexual orientation belief profiles, attitudes
among gay, lesbian, and bisexual identifying participants (n= 389), and psycho­
and, subsequently, into their respective probability of predicting logical distress scale (n= 416).
profile membership per the variable in question using Profile 1: Profile 2:
a calculator; only statistically significant results are reported. Naturalness- Multidimensional Profile 3:
Only Essentialism High DEI
Race and Ethnicity Outcome Variable M (SE) M (SE) M (SE)
Acceptance Concerns 3.137a (.119) 3.689b (.097) 4.766c(.101)
People of color were significantly more likely (87.36% chance) Concealment 4.139a(.135) 4.203a(.105) 4.864c(.083)
to be in the high DEI group than the naturalness-only group, B Motivation
= 1.933 (SE = 5.158, p = < .0001) or the multidimensional Difficult Process 3.122a(.130) 3.295a(.105) 3.823c(.081)
Internalized 1.867a(.107) 2.728b(.117) 4.613c(.142)
essentialism profile (79.45% chance), B= 1.352 (SE = 3.329, Homonegativity
p < .001). Identity Uncertainty 2.034a(.106) 2.536b(.118) 4.653c(.132)
Identity Centrality 3.355a(.117) 3.77b(.082) 4.451c(.055)
Identity Affirmation 4.291a(.117) 4.259a(.092) 5.075c(.084)
Sexual Orientation Identity Superiority 1.612a(.087) 2.483b(.110) 4.614c(.120)
Monosexuality predicted membership in the naturalness-only Psychological Distress 2.11a(0.081) 2.309a (.095) 3.737c(.103)
profile over the multidimensional essentialism profile, while Means not sharing a subscript in a row indicate significant differences (p < . 01);
non-monosexuality predicted a higher likelihood of member­ Acceptance Concerns = LGBIS-Acceptance Concerns Subscale; Concealment
ship in the multidimensional essentialism profile over the Motivation = LGBIS-Concealment Motivation Subscale; Difficult Process = LGBIS-
Difficult Process Subscale; Internalized Homonegativity = LGBIS-Internalized
naturalness-only profile, B = 0.906 (SE = 2.766, p = .006). Homonegativity Subscale; Identity Uncertainty = LGBIS-Identity Uncertainty
There was a 71.22% chance a non-monosexual participant Subscale; Identity Centrality = LGBIS-Identity Centrality Subscale; Identity
would endorse the multidimensional essentialism profile over Affirmation = LGBIS- Identity Affirmation Subscale; Psychological Distress =
Kessler Psychological Distress Scale.
the naturalness-only profile. Monosexuality and non-
monosexuality did not predict membership in the high DEI
profile relative to either of the other profiles (p > .05).
results suggested that high DEI profile members endorsed
Developmental Milestones significantly higher levels of all positive attitudes, negative
The retrospectively recalled age of first identification as non- attitudes, and psychological distress than the members of the
heterosexual (M = 19.60, SD = 5.72, range: 5–48), first sexual naturalness-only profiles and the multidimensional essential­
experience with a member of the same sex (M = 19.75, ism profiles across all measures (see Table 2). Members of the
SD = 5.85, range: 3–50), first coming out to others as non- multidimensional essentialism profile endorsed higher levels of
heterosexual (M = 21.29, SD = 5.71, range: 6–43), and com­ acceptance concerns, internalized homonegativity, identity
bined means of milestone ages (M = 18.87, SD = 5.17, range: uncertainty, centrality, and superiority than members of the
3.5–41) did not predict sexual orientation belief patterns naturalness-only profile.
(p> .05). Those who recalled experiencing their first same-sex
attraction later in life (M = 16.12, SD = 6.64, range: 0–46) were
Discussion
significantly more likely to endorse beliefs consistent with the
high DEI profile than the naturalness-only profile, B = .202 This was the first study to explore sexual orientation belief
(SE = .054, p = .000) or the multidimensional essentialism profiles among an exclusively sexual minority sample. Our
profile, B = .103 (SE = .043, p = .035). People who reached results suggest that there are three distinct sexual orientation
this milestone later were also more likely to endorse the multi­ belief profiles among sexual minorities: the naturalness-only
dimensional essentialism profile as opposed to the naturalness- (NO) belief profile, the multidimensional essentialism (ME)
only profile, B = .100 (SE = .043 , p = .021). Conversely, belief profile, and the high discreteness, entitativity, and impor­
participants recalling first same-sex attraction at a younger tance (high DEI) belief profile. Previous studies have found
age were more likely to endorse the naturalness-only profile similar profiles among predominantly straight samples
relative to both other profiles. (Agadullina et al., 2018; Grzanka et al., 2016, 2020). The pre­
sent study replicated these previous results with a survey devel­
oped for use specifically with sexual minorities (i.e., the SOBS
Outcome Variables
Form 1) and with a different population from previous studies.
Finally, to explore the relationship between the belief profiles Together, our results suggest that the three sexual orientation
and attitudes about sexual orientation, we utilized the BCH belief profiles are both robust and reliable.
method to compare the mean levels of attitudinal endorsement The current study is the first to explore how social identity
across sexual orientation belief profiles (Asparouhov & and identity development predict sexual orientation belief pro­
Muthén, 2014b). Like the 3-Step method to latent profile ana­ files. Our results suggest that endorsement of these beliefs was
lysis described above, the BCH method reduces the likelihood predicted by both race and sexual orientation. Participants who
of profiles shifting (i.e., either the shape of the profile or the identified as people of color were over 75% more likely to
number of profile members) through the addition of error respond in a way consistent with the high DEI profile than to
from the introduction of distal variables into the model. The respond in a way that was consistent with either of the other
BCH conducts a Wald’s Chi-Square Test to compare the means profiles. There was an 87.36% chance of a White person
of these outcomes between the profiles while providing the responding in a way consistent with the naturalness-only pro­
latent means for each distal outcome per a given profile. The file as opposed to the high DEI profile. With regards to sexual
632 D. TIERNEY ET AL.

orientation, there was a 71.22% chance that participants who the older participants were when they experienced their first
identified solely as gay or lesbian would respond in a manner same-sex attraction, the more likely they were to endorse
consistent with the naturalness-only profile instead of the beliefs consistent with the high DEI profile relative to both
multidimensional essentialism profile. Thus, the naturalness- other profiles. Accordingly, identity development processes
only profile can be summarized as disproportionately com­ throughout the lifespan may predict the types of beliefs sexual
posed of White people relative to the high DEI profile. minority people have about sexual orientation. Interestingly,
Moreover, naturalness-only profile members were significantly the results suggest that people who reach this milestone later
more likely to identify as gay and lesbian (i.e., monosexual) in life are also more likely to simultaneously endorse beliefs in
than members of the multidimensional essentialism profile. In the discreteness, entitativity, and personal and social impor­
brief, these findings are significant in that they do suggest that tance of sexual orientation, potentially as a means of under­
beliefs about sexual orientation are racialized and related to standing their identity. The naturalness-only profile members
sexual orientation. Building upon existing literature suggesting reported first same-sex attraction younger in life as well as
beliefs may impact how sexual minorities choose to identify higher levels of psychological distress. Notably, the other
(McCormack & Savin-Williams, 2018), these results suggest developmental milestones were not significantly related to
that the navigation of these identities and beliefs is also rooted sexual orientation beliefs. Previous work consistently demon­
deeply within social systems. strates that the age of first same-sex attraction is the first
To understand these findings, we rely on Black feminist milestone achieved across sexual minority groups (Bishop
sociologists Dill and Kohlman’s (2012) framework of “strong et al., 2020; Calzo et al., 2011; Floyd & Stein, 2002; Rendina
intersectionality,” which challenges us to examine the ways in et al., 2019). It is possible that the age of this early experience
which systems of power, privilege, and oppression interweave of sexual identity becomes foundational for how sexual min­
to shape experience (Crenshaw, 1991; Moradi & Grzanka, ority groups understand sexual identity broadly as they age.
2017). Sexual minority people of color and non-monosexual Experiences of the other milestones, which typically occur
people, particularly bisexual people, are both groups whose later, may be less informative to subsequent beliefs regarding
identities are systematically deemed invisible and are more sexual orientation; this may explain why these other mile­
likely to experience erasure (American Psychological stones were not found to significantly predict sexual orienta­
Association, 2012; Dworkin, 2001). Consistent with these tion beliefs. Conversely, it is also possible that sexual
experiences, participants of color and non-monosexual parti­ minorities’ sexual orientation beliefs influence how they ret­
cipants were more likely to have response patterns with rospectively recall their own identity development. Given that
higher levels of essentialist beliefs across three of the four these identity development trajectories are broad, diverse, and
belief domains (i.e., discreteness, entitativity, and impor­ change over time (Bishop et al., 2020; Kaestle, 2019), and that
tance). These results can be understood through the lens of causation cannot be determined using cross-sectional data,
power in that systemic erasure of these groups functionally future longitudinal work is needed to explore the relation­
essentializes qualities of these identities (Foucault, 1990). In ships between these sexual orientation beliefs, identity, and
other words, when facing invisibility, essentialist beliefs may other variables over time. Nonetheless, to the knowledge of
serve the unique function of validating the identities of sexual the authors, this represents a novel finding to the literature,
minorities of color and non-monosexual people. Ryazanov demonstrating that at least one identity developmental mile­
and Christenfeld (2018) described how essentialist beliefs stone is related to how sexual minority groups understand
may also hold strategic value within minority populations sexual orientation more broadly.
as a means of fostering positive identity development.
Qualitative research supports this interpretation. For exam­
ple, Gonzalez et al. (2017) found that bisexual people char­ Outcome Variables
acterize their sexuality as enduring and distinctly definable in The current study’s most novel contribution may be in demon­
attempts to combat erasure. Research also shows that sexual strating the multitude of relationships between sexual orienta­
minority people of color turn experiences of erasure into tion beliefs (i.e., the ontological cognitive representations),
empowerment via the creation and construction of their with attitudes (i.e., global representations encompassing affec­
own identities (Alimahomed, 2010; Logie & Rwigema, tive, cognitive and behavioral domains) about sexual orienta­
2014). Thus, we can view participants’ endorsement of the tion and psychological distress. Previous work (with a variable-
multidimensional essentialism profile and the high DEI pro­ centered statistical approach) demonstrated that sexual orien­
file as potentially reflective of resistance to systems of power tation beliefs predict internalized homonegativity and identity
that erase the existence of these groups. Consequently, the uncertainty attitudes, albeit inconsistently (Morandini et al.,
results suggest essentialist beliefs about sexual orientation 2017, 2015). Ours is the first study to demonstrate that sexual
may hold strategic value for sexual minority people of color orientation belief profiles can predict all eight sexual orienta­
and non-monosexual people. tion attitudes measured within the widely used Lesbian, Gay,
Per the current study, a participant recalling first same-sex Bisexual, Identity Scale among sexual minorities (LGBIS; Mohr
attraction later in life predicted sexual orientation beliefs that & Kendra, 2011). Furthermore, while previous studies have
were consistent with the high DEI and multidimensional proposed a link between these attitudes and psychological
essentialism profiles relative to the naturalness-only profile. distress, our data exhibit a significant association between
These effects were most substantial for the high DEI profile, as sexual orientation belief profiles themselves and psychological
THE JOURNAL OF SEX RESEARCH 633

distress. The profiles, of course, account for how individuals high DEI profile indicated the highest level of positive atti­
hold multiple beliefs simultaneously. tudes, as well. These findings are not completely unfounded,
With regard to endorsement of negative sexual orientation as previous research has demonstrated a positive association
attitudes, these attitudes followed a nearly step-wise function between these attitudes and psychological distress (Cramer
across sexual orientation belief profiles. The naturalness-only et al., 2017). Members of this profile also endorsed the
profile members endorsed the lowest levels of internalized highest levels of internalized homonegativity, which is asso­
homonegativity and identity uncertainty. The high DEI profile ciated with psychological distress (Rosser et al., 2008). These
members endorsed the highest levels of negative attitudes of findings are the first of their kind in demonstrating that
each of these profiles. The multidimensional essentialism pro­ beliefs about sexual orientation, not just attitudes regarding
file members endorsed higher levels of these than the natural­ it, may be related to psychological distress. These results
ness-only profile but endorsed significantly lower levels of may suggest that the potentially negative impact of sexual
these attitudes than the high DEI profile members. It is through orientation beliefs occurs when discreteness, entitativity, and
this step-wise nature that the current study can be seen as one importance beliefs about sexual orientation are simulta­
of a growing number to suggest that naturalness beliefs regard­ neously endorsed at high levels (e.g., mean scores above
ing sexual orientation are not the sexual orientation beliefs that the midpoint).
primarily determine negative attitudes about sexual orientation It is also possible this relationship is more complicated, and
in sexual minorities (Morandini et al., 2017, 2015). The multi­ may be reflective of sexual minorities working to navigate
dimensional essentialism and the high DEI profile members systems of power. The high DEI group is disproportionately
did not endorse significantly different levels of naturalness composed of sexual minority people of color, who often face
beliefs, yet their profiles were differentially predictive of inter­ invalidation of their identities in ways that White sexual min­
nalized homonegativity and identity uncertainty. While the ority people do not (Ghabrial, 2017; Ramirez-Valles, 2007).
naturalness-only profile members endorsed the lowest levels This erasure can be understood as what Burke (1991) called
of internalized homonegativity and identity uncertainty in our identity interruptions, which are moments wherein a minority
data, members of this profile also had significantly lower levels receives interpersonal feedback that is incompatible with their
of essentialist beliefs across the other three subscales. Taken concept of the self. Our results suggest that in the face of
together, this suggests that the impact of naturalness sexual identity interruptions, sexual minorities – particularly sexual
orientation beliefs on negative attitudes regarding sexual orien­ minority people of color – may strategically utilize higher levels
tation is small and potentially better accounted for by other co- of co-occurring discreteness, entitativity, and importance
occurring essentialist beliefs (see also Grzanka et al., 2016, beliefs as a means of facilitating a sense of group identity.
2020). These results may also be consistent with the rejection-
The positive attitudes regarding sexual orientation appear identification model, which holds experiences of prejudice
to, in part, follow the same step-wise function. Members of and minority stress are associated with negative impacts on
the high DEI profile endorsed higher levels of identity super­ psychological well-being, and that this subsequent stronger
iority and centrality than the multidimensional essentialism identification with a minority group can act as a protective
profile, which endorsed higher levels of the attitudes than the buffer (Branscombe et al., 1999). While offering potential ben­
naturalness-only profile. With respect to identity affirmation, efit in the face of discrimination, these essentialist beliefs may
the multidimensional essentialism and naturalness-only pro­ reduce flexibility (Ryazanov & Christenfeld, 2018), which may
files did not significantly differ from each other. Still, both the make it more difficult for sexual minority people of color in
naturalness-only and multidimensional essentialism profile particular to cope with discrimination (Bhambhani et al., 2020;
members endorsed a lower mean level of these attitudes Brewster et al., 2013; Singh & O’Brien, 2020).
than the high DEI profile. These results might suggest that
there are well-being benefits conferred with regards to the
Implications
high DEI and multidimensional essentialism profiles. One
possible explanation for this is that essentialist beliefs may The current study has a number of implications. From the
undergird the development of attitudes regardless of valence. perspective of advocacy, these results suggest that activists
Another possibility is that these attitudes may reflect the and advocates should not center any particular set of beliefs
process of stronger group identification following greater regarding sexual orientation as a means of achieving equality
experiences of exclusion and prejudice in community samples (Epstein, 1987). Our findings suggest that these beliefs are
(Cramer et al., 2017), and these beliefs may be associated with multi-functional and come with both costs and benefits
this process. More broadly, given that previous research has depending upon the social position of those who hold them.
demonstrated that positive attitudes regarding sexual orienta­ Clinically, this work suggests that practitioners move away
tion are associated with developmental milestones among from deterministic views of sexual orientation beliefs and
sexual minorities (Rosario et al., 2006), these results point to their impact on attitudes about sexual orientation among sex­
the possibility that beliefs about sexual orientation may be ual minorities. When working with sexual minority clients,
acting as a lurking variable. clinicians should be mindful of how certain beliefs that may
Members of the high DEI profile endorsed significantly seemingly underlie internalized stigma may also facilitate
greater psychological distress than members of either of the stronger in-group identification. Future research that explicitly
other profiles. This was somewhat surprising given that the centers sexual minority people of color and bisexual people’s
634 D. TIERNEY ET AL.

beliefs regarding sexual orientation is necessary to better elu­ reliability, we believe that our screening procedures alongside
cidate these relationships. this platform provided quality data that might not otherwise be
possible through other methods. However, our reliance upon
cross-sectional data makes it impossible to determine causality.
Limitations
Future work should incorporate longitudinal measures as
Our methods necessitated the dichotomization of race and a means of determining how these relationships occur over
sexual orientation as a consequence of the 3-Step method’s time, particularly because the present findings suggest that
analytic constraints. Due to this dichotomization, we lost sub­ sexual orientation beliefs may be better understood in devel­
stantial amounts of variance that would allow for potentially opmental relief than in temporal isolation.
more nuanced understanding within our data and led us to
make generalizations and assumptions within our analytic
process that we ourselves do not hold as researchers. For Acknowledgments
example, while we analyzed the experiences of people of
The authors wish to thank Katharine H. Zeiders, Joe Miles, and Kirsten
color, certain racial and ethnic identities were not well repre­ Gonzalez for their feedback and consultation on an earlier draft.
sented within our sample, and, therefore, these data may not be
generalizable across racial and ethnic minority groups in the
U.S. Still, the robustness of the current findings suggests that Disclosure Statement
these questions would benefit from future examination utiliz­
ing other methods that could account for these differences. No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.
Our study would also benefit from additional attitudinal
measures that are more inclusive of sexual minorities who do
not identify as lesbian, gay, or bisexual, as well as investigation Funding
of potential confounding variables associated with the LGBIS. This work was supported by a 2019 Dissertation Research Grant from the
Because the LGBIS is not normed for use with respondents American Psychological Association and a Student/Faculty Research
who do not identify as lesbian, gay, or bisexual, we did not Award from The Graduate School, University of Tennessee, Knoxville.
assess their attitudes about sexual orientation. This is
a significant limitation of the study because our results exclude
the perspectives of those whose sexual orientation identity does ORCID
not correspond with at least one of these three labels. This is David Tierney http://orcid.org/0000-0002-2942-2405
particularly consequential given that research shows that these Elliot S. Spengler http://orcid.org/0000-0002-6039-4019
sexual identity labels are increasingly common (Galupo et al., Patrick R. Grzanka http://orcid.org/0000-0002-7364-9965
2016; Watson, 2019). Additional instrumentation could help to
elucidate if our findings generalize to these groups. We also References
found that the identity superiority attitude subscale had
a positive relationship with psychological distress. While this Adler, N. E., Epel, E., Castellazzo, G., & Ickovics, J. (2000). Relationship of
mirrors previous findings from Cramer et al. (2017) that sug­ subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiolo­
gical functioning: Preliminary data in healthy white women. Health
gest the scale likely captures in-group identification, additional Psychology, 19, 586–592. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278–6133.19.6.586
measures intended to directly capture in-group identification Agadullina, E. R., Lovakov, A. V., & Malysheva, N. G. (2018). Essentialist
are necessary to verify these findings. Furthermore, as this was beliefs and social distance towards gay men and lesbian women:
exploratory, we did not directly measure experiences of erasure A latent profile analysis. Psychology & Sexuality, 9(4), 288–304.
or minority stress, two constructs that we now hypothesize https://doi.org/10.1080/19419899.2018.148876442-55
Aguero, J. E., Bloch, L., & Byrne, D. (1984). The relationships among
could explain some of our results. We believe that given the sexual beliefs, attitudes, experience, and homophobia. Journal of
strength and overall consistency within these findings, this line Homosexuality, 10(1–2), 95–107. https://doi.org/10.1300/j082v10n01_
of research warrants further study. 07
Finally, there are possible issues regarding our data collec­ Alimahomed, S. (2010). Thinking outside the rainbow: Women of color
tion. One potential weakness of the current method was our redefining queer politics and identity. Social Identities, 16(2), 151–168.
https://doi.org/10.1080/13504631003688849
reliance upon distributed data collection, and that we had to American Psychological Association. (2012). Guidelines for psychological
remove a significant number of participants as a result of practice with lesbian, gay, and bisexual clients. American Psychologist,
inadequate quality responses. While relying upon distributed 67(1), 10–42. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024659
data collection comes with several drawbacks, it also comes Arseneau, J. R., Grzanka, P. R., Miles, J. R., & Fassinger, R. E. (2013).
with a number of advantages. MTurk allows for a more repre­ Development and initial validation of the Sexual Orientation Beliefs
Scale (SOBS). Journal of Counseling Psychology, 60(3), 407–420. https://
sentative sample of the current U.S. population than relying doi.org/10.1037/a0032799
upon sampling through a college campus and other Internet Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2014a). Auxiliary variables in mixture
sampling methods and provides quality data (Buhrmester et al., modeling: Three-step approaches using Mplus. Structural Equation
2016). Furthermore, previous work has shown that MTurk Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 21(3), 329–341. https://doi.org/
provides a means for collecting representative samples when 10.1080/10705511.2014.915181
Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2014b). Auxiliary variables in mixture
working with sexual minority populations (Choi et al., 2017; modeling: Using the BCH method in Mplus to estimate a distal out­
Israel et al., 2019). Given the advantages of this platform of data come model and an arbitrary secondary model. Mplus Web Notes,
collection with regards to representation and demonstrated 21(2), 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2014.915181
THE JOURNAL OF SEX RESEARCH 635

Bastian, B., & Haslam, N. (2006). Psychological essentialism and stereo­ Diamond, L. M., & Rosky, C. J. (2016). Scrutinizing immutability: Research
type endorsement. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(2), on sexual orientation and US legal advocacy for sexual minorities. Journal
228–235. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2005.03.003 of Sex Research, 53(4–5), 363–391. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2016.
Bhambhani, Y., Flynn, M. K., Kellum, K. K., & Wilson, K. G. (2020). The 1139665
role of psychological flexibility as a mediator between experienced Dill, B. T., & Kohlman, M. H. (2012). Intersectionality: A transformative
sexual racism and psychological distress among men of color who paradigm in feminist theory and social justice. In S. N. Hesse-Biber
have sex with men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 49(2), 711–720. (Ed.), The handbook of feminist research: Theory and praxis (2nd ed.,
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-018-1269-5 pp. 154–174). Sage Publications.
Bishop, M. D., Fish, J. N., Hammack, P. L., & Russell, S. T. (2020). Sexual Dworkin, S. H. (2001). Treating the bisexual client. Journal of Clinical
identity development milestones in three generations of sexual minor­ Psychology, 57(5), 671–680. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.1036
ity people: A national probability sample. Developmental Psychology, Epstein, S. (1987). Gay politics, ethnic identity: The limits of social
56(11), 2177–2193. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001105 constructionism. Socialist Review, 93(3), 9–54.
Branscombe, N. R., Schmitt, M. T., & Harvey, R. D. (1999). Perceiving Ernulf, K. E., Innala, S. M., & Whitam, F. (1989). Biological explana­
pervasive discrimination among African Americans: Implications for tion, psychological explanation, and tolerance of homosexuals: A
group identification and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social cross-national analysis of beliefs and attitudes. Psychological
Psychology, 77(1), 135–149. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.77.1.135 Reports, 65(3), 1003–1010. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1989.65.3.
Brewster, M. E., Moradi, B., DeBlaere, C., & Velez, B. L. (2013). Navigating the 1003
borderlands: The roles of minority stressors, bicultural self-efficacy, and Floyd, F. J., & Stein, T. S. (2002). Sexual orientation identity formation
cognitive flexibility in the mental health of bisexual individuals. Journal of among gay, lesbian, and bisexual youths: Multiple patterns of milestone
Counseling Psychology, 60(4), 543–556. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033224 experiences. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 12(2), 167–191.
Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2016). Amazon’s Mechanical https://doi.org/10.1111/1532-7795.00030
Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality data? In Foucault, M. (1990). The history of sexuality: Vol. 1. An introduction.
A. E. Kazdin (Ed.), Methodological issues and strategies in clinical Vintage.
research (pp. 133–139). American Psychological Association. https:// Friedman, M. S., Marshal, M. P., Stall, R., Cheong, J. W., & Wright, E. R.
doi.org/10.1037/14805-009 (2008). Gay-related development, early abuse and mental health out­
Burke, P. (1991). Identity processes and social stress. American comes among gay males. AIDS and Behavior, 12(6), 891–902. https://
Sociological Review, 56(6), 836–849. https://doi.org/10.2307/2096259 doi.org/10.1007/s10461-007-9319-3
Butler, J. (1990). Gender trouble. Routledge. Fry, K. M., Grzanka, P. R., Miles, J. R., & DeVore, E. N. (2020). Is
Calzo, J. P., Antonucci, T. C., Mays, V. M., & Cochran, S. D. (2011). essentialism essential? Reducing homonegative prejudice by targeting
Retrospective recall of sexual orientation identity development among diverse sexual orientation beliefs. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 49(5),
gay, lesbian, and bisexual adults. Developmental Psychology, 47(6), 1725–1739. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-020-01706-x
1658–1673. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025508 Gaga, L. (2011). Born this way. On Born This Way [CD]. Interscope
Cass, V. C. (1979). Homosexual identity formation: A theoretical model. Records.
Journal of Homosexuality, 4(3), 219–235. https://doi.org/10.1300/ Galupo, M. P., Henise, S. B., & Mercer, N. L. (2016). “The labels don’t
j082v04n03_01 work very well”: Transgender individuals’ conceptualizations of sexual
Choi, A. Y., Merrill, C. R. S., & Israel, T. (2017). Factor structure of the orientation and sexual identity. International Journal of
Internalized Homonegativity Inventory (IHNI). Psychology of Sexual Transgenderism, 17(2), 93–104. https://doi.org/10.1080/15532739.
Orientation and Gender Diversity, 4(4), 491–498. https://doi.org/10. 2016.1189373
1037/sgd0000245 Ghabrial, M. A. (2017). “Trying to figure out where we belong”. Narratives
Cole, E. R., Avery, L. R., Dodson, C., & Goodman, K. D. (2012). Against of racialized sexual minorities on community, identity, discrimination,
nature: How arguments about the naturalness of marriage privilege and health. Sexuality Research and Social Policy, 14(1), 42–55. https://
heterosexuality. Journal of Social Issues, 68(1), 46–62. https://doi.org/ doi.org/10.1007/s13178-016-0229-x
10.1111/j.1540-4560.2012.01735.x Gonzalez, K. A., Ramirez, J. L., & Galupo, M. P. (2017). “I was and still
Collins, L. M., & Lanza, S. T. (2010). Latent class and latent transition am”: Narratives of bisexual marking in the #StillBisexual campaign.
analysis for the social, behavioral, and health sciences. Wiley. Sexuality & Culture, 21(2), 493–515. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12119-
Connors, M. H., & Halligan, P. W. (2015). A cognitive account of belief: A 016-9401-y
tentative roadmap. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(1588), 1–14. https://doi. Grov, C., Rendina, H. J., & Parsons, J. T. (2018). Birth cohort differences in
org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01588 sexual identity development milestones among HIV-negative gay and
Cramer, R. J., Burks, A. C., Golom, F. D., Stroud, C. H., & Graham, J. L. bisexual men in the United States. Journal of Sex Research, 55(8),
(2017). The Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual Identity Scale: Factor analytic 984–994. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2017.1375451
evidence and associations with health and well-being. Measurement Grzanka, P. R. (2016). Queer survey research and the ontological dimen­
and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 50(1–2), 71–88. sions of heterosexism. WSQ: Women’s Studies Quarterly, 44(3),
https://doi.org/10.1177/0748175616664014 131–149. https://doi.org/10.1353/wsq.2016.0039
Crenshaw, K. (1991). Mapping the margins: Intersectionality, identity Grzanka, P. R., Zeiders, K. H., & Miles, J. R. (2016). Beyond “born this
politics, and violence against women of color. Stanford Law Review, way”? Reconsidering sexual orientation beliefs and attitudes. Journal of
43(6), 1241–1299. https://doi.org/10.2307/1229039 Counseling Psychology, 63(1), 67–75. https://doi.org/10.1037/
D’Augelli, A. R. (1994). Identity development and sexual orientation: cou0000124
Toward a model of lesbian, gay, and bisexual development. In Grzanka, P. R., Zeiders, K. H., Spengler, E. S., Hoyt, L. T., & Toomey, R. B.
E. J. Trickett, R. J. Watts, & D. Birman (Eds.), The Jossey-Bass social (2020). Do beliefs about sexual orientation predict voting behavior?
and behavioral science series. Human diversity: Perspectives on people in Results from the 2016 U.S. presidential election. Psychology of Sexual
context (pp. 312–333). Jossey-Bass/Wiley. Orientation and Gender Diversity, 7(3), 241–252. https://doi.org/10.
D’Augelli, A. R., Grossman, A. H., Hershberger, S. L., & O’Connell, T. S. 1037/sgd0000434
(2001). Aspects of mental health among older lesbian, gay, and bisexual Haddock, G., & Maio, G. R. (2004). Introduction and overview. In
adults. Aging & Mental Health, 5(2), 149–158. https://doi.org/10.1080/ G. Haddock & G. R. Maio (Eds.), Contemporary perspectives on the
13607860120038366 psychology of attitudes (pp. 36–43). Abingdon, UK: Routledge.
DeLamater, J. D., & Hyde, J. S. (1998). Essentialism vs. social construc­ Haider-Markel, D. P., & Joslyn, M. R. (2008). Beliefs about the origins of
tionism in the study of human sexuality. Journal of Sex Research, 35(1), homosexuality and support for gay rights: An empirical test of attribu­
10–18. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499809551913 tion theory. Public Opinion Quarterly, 72(2), 291–310. https://doi.org/
Diamond, L. M. (2008). Sexual fluidity. Harvard University Press. 10.1093/poq/nfn015
636 D. TIERNEY ET AL.

Haslam, N., & Levy, S. R. (2006). Essentialist beliefs about homosexuality: Gay, and Bisexual Identity Scale. Journal of Counseling Psychology,
Structure and implications for prejudice. Personality and Social 58(2), 234–245. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022858
Psychology Bulletin, 32(4), 471–485. https://doi.org/10.1177/ Moradi, B., & Grzanka, P. R. (2017). Using intersectionality responsibly:
0146167205276516 Toward critical epistemology, structural analysis, and social justice
Haslam, N., Rothschild, L., & Ernst, D. (2002). Are essentialist beliefs activism. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 64(5), 500–513. https://
associated with prejudice? British Journal of Social Psychology, 41(1), doi.org/10.1037/cou0000203
87–100. https://doi.org/10.1037/e633872013-267 Morandini, J. S., Blaszczynski, A., Costa, D. S., Godwin, A., & Dar-
Hegarty, P. (2002). “It’s not a choice, it’s the way we’re built”: Symbolic Nimrod, I. (2017). Born this way: Sexual orientation beliefs and
beliefs about sexual orientation in the U.S. and Britain. Journal of their correlates in lesbian and bisexual women. Journal of
Community and Applied Social Psychology, 12(3), 153–166. https:// Counseling Psychology, 64(5), 560–573. https://doi.org/10.1037/
doi.org/10.1002/casp.669 cou0000209
Hegarty, P., & Pratto, F. (2001). Sexual orientation beliefs: Their relationship Morandini, J. S., Blaszczynski, A., Ross, M. W., Costa, D. S. J., & Dar-
to anti-gay attitudes and biological determinist attitudes. Journal of Nimrod, I. (2015). Essentialist beliefs, sexual identity uncertainty, inter­
Homosexuality, 41(1), 121–135. https://doi.org/10.1300/j082v41n01_04 nalized homonegativity and psychological wellbeing in gay men. Journal
Horn, S. S., & Heinze, J. (2011). “She can’t help it, she was born that way”: of Counseling Psychology, 62(3), 413–424. https://doi.org/10.1037/
Adolescents’ beliefs about the origins of homosexuality and sexual cou0000072
prejudice. Annals of Psychology, 27(3), 688–697. https://doi.org/10. Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2017). Mixture modeling with
1037/e633982013-908 cross-sectional data. In Mplus user’s guide (8th ed., pp. 165–220). Los
Hubbard, K., & de Visser, R. O. (2015). Not just bi the bi: The relationship Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.
between essentialist beliefs and attitudes about bisexuality. Psychology & Overby, L. M. (2014). Etiology and attitudes: Beliefs about the origins of
Sexuality, 6(3), 258–274. https://doi.org/10.1080/19419899.2014.987682 homosexuality and their implications for public policy. Journal of
Israel, T., Choi, A. Y., Goodman, J. A., Matsuno, E., Lin, Y.-J., Kary, K. G., Homosexuality, 61(4), 568–587. https://doi.org/10.1080/00918369.
& Merrill, C. R. S. (2019). Reducing internalized binegativity: 2013.806175
Development and efficacy of an online intervention. Psychology of Ramirez, J. L., Gonzalez, K. A., & Galupo, M. P. (2018). “Invisible during
Sexual Orientation and Gender Diversity, 6(2), 149–159. https://doi. my own crisis”: Responses of LGBT people of color to the Orlando
org/10.1037/sgd0000314 shooting. Journal of Homosexuality, 65(5), 579–599. https://doi.org/10.
Kaestle, C. E. (2019). Sexual orientation trajectories based on sexual 1080/00918369.2017.1328217
attractions, partners, and identity: A longitudinal investigation from Ramirez-Valles, J. (2007). “I don’t fit anywhere”: How race and sexuality
adolescence through young adulthood using a U.S. representative sam­ shape Latino gay and bisexual men’s health. In I. H. Meyer &
ple. Journal of Sex Research, 56(7), 811–826. https://doi.org/10.1080/ M. E. Northridge (Eds.), The health of sexual minorities (pp. 301–319).
00224499.2019 New York, NY: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-31334-4_12
Katz-Wise, S. L., Rosario, M., Calzo, J. P., Scherer, E. A., Sarda, E. A., & Rendina, H. J., Carter, J. A., Wahl, L., Millar, B. M., & Parsons, J. T. (2019).
Austin, S. B. (2017). Endorsement and timing of sexual orientation Trajectories of sexual identity development and psychological
developmental milestones among sexual minority young adults in the well-being for highly sexually active gay and bisexual men: A latent
Growing Up Today Study. Journal of Sex Research, 54(2), 172–185. growth curve analysis. Psychology of Sexual Orientation and Gender
https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2016.1170757 Diversity, 6(1), 64–74. https://doi.org/10.1037/sgd0000308
Kessler, R. A. G., Colpe, L., Hiripi, E., Mroczek, D., Normand, S., & Rosario, M., Schrimshaw, E. W., & Hunter, J. (2004). Ethnic/racial differ­
Zaslavsky, A. (2002). Short screening scales to monitor population ences in the coming-out process of lesbian, gay, and bisexual youths:
prevalences and trends in non-specific psychological distress. A comparison of sexual identity development over time. Cultural
Psychological Medicine, 32(6), 959–976. https://doi.org/10.1017/ Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 10(3), 215–228. https://doi.
S0033291702006074 org/10.1037/1099-9809.10.3.215
Lanza, S. T., Tan, X., & Bray, B. C. (2013). Latent class analysis with distal Rosario, M., Schrimshaw, E. W., & Hunter, J. (2011). Different patterns of
outcomes: A flexible model-based approach. Structural Equation sexual identity development over time: Implications for the psycholo­
Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 20(1), 1–26. https://doi.org/10. gical adjustment of lesbian, gay, and bisexual youths. Journal of Sex
1080/10705511.2013.742377 Research, 48(1), 3–15. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224490903331067
Legate, N., Ryan, R. M., & Weinstein, N. (2012). Is coming out always Rosario, M., Schrimshaw, E. W., & Hunter, J. (2012). Risk factors for
a “good thing”? Exploring the relations of autonomy support, outness, homelessness among lesbian, gay, and bisexual youths:
and wellness for lesbian, gay, and bisexual individuals. Social A developmental milestone approach. Children and Youth Services
Psychological and Personality Science, 3(2), 145–152. https://doi.org/ Review, 34(1), 186–193. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2011.09.016
10.1177/1948550611411929 Rosario, M., Schrimshaw, E. W., Hunter, J., & Braun, L. (2006). Sexual identity
Logie, C. H., & Rwigema, M. J. (2014). “The normative idea of queer is development among lesbian, gay, and bisexual youths: Consistency and
a white person”: Understanding perceptions of white privilege among change over time. Journal of Sex Research, 43(1), 46–58. https://doi.org/10.
lesbian, bisexual, and queer women of color in Toronto, Canada. 1080/00224490609552298
Journal of Lesbian Studies, 18(2), 174–191. https://doi.org/10.1080/ Rosser, B. S., Bockting, W. O., Ross, M. W., Miner, M. H., & Coleman, E.
10894160.2014.849165 (2008). The relationship between homosexuality, internalized
Martos, A. J., Nezhad, S., & Meyer, I. H. (2015). Variations in sexual homo-negativity, and mental health in men who have sex with men.
identity milestones among lesbians, gay men, and bisexuals. Sexuality Journal of Homosexuality, 55(2), 185–203. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Research and Social Policy, 12, 24–33. https://doi.org/10.1007% 00918360802129394
2Fs13178-014-0167–4 Ryazanov, A. A., & Christenfeld, N. J. S. (2018). The strategic value of
McCormack, M., & Savin-Williams, R. (2018). Young men’s rationales for essentialism. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 12(1), e12370.
non-exclusive gay sexualities. Culture, Health & Sexuality, 20(8), https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12370
929–944. https://doi.org/10.1080/13691058.2017.1398349 Sakalli, N. (2002). Application of the attribution-value model of prejudice
Mitchell, R. W., & Dezarn, L. (2014). Does knowing why someone is gay to homosexuality. Journal of Social Psychology, 142(2), 264–271. https://
influence tolerance? Genetic, environmental, choice and ‘reparative’ doi.org/10.1080/00224540209603899
explanations. Sexuality & Culture, 18, 994–1009. https://doi.org/10. Savin-Williams, R. C., & Cohen, K. M. (2015). Developmental trajectories
1007/s12119-014-9233–6 and milestones of lesbian, gay, and bisexual young people.
Mohr, J. J., & Kendra, M. S. (2011). Revision and extension of International Review of Psychiatry, 27(5), 357–366. https://doi.org/10.
a multidimensional measure of sexual minority identity: The Lesbian, 3109/09540261.2015.1093465
THE JOURNAL OF SEX RESEARCH 637

Schlomer, G. L., Bauman, S., & Card, N. A. (2010). Best practices for Troiden, D. R. R. (1989). The formation of homosexual identities. Journal of
missing data management in counseling psychology. Journal of Homosexuality, 17(1–2), 43–74. https://doi.org/10.1300/j082v17n01_02
Counseling Psychology, 57(1), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1037/a001808 Watson, L. B. (2019). Gender identity and expression in LGBTQ+ commu­
Silva, T. (2019). “Daddies,” “cougars,” and their partners past midlife: nities: Implications for the practice of psychology. Psychology of Women
Gender attitudes and relationship and sexual well-being among older Quarterly, 43(3), 298–302. https://doi.org/10.1177/0361684319846498
adults in age-heterogenous partnerships. Socius: Sociological Research Westfall, P., & Henning, K. S. (2013). Understanding advanced statistical
for a Dynamic World, 5(1), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1177% methods. CRC Press.
2F2378023119869452 Whitley, B. E., Jr. (1990). The relationship of heterosexuals’ attributions
Singh, R. S., & O’Brien, W. H. (2020). The impact of work stress on sexual for the causes of homosexuality to attitudes toward lesbians and gay
minority employees: Could psychological flexibility be a helpful solution? men. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16(2), 369–377. https://
Stress and Health, 36(1), 59–74. https://doi.org/10.1002/smi.2913 doi.org/10.1177/0146167290162016
Tozer, E. E., & Hayes, J. A. (2004). Why do individuals seek conversion Zeiders, K. H., Roosa, M. W., Knight, G. P., & Gonzales, N. A. (2013).
therapy?: The role of religiosity, internalized homonegativity, and Mexican American adolescents’ profiles of risk and mental health:
identity development. The Counseling Psychologist, 32(5), 716–740. A person-centered longitudinal approach. Journal of Adolescence,
https://doi.org/10.1177/0011000004267563 36(3), 603–612. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2013.03

You might also like