You are on page 1of 12

REVIEW Pain Management

For reprint orders, please contact: reprints@futuremedicine.com

Social pain and physical pain: shared


paths to resilience
John A Sturgeon*,1 & Alex J Zautra2

Practice points
Overlap of physical & social pain
●● Emerging neuroscience and psychological evidence suggests a substantial overlap between physical pain and social
pain, which includes commonalities in genetic variants, inflammatory responses and neural pathways.
●● Social pain, like physical pain, may serve an adaptive evolutionary function, which may explain its similarities with
physical pain.
Social factors & vulnerability in chronic pain
●● Social factors may increase vulnerability to chronic pain via both focal exposure to major life stressors (e.g., trauma)
and through chronic exposure to socially painful situations (e.g., ostracism, isolation and prolonged social conflict).
●● Heightened pain sensitivity and maladaptive pain coping may be more likely due to exacerbated negative emotional
states that are reactive to social factors such as stress and interpersonal conflict.
Social factors & resilience in chronic pain
●● Two primary contributors to more effective pain adaptation are positive emotional states and meaningful social ties,
including social support, which may predict lower levels of pain intensity and bolster more effective psychological
responses under painful conditions.
Social relationships of those in chronic pain: the importance of social intelligence
●● The presence of social support is not always a sufficient condition to promote effective pain adaptation, as overly
responsive social networks may compromise self-sufficiency and underutilized social networks may not confer
maximal benefits to psychological well-being and pain coping.
●● Greater attention to individual factors, such as goals and strategies for bolstering enjoyment and meaningful
interactions with one’s social networks, may yield benefits in improving physical and psychological functioning.
●● Novel interventions, such as those to enhance social intelligence, may help to address the social distress and
decreased interpersonal enjoyment that are common in people with a chronic pain condition.
Interventions for caregivers
●● Addressing the adaptation of caregivers and family members of individuals with chronic pain may serve to
ameliorate the pain and emotional distress experienced by both patients and their loved ones.
●● Interventions for clinicians that enhance compassionate and empathic responses to patients with chronic pain may
enhance therapeutic alliance and improve treatment outcomes.
Future directions for study
●● Positive psychology interventions are a promising area of psychological intervention that may improve interpersonal
ties and bolster positive emotions in individuals with chronic pain, though additional study is still needed to establish
their efficacy.
●● Although psychotherapy is generally considered a relatively low-risk intervention, there is relatively little evidence
examining what, if any, risks this approach may pose in the context of treatment for chronic pain.

1
Department of Anesthesiology, Perioperative & Pain Medicine, Stanford University, Palo Alto, CA 94305, USA
2
Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ 85287, USA
*Author for correspondence: jasturge@stanford.edu part of

10.2217/pmt.15.56 © 2016 Future Medicine Ltd Pain Manag. (2016) 6(1), 63–74 ISSN 1758-1869 63
Review Sturgeon & Zautra

Although clinical models have traditionally defined pain by its consequences for the
behavior and internal states of the sufferer, recent evidence has highlighted the importance
of examining pain in the context of the broader social environment. Neuroscience research
has highlighted commonalities of neural pathways connecting the experience of physical
and social pain, suggesting a substantial overlap between these phenomena. Further,
interpersonal ties, support and aspects of the social environment can impair or promote
effective adaptation to chronic pain through changes in pain perception, coping and
emotional states. The current paper reviews the role of social factors in extant psychological
interventions for chronic pain, and discusses how greater attention to these factors may
inform future research and clinical care.

First draft submitted: 17 August 2015; Accepted for publication: 14 October 2015; Published
online: 17 December 2015

KEYWORDS Pain has long been defined as a multidimen- role in inhibitory pain processing [3] . Similarly,
• chronic pain • negative sional construct; traditionally, researchers have some researchers examined the role of learning
emotions • pain coping acknowledged not only the physical or sensory processes in pain, suggesting that individuals
• pain perception • positive aspects of pain, but also the cognitive and emo- develop adaptive or maladaptive responses to
emotions • psychological tional aspects of pain [1] . Early models proposed pain based on environmental and internal con-
interventions • resilience that the experience of pain in humans occurs tingencies, such as the magnitude of pain exac-
• social pain • social support in a relatively invariant sequence of events. For erbation or relief after performing an activity [4] .
example, Wade and colleagues [2] proposed that In these models, psychological factors were pro-
pain is experienced in 4 stages: pain is first expe- posed as modifiers of the general processes of
rienced according to its sensory-discriminative pain transmission and subsequent behavioral
features that indicate the location, timing and responses. However, although these models did
other physical aspects of the pain experience; acknowledge environmental factors as contribu-
immediately following the sensory experience tory in pain processing and coping, they primar-
of pain, individuals experience an affective reac- ily characterized pain as a phenomenon that has
tion to pain that may evoke, for example, fears implications only for the internal states of the
of threat or bodily damage; the experience of person experiencing pain.
pain is then modified by cognitive processes that In recent years, there has been a prolifera-
may involve prior painful experiences, the cur- tion of research examining not only the effects
rent context of pain experience, and expectations of pain on the sufferer, but also an increasingly
and beliefs about the short- and long-term conse- large emphasis on the transactional relation-
quences of pain; and pain is expressed via overt ships between pain within the sufferer and
and visible behaviors that may have longer term various aspects of the social environment. In
physical and social consequences (e.g., avoid- this paper, we will review the evidence suggest-
ing painful activities, grimacing, bracing and ing a significant psychological and neurologi-
protecting painful areas). These models have cal overlap between physical and social factors
emphasized that behavioral changes occur only underlying the experience of pain, how these fac-
at a comparatively later stage during the experi- tors impede or facilitate pain adaptation, and
ence of pain, while elements of the social world review their potential role in existing and future
are acknowledged primarily as outcomes of the interventions.
experience of pain, rather than contributing Early models examining the role of social
factors. factors in pain focused on constructs like social
Other key early models in chronic pain have support, which has demonstrated broad value
sought to quantify pain signaling and explain in explaining trajectories of pain-related adap-
how nociceptive signals may be modified by tation [5] . These models emphasized a trans-
CNS factors, such as descending inhibitory actional model: when pain causes a problem,
processes from the brain [3] . The Gate Control it may spur changes in mood or behavior and
Theory of Pain emphasized, for example, that prompt a supportive response from others. More
psychological variables such as past experience recently, this model has been expanded signifi-
and ascribed meaning of pain might be acti- cantly by neuroscience research that has con-
vated by activity of large nerve fibers and play a nected the experience of physical pain with the

64 Pain Manag. (2016) 6(1) future science group


Social pain & physical pain Review

experience of ‘social pain’, including experiences pain. Emerging evidence suggests that genetic
of ostracism, loss and interpersonal conflict, by factors may also affect this phenomenon; gene
identifying a shared underlying neural pathway. polymorphisms in the μ-opioid receptor gene,
The role of this shared neural pathway was first a gene that has been strongly associated with
suggested by functional MRI studies in the early physical pain, predict not only ratings of pain
2000s, which reported that regions of the brain unpleasantness, but also ratings of social pain
that have been traditionally associated with in response to experimental social exclusion
the salience of pain and pain-related distress, paradigms [17] .
the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and ventral So, why might physical and social pain dem-
prefrontal cortex, also show similar patterns of onstrate similar patterns of activation and pro-
activation in paradigms designed to elicit social cessing in the brain? Recent theories have sug-
pain through social exclusion [6] . Subsequently, gested that pain is an important self-regulatory
this model has been expanded to include the signal that aids the organism in directing efforts
insula, another region that has been traditionally to return the body to its natural homeostatic
associated with physical pain [7] . Similar neural set point [18] . It is in this way that acute physi-
activation patterns have since been reported in cal pain serves a key biological and evolution-
the experience of social rejection [8] and both ary function: to spur adaptive responses when
acute [9] and prolonged bereavement [10] . an organism is experiencing heightened threat
The overlapping nature of physical and social or physical harm. In evolutionary models that
pain has also been supported by clinical and define physical pain as arising from tissue dam-
experimental data. Prior studies have indicated age, the presence of pain provides an important
that measures such as cutaneous heat pain sen- indication regarding the safety of the organism;
sitivity are correlated with the degree of distress when pain is present in its acute form, it fosters
reported after a social exclusion paradigm [11] . unpleasant and distressing psychological states
Similarly, observational studies have found that that lead to protective responses, such as avoid-
daily ratings of loneliness, a socially based nega- ing use of the injured area and avoidance and
tive emotional state, are predictive of later pain escape behaviors to prevent further damage.
intensity [12] . The connection between physi- This paradigm of acute pain as a physical safety
cal pain and social factors may be explained by mechanism has subsequently been applied to
several factors, including cognitive and physi- evolutionary models for explaining social pain;
ological processes. Wolf and colleagues note that just as physical damage to an organism threatens
the relationship between loneliness and pain its long-term survival, so too might separation
intensity appears to be mediated by increased from the organism’s social group. Given that
levels of pain catastrophizing, suggesting that humans have historically thrived in communi-
cognitive appraisal of pain and social processes ties, loss of a community and the protections
may be a key mechanism [12] . Similarly, this it provides (e.g., from predation or starvation)
overlap between physical and social pain may likely predicts a much higher rate of mortal-
be connected by inflammatory responses in the ity [19] . Consequently, social pain may have
body. Social stressors have been found to cor- promoted physical safety in a similar manner as
relate to increased circulation of proinflamma- physical pain; when a ‘socially painful’ event has
tory cytokines [13] . Similarly, proinflammatory occurred, it may spur the individual to repair the
cytokines like IL-6 have been found to mediate social schism or to seek new sources of support.
the relationship between social exclusion and Although aversive or distressing social experi-
depression [14] , and show correlations with pain ences may not constitute an equivalent experi-
intensity in some populations [15] . The role of ence to physical pain, this evolutionary model
inflammatory processes in social pain has also does suggest that social pain plays a similar role
been emphasized by DeWall and colleagues, who to physical pain. Given that both physical and
reported that a 3-week course of the NSAID social pain might be expected to foster long-term
acetaminophen buffered against feelings of survival, it is not unreasonable to expect that
social pain after a social exclusion paradigm [16] . they may interact in determining the affective,
These authors noted that these effects appear to cognitive and behavioral reactions to pain.
be mediated by decreased activity in the dorsal Although the aforementioned findings pro-
anterior cingulate cortex and anterior insula, vide a compelling indication that physical and
both regions implicated traditionally in physical social pain may share a much greater degree of

future science group www.futuremedicine.com 65


Review Sturgeon & Zautra

similarity than once thought, it is also important ostracism or social conflict, may be etiologic in
to note that this area of study remains in a rela- the development of later chronic pain [23] . In
tively early stage. For example, some researchers much the same way that the experience of physi-
have recently challenged the assertion of shared cal pain may inspire negative emotional states,
common pathways for physical and social pain, activate cognitive appraisal patterns, and spur
noting that underlying neural representations of behavior changes, a similar response pattern may
these phenomena may vary significantly, despite be noted in situations that provoke acute social
similarities in activation patterns across broader pain. In situations indicating an emergent threat,
neuroanatomical regions [20] . These findings such as a broken bone or an external threat to
suggest that the level of analysis concerning physical safety, responses such as escape, hostil-
these processes is of central importance, and that ity or avoidance may be useful. However, models
the degree of overlap of physically and socially of chronic physical pain have highlighted how
painful experiences may depend on how they are behaviors that may be adaptive when an individ-
measured. Consequently, continued replication ual experiences acute pain (e.g., avoiding activi-
of these findings remains an important step for ties that increase pain, engaging in social behav-
future empirical models. iors that signal a need for help) may develop into
In summary, evidence connecting the phe- patterns of behavior that are maladaptive and
nomenological and physiological aspects of impair long-term health when pain is chronic.
physically and socially painful experiences con- Similarly, when an individual experiences a form
stitutes an emergent but fruitful area of research. of social pain (e.g., social exclusion or rejection),
However, physical pain and social factors have their responses may be situationally appropri-
long shown a relationship of mutual influ- ate (e.g., feeling angry or avoiding a group after
ence. In the next section, we will discuss the being rejected). However, when social pain
significant role of social factors in clinical and becomes a chronic issue, this may compromise
­empirical models of pain. self-esteem, increase aggression against others,
and lead to less effective coping [24] and proso-
Social factors & vulnerability in chronic cial behavior [25] . When prosocial behaviors are
pain reduced, individuals may become less likely
Social factors have long been linked to the etiol- to attempt to repair their social relationships
ogy and maintenance of chronic pain. In addi- when problems arise, prolonging difficulties
tion to the potential overlapping patterns of neu- with social alienation and contributing to less
ral activation described in the previous section, utilization and benefit from existing social sup-
social and physical pain may interact through port [26] . Of note, socially painful events may
a variety of other mutually influential factors, create ‘socially painful memories’ that foster a
including cognition, behavioral responses, affec- persistent fear of rejection, which increases the
tive states and neurophysiological responses (see risk of later psychological difficulties [27] and
Figure 1). Notably, there has been considerable exacerbates social problems by decreasing proso-
evidence that the social environment may play cial behavior and leading to poorer appraisals
a protective or exacerbating role in the devel- of existing relationships [28] . As a result of these
opment of chronic pain. One salient social fac- social challenges, individuals may face elevated
tor is early life stress; trauma and other major risks of chronic physical pain due to a variety
stressors early in life have been shown to pre- of factors, including a hypervigilance to both
dispose individuals to increased rates of chronic socially and physically threatening cues, insecure
pain later in life [21] . Though some degree of styles of interpersonal attachment and impaired
this effect appears to be due to differences in processes of self-regulation [23] .
nervous system function after trauma [22] , early In cases of social pain, the difficulties stem-
life stressors may also predispose an individual ming from these situations may interact with
to exposure to other stressful environments at a perception and coping processes in physi-
later time, thereby perpetuating their deleterious cal pain; there are mechanisms that are both
consequences [21] . responsive to social factors that influence pain
However, it is not only focal and highly stress- perception and adaptation in both experimental
ful events early in life that may increase vulner- and observational studies, most notably affective
ability to chronic pain, as more chronic forms states. Negative emotional states are commonly
of social stress, such as prolonged isolation, found to exacerbate pain intensity [29,30] . Social

66 Pain Manag. (2016) 6(1) future science group


Social pain & physical pain Review

History of History of
physical pain social pain
experiences experiences
(prior injuries, (trauma,
chronic pain) Physical pain Social pain
alienation)
dimensions dimensions

Acute physical Acute social


changes (injury, stressors
inflammation) Neural (criticism,
activation exclusion)

Affective
states

Cognitive–
behavioral
responses

Social
relationships

Figure 1. Conceptual model depicting antecedents and shared mechanisms of physical and social pain.

problems, in turn, are a key precipitant of these emotional suffering inspired by these challenges
negative emotional states [31,32] . Indeed, there may interact and complicate effective adaptation
is evidence that social factors may play a more to pain.
salient role in pain-related mood dysregulation In addition to social conflict, there are also
than overall physical ability [33] . Unfortunately, significant pain-related risks to those individu-
evidence also suggests that individuals with a als who do not have access to sufficient levels of
chronic pain condition are vulnerable to more social support or those who do not fully utilize
frequent social conflict [31] and show greater the social resources at their disposal. Withdrawal
reactivity to interpersonal stressors [34] . There from one’s social relationships, a common issue
is also evidence that emotional states and social with elevated pain, has been shown to worsen
functioning are mutually influential: the sever- the emotional consequences of chronic pain [32] .
ity of current depressive symptoms is predictive Similarly, individuals with chronic pain who
of poorer psychosocial functioning, above and report higher levels of isolation tend to report
beyond the experience of pain [35] , and individ- greater levels of depression [37] and lower lev-
uals tend to appraise their social relationships els of physical functionality [38] . Interestingly,
more negatively when experiencing elevated the pain-exacerbating effects of loneliness may
levels of stress or negative emotion [36] . These be explained to some extent by dysregulations
findings suggest that both the presence of social in stress hormone activity and gene transcrip-
conflict and the evaluation of one’s social rela- tion, which increase long-term vulnerability to
tionships have implications for emotional states inflammatory disorders [39] . Given the relation-
in chronic pain. Further, given that both con- ship between inflammation and pain in some
flictual social interactions and physical pain may disorders [15] , these effects may translate to
increase emotional distress, it may be that the chronic pain disorders, as well.

future science group www.futuremedicine.com 67


Review Sturgeon & Zautra

Social factors & resilience in chronic pain the social world and positive emotions is decid-
Although problems in the social environment can edly complex, particularly for individuals living
exacerbate pain and pain-related disturbances in with a chronic pain condition.
daily life, positive aspects of one’s social world
may improve coping responses and overall func- Social relationships of those in chronic
tion for individuals with chronic pain. There pain: the importance of social intelligence
are a few key variables that have proven to be Although the presence of social support is a key
powerful predictors of effective adaptation to ingredient in positive pain coping, the mere pres-
pain: social relationships and positive emotion. ence of supportive others does not guarantee
Social relationships may enhance pain coping either short-term or long-term positive benefit.
both directly, via provision of social support, Indeed, there are a number of moderating factors
and indirectly, by enhancing other factors that that determine the extent to which an individual
improve resilience to pain through enjoyable or with chronic pain may utilize or benefit from
positive interpersonal exchanges. Early defini- extant social support. Broadly, it has been sug-
tions of resilience in adults identified effective gested that the quality of social support, rather
utilization of social resources as a key contributor than the quantity of people within one’s social
to effective coping [40,41] , and subsequent defini- network, is likely a more important predictor
tions regarding pain-specific resilience have ech- of beneficial social support [53] . In fact, some
oed the importance of this process [42,43] . Indeed, individuals with chronic pain may show decre-
social support has been found to meaningfully ments in their physical or psychological function
predict ratings of pain and functional status in if their social network is excessively supportive,
individuals with chronic pain 5 years later [5] . which may compromise the development of
Greater social support has been associated with self‑sufficiency and effective pain coping [53] .
lower levels of pain intensity in response to pain- There are also important cognitive factors at
ful experimental stimuli, and these effects have play in social relationships. For some, the pres-
been noted regardless of whether the supportive ence of emotional or instrumental support may
other is physically present [44,45] . Meaningful be rated as more important than relief from
social connections may also serve a protective role pain or negative emotions [54] . Similarly, some
in reducing nervous system responses in response individuals with chronic pain may emphasize
to pain and stress [44] and by engaging neural net- social validation of their pain above all other
works associated with more adaptive responses goals, as they may feel ostracized or isolated due
to pain, such as reward circuitry [45] . Individuals to a belief that their pain is poorly understood
receiving social support also appear to be more by others. However, this predominant focus on
likely to engage in prosocial behavior, protect- social validation has been associated with less
ing them against pain-related e­xacerbations in positive functional outcomes, as it may come
n­egative mood [46] . at the expense of attempts to remain self-suf-
Similarly, positive emotions are among the ficient [55] . For example, individuals with pain
most important predictors of effective pain who experience more frequent solicitous behav-
adaptation [42,43] , and are accessed significantly ior from their loved ones tend to show poorer
via one’s social relationships [32] . Positive emo- adjustment to pain and a greater vulnerability to
tional states have demonstrated a variety of bene- disability [56,57] . Individual goals are therefore of
fits in both healthy individuals and in those with central importance in determining the degree of
a chronic pain condition, including enhanced benefit one receives from his or her social support
stress recovery [47] , improved immune func- network: for an individual who has maintained
tion [48] and better cognitive performance under focus on improving function, this support may
stressful conditions [49] . Positive affective states serve as a key stepping stone back to engagement
also demonstrate a variety of pain-specific ben- in personally meaningful activities. However,
efits, including reducing ratings of pain inten- when one’s social network shows an excessive or
sity [50] , promoting adaptive cognitive responses deficient response to the needs of a person with
to pain [51] and increasing the ability to persist pain, the goals of that individual may gravitate
in goal-directed behavior under painful condi- toward garnering additional support, even if this
tions [52] . It is thus clear that positive emotional support comes at the cost of self-sufficiency.
states are key predictors of positive pain-related These findings highlight the complexity of
outcomes. However, the relationship between the social relationships of those individuals

68 Pain Manag. (2016) 6(1) future science group


Social pain & physical pain Review

living with chronic pain. It is not simply that other than one’s own [59] . Similarly, there is also
the presence of others and whether they are some evidence that acceptance and commitment
willing or able to provide support that pre- therapy may have benefits in the context of bol-
dicts more positive trajectories of adaptation to stering social function, which may be achieved
chronic pain. Rather, it is equally meaningful through a focus in enhancing psychological flex-
to identify ways to reverse the troubled rela- ibility [60] . However, it is possible that these find-
tionships of chronic pain sufferers with their ings may be secondary to a broader psychological
peers and loved ones and to identify ways in improvement that occurs as a result of these treat-
which these individuals can establish new posi- ments, as patients commonly report significant
tive social connections and enrich their current improvement in their levels of emotional distress
sources of positive interpersonal engagement. and greater overall function after completion of
In existing psychological treatments for chronic these treatments [61] . It is important to note that
pain, social relationships are often addressed we are not proposing that these interventions do
as an incidental, rather than central, focus of not positively affect social function, but rather
intervention. In cognitive–behavioral therapy that their broader scope may not leave sufficient
(CBT) for pain, treatment may target deficien- time to ameliorate psychosocial distress that may
cies in social function, either through the use of co-occur or even predate an individual’s ongoing
assertive communication strategies to address pain symptoms. In sum, the extant psychological
difficult or conflictual relationships, or through interventions for chronic pain have typically not
a planned approach toward reincorporation of made social relations a central focus, and their
positive interpersonal events, as in the case of benefits in this area may be secondary to empha-
positive activity scheduling [58] . Incorporation sizing other factors that are broadly applicable
of these techniques in CBT for pain assumes a to both pain coping and interpersonal relation-
specific set of difficulties experienced by indi- ships, such as improved mood, reduced behavio-
viduals with pain; more specifically, targeting ral avoidance and promotion of nonjudgmental
of communication strategies appears intended perspectives. Consequently, a more explicit focus
to address feelings of alienation or being poorly of treatment on ameliorating interpersonal dis-
understood by others, while helping patients to tress and enriching enjoyment of one’s relation-
re-establish appropriate boundaries and expec- ships may serve to provide even greater benefit
tations in their social relationships, which may in this domain.
require recalibration due to limitations or chal- One potential intervention that may help to
lenges caused by physical pain. Positive activ- address these interpersonal issues in chronic
ity scheduling, meanwhile, may provide incre- pain may focus on bolstering social intelligence.
mental value for patients with chronic pain, Recently, an online social intelligence interven-
who may avoid positive social activities due to tion has been developed that targets the develop-
pain [32] and may thus benefit from behavioral ment of skills that are instrumental in sustaining
activation approaches, particularly those with a positive social relationships [62–65] . The social
social focus. intelligence training program utilizes recent evi-
In newer wave treatments for chronic pain, dence from social neuroscience and emphasizes
such as acceptance and commitment therapy and the humanization of relationships with atten-
mindfulness-based stress reduction, treatments tion to cognitions that facilitate healthy social
are largely nondirective and a greater emphasis is connections [66] . However, the approach extends
placed on personally held values; while the flex- beyond cognitive models and behavioral princi-
ibility afforded in these approaches is likely to ples to include attention to evidence of barriers
be a largely positive influence, they may or may to social-emotional development from adverse
not emphasize interpersonal relationships as a experiences in childhood and adult life, and
central component of effective pain coping. One ways to move beyond those stressful experiences.
exception is loving kindness meditation, which Recent empirical evidence shows this type of
is traditionally included in mindfulness-based program offers a promising resource for chronic
stress reduction and does emphasize compas- pain patients and those who care for them [64,66] .
sion toward not only the self, but also others; We, along with others, are currently engaged in
this meditation has shown some benefit in the developing a parallel program, tailored to the
context of improving important interpersonal social relationship challenges among chronic
factors such as the ability to take perspectives pain patients [67] .

future science group www.futuremedicine.com 69


Review Sturgeon & Zautra

Interventions for caregivers umbrella of an approach referred to as ‘person-


Another way to address the social factors at work centered care’. Kitwood [75] , a British gerontolo-
in individuals with chronic pain is to develop gist, introduced person-centered care as an aspi-
interventions that incorporate family members rational goal for elder care: to humanize social
into the treatment itself. Indeed, interventions interactions between patient and healthcare pro-
targeting the relationships of individuals coping viders [76] . The Social Intelligence Institute [77] ,
with a chronic condition have benefits both for for example, is training direct care workers at
the psychiatric distress of the individual with life care communities to see those they care for
pain and in terms of reducing psychiatric distress as fellow humans with needs and wants rather
and perceived levels of family burden [68] . This than as a collection of body parts that demand
approach has been adopted more readily in the attention. Physicians are important yet elusive
cases of pediatric and adolescent chronic pain, targets for these types of interventions. The time
where family functioning is not only a contribu- it takes to acknowledge the humanity of those
tor to adaptation, but also regularly referenced under their care is measureable in seconds, but
as a meaningful outcome [69] . In adults with an emphasis on humanizing the relationships
chronic pain, a more regular area of inquiry is between physicians and patients has remained
the examination of interventions focusing on relatively underemphasized in current training
marital relationships when one partner has a models.
pain condition; meta-analyses of extant treat-
ments suggest that couple-focused interventions Future directions for study
may reduce pain and emotional distress while It is also worth noting some emergent interven-
improving family functioning, though effect tions in the context of chronic pain that focus
sizes are generally small [70] and may not be con- on enhancement of positive psychological states.
sistent, particularly with regard to the effects of In the past 2–3 years, researchers have begun to
marital relationship factors on pain intensity [71] . devote additional attention to adapting exercises
The value of positive social relations between and treatments that have long been connected to
professional healthcare providers and their psychological health in non-pain areas, such as
patients is also critical. Evidence suggests that depression. These interventions have attempted
the relationship between patient and provider to encapsulate traditional positive psychological
consistently predicts outcomes in overall medical approaches by targeting mechanisms that bol-
care, though the relationship has been somewhat ster positive emotional states and improve social
understudied in chronic pain populations [72] . relations. These activities may include exercises
When these relationships are characterized by that promote forgiveness of others for prior
a more prominent belief that pain is due to a harms, sharing of gratitude with supportive
psychosocial cause, providers may feel that the others, promoting prosocial behaviors (e.g., per-
patient is trying to be deceptive and may feel forming random acts of kindness), cultivating
less empathy and more mistrust toward the compassion toward the self and others, savoring
patient, compromising the quality of the treat- positive experiences, enhancing optimism and
ing relationship [73] . Similarly, patients who feel pursuing personally meaningful goals [78,79] .
that their providers do not believe the legitimacy These interventions constitute a promising
of their pain complaints may begin to feel stig- step away from treating pain using strictly a
matized by the ‘invisible’ nature of their condi- problem-focused approach and instead promot-
tions, making them more susceptible to emo- ing overall health and meaningful function.
tional distress and social isolation and decreasing However, to date, there have been relatively few
their willingness to seek appropriate care in the studies that have examined the efficacy of these
future [74] . These findings highlight the potential treatments in chronic pain, and existing data
value of interventions that may facilitate com- are limited. The Müller study reported promis-
passionate and empathic responses from pain ing results suggesting improvements in multiple
practitioners, in order to improve treatment domains in individuals with chronic pain and
response and reducing attrition from treatment. a co-occurring physical disability, including
Attention to patient concerns has a rich tra- pain intensity and perceived pain control, by
dition within healthcare, especially nursing. delivering a customizable online intervention
Current efforts to elevate the quality of care using several of these techniques [78] . Flink and
to elders in life-care facilities fall under the colleagues, who utilized a replicated single-case

70 Pain Manag. (2016) 6(1) future science group


Social pain & physical pain Review

design, reported some improvements in disa- are superordinate constructs that may explain
bility ratings and the severity of catastrophic the degree of overlap in these constructs, or
appraisals of one’s pain; however, the size of whether delineations between these constructs
these effects was relatively modest [79] . It thus may be found by adopting different analytical
appears that additional study is warranted to approaches, as suggested by Woo [20] . Similarly,
determine whether these approaches are suf- it may be that some of these constructs may
ficient as a standalone treatment for chronic be related spuriously by third variables not yet
pain, or whether they are better employed as incorporated into our model. For example, it
supplemental techniques in existing treatment may be that neuroticism, a personality trait
protocols, such as CBT. commonly associated with increased negative
From a broader perspective, there is also a emotionality, may increase levels of emotional
relative lack of research examining the possibil- distress and maladaptive cognitions in situa-
ity of adverse effects of psychotherapy, which tions that are either physically or socially pain-
may be due to a manipulative or otherwise poor ful, thereby inflating the correlation between
therapeutic alliance or through a failure of the these variables. In sum, there remains consid-
provider to promote independence and effec- erable work to be done to better characterize
tive coping outside of therapy sessions, thereby the degrees of overlap and distinctiveness of
fostering dependence on therapy itself [80] . domains that may connect the experiences of
Although some studies have noted that these physical and social pain.
risks likely do exist in some cases, particularly
in cases where therapists may not be adequately Conclusion
trained in ethics and effective psychothera­peutic In the past three decades, significant advances
techniques [81] , this possibility has not been have occurred in the conceptualization and
directly addressed in the context of medical or treatment of chronic pain. In recent years, this
psychological treatment in chronic pain, and improvement has included an increased empha-
warrants attention in future studies. sis on examining not only the effects of pain
Although we have proposed a conceptual within the individual, but also the importance
model to explain the potential overlap between of examining the social context in which pain
physical and social pain, it should be viewed as occurs. Pain itself may be more appropriately
a preliminary model that requires refinement considered as both an internal and social phe-
through further empirical study. There are sev- nomenon, based on the evidence provided
eral ways that it may be possible to improve from psychological and neuroscience research.
our model by revising or replicating the paths Consequently, there is significant value in
we have proposed. The directionality of effects emphasizing the importance of social factors in
between domains of pain experience remains to future studies, in order to characterize trajec-
be clarified in many cases; it may be that certain tories of vulnerability and resilience for people
variables demonstrate a degree of mutual influ- facing chronic pain and to more effectively treat
ence with others, while other effects may be bet- concurrent social factors that may serve to either
ter characterized as unidirectional. For example, facilitate or impede effective pain adaptation.
Wolf and colleagues [12] provide evidence that
feelings of loneliness may predispose individu- Future perspective
als to experiencing more intense pain at a later Incorporation of social factors into medical and
time by increasing an individual’s tendency to psychological interventions for chronic pain may
appraise their pain as being a catastrophic influ- significantly enhance their benefit. Elucidation
ence on his or her life; however, it may also be of the social influences on pain experience, pain
that pain may induce loneliness by disrupting coping and medication use can help to highlight
valued social activities, which may impair an new and novel targets for intervention. In the
individual’s ability to seek the degree of social future, interventions will promote wellness in
engagement that they might otherwise desire. chronic pain by addressing not only the efforts
Further, it may be that some of the domains of the patient to adapt to pain, but also the social
proposed in our model share significant overlap, context within which pain adaptation occurs.
as in the case of the overlapping neural regions This process may occur by treating the reactions
implicated in social and physical pain; in these of loved ones who may be closely involved in
cases, it may be worthwhile to determine if there the care of the patient (e.g., family members)

future science group www.futuremedicine.com 71


Review Sturgeon & Zautra

and by identifying strategies for enhancing the Financial & competing interests disclosure
therapeutic relationship between patients and Research was supported by the National Institute on Drug
providers. With the accumulation of emergent Abuse (NIDA) of the NIH under grant number
neuroscience and psychological evidence that 3T32DA035165-02S1. The authors have no other relevant
highlights the overlap between physical and affiliations or financial involvement with any organization
social pain, the conceptualization of pain will or entity with a financial interest in or financial conflict
move closer to being truly multidimensional, withthe subject matter or materials discussed in the
necessitating the examination of physical, psy- m­anuscript apart from those disclosed.
chological and social factors in future models of No writing assistance was utilized in the production of
pain research and treatment. this manuscript.

References reward center. NeuroImage 42(2), 969–972 social rejection. Nat. Commun. 5, 5380
Papers of special note have been highlighted as: (2008). (2014).
• of interest; •• of considerable interest 11 Eisenberger NI, Jarcho JM, Lieberman MD, 21 Davis DA, Luecken LJ, Zautra AJ. Are reports
1 Melzack R. The McGill Pain Questionnaire: Naliboff BD. An experimental study of shared of childhood abuse related to the experience
major properties and scoring methods. Pain sensitivity to physical pain and social of chronic pain in adulthood? A meta-analytic
1(3), 277–299 (1975). rejection. Pain 126(1–3), 132–138 (2006). review of the literature. Clin. J. Pain 21(5),
12 Wolf LD, Davis MC, Yeung EW, Tennen 398–405 (2005).
2 Wade JB, Dougherty LM, Archer CR, Price
DD. Assessing the stages of pain processing: a HA. The within-day relation between lonely 22 Rutten BP, Hammels C, Geschwind N et al.
multivariate analytical approach. Pain 68(1), episodes and subsequent clinical pain in Resilience in mental health: linking
157–167 (1996). individuals with fibromyalgia: mediating role psychological and neurobiological
of pain cognitions. J. Psychosom. Res.79(3), perspectives. Acta Psychiat. Scand. 128(1),
3 Melzack R. From the gate to the neuromatrix.
202–206 (2015). 3–20 (2013).
Pain 82, S121–S126 (1999).
13 Slavich GM, Way BM, Eisenberger NI, Taylor 23 Riva P, Wesselmann ED, Wirth JH,
•• A seminal study highlighting the overlap of
SE. Neural sensitivity to social rejection is Carter-Sowell AR, Williams KD. When pain
neural systems implicated in physical and
associated with inflammatory responses to does not heal: the common antecedents and
social pain.
social stress. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA consequences of chronic social and physical
4 Fordyce WE. Behavioral Methods for 107(33), 14817–14822 (2010). pain. Basic Appl. Soc. Psych. 36(4), 329–346
Chronic Pain and Illness. Mosby, IL, USA (2014).
14 Eisenberger NI, Inagaki TK, Rameson LT,
(1976).
Mashal NM, Irwin MR. An fMRI study of • Provides a conceptual framework for
5 Evers AWM, Kraaimaat FW, Geenen R, cytokine-induced depressed mood and social explaining the long-term implications of
Jacobs JWG, Bijlsma JWJ. Pain coping and pain: the role of sex differences. NeuroImage chronic social pain for physical pain
social support as predictors of long-term 47(3), 881–890 (2009). conditions.
functional disability and pain in early
15 Watkins LR, Maier SF. The pain of being 24 Riva P, Wirth JH, Williams KD. The
rheumatoid arthritis. Behav. Res. Ther. 41(11),
sick: implications of immune-to-brain consequences of pain: the social and physical
1295–1310 (2003).
communication for understanding pain. pain overlap on psychological responses. Eur.
6 Eisenberger NI, Lieberman MD, Williams Annu. Rev. Psychol. 51(1), 29–57 (2000). J. Soc. Psychol. 41(6), 681–687 (2011).
KD. Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of
16 Dewall CN, Macdonald G, Webster GD et al. 25 Twenge JM, Baumeister RF, Dewall CN,
social exclusion. Science 302(5643), 290–292
Acetaminophen reduces social pain: Ciarocco NJ, Bartels JM. Social exclusion
(2003).
behavioral and neural evidence. Psychol. Sci. decreases prosocial behavior. J. Pers. Soc.
7 Craig A. Significance of the insula for the 21(7), 931–937 (2010). Psychol. 92(1), 56–66 (2007).
evolution of human awareness of feelings
17 Way BM, Taylor SE, Eisenberger NI. 26 Maner JK, Dewall CN, Baumeister RF,
from the body. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 1225,
Variation in the μ-opioid receptor gene Schaller M. Does social exclusion motivate
72–82 (2011).
(OPRM1) is associated with dispositional and interpersonal reconnection? Resolving the
8 Kross E, Berman MG, Mischel W, Smith EE, neural sensitivity to social rejection. Proc. Natl “porcupine problem.” J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.
Wager TD. Social rejection shares Acad. Sci. USA 106(35), 15079–15084 92(1), 42 (2007).
somatosensory representations with physical (2009).
pain. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108(15), 27 Ehnvall A, Mitchell PB, Hadzi-Pavlovic D,
18 Craig A. An interoceptive neuroanatomical Malhi GS, Parker G. Pain during depression
6270–6275 (2011).
perspective on feelings, energy, and effort. and relationship to rejection sensitivity. Acta
9 Gundel H, O’Connor MF, Littrell L, Fort C, Behav. Brain Sci. 36(6), 685–686 (2013). Psychiatr. Scand. 119(5), 375–382 (2009).
Lane RD. Functional neuroanatomy of grief:
19 Eisenberger NI. The pain of social 28 Klages SV, Wirth JH. Excluded by laughter:
an FMRI study. Am. J. Psychiatry 160(11),
disconnection: examining the shared neural laughing until it hurts someone else. J. Soc.
1946–1953 (2003).
underpinnings of physical and social pain. Psychol. 154(1), 8–13 (2014).
10 O’connor MF, Wellisch DK, Stanton AL, Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13(6), 421–434 (2012).
Eisenberger NI, Irwin MR, Lieberman MD. 29 Bruehl S, Burns JW, Chung OY, Ward P,
20 Woo C-W, Koban L, Kross E et al. Separate Johnson B. Anger and pain sensitivity in
Craving love? Enduring grief activates brain’s
neural representations for physical pain and

72 Pain Manag. (2016) 6(1) future science group


Social pain & physical pain Review

chronic low back pain patients and pain-free 41 Connor KM, Davidson JR. Development of a through lower pain catastrophizing. Pers.
controls: the role of endogenous opioids. Pain new resilience scale: the Connor-Davidson Individ. Dif. 52(3), 401–405 (2012).
99(1), 223–233 (2002). Resilience Scale (CD-RISC). Depress. Anxiety 52 Karsdorp PA, Ranson S, Nijst S, Vlaeyen JW.
30 Thibodeau MA, Welch PG, Katz J, 18(2), 76–82 (2003). Goals, mood and performance duration on
Asmundson GJ. Pain-related anxiety 42 Smith BW, Zautra AJ. Vulnerability and cognitive tasks during experimentally induced
influences pain perception differently in men resilience in women with arthritis: test of a mechanical pressure pain. J. Behav. Ther. Exp.
and women: a quantitative sensory test across two-factor model. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. Psychiatry 44(2), 240–247 (2013).
thermal pain modalities. Pain 154(3), 76(5), 799–810 (2008). 53 Franks HM, Cronan TA, Oliver K. Social
419–426 (2013). •• Early study examining vulnerability and support in women with fibromyalgia: is quality
31 Feldman SI, Downey G, Schaffer-Neitz R. resilience factors in chronic pain, more important than quantity? J. Community
Pain, negative mood, and perceived support highlighting the importance of both social Psychol. 32(4), 425–438 (2004).
in chronic pain patients: a daily diary study of resources and positive emotional states for 54 Sullivan MJ, Thorn B, Haythornthwaite JA
people with reflex sympathetic dystrophy promotion of effective pain adaptation. et al. Theoretical perspectives on the relation
syndrome. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 67(5), between catastrophizing and pain. Clin. J.
43 Sturgeon JA, Zautra AJ. Resilience: a new
776–785 (1999). Pain 17(1), 52–64 (2001).
paradigm for adaptation to chronic pain.
32 Sturgeon JA, Zautra AJ, Arewasikporn A. A Curr. Pain Headache Rep. 14(2), 105–112 55 Hamilton NA, Karoly P, Zautra AJ. Health
multilevel structural equation modeling (2010). goal cognition and adjustment in women with
analysis of vulnerabilities and resilience fibromyalgia. J. Behav. Med. 28(5), 455–466
44 Montoya P, Larbig W, Braun C, Preissl H,
resources influencing affective adaptation to (2005).
Birbaumer N. Influence of social support and
chronic pain. Pain 155(2), 292–298 (2014).
emotional context on pain processing and 56 Romano JM, Turner JA, Jensen MP et al.
33 Sturgeon JA, Dixon EA, Darnall BD, Mackey magnetic brain responses in fibromyalgia. Chronic pain patient-spouse behavioral
SC. Contributions of physical function and Arthritis Rheum. 50(12), 4035–4044 (2004). interactions predict patient disability. Pain
satisfaction with social roles to emotional 63(3), 353–360 (1995).
45 Younger J, Aron A, Parke S, Chatterjee N,
distress in chronic pain: a Collaborative Health
Mackey S. Viewing pictures of a romantic 57 Boothby JL, Thorn BE, Overduin LY, Ward
Outcomes Information Registry (CHOIR)
partner reduces experimental pain: LC. Catastrophizing and perceived partner
study. Pain 156(12), 2627– 2633 (2015).
involvement of neural reward systems. PLoS responses to pain. Pain 109(3), 500–506
34 Zautra AJ, Hamilton NA, Burke HM. ONE 5(10), e13309 (2010). (2004).
Comparison of stress responses in women
46 Onoda K, Okamoto Y, Nakashima K, 58 Linton SJ, Ryberg M. A cognitive–behavioral
with two types of chronic pain: fibromyalgia
Nittono H, Ura M, Yamawaki S. Decreased group intervention as prevention for persistent
and osteoarthritis. Cognit. Ther. Res. 23(2),
ventral anterior cingulate cortex activity is neck and back pain in a non-patient
209–230 (1999).
associated with reduced social pain during population: a randomized controlled trial.
35 Holzberg AD, Robinson ME, Geisser ME, emotional support. Soc. Neurosci. 4(5), Pain 90(1), 83–90 (2001).
Gremillion HA. The effects of depression and 443–454 (2009). 59 Birnie K, Speca M, Carlson LE. Exploring
chronic pain on psychosocial and physical
47 Fredrickson BL, Levenson RW. Positive self-compassion and empathy in the context of
functioning. Clin. J. Pain 12(2), 118–125
emotions speed recovery from the mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR).
(1996).
cardiovascular sequelae of negative emotions. Stress Health 26(5), 359–371 (2010).
36 Davis MC, Zautra AJ, Smith BW. Chronic Cogn. Emot. 12(2), 191–220 (1998). 60 Mccracken LM, Vowles KE. Acceptance and
pain, stress, and the dynamics of affective
48 Pressman SD, Black LL. Positive emotions commitment therapy and mindfulness for
differentiation. J. Pers. 72(6), 1133–1159
and immunity. In: The Oxford Handbook of chronic pain: model, process, and progress.
(2004).
Psychoneuroimmunology. Segerstrom SC (Ed.). Am. Psychol. 69(2), 178 (2014).
37 Penninx BW, Leveille S, Ferrucci L, Van Eijk Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, 92 61 Veehof MM, Oskam M-J, Schreurs KM,
J, Guralnik JM. Exploring the effect of (2012). Bohlmeijer ET. Acceptance-based
depression on physical disability: longitudinal
49 Fredrickson BL. The role of positive emotions interventions for the treatment of chronic
evidence from the established populations for
in positive psychology. The broaden-and- pain: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
epidemiologic studies of the elderly. Am. J.
build theory of positive emotions. Am. Pain 152(3), 533–542 (2011).
Public Health 89(9), 1346–1352 (1999).
Psychol. 56(3), 218–226 (2001). • Systemic review of the efficacy of third-wave
38 Hawkley LC, Thisted RA, Cacioppo JT.
Loneliness predicts reduced physical activity: •• Proposes the ‘broaden-and-build’ theory of psychotherapies (mindfulness-based stress
cross-sectional & longitudinal analyses. positive emotions, explaining how the reduction and acceptance and commitment
Health Psychol. 28(3), 354–363 (2009). experience of these emotions can have therapy) in chronic pain populations.
enhancing effects for cognition and behavior. 62 Goleman D. Emotional intelligence. Why it
39 Cole SW, Hawkley LC, Arevalo JM, Sung
CY, Rose RM, Cacioppo JT. Social regulation 50 Finan PH, Quartana PJ, Smith MT. Positive can matter more than IQ. Learning 24(6),
of gene expression in human leukocytes. and negative affect dimensions in chronic 49–50 (1996).
Genome Biol. 8(9), R189 (2007). knee osteoarthritis: effects on clinical and 63 Lieberman MD. Social: Why Our Brains are
laboratory pain. Psychosom. Med. 75(5), Wired to Connect. Oxford University Press,
40 Friborg O, Hjemdal O, Rosenvinge JH,
463–470 (2013). Oxford, UK (2013).
Martinussen M, Aslaksen PM, Flaten MA.
Resilience as a moderator of pain and stress. 51 Hood A, Pulvers K, Carrillo J, Merchant G, 64 Zautra EK, Zautra AJ, Gallardo CE, Velasco
J. Psychosom. Res. 61(2), 213–219 (2006). Thomas M. Positive traits linked to less pain L. Can we learn to treat one another better? A

future science group www.futuremedicine.com 73


Review Sturgeon & Zautra

test of a social intelligence curriculum. 69 Palermo TM, Eccleston C, Lewandowski AS, 75 Kitwood T. The dialectics of dementia: with
PLoS ONE 10(6), e0128638 (2015). Williams ACDC, Morley S. Randomized particular reference to Alzheimer’s disease.
•• Recent study examining the benefits from a controlled trials of psychological therapies for Ageing Soc.10(2), 177–196 (1990).
management of chronic pain in children and 76 Zimmerman S, Shier V, Saliba D.
novel intervention designed to improve
adolescents: an updated meta-analytic review. Transforming nursing home culture: evidence
social ties by improving indices of social
Pain 148(3), 387–397 (2010). for practice and policy. Gerontologist
intelligence in young adults.
70 Martire LM, Schulz R, Helgeson VS, Small 54(Suppl. 1), S1–S5 (2014).
65 Masi CM, Chen HY, Hawkley LC, Cacioppo
BJ, Saghafi EM. Review and meta-analysis of 77 Social Intelligence Institute.
JT. A meta-analysis of interventions to reduce
couple-oriented interventions for chronic www.socialintelligenceinstitute.org/
loneliness. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 15(3),
illness. Ann. Behav. Med. 40(3), 325–342
219–266 (2011). 78 Müller R, Gertz KJ, Molton IR et al. Effects
(2010).
66 Zautra Alex J, Infurna Frank J., Zautra E, of a tailored positive psychology intervention
71 Leonard MT, Cano A, Johansen AB. Chronic on well-being and pain in individuals with
Gallardo C, Velasco L. The humanization of
pain in a couples context: a review and chronic pain and a physical disability: a
social relations: nourishment for resilience in
integration of theoretical models and feasibility trial. Clin. J. Pain doi:10.1097/
mid-life. In: New Developments in Emotional
empirical evidence. J. Pain 7(6), 377–390 AJP.0000000000000225 (2015) (Epub ahead
Aging. Ong A, Löckenhoff C (Eds). American
(2006). of print).
Psychological Association, Washington, DC,
USA (2015) (In Press). 72 Vowles KE, Thompson M. The patient- •• Among the first studies to examine the
provider relationship in chronic pain. Curr.
67 Zautra AJ. Fostering resilience to chronic pain efficacy of a positive psychology intervention
Pain Headache Rep. 16(2), 133–138 (2012).
through the humanization of social relations. in a sample of individuals with chronic pain
Presented at: Advancing Pain Care through 73 De Ruddere L, Goubert L, Stevens MaL, and physical disability.
Behavior Change Science & Mobile Health Deveugele M, Craig KD, Crombez G. Health
79 Flink IK, Smeets E, Bergbom S, Peters ML.
Technologies Conference: The Translational care professionals’ reactions to patient pain:
Happy despite pain: pilot study of a positive
Research Institute on Pain in Later Life impact of knowledge about medical evidence
psychology intervention for patients with
(TRIPLL). New York, NY, USA, 1 May 2015. and psychosocial influences. J. Pain 15(3),
chronic pain. Scand. J. Pain 7, 71–79 (2015).
262–270 (2014).
68 Martire LM, Lustig AP, Schulz R, Miller GE, 80 Green B. Adverse effects of psychotherapy.
Helgeson VS. Is it beneficial to involve a 74 Newton BJ, Southall JL, Raphael JH, Ashford
Adv. Psychiatr. Treat. 17(6), 476–476 (2011).
family member? A meta-analysis of RL, Lemarchand K. A narrative review of the
impact of disbelief in chronic pain. Pain 81 Barlow DH. Negative effects from
psychosocial interventions for chronic illness.
Manag. Nurs. 14(3), 161–171 (2013). psychological treatments: a perspective. Am.
Health Psychol. 23(6), 599 (2004).
Psychol. 65(1), 13–20 (2010).

74 Pain Manag. (2016) 6(1) future science group

You might also like