You are on page 1of 297

Hyperacusis and Disorders

of Sound Intolerance
Clinical and Research Perspectives
Editor-in-Chief for Audiology
Brad A. Stach, PhD
Hyperacusis and Disorders
of Sound Intolerance
Clinical and Research Perspectives

Marc Fagelson, PhD


David M. Baguley, BSc, MSc, MBA, PhD
5521 Ruffin Road
San Diego, CA 92123

e-mail: info@pluralpublishing.com
website: http://www.pluralpublishing.com

Copyright © by Plural Publishing, Inc. 2018

Typeset in 11/13 Adobe Garamond by Achorn International


Printed in the United States of America by McNaughton & Gunn

All rights, including that of translation, reserved. No part of this publication may be
reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means,
electronic, mechanical, recording, or otherwise, including photocopying, recording, taping,
Web distribution, or information storage and retrieval systems without the prior written
consent of the publisher.

For permission to use material from this text, contact us by


Telephone: (866) 758-7251
Fax: (888) 758-7255
e-mail: permissions@pluralpublishing.com

Every attempt has been made to contact the copyright holders for material originally printed in
another source. If any have been inadvertently overlooked, the publishers will gladly make the
necessary arrangements at the first opportunity.

Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data

Names: Fagelson, Marc, editor. | Baguley, David (David M.), editor.


Title: Hyperacusis and disorders of sound intolerance : clinical and research
perspectives / [edited by] Marc Fagelson, David M. Baguley.
Description: San Diego, CA : Plural Publishing, [2018] | Includes bibliographical
references and index.
Identifiers: LCCN 2017058142| ISBN 9781944883287 (alk. paper) |
ISBN 1944883282 (alk. paper)
Subjects: | MESH: Hyperacusis—diagnosis | Hyperacusis—therapy | Tinnitus—diagnosis |
Tinnitus—therapy
Classification: LCC RF293.7 | NLM WV 270 | DDC 617.8/9—dc23
LC record available at https://lccn.loc.gov/2017058142
Contents

Preface vii
Contributors ix

SECTION I. DEFINITIONS, MEASUREMENT, AND EPIDEMIOLOGY

1 Disorders of Sound Tolerance: History and Terminology 3


Marc Fagelson and David M. Baguley
2 Audiological Assessment of Decreased Sound Tolerance 15
Glynnis A. Tidball and Marc Fagelson
3 The Epidemiology and Natural History of Disorders of Loudness Perception 33
David M. Baguley
4 Scales and Questionnaires for Decreased Sound Tolerance 43
Kathryn Fackrell and Derek J. Hoare

SECTION II. MECHANISMS

5 Peripheral Mechanisms of Decreased Sound Tolerance 61


Roland Schaette
6 Tinnitus and Hyperacusis: Relationship, Mechanisms, and Initiating Conditions 77
Larry E. Roberts, Tanit Ganz Sanchez, and Ian C. Bruce
7 Hyperacusis: Medical Diagnoses and Associated Syndromes 107
Don McFerran
8 Animal Models of Hyperacusis and Decreased Sound Tolerance 133
Jos J. Eggermont
9 Traumatic Brain Injury and Auditory Processing 149
Melissa A. Papesh, Sarah M. Theodoroff, and Frederick J. Gallun
10 Psychological Aspects and Management of Hyperacusis 167
Gerhard Andersson

SECTION III. AUDITORY DISORDERS: MANIFESTATIONS

11 Reflections on the Association between Hyperacusis and Tinnitus 181


David M. Baguley

v
vi Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

12 Diplacusis 191
Marc Fagelson

SECTION IV. MANAGEMENT

13 Increased Sound Sensitivity in Children 207


Veronica Kennedy, Claire Benton, and Rosie Kentish
14 Hearing Aids for Decreased Sound Tolerance and Minimal Hearing Loss: 223
Gain Without Pain
Grant D. Searchfield and Caroline Selvaratnam
15 Hyperacusis Management: A Patient’s Perspective 241
Rob Littwin
16 Hyperacusis: Past, Present, and Future 265
Marc Fagelson and David M. Baguley

Index 271
Preface

For most of us, life is full of sound. While there demiology and natural history of hyperacusis
are some individuals who lead a solitary, silent, is sparse, and inconsistent where it does exist.
or contemplative life, the more common path Some aspects of the physiological mechanisms
is replete with auditory stimulation. On an of loudness or sound intensity perception in
early morning walk in a North American city the auditory brain remain obscure, and there
before conference presentations, the two edi- is a disconnect between the auditory neu-
tors of this book were intentionally attentive roscience and the clinical communities that
to the sounds around them, including sirens, remains difficult to bridge. The experiences
engine sounds, phones ringing, and shout- of individuals with reduced sound tolerance
ing. Many of the sound alerts in common use is heterogeneous, and can vary on a day to
have been designed so as to be as compelling, day, or hour to hour basis, and in some this
and in a sense, as annoying as possible, cut- is modulated by emotional and psychologi-
ting through distractions and other sounds to cal state as well as the auditory environment.
demand immediate and sustained attention Tools to assess the extent and severity of loud-
(Patterson, 1990; Vastfjall et al., 2014). ness tolerance symptoms are crude, and in
For many years substantial scientific and some cases may be deeply uncomfortable for
clinical effort has been expended in understand- the patient, as may be the case in some meth-
ing and ameliorating the impact of reduced ods of ascertaining the threshold of loudness
hearing, resulting in sophisticated technologies discomfort using sound stimulation. There is
such as hearing aids and auditory implants, little in the way of hard evidence regarding
advanced surgical techniques, and intensive therapy, and which interventions might be
rehabilitation strategies. Far, far less attention optimal for which type of patient.
and scrutiny have been given to the experiences All this may seem daunting, and lead one
of individuals for whom the world of sound is to consider that the topic of decreased sound
more intense, more vivid, and perhaps perceived tolerance cannot sensibly be addressed at all.
as more toxic than is usual. Most audiologists However, looking at other fields would lead us
and otologists know of patients for whom every- to disagree. The field of pain studies was, until
day sound evokes discomfort, distress, aversion, recently, in a similar state to that which is ob-
and in some, pain. served regarding hyperacusis (Chen, 2011),
This book seeks to explore and help the but systematic and focused endeavor has led
reader begin to understand those experiences. to deeper understanding of mechanisms and
Some challenges will be encountered. The the development of therapies that can be rig-
vocabulary used to describe such experiences orously assessed in well-designed clinical tri-
is varied and imprecise, including decreased, als. The present volume represents an attempt
reduced, or collapsed sound tolerance, and to take a first step on a similar journey, and
hyperacusis. Definitions of each of these terms it is our hope that it will inspire and provoke
varies; given such fundamental differences, it colleagues in both the auditory neuroscience
is not surprising that data regarding the epi- and the clinical (especially audiology, otology,

vii
viii Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

and neurology) communities to discuss, ex-


plore, and research hyperacusis and related
References
phenomena. Given the present state of knowl-
edge, different authors have used different ter- Chen, J. (2011). History of pain theories. Neurosci.
minology, and have varied opinions, in their Bull., Oct, 27(5), 343–350. doi: 10.1007/s12264
chapters: while we have edited with a goal of -011-0139-0.
improving clarity and consistency, we have Patterson, R. (1990). Auditory warning sounds in
preserved some of these variations so that dis- the work environment. Philos. Trans. R Soc. Lond.
tinctive opinions, interpretations, and hypoth- B Biol. Sci., 327(1241), 485–492.
eses might be heard. Vastfjall, D., Bergman, P., Sköld, A., Tajadura, A.,
& Larsson, P. (2012). Emotional responses to
Thanks are due to each of the authors
information and warning sounds. J. Ergonom-
for their fine contributions, and the time and ics, 1, 106. doi:10.4172/2165-7556.1000106.
effort expended on them, to Plural Publishing
for agreeing to support this work, and to our Note. DB is funded through the NIHR Bio-
families and colleagues for their support and medical Research Centre program, however,
forbearance. We would like to express our ap- the views expressed are those of the authors
preciation also to the many patients who have and not necessarily those of the NIHR, the
shared their experiences of hyperacusis and NHS, or the Department of Health.
other phenomena of disordered loudness per-
ception, from whom we have gleaned knowl-
edge, experience, and inspiration.

—Marc Fagelson
David M. Baguley
Contributors

Gerhard Andersson, PhD, Dr. Med. Sci. Department of Electrical and Computer
Professor in Clinical Psychology Engineering
Department of Behavioural Sciences McMaster University
and Learning Hamilton, Ontario, Canada
Linköping University Chapter 6
Linköping, Sweden
Department of Clinical Neuroscience Jos J. Eggermont, PhD
Karolinska Institute Emeritus Professor
Stockholm, Sweden Physiology and Pharmacology, and
Chapter 10 Psychology
University of Calgary
David M. Baguley, BSc, MSc, MBA, Calgary, Alberta, Canada
PhD, FAAA Chapter 8
Professor of Hearing Services
President, British Tinnitus Association Kathryn Fackrell, PhD
Nottingham Biomedical Research Centre Research Fellow
University of Nottingham University of Nottingham
Otology and Hearing Group Nottingham, United Kingdom
Division of Clinical Neuroscience Chapter 4
University of Nottingham
Honorary Consultant Clinical Scientist Marc Fagelson, PhD
(Audiology) Professor
Nottingham University Hospitals NHS Department of Audiology and Speech
Foundation Trust Language Pathology
Nottingham, United Kingdom East Tennessee State University
Chapters 1, 3, 11, and 16 Johnson City, Tennessee
Chapters 1, 2, 12, and 16
Claire Benton, MSc
Consultant Clinical Scientist (Audiology) Frederick J. Gallun, PhD
Nottingham Audiology Services, Research Investigator, VA RR&D
Nottingham University Hospitals NHS National Center for Rehabilitative Auditory
Foundation Trust Research
Nottingham, UK Assistant Professor
Chapter 13 Department of Otolaryngology—Head
and Neck Surgery
Ian C. Bruce, PhD, PEng, FASA Oregon Health and Science University
Professor and Associate Chair for Graduate Portland, Oregon
Studies Chapter 9

ix
x Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

Derek J. Hoare, PhD McMaster University


Senior Research Fellow Hamilton, Ontario, Canada
University of Nottingham Chapter 6
Nottingham, United Kingdom
Chapter 4 Prof. Dr. Tanit Ganz Sanchez, MD, PhD
Associate Professor of Otolaryngology
Veronica Kennedy, FRCS(Oto), FRCP, University São Paulo School of Medicine
FRCPCH Founder of Instituto Ganz Sanchez
Consultant Audiovestibular Physician São Paulo, Brazil
Bolton NHS Foundation Trust Chapter 6
Bolton, United Kingdom
Chapter 13 Roland Schaette, PhD
Lecturer
Rosie Kentish, MA University College London, Ear Institute
Consultant Clinical Psychologist London, United Kingdom
Royal National Throat Nose and Ear Chapter 5
Hospital
London, United Kingdom Grant D. Searchfield, BSc, MAud(Hons),
Chapter 13 PhD
Associate Professor, Audiology
Rob Littwin, MA Director, Hearing and Tinnitus Clinic
Musician The University of Auckland
Producer Member, The University of Auckland Centre
Arranger for Brain Research and Brain Research
Atlanta, Georgia New Zealand
Chapter 15 Scientific Director, TinnitusTunes
Auckland, New Zealand
Don McFerran, MA, FRCS Chapter 14
Consultant ENT Surgeon
Colchester Hospital Caroline Selvaratnam, BSc, MAud,
University NHS Foundation Trust MNZAS
Colchester, United Kingdom Audiologist
Chapter 7 University of Auckland Clinics
Hearing and Tinnitus Clinic
Melissa A. Papesh, PhD, AuD University of Auckland
Research Investigator, VA RR&D Auckland, New Zealand
National Center for Rehabilitative Auditory Chapter 14
Research
Portland, Oregon Sarah M. Theodoroff, PhD
Chapter 9 Research Investigator, VA RR&D
National Center for Rehabilitative Auditory
Larry E. Roberts, PhD Research
Professor Emeritus Assistant Professor
Department of Psychology Neuroscience Department of Otolaryngology—Head
and Behaviour and Neck Surgery
Contributors xi

Oregon Health and Science University University of British Columbia


Portland, Oregon Audiologist
Chapter 9 Department of Audiology
St. Paul’s Hospital
Glynnis A. Tidball, MSc, RAUD Vancouver, Canada
Clinical Instructor Chapter 2
School of Audiology and Speech Sciences
Marc and David, as well as many other professionals and the contributors to this text,
continue to learn what it means for people and their families to live with hyperacusis
and other forms of sound intolerance; this book is dedicated to the many individuals,
patients, and providers, who sparked and continue to nurture that interest.
SECTION I
Definitions, Measurement,
and Epidemiology
1
Disorders of Sound Tolerance:
History and Terminology
Marc Fagelson and David M. Baguley

diologic tests or routine activities precludes


Introduction
development of interventions for individuals
with hearing impairment that are consistently
Traditionally the discipline of audiology has as effective as, for example, glasses for myopia.
been concerned with the challenges faced by A similar, complex and multifactorial relation
people with reduced hearing abilities in order appears to exist between disorders of loudness
to alleviate the effects associated with hearing perception, their impact, and the handicap
loss. While there is a multitude of such peo- that is associated. This chapter will focus spe-
ple, and the burdens they face are certainly cifically on basic elements related to disorders
substantial, in recent years there has been in- of sound tolerance  —items such as terminol-
creasing awareness of a population of people ogy and models, both auditory-based and
who have the experience of sound being too psychology-based, that support the terminol-
intense for them, rather than being too quiet. ogy  —that continue to vex providers, patients,
In this chapter we introduce this topic, and and students who seek clarity when managing
critically examine the various definitions and patients in distress.
models that have been proposed, and identify
where present knowledge is in­adequate.
The impact of hearing loss, particularly
History
of sensory and/or neural origin on the audi-
tory system’s loudness processing, is difficult
to predict, and prone to the influence of many The concept that human hearing varies with the
non-auditory factors. Although hearing aid sound environment and context in which the
use often improves an individual’s communi- person is listening has long been accepted,
cation ability and quality of life, the lack of the English phrase, ‘I could hear a pin drop’ ar-
a straightforward relation between a patient’s ticulating the experience of intense and active
sensitivity and their behavior on specific au- listening. The experience of hearing becoming

3
4 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

acute at times of emotional intensity is also in terms of dynamic range, or the signal inten-
known, and used extensively in horror films: sities that comprise the range of values span-
for example, the trope of increasing the loud- ning the patient’s threshold of sensitivity to
ness of an enemy’s footsteps as they approach their threshold of pain or discomfort. In hear-
along a gravel drive is widespread. The fact ing impaired listeners, the dynamic range is
that an increase in the perceived intensity of reduced in one of two ways. First, in the pres-
the sound environment can be involuntary is ence of loudness recruitment thresholds of
less well known, however. This forms a major sensitivity may be elevated while thresholds of
plot device in the novel The Woman in White discomfort remain essentially unchanged. Al­
by Wilkie Collins (1860), although the symp- ternatively, in cases of hyperacusis, the thresh­
tom is un-named. The heroine of the novel is old of discomfort may change over time, and
Miss Laura Fairlie, who as the novel opens has become abnormally low; regardless of absolute
recently been orphaned, and has inherited some sensitivity, the patient’s dynamic range would
money, which renders her attractive to pred- be reduced. In extreme cases, perhaps such
atory suitors, and she seeks the protection of as that described by Mr. Fairlie, the patient’s
her uncle, Mr Frederick Fairlie. Unfortunately, dynamic range may collapse and become lim-
he is unable to support her, as he is living as ited to such a narrow range of intensities that
a reclusive invalid, due to a number of symp- the patient develops aversions to most routine
toms, one of which is a decreased ability to tol- sounds and curtails life activities accordingly.
erate sound. In the first conversation between No mention is made of Mr. Fairlie hav-
niece and uncle, held at some distance across ing a hearing loss, and he is probably an illus­
a semi-darkened room, Frederick Fairlie artic- tration of decreased sound tolerance with nor-
ulates his situation thus: mal or age-appropriate hearing thresholds.
In the case of a person with hearing loss, it
Pray excuse me. But could you contrive to is valuable to consider the paradox at work,
speak in a lower key? In the wretched state as the presence of that hearing loss should, at
of my nerves, loud sound of any kind is in- least on the surface, allow a person to tolerate
describable torture to me. You will pardon an moderate sound levels without feeling dis-
invalid? comfort. However, many patients with hear-
ing loss experience inordinate loudness in the
Mr. Fairlie thus describes intense sound- presence of what should be acceptable sound
evoked pain, leading to invalidity. While levels, powerful negative emotions in the pres­
Laura eventually transcends the multiple ad- ence of sounds that most other people do not
verse events that befall her, her uncle’s situa- find objectionable, or sounds that cause pain
tion, which is normally seen by literary critics in and around the ears. Patients affected by
as an abrogation of his male responsibilities, is such sounds may practice avoidance strategies
a major setback. to minimize their discomfort; we must also
Sound intolerance, such as that de- consider that strategy’s shortcomings.
scribed by the unfortunate Mr. Fairlie, often ac­ We hope to expand upon the idea, ex-
companies hearing loss across patient groups pressed in the book’s preface, that patients
of all ages, backgrounds, and auditory history, would benefit from greater consensus among
although it may also be present in individuals professionals regarding terms and definitions
with hearing thresholds within the range of related to unusual sound experiences. If a
normal. In practice, this situation is described patient avoids situations and environments
1. Disorders of Sound Tolerance: History and Terminology 5

because of aversions to specific sounds or is, signal level 10 dB higher than the patient’s
sound levels, then that patient’s discomfort threshold of hearing) was rated as a loudness
and emotional response could be exacerbated (i.e., on a 1 to 100 scale) higher than the value
by seemingly inconsistent advice and counsel- provided by a normal-hearing person. Further,
ing from health care providers. Accurate and the rating provided by the listener with hearing
effective counseling relies upon many factors; loss increased by a greater amount than that
certainly, the list would include an agreed- of the normal hearing listener given identical
upon lexicon. In this regard, audiologists have increases in stimulus level. When loudness
much to learn from the psychological and rating was graphed as a function of stimulus
trauma literatures in which a variety of clini­ level, the hearing-impaired listener functions
cal approaches that target patients’ beliefs and were steeper than those for normal hearing
understanding take a primary role in the inter- listeners. Investigators also found that, in gen-
vention. When basic terminology cannot be eral, at stimulus levels higher than 90 to 95 dB
agreed upon, the potential for patient learn- SPL, regardless of the hearing loss magnitude,
ing and adapting to a challenging condition the loudness functions as rated by hearing-
likely decreases. In this chapter, we will review impaired listeners approached and often could
definitions and terms currently in use; our in- be superimposed upon those functions from
tent is to move the conversation forward in a normal hearing listeners. This loudness growth
way that prioritizes lexical items supporting pattern was termed “loudness recruitment” by
the needs of patients and practitioners. Fowler.
The loudness recruitment label captured
a few interesting aspects of auditory system
function. First, Fowler and other investigators
employed the term to describe the way that
Hearing Loss and an array of auditory nerve fibers would be in-
Loudness Growth fluenced by damage to the cochlear hair cells.
Given that not all fibers in the auditory nerve
displayed identical thresholds, the loss of hair
Loudness Recruitment cells should have the most substantial effect on
fibers with low thresholds, or those that would
The development of the audiometer improved under normal conditions respond to audible
the precision with which auditory thresholds signals of low intensity. The change in nerve
were estimated. Audiometers generated signals innervation would commonly follow damage
covering a broad range of frequency and inten- to some of the cochlea’s most vulnerable com-
sity, and in addition to threshold measures fa- ponents, the outer hair cells. While the sensory
cilitated identifying the relation between sound fiber population serving the outer hair cells
intensity and the perception of loudness. Early would not be expected to influence audibility,
investigators (Fowler, 1936; Steinberg & Gard- the weakened contribution of the outer hair
ner, 1937) reported that patients with hear- cells to basilar membrane mechanics would re-
ing loss experienced, in addition to elevated sult in reduced stimulation of inner hair cells,
thresholds, an unusual relation between the in- thereby producing mild-moderate hearing loss.
tensity of a stimulus and the loudness it evoked. At stimulus levels exceeding 50 to 60 dB,
For some patients with hearing loss, a signal however, the relative contribution of the
10 dB SL regarding the puretone threshold (that outer hair cell system to cochlear mechanics
6 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

decreases under normal conditions, and the soft (Buus & Florentine, 2002), as patients re-
displacement along the basilar membrane is ported sounds in the hearing loss region, usu-
sufficient to activate inner hair cells and gener- ally the high frequency region, produced sub-
ate an audible response regardless of the outer stantial loudness even at intensity levels that
hair cell component. The higher stimulus lev- barely exceeded threshold. A sound was louder
els would increase the likelihood that fibers for such patients at threshold than it would be
with higher thresholds would be stimulated. for an unaffected listener; patients either did
Therefore, the ensuing neural activity would not hear a sound in the hearing loss region, or,
be dominated by fibers that respond exclu- when they did, even at low sensation levels, the
sively to moderate or high-level signals. Be- sound was never soft. Rather, the ability to hear
cause these fibers code high-intensity stimuli, a sound as soft appeared beyond the patient’s
the ensuing perception would feature the sen- processing ability, and Buus and Florentine
sation of high or perhaps excessive loudness (2002) suggested that the unusual experience
even though the sound did not exceed the lis- of pronounced loudness at threshold was likely
tener’s threshold by a substantial amount. a better way to describe the effect of recruit-
At the upper limit of loudness tolerance, ment than that provided by the traditional
the hearing-impaired listener’s experience of definition of abnormal growth of loudness ac-
loudness mimicked that of normal hearing lis­ companying suprathreshold signals. Addition-
teners even though the stimulus did not exceed ally, patients with trauma histories, or combat
their thresholds by a similar amount, or sen- exposure, often reported the exaggerated startle
sation level. Such patient reports were consis- response that they experienced when sounds
tent with the idea that the high-threshold fi- would appear to “come out of nowhere” and
bers were “recruited” into action even though grow to painful loudness instantaneously. The
the lower-threshold, more sensitive fibers were potential value of hearing aids may be limited
no longer capable of encoding low intensity for such patients, and the reader is urged to
sounds. Hence, the observation that, for high consider a listening program such as that pro-
level signals, while the relation between loud- vided by Rob Littwin in Chapter 15.
ness and stimulus is similar across populations Unfortunately, many patients who ex-
with dif­ferent amounts of hearing loss, the rela- perience routine events that produce unusual
tion is substantially different at low-to-moderate loudness experiences often respond to the dis­
levels. comfort by avoiding what they perceive as un-
As discussed in Chapter 4, the measures safe areas, or by wearing hearing protection
of loudness tolerance may be of more value for even when sound levels pose no threat. As the
decisions regarding intervention than those patients often have hearing loss, the use of
of absolute threshold despite the lack of a hearing protection seems intuitive, and may be
method for loudness measures as reliable and urged by an audiologist or other professional.
replicable as that used for puretone thresholds. After all, if the loss of ability to hear soft sounds
The loudness growth pattern described above is related to hearing loss, then protecting the
would correspond to cochlear hearing loss; remaining hearing must be a priority. It there-
indeed, many audiology students were taught fore surprises many patients, and even some
that loudness recruitment was the “hallmark” professionals, to learn that hearing protection
of a cochlear hearing loss. However, hearing may be overused, or even abused by patients
loss may produce an unusual perception that was whose intention is merely to protect what hear­
described as a loss of ability to hear sounds as ing they have left.
1. Disorders of Sound Tolerance: History and Terminology 7

Hyperacusis “collapsed sound tolerance.” His difficulties


with sound began at a long-anticipated sports
Although recruitment is a typical consequence event, attended with his children. His family
of cochlear hearing loss, the term is some- was seated close to a blaring loudspeaker, and
times used interchangeably with hyperacusis. Dan soon realized he was being injured as an
This situation is unfortunate because the two immediate reduction in his ability to tolerate
disorders are profoundly different with re- sound became increasingly apparent, exacer-
spect to their prevalence and effect on a pa- bated by further accidental exposure to loud
tient’s quality of life. Although recruitment sound. His description of his troubles is mov-
is consistent with reduced dynamic range of ing and lucid:
hearing, hyperacusis suggests a more substan-
tial reduction, perhaps a collapse, of loudness My symptoms were severe and my life was
tolerance, and by extension, dynamic range. collapsing around me. In a matter of only a
Patients may experience pain when they hear few days I could no longer tolerate the normal
sounds that were once pleasant, such as con- sounds of life (conversation, telephone, televi-
sion). Even my young children whispering to
versations or amplified music. The change in
me or the sound of turning my head on my pil-
auditory processing associated with hyper­
low at night was distressing. Malcore (2013)
acusis can arise from many potential sources.
Patient narratives and passages in literature
illuminate the confounding and existential Adults who experience hyperacusis are often
thoughts accompanying offending sounds (Ba- able to articulate their situation verbally, but
guley, 2016). this may not be the case for children. In such
One recent example is provided by the a situation, it is possible to ask them to draw
poet Ruby Robinson who incorporated her a picture of themselves with their symptoms,
challenges with sound tolerance in a poem en­ and an example is given in Figure 1–1. This
titled Internal Gain (2016), from which this young man experiencing severe sound tolerance
is an excerpt: issues, picturing himself as buffeted by sound,
assailed by it as it strikes him. Such pictures en­
able the patient to be heard, and so can be em-
My room was vibrating with electricity sockets
and light beams powering: they also allow parents, teachers,
and I could hear every little sound my and clinicians to see inside the lived experience
mouth made. of hyperacusis.
One can imagine the lure that hearing
Outside my window protection devices would afford a person go-
a butterfly, miniscule on a roof tile ing through what the people above describe.
rubbed its wings together If sounds were painful, logic would dictate
excruciatingly. that they were also potentially damaging. As
the patient may experience hearing loss con-
Such literary depictions of decreased current with their difficulties tolerating rea-
sound tolerance chime with written descrip- sonable sound levels, they may be encouraged
tions of the experience. Dan Malcore is the by a professional to protect their ears in or-
founder of the Internet community at www der to preserve residual hearing. The patient
.hyperacusis.net, which is an invaluable re- may be convinced that hearing protection is
source for people with what is described as needed in an increasing number of situations,
8 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

Figure 1–1. A young patient’s depiction of the hyperacusis experience.

again to minimize the probability that they will termination. Clearly, the auditory pathway’s
incur additional damage. Unfortunately, for loudness processing evolved rapidly to chang-
patients in both clinics and experimental pro- ing conditions in the environment, or with re­
tocols, hearing protection often was not the spect to the peripheral system’s integrity; this
panacea it initially appeared to be. finding illustrated that hearing protection could
Use of earplugs was linked to changes in be overused, thereby exacerbating the problem
loudness tolerance in innovative experiments for which it was intended.
by Formby and colleagues (Formby et al., 2003). Indeed, for some individuals, habitual
Participants wore an earplug in one ear and a hearing protection use reduces loudness tol-
low-level in-ear masking device in the other erance to the point that routine activities can-
ear throughout the day. At the end of two not be managed or endured. Again, it is as if
weeks, loudness discomfort levels (LDLs) were the ability to tolerate most sound collapses. To
compared to the pre-experiment values. In date, medical terminology and definitions of
two weeks’ time, the plugged ears displayed a decreased sound tolerance have not quite been
lowering (less tolerance) than pre-experiment able to articulate the depth of patient experi-
levels while the masker ears displayed an in- ences, and the anguish that may be associated.
creased LDL relative to baseline. The changes The term hyperacusis derives from the Greek
occurred relatively quickly, and reversed them- words for “over” or “above” and “hearing.”
selves within a few days after the experiment’s This translation may be misleading for our
1. Disorders of Sound Tolerance: History and Terminology 9

purposes as it implies better than average sen- First, Jack Vernon was a pioneer of the clinical
sitivity, but at the threshold level; our interest understanding of tinnitus who noticed that
is at the other end of the dynamic range. In- some of his tinnitus patients experienced de-
terestingly, Google translation of hyperacusis creased sound tolerance, though his estimates
into Greek, and then back to English, yields of this fall considerably short of that which is
the term “infrared,” another example of the presently believed (see Chapter 11). Vernon de­
inadequacies of unsophisticated translation. fined hyperacusis as “unusual tolerance to or-
Hyperacusis refers to a general perception of dinary environmental sounds” (Vernon, 1987).
the sound environment as intense and adverse. Vernon’s neutral definition contrasts with a con­
An early mention of the hyperacusic response temporary account from the otology litera-
to sound was reported by Paul of Aegina (625 ture, which has a pejora­tive tone:
to 690 AD) (Adams, 1844, cited in Stephens,
2000). When writing about individuals who consistently exaggerated or inappropriate re-
experienced persistent tinnitus, the physician sponses or complaints to sounds that are nei-
indicated that some of those patients also suf- ther intrinsically threatening or uncomfortably
fered from what he termed “increased sensi- loud to a typical person. (Klein et al., 1990)
bility,” or an experience of inordinate loudness
to sounds that others could tolerate easily. It In a further attention to strike a neutral or cau­
must be noted that Paul wrote during a time tiously positive tone, a partnership between an
when there were few intense sounds produced audiologist and a psychologist resulted in:
by humans; perhaps a blacksmith, or a child
shouting/screaming would fit the bill. experience of inordinate loudness of sound
Laurence Turnbull (1881) identified sev- that most people tolerate well, associated with
eral aspects of hearing loss whose remediation a component of distress. (Baguley & Anders-
would continue to frustrate clinicians to this son, 2007)
day. In addition to a prescient review of electri-
cal stimulation to ameliorate the effects of tin- In a scoping review, Fackrell et al. (2017) con-
nitus, Turnbull described patients who reacted sidered definitions of hyperacusis in 43 pub-
powerfully to sound levels that should not pro- lished papers, finding that 14 (33%) did not
duce excessive loudness. He did not specifically provide a working definition of hyperacusis.
link such behavior to the term hyperacusis. Of those that did, the definitions used vari-
Later, a description of reduced sound tolerance ously focused on reductions in sound tolerance,
was offered in the otolaryngology medical litera­ on increased sensitivity to sound, on intolerance
ture in 1938 by Perlman, who used the hyper- to sound, and on reaction to sound. Underpin-
acusis term. The addition of a suffix led to the ning all these themes was a concept of the tol-
term hyperacusis dolorosa by Mathieson in 1969, erance to sound being different from ‘normal.’
referring to the profound sadness experienced The potential for sound to produce such
by some individuals with reduced sound toler­ responses should remind us that one person’s
ance, but this phrase did not achieve widespread hyperacusis may be another person’s enjoyable
acceptance. night out on the town, in that some people will
Several definitions of hyperacusis exist in seek out hyperintense and discordant sound
the literature, perhaps due in part to the var- experiences for pleasure. This is analogous to
ied and sometimes intense nature of patients’ the dissonance between the distressing experi-
reports. Note in those cited below compo- ence of a patient with vertigo, and the exhila-
nents of both auditory and psychological state. rating experience of riding a rollercoaster.
10 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

Auditory status alone cannot predict the of a small creek, or stream, is played out on
sounds that will prove aversive to a listener, the sound generator. When asked about the re­
rather experience and mental health status sponse, the patient may reveal that the sound
influence in powerful ways any perceptual is reminiscent of a stream that flowed by the
event. The one source of security to which location of a war-time facility at which the pa-
many patients may turn, namely hearing pro- tient was a prisoner. Juxtaposed against the
tection, seemingly tricks the auditory path- typical patient response, the jarring and imme­
way into an acceleration of the processing that diate discomfort demonstrated by the affected
contributed to the excessive loudness in the Veteran affirms that one sound may be pro-
first place. If avoidance and hearing protec- cessed in profoundly different ways by two ob­
tion fail to address loudness intolerance, then servers with identical hearing sensitivity, but
an effective method for desensitizing listeners different lifetime experiences. The sound is
must be considered. Rob Littwin’s chapter ex- not excessively loud; however, it produces sub­
pands upon many elements indicated on Dan stantial arousal.
Malcore’s network for managing negative re- Phonophobia has been specifically asso­
sponses to sound. We are fortunate to share ciated with migraine, and this situation is ex­
his work in Chapter 15. plored in Chapter 7, by Don McFerran, wherein
the issues of a terminology based on fear, or
phobia, when the migraineurs experience may
Beyond Audibility: be of irritation or annoyance, is described.
Another form of negative patient re-
Phonophobia and Misophonia
sponse to sound is termed misophonia (  Jastre-
boff & Jastreboff, 2004); while this term has
Patients may experience a variety of other not yet gained wide acceptance among pro-
aversive reactions to sound, in addition to hy- fessionals (although consensus is growing),
peracusis and loudness recruitment. Phono- its presence on a number of self-help Internet
phobia, from the Greek words phone mean- boards ensures that its use will continue. The
ing voice, or sound, and phobia referring to term’s Greek roots are somewhat vague; misos
fear, is a vague term applied to sounds that connoting hatred, and phone, as above, refer-
provoke responses of fear. While some sounds ring to voice or sound. Following its initial use
evoke fear in the majority of hearers, for ex- by Jastreboff and Jastreboff (2004) as a blan-
ample, screams or approaching large vehicles ket term for any annoying sound (fingers on
producing a warning sound, other sounds are a chalkboard, the sound of someone cough-
innocuous to most people, yet provoke fear ing uncontrollably, an annoying dog barking
in others. The response to a sound can change hour after hour), the term has been applied
over time, and it is common in clinical set- primarily to individuals whose lives have been
tings for patients to express that certain sounds, altered by a powerful aversion to the sound of
once pleasing, no longer provide relief, they people chewing, sipping drinks, or clattering
produce distress. Such patients may indicate silverware on a plate (see McFerran, 2016, for
that a bedside sound generator, for use to fa­ review). In a sense, most humans have spe-
cilitate sleep, may exert the opposite effect cific sounds that trigger sensations of anger or
when it is turned on. The chapter’s first author discomfort; however, the patient with miso-
has abundant experience with patients who phonia will report an inventory of additional
are military Veterans, startling and immedi­ sounds that trigger similar, or more powerful,
ately moving to leave the room when the sound emotional responses.
1. Disorders of Sound Tolerance: History and Terminology 11

The possibility that patients complaining between areas associated with the aforemen-
of misophonia were demonstrably different tioned interoception, or the anterior insular
from those who do not was long supported cortex, and the brain areas regulating arousal
by patient narratives and comments on on- and emotional state. The finding reiterated
line discussion boards. However, evidence Garfinkel et al.’s (2016) report that misopho-
supporting specific neural mechanisms under­ nics, as well as individuals on the autism spec-
lying the powerful psychological responses trum, displayed an unusual awareness and
was lacking. Kumar et al. (2016) compared attention to internal sensations. Kumar et al.
20 patients with specific complaints of miso- (2016) asserted that the group differences
phonia to 22 age-matched control subjects. could explain the unusual patient reports that
The participants were presented with three stymied rehabilitation efforts of clinicians
types of sound: neutral sounds that were pre- from a variety of professions. In conclusion,
sumed to evoke a minimal response (such as the investigators reported their data “suggest
rain falling), unpleasant sounds that would that abnormal salience attributed to otherwise
be disliked by most listeners (such as a baby innocuous sounds, coupled with atypical per­
crying), and sounds that misophonic pa- ception of internal body states, underlies mi­
tients routinely cite as triggers (such as people sophonia” (p. 532).
eating or loud breathing). The investiga- Patients with either phonophobia or
tors measured levels of distress by patient re­­ misophonia illustrate that sounds do not have
port. Additionally, they measured a variety of to be loud to be distressing, or to provoke
physiologic responses to the sound, includ- avoidance. Routinely experienced, everyday
ing heart rate and skin conductivity, as well sounds can induce in certain listeners power­
as activity in brain regions associated with ful emotions, such as anger, fear, or disgust;
arousal, monitoring of emotions, and intero- at the same time, other sounds may evoke an-
ception, here referring to a signal’s salience noyance, and in others, pain. The provider is
and identity that may contribute to the mod- essentially at the patient’s mercy when trying
ification of a person’s monitoring of body to categorize or understand the patient’s re-
functions such as the heart rate and respira- ports as the clinician has little or no confirma-
tion rate. tory evidence with which to work. Patients’
The investigators reported substantial hearing thresholds add little to the rehabili-
group differences between the patient ratings tative effort, although accurate and replicable
of sound, as well as the neural centers acti- measurement of LDLs could provide valuable
vated by the sounds. For example, the patients information.
in the misophonia group rated the trigger A model that attempted to describe the
sounds as substantially more distressing than space occupied by a patient with hyperacu-
the unpleasant sounds whereas the nonmiso- sis was proposed by Baguley and Andersson
phonic participants not only expressed a min- (2007), with a particular aim to delineate the
imal difference between the two, on average, influences on life imposed by decreased sound
they rated the unpleasant sounds as more an- tolerance (Figure 1–2). The factors described
noying than the sounds serving as triggers for were noise sensitivity, fear of sound and in-
the misophonics. Both groups rated neutral jury, and irritation/annoyance. In an alterna-
sounds as neither distressing nor annoying. tive proposal, the spectrum of patients’ com-
Physiologic data also delineated the two plaints and resulting challenges for clinicians
groups effectively. In particular, the investiga- was addressed to some degree by a loudness la-
tors noted a consistent functional connectivity beling scheme proposed by Tyler et al. (2014).
12 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

Fear hyperacusis was described by Tyler


et al. (2014) as a response to sound that was
not only aversive, it also provoked stress of an-
ticipation. As such, patients experiencing fear
hyperacusis would display avoidance behav-
iors and would limit their activities when they
were concerned the offending sounds would
be present. Fear hyperacusis could be consid-
ered an analog of phonophobia, as described
above, with the same negative consequences
Figure 1–2. Model describing the space oc-
cupied by a patient with hyperacusis including
for the patient who gives in to temptation to
the patient’s noise sensitivity, fear of sound avoid sound or overprotect hearing.
and injury, and irritation/annoyance. The definition of annoyance hyperacusis
focused on emotions and reactions to certain
sounds. As with some cases of fear hyper-
acusis, the offending sounds might not be
excessively powerful or damaging. Yet despite
the knowledge that the sounds would not hurt
Types of Hyperacusis
hearing, the patient avoided such sounds as
though they were toxic and emotionally
Tyler et al. (2014) proposed four distinct types scarring. Sounds that evoke annoyance hyper-
of hyperacusis in an attempt to simplify ter- acusis would contribute to a patient’s state of
minology, and by doing so, firm the field’s defi- mind by inducing anxiety, irritation, and stress
nitions of these challenging conditions to in a manner outlined convincingly by Kumar
terms that could enjoy broader acceptance, et al. (2016). Similarly, annoyance hyperacusis
online discussion boards notwithstanding. may be considered an analog of misophonia.
Tyler et al. suggested four distinct types of hy- Pain hyperacusis was the fourth label
peracusis, all of which capture one or more of provided by Tyler et al. (2014) and referred
the sound-related challenges to patients sum- to observers experiencing pain in the presence
marized above. of certain sounds. Although the mechanism
Tyler et al.’s four hyperacusis labels in- by which ear pain would be generated in re-
cluded loudness, annoyance, fear, and pain. sponse to sound remained unclear in Tyler
Loudness hyperacusis was the broadest of the et al.’s scheme, Eggermont (in Chapter 8)
terms, indicating that some patients expe- provides a review of current experimentation
rience uncomfortable loudness idiosyncrat- linking inner ear damage in mice to avoidance
ically. Although a patient might display a behaviors via a special class of pain receptors
recruitment-like (i.e., typical) experience with (nociceptors) located in the cochlear nuclei and
loudness, it would be possible for some sounds activated by cochlear hair cells. Although the
to appear louder than others due to their spec- inner ear does not employ pain receptors per
tral content and the uniqueness of the patient’s se, damage to inner ear sensory cells may acti-
hearing loss. In such cases, a patient would vate pain receptors in the brainstem, thereby
complain about certain sounds being uncom- resulting in a sensation of pain deep in the ear
fortably loud without necessarily mentioning in response to loud sounds.
powerful emotions being triggered by such While Tyler and colleagues undertook a
sounds. robust attempt to survey the available litera-
1. Disorders of Sound Tolerance: History and Terminology 13

ture, the four-category model of hyperacusis our fears by giving it a name (Hyperacusis)
has not yet been empirically verified, and the and telling us that it was non-life threatening.
clinical experience of the present authors in- However, it took away many aspects of life
dicates that patients may easily inhabit two or because it is SO limiting. I was unable to con-
more of the categories, and that these are not tinue in my job, as well as in my voluntary
work and all my social activities. I was unable
distinct nor mutually exclusive. Perhaps the
to travel either by car, bus, train or plane, un-
experience of hyperacusis, like that of pain, able to go anywhere, other than for quiet walks
defies simple categorization or description, in ’round the village; unable to visit family or
which case, the final reflections of this chapter friends (near or far), unable to work, difficult
should go to a patient. to talk to anyone on the phone, couldn’t cook
meals in the kitchen, etc. It was like being a
prisoner in my own home. Thankfully, my hus-
band was able to arrange to work from home
where he had to do the cooking, as well as go
A Patient Summarizes with me wherever I went in case I had a panic
attack. (Quoted from Baguley, 2008)
The magnitude of distress experienced by
patients is probably best described by the
patients themselves. What follows is a brief References
report from a patient whose experiences en-
capsulate many aspects of the information
above. The lived experience of reduced sound Baguley, D. M. (2008). Hyperacusis. In R. S. Ty-
tolerance, when chronic, as indicated in this ler (Ed.), The consumer handbook on tinnitus.
Auricle Ink: Sedona, AZ.
excerpt from a female hyperacusis patient,
Baguley, D. M. (2016). Tinnitus in literature. In
in her 4th decade, make it possible for the
D. M. Baguley and M. Fagelson (Eds.), Tinni-
reader to view the patient as a “prisoner” of tus: Clinical and research perspectives (pp. 1–12).
her symptoms: the purpose of this book is to San Diego, CA: Plural.
try to understand how this can happen, and Baguley, D. M. & Andersson, G. (2007). Hyper-
how the situation can be improved. acusis mechanisms, diagnosis, and therapies. San
Diego, CA: Plural.
My partner and I were still unaware what Buus, S. & Florentine, M. (2002). Growth of
this hypersensitivity to noise was and began to loudness in listeners with cochlear hearing
be very anxious, wondering what was wrong losses: recruitment reconsidered. J. Assoc. Res.
with me, and wondering if it might be a brain Otolaryngol, 3, 120–139. doi: 10.1007/s1016
tumour. By now, I couldn’t bear to be in the 20010084
kitchen (couldn’t tolerate the noise of the ket- Collins, W. (1860). The woman in white. London,
tle, boiler, microwave, fridge, etc.), or use the UK: Penguin Classics.
vacuum cleaner, listen to music, TV, play pi- Fackrell K., Potgeiter I., Shekhawat G. S., Baguley
ano. At its worst I couldn’t bear the noise of D. M., Sereda M., & Hoare D. J. (2017). Clin-
the hair dryer, or the sound of cutlery on the ical interventions for Hyperacusis in Adults: a
crockery. Equally, I couldn’t bear to be outside scoping review to assess the current position and
and tolerate noises such as birds chirping, traffic determine priorites for research. BioMed Research
(either close to or in the distance), planes flying International, doi.org/10.1155/2017/2723715
overhead, lawn mowers, [with] the last of these Formby, C., Sherlock, L. P., & Gold, S. L. (2003).
triggering panic attacks. When we finally saw Adaptive plasticity of loudness induced by
the Ear Nose and Throat Specialist, he allayed chronic attenuation and enhancement of the
14 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

acoustic background. Journal of the Acoustical at http://www.entandaudiologynews.com/assets


Society of America, 114, 55–58. /Uploads/Pdfs/Hyperacusis-JF13.pdf
Fowler, E. P. A method for the early detection of McFerran, D. (2016). Misophonia and phonopho-
otosclerosis. Arch Otolaryng, 24, 731–734. bia. In D. M. Baguley & M. Fagelson (Eds.), Tin-
Garfinkel, S. N., Tiley, C., O’Keeffe, S., Harrison, nitus: Clinical and research perspectives (pp. 245–
N. A., Seth, A. K., & Critchley, H. D. (2016). 260). San Diego, CA: Plural.
Discrepancies between dimensions of intercep- Perlman, H. B. (1938). Hyperacusis. Annals of Oto­l­
tion in autism: implications for emotion and ogy, Rhinology, Laryngology, 47, 947–953.
anxiety. Biol. Psychol., 114, 117–126. Robinson, R. (2016). Every little sound. Liverpool,
Jastreboff, P. J., & Jastreboff, M. M. (2004). De- UK: Liverpool University Press.
creased sound tolerance. In: J. B. Snow (Ed.), Steinberg, J. C. & Gardener, M. B. (1937). The
Tinnitus: Theory and management (pp. 8–15). dependency of hearing impairment on sound
London, UK: BC Decker. intensity. J. Acou. Soc. Am., 9, 11–23.
Klein, A. A. J., Armstrong, B. L., Greer, M. K., & Stephens, D. (2000). A history of tinnitus. In
Brown, III, F. R. (1990). Hyperacusis and otitis R. Tyler (Ed.), Tinnitus handbook (pp. 437–448).
media in individuals with Williams syndrome. Clifton Park, NJ: Thompson Learning.
Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55, Turnbull, L. (1881). Imperfect hearing and the
339–344. hygiene of the ear. Philadelphia, PA: JB Lippin-
Kumar, S., Tansley-Hancock, O., Sedley, W., . . . cott & Co.
Griffiths, T. D. (2016). The brain basis for miso­ Tyler, R. S., Pienkowski, M., Roncancio, E. R., . . . ,
phonia. Current Biology, 27, 527–533. Moore, B. C. J. (2014). A review of hyper­
Mathiesen, H. (1969). Phonophobia after stape­ acusis and future directions: Part I. Definitions
dectomy. Acta Oto-Laryngologica (Stocklhom), 68, and manifestations. Am. J. Aud., (23), 402–419.
73–77. Vernon, J. A. (1987). Pathophysiology of tinnitus:
Malcore, D. (2013). The hyperacusis network. ENT A special case—hyperacusis and a proposed
and Audiology News, 21(6), 72–73. Available treatment. Am. J. Otol., 8, 201–202.
2
Audiological Assessment of
Decreased Sound Tolerance
Glynnis A. Tidball and Marc Fagelson

an audiological setting, as well as the merits and


Introduction
limitations of current assessment techniques
and tools, provide the focus of this chapter.
Decreased sound tolerance (DST), here de- The discussion of assessment here is meant to
fined as an abnormal physical and emotional apply to both adult and pediatric populations;
response to routinely experienced everyday issues specific to pediatric assessment will be
sounds, is an auditory complaint arising from considered later in the chapter.
damaging sound exposure, physical disease or
dysfunction, mental health conditions or in-
jury, or as a side effect of some medications. Rationale for Assessment of
DST has also been reported in individuals with Decreased Sound Tolerance
certain genetic syndromes such as William’s
syndrome (Katzenell & Segal, 2001).
Sounds can be deemed intolerable by Given the multidimensional challenges faced
patients because they are perceived as overly by patients with DST, a successful interven­
loud, annoying, fear-inducing, or painful (Ty- tion approach likely must consider input from
ler et al., 2014). General usage of the term in an inter-professional team of providers. Pa-
medical, psychological, and audiological texts tients with DST are likely to access care for
suggests that DST is a subjective complaint their condition through an audiologist and/
without an objective or easily measured cor- or otolaryngologist. The lack of specific mea-
relate. Behaviors reported or observed in those sures precludes definitive statements regarding
with DST include sound avoidance, over-use the prevalence of DST; however, the reported
of hearing protection, exaggerated startle re- overlap of tinnitus, hearing loss and DST (see
sponse to sound, and increased awareness of Chapter 3, Epidemiology) suggests that audi-
ambient sounds. The means by which DST ologists should be equipped to diagnose, man-
can be identified, measured, and managed in age, and direct patients to appropriate care in

15
16 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

order to establish and act upon the causes and symptomatic of significant medical pathology,
effects of DST and thus optimize opportuni- including both otological and non-otological
ties for intervention. causes (see Chapters 7 and 9). Otological con-
Any patient being assessed for hearing ditions that include DST as a possible accom-
function has the potential to present with panying symptom include acoustic shock dis-
DST. Routine audiological tests may involve order and superior canal dehiscence (SCD).
high-level sound stimulation that individuals SCD frequently alters sound tolerance due to
with DST find uncomfortable or painful (e.g., the enhanced transmission of sounds via bone
vestibular-evoked myogenic potential, auditory conduction, labelled “conductive DST” (Ba-
brainstem responses, acoustic reflex thresh- nerjee, 2005) or “bone hyperacusis” (Ward
olds). Patients with DST may report that these et al., 2017). Patients with SCD may experi-
and other routine audiological tests exacer- ence their own voice, heel strikes as they walk
bate tinnitus or contribute to and heighten or run, and even eye movements as unusu-
sound-provoked anxiety. Indeed, Aazh and ally loud, and may report that loud sounds or
Moore (2017b) found that 21% of patients pressure changes evoke a spinning sensation
at a clinic specializing in management of tinni- or nausea (the so-called Tullio phenomenon)
tus and hyperacusis experienced stimulus levels as well as the perception that still objects are
exceeding loudness discomfort levels (LDLs) moving. An example of SCD appears as the
when completing the basic audiologic test bat- second case in this chapter.
tery performed according to the British Soci- The diagnostic significance of DST un-
ety of Audiology recommended procedures. derscores the need to ask patients not only if
Screening or interviewing the patient for DST they are intolerant of certain sounds, but also
prior to testing is advisable in the interest of to describe the quality or nature of sounds
preserving patient comfort and clinical rap- that are not well tolerated, the effect that the
port, and to avoid possible iatrogenic effects sounds elicit, and the circumstances under
of testing such as provoking reactive tinnitus. which DST developed. Patients may not vol-
If DST is suspected, the test battery should be unteer this information during the case his-
modified accordingly. If a test using high- tory unless specifically asked about sound tol-
intensity stimuli is attempted, the rationale for erance as they may believe the symptoms are
performing the test should be explained to unusual or incidental. Therefore, it is essential
the patient beforehand, giving the patient the for audiologists to interpret case history infor­
choice of opting out of or terminating the test. mation thoroughly and to consider the pos-
Indeed, test battery adjustments, as suggested sibility that DST influences the patient’s au-
by Aazh and Moore (2017b), should be con- ditory behaviors by provoking, for example,
sidered for these patients following a rationale avoidance strategies, modifications of lifestyle,
similar to that provided when performing pe- or over-use of hearing protection.
diatric audiological assessment under normal From the perspective of hearing loss man-
conditions; the audiologist’s goal is to com- agement, identification and measurement of
plete the test battery, but not at the expense DST is essential for patient satisfaction, accep­
of the patient’s well-being, psychological state, tance of new devices, and optimal speech per-
and with care not to render the patient fearful formance when hearing aids are fit. Indeed,
of the audiometric test suite. several studies have indicated that loudness
Identification of DST is informative from in tolerance contributes to dissatisfaction with
a diagnostic perspective as its presence may be amplification for many users. Kochkin (2000)
2. Audiological Assessment of Decreased Sound Tolerance 17

found that 25.3% of survey respondents cited support optimal speech intelligibility. Because
loudness of amplified sound as a reason they hearing aid output can be easily measured,
did not use their hearing aids. Specifically, re- there is no excuse for professionals fitting
spondents reported that amplification made hearing aids to abrogate their responsibility to
noise painful, annoying, distracting, or unac­ consider patient reports and tolerance limits.
ceptable. In a subsequent survey of hearing Further, hearing aid returns are costly to the
aid users, Kochkin (2002) found that 58% of dispenser and manufacturer and worse, there is
survey respondents indicated that they would a cost to the patient who has a poor first expe-
like to see new hearing aid technology make rience with hearing aids and concludes that, “I
“loud sounds less painful.” A scoping review by tried hearing aids and they didn’t work.” Such
McCormack and Fortnum published in 2013 patients may then be reluctant to pursue in-
suggests that noise and comfort is still an is­­ struments in the future.
sue with regard to patients’ rejection of hear- A fitting protocol that includes manage-
ing aids. Humes et al. (2003) found that ment of DST could improve user satisfaction
successful hearing aid users judged low-level and ultimately allow patients to use amplifica-
sounds as less loud and had higher loudness dis- tion at settings optimal for speech intelligibil-
comfort levels (LDLs) overall when compared ity rather than wearing hearing aids set with
to pa­tients who used their hearing aids less lower outputs, as are often preferred by pa-
frequently. tients at initial fittings. A treatment approach
While none of these surveys identified described by Formby et al. (2015) employed
individuals with DST, they suggested that dis- wideband sound therapy, delivered via ear-
satisfaction with loudness of amplified sound level sound generators, and counseling that
contributed to hearing aid returns. The rate of prioritized increasing loudness tolerance and
returns for hearing aids in the United States expanding dynamic range in hearing-impaired
in 2014 was 19.4%. Approximately 60% of individuals. As the use of wideband sound fos-
all hearing aids returned to the manufacturer tered desensitization to offending sounds in
were returned with “first-fit” settings (Koch- patients, the authors postulated that the added
kin, 2000), which suggested that in most cases sound decreased central auditory gain, consid-
the output of the returned hearing aids was set ered a putative mechanism by which the au-
according to predicted loudness tolerance, or ditory pathway would compensate for loss of
that the patient was not consulted regarding sensitivity. The authors reported that subjects
sounds that caused pain or discomfort. preferred a higher presentation level of speech
Individuals with DST who find that out- (most comfortable level, or MCL) after using
put of amplification uncomfortably or pain- the devices for several weeks. Subjects toler-
fully exceeds loudness tolerance may be reluc- ated target gain values and related audibility
tant to wear their instruments, or insist that of speech sounds more consistently and with
the gain be reduced to accommodate DST. fewer complaints of uncomfortable loudness
When tinnitus and DST are present, patients at the end of the study period.
may also report that amplification exacerbates Investigations of DST prevalence indi-
tinnitus. It is reasonable to expect that DST cate that DST frequently co-occurs with tin-
will be an impediment to acceptance of ampli- nitus, and affects an estimated 40% of tinnitus
fication, or that tolerance problems may com- patients (Baguley & Andersson, 2008). Indi-
pel users to set their hearing instruments below viduals presenting with both DST and tinni­
target gain and therefore at levels inadequate to tus frequently report that tinnitus reacts (i.e.,
18 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

worsens) following intense sound exposures. patient’s efforts to obtain relief from tinnitus
Schecklmann et al. (2014) indicate that 86% through acceptance of masking sounds.
of individuals with intrusive tinnitus and DST Perhaps the most compelling reason to
(defined in the study as “sounds cause pain identify DST in an audiology setting is the
or physical discomfort”) report increased tin- significant and detrimental impact on quality
nitus loudness following exposure to noise. of life for patients and those around them (see
Given that amplification and sound therapy Chapter 10). Individuals with DST are likely
are rou­tinely used in tinnitus management to avoid situations in which intolerable sounds
(e.g., Henry et al., 2008; Jastreboff & Jastre- are anticipated; their worlds can become func-
boff, 2000; Searchfield, 2015), tinnitus inter- tionally and metaphorically very small. Pa-
vention should include assessment of DST to tients’ ability to function at work and at home
ensure that neither amplification nor sound may be compromised, and they may feel iso-
used for sound therapy exacerbate tinnitus. Jas- lated, misunderstood, and unsupported. Iden-
treboff and Jastreboff (2000) assert that DST tifying and acknowledging the presence of
should be managed prior to tinnitus inter- DST and its impact is an important first step
vention. Patients will be more likely to accept toward patient management and helping the
masking noise, amplification, and other sound audiologist gain the patient’s trust.
therapies used in specific tinnitus interventions
when loudness tolerance or specific aversions
to sounds have been addressed.
The rationale for sound therapy in tinni- Assessment Components
tus management is that sound, if acceptable to
the patient, will ultimately reduce tinnitus dis-
tress. When patients experience reduced sound Case History
tolerance, however, their willingness to use
sound therapy may be limited due to an aver- Individuals may present to their care provider
sive response to the very sounds whose intent is with DST as their primary complaint and
therapeutic. In cases of tinnitus accompanying concern. Others may not volunteer that they
hearing loss, the potential value of hearing aids have difficulty tolerating sound, especially when
or combination instruments may be reduced the focus of assessment is hearing loss or tin-
when patients discover the devices produce un- nitus. Patients may feel that DST is not rele-
comfortable sound levels that provoke avoid- vant to their assessment, or believe that their
ance rather than participation. Sound therapy complaints are unusual or untreatable. Given
can utilize sounds that mix with or mask tin- the prevalence of DST in the general popu-
nitus, or it can prioritize the use of environ- lation and especially in clinical populations
mental sounds, or other information-bearing in which tinnitus is present (see Chapter 3),
signals, to interfere with tinnitus perception. including a general question in the standard
However, those patients affected by DST will case history is recommended.
have, by definition, a limited palate of sounds Asking the patient, “Are you bothered by
from which to choose, and within the limited sound in any way?” is a simple question that al­
set of acceptable sounds, tolerable sound lev- lows the patient to acknowledge whether sound
els may restrict further the options available tolerance and/or routinely ex­perienced sounds
to patient and clinician. A careful process of or environments present consistent prob­lems.
desensitization, perhaps similar to that offered It is useful to remember that sound toler­ance
by Rob Littwin in Chapter 15 can support the will vary depending on sound characteristics
2. Audiological Assessment of Decreased Sound Tolerance 19

(intensity, frequency, rise time, and context), Hypersensitivity to: Yes answers (%)
the patient’s psychological health status, in- Low frequency sounds
cluding levels of stress and arousal, and the
patient’s beliefs about sound. Some questions • Drilling machine 83
for clinicians to consider may be drawn from • Traffic noise 72
examples below. • Dog barking 62

High frequency sounds


Description of Problem
• Rattling of dishes 88
How and when did this start? • Child crying 84
• Paper rustling 71
DST may develop secondary to an acoustic • Applause 47
event such as exposure to music, occupational • Dentist’s drill 44
noise, unanticipated sound proximal to the • Birdsong 24
ear; health event (such as an otologic or neu-
rologic condition or event, physical injury, Broad band noise
prescribed and/or illicit drug use); or psycho-
logical event, perhaps after trauma or a period • TV 49
of high stress or anxiety (Anari et al., 1999). • Speech 44
The conditions under which DST began pro­
vide insights into possible mechanisms or con­ Sudden sounds
tributing factors to DST. When tinnitus is
present, DST may arise coincidental to tinni­ • Hammering 87
tus, or before or after tinnitus onset. Sudden- • Door slamming 72
onset DST, similar to sudden-onset tinnitus,
likely produces a more severe disruption to Some individuals will have lowered toler-
daily life and greater levels of psychological ance of sound because sounds provoke a neg­
stress for the patient than those same condi- ative emotional response such as annoyance,
tions emerging in a gradual manner (Fagelson, rage, or fear. Often it is the context or asso-
2007; Kreuzer et al., 2014). ciation of the sound rather than its acoustic
characteristics that drives the response. The
Describe sounds that bother you. term “misophonia” as defined by Jastreboff
and Jastreboff (2002) is intended to describe
Patients may easily recall specific sounds or an intense dislike of sounds, and approximates
sound qualities that they find bothersome, or the definition of annoyance hyperacusis (Tyler
they may struggle to identify any examples of et al., 2014). “Selective sound sensitivity syn-
problematic sounds. Providing patients with drome” is used to describe an aversive reaction
examples of sounds that are commonly identi- to a specific subset of “trigger” sounds (Bern-
fied as problematic can help them to describe stein et al., 2013). Misophonia also appears in
their experience. Anari et al. (1999) surveyed the psychiatric literature to describe this subset
100 patients with DST to determine sounds of DST reports (Schröder et al., 2013). Exam-
most commonly identified as problematic from ples of bothersome or trigger sounds include
a closed list of 13 sounds. The list below sum- oral or eating sounds (chewing, swallowing,
marizes the authors’ findings. lip smacking, etc.), breathing sounds, and
20 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

repetitive sounds (e.g., clicking of keyboard to sound levels well below 120 dB SPL, the
keys or a pen, leg jostling) (Schröder et al., pain threshold for individuals with normal
2013). The same sounds may not be reported loudness tolerance (Hood & Poole, 1966).
as problematic when made by the patients Both peripheral and central mechanisms for
themselves (Edelstein et al., 2013). Identifica- a pain response to sound have been proposed.
tion of misophonia during assessment is crit- Liu et al. (2015) demonstrated that Type II af-
ical in order to establish appropriate counsel- ferent neural fibers terminating on the outer
ling and treatment options. hair cells were selectively activated when hair
cell damage occurred in the cochlea. These
What happens when you hear these sounds? Type II fibers activated neurons in the cochlear
nucleus when damaging sounds were subse-
Tyler et al. (2014) proposed using hyperacusis quently presented to the cochlea, and Flores
as an umbrella term for DST, and using addi- et al. (2015) proposed activation of these
tional subcategories of annoyance (described Type II nerve fibers might serve as a mechanism
above), loudness, fear, and pain DST to cat- for pain hyperacusis (see Chapters 5 and 8).
egorize the impact of sound on the listener. Patients with pain hyperacusis frequently
These subcategories could provide structure to describe a sharp or “jabbing” pain, as well as
the case history interview, although we must a variety of other symptoms associated with
acknowledge that the subcategories overlap offending sounds. Additional symptoms in-
with other terms and definitions as outlined clude dull ear ache, burning or numbness in or
in Chapter 3. Patients may experience over- around the ear or face, pain that radiates down
whelming feelings of aversion or may expe- the neck, tinnitus, transient aural fullness, “muf­
rience a variety of strong emotions related to fled” or distorted hearing, slight nausea or diz-
sound exposures. Their ability to put such feel- ziness/vertigo, headache, popping, clicking, or
ings into words, or being provided a lexicon a fluttering sensation in the ear (Westcott et al.,
that supports understanding of such feelings, 2013). Klockoff (1978) first described this
can be beneficial. In a manner similar to that cluster of symptoms as “tonic tensor tympani
employed during psychological counseling, the syndrome” (TTTS). He suggested an anxiety-
patient can be educated regarding their re- based condition developed involuntarily from
sponses to sound, and most effectively when the a lowered threshold of activation of the tensor
patient uses prior knowledge or logic to shape tympani muscle in the middle ear, resulting in
their approach to managing challenging prob- spasm or rhythmic contraction of the muscle.
lems (Bandura, 1986). For example, a patient These symptoms could arise from and worsen
who avoids going to a coffeehouse because the with sound exposure, particularly sound pres-
music is experienced as annoyingly loud can be sures that caused acoustic trauma or acoustic
counseled to observe the reactions of others in shock. A similar cluster of symptoms—otalgia,
the same environment. If other patrons did not tinnitus, vertigo, and subjective hearing loss—
appear to be disturbed, or if nobody else made was described in individuals with temporo-
their way, hands to their ears, to the door, then mandibular dysfunction (TMD), which may
it would be likely that the sound level was not be present when there is history of head and/
damaging, and although uncomfortable, could or neck injury or dysfunction (Kisnisci et al.,
contribute to a process of desensitization that 1999). These symptoms are challenging for
should be the patient’s long-term goal. both patient and clinician; it is also possible
“Pain hyperacusis” has been described as that any or all occur in the absence of any iden-
pain occurring in the ear or head in response tifiable underlying otologic pathology.
2. Audiological Assessment of Decreased Sound Tolerance 21

When a patient experiences these somatic are involved in a medicolegal or workplace


symptoms and associates them with sound ex- dispute in which DST is a factor, clinicians
posure, the symptoms are likely to contribute may be asked to provide an opinion on what
to the patient’s fear and avoidance of sound as level and type of sound exposure is appropri-
well as the belief that routine and moderate ate for the patient in the short and long term.
levels of sound are damaging. Inquiring about
somatic complaints associated with DST can How important is it to you to do something
provide direction for referrals, counseling, and about your difficulty with sound?
reassurance. When head/neck injury or dys-
function is reported, the presence of somatic Assessing patient motivation and readiness
complaints may suggest that referral to dental, to address DST, as any disorder, informs the
physiotherapy, or other relevant health care clinician about how to proceed with recom-
provider is indicated; unfortunately, clinical mendations and provides an opportunity to
guidelines on the treatment pathway for pain open discussion. Patients may feel that their
DST do not yet exist. adaptations to DST are reasonable, perhaps
Patients reporting sound- and pressure- due to fear of exposure or satisfaction with
induced spinning sensations upon hearing loud their choices. As discussed by Veronica Ken-
sounds should be queried for other commonly nedy (Chapter 14), a child affected by DST
reported symptoms of SCD such as increased influences the activity level and options of the
loudness of internally generated sounds includ­ family. The presence of DST in adults can also
ing their own voice (autophony) and pulsatile influence family life when childcare, family ac-
tinnitus. tivities and household chores are experienced
as difficult to tolerate. Counseling and identi­
How does this problem influence your daily life fication of strategies, therefore, may need to
at home? At work? address the concerns of a family, not just the
individual. On a positive note, the involvement
The impact of DST will depend on patients’ of family members will illustrate for all stake-
circumstances at home and at work. Individ- holders the challenges experienced by the pa-
uals may find that activities of daily living reg- tient. The load carried by the patient may then
ularly expose them to sounds that they find be shared in a more reasonable manner by the
uncomfortably loud, painful, fear-inducing, people supporting the patient rather than by
and that worsen DST and tinnitus, when the patient alone, particularly as the condition
present. In some cases, patients may feel the could make the patient feel isolated from po-
need to remove themselves temporarily or tential sources of assistance.
permanently from their work environment
and to modify their home and social environ- When and how do you use hearing protection?
ments to limit exposure. The audiologist or
other health care professional needs to assess Audiologists often find indications that an in­­
the appropriateness of patients’ adaptations to dividual with DST misuses hearing protec-
sound exposure—for example, are patients’ tion; indeed, Formby et al. (2003) demon-
avoidance behaviors reasonable or overly pro- strated that overuse of hearing protection may
tective? Littwin (Chapter 15) provides several induce DST. Beyond DST effects, overuse or
examples of such behaviors, as well as strate- unwarranted doubling up of hearing protec-
gies for managing patients’ perceived need to tion in an occupational or military setting can
avoid certain environments. When patients put the individual and those around them in
22 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

jeopardy by attenuating warning signals in- traumatic stress disorder. The patient’s mental
cluding verbal alerts from co-workers. Asking health status, particularly in cases associated with
patients about their use of hearing protection hyperarousal such as PTSD, may enhance aware-
provides an opportunity to discuss safe levels ness and increase startle responses to external
of sound and appropriate hearing protection, sounds in a manner that contributes to DST
as well as alternatives to conventional hearing (Fagelson, 2007). The relation between DST
protection such as electronic hearing protection and psychological state is bidirectional; not only
devices that permit warning sounds and speech can the psychological state amplify sound tol-
to reach the ears while attenuating hazardous erance problems, DST can also negatively in-
noise. The key to managing the use of hearing fluence mental health by contributing to stress
protection may rely upon the clinician con- arousal, hypervigilance, feelings of persecution,
firming that most environments specified by and isolation through sound avoidance. DST,
the patient as troublesome likely do not contain like tinnitus, thus differs from other auditory
damaging levels of noise unless, as indicated disorders such as loss of sensitivity, which can
previously, others in the vicinity are required reasonably be managed and rehabilitated using
to shout to people standing within one meter hearing aids or other assistive devices with ap-
to be understood, or are holding their ears and propriate orientation. The amount and depth
fleeing the environment. Patients may feel reas- of counseling differs greatly in cases of tinnitus
sured with this knowledge but may still prefer to and DST. While hearing loss may contribute to
keep hearing protection at hand or have an exit feelings of isolation and depression (Arlinger,
strategy in case noise levels become intolerable. 2003), pure tone sensitivity does not worsen
with mental health disorders. This mutually re­­
Do you have a history of anxiety, depression, or inforcing dynamic between DST and mental
other mental health concerns? Do you feel that health conditions underscores the value of dis-
you may be anxious or depressed now? cussion of mental health in DST assessment,
and management of DST should benefit from
Several investigators have demonstrated a po­s­ identification and treatment of co-morbid men-
itive correlation between mental health disor­ tal health disorders such as anxiety.
ders and sound tolerance. Jüris et al. (2013) Unfortunately, hearing health care pro-
found that 56% of individuals with DST also fessionals may feel unqualified or uncomfort­
had at least one psychiatric disorder, most fre­ able broaching the topic of mental health
quently an anxiety disorder. DST was also as­ with patients. Our experience is that an open
sociated with higher risk of co-morbid depres­ and neutral approach to discussing mental
sive symptoms. health in the context of DST is often wel-
With DST, as with tinnitus, mental health comed by patients and can motivate them to
has the potential to contribute to the magni- consider options such as behavioral therapies
tude of the symptoms. Patients commonly re- or sound therapy in their treatment plan.
port that DST worsens with stress and anxiety Screening tools for psychological disorders,
levels. Sahley and Nodar (2001) propose that such as the Hospital Anxiety and Depression
emotional and/or physical stress can exacerbate Scale (HADS; Zigmond & Snaith, 1983), Pa-
DST through the excitatory effects of stress on tient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-P; Mar-
the auditory system. Mood can also alter at- tin et al., 2006), and the Generalized Anxiety
tentional processes, and such changes are ob- Disorder-7 (GAD-7; Spitzer et al., 2006), can
served in conditions such as anxiety or high indicate when referral to the family physician
stress, obsessive compulsive disorders, and post- or mental health care provider is warranted.
2. Audiological Assessment of Decreased Sound Tolerance 23

exposure or ageing, is believed to contribute to


Pure Tone Audiometry “hidden hearing loss” cases in which sensitivity
to low intensity sounds is preserved but coding
Pure tone audiometry is essential in determin- of high-intensity signals in continuous noise
ing the severity, nature, and possible etiology is impaired (Liberman et al., 2016). Cochlear
of disordered hearing in patients with DST. synaptopathy has been proposed as a generator
The relation between hearing loss and DST for DST as changes in central auditory gain
as indicated by pure tone audiometry is, how- may occur secondary to the peripheral damage
ever, unclear (Tyler et al., 2014). Research in­ (Knipper et al., 2013). Future developments in
dicates that pure tone threshold loss at con- the assessment of cochlear synaptopathy may
ventional test frequencies is not required for provide valuable insights into mechansims of
development of DST. Sheldrake et al. (2015) DST and assist DST assessment and treatment.
report that a third of patients with a primary Pure tone audiometry for patients report-
complaint of DST present with clinically nor- ing DST merits a few modifications to con-
mal hearing thresholds (<25 dB HL for test ventional diagnostic testing. Extending the fre­
frequencies up to 8 kHz), while the remain- quency range of pure tone testing to include
der of patients demonstrate pure tone thresh- thresholds at 10 to 16 kHz may help to identify
olds in the range of mild to severe hearing “hidden hearing loss” (Liberman et al., 2016).
loss. Brandy and Lynn’s (1995) comparison When patient DST symptoms are consistent
of pure tone thresholds of 25 individuals with with SCD, inclusion of 250 Hz air and bone
DST to those of 13 controls without DST conduction threshold measures with testing of
demonstrate no significant difference in hear- bone conduction below 0 dB HL is necessary
ing thresholds between groups. Andersson to identify possible changes in transmission of
et al.’s (2002) survey of individuals about their sound via bone conduction secondary to third
DST and hearing impairment via an Internet window pathology (Ward et al., 2017). Finally,
and postal questionnaire asserts that the ma- pure tone thresholds can be measured by us-
jority of individuals reporting DST do not ing an ascending technique rather than a de-
identify having a hearing impairment; how- scending method, thereby minimizing patient
ever, those reporting DST are more likely to discomfort.
also report hearing impairment than respon-
dents without DST complaints.
Conventional pure tone audiometry Behavioral Measures
does not accurately predict auditory function of Loudness
in everyday situations nor does it reflect the
integrity of the auditory system as a whole. A patient’s loudness tolerance is derived by as-
Conventional diagnostic audiometry mea- sessing the upper limit of the auditory system’s
sures threshold of audibility at 250 to 8000 Hz dynamic range. Measurement of behavioral
(ASHA, 2005) but does not capture pure tone threshold of sensitivity is a well-agreed upon
threshold loss at the upper frequency range metric and one that has remained unques-
of human audibility. Furthermore, pure tone tioned and unchanged for decades. Threshold
thresholds may fall into the range of normal of sensitivity defines the lower limit of the pa-
hearing despite changes in auditory function tient’s dynamic range, but as has been pointed
for suprathreshold stimuli. Cochlear synap- out in a variety of investigations, audiomet-
topathy, the loss of the synapses between inner ric thresholds do not necessarily correlate
hair cells and cochlear neurons due to noise with the integrity of an individual’s hearing
24 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

pathway (Kujawa & Liberman, 2009; Myers when managing tinnitus (Jastreboff & Jastre-
et al., 2009). Paradoxically, the measure of boff, 2000).
loudness tolerance is not standardized, and its One of the more widely recognized tech-
interpretation may be complicated by non- niques for establishing LDLs remains Cox
auditory factors such as patient psychologi- et al.’s (1997) contour test of loudness percep-
cal health. This said, loudness measurements tion. The contour test utilizes patient ratings of
may provide results that illuminate in more loudness similar to those employed previously
useful ways a patient’s auditory status. For ex- by Hawkins (1980). The test employs sound
ample, during the fitting of hearing aids in presentations across a wide range of stimulus
which loudness tolerance must be considered, intensities and spectra and establishes loud-
a patient’s reduced dynamic range is more sub­ ness growth characteristics covering a patient’s
stantially limited by DST than by elevated ab­ dynamic range. Formby et al. (2017) recently
solute thresholds alone. proposed streamlining this test as they deter-
The measure of loudness tolerance ap- mined signal levels corresponding to patients’
pears on the surface a straightforward prop- ratings of “soft,” “comfortable,” and “loud, but
osition. A patient can be provided a list of OK” were most relevant to rehabilitation ef-
descriptors, such as “soft,” “loud but OK,” forts. The authors stress that such patient rat-
or “uncomfortably loud” to describe a signal ings could substantially improve hearing aid
controlled by a clinician, researcher, or the fittings as uncomfortable loudness is a frequent
patient. Alternatively, patients can employ a patient complaint, particularly for first-time
numerical scale, or some other form of cat- users. Such conclusions reinforce the notion
egorization scheme that allows them to map that understanding a patient’s loudness toler-
their perceptual experiences to stimuli of var- ance is at least as relevant to clinical practice as
ious magnitudes and spectral composition. In the measure of absolute threshold.
either case, it is clear from dozens of studies In addition to loudness measures and rat-
that the ratings upon which a patient settles ings, the reaction time of humans and animals
will be affected by, among other things, in- to sounds of various levels illustrates the rela-
structions (Hawkins, 1980), number of times tion between signal strength and the response
the test was conducted, and/or training that it evokes. Recently, Radziwon et al. (2017) re­
was provided (Silverman, 1947; Sherlock & ported that salicylate-induced hyperacusis de­
Formby, 2005), as well as concurrent drug creased relative to controls in the response times
use (Globus et al., 1978), presence of tinni­ in rats. Their work presented various doses of
tus, and history of traumatic brain injury (Fa­­ salicylate and assessed reaction times to tones
gelson, 2007; Myers et al., 2009). Despite of a wide range of frequencies. The authors
thorough reviews of techniques and findings reported that the influence of high doses of
since Silverman’s work was published, the la- salicylate, 150 mg/kg or higher, shortened re-
bile nature of loudness measures continues to action times of the animals across all stimulus
stymie the audiologist who seeks patient re- frequencies tested. The authors concluded that
sponses that are as easy to obtain, and as con- the drug-induced hyperacusis related to central
sistent over time, as the measures of threshold mechanisms whose excitability was influenced
of sensitivity. Despite their imperfections, by the drug across a wide range of stimuli.
loudness tolerance measures remain impor­ Behavioral or psychoacoustic measures of
tant because loudness perception contributes loudness tolerance for pure tones and speech
to success of hearing aid fittings (Formby are commonly used by audiologists when fit-
et al., 2017) and is an important consideration ting hearing aids. The threshold of pain for
2. Audiological Assessment of Decreased Sound Tolerance 25

most people is typically reached when sound patient considers the test suite an unsafe envi-
intensity reaches 120 dB SPL (Hood & Poole, ronment, thereby complicating future contact.
1966), whereas permanent hearing loss can Alternatively, patients may feel that measure-
arise following a single exposure to 140 dB SPL ment of LDLs validates their complaints. Given
(Chandler, 2006). In a clinical setting, typical the potential for patient distress, the decision
loudness tolerance is often found between 90 to perform loudness measures in assessment of
and 105 dB HL for pure tones presented via DST should be a joint decision between the cli-
earphones. nician and the patient.
LDLs are used in some tinnitus and
hyperacusis centers (Aazh et al., 2016) as a
method of identifying and quantifying DST. Immittance
Unfortunately, the evidence for use of LDL
in assessment of DST is lacking. Zaugg et al.
Acoustic reflex threshold testing can provide
(2016) found only weak correlations between
valuable diagnostic information during audio-
patients’ subjective report of DST and LDL
logical assessment (see Musiek & Rintelmann,
for pure tones and speech in a Veteran popu­
1999 for full discussion of ART in diagnostic
lation with co-morbid tinnitus. Meeus et al.
audiology). In cases when loss of the stapedial
(2010) found no correlation between audiolog­
reflex is suspected such as with Bell’s palsy, or
ical measurements and DST complaints in a
when there is normal tympanometry in an ear
cohort of ENT clinic patients using question-
with a conductive hearing loss such as with
naire scores. Jüris et al. (2013) did not find a
superior canal dehiscence, ARTs may provide
consistent correlation between LDL and other
insight into an underlying audiological mech-
DST measures although there was a correlation
anism of DST. For identifying or measuring
between the Hospital Anxiety and Depression
DST, however, the rationale for inclusion of
Scale anxiety scale scores and LDLs. The util-
ART in assessment is limited. Changes in ART
ity of LDL measurements is further limited by
secondary to DST due to mechanisms other
test-retest variability across and within indi-
than dysfunction of the stapedial reflex have
viduals (Stephens et al., 1977). It may be that
not been demonstrated as significantly altered
speech or pure tones of varying frequencies
in patients with DST (Anari et al., 1999). In-
presented in a test booth do not capture the
clusion of the ART test specifically for assess-
complexity of sounds in the real world. At the
ment of DST is not warranted and should be
same time, “real world” sound may be poorly
carefully considered and discussed with pa-
tolerated because of characteristics other than
tients during routine audiological testing.
intensity and frequency such as sound dura-
tion, rise time, or because of acoustical and sit-
uational contexts or memories associated with
such sounds. Otoacoustic Emissions
It is important to consider the patient per-
spective when performing LDL measurements. The utility of otoacoustic emissions in DST as-
Individuals with DST are likely to find that sessment has been previously proposed ( Jastre-
entering a sound booth and submitting to lis- boff & Jastreboff, 2000). Bartnik et al. (2008)
tening to sounds that reach or approximate the compared input-output function of distortion
point of discomfort is an extremely unpleasant product otoacoustic emissions (DPOAEs) of
or fearful experience. This experience can un- normal-hearing controls to those of subjects
dermine clinical rapport and ensure that the with tinnitus only, hyperacusis only, and both
26 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

tinnitus and hyperacusis. Input-output func- Questionnaire and LDL measurements (Aazh
tions only partially distinguished hearing dis- & Moore, 2017; Meeus et al., 2010; Aazh
ordered subjects from controls, and did not & Moore, in press). Sherlock and Formby
distinguish between the three hearing disor- (2017) developed the Sound Tolerance Inter-
dered groups. view and Questionnaire Instrument (STIQI)
A measure that more effectively distin- to qualify and quantify sound tolerance in in-
guished subject groups was reported by Attias dividuals considering hearing aids. Although
et al. (2005) following assessment of transient the STIQI had not yet been validated at the
evoked otoacoustic emissions (TEOAE) in time of this writing, this assessment tool has
the presence of contralateral masking noise. the potential to measure the effects of treat-
The use of contralateral masking during OAE ment of DST and changes in sound tolerance
measurement targets auditory efferent activity that occur with hearing aid use and provides
that is known to have a suppressive effect on additional guidance for case history of DST.
cochlear and associated neural activity (Berlin The reader is referred to Chapter 5, Scales and
et al., 1993). The absence of such an effect Questionnaires for DST, for further discus-
could contribute to auditory complaints as­ sion of available instruments.
sociated with loudness intolerance. Attias et al.
compared patients with traumatic brain in-
jury (TBI) histories and loudness tolerance
DST in Children
issues to those with TBI but without loudness
complaints, as well as to a group of normal
controls. They reported 87% of the patients Coelho et al. (2007) reported that 43.5%
in the group with loudness intolerance lacked (222/506) of children aged 5 to 12 years an-
the typical suppression effect that was present swered “yes” to the question, “Are you both-
in the patients who did not report the audi- ered by loud sounds?,” although only a subset
tory problem. Amplitudes of TEOAEs across of children that answered “yes” (40 of 222) had
groups differed as well, which complicated in- LDLs below 90 dB HL in at least one test fre­­
terpretation of the findings; however, the au- quency in one ear. In a smaller sample of pe-
thors concluded that dysfunction of the effer- diatric patients aged 7 to 18 years, Aazh and
ent, and putatively suppressive effect was at least McFerran (personal com­ munication) found
partially responsible for the observed loudness the average LDL, mea­sured at 0.25, 0.5, 1, 2,
tolerance issues expressed by the participants. 4, and 8 kHz, was below 80 dB HL in at least
one ear for 90.5% (19/21) of patients tested.
Seventy-one percent of patients (17/24) had
Questionnaires LDLs of 60 dB HL or less for at least one of the
measured frequencies for at least one ear. LDLs
Questionnaires can aid in identification of of less than 60 dB HL were most frequently
DST, measurement of DST severity, and found at 8000 Hz. There was no correlation
provide a baseline measure for treatment ef- between LDLs and results from the Hyper­
ficacy, and are discussed in detail in Chap­ acusis Questionnaire (HQ; Khalfa et al., 2002);
ter 5. Of note, Khalfa et al. (2002) developed however, the authors reported a significant cor-
the Hyperacusis Questionnaire to identify relation between the HADS questionnaire and
individuals with DST. However, several stud- the HQ. This latter finding demonstrated the
ies have found lack of correlation between potential that co-morbid mental health issues
DST as identified through the Hyperacusis existed in children, as with adults, and that
2. Audiological Assessment of Decreased Sound Tolerance 27

psychological evaluation should be considered in the way of audiological testing to confirm


when hyperacusis has been identified. the presence of DST or its severity through
In practice, clinicians may opt out of objective measures. Measurement of DST us­
LDL testing to preserve patient comfort and ing electrophysiological tests such as auditory
clinical rapport, and because LDLs may be brainstem response (ABR), middle latency re-
of limited value in terms of management (see sponse (MLR; Formby et al., 2017), as well as
Chapter 14). As with adults, it is therefore im­ auditory steady-state response (ASSR; Ménard
portant to engage the pediatric patient and et al., 2008) has been proposed and may ul-
his or her family in order to identify the sounds timately augment current clinical tools used
they find bothersome, when and under what to assess DST. Aron et al. (2015) reported
circumstances DST began, what they experi- changes in intra-subject tympanometric mea-
ence when they hear offending sounds, and surements that reflected voluntary engagement
how DST may be limiting activities and influ- of the tensor tympani muscle and which could
encing family dynamics. Indeed, in a problem- prove useful in identification of tonic tensor
based approach to assessment, children are en- tympani syndrome. Establishing normative
couraged to recount their story and thoughts data in objective testing for subjects with DST
regarding DST, such as is described by Veron- should lead to more options for clinical assess-
ica Kennedy in Chapter 14. Younger children ment of DST in audiology.
may have an easier time describing their expe-
rience by drawing a picture to illustrate how
sounds affect them. Case Examples
Finally, it is essential for the clinician to
get a sense of whether there is any exaggera­
tion of DST complaints that may result in sec­ Case Example 1
ondary gain, such as absence from school, and
whether the family understands the validity KL was a 62-year-old male who presented to
of DST complaints and can constructively sup­ a tinnitus and hyperacusis clinic with suicidal
port their child. ideations, distressing tinnitus, and hearing
loss. He summarized his experience of hear-
ing loss and tinnitus as feeling “stuck in a bub-
Future Developments ble with air hissing into it through a hole and
in Audiological no way to escape.” KL reported extreme irrita-
bility that was affecting relationships at work
Assessment of DST
and at home, and said he was suffering finan-
cial distress as he was unable to work full-
There is no unique clinical presentation of time due to excessive fatigue and difficulty
DST and no test or test battery that can ade- with concentration. KL indicated that he tried
quately identify and quantify DST at present. hearing aids several years earlier but found
Audiological assessment of DST will continue that they aggravated his tinnitus; he returned
to develop as the components and mecha- the instruments to his dispenser at that time.
nisms of DST are better understood and as Upon further questioning, he said he was
a consensus on the ideal set of DST test in- aware that loud and especially high-frequency
struments and definitions emerges. Currently sounds contributed to his irritability but did
identification of DST relies heavily on patient not identify that he had lowered tolerance of
report and behavioral measures, with little sound per se. When asked about his mental
28 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

health, KL revealed a significant history of anx- with tinnitus and DST. Her accompanying re-
iety and depression that had worsened with ferral documents included an otolaryngology
antidepressant medication in the past. consult note, audiometry results, and a report
Pure tone audiometry revealed a mod- from a community hearing instrument clinic.
erate to severe high-frequency sensorineural The otolaryngologist noted tinnitus and dizzi-
hearing loss that was apparently long-standing ness secondary to a head injury. Audiological
and secondary to occupational noise exposure. assessment indicated essentially normal pure
LDLs were obtained bilaterally at 70 to 90 dB tone air- and bone-conduction thresholds for
HL. Acoustic reflex testing was not attempted all measured frequencies. Acoustic immittance
as the audiologist felt that the information results (tympanometry, acoustic reflex thresh-
would not add to the assessment and could ex- olds) were reported as normal.
acerbate the patient’s stated concerns. DM’s primary complaints in the group
KL’s worsening of tinnitus with amplifica- session were DST, “ringing” tinnitus centered
tion suggested the presence of DST, confirmed in her head, and dizziness since her head injury.
by further questioning of the patient and LDL She denied any difficulty hearing sound or pul-
measurements. KL was fit with hearing instru- satile quality to her tinnitus, although she noted
ments with amplification and built-in sound hearing her heart beat in the left ear. When asked
generator for tinnitus masking. Amplification about the effect sound had on her, she said loud
was set initially below target gain to accommo- sounds made her feel “off.” Further probing re-
date LDLs and eventually increased to optimal vealed that she perceived her own voice (auto-
settings for speech understanding. KL also re­ phony) and footsteps as louder in the left ear.
ceived informational counseling regarding DST, DM was brought in for a full diagnos-
tinnitus, and hearing loss. KL chose to try tic audiogram, including bone conduction
mindfulness-based stress reduction to address thresholds at 250 Hz and vestibular-evoked
emotional health but found quiet reading pro- myogenic potential (VEMP) testing. Pure tone
vided greater sense of relaxation once the tin­ thresholds indicated a 15-dB air-bone gap
nitus was manageable. for the left ear at 250 Hz with normal tym-
Seven months after his initial fitting, KL panometry and reflexes bilaterally. Vestibu-
reported that sounds were now “crisp and won- lar testing indicated a subthreshold cervical
derful” and that his “brain was finally able to VEMP response (65 dB nHL) on the left and
relax” when using his hearing instruments. He an asymmetrical ocular VEMP (50% asym-
was eventually able to resume full-time work metry ratio), with a higher amplitude response
and reported little tinnitus distress or reaction on the left. Video head-impulse test re­sults re-
to the sounds that had bothered him previously. vealed corrective saccades when the left ante-
While tinnitus and hearing loss were KL’s pri- rior canal was assessed. A 512-Hz tuning fork
mary complaints, his case illustrates the value of placed on the left ankle bone (lateral malleo­
inclusion of DST assessment with respect to the lus) was reported by the patient as audible in
rehabilitative aims regarding flexibility in hear- the left ear. DM was referred back to ENT
ing aid fitting and related patient expectations. and a high-resolution temporal bone CT scan
was ordered (Figures 2–1 and 2–2).
The radiology report confirmed a dehis-
Case Example 2 cence of the right superior canal. DM’s report
illustrates the value of a comprehensive case his-
Patient DM was referred to a hospital-based tory following complaints of DST, as such in-
tinnitus clinic for a group session for patients formation gathering can support identification
Figure 2–1. High resolution axial CT image showing left superior canal
dehiscence of the anterior limb (arrow).

Figure 2–2. Oblique CT scan showing dehiscence of the left superior ca-
nal of the anterior limb/apex.

29
30 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

of superior canal dehiscence, with correspond- Baguley, D., Andersson, G., McFerran, D., &
ing intervention considerations. McKenna, L. (2007). Hyperacusis. In Tin-
nitus: A multidisciplinary approach (2nd ed.)
(pp. 133–147). London, UK: Whirr.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought
References and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, Inc.
Bartnik, G., Hawley, M., Rogowski, M., Raj-
Aazh, H., & McFerran, D. (2017). Factors related Koziak, D., Fabijanska, A., & Formby, C. (2008).
to tinnitus and hyperacusis handicap in chil­dren Distortion product otoacoustic emission levels
and adolescents (personal communication). and input/output-growth functions in normal-
Aazh, H., & Moore, B. C. J. (2017a). Thoughts hearing individuals with tinnitus and/or hyper-
about suicide and self-harm in patients with acusis. Otolaryngologia Polska (The Polish Oto-
tinnitus and hyperacusis. Journal of American laryngology), 63(2), 171–181.
Academy of Audiology, in press. Berlin, C. I., Hood, L. J., Wen, H., Szabo, P., Ce-
Aazh, H., & Moore, B. C. J. (2017b). Incidence cola, R. P., Rigby, P., & Jackson, D. F. (1993).
of discomfort during pure-tone audiometry and Contralteral suppression of nonlinear click-
measurement of uncomfortable loudness levels evoked otoacoustic emissions. Hearing Research,
among people seeking help for tinnitus and/or 71, 1–11.
hyperacusis. American Journal of Audiology, in Bernstein, R. E., Angell, K. L., & Dehle, C. M.
press. (2013). A brief course of cognitive behavioural
Aazh, H., & Moore, B. C. J. (2017c). Usefulness therapy for the treatment of misophonia: A
of self-report questionnaires for psychological case example. The Cognitive Behaviour Thera-
assessment of patients with tinnitus and hyper­ pist, 6, e10.
acusis and patients’ views of the questionnaires. British Tinnitus Association. Annual Tinnitus
International Journal of Audiolog  y, Mar 09, 1–10. Research Review 2016. Available from www
Aazh, H., Moore, B. C. J., Lammaing, K., & .tinnitus.org.uk/ATRR2016. (Accessed Octo-
Cropley, M. (2016). Tinnitus and hyperacusis ber 16, 2016.)
therapy in a UK National Health Service audi- Canlon, B., Theorell, T., & Hasson, D. (2013).
ology department: Patients’ evaluations of the Associations between stress and hearing prob-
effectiveness of treatments. International Jour- lems in humans. Hearing Research, 295, 9–15.
nal of Audiology, 55(9), 514–522. Ceranic, B. J., Prasher, D. K., Raglan, E., & Luxon,
Aazh, H., & Allott. R. (2016). Cognitive behav- L. M. (1998). Tinnitus after head injury: Evidence
ioral therapy in management of DST: a narra- from otoacoustic emissions. Journal of Neurolog  y,
tive review and clinical implementation. Audi- Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 65(4), 523–529.
tory and Vestibular Research, 25(2), 63–74. Chandler, D. (2006). Blast-related ear injury in
Anari, M., Axelsson, A., Eliasson, A., & Magnus­ current U.S. military operations. ASHA Leader,
son, L. (1999). Hypersensitivity to sound: ques­ 11, 9–29.
tionnaire data, audiometry and classification. Coelho, C. B., Sanchez, T. G., Tyler, R. S. (2007).
Scandinavian Audiology, 28(4), 219–230. Hyperacusis, sound annoyance, and loudness
Arlinger, S. (2003). Negative consequences of un- hypersensitivity in children. Progress in Brain
corrected hearing loss––a review. International Research, 166, 169–178.
Journal of Audiology, 42, 2 S17–2 S20. Edelstein, M., Brang, D., Rouw, R., & Rama­
Axelsson, A., & Hamernik, R. P. (1987). Acute chandran, V. S. (2013). Misophonia: Physiologi­
acoustic trauma. Acta Oto-laryngologica, 104(3–4), cal investigations and case descriptions. Child-
225–233. hood, 3, 27.
Baguley, D. M., & Andersson, G. (2007). DST: Fackrell, K., Fearnley, C., Hoare, D. J., & Sereda,
Mechanisms. In Diagnosis and therapies, San M. (2015). Hyperacusis questionnaire as a tool
Diego, CA: Plural. for measuring hypersensitivity to sound in a
2. Audiological Assessment of Decreased Sound Tolerance 31

tinnitus research population. BioMed Research therapy (TRT). Australian and New Zealand
International. Journal of Audiology, 24(2), 74.
Fagelson, M. A. (2007). The association between Jüris, L., Ekselius, L., Andersson, G., & Larsen,
tinnitus and posttraumatic stress disorder. Amer­ H. C. (2013). The Hyperacusis Questionnaire,
ican Journal of Audiology, (16),107–117. loudness discomfort levels, and the Hospital
Flores, E. N., Duggan, A., Madathany, T., Hogan, Anxiety and Depression Scale: A cross-sectional
A. K., Márquez, F. G., Kumar, G., . . . & García- study. Hearing, Balance and Communication,
Añoveros, J. (2015). A non-canonical pathway 11(2), 72–79.
from cochlea to brain signals tissue-damaging Katzenell, U., & Segal, S. (2001). Hyperacusis:
noise. Current Biology, 25(5), 606–612. Review and clinical guidelines. Otology & Neu-
Formby, C., Korczak, P., Sherlock, L. P., Hawley, rotology, 22(3), 321–327.
M. L., & Gold, S. (2017). Auditory brainstem Khalfa, S., Dubal, S., Veuillet, E., Perez-Diaz, F.,
and middle latency responses measured pre-and Jouvent, R., & Collet, L. (2002). Psychometric
posttreatment for hyperacusic hearing-impaired normalization of a hyperacusis questionnaire.
persons successfully treated to improve sound ORL; Journal for Oto-Rhino-Laryngology and Its
tolerance and to expand the dynamic range for Related Specialties, Nov–Dec, 64(6), 436–442.
loudness: Case evidence. Semin. Hear., 38, 71–93. Kisnisci, R., Onder, E., & Tuz, H. (1999). Prev-
Formby, C., Payne, M. S., Yang, X., Wu, D., & alence of otologic complaints in temporoman-
Parton, J. M. (2017). Repeated measurement dibular disorder patients. International Journal
of absolute and relative judgments of loudness: of Oral and Maxillofacial Surgery, 28:101–102.
Clinical relevance for prescriptive fitting of aided Klochoff, I. (1978). The tensor tympani syn-
target gains for soft, comfortable, and loud, but drome—a source of vertigo. Abstracts, Barany So­
OK sound levels. Semin. Hear., 38, 26–52. ciety Ordinary Meeting; Uppsala, Sweden, 31–32.
Globus, G. G., Cohen, H. B., Kramer, J. C., El- Kochkin, S. & MarkeTrak, V. (2000). “Why my
liott, H. W., & Sharp, R. (1978). Effect of mar- hearing aids are in the drawer”: The consumers’
ijuana induced ‘altered state of consciousness’ perspective. The Hearing Journal, 53(2), 34–36.
on auditory perception. Journal of Psychedelic Kochkin, S. (2002). Consumers rate improvements
Drugs, 10(1), 71–76. sought in hearing instruments. Hearing Review,
Hawkins, D. B. (1980). Loudness discomfort lev- 9(11), 18–22.
els: A clinical procedure for hearing aid evalua- Kreuzer, P. M., Landgrebe, M., Schecklmann, M.,
tions. J. Speech Hear. Disord., 45(1), 3–15. Staudinger, S., & Langguth, B. (2012). Trauma-
Henry, J. A., Zaugg, T. L., Myers, P. J., & Schech- associated tinnitus: Audiological, demographic
ter, M. A. (2008). Using therapeutic sound and clinical characteristics; The TRI Database
with progressive audiologic tinnitus manage- Study Group. PLoS One, 7(9), e45599. Retrieved
ment. Trends in Amplification, 12(3), 188–209. from www.plosone.org.
Hood, J. D., & Poole, J. D. (1966). Tolerable Liu, C., Glowatzki, E., & Fuchs, P. (2015). Unmy-
limit of loudness: Its clinical and physiological elinated type II afferent neurons report cochlear
significance. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 40, 47–53. damage. Proceedings of the National Academy of
http://www.hearingreview.com/2015/01/hearing Sciences USA, 112(47), 14723–14727. doi: 10
-aid-sales-increase-4-8-2014-rics-continue-market .1073/pnas.1515228112.
-domination/. Martin, A., Rief, W., Klaiberg, A., & Braehler, E.
http://www.hearingreview.com/2015/10/best-prac (2006). Validity of the brief patient health ques-
tices-best-results/ tionnaire mood scale (PHQ-9) in the general
Jastreboff, P. J., & Jastreboff, M M. (2000). Tin- pop­ulation. General Hospital Psychiatry, 28(1),
nitus retraining therapy (TRT) as a method for 71–77.
treatment of tinnitus and hyperacusis patients. McCormack, A., & Fortnum, H. (2013). Why do
J. Am. Acad. Audiology, 11(3), 156–161. people fitted with hearing aids not wear them?
Jastreboff, M. M., & Jastreboff, P. J. (2002). De- International Journal of Audiology, 52(5), 360–
creased sound tolerance and tinnitus retraining 368.
32 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

McFerran, D. J., & Baguley, D. M. (2007). Acous- ison of procedures, and test-retest reliability. J.
tic shock. The Journal of Laryngology & Otology, Am. Acad. Audiol., 16(2), 85–100.
121(04), 301–305. Sherlock, L. P., & Formby, C. (2017). Consider-
Meeus, O. M., Spaepen, M., Ridder, D. D., & ations in the development of a sound tolerance
Heyning, P. H. V. D. (2010). Correlation be- interview and questionnaire instrument. Semi-
tween hyperacusis measurements in daily ENT nars in Hearing, 38(01), 53–70.
practice. International Journal of Audiology, Silverman, S. R. (1947). Tolerance for pure tones
49(1), 7–13. and speech in normal and defective hearing.
Ménard, M., GallÈgo, S., Berger-Vachon, C., Col- Ann. Otol. Rhinol. Laryngol., 56(3), 658–677.
let, L., & Thai-Van, H. (2008). Relationship Spitzer, R. L., Kroenke, K., Williams, J. B., &
between loudness growth function and audi- Löwe, B. (2006). A brief measure for assess-
tory steady-state response in normal-hearing ing generalized anxiety disorder: The GAD-7.
subjects. Hearing Research, 235(1), 105–113. Archives of Internal Medicine, 166(10), 1092–
Mueller, G. H., & Bentler, R. A. (2005). Fitting 1097.
hearing aids using clinical measures of loudness Stephens, S. D. G. (1970). Personality and the
discomfort levels: An evidence-based review of slope of loudness function. The Quarterly Jour-
effectiveness. Journal of the American Academy nal of Experimental Psychology, 22(1), 9–13.
of Audiology, 16(7), 461–472. Stephens, S. D. G., Blegvad, B., & Krogh, H. J.
Musiek, F. E., & Rintelmann, W. F. (1999). Con- (1977). The value of some suprathreshold au-
temporary perspectives in hearing assessment. ditory measures. Scandinavian Audiology, 6(4),
Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon. 213–221.
Radziwon K., Holfoth, D., Lindner, J., Kaier- Tyler, R. S., Pienkowski, M., Roncancio, E. R., Jun,
Green, Z., Bowler, R., Urban, M., Salvi, R. H. J., Brozoski, T., Dauman, N., & Martin, N.
(2017). Salicylate-induced hyperacusis in rats: (2014). A review of DST and future directions:
Dose- and frequency-dependent effects. Hear- Part I. Definitions and manifestations. American
ing Research, 350(1), 133–138. doi: 10.1016/j Journal of Audiolog  y, 23(4), 402–419.
.heares.2017.04.004. Epub 2017 Apr 27. Ward, B. K., Carey, J. P., & Minor, L. B. (2017).
Sahley, T. L., & Nodar, R. H. (2001). A biochem- Superior canal dehiscence syndrome: Lessons
ical model of peripheral tinnitus. Hearing Re- from the first 20 years. Frontiers in Neurology,
search, 152(1), 43–54. 8, 177.
Schecklmann, M., Landgrebe, M., Langguth, B., Westcott, M., Sanchez, T. G., Diges, I., Saba, C.,
& TRI Database Study Group. (2014). Pheno- Dineen, R., McNeill, C., Chiam, A., O’Keefe,
typic characteristics of DST in tinnitus. PLoS M., & Sharples, T. (2013). Tonic tensor tym-
One, 9(1), e86944. pani syndrome in tinnitus and DST patients: A
Searchfield, G. D. (2015). Hearing aids for tinni- multi-clinic prevalence study. Noise and Health,
tus. In Tinnitus: Clinical and research perspec- 15(63), 117.
tives. San Diego, CA: Plural. Zaugg, T. L., Thielman, E. J., Griest, S., & Henry,
Sheldrake, J., Diehl, P. U., & Schaette, R. (2015). J. A. (2016). Subjective reports of trouble toler­
Audiometric characteristics of hyperacusis pa- ating sound in daily life versus loudness discom-
tients. Frontiers in Neurology, 6, 105. fort levels. American Journal of Audiology, 1–5.
Sherlock, L. P., & Formby, C. (2005). Estimates Zigmond, A. S. & Snaith, R. P. (1983). The hospi-
of loudness, loudness discomfort, and auditory tal anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr.
dynamic range: Normative estimates, compar- Scand., 67(6), 361–370.
3
The Epidemiology and
Natural History of Disorders
of Loudness Perception
David M. Baguley

Introduction Recruitment

For any clinical condition, reliable data about As described in Chapter 2, loudness recruit­
the prevalence, incidence, and natural history ment is a cochlear phenomenon, associated
of the disorder is invaluable, both for car­ with outer hair cell (OHC) dysfunction (Smeds
ing for the individual patient, for a wide and & Leijon, 2011). The prevalence of recruit­
deep understanding of the condition, and for ment has not been formally determined, but it
public health planning and commissioning is reasonable to expect that it will be in tandem
of clinical services. Unfortunately, our knowl­ with that of cochlear hearing loss, although it is
edge in this regard is incomplete for each of apparent that not all individuals with this con­
the subtypes of the disorders of loudness per­ dition also experience loudness similarly. The
ception defined in Chapter 1 (Tyler et al., reasons why some individuals have abnormal
2014), and this is a major shortcoming as loudness growth, whereas others with similar
scientists and clinicians try to understand the audiograms do not, are obscure. The natural
lived and shared experience of people with history of recruitment has also not been for­
sound tolerance challenges. In this chapter we mally determined, although it is reasonable to
will delineate the present state of knowledge, expect that as cochlear hearing loss progresses
and indicate where there are opportunities for with age, the loudness perception dysfunction,
focused and highly relevant research in this particularly the patient’s observation regarding
regard. The specific issue of how hyperacusis the inability to hear sounds in the hearing loss
and tinnitus are associated is discussed in de­ region as “soft” (Buus & Florentine, 2002)
tail in Chapter 11. would progress also. While recruitment does

33
34 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

vary with the sound environment (e.g., the in­ on the cultural or socioeconomic variations
tensity of sound being heard), it does not vary that may have led to that.
with emotion or mood: in some cases this can The first study on the epidemiology of
be useful to disambiguate the contribution of hyperacusis published in the peer-review lit­
recruitment from that of hyperacusis (which erature originated in Sweden, and for many
can be modulated by mood state) in an indi­ years the work of Andersson et al. (2002) was
vidual patient. definitive in this area. They used the following
definition of hyperacusis (translation in Ba­
guley and Andersson, 2007):
Hyperacusis in Adults
In our society we are surrounded by sounds
of many kinds. Some of these sounds can be
There have been a small number of epidemi­ annoying or even unpleasant in character. We
ological studies of hyperacusis in the adult all differ in how vulnerable we are to these
population, generally using a random postal sounds. In this survey we study sensitivity to
questionnaire methodology. One issue with everyday sounds in the sense that they evoke ad-
verse reactions. By this we mean, for example,
such research is that response rates are gener­
reactions to a conversation, chirping of birds,
ally rather low, and as a result the findings may paper noises (rustle), the ringing sound at a
overestimate the prevalence of hyperacusis, as pedestrian crossing, or the sounds of a running
human nature dictates that a person is more water tap. In other words, we ask about sounds
likely to complete a questionnaire on a symp­ of moderate loudness that most people experi-
tom or condition of which they have some ence daily without being annoyed. Our inter-
experience. est in this is not restricted to loud sounds such
Two conference reports were the first as drilling machines or low flying aircraft.
to raise the issue. The first was from Sweden,
where Rubinstein and colleagues (1996) un­ The methodology was twofold. The first
dertook a postal study which sought to identify strategy was to recruit respondents through
the prevalence and associations between hy­ the website of a major and reputable Swed­
peracusis, headaches, temporomandibular dis­ ish newspaper, where readers were invited
orders, and amalgam fillings. Data from 1023 to click through to the survey via a banner
female respondents was reported, indicating a on the front page. The Swedish population
point prevalence of 23%, but unfortunately in­ were early adopters of the Internet, even in
formation in the conference proceedings about 2002, and uptake was equitable across class
the definition of hyperacusis, and the response and gender (Andersson, personal communi­
rate of the study, are both so scant that this cation). While 1167 people clicked on the
study is only of note as the first in the area. web banner, only 595 responded; thus, the
In Poland, Fabijanska and colleagues response rate was 52%, although many more
(1996) undertook a postal questionnaire on people may have seen the banner and been
hyperacusis, again with an unspecified index uninterested. The second arm was a postal
question. There were 10,349 respondents, of survey in Uppsala, Sweden, where 987 peo­
whom 15.2% reported hyperacusis, with a ple were mailed the questionnaire pack, and
slightly higher prevalence in females. Interest­ 589 responded (60%). With the aim of sam­
ingly, some regional differences were reported, pling hyperacusis rather than recruitment,
though no supporting information shed light the authors excluded anyone self-identifying
3. The Epidemiology and Natural History of Disorders of Loudness Perception 35

as hearing impaired from the analysis. A point Table 3–2. Reactions to sounds from An-
prevalence rate of 7.7% was determined for dersson et al. (2002). The question was
the Internet group, and 5.9% in the postal “How do you feel when you are being ex-
survey. Of the individuals with hyperacusis, posed to disturbing sounds?”, again with
in the Internet group 51% were female, and 1157 respondents.
in the postal group, 55% were female, indicat­ n %
ing a slight preponderance.
Of interest was the fact that Gerhard An­ Irritated 862 75
dersson and colleagues reported both the types Poor concentration 479 41
of sound the respondents found aversive, and
Angry 141 12
the reactions that were reported, both of these
being chosen from provided lists. These are de­ Tense 199 10
tailed in Tables 3–1 and 3–2, respectively. Pain 57 5
While ostensibly a study about hyper­ Afraid 16 1
acusis, a number of reflections can be made. The
Source: Adapted from Baguley and Andersson
list of annoying sounds did not include eating
(2007).
or breathing sounds, but potentially the category
of “other everyday sounds” included these, and
thus could represent misophonia. Also, most
reactions were irritation, distraction, and an­ The fact that a number of respondents,
noyance, which also map more onto a modern albeit small, reported sound-induced pain was
conception of misophonia than hyperacusis. also of interest. This was higher in those self-
reporting reduced sound tolerance than those
who did not (12.5% vs. 4.5%, respectively),
and the sound-induced pain and/or fear re­
Table 3–1. Ranking of sounds considered ports may represent what would now be classi­
aversive in Andersson et al. (2002). The fied as hyperacusis. As suggested in Chapter 1,
question was “What kind of sounds do you such patients likely endured symptoms asso­
consider aversive?” and the number of re- ciated with more than one of the Tyler et al.
spondents 1151. (2014) categories, as sounds could induce pain
in the short term, and a durable sensation of
n % fear over time.
Another postal questionnaire study from
Noise 660 57
Scandinavia, this time from Finland, included
Mechanical, monotonous 326 28 a question about hyperacusis in a wider con­
sounds sideration of self-reported hearing problems
Music 309 27 in older adults (Hannula et al., 2011). A co­
Other everyday sounds 274 24 hort of adults were sampled (n = 1428, aged
Clatter 171 15 54 to 66 years, respondents n = 850, response
rate 59.5%). Participant selection was said to
Paper noises 55 5
be random, but the report indicated that the
Talk 39 3 individuals were also participating in a large
Source: Adapted from Baguley and Andersson multicenter trial of hearing impairment, and
(2007). had undergone otoscopy and audiometry as
36 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

part of that. Four questions were asked in the and self-identified groups (66% and 70% fe­
survey, and that pertaining to sound tolerance male, respectively).
was presented as follows: Given the generally low response rate, and
the motivational issue regarding nonresponse
Are you particularly sensitive to loud by people with no experience of reduced sound
sounds? tolerance, a skeptical view would be to recalcu­
late prevalence rates with the assumption that
A total of 146 individuals responded yes nonresponse equates to no sound tolerance
(17.1%), but this data was of limited value problems, and this is undertaken in Table 3–3.
as the question was rather nonspecific, and Once undertaken, this reanalysis brings
would capture individuals with recruitment the estimates closer in line with previous
as well as hyperacusis. Of these individuals, clinician-based estimates of 2 to 3% of the adult
102 (70%) were female. population having troublesome hyperacusis
In a study with the main focus on hyper­ (Jastreboff, 2000; Baguley & Andersson, 2007).
acusis, Paulin et al. (2016) reported data from One further reflection on the population
a cohort of adult individuals who were on­ studies described above is the possible impli­
going participants in the Vasterbotten Envi­ cation that patients experience reduced sound
ronmental Health Study in Sweden. Of the tolerance along a continuum. Many adults ap­
8520 people (aged 18 to 79 years) contacted pear to experience aversion or intolerance to
by post, 3406 agreed to participate (40.0%), some sounds, at least at some times, and trou­
and were asked the following questions: blesome hyperacusis may then be an extreme
and persistent form of that.
Until recently, little attention has been
Have you been diagnosed with sound
paid to hearing thresholds in patients with hy­
intolerance by a physician?
peracusis. Anari et al. (1999) noted that pure
Do you have a hard time tolerating tone audiograms “show normal hearing or a
everyday sounds that you believe most slightly high tone loss,” and Hannula and col­
other people can tolerate? leagues (2011) found that while patients with
hyperacusis had statistically significantly worse
As with other studies reviewed above, it is dif­ hearing than controls, the hyperacusis was not
ficult to rule out the possibility that the sound associated with increased hearing handicap,
tolerance question might also capture persons unlike tinnitus, or self-reported problems with
with recruitment. Of the cohort, 66 (1.9%) hearing in noise. A study with the audiometric
identified as having been diagnosed by a phy­ characteristics of patients with hyperacusis as
sician, and 313 (9.2%) self-identified with re­ its main focus was a welcome addition to the
duced sound tolerance: this latter group also literature (Sheldrake et al., 2015). Audiomet­
included all the people that responded “yes” ric findings, both pure tone audiometry (PTA)
to the physician question. The physician- and loudness discomfort level (LDL) in a se­
diagnosed group was found to be older, to ries of 381 patients (170 female – 45%, and
have more frequent problems and for longer 211 male – 55%) with a primary complaint of
duration, and a slightly longer history than hyperacusis were retrospectively analyzed. The
the self-identified group although no statisti­ mean age was 43.9 years (s.d. 15.0). The clinic
cal analysis was undertaken. There was a sta­ at which they were seen was in the U.K. inde­
tistically significant preponderance of women pendent sector; therefore, they were self-pay, or
over men in both the physician-diagnosed had private insurance; in the United Kingdom,
3. The Epidemiology and Natural History of Disorders of Loudness Perception 37

Table 3–3. Prevalence rates recalculated on the assumption that non-response equates
to no sound tolerance problems.

Authors Response Reported Recalculated


(year) Method Rate Prevalence Rate Prevalence Rate

Andersson Internet cohort 52% 7.7% 4.0%


et al. (2002) Postal cohort 60% 5.9% 3.5%
Hannula Postal cohort 60% 17.1% 10.3%
et al. (2011)
Paulin et al. Postal cohort 40%
(2016) Physician 1.9% 0.76%
diagnosed
Self-identified 9.2% 3.68%

this might bias the results as the patients might covered in more detail in Chapter 2. The con­
have been more severely affected than a more clusions that can be drawn from this study,
general hyperacusis population. Pure tone au­ which was by far and away the largest series of
diometry was undertaken up to 8 kHz, using hyperacusis patients reported upon, included
standard techniques, and LDLs performed the finding that cochlear hearing loss was not
using pure tones of 0.5 sec duration, increas­ required to develop hyperacusis, distinguish­
ing intensity in increments of 5 dB, and the ing it from recruitment, and that reduced
patient indicating verbally when they wished dynamic range in hyperacusis occurred across
not to be presented with the next sound. The the frequency range.
authors found that patients presented normal In the introduction to this chapter it was
hearing thresholds (i.e., =<20dB HL) at low proposed that knowledge about the natural
frequencies with mild hearing loss (specified history of a symptom or condition is impor­
as 20 to 40 dB HL) at 4 to 8 kHz; no compar­ tant for the effective counseling and treatment
ison was made with age-matched normative of the patient. Unfortunately, this informa­
audiometric data. The LDL data was com­ tion was not available for hyperacusis, and
pared with a normative data set from Sherlock no longitudinal studies have been published.
and Formby (2005), and group data indi­ While one of the population surveys described
cated that the hyperacusis patients had lower above did collect information about length
LDLs (e.g., reduced dynamic range) on the of history (Paulin et al., 2016), that informa­
order of 15 dB: this effect was noted across tion only pertained to those individuals in
the frequency range. Some anomalies in indi­ whom sound tolerance issues persisted, and
vidual LDL patterns were noted: an unspec­ participants were not asked, “Did you have
ified but small number of patients had LDL reduced sensitivity to sound that has now re­
measurements at 30 dB HL, whereas others solved?” Andersson et al. (2002) did reflect on
did not reach an LDL at the upper limit of a longitudinal study of a cohort of tinnitus
the audiometer. This may call the diagnostic patients in whom the incidence of “sensitivity
reliability of LDL into question, a situation to noise” increased from 38% to 85% over
38 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

a 5-year period. There are opportunities for tively often (about 50% of the time), often
robust longitudinal studies of the natural his­ (>50% of the time), or always (100% of the
tory of hyperacusis. time). Of the participants, 39.2% reported
noise sensitivity 50% of the time or more. The
authors considered 50% or more as the point
of clinical significance, although they acknowl­
Hyperacusis in Childhood
edged that they sampled the frequency of the
experience rather than severity.
As is the case with adults, data regarding the The Avon Longitudinal Study of Par­
epidemiology of hyperacusis in childhood is ents and Children (ALSPAC) (Golding et al.,
sparse, and characterized by major differences 2001) is a large prospective study based in the
in definitions, populations studied, and hence Bristol district of the United Kingdom. More
in the magnitude of prevalence estimates. An than 14,000 pregnant women were recruited
attempt at systematic review was made by Nem­ in 1991/1992, and the health and socioeco­
holt Rosing et al. (2016), and while the team nomic status of the children and mothers has
identified three relevant articles (Coeho et al., been sampled regularly since that time. Au­
2007; Landalv et al., 2013; Widen & Erlands­ diological aspects of the children were stud­
son, 2004), they reluctantly concluded that the ied at age 11, and this included questions on
heterogeneity of the literature was so high that hyperacusis and tinnitus (reported in Hall
it was not feasible to pool the studies. et al., 2016 and Humphriss et al., 2016, respec­
In a study of self-reported tinnitus and tively). The relevant questions used were: “Do
“noise sensitivity” among adolescents (13 to you ever experience over-sensitivity or distress
19 years) in Sweden, Widen and Erlandsson to particular sounds?” and “Do you ever get
(2004) included the question, “Do you con­ noises in your ears? (not associated with noise
sider yourself to be oversensitive to noise?” in exposure)” (Humphriss et al., 2016). Of the
their questionnaire. Of the 1547 young people 7097 children that were sampled, 7092 an­
to whom it was issued, data from 1238 individ­ swered the question about sound tolerance,
uals (77.8%) was complete and was analyzed. and 261 reported that they were oversensi­
Of this cohort, it was reported that 17.1% an­ tive to sound. Of these, 157 were male, and
swered the noise sensitivity question positively, 104 were female. This group was also asked
and of these, 4.0% indicated they had both about behavioral avoidance of activities or
noise sensitivity and tinnitus; this group was places due to over-sensitivity to sound, and
also more likely to worry about their hearing, 112 young people (42.9%) responded affir­
and to use hearing protection. The young peo­ matively; 21 (8.0%) used hearing protection
ple were stratified into two groups (13 to 15 for some sounds. The possibility of sensitiv­
and 16 to 19 years) on the basis of schooling, ity to other sensory stimuli was also investi­
and the older group was reported to be more gated, the most common being to light/colors
likely to experience noise sensitivity. (n = 22, 8.4% of those with hyperacusis), touch
The same noise sensitivity question was (n = 17, 6.5%), and smell (n = 14, 5.4%). In­
used by another Swedish research group (Lan­ formation was available on the presence or
dalv et al., 2013) who reported data from 281 absence of tinnitus in 260 of the 261 young
youths aged 15 to 19 years. A modification people with hyperacusis, and a strong associ­
was that the participants were asked to rank ation was reported with 109 (41.9%) of the
the frequency of this experience, as never (0% individuals with hyperacusis also experienc­
of the time), seldom (<25% of the time), rela­ ing tinnitus.
3. The Epidemiology and Natural History of Disorders of Loudness Perception 39

No association was found between hyper­ on the listening habits in 9-year-old children,
acusis and birth weight, housing status (rented and the association with hearing thresholds
or owned), or social class, but there were asso­ and tinnitus. While 21 of the 411 children
ciations identified with being male, an admis­ (5.3%) reported tinnitus, hyperacusis (not de­
sion to hospital within the first four weeks of fined) was said to be present in only one child
life (but not admission to a Special Care Baby (0.2%), although it was not clear if issues with
Unit in the neonatal period), and, surprisingly, sound tolerance were formally sought.
a higher level of maternal education. Regard­ Regarding the incidence of hyperacusis in
ing otologic/audiologic status, the children with childhood, two studies are of note. In a survey
hyperacusis were not more likely to have had of clinical practice regarding childhood tinni­
a history of otitis media with effusion, or el­ tus, Baguley et al. (2012) reported data from 4
evated stapedial reflex thresholds, and no sig­ expert centers in Europe. Of the 88 children that
nificant associations were found with hearing had been referred to these centers in a calendar
thresholds on PTA. An interesting association year, data on the presence of hyperacusis was
was found with larger otoacoustic emission available in 87, and in these cases hyperacusis
amplitudes to low-intensity stimuli, but not to was present in 34 (39%), but no other infor­
higher-intensity stimuli. Unfortunately, data mation was collected. In a study from Southern
was not available on diagnoses of autistic spec­ Denmark, Nemholt-Rosing (2016) reported
trum disorder (see Chapter 7). The authors data from 15 ENT clinics over a 5-year period
noted that the associations with hyperacusis regarding children and adolescents referred for
that were identified raised as many questions care for tinnitus and/or hyperacusis. In that pe­
as answers, but also that the lack of a link with riod 69 children were referred, and of those 9
otitis media with effusion ran counter to some (12.8%) had a primary complaint of hyper­
previous reports (Sun et al., 2011). acusis, and a total of 11 cases (15.7%) experi­
With the aim of researching both child­ enced both tinnitus and hyperacusis, leaving
hood tinnitus and hyperacusis, Coelho et al. 58 (84.3%) cases with tinnitus alone.
(2007) reported data from a cohort of 506 To the author’s knowledge there are no
children (240 female – 47.4%, 266 male – studies that have described the natural history
52.6%) with a mean age of 9.46 years (SD 2.1). of hyperacusis in childhood. Whether the on­
The presence of hyperacusis was sought using set is abrupt or insidious, the nature of the
the question “Are you bothered by any kind relation with tinnitus (see Chapter 11), and
of sound or noise?”, and that of phonophobia the progression over time, all remain obscure.
by “Are you afraid of sounds?” Hyperacusis As with the situation regarding adult hyper­
was identified in 16 of the children (3.2%), acusis, the opportunities for research abound.
and phonophobia in 47 (9.3%); phonopho­
bia was reported to be present in 5 (1.2%) of
the children with hyperacusis. Of the 16 chil­
Misophonia
dren with hyperacusis, 8 (50%) experienced
tinnitus, but no mention was made of sever­
ity; of the 490 children without hyperacusis, Given the relatively recent consideration of
96 (19.6%) experienced tinnitus. Having a misophonia as a discrete clinical entity, it is
mild hearing loss in the left ear was associated not a surprise that there is a paucity of pub­
with an elevated risk of hyperacusis. lished data on the epidemiology and natural
Hyperacusis was mentioned tangentially history. The experience of misophonia is usu­
in a study by Basjo et al. (2016), which focused ally associated with a specific sound (eating
40 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

or sniffing, for example) (McFerran, 2016), fit of the doubt; the possibility must be consid­
and it has been suggested that there are three ered that a dismissive view from society or pro­
characteristic elements ( Jastreboff & Jastreboff, fessionals could exacerbate unnecessarily the
2014; Rouw et al., 2017): patient’s suffering.
Some data is becoming available about
• Disproportional aversive responses to the natural history of clinical misophonia.
the trigger Rouw and Erfanian (2017) report data from a
• Awareness that this response is group of 300 self-identified people with miso­
disproportionate phonia, who responded online to their enqui­
• No clear physical feature (such as ries. While by definition, such persons may be
loudness of the sound) to explain the at the extreme end of such experiences, and
response their natural history has not been one of a re­
version to non-misophonia, the fact that the ex­
Minimal prevalence data is available. Wu et al. perience of the majority of this group (n = 232,
(2014) studied misophonic experiences in a 77%) was one of a worsening over time of their
group on university undergraduates. The re­ symptoms is of note. It was also noted that the
port was that nearly 20% had experiences of most common age of onset was in childhood
misophonia, which is several orders of magni­ or early adolescence, as has been noted else­
tude higher than that which is seen in psychi­ where (McFerran, 2016), and that participants
atry or audiology clinics. The popular press spanned the age range, with broadly equal im­
in the United Kingdom have raised concerns pact of symptoms across ages.
that some reports or apparent experiences of Following the identification of a family
misophonia may be examples of misanthropic with 15 members who were misophonic, San­
behavior (Figure 3–1). This does not honor chez et al. (2017) raised the possibility that
the extreme distress and aversion some clin­ there might be an inherited component. How­
ical patients with misophonia experience. As ever, in the study by Rouw et al. (2017), only
with hyperacusis, if there exists a continuum a minority of participants had another fam­
of such experiences, with clinical misophonia ily member with similar symptoms (n = 67,
at the extreme end, then patient complaints 22%) so familial inheritance may not be re­
must be analyzed carefully and given the bene­ sponsible for the usual pattern.

Figure 3–1. From the U.K. Guardian newspaper, August 18, 2017.
https://www.theguardian.com/commentisfree/2017/aug/18/hate-hearing
-person-eat-sound-misophonia-misanthropy.
3. The Epidemiology and Natural History of Disorders of Loudness Perception 41

While information about misophonia is Båsjö, S., Möller, C., Widén, S., Jutengren, G., &
emerging, there is also evidence that opinions Kähäri, K. (2016). Hearing thresholds, tinnitus,
on some aspects are polarized. Examples are and headphone listening habits in nine-year-
whether this is a primary psychiatric disorder, old children. Intl. J. Audiol., Oct, 55(10), 587–
or an auditory dysfunction (see Cavanna & 596. doi: 10.1080/14992027.2016.1190871.
Buus, S., & Florentine, M. (2002). Growth of loud­
Seri, 2015, for discussion), and also whether
ness in listeners with cochlear hearing losses:
the characteristic emotional state is annoyance/ recruitment reconsidered. J. Assoc. Res. Otolar-
irritation (Rouw & Erfanian, 2017; Kumar & yngol., 3, 120–139. DOI: 10.1007/s1016200
Griffiths, 2017), or anger/rage (Schröder et al., 10084.
2017). This is a literature to be watched with Cavanna, A. E., & Seri, S. (2015). Misophonia:
interest. Current perspectives. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat.,
18(11), 2117–2123. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S81438.
Coelho, C., Sanchez, T. G., & Tyler, R. S. (2007).
Hyperacusis, sound annoyance, and loudness
Summary
hypersensitivity in children. Progress in Brain
Research, 166, 168–178.
Data regarding the epidemiology and natural Degeest, S., Corthals, P., Dhooge, I., & Keppler, H.
history of disorders of sound tolerance is sparse, (2016). The impact of tinnitus characteristics
and opportunities for research are plentiful. As and associated variables on tinnitus-related hand­
icap. J. Laryngol. Otol., 130(1), 25–31.
will be discussed elsewhere (Chapter 16), one
Fabijanska, A., Rogowski, M., Bartnik, G., &
major constraint is the lack of available research
Skarzynski, H. (1999). Epidemiology of tinni­
funding in this area, which is certainly worthy tus and hyperacusis in Poland. In Proceedings of
of urgent clinical and scientific research. the sixth international seminar (pp. 569–571).
J. W. P. Hazell (Ed.). London, U.K.: Tinnitus
and Hyperacusis Centre.
References Golding, J., Pembrey, M., & Jones, R. (2001).
ALSPAC Study Team (ALSPAC): The Avon
longitudinal study of parents and children. I.
Anari, M., Axelsson, A., Eliasson, A., & Mag­ Study methodology. Paediatr. Perinat. Epide-
nusson, L. (1999). Hypersensitivity to sound– miol., 15, 74–87.
questionnaire data, audiometry and classifica­ Hall, A. J., Humphriss, R., Baguley, D. M., Parker,
tion. Scand. Audiol., 28(4), 219–230. M., & Steer, C. D. (2016). Prevalence and risk
Andersson, G., Lindvall, N., Hursti, T., & Carl­ factors for reduced sound tolerance (hyperacusis)
bring, P. (2002). Hypersensitivity to sound in children. Int. J. Audiology, 55(3), 135–141.
(hyperacusis): A prevalence study conducted Hannula, S., Bloigu, R., Majamaa, K., Sorri, M.,
via the Internet and post. Intl. J. Audiol., 41(8), & Maki-Torkko, E. (2011). Self-reported hear­
545–554. ing problems among older adults: Prevalence
Baguley, D. M., & Andersson, G. (2007). Hyper- and comparison to measured hearing impair­
acusis: Mechanisms, diagnosis, and therapies. San ment. J. Am. Acad. Audiol., 22 (8), 550–559.
Diego, CA: Plural. Humphriss, R., Hall, A. J., & Baguley, D. M. (2016).
Baguley, D. M., Bartnik, G., Kleinjung, T., Savas­ Prevalence and characteristics of spontaneous
tano, M., & Hough, E. A. (2013). Troublesome tinnitus in 11-year-old children. Int. J. Audiol.,
tinnitus in childhood and adolescence: Data 55(3), 142–148. doi: 10.3109/14992027.2015
from expert centres. Intl. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolar- .1120890.
yngol., Feb, 77(2), 248–251. doi: 10.1016/j.ijporl Jastreboff, P. J. (2000). Tinnitus habituation therapy
.2012.11.009. (THT) and tinnitus retraining therapy (TRT).
42 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

In Tyler, R. S. (Ed.). Tinnitus handbook (pp. 357– drome: Evidence for autosomal dominant in­
376). San Diego, CA: Singular. heritance? Braz. J. Otorhinolaryngol., pii: S1808–
Jastreboff, P., & Jastreboff, M. (2014). Treatments 8694(17), 30114–30113. doi: 10.1016/j.bjorl
for decreased sound tolerance (hyperacusis and .2017.06.014. [Epub ahead of print]
misophonia). Sem. in Hear., 35(02), 105–120. Schröder, A., van Wingen, G., Vulink, N. C., &
Kumar, S., & Griffiths, T. D. (2017). Response: Denys, D. (2017). Commentary: the brain ba­
Commentary: The brain basis for misophonia. sis for misophonia. Front. Behav. Neurosci., Jun
Front. Behav. Neurosci., 29(11), 127. doi: 10.3389 2, 11, 111. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00111.
/fnbeh.2017.00127. Sheldrake, J., Diehl, P. U., & Schaette, R. (2015).
Landälv, D. L., Malmström, L., & Widén, S. E. Audiometric characteristics of hyperacusis pa­
(2013). Adolescents’ reported hearing symptoms tients. Front. Neurol., May 15, 6, 105. doi: 10
and attitudes toward loud music. Noise Health, .3389/fneur.2015.00105.
15(66), 347–354. doi: 10.4103/1463-1741 Sherlock, L. P., & Formby, C. (2005). Estimates of
.116584. loudness, loudness discomfort, and the audi­
McFerran, D. J. (2016). In Baguley, D. M., & Fa­ tory dynamic range: Normative estimates, com­
gelson, M. (Eds.). Tinnitus: clinical and research parison of procedures, and test-retest reliability.
perspectives. San Diego, CA: Plural. J. Am. Acad. Audiol., 16(2), 85–100.
Nemholt-Rosing, S., Hvass Schmidt, J., Wedder­ Smeds, K., & Leijon, A. (2014). Loudness and
kopp, N., & Baguley, D. M. (2016). Prevalence hearing loss. In Florentine, M., Popper, A. N.,
of tinnitus and hyperacusis in children and ado­ & Fay, R. R. (Eds.). Loudness (pp. 223–259).
lescents: A systematic review. BMJ Open, 6, 1–20. New York, NY: Springer.
Paulin, J., Andersson, L., & Nordin, S. (2016). Sun, W., Manohar, S., Jayaram, A., Kumaraguru, A.,
Characteristics of hyperacusis in the general Fu, Q., Li, J., & Allman, B. (2011). Early age
population. Noise & Health, 18(3), 178–184. conductive hearing loss causes audiogenic sei­
Rosing, S. N., Kapandais, A., Schmidt, J. H., & zure and hyperacusis behavior. Hear Res., Dec,
Baguley, D. M. (2016). Demographic data, 282(1–2), 178–183. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2011
referral patterns and interventions used for .08.004.
children and adolescents with tinnitus and hy­ Tyler, R. S., Pienkowski, M., Roncancio, E. R.,
peracusis in Denmark. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhi- Jun, H. J., Brozoski, T., Dauman, N., Dau­
nolaryngol., Oct, 89, 112–120. doi: 10.1016/j man, N., Andersson, G., Keiner, A. J., Cacace,
.ijporl.2016.07.036. A. T., Martin, N., & Moore, B. C. (2014). A
Rouw, R., & Erfanian, M. (2017). A large-scale review of hyperacusis and future directions:
study of misophonia. J. Clin. Psychol., epub ahead part I. Definitions and manifestations. Am. J.
of print. Aud., 23(4), 402–419.
Rubinstein, B., Ahlqwist, M., & Bengtsson, C. Widén, S. E., & Erlandsson, S. I. (2004). Self-
(1996). Hyperacusis, headache, temporoman­ reported tinnitus and noise sensitivity among
dibular disorder and amalgam fillings—an epi­ adolescents in Sweden. Noise Health; Oct–Dec,
demiological study. In Reich, G. E., & Vernon, 7(25), 29–40.
J. A. (Eds.). Proceedings of the fifth international Wu, M. S., Lewin, A. B., Murphy, T. K., &
tinnitus seminar (pp. 657–658). Portland: Amer­ Storch, E. A. (2104). Misophonia: Incidence,
ican Tinnitus Association. phenomenology, and clinical correlates in an
Sanchez, T. G., & Silva, F. E. D. (2017). Familial undergraduate student sample. J. Clin. Psychol.,
misophonia or selective sound sensitivity syn­ 70(10), 994–1007.
4
Scales and Questionnaires for
Decreased Sound Tolerance
Kathryn Fackrell and Derek J. Hoare

(2011). This document describes the objective


Introduction
of ULL measurement as being to “identify
the minimum level of sound that is judged
Reliable measurement of decreased sound tol- to be uncomfortably loud by the subject.” It
erance (DST) problems, including hyperacusis refers also to the term loudness discomfort
and misophonia, is important to both deter- level (LDL) as being used synonymously with
mining the nature and severity of the problem, ULL but expressly recommends ULL as the
and to the evaluation of any treatment-related preferred term.
change. For assessment of these conditions, The ULL is effectively an absolute judg-
clinicians and researchers rely on audiometric ment often used by clinicians, for example, as
measures of DST (e.g., uncomfortable loud- a threshold for sound tolerance when deriving
ness levels [ULLs], most comfortable listen- information for limiting hearing aid output
ing levels [MCLs], loudness scaling), and/or (Morgan, Dirks, Bower, & Kamm, 1979). In
patient self-reported measurement tools (i.e., a standard clinical set-up, ULLs are estimated
questionnaires). using an audiometer and measured in both
ears at 500, 1000, 2000, and 4000 Hz. Sound
stimuli are presented with a duration of about
1000 msec, and an inter-stimulus interval of
Audiometric Measures of
about 400 msec. Patients are instructed to ei-
Decreased Sound Tolerance
ther press a handheld response button or to
inform the investigator by other means (i.e.,
Measurements of ULLs have long been used hand raise or verbal acknowledgment) when
by hearing health professionals (Liu & Chen, the signal level becomes uncomfortable.
2000) and an evidence-based recommended In determining their clinical value, Ste-
procedure for the measurement of ULLs is phens, Blegvad, and Krogh (1977) examined
provided by the British Society of Audiology the reliability of various audiometric measures

43
44 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

including ULLs in a sample of 10 adults with patients with a primary complaint of severely
normal hearing. They found a high level of disabling tinnitus, providing a methodology
variance in ULLs both within and between for hyperacusis assessment and severity classifi­
subjects, although within subject variance was ca­tion. They recommended performing (1) pure
smaller, and similar to that observed for an tone audiometry, Feldmann masking curves
objective measure (the acoustic reflex thresh- and the Metz test for recruitment, and (2) ULL
old) in the same participants. For 5 of their measurement at frequencies from 250 to
10 participants, ULLs for low frequency sound 8000 Hz, and at a frequency matched to tinni-
(250 Hz) exceeded the output limit of the tus confirmed over three trials. For diagnosis,
audiometer. they recommended that ULLs of 90 dB HL or
Sherlock and Formby (2005) conducted less at two or more frequencies, or a dynamic
a series of experiments to estimate normative range (the difference between thresholds of
values for absolute and relative judgments of audibility and ULLs) of 55 dB or less at any
ULLs, and to evaluate the reliability of within- frequency should be taken as indicative of
and between-subject variability in a test-retest hy­peracusis. Conversely, hyperacusis was not
situation in a group of 59 younger adults (21 evinced if ULLs were 95 dB HL or greater at
males, 38 females) with normal hearing and all frequencies and if the dynamic range was
without sound tolerance problems. The result- 60 dB or greater at all frequencies. How mea-
ing estimated normative value of the ULL was surements might be interpreted using the clas-
about 100 dB HL and was deemed sufficiently sification system proposed by Goldstein and
reproducible for the authors to conclude that Shulman (1996) is summarized in Table 4–1.
a simple ULL estimate of loudness discom- It is important to say here that unless
fort would be a valid clinical measure of the ULLs are performed with care, exposing an
“threshold of discomfort” for sound. More re- individual who suffers from hyperacusis or tin-
cently, however, Sheldrake, Diehl, and Schaette nitus to sounds at or close to an intensity that
(2015) measured hearing thresholds and ULLs evokes pain or discomfort can be unpleasant.
in 381 patients with hyperacusis and mild When this is the case, there is a risk that the
high-frequency hearing loss or normal hearing therapeutic relationship between patient and
thresholds. They found ULLs to be signifi- clinician is undermined by using the procedure.
cantly decreased compared to a normal-hearing In general, clinicians are advised to proceed
reference group, at an average of 85 dB HL with great caution when performing ULLs.
across frequencies. Using receiver operating ULLs are affected by various factors
characteristic (ROC) analysis they showed that including the choice of stimulus (Beattie &
ULL measurements were not sufficiently sensi- Boyd, 1986; Walker, Dillon, Byrne, & Chris-
tive or specific for them to be considered a con- ten, 1984), the instruction given to the pa-
clusive test for hyperacusis. Interestingly, ULLs tient (Beattie, Svihovec, Carmen, & Kunkel,
tended to be higher at hearing loss frequencies 1980; Borstein & Musiek, 1993; Hawkins,
suggesting that hyperacusis was unlikely to Walden, Montgomery, & Prosek, 1987), and
be triggered by increased hearing threshold. the psychophysical method used (Hawkins,
Rather, they concluded that hyperacusis might 1980; Morgan, Wilson, & Dirks, 1972).
be due to a general increase in auditory gain. Anari, Axelsson, Eliasson, and Magnus-
Goldstein and Shulman (1996) exam- son (1999) compared ULLs for pure tone and
ined correlations between tinnitus, hyper­ real-life sounds in 100 consecutive patients
acusis complaint, and ULLs in 42 consecutive complaining of severe oversensitivity to envi­
4. Scales and Questionnaires for Decreased Sound Tolerance 45

Table 4–1. Interpretation severity of sound tolerance problems.

Problem Level Loudness Discomfort Level Dynamic Range


Severe <60 dB (at least 2 frequencies) <35 dB (at least one
hyperacusis frequency)
Moderate Up to 75 dB (at least 2 frequencies) Up to 45 dB (at least one
hyperacusis frequency)
Mild hyperacusis Up to 90 dB (at least 2 frequencies) Up to 55 dB (at least one
frequency)
No problem >95 dB (at all frequencies) >60 dB (at all frequencies)
Source: Adapted from Goldstein & Shulman, 1996.

ron­mental sounds. ULLs for pure tones were who socialized or worked in noisy environ-
tested at 500, 1000, 2000, 3000, and 4000 Hz. ments were recruited directly from being in
Tone pulses were started at 50 dB or 30 dB those environments, underwent ULL testing,
above hearing threshold, and raised in 5 dB and completed a multi-item questionnaire. The
steps until the patient reported discomfort. questionnaire involved participants rating the
The authors then measured ULLs for every- percentage of time in the environment they re-
day sounds such as a baby crying, a dog bark- called the noise level being of a particular com-
ing, a bird singing, and speech-weighted noise. fort level, using a 9-point scale from “very soft”
Meaningful comparison of the different stimuli to “painfully loud.” Their results indicated
was achieved by measuring long-term sound large discrepancies in measures from which
spectra and short-term levels in narrow bands they concluded that ULLs may not accurately
with center frequencies from 250 to 8000 Hz. predict a patient’s impression of uncomfort-
They found that, far from being a consistent able loudness. They concluded the procedure
measure, ULLs of pure tones varied between may not provide a reliable basis for setting the
patients from extreme hypersensitivity (ULLs sound level of hearing aids, for example.
between 35 and 45 dB HL) to apparently nor- Bornstein and Musiek (1993) measured
mal ULLs sometimes exceeding 110 dB HL at ULLs for a 12-speaker babble in 20 partici-
all frequencies. ULLs for the everyday sounds pants with normal hearing sensitivity using
were generally lower and not directly relatable two different sets of participant instructions
to the ULLs for pure tones. Interestingly, there and a simple up/down adaptive procedure.
was a clear relation between the ULLs for ev- In one set of instructions, participants were
eryday sounds and those sounds to which pa- asked to identify a loudness level at which they
tients self-reported as being most (baby crying) would choose “not to listen for any period of
and least (dog barking) sensitive. time.” In the second set of instructions, partic-
In an earlier study, Filion and Margolis ipants were asked to identify a loudness level
(1992) tried to determine whether clinical- at which they “would choose not to listen for
derived ULLs were valid predictors of subjec- 15 minutes or longer.” Data were collected on
tive impressions of uncomfortable loudness in four occasions over 13 weeks. The difference
real-life environments. Groups of individuals in results was striking; ULLs generated using
46 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

the “not to listen for any period of time” were loudness of the signal by first increasing it until
9 dB higher than those using the “not to listen they judged the signal to be too loud, and then
for 15 minutes or longer” instruction. There decreasing the signal level until they judged it
was also a clear difference in the reliability of to be too soft, ultimately increasing and de-
ULLs; for the “not to listen for any period of creasing the loudness until an MCL level was
time” repeated measure correlations ranged selected. Brännström et al. (2014) found a
from 0.90 to 0.97, but for the criterion “not highly significant order effect, in that the first
to listen for 15 minutes or longer” correlations MCL estimate was significantly different from
ranged from 0.62 to 0.89. This study clearly the estimates derived from repeated measures,
demonstrated that ULLs could be reproduc- an outcome attributed to procedural learning
ible, particularly at levels approximating the effects. Other studies found MCL estimates
upper limit of the dynamic range, and it high- to be reproducible over time regardless of the
lighted the importance of knowing the impact specific methodology used (Kopra & Blosser,
of instruction on both the estimate and reli- 1967), or to have a trend toward in­creasing
ability. Johnson (1999) also reiterated the im- over time (Sammeth, Birman, & Hecox, 1989).
portance of consistent testing and instructions. Others have observed MCL estimates to be af-
In addition to consistent use of the wording fected by variables such as age (Coren, 1994),
described by Bornstein and Musiek (1993), hearing loss (Kamm, Dirks, & Mickey, 1978),
Johnson recommended a slow ascending ap- and phonological working memory (Bränn-
proach to measuring ULLs to avoid exceeding ström et al., 2014).
tolerance levels, and to use 1-dB or 2-dB steps A third audiometric method sometimes
when there was an indication of severe hyper- used to estimate DST is loudness scaling.
acusis, for example, if the patient often wore Loudness scaling methods involve either mag-
earplugs or went to great lengths to avoid noise nitude estimation or category rating (Hellman
exposure. & Meiselman, 1990; Elberling & Nielsen,
Another audiometric measure of sound 1993). In magnitude estimation, participants
tolerance is the “most comfortable listening listen to sound stimuli of different intensity lev-
level (MCL).” This was defined as “the level els and provide loudness ratings using a scale
at which the listener would set the stimulus typically ranging from 0 to 100 in response,
were he obliged to listen to it for a long time” from which a ratio scale can be derived. This
(Stephens et al., 1977, p. 213). As with ULLs, method is associated with logarithmic response
various instruction sets have been used in es- bias; for example, it is typically found that a pa-
timating MCLs. Variants of MCL tests have tient rating of loudness doubling corresponds
included the use of semantic or nonsemantic to a 10 dB increase in level, which is a value
(unintelligible) speech signals or pure tone that does not correspond to a doubling (or
stimuli. For example, Brännström et al. (2014) 3 dB increase) in signal intensity. In contrast,
described testing for MCLs using nonseman- category rating involves participants producing
tic signals in a group of 32 normal hearing responses to stimuli that are based on what is
adults. In their procedure, participants were presumed to be units that more closely repre-
instructed to first listen to a speech signal pre- sent linear intervals, thus avoiding logarithmic
sented diotically through headphones at 48 dB response bias. The relative validity of these two
SPL. The participants then adjusted the sound methods is long-debated with published loud-
level to their MCL using an up-and-down pro- ness scaling data obtained with various pro-
cedure. Participants were required to adjust the cedures, and from participants with normal
4. Scales and Questionnaires for Decreased Sound Tolerance 47

hearing or hearing loss, producing both sub- provide a means of measuring these complaints.
stantially different or remarkably similar esti- When effective, questionnaires may yield a sin-
mates (Elberling, 1999). gle severity value that identifies varying levels
of problems with avoidance of social situations,
fear, anxiety, and annoyance across patients.
Questions addressing similar symptoms can
Questionnaires
also form subscales, with separate and statis-
tically independent scores that help clinicians
Health-related questionnaires that are clinically and researchers identify specific areas of con-
useful should ideally (1) provide thorough as- cern and target interventions accordingly. That
sessment of the relevant presenting symptoms said, only a few questionnaires have been devel-
of the construct (i.e., decreased sound toler- oped that specifically quantify decreased sound
ance), (2) provide reliable characterization and tolerance, and for those in use, the evidence of
quantification of individual differences in terms appropriately established psychometric proper-
of the perceived severity, and/or (3) have the ties that would ensure instrument validity and
capacity to responsively estimate changes in generalizability is limited. Here we review some
health status pre- and post-intervention to pro­ commonly used questionnaires and others that
vide evidence for clinical efficacy of interven­ are less cited or in development.
tions. To have confidence that a given ques-
tionnaire is performing as anticipated, it is
essential to critically evaluate its ability to ful­
fill these requirements, and to ask whether Hyperacusis
there is sufficient evidence that the question- Questionnaire (HQ)
naire is fit for its intended purpose (i.e., diag-
nosing decreased sound tolerance in clinic or The Hyperacusis Questionnaire (HQ) was de­
measuring changes in a clinical trial) as well as veloped in France by Khalfa et al. (2002).
to determine its appropriateness for use in the Its primary function is to quantify and charac-
intended population (Terwee et al., 2007). Just terize hypersensitivity to sound, but it has also
because a questionnaire has been found to be been used as an outcome measure of changes in
a reliable measure in one population does not sound tolerance (e.g., Jüris, Andersson, Larsen,
necessarily mean that the same questionnaire & Ekselius, 2014; Mertens, Bodt, & Van de
will be reliable for use with a different popula- Heyning, 2016). The HQ was developed as a
tion. Outlined in Table 4–2 are four validation two-part questionnaire. The first part consists
topics with 10 quality criteria proposed by Ter- of binary questions collecting general informa-
wee et al. (2007) as essential in questionnaire tion on noise exposure history and auditory
validation. These quality criteria should be disorders. These questions are not scored. The
assessed to ensure the integrity of study find- second part of the HQ consists of 14 nega-
ings and clinical interpretations for a given tively worded items. For each item, individuals
population. indicate their level of agreement by rating one
Given that decreased sound tolerance is of four response options on a 4-point Likert
associated with physiological, behavioral, cog- scale: “no” (0 points), “yes, a little” (1 point),
nitive, and emotional complaints (Baguley & “yes, quite a lot” (2 points), and “yes, a lot”
Andersson, 2008; Tyler et al., 2014; Pienkow- (3 points). The mean global score reflects the
ski et al., 2014), clinical questionnaires should sum of all responses with a maximum possible
48 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

score of 42, and a global score of >28 indica- consisted exclusively of 14 items and three
tive of significant hyperacusis. binary questions.
The development procedure of the HQ Examination of questionnaire structure
is not fully documented. Khalfa et al. (2002) is recommended during development to es-
based the questionnaire’s construct on dimen- tablish the degree of variance explained by the
sions of behavioral and adaptive consequences interrelationships between the set of observed
of offending noise, as well as cognitive and variables. Exploratory factor analysis is used to
emotional aspects of aversive listening expe- determine the structure without postulating a
riences as reported in the literature. No infor- theoretical structure. In their exploratory factor
mation was provided on how the items were analysis, Khalfa et al. (2002) identified three
derived from this information and the au- factors (subscales) for the HQ; attentional,
thors did not conduct item reduction. From social, and emotional, with most factor load-
initial conception, the HQ appears to have ings above 0.4. However, taken together these

Table 4–2. Quality criteria for evaluating questionnaires.

Property Technique Quality Criteria


Validity Content The items in the questionnaires are a correct
validity and comprehensive reflection of the populations/
constructs that the questionnaires are intended to
measure.
Structure The items in the questionnaire adequately reflect
validity the underlying unidimensional construct (e.g.,
sensitivity to sound) or multidimensional construct
(i.e., composite measures) being measured.
Explaining at least 50% of the variance and factor
loadings >0.4. Factor analysis conducted on
adequate sample size >100.
Construct The scores demonstrate expected correlations
validity between similar measures (convergent validity)
and expected differences between unrelated
measures (discriminant validity) and these
hypotheses should be defined a priori.
Convergent r Discriminant
Excellent > 0.60 Inadequate
Acceptable 0.30–0.59 Acceptable
Inadequate < 0.30 Excellent
Reliability Internal The hyperacusis and misophonia questionnaires
consistency and any subscales assess different constructs,
and all the items in a questionnaire or subscale
measure the same construct. Internal consistency
is poor if alpha is very low (indicates items in a
subscale are not measuring the same construct)
or very high (indicates some items in the subscale
are redundant).
4. Scales and Questionnaires for Decreased Sound Tolerance 49

Table 4–2. (continued )

Property Technique Quality Criteria


Reliability People with hyperacusis and misophonia
respectively can be distinguished from each
other, despite measurement errors (relative
measurement error).
Agreement Each of the respective hyperacusis and
misophonia questionnaire scores for individuals
tested on multiple occasions over a short time
period (1 to 2 weeks) are close to each other
(absolute measurement error).
Responsiveness Responsive The hyperacusis and misophonia questionnaires
are able to detect clinically important change over
time.
Floor and Few respondents (<15%) achieve the lowest or
ceiling effects highest possible score so that the hyperacusis
and misophonia questionnaires do not
compromise responsiveness, nor content validity
and reliability.
Interpretation Interpretability It is possible to assign qualitative meanings to
subgroups of scores providing a grading system,
and to identify what change represents a minimal
clinically important change for the patient group.

factors only accounted for 48% of the overall be confounding when presented to a tinni­
variance in the data; the unexplained variance tus population. Item 1, for instance, asks, “Do
implied measurement error associated with you ever use earplugs or earmuffs to reduce
this structure (Terwee et al., 2007). There was your noise perception?” and while in a general
also some cross loading of items in the social population this might assess an attentional
factor with the other two factors. Altogether, component of those with decreased sound
this set of findings called into question the in- tolerance, for a tinnitus population using ear
dependence of the subscales and validity of the protection in normal sound environments it is
proposed HQ structure. not encouraged, and so this item would not
An additional issue with the HQ relates likely be endorsed. Fackrell, Fearnley, Hoare,
to its initial validation in a general popula- and Sereda (2015) examined the validity and
tion, and not a population who reported de- reliability of the HQ in a population of 264
creased sound tolerance. In fact, the HQ has adult research participants who had tinnitus.
never been validated on a population whose They found the HQ to have high internal con-
primary complaint was decreased sound tol- sistency (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.88) but Con-
erance. Although Khalfa et al. (2002) were firmatory Factor Analysis (which tests pre-
not explicit about the target population for defined questionnaire structure) showed that
the HQ, items that have been included could the originally proposed three-factor, and an
50 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

alternative one-factor structure, were poor HQ score (>28). The authors’ exploratory
fits to the data. As a measurement tool the factor analysis yielded three factors from the
questionnaire did not perform well in this HQ that were reflective of those reported by
population. Four problematic items, includ- Khalfa et al. (2002). However, in this case,
ing item 1 mentioned above, were identified the three factors accounted for 57.6% of the
and removed from the questionnaire. When variance, and therefore less measurement er-
an exploratory factor analysis was then per- ror, despite most items loading onto one fac-
formed on the remaining 10 items, a two- tor. Internal consistency was acceptable for
factor (attentional and social) structure emerged both subscale and total scores (Cronbach’s
that provided a reasonably good fit. The au- alpha = 0.65 to 0.86). HQ scores and ULLs
thors concluded that this modified version were weakly correlated in those subjects with
of the questionnaire could reliably measure intermediate and high levels of emotional
decreased sound tolerance in a tinnitus pop- exhaustion (r < 0.3), but there was little to
ulation; however, this analysis would need to no correlation in those with low emotional
be confirmed in a new study. Incidentally, exhaustion (r < 0.1). The authors concluded
Fackrell et al. (2015) also provided evidence the need for clinicians to be vigilant to factors
of acceptable convergent and discriminant such as emotional exhaustion (e.g., due to
validity. Weak to moderate correlations (r < long-term stress) when using the HQ.
0.4) were observed between the (14 item) The HQ has now been translated into
HQ and Tinnitus Handicap Questionnaire several languages including Egyptian, Italian,
(THQ; Kuk, Tyler, Russell, & Jordan, 1990), and Japanese, and undergone further testing.
the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI; Beck, Ep- Shabana, Selim, El Refaie, El Dessouky, and
stein, Brown, & Steer, 1988), and the Beck Soliman (2011) reported an assessment using
Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck, Steer, & the (3-factor) Egyptian version of the HQ in
Brown, 1996), suggesting the HQ measured a population of 60 adults (aged 19 to 45) with
a reasonably distinct construct. Furthermore, and without tinnitus who complained of de-
correlations > 0.5 were reported between the creased sound tolerance, and a control group
HQ and ULLs implying some commonality without complaint. The sample was not suffi-
in what the two procedures measured. These cient to perform factor analysis, but data were
correlations should be re-examined if and explored descriptively with some interesting
when the proposed 10-item HQ for tinnitus observations. For example, it was noted that
patients is tested. the emotional domain score was significantly
In another interesting study, Wallén, higher in female patients, and the attentional
Hasson, Theorell, and Canlon (2012) exam- domain score was significantly higher in males,
ined the validity of the HQ in patients with and that scores on the social domain were
emotional exhaustion, comparing HQ scores highly correlated with age. Formal validation
with ULLs and different categories of emo- studies are indicated.
tional exhaustion derived from the emotional In validating the Italian version of the
exhaustion-subscale of the Maslach burnout HQ, Fioretti et al. (2015) recruited 117 pa-
inventory general survey (MBI-GS; Maslach, tients with tinnitus (64 male, 53 female, aged
Jackson, & Leiter, 1996). This study included 14 to 88) and subjected them to a test battery
348 participants (140 men, 208 women) with including ULLs. They found the question-
low, intermediate, and high levels of emo- naire to have high internal consistency (Cron-
tional exhaustion, of which just four individ­ bach’s alpha = 0.89) and strong correlations
uals (1.1%) were hyperacusic according to their between HQ scores and ULLs, once again
4. Scales and Questionnaires for Decreased Sound Tolerance 51

indicating communality between constructs be hindered by noise discomfort, (2) separate


being measured. However, no predictions on noise annoyance from noise-induced deteriora-
expected correlations were made a priori. tion of speech understanding, and (3) quantify
ROC analysis identified a cut-off score of just the severity of hyperacusis over time (Dauman
16 points on the HQ as indicative of signifi- & Bouscau-Faure, 2005). It is a clinician-led
cant hyperacusis. This was in startling contrast questionnaire describing 15 activities, includ-
to the score of 28 initially proposed and gen- ing “driving a car,” “pottering about,” and
erally referred to. The original cut-off score of “other.” Patients provide a score from 0 to 10 to
28 was also questioned by Meeus, Spaepen, indicate their level of annoyance relative to that
De Ridder, and Van de Heyning (2010), who activity. The questionnaire allows for irrelevant
found that most patients reporting lower tol­er­ items to be deleted, for example, if a person is
ance for noise scored <28 on the HQ. There simply not interested in live music events, and
is a risk, therefore, of underestimating the does not attend them, then that item would
presence of significant hyperacusis if the HQ not have to be scored. Cinema and TV activi-
is relied upon clinically. ties are grouped within the questionnaire with
Most recently, Oishi et al. (2017) exam- one score representing both; if a patient scores
ined the validity of a Japanese version of the the two activities differently, then the highest
HQ in a sample of 112 patients with a com- individual score is taken as the score for both.
plaint of hyperacusis, and 103 patients with A mean MASH score is calculated by dividing
hearing loss and/or tinnitus but without hy- the total score by the number of activities, giv-
peracusis. The groups were similar in age (aver- ing a possible range of 0 to 10.
age of 59.1 and 63.4 years, respectively). Over- The development procedure of the
all internal consistency of the HQ was high MASH is not documented. No information
(Cronbach’s alpha = 0.92). Patients complain- was provided on how or why the activities
ing of hyperacusis scored higher on the ques- were chosen or the number of items that could
tionnaire (average = 11.8 points) compared to be deleted and still have a valid overall score.
the other group (average = 5.7 points). Within Dauman and Bouscau-Faure (2005) reported
the hyperacusis group, those who reported hy- acceptable convergent validity with strong
peracusis as their primary complaint scored correlations between the mean MASH scores
higher (average = 18.1 points) than those for and scores on an overall annoyance 10-point
whom hyperacusis was no more or less a prob- Likert scale in a tinnitus population. Corre-
lem than tinnitus or hearing loss (average = spondingly, Meeus et al. (2010) also found
9.9 points). The authors concluded that a cut- that MASH scores strongly correlated with
off score of 16 was essential in selecting which HQ scores in a tinnitus population; indicat-
individuals with hyperacusis require treatment. ing that similar underlying constructs were be-
ing measured. However, Meeus et al. (2010)
found that MASH scores showed similar cor­
relations with the Tinnitus Questionnaire (TQ;
Multiple-Activity Scale for Hallam, Jakes, & Hinchcliffe, 1988; Hiller &
Hyperacusis (MASH) Goebel, 1992) scores. Therefore, the common
construct may be one of annoyance as opposed
The Multiple-Activity Scale for Hyperacusis to some hyperacusis-specific construct. Fur-
(MASH) was developed in France by Dauman thermore, given that patients can delete items,
and Bouscau-Faure (2005). It was designed the underlying structure needs to be further
to (1) account for diverse situations that can examined. It would be interesting to determine
52 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

whether all the items were related to an under- team used principal components analysis and
lying construct of annoyance and whether this identified a three-factor solution explaining
relation changed depending on the number 50.6% of the variance in the data. The factors
of items. Patient-generated measures, such as were named cognitive responses to the hyper-
the MASH, on which patients can choose to acusis, actional/somatic behavior, and emo-
add or delete items (activities in this case), are tional response to external noise source. Based
believed to be more relevant and meaningful on results of the factor analysis and internal
to patients. However, although appealing for consistency analysis, the number of items was
individual assessment, they are liable to cloud again reduced to the final 15 items included
comparisons across individuals as the com­ in the questionnaire, to yield a total score of
posite scores would be difficult to interpret. It 0–45. Initial validation of the GÜF showed
is unlikely to be a suitable measure of effect of evidence of high internal consistency, good
treatment in a clinical trial, for example (Patel, reliability within the factors, and acceptable
Veenstra, & Patrick, 2003). convergent validity with a strong correlation
(r = 0.8) between the GÜF and therapeutic ex-
pert assessment of hyperacusis (Nelting et al.,
2002). However, the authors also reported
strong correlations between the GÜF and the
German Questionnaire on
TQ, in particular with the TQ hearing prob-
Hypersensitivity to Sound lem subscale indicating that the GÜF was po-
(Geräuschüberempfindlichkeit: tentially measuring aspects of tinnitus-related
GÜF) hearing problems.
Bläsing, Goebel, Flötzinger, Berthold,
The German Questionnaire on Hypersensitiv- and Kröner-Herwig (2010) further validated
ity to Sound (GÜF) is a 27-item questionnaire the GÜF in a sample of 91 inpatients (55 males,
developed by Nelting, Reinhoff, Hesse, and 36 females, aged between 15 and 76 years)
Lam (2002) who described its purpose as pro- who also had both tinnitus and hyperacusis.
viding a brief tool to inform treatment needs They found the questionnaire to have good in-
(identify urgency) and treatment planning. It ternal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.92),
was developed by a team in Germany which and while their explorative principal compo-
included physicians, psychologists, and ther- nents factor analysis produced a three-factor
apists. Based on clinical experience and what solution to their data, the factors did not con­
patients had reported within group therapy cur with the factor structure identified by Net­
sessions, the team generated an initial set of ling et al. (2002).
85 items that was later distilled to 27 items the The GÜF has been translated into Span-
team considered representative of the behav- ish (Test Hypersensitivity to Sound; THS;
iors of interest, for example, “because of sensi- Herráiz, de los Santos, Diges, Díez, & Apa-
tivity to sound I am nervous and irritable.” The ricio, 2006) and its use examined in a study
possible responses to each item were “not true,” involving 40 patients (age range = 27 to 68) re­
“sometimes true,” “often,” or “always true,” ferred to clinic because of hyperacusis. Thirty-
scored 0 to 3, respectively, with greater total six of those participants also had tinnitus.
scores indicating a bigger problem. The initial Face validity of the questionnaire was assessed;
validation work involved 226 patients (94 fe- 97% of participants rated it as simple, clear,
male, 132 male, aged 15 to 76) who complained and easy to understand. Internal consistency
of both hyperacusis and tinnitus. The study was high (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.90). The team
4. Scales and Questionnaires for Decreased Sound Tolerance 53

found that higher THS scores correlated with (2014). Questionnaire items were derived from
higher self-rating of severity of hyperacusis. a search of research literature, reviewed by psy-
Based on this small sample they concluded chologists and psychiatrists with clinical ex-
that the THS could be reliably used in clinical perience related to misophonia, and checked
practice to measure the impact of hyperacusis for clarity and readability by two patients with
on a patient’s quality of life. Further work is misophonia in an outpatient clinic for obses-
required to evaluate the factor structure and sive compulsive disorder. A preliminary psy-
responsiveness to change in patient complaint chometrics evaluation was then conducted
of this version of the questionnaire. on a general population of 483 undergradu-
An English translation of the GÜF is ate students (79 males: 404 females, mean age
also available (Noise Avoidance Questionnaire: 21.4 years). Using exploratory factor analysis, a
NAQ; Bläsing & Kroener-Herwig, 2012); how­ three-factor solution was determined and used
ever, this version has not been subject to valida- to define 3 MQ subscales. The “Misophonia
tion studies and has not yet been used in the Symptom Scale” included items that specify
clinics or research. sound sensitivities (e.g., dislike of eating or
tapping sounds). The “Misophonia Emotions
and Behaviors Scale” measured emotional and
Sound Sensitive behavioral reactions to misophonia symptoms
Tinnitus Index (e.g., leaving the environment where a particu-
lar sound was heard). Items in both these scales
The Sound Sensitive Tinnitus Index (SSTI; were rated on a 0 (not at all true) to 4 (always
Greenberg, Walsh, & Carlos, 2016) is a ques- true) scale, and summed to give a total score
tionnaire specifically developed to measure from 0 to 68. The third subscale, the “Miso-
sound tolerance in patients with tinnitus. As phonia Severity Scale,” was an adaptation of
sound tolerance problems are often co-morbid the National Institute of Mental Health Global
with tinnitus, this is a welcome addition to Obsessive-Compulsive Scale (NIMH GOCS;
the clinician toolkit. The SSTI is a 33-item Murphy, Pickar, & Alterman, 1982). On this
questionnaire developed using items identi- scale patients rated their sound sensitivity on
fied through literature review, including items a 15-point scale (“minimal” to “very severe”)
from the HQ and MASH. The first validation with a score of 7 indicating sound sensitivities
study of the SSTI was conducted online and that caused a substantial problem. Internal
involved 277 adults with tinnitus from 32 dif- consistency of the three subscales was adequate
ferent countries (primarily the United States). (α = 0.86, 0.86, and 0.89, respectively). The
For inclusion participants had to report that MQ total score showed moderate convergent
loud sounds at least sometimes exacerbated validity; correlation with the Adult Sensory
tinnitus. Further detail on the questionnaire Questionnaire (ASQ; Kinnealey & Oliver,
has yet to be published and analysis of this 2002) sound sensitivity question was moderate
data is ongoing; both will be important addi- (r = 0.05). Discriminant validity was high; cor-
tions to the literature. relation between the MQ total score and the
ASQ visual, olfactory, and tactile sensitivities
were low (r = 0.33, 0.28, and 0.34, respectively).
Misophonia Questionnaire To date the MQ has also been used for
reporting pre- and post-treatment effects; Mc­
The Misophonia Questionnaire (MQ) was de- Guire, Wu, and Storch (2015) reported cases
veloped by Wu, Lewin, Murphy, and Storch of two girls, ages 17 and 11 years, who underwent
54 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

cognitive behavior therapy for misophonia. misophonia should be delineated as a separate


Whereas the 17-year-old girl completed the psychiatric disorder.
MQ independently, the 11-year-old girl com- Some of the multi-item misophonia ques-
pleted it with the guidance of her mother. tionnaires reported by Dozier (2015a, 2015b)
After treatment both girls showed a reduc- appear to have evolved in clinical practice and
tion in total MQ score which was reported as have yet to be validated. These include the
a large reduction in misophonia symptoms. Misophonia Assessment Questionnaire (MAQ)
The MQ’s validity for these different ages and and the Misophonia Coping Responses (MCR)
for different methods of completion is yet to surveys which Dozier attributed to Marsha
be confirmed. A clinically-meaningful change Johnson, and the authors’ own questionnaire,
in MQ score must be determined before mea- the Misophonia Emotional Response (MER)
surement recommendations can be made. survey.
The MAQ consists of 21 items that ask
about the negative impact misophonia has
on a person’s activities, thoughts, and feel-
ings (e.g., My sound issues have recently created
Other Misophonia Tools problems for me in groups, or My sound issues
are classified as “crazy”  ). Items are rated from
Many other questionnaire measures of miso- 0 (not at all) to 3 (almost all the time) giving
phonia have recently been developed and a maximum questionnaire score of 63. John-
used in reporting but have yet to undergo psy­ son (2014) provided an interpretation of the
chometric evaluation. The Amsterdam Miso- total score as mild (scoring 0 to 21), moderate
phonia Scale (A-MISO-S; Schröder, Vulink, (scoring 22 to 42), or severe (scoring 43 to 63)
& Denys, 2013) is an adapted version of the misophonia. The MCR uses the same 4-point
Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale (Y- scale as the MAQ to rate the frequency of cer-
BOCS; Goodman et al., 1989) developed to tain behaviors that are elicited by misopho-
measure the severity of misophonia symp- nia trigger sounds (e.g., Put on headphones, or
toms. The scale contains 6 items asking pa- Walk away from the person making the sound  ).
tients about (1) time they spent thinking The MER uses a 4-point scale to rate the fre-
about misophonia; (2) interference with social quency of emotions that are elicited by miso-
functioning; (3) level of anger; (4) resistance phonia trigger sounds (e.g., You wish you were
against the impulse; (5) control they had over deaf, or You want to physically assault the person
their thoughts and anger; and (6) time they making the noise), providing important informa-
spent avoiding misophonic situations. Each tion to support treatment planning. However,
item is scored on a scale from 0 to 4 giving both surveys’ value would increase upon deter-
a total score ranging from 0 to 24. Schröder mination of their psychometric properties.
et al. (2013) also offered an interpretation of
total scores whereby scores 0 to 4 indicated
subclinical, 5 to 9 indicated mild, 10 to 14 in-
dicated moderate, 15 to 19 indicated severe,
Conclusion
and 20 to 24 indicated extreme misophonic
symptoms. In their study, A-MISO-S scores
in 42 adults with misophonia were com- There is general agreement in the literature
pared to their scores on standard psychiatric that ULL estimates of decreased sound tol-
questionnaires. The authors concluded that erance provide a valid clinical measure of the
4. Scales and Questionnaires for Decreased Sound Tolerance 55

“threshold of discomfort” for sound, although from one specific population may not repre-
several studies have commented upon the un- sent the complaints or beliefs of a different
satisfactory variability of this and similar proce- population. Overall, this would influence
dures. ULLs can be affected by factors such as other aspects of the questionnaire’s psycho-
the choice of stimulus or the set of instructions metric properties and clinical utility, that is,
used; consistency of methodology is key to re- the factor structure and reliability of their
liable repeated measures. Some research stud- use over time. The accuracy with which the
ies also confirm overlap in the health construct current hyperacusis questionnaires measure
that is measured by ULLs and clinical ques- sound tolerance specifically in patients with
tionnaire measures of decreased sound toler- both hyperacusis and tinnitus is questionable
ance, suggesting the two procedures should be (Fackrell et al., 2015). Indeed, in general, and
complementary when used clinically (Meeus for both hyperacusis and misophonia, the cur­
et al., 2012; Wallén et al., 2012). ULLs are par- rent lack of psychometrically validated instru­
ticularly indicated when the clinical question- ments suggests there is still much to know
naire has not been formally validated for use about (1) how well the questionnaires currently
or there is no robust method of interpreting available measure the construct of in­terest,
the scores. Based on the evidence identified for (2) how scores can be meaningfully inter­preted
this chapter, results obtained from all existing in their target populations and in prev­alent
questionnaires would benefit from further psy- clinical subgroups, and then (3) whether new
chometric evaluation. methods of interpreting scores are needed for
Problems of decreased sound tolerance particular subgroups, and (4) whether new
are commonly co-morbid with tinnitus. In clinical questionnaires need to be systemati-
an analysis of the Tinnitus Research Initiative cally developed.
patient database, Schecklmann et al. (2014)
found that of 1713 tinnitus patients 935
responded “yes” to the single question “Do
sounds cause you pain or physical discom- References
fort?” when completing the Tinnitus Sample
Case History Questionnaire (TSCHQ; Land- Anari, M., Axelsson, A., Eliasson, A., & Mag-
grebe et al., 2010). Variables significantly nusson, L. (1999). Hypersensitivity to sound:
associated with reporting both tinnitus and Questionnaire data, audiometry and classifica-
decreased sound tolerance were younger age, tion. Scandinavian Audiology, 28, 219–230.
higher tinnitus-related distress, and higher Baguley, D. M., & Andersson, G. (2008). Hyper­
ratings of pain (headache, neck pain) or ver- acusis: Mechanisms, diagnosis, and therapies. San
tigo. It is not surprising therefore that the Diego, CA: Plural.
development and validation studies of hy- Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996).
peracusis questionnaires reviewed here have Beck depression inventory-II. San Antonio, TX.
Beck, A. T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., & Steer, R. A.
largely been based on populations reporting
(1988). An inventory for measuring clinical anx­
tinnitus over and above hyperacusis. This af-
iety: Psychometric properties. Journal of Consult­
fects the utility, reliability, and validity of the ing and Clinical Psychology, 56, 893.
questionnaires. Items must be representative Beattie, R. C., Svihovec, D. A., Carmen, R. E., &
of the construct they measure, such that the Kunkel, H. A. (1980). Loudness discomfort
aims and objectives of the questionnaire are level for speech: comparison of two instructional
relevant to the population of interest. Item se- sets for saturation sound pressure level selection.
lection and reduction based on the responses Ear and Hearing, 1, 197–205.
56 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

Beattie, R. C., & Boyd, R. L. (1986). Relation- Elberling, C. (1999). Loudness scaling revisited.
ship between pure-tone and speech loudness Journal of the American Academy of Audiology,
discomfort levels among hearing-impaired sub- 10, 248–260.
jects. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, Fackrell, K., Fearnley, C., Hoare, D. J., & Sereda, M.
51, 120–125. (2015). Hyperacusis questionnaire as a tool
Bläsing, L., & Kroener-Herwig, B. (2012). Self- for measuring hypersensitivity to sound in a
reported and behavioral sound avoidance in tin­ tinnitus research population. BioMed Research
nitus and hyperacusis subjects, and association International, Article ID 290425.
with anxiety ratings. International Journal of Au­ Fioretti, A., Tortorella, F., Masedu, F., Valenti, M.,
diology, 51, 611–617. Fusetti, M., & Pavaci, S. (2015). Validity of
Bläsing, L., Goebel, G., Flötzinger, U., Berthold, A., the Italian version of Khalfa’s Questionnaire on
& Kröner-Herwig, B. (2010). Hypersensitivity hyperacusis. Acta Otorhinolaryngologica Italica,
to sound in tinnitus patients: an analysis of a 35, 110.
construct based on questionnaire and audio­ Filion, P., & Margolis, R. (1992). Comparison
logical data. International Journal of Audiology, of clinical and real-life judgments of loudness
49, 518–526. discomfort. Journal of the American Academy of
Bornstein, S. P., & Musick, F. E. (1993). Loudness Audiology, 3, 193–199.
discomfort level and reliability as a function of Goldstein, B., & Shulman, A. (1996). Tinnitus-
instructional set. Scandinavian Audiology, 22, hyperacusis and the loudness discomfort level
125–131. test—A preliminary report. The International
Brännström, K. J., Olsen, S. Ø., Holm, L., Kast- Tinnitus Journal, 2, 83–89.
berg, T., & Ibertsson, T. (2014). The effect of Goodman, W K., Price, L. H., Rasmussen, S. A.,
repeated measurements and working memory Mazure, C., Fleischmann, R. L. et al. (1989).
on the most comfortable level in the ANL The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale.
test. International journal of Audiology, 53(11), I. Development, use, and reliability. Archives of
787–795. General Psychiatry, 46, 1006–1011.
British Society of Audiology. (2011). Recom- Greenberg, B., Walsh, B., & Carlos, M. (2016).
mended Procedure: Determination of Uncom- Assessing the impact of sound sensitivity in tin-
fortable Loudness Levels. Available from www nitus: Preliminary data from the Sound Sensi-
.thebsa.org.uk/. Last accessed January 6, 2017. tive Tinnitus Index. Proceeding of the American
Coren, S. (1994). Most comfortable listening level Auditory Society.
as a function of age. Ergonomics, 37, 1269–1274. Hallam, R. S., Jakes, S. C., & Hinchcliffe, R.
Dauman, R., & Bouscau-Faure, F. (2005). As- (1988). Cognitive variables in tinnitus annoy-
sessment and amelioration of hyperacusis in ance. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 27,
tinnitus patients. Acta Oto-Laryngologica, 125, 213–222.
503–509. Hawkins, D. B. (1980). Loudness discomfort lev-
Dozier, T. H. (2015a). Counterconditioning treat- els. A clinical procedure for hearing aid evalu-
ment for misophonia. Clinical Case Studies, 14, ations. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders,
374–387. 45, 3–15.
Dozier, T. H. (2015b). Treating the initial physical Hawkins, D. B., Walden, B. E., Montgomery, A.,
reflex of misophonia with the neural repattern- & Prosek, R. A. (1987). Description and vali-
ing technique: A counterconditioning proce- dation of an LDL procedure designed to select
dure. Psychological Thought, 8, 189–210. SSPLSO. Ear and Hearing, 8, 162–169.
Elberling, C., & Nielsen, C. (1993). The dynam- Hellman, R. P., & Meiselman, C. H. (1990).
ics of speech and the auditory dynamic range Loudness relations for individuals and groups
in sensorineural hearing impairment. In J. Bei- in normal and impaired hearing. Journal of the
lin and G. R. Jensen (Eds.). Recent developments Acoustical Society of America, 88, 2596–2606.
in hearing instrument technology (pp. 99–134). Herráiz, C., De los Santos, G., Diges, I., Díez, R.,
Denmark: Danavox Jubilee Foundation. & Aparicio, J. M. (2006). Evaluación de
4. Scales and Questionnaires for Decreased Sound Tolerance 57

la hiperacusia: Test de hipersensibilidad al McGuire, J. F., Wu, M. S., & Storch, E. A. (2015).
sonido. Acta Otorrinolaringológica Española, 57, Cognitive-behavioral therapy for 2 youths with
303–306. misophonia. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry,
Hiller, W., & Goebel, G. (1992). A psychometric 76, 573–574.
study of complaints in chronic tinnitus. Jour­ Meeus, O. M., Spaepen, M., De Ridder, D., &
nal of Psychosomatic Research, 36, 337–348. Van de Heyning, P. H. (2010). Correlation be-
Johnson, M. (1999). A tool for measuring hyper­ tween hyperacusis measurements in daily ENT
acusis. The Hearing Journal, 52, 34–35. practice. International Journal of Audiology, 49,
Johnson, M. (2014). 50 cases of misophonia using 7–13.
the MMP. Proceedings of the Misophonia Con­ Mertens, G., De Bodt, M., & Van de Heyning, P.
ference of the Tinnitus Practitioners Association, (2016). Cochlear implantation as a long-term
Atlanta, GA. treatment for ipsilateral incapacitating tinni­
Jüris, L., Andersson, G., Larsen, H. C., & Ekse- tus in subjects with unilateral hearing loss up
lius, L. (2014). Cognitive behaviour therapy to 10 years. Hearing Research, 331, 1–6.
for hyperacusis: A randomized controlled trial. Morgan, D. E., Wilson, R. H., & Dirks, D. D.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 54, 30–37. (1974). Loudness discomfort level: selected
Kamm, C., Dirks, D. D., & Mickey, M. R. (1978). methods and stimuli. The Journal of the Acous­
Effect of sensorineural hearing loss on loudness tical Society of America, 56, 577–581.
discomfort level and most comfortable loud- Morgan, D. E., Dirks, D. D., Bower, D., &
ness judgments. Journal of Speech, Language, Kamm, C. A. (1979). Loudness discomfort
and Hearing Research, 21, 668–681. level and acoustic reflex threshold for speech
Khalfa, S., Dubal, S., Veuillet, E., Perez-Diaz, F., stimuli. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hear­
Jouvent, R., & Collet, L. (2002). Psychometric ing Research, 22, 849–861.
normalization of a hyperacusis questionnaire. Murphy, D. L., Pickar, D., & Alterman, I. S.
ORL, 64, 436–442. (1982). Methods for the quantitative assess-
Kinnealey, M., & Oliver, B. (2002). Adult sensory ment of depressive and manic behavior. In E. I.
questionnaire. Philadelphia, PA: Temple Uni- Burdock, A. Sudilovsku, & S. Gershon (Eds.),
versity, College of Allied Health Professionals. The behaviour of psychiatric patients: Quantita­
Kopra, L. L., & Blosser, D. (1968). Effects of tive techniques for evaluation (pp. 335–392).
method of measurement on most comfortable New York, NY: Marcel Dekker.
loudness level for speech. Journal of Speech, Lan­ Nelting, M., Rienhoff, N. K., Hesse, G., & Lam-
guage, and Hearing Research, 11, 497–508. parter, U. (2002). Die Erfassung des subjektiven
Kuk, F. K., Tyler, R. S., Russell, D., & Jordan, H. Leidens unter Hyperakusis mit einem Selbst-
(1990). The psychometric properties of a tinni- beurteilungsbogen zur Geräuschüberempfind-
tus handicap questionnaire. Ear and Hearing, lichkeit (GÜF). Laryngo-Rhino-Otologie, 81,
11, 434–445. 327–334.
Landgrebe, M., Zeman, F., Koller, M., Eberl, Y., Oishi, N., Yamada, H., Kanzaki, S., Kurita, A.,
Mohr, M., et al. (2010). The Tinnitus Research Takiguchi, Y., Yuge, I., . . . & Ogawa, K. (2017).
Initiative (TRI) database: A new approach for Assessment of hyperacusis with a newly pro-
delineation of tinnitus subtypes and generation duced Japanese version of the Khalfa hyper­
of predictors for treatment outcome. BMC Med­ acusis questionnaire. Acta Oto-Laryngologica, 1–5.
ical Informatics and Decision Making, 10, 42. Patel, K. K., Veenstra, D. L., & Patrick, D. L.
Liu, T. C., & Chen, Y. S. (2000). Loudness dis- (2003). A review of selected patient-generated
comfort levels in patients with conductive and outcome measures and their application in
mixed hearing loss. Auris Nasus Larynx, 27, clinical trials. Value in Health, 6, 595–603.
101–104. Pienkowski, M., Tyler, R. S., Roncancio, E. R.,
Maslach C., Jackson S. E., & Leiter M. P. (1996). Jun, H. J., Brozoski, T., Dauman, N., . . . &
Maslach burnout inventory manual. Palo Alto, Martin, N. (2014). A review of hyperacusis
CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. and future directions: Part II. Measurement,
58 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

mechanisms, and treatment. American Journal ditory measures. Scandinavian Audiolog  y, 6,


of Audiology, 23, 420–436. 213–221.
Sammeth, C. A., Birman, M., & Hecox, K. E. Terwee, C. B., Bot, S. D., de Boer, M. R., van der
(1989). Variability of most comfortable and un- Windt, D. A., Knol, D. L., Dekker, J., . . . &
comfortable loudness levels to speech stimuli in de Vet, H. C. (2007). Quality criteria were pro-
the hearing impaired. Ear and Hearing, 10, posed for measurement properties of health sta-
94–100. tus questionnaires. Journal of Clinical Epidemiol­
Schecklmann, M., Landgrebe, M., Langguth, B., & ogy, 60, 34–42.
TRI Database Study Group. (2014). Pheno- Tyler, R. S., Pienkowski, M., Roncancio, E. R.,
typic characteristics of hyperacusis in tinnitus. Jun, H. J., Brozoski, T., Dauman, N., . . . &
PLoS One, 9, e86944. Martin, N. (2014). A review of hyperacusis and
Shabana, M. I., Selim, M. H., El Refaie, A., El Des- future directions: part I. Definitions and man-
souky, T. M., & Soliman, R. Y. (2011). Assessment ifestations. American Journal of Audiology, 23,
of hyperacusis in Egyptian patients: Evaluation of 402–419.
the Arabic version of the Khalfa questionnaire. Wallén, M. B., Hasson, D., Theorell, T., & Can-
Audiological Medicine, 9, 127–134. lon, B. (2012). The correlation between the
Sheldrake, J., Diehl, P. U., & Schaette, R. (2015). hyperacusis questionnaire and uncomfortable
Audiometric characteristics of hyperacusis pa- loudness levels is dependent on emotional ex-
tients. Frontiers in Neurology, 6, 105. haustion. International Journal of Audiology, 51,
Schröder, A., Vulink N., & Denys, D. (2013). 722–729.
Misophonia: Diagnostic criteria for a new psy- Walker, G., Dillon, H., Byrne, D., & Christen, R.
chiatric disorder. PLoS ONE, 8, e54706. (1984). The use of loudness discomfort levels
Sherlock, L. P., & Formby, C. (2005). Estimates of for selecting the maximum output of hearing
loudness, loudness discomfort, and the auditory aids. Australian Journal of Audiology, 6, 23–32.
dynamic range: Normative estimates, comparison Wu, M. S., Lewin, A. B., Murphy, T. K., & Storch,
of procedures, and test-retest reliability. Journal of E. A. (2014). Misophonia: Incidence, phenom­
the American Academy of Audiology, 16, 85–100. enology, and clinical correlates in an under-
Stephens, S. D. G., Blegvad, B., & Krogh, H. J. graduate student sample. Journal of Clinical
(1977). The value of some suprathreshold au- Psychology, 70, 994–1007.
SECTION II
Mechanisms
5
Peripheral Mechanisms of
Decreased Sound Tolerance
Roland Schaette

Decreased sound tolerance (DST) is a relatively listeners (Sherlock and Formby, 2005). In
widespread phenomenon. The estimates for its some cases, however, LDLs were as low as 30
prevalence differ widely in the literature; values to 40 dB HL (Sheldrake et al., 2015). Per-
of 2% (Sammeth et al., 2000), 8.6% (Anders- ceived loudness measured through categorical
son et al., 2002), or even 15.2% (Fabijanska, loudness scaling (Cox et al., 1997) or similar
1999) of the adult population have been re- rating procedures showed a rapid growth of
ported. A large-scale questionnaire-based study loudness with sound intensity in hyperacusis
from Sweden revealed a prevalence of 2.2% for patients, and they rated sounds as “too loud”
physician-diagnosed hyperacusis, and 10.5% at lower levels than control subjects (Brandy
for self-reported hyperacusis (Paulin et al., and Lynn, 1995; Norena and Chery-Croze,
2016). For children, the estimates for the prev- 2007). Interestingly, the pattern of decreased
alence of DST range from 3.2 to 17.1%. (Ros- sound tolerance does not seem to show a dis-
ing et al., 2016). One of the reasons for the tinct frequency dependence, as there was a
wide range of estimates might be that different consistent reduction of LDLs across all tested
surveys have used different definitions of what frequencies from 125 Hz to 8 kHz (Sheldrake
constitutes decreased sound tolerance. How- et al., 2015). Categorical loudness scaling mea-
ever, even the most conservative estimates of surements also showed that loudness growth
around 2% indicate that decreased sound tol- functions measured at different frequencies in
erance could affect millions. hearing-impaired patients converged at high
Changes in loudness perception with sound intensities, even though the functions
DST have been characterized by measuring differed considerably at low sound intensities
loudness discomfort levels (LDLs) and loud- due to hearing threshold differences (Norena
ness growth functions. On average, the LDLs and Chery-Croze, 2007).
of affected patients were 15 to 20 dB (Anari Unfortunately, measurements of per-
et al., 1999; Formby et al., 2007; Sheldrake et ceived loudness, and especially LDL data,
al., 2015) lower than those of normal-hearing likely yield an incomplete characterization of

61
62 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

symptoms when introducing the phenotype of do not elicit a pain sensation. It is currently not
DST. First, LDLs are not sensitive or specific known whether the same mechanisms that alter
enough when used in isolation to diagnose loudness perception and decrease the LDLs are
hyperacusis (Sheldrake et al., 2015). A study also responsible for lowering the threshold for
that investigated LDLs and subjective reports triggering the experience of pain from sound.
of sound tolerance problems in daily life found Moreover, it is even conceivable that fear of ex-
only weak correlations between LDLs (mea- periencing auditory pain might influence LDL
sured for pure tones and speech) and subjec- measurements, causing patients to abort the
tive ratings for sound tolerance (Zaugg et al., test early, and the expectation of pain could also
2016). Measurements of loudness might thus change the evaluation of sounds in the brain,
not accurately reflect the difficulties experi- thereby altering loudness perception.
enced in daily life by patients. In particular, In the following, I will review and evalu-
the experience of pain from sounds might ate potential mechanisms of how altered loud-
be insufficiently captured by LDL measure- ness perception and pain from sound might be
ments. The threshold for experiencing pain generated, focusing on the auditory periphery.
from sound is between 120 and 140 dB SPL As the relation between the two phenomena
in normal-hearing listeners (Plutchik, 1963; is unclear, I will treat alterations of loudness
American Institute of Physics and Gray, 1972), perception separate from auditory pain. For
but in patients with DST, the level can be sub- simplicity, I will use the term “pain hyper­
stantially reduced. For obvious reasons, there is acusis” (Tyler et al., 2014) for the symptom of a
no systematic data in the literature on the au- decreased threshold for experiencing pain from
ditory pain threshold in hyperacusis patients, sound, and the term “loudness hyperacusis”
although in a study of 100 patients with sound for increased loudness perception. Please bear
sensitivity problems, 88 reported experienc- in mind that these two symptoms are not mu-
ing pain from sound (Anari et al., 1999). In tually exclusive, and that patients with DST
a recent questionnaire study on members of frequently suffer from a combination of both.
a hyperacusis Facebook group, we found that
51 out of 58 responders (88%) reported that
they experienced pain from loud sounds ( Jeraj
& Schaette, unpublished). Similarly, a recent A Potential Peripheral
survey conducted by the CORDAS (Coordi- Mechanism for Pain
nation of Rare Diseases at Sanford) Registry
from Sound
showed that 95% of the respondents with
hyperacusis experienced auditory pain at least
occasionally, and 66% continuously or daily One of the most distressing aspects of hyper-
(Pollard, 2017, unpublished), with the ear acusis is the experience of pain from sound at
pain triggered by loud sounds. The experience levels that do not cause a pain sensation in
of pain from sound might thus be a major or normal-hearing subjects. The pain threshold for
even the defining component of DST. normal-hearing listeners is stated to be between
The main features of DST are therefore 120 and 140 dB SPL (Plutchik, 1963; Amer-
changes in the perception of the loudness of ican Institute of Physics and Gray, 1972). The
sounds, in that sounds are perceived as too loud auditory pain threshold is thus in the intensity
despite being judged as tolerable by normal- range at which even relatively brief exposures
hearing listeners, and the experience of pain can cause cochlear damage leading to perma-
from sound at sound intensities that normally nent hearing loss. Such sound levels would
5. Peripheral Mechanisms of Decreased Sound Tolerance 63

thus be well suited to trigger a behavioral re- fiber generate an action potential (Weisz et
sponse intended to prevent further damage to al., 2009, 2012), which might only happen
the sensitive structures of the inner ear, thereby for sounds at very high sound intensities.
supporting a role for auditory pain as a signal A recent demonstration by Liu and
for risk of tissue damage. Up until relatively co-workers (2015) indicated that type II ANFs
recently, however, it was unknown which cells can sense tissue damage in the cochlea. In an
and fibers detected tissue damage in the ear and in-vitro preparation, they obtained patch-
created a pain signal. In 2015, two studies were clamp recordings from type II fibers in excised
published that provided evidence for a role of cochleas. When they mechanically destroyed
the type II auditory nerve fibers (ANFs), first an OHC that had a synaptic contact with the
as detectors of tissue damage in the ear (Liu fiber from which they were recording, action
et al., 2015), and second as a pathway to con- potentials were observed in response. On the
vey a damage signal to the brain (Flores et al., other hand, destruction of adjacent, but un-
2015). Based on these reports, the type II fi- connected, OHCs did not activate the fiber.
bers would be prime candidates for a role as The type II fibers thus do sense and report
pain fibers of the ear. to higher centers damage to OHCs in a very
There are two types of ANFs, type I and specific fashion; this action makes them prime
type II fibers. Type I fibers constitute about candidates for serving as the pain sensors or
90 to 95% of the auditory nerve fibers, and pain fibers of the ear. Interestingly, the damage
are often colloquially referred to simply as response of the type II fibers could be com-
auditory nerve fibers. They are thick and mye­ pletely blocked through the bath application
linated, and synapse onto the inner hair cells of retigabine, suggesting that a “painkiller for
(IHCs). Each type I fiber contacts only a the ear” might be a possibility.
single IHC, and each IHC is contacted by Direct mechanical destruction of OHCs
around 10 to 15 fibers (Ryugo, 1992). The might not correspond to the situation that is
type I fibers are activated by sound and they created inside the inner ear in vivo by loud
are essential for hearing, as they convey the in- sounds. Flores and colleagues (2015) demon-
formation about the auditory signal received strated that the type II fiber pathway could
by the ear to the brain. Type II fibers, in con- also be activated by acoustic stimuli deliv-
trast, are less numerous, as only 5 to 10% of ered at intensities high enough to cause tis-
the AN fiber population are type II. These sue damage in the inner ear. To differentiate
fibers are thin and unmyelinated, and they activation of the type II fiber pathway from
contact the outer hair cells (OHCs) of the sound-evoked neural activity of the type I fi-
cochlea. Each type II fiber contacts approx­ ber pathways, they used mice that had been
imately 7 OHCs (Weisz et al., 2012). Type II genetically modified to render the type I fi-
ANFs receive glutamatergic synaptic exci- bers inactive. In these mice, sounds presented
tation from OHCs, but the synapses between at 120 dB SPL (which caused major damage
OHCs and type II fibers are weak (Weisz et to the OHCs) activated cells in those regions
al., 2009); the lesser magnitude of synaptic of the cochlear nucleus that receive projec-
drive received by the type II fibers indicates tions from type II fibers. In contrast, a lower
that they play a fundamentally different role sound intensity of 80 dB SPL, which did not
in auditory signaling from that of type I affer- damage OHCs, did not generate neural ac-
ents. It has been estimated that many of the tivity in the cochlear nucleus. This study thus
presynaptic OHCs would need to release a demonstrated that damaging sound levels
neurotransmitter in order to make a type II activated the type II fiber pathway, and that
64 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

this pathway conveyed a signal about the and magnitude of sound-evoked vibration
OHC damage to the cochlear nucleus, the first along the basilar membrane (BM). Predic-
central processing stage of the auditory sys­ tions about the loudness of a given sound are
tem. How this signal is processed further, and then derived from the cochlear excitation pat-
which other brain regions in the ascending tern. Narrow-band sounds at low intensities
auditory pathway are involved in this process, only excite a relatively small, restricted patch
remains to be determined. of the BM. Increasing sound intensity results
This discovery of an auditory pain path- not only in a larger BM vibration amplitude,
way is a major breakthrough for research on but also in a spread of excitation to neighbor-
pain hyperacusis, as it provides a target for re- ing regions of the cochlea. It is especially im-
search and therapies. However, the mechanism portant to note that this spread of excitation
responsible for the activation of this pathway is asymmetric, spreading more in the basal
at much lower sound intensities, as would be direction to basilar membrane locations with
required to generate the symptoms observed higher characteristic frequencies, and much
in patients with pain hyperacusis, remains to less in the apical direction to BM locations
be determined. One possible scenario could with lower characteristic frequencies (illus-
involve cross-talk between type I and type II trated for ANF activity in Figure 5–1A). With
ANFs, which might happen, for example, the use of an appropriate basilar membrane
when the myelin sheath around type I ANFs model, the area under the cochlear excitation
is damaged, as has been observed after acoustic pattern is directly proportional to the per-
trauma (Tagoe et al., 2014) and cisplatin ad- ceived loudness measured on the sone scale
ministration (van Ruijven et al., 2004). (Chen et al., 2011). Moreover, when a more
detailed physiological model of cochlea and
auditory nerve is used, it can also be demon-
strated that loudness is directly proportional
Loudness Perception
to the overall activity of type I ANFs (Fig­
ure 5–1B) (Diehl & Schaette, 2015). The model
Before we can start investigating potential simulations also show that the spread of ex-
peripheral mechanisms of loudness hyper­ citation at high sound intensities can be so
acusis, we first must establish what is currently extreme that even low-frequency sounds (e.g.,
known about the relation between peripheral <500 Hz) can excite the cochlea up to the basal
auditory processing and normal loudness per- end when presented at sufficient volume,
ception. Our understanding of the relation and thus provide excitation to the “high-
between the intensity of auditory stimuli and frequency channels” of auditory processing.
the sensation of loudness caused by them is For the assessment of potential peripheral
based on theoretical models. The standard mechanisms of loudness hyperacusis, the most
models of loudness can account for all major important conclusion from the existing mod-
aspects of normal loudness perception, even els is that perceived loudness is determined to
though they are entirely based on the outer a first approximation by the magnitude of the
and inner ear, and do not take central audi- auditory nerve signal. It is currently unknown
tory processing in the brain into account. where in the brain this signal is then evaluated
The most commonly used loudness mod- to generate the loudness percept. Potentially,
els are based on cochlear filter bank models the site of loudness perception is located at a
used to simulate the cochlear excitation pat- relatively high level of auditory processing, at
tern (Moore, 2014), that is, the distribution which signals from both ears are pooled and
5. Peripheral Mechanisms of Decreased Sound Tolerance 65

evaluated together (Munro et al., 2014). To


cause the perception of increased loudness and
steeper loudness growth, there are then two
possibilities. First, through some pathological
change, the output of the cochlea, that is, the
summed activity of all type I ANFs, might be
increased and thus provide a “louder” signal
to the brain. Second, there might be changes
in the central auditory system, either patho-
logical amplification—for example, through
increased excitation or disinhibition—of the
signal in the first stages of auditory processing
before it reaches the brain area that evaluates
loudness, or changes in the “loudness evalua-
tion circuit” itself could result in an auditory
nerve signal of low magnitude being evaluated
as too loud. The first scenario would consti-
tute a direct peripheral effect, whereas the lat-
ter scenario represents changes in the central
auditory system that might nevertheless be
triggered by a peripheral event such as hearing
loss. Both scenarios will be investigated in the
following paragraphs.

Figure 5–1. Illustration of a loudness model


based on simulations of basilar membrane and
auditory nerve responses. A. Simulated activ-
ity pattern of a population of ANFs with char- Review of Potential
acteristic frequencies from 40 Hz to 13 kHz in Peripheral Mechanisms for
response to brief 1-kHz tones from 0 to 90 dB Loudness Hyperacusis
SPL. The simulations were obtained using a
modified version of the MAP model (Meddis et
al., 2013). At high sound intensities, the spread A peripheral mechanism for loudness hyper-
of excitation is very pronounced, as the 1-kHz acusis would need to influence cochlear pro-
tones also strongly excite ANFs with the high- cessing such that the overall activity of the
est characteristic frequencies. B. Relation be- type I ANF population would be increased.
tween the summed activity of all model ANFs There are two putative scenarios for how the
(summed excitation) and the perceived loud- output of the cochlea could be pathologically
ness in sones according to ANSI S3.4-2007. increased. The increase could either be di-
The dashed line shows a fitted power-law func-
rectly caused as a side-effect of cochlear dam-
tion with a slope of 1.1, illustrating that per-
age, or it might be due to changes in the ef-
ceived loudness is proportional to the overall
sound-evoked activity in the auditory nerve. ferent feedback that the cochlea receives. The
To view this image in full color visit the Plural following offers a review of the evidence for
Publishing website for the book at http://plural and against both scenarios. Please note that
publishing.com/publication_hdsi.htm. for simplicity, “ANF” always means “type I
ANF” in the following.
66 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

Zwislocki, 1980). Neither spontaneous nor


Effect of Different Types of maximum discharge rates of ANFs, however, are
Cochlear Damage on the altered by OHC loss (Dallos & Harris, 1978;
Schmiedt & Zwislocki, 1980). After OHC loss,
Activity of Type I ANFs
the activity of ANFs may thus grow more rap-
idly with signal level for suprathreshold sounds,
Damage to the cochlea can be caused by a va- but their activity will not increase beyond rates
riety of factors, with noise-induced and age- normally produced by high-level sounds.
related damage probably the most common. The Both stereocilia damage and OHC loss
main effects of noise exposure on the inner ear can thus potentially lead to a steeper growth
are damage to the stereocilia of cochlear hair of perceived loudness for sounds that are
cells (Liberman & Dodds, 1984a; Liberman above the increased hearing threshold. When
& Kiang, 1984; Kaltenbach et al., 1992), loss considered with impaired hearing thresholds,
of synaptic ribbons (synaptopathy) (Kujawa & the OHC loss reduces the dynamic range, al-
Liberman, 2009; Liberman et al., 2015; Song though the output of the cochlea would not
et al., 2016), degeneration of ANFs (neuropa- exceed normal levels at high sound intensities
thy) (Kujawa & Liberman, 2009), and loss of (Figure 5–2). This “catch-up-behavior” would
outer as well as inner hair cells (Liberman & be consistent with recruitment (Cox et al.,
Dodds, 1984a; Kaltenbach et al., 1992; Chen 1997), but not with loudness hyperacusis.
& Fechter, 2003). Aging also damages hair cell While hyperacusis patients may present
stereocilia, triggers apoptosis of cochlear hair with all degrees of hearing loss, about a third
cells (Soucek et al., 1986; Francis et al., 2003), have hearing thresholds within the normal
and produces degeneration of ANFs (Makary range (Anari et al., 1999; Sheldrake et al., 2015).
et al., 2011; Sergeyenko et al., 2013), as well However, a normal audiogram does not neces-
as atrophy of the stria vascularis (Gates & sarily indicate the absence of cochlear damage;
Mills, 2005). a substantial degree of cochlear synaptopathy
Damage to hair cell stereocilia affects ac- (Kujawa and Liberman, 2009), neuropathy
tivity of ANFs primarily by increasing their (Makary et al., 2011), or even loss of IHCs (Lo-
response thresholds (Liberman, 1984; Liber- barinas et al., 2013) may co-occur with normal
man & Dodds, 1984a). Moreover, while the hearing thresholds. Interestingly, a recent study
spontaneous discharge rates of ANFs may be in mice has indicated that synaptopathy could
reduced as a consequence of stereocilia damage lead to behavioral signs consistent with loudness
(Liberman & Dodds, 1984b), the maximum hyperacusis (Hickox & Liberman, 2014).
firing rates evoked by high-intensity sounds re­ Loss of inner hair cells through noise- or
portedly remain comparable to those from con­ age-related damage will remove the input sig-
trol ANFs (Liberman & Kiang, 1984). nal from all type I ANFs that contact these hair
The main function of the OHCs is to cells, thereby rendering them unresponsive to
provide amplification of basilar membrane mo­ sound. Therefore, if IHC loss is scattered along
tion at low sound intensities, and compres­ the length of the cochlea, then it will lead to a
sion at medium to high levels. Loss of OHCs reduction of the overall auditory nerve signal in
thus increases the response thresholds of ANFs proportion to the degree of IHC loss (Schaette
(Dallos & Harris, 1978), and the loss of com- & Kempter, 2006). In a chinchilla model, se-
pression might be reflected in a steepening of lective loss of IHCs through carboplatin ad-
the rate-level functions of ANFs (Schmiedt & ministration has been shown to be scattered
5. Peripheral Mechanisms of Decreased Sound Tolerance 67

Figure 5–2. Simulation of the effects of different types of damage to or loss


of cochlear hair cells on ANF responses. (Top row) Generic audiograms for
noise-induced cochlear synaptopathy (hidden hearing loss), noise-induced
hearing loss (NIHL), and outer hair cell (OHC) loss. (Middle row) Schematic
picture of an IHC with type I ANFs, to illustrate the degree of cochlear synap-
topathy, which renders the affected fibers unresponsive to sound (gray fibers).
(Bottom row) Simulated AN population responses for AN fibers with character-
istic frequencies of 4 to 8 kHz (indicated by shaded area in the top row). The
MAP model (Meddis et al., 2013) has been used to simulate AN responses to
short bursts of broadband noise for an AN fiber population comprising low-,
medium-, and high-threshold AN fibers, with the extent of hearing loss corre-
sponding to the audiograms in the top row. For NIHL, a combination of stereo-
cilia damage, synaptopathy, and hair cell loss was assumed. The simulations
demonstrate that the different kinds of damage to cochlear hair cells all reduce
overall AN responses, indicating that damage to cochlear hair cells is unlikely
to produce a neural correlate of loudness hyperacusis at the level of the AN. To
view this image in full color visit the Plural Publishing website for the book at http://
pluralpublishing.com/publication_hdsi.htm.

along the cochlea and to reduce the compound and might make certain frequency ranges com-
action potential of the auditory nerve in pro- pletely inaudible. As for scattered IHC loss, the
portion to the degree of hair cell loss (Wang subsequent cochlear dead regions will reduce
et al., 1997). Complete loss of IHCs along a auditory nerve responses to sounds when such
stretch of the basilar membrane creates what is responses are compared to the output from a
called a cochlear dead region (Moore, 2004), healthy cochlea. The magnitude of this effect
68 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

will differ considerably for sounds within and (Anari et al., 1999; Sheldrake et al., 2015),
outside the dead region. nor a relation between the pattern or degree of
Noise- and age-related damage can cause hearing loss and the degree of LDL reduction
cochlear synaptopathy, that is, a permanent or the shape of the LDL curve (Sheldrake
loss of synaptic ribbons at the synapse between et al., 2015).
inner hair cells and type I ANFs (Kujawa &
Liberman, 2009; Sergeyenko et al., 2013). In
mice, more than 50% loss of synaptic ribbons
has been observed in the high-frequency parts Failure of Efferent Feedback
of the cochlea after noise exposures that only Could Potentially Increase ANF
caused a temporary increase of the hearing
Responses to Loud Sounds
thresholds (Kujawa & Liberman, 2009). This
loss of an essential part of the presynaptic ma-
chinery of the synapse has a strong effect on the Both the middle ear and the inner ear receive
responses of type I ANFs to sound, which can efferent innervation. One of the functions of
be observed in a proportional reduction in the the efferent feedback to the auditory periphery
amplitude of wave I of the auditory brainstem is to regulate the responses of the inner ear to
response (Kujawa & Liberman, 2009; Furman loud sounds. Feedback to the middle ear trig-
et al., 2013; Hesse et al., 2016), indicating gers the acoustic reflex, which reduces the ef-
that the affected auditory nerve fibers become ficiency of energy transfer to the cochlea. The
less responsive to sound, if they respond at all. inner ear receives efferent feedback through
Over time, synaptopathy is followed by neu- two distinct pathways, the medial and the
ropathy, that is, degeneration of type I ANFs lateral olivocochlear bundle. The fibers of the
(Kujawa & Liberman, 2009; Makary et al., medial olivocochlear (MOC) bundle project
2011; Sergeyenko et al., 2013; Fernandez et onto the OHCs, and the fibers of the lateral
al., 2015), which might further reduce the olivocochlear (LOC) bundle contact type I
number of ANFs that respond to sound. The ANFs just behind the synaptic contact be-
overall effect of cochlear synaptopathy and tween type I fibers and IHCs. MOC feedback
neuropathy will therefore be a reduction of to OHCs reduces the effect of the cochlear
the magnitude of the signal transmitted by the amplifier (Guinan, 2006). Moreover, efferent
auditory nerve to the brain (Figure 5–2). Syn- feedback has a protective effect on the inner
aptopathy and neuropathy thus generate the ear (Lendvai et al., 2011; Maison et al., 2013;
opposite form of peripheral change required Liberman et al., 2014), and it has been shown
to create a correlate of loudness hyperacusis at that efferent innervation of the ear may un-
the level of the auditory nerve. dergo plastic remodeling with aging and after
In summary, one can conclude that the noise trauma (Nouvian et al., 2015). It is thus
overall effect for noise-induced or age-related conceivable that a disruption of efferent feed-
damage to cochlear hair cells is a reduction back could contribute to hyperacusis, because
of AN activity, and therefore they are highly malfunction of the efferent system might
unlikely to produce pathological increases in lead to abnormal auditory nerve responses to
the activity of type I ANFs that would be re- a wide range of sound levels, potentially de-
quired as a peripheral mechanism for loud­ creasing sound tolerance.
ness hyperacusis. This is matched by audio- The acoustic or so-called middle ear re-
metric findings that reveal neither a “typical” flex is a brainstem reflex that involves the bi-
shape of the audiogram in hyperacusis patients lateral contraction of the middle ear muscles
5. Peripheral Mechanisms of Decreased Sound Tolerance 69

in response to a high sound level presented improve the symptoms of Meniere’s disease.
to either ear (Borg, 1973). The contraction Thus far, the effects of this intervention have
of the stapedius muscle in the middle ear only been investigated for tinnitus, where it
stiffens the ossicular chain, thereby reducing did not have a consistent effect (Baguley et al.,
the transmission of primarily low-frequency 2002). Whether the vestibular nerve section
sound energy to the inner ear by approxi- alters loudness perception and hyperacusis re-
mately 15 dB. The threshold for eliciting the mains to be investigated.
acoustic reflex is usually between 70 and 100 dB The LOC feedback system utilizes a vari-
HL, depending on stimulus frequency and ety of neurotransmitters, especially acetylcho-
bandwidth (Munro et al., 2014). A reduction line, GABA, and dopamine (Elgoyhen, 2010),
of the strength of the acoustic reflex, or even suggesting a modulatory or even inhibitory
a complete absence, could thus increase the function. One of the potential functions of the
stimulus intensity received by the inner ear, LOC feedback system is to fine-tune auditory
theoretically resulting in a higher cochlear nerve responses to balance interaural sensitivity
output that could contribute to DST. How- (Darrow et al., 2006). After removal or block-
ever, the ARTs of hyperacusis patients have ing of LOC feedback to the cochlea, both in-
been reported to be in the normal range creases (Darrow et al., 2007) and decreases (Le
(Brandy & Lynn, 1995; Anari et al., 1999), Prell et al., 2005) of auditory nerve responses,
and it is thus unlikely that malfunction of the quantified through measurements of ABR
acoustic reflex contributes to hyperacusis. wave I magnitude, have been observed. It is
The main effect of MOC feedback to the thus unclear whether malfunction of the LOC
inner ear is to reduce the amplification pro- feedback would increase or decrease sound
vided by the OHCs (Cooper & Guinan, 2006; sensitivity.
Guinan, 2006), which shifts the response func- Complete removal of both MOC and
tions of type I ANFs to higher sound intensities LOC feedback to the cochlea has been inves-
(Guinan & Gifford, 1988). MOC feedback is tigated in a rat model. Following chronic co-
elicited by sound in a level-dependent fashion chlear de-efferentation produced by section-
(Smalt et al., 2014), with time constants on the ing the olivocochlear bundle, type I ANFs
order of hundreds of milliseconds (Backus & showed increased sound-evoked activity and
Guinan, 2006). While a malfunction of this decreased dynamic range of their rate-level func-
mechanism could in principle increase type I tions, more pronounced responses to sound
ANF responses to loud sounds, the effect onsets, and decreased thresholds in the tails
might actually be most pronounced at moder- of their tuning curves (Zheng et al., 1999).
ate sound intensities, in the compressive range These effects were most pronounced in ANFs
of the OHC contribution to the basilar mem- with high characteristic frequencies (2 to 8
brane response. At very high sound intensities, kHz), and would be consistent with loudness
the basilar membrane response becomes lin- hyperacusis.
ear again and is not governed by the OHCs, In conclusion, changes in efferent feed-
and thus the effects of MOC feedback (and back to the cochlea could enhance auditory
lack thereof    ) might be limited in this inten- nerve responses to loud sounds, which could
sity range (compare also the depiction of the make loud environments uncomfortable and
effects of OHC loss on ANF activity in Fig­ would be consistent with loudness hyper­
ure 5–2). In human patients, removal of MOC acusis. Whether efferent malfunctions are ac­
feedback to the cochlea can occur, for exam- tually present in patients with decreased sound
ple, when the vestibular nerve is sectioned to tolerance remains to be determined.
70 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

by a generalized, frequency-independent in-


Indications for Peripheral crease in nonlinear gain in the central audi-
Processes as a Trigger for tory system. In the model, gain was applied
Central Changes that Might to suprathreshold responses only, that is, af­
Underlie Loudness Hyperacusis ter subtracting the spontaneous activity of the
auditory nerve, thereby differentiating “hyper­
acusis gain” from gain mechanisms postulated to
Thus far, I have only considered how changes underlie the development of tinnitus (Schaette
in the auditory periphery could directly cre- & Kempter, 2006). Considering the research
ate decreased sound tolerance (DST) and pain findings summarized above, these modeling
from sound. However, when we consider pe- studies, and any theories about an increase in
ripheral mechanisms of sound sensitivity, we neuronal gain in the central auditory system as
also must consider the case in which DST a mechanism of hyperacusis, should probably
might be triggered through a peripheral event, be considered as models for loudness hyper­
for example, cochlear damage, even when the acusis only.
pathological changes that underlie the sound Peripheral damage or hearing loss might
sensitivity problems occur in the central au- trigger changes in the central auditory system
ditory system. It is now well established that related to loudness hyperacusis. Many studies
cochlear damage can trigger neuronal plasticity investigated the effects of simulating a con-
in the central auditory system (Roberts et al., ductive hearing loss on the auditory system
2010), and it has been proposed that neuro- by letting normal-hearing volunteers wear ear-
plastic changes might underlie the develop- plugs for a few days. This relatively mild form
ment of hyperacusis (Auerbach et al., 2014). of auditory deprivation triggered a variety of
A recent neuroimaging study reported in- changes in the central auditory system in the
creases in sound-evoked neuronal responses in absence of any peripheral damage. First, wear-
the auditory midbrain, thalamus, and cortex of ing an earplug for several days in one or both
subjects with DST (Gu et al., 2010). As fMRI, ears increased perceived loudness (Formby et
the imaging technique used in this study, could al., 2003; Munro et al., 2014). Changes in
not measure auditory nerve activity, it has re- perceived loudness developed over days, and
mained unclear whether the enhanced acti- reverted back to pre-occlusion ratings within
vation seen throughout the central auditory 24 hours after earplug removal (Munro et al.,
system might be generated by increased input 2014). Interestingly, when only one ear was
from the auditory nerve, or through amplifica- plugged, perceived loudness was increased in
tion of the auditory nerve signal along the au- both ears (Munro et al., 2014), indicating that
ditory pathway through pathological changes the changes in loudness might be due to plas-
within the central auditory system. A theoreti- ticity at a relatively high level of the auditory
cal study postulated that hyperacusis might be processing chain, where input from both ears
due to an abnormal increase in neuronal gain in is integrated and evaluated. In parallel with
the central auditory system (Zeng, 2013). Spe- changes in perceived loudness, changes in
cifically, it was hypothesized that hyperacusis acous­tic reflex thresholds (elicited via a brain-
might be due to an increase in a nonlinear gain stem reflex arc) were observed, but here the
mechanism. This hypothesis was investigated changes went in opposite directions in the
in more detail by Diehl and Schaette (2015), plugged and the open ear (Munro & Blount,
who concluded that the decreased LDLs of 2009; Munro et al., 2014). Plasticity of loud-
hyperacusis patients were best accounted for ness works in both directions, as additional
5. Peripheral Mechanisms of Decreased Sound Tolerance 71

stimulation through noise generators (Formby temporary conductive hearing loss caused
et al., 2003) or low-gain hearing aids (Munro lasting changes in the brain, with increased
& Merrett, 2013) decreased perceived loud- neuronal gain upstream of the cochlear nu-
ness when measured in the same manner as cleus and synaptic changes in the cochlear nu-
in the plugged ears. These studies indicated cleus (Clarkson et al., 2016). The gain increase
that the auditory system adapted to changes would be consistent with hypotheses on loud-
in the input it received from the periphery, ness hyperacusis. However, the interpretation of
and that loudness perception was modulated animal results remains hampered by the lack of
consequently. The observed changes in loud- an animal model of hyperacusis. Many studies
ness and acoustic reflex threshold were inter- have relied on measuring the amplitude of the
preted as changes in neuronal gain in the au- acoustic startle reflex, with increases in amplitude
ditory system, and it was proposed that such interpreted as a sign of hyperacusis. However, a
gain changes might underlie abnormal loud- recent study in humans showed that while ele-
ness perception in hyperacusis (Brotherton vated startle responses were related to reductions
et al., 2015). LDL measurements in hyper­ in LDLs, they did not correlate with the patients’
acusis patients generally show a flat, frequency- self-reports of hyper­acusis, as assessed via ques-
independent pattern of LDL decreases (Anari tionnaires (Knudson and Melcher, 2016).
et al., 1999; Sheldrake et al., 2015). Theoretical
modeling showed that similar decreases in
LDLs could be generated by gain mechanisms
Conclusions
operating in a frequency-specific manner or
across frequencies (Diehl & Schaette, 2015). It
remains unclear whether plasticity of loudness Recent results have delivered compelling evi-
might occur in a frequency-specific manner, dence that type II ANFs might act as the pain
for example, driven by the amount of input sensors of the ear (Flores et al., 2015; Liu
deprivation or additional stimulation at each et al., 2015). While we do not yet understand
frequency, or whether it occurs in an unspecific why the threshold for experiencing pain from
manner through a generalized increase in sen- sound is decreased in hyperacusis patients,
sitivity that affects all frequencies. investigations of pathological changes in this
In the earplug experiments, adult volun- system at a peripheral level would be a logi-
teers wore the ear plugs for a maximum du- cal starting point to uncover the mechanisms.
ration of 14 days (Formby et al., 2003), and For loudness hyperacusis, the evaluation of
changes in loudness perception were fully re- potential peripheral mechanisms indicated
versible within hours to days after earplug re- that malfunction of efferent feedback might
moval. However, animal studies provided indi- produce changes to the activity of type I
cations that periods of conductive hearing loss ANFs that would be consistent with decreased
may have long-lasting effects, especially when LDLs and increased loudness growth. How-
they occur early in life. When conductive hear- ever, if we consider that many patients with
ing loss was induced by damaging the tym- DST experience pain from sound, a change in
panic membrane in young rats, the animals the evaluation of sounds in the central audi-
showed enhanced acoustic startle responses, tory system must also be considered. Offend-
and even an increased susceptibility to au- ing sound exposures could modify a patient’s
diogenic seizures. These changes persisted for perception of loudness as a secondary effect
months even after the hearing loss recovered of the experience of pain from sound, thereby
(Sun et al., 2011). It was also demonstrated that initiating changes in behavior. We have seen
72 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

that reducing the input signal to the central Andersson, G., Lindvall, N., Hursti, T., & Carl-
auditory system through wearing an earplug bring, P. (2002). Hypersensitivity to sound (hy-
can increases loudness perception after the peracusis): A prevalence study conducted via the
plug is removed (Formby et al., 2003; Munro Internet and post. Int. J. Audiol., 41, 545–554.
et al., 2014), and sound avoidance out of fear Auerbach, B. D., Rodrigues, P. V., & Salvi, R. J.
(2014). Central gain control in tinnitus and
of pain might have a similar effect. A treat-
hyperacusis. Front. Neurol., 5, 206.
ment that reduces the likelihood of experienc- Backus, B. C., & Guinan, J. J., Jr. (2006). Time-
ing pain from sound might thus also help to course of the human medial olivocochlear re-
renormalize loudness perception. flex. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 119, 2889–2904.
In the future, a better understanding Baguley, D. M., Axon, P., Winter, I. M., & Mof-
of the physiology of type II ANFs, especially fat, D. A. (2002). The effect of vestibular nerve
regarding their activation in patients who ex­ section upon tinnitus. Clin. Otolaryngol. Allied
perience sound-induced pain, might facili- Sci., 27, 219–226.
tate development of new treatments for hy­ Borg, E. (1973). On the neuronal organiza­
peracusis. However, a better understanding of tion of the acoustic middle ear reflex. A phys-
the physiology, and the development of new iological and anatomical study. Brain Res., 49,
drugs, requires an animal model of pain hy- 101–123.
Brandy, W. T., & Lynn, J. M. (1995). Audiologic
peracusis, which is unfortunately not available
findings in hyperacusic and nonhyperacusic sub­
yet. Nevertheless, the finding that retigabine jects. Am. J. Audiol., 4, 46–51.
can block the damage response of type II AN Brotherton, H., Plack, C. J., Maslin, M., Schaette,
fibers (Liu et al., 2015) provides a promising R., & Munro, K. J. (2015) Pump up the vol-
start for the development of pharmacological ume: Could excessive neural gain explain tin-
interventions, as this drug is already approved nitus and hyperacusis? Audiol. Neurootol., 20,
for human use. Moreover, if the induction of 273–282.
auditory pain in hyperacusis is at the periph- Chen, G. D., & Fechter, L. D. (2003). The re-
ery, then the side-effects of retigabine (and lationship between noise-induced hearing loss
those of any other potential new drug) could and hair cell loss in rats. Hear Res., 177, 81–90.
be minimized if delivered locally through sys- Chen, Z., Hu, G., Glasberg, B. R., & Moore, B. C.
tems for drug administration to the inner ear, (2011). A new method of calculating auditory
excitation patterns and loudness for steady
and we might thus see some progress toward
sounds. Hear Res., 282, 204–215.
clinical trials in the not too distant future. Clarkson, C., Antunes, F. M., & Rubio, M. E.
(2016). Conductive hearing loss has long-lasting
structural and molecular effects on presynaptic
References and postsynaptic structures of auditory nerve
synapses in the cochlear nucleus. J. Neurosci., 36,
10214–10227.
American Institute of Physics & Gray, D. E. Cooper, N. P., & Guinan, J. J., Jr. (2006). Efferent-
(1972). American Institute of Physics Handbook mediated control of basilar membrane motion.
(3rd ed.). Section editors: B. H. Billings [and J. Physiol., 576, 49–54.
others]. Coordinating editor: D. E. Gray. New Cox, R. M., Alexander, G. C., Taylor, I. M., &
York, NY: McGraw-Hill. Gray, G. A. (1997). The contour test of loud-
Anari, M., Axelsson, A., Eliasson, A., & Mag­ ness perception. Ear Hear., 18, 388–400.
nusson, L. (1999). Hypersensitivity to sound— Dallos, P., & Harris, D. (1978). Properties of au-
questionnaire data, audiometry and classifica- ditory nerve responses in absence of outer hair
tion. Scand. Audiol., 28, 219–230. cells. J. Neurophysiol., 41, 365–383.
5. Peripheral Mechanisms of Decreased Sound Tolerance 73

Darrow, K. N., Maison, S. F., & Liberman, M. C. selective for fibers with low spontaneous rates.
(2006). Cochlear efferent feedback balances in- J. Neurophysiol., 110, 577–586.
teraural sensitivity. Nat. Neurosci., 9, 1474–1476. Gates, G. A., & Mills, J. H. (2005). Presbycusis.
Darrow, K. N., Maison, S. F., & Liberman, M. C. Lancet, 366, 1111–1120.
(2007). Selective removal of lateral olivocochlear Gu, J. W., Halpin, C. F., Nam, E. C., Levine, R. A.,
efferents increases vulnerability to acute acoustic & Melcher, J. R. (2010). Tinnitus, diminished
injury. J. Neurophysiol., 97, 1775–1785. sound-level tolerance, and elevated auditory ac-
Diehl, P. U., & Schaette, R. (2015). Abnormal au- tivity in humans with clinically normal hearing
ditory gain in hyperacusis: Investigation with a sensitivity. J. Neurophysiol., 104, 3361–3370.
computational model. Front. Neurol., 6, 157. Guinan, J. J., Jr. (2006). Olivocochlear efferents:
Elgoyhen, A. B. F. P. A. (2010). Efferent innerva- Anatomy, physiology, function, and the mea-
tion and function. In: Fuchs, P. A. (Ed.), Oxford surement of efferent effects in humans. Ear
Handbook of Auditory Science: The Ear (pp. 283– Hear., 27, 589–607.
306). Oxford, U.K.: Oxford University Press. Guinan, J. J., Jr., & Gifford, M. L. (1988). Ef-
Fabijanska, A., Rogowski, M., Bartnik, G., & fects of electrical stimulation of efferent olivo-
Skarzynski, H. (1999). Epidemiology of tinni- cochlear neurons on cat auditory-nerve fibers.
tus and hyperacusis in Poland. In: Sixth Interna- I. Rate-level functions. Hear Res., 33, 97–113.
tional Tinnitus Seminar (pp. 569–571). London, Hesse, L. L., Bakay, W. H., Anderson, L., Ong,
U.K.: The Tinnitus and Hyperacusis Centre. H.- C., Ashmore, J., McAlpine, D., Linden, J. F.,
Fernandez, K. A., Jeffers, P. W., Lall, K., Liberman, & Schaette, R. (2016). Non-monotonic relation
M. C., & Kujawa, S. G. (2015). Aging after between noise exposure severity and neuronal
noise exposure: Acceleration of cochlear syn- hyperactivity in the auditory midbrain. Front.
aptopathy in “recovered” ears. J. Neurosci., 35, Neurol., 7.
7509–7520. Hickox, A. E., & Liberman, M. C. (2014). Is
Flores, E. N., Duggan, A., Madathany, T., Hogan, noise-induced cochlear neuropathy key to the
A. K., Marquez, F. G., Kumar, G., Seal, R. P., generation of hyperacusis or tinnitus? J. Neuro-
Edwards, R. H., Liberman, M. C., & Garcia- physiol., 111, 552–564.
Anoveros, J. (2015) A non-canonical pathway Kaltenbach, J. A., Schmidt, R. N., & Kaplan, C. R.
from cochlea to brain signals tissue-damaging (1992). Tone-induced stereocilia lesions as a
noise. Curr. Biol., 25, 606–612. function of exposure level and duration in the
Formby, C., Sherlock, L. P., & Gold, S. L. (2003). hamster cochlea. Hear. Res., 60, 205–215.
Adaptive plasticity of loudness induced by Knudson, I. M., & Melcher, J. R. (2016). Elevated
chronic attenuation and enhancement of the acoustic startle responses in humans: Relation-
acoustic background. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 114, ship to reduced loudness discomfort level, but
55–58. not self-report of hyperacusis. J. Assoc. Res. Oto-
Formby, C., Gold, S. L., Keaser, M. L., Block, laryngol., 17, 223–235.
K. L., & Hawley, M. L. (2007). Secondary bene­ Kujawa, S. G., & Liberman, M. C. (2009). Adding
fits from tinnitus retraining therapy: Clinically insult to injury: Cochlear nerve degeneration
significant increases in loudness discomfort level after “temporary” noise-induced hearing loss.
and expansion of the auditory dynamic range. J. Neurosci., 29, 14077–14085.
Seminars in Hearing, 28, 227–260. Le Prell, C. G., Halsey, K., Hughes, L. F., Dolan,
Francis, H. W., Ryugo, D. K., Gorelikow, M. J., D. F., & Bledsoe, S. C., Jr. (2005). Disruption
Prosen, C. A., & May, B. J. (2003). The functional of lateral olivocochlear neurons via a dopaminer-
age of hearing loss in a mouse model of pres- gic neurotoxin depresses sound-evoked auditory
bycusis. II. Neuroanatomical correlates. Hear. nerve activity. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol., 6, 48–62.
Res., 183, 29–36. Lendvai, B., Halmos, G. B., Polony, G., Kapocsi,
Furman, A. C., Kujawa, S. G., & Liberman, M. C. J., Horvath, T., Aller, M., Sylvester Vizi, E., &
(2013). Noise-induced cochlear neuropathy is Zelles, T. (2011). Chemical neuroprotection in
74 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

the cochlea: The modulation of dopamine re- Moore, B. C. (2004). Dead regions in the cochlea:
lease from lateral olivocochlear efferents. Neu- conceptual foundations, diagnosis, and clinical
rochem. Int., 59, 150–158. applications. Ear Hear., 25, 98–116.
Liberman, L. D., Suzuki, J., & Liberman, M. C. Munro, K. J., & Blount, J. (2009). Adaptive plas-
(2015). Dynamics of cochlear synaptopathy ticity in brainstem of adult listeners following
after acoustic overexposure. J. Assoc. Res. Oto- earplug-induced deprivation. J. Acoust. Soc. Am.,
laryngol., 16, 205–219. 126, 568–571.
Liberman, M. C. (1984). Single-neuron labeling Munro, K. J., Merrett, J. F. (2013). Brainstem plas-
and chronic cochlear pathology. I. Threshold ticity and modified loudness following short-
shift and characteristic-frequency shift. Hear. term use of hearing aids. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 133,
Res., 16, 33–41. 343–349.
Liberman, M. C., & Dodds, L. W. (1984a). Single- Munro, K. J., Turtle, C., & Schaette, R. (2014).
neuron labeling and chronic cochlear pathology. Sub-cortical plasticity and modified loudness fol-
III. Stereocilia damage and alterations of thresh­ lowing short-term unilateral deprivation: evidence
old tuning curves. Hear. Res., 16, 55–74. of multiple neural gain mechanisms within the
Liberman, M. C., & Dodds, L. W. (1984b). Single- auditory system. To appear in J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
neuron labeling and chronic cochlear pathology. Norena, A. J., & Chery-Croze, S. (2007). Enriched
II. Stereocilia damage and alterations of spon- acoustic environment rescales auditory sensitiv-
taneous discharge rates. Hear. Res., 16, 43–53. ity. Neuroreport., 18, 1251–1255.
Liberman, M. C., & Kiang, N. Y. (1984). Single- Nouvian, R., Eybalin, M., & Puel, J. L. (2015). Co-
neuron labeling and chronic cochlear pathology. chlear efferents in developing adult and patholog-
IV. Stereocilia damage and alterations in rate- ical conditions. Cell Tissue Res., 361, 301–309.
and phase-level functions. Hear. Res., 16, 75–90. Paulin, J., Andersson, L., & Nordin, S. (2016).
Liberman, M. C., Liberman, L. D., & Maison, Characteristics of hyperacusis in the general
S. F. (2014). Efferent feedback slows cochlear population. Noise Health, 18, 178–184.
aging. J. Neurosci., 34, 4599–4607. Plutchik, R. (1963). Auditory pain thresholds for
Liu, C., Glowatzki, E., & Fuchs, P. A. (2015). intermittent, “beat” and steady signals. Percept.
Unmyelinated type II afferent neurons report Mot. Skills, 16, 863–870.
cochlear damage. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., Roberts, L. E., Eggermont, J. J., Caspary, D. M.,
112, 14723–14727. Shore, S. E., Melcher, J. R., & Kaltenbach, J. A.
Lobarinas, E., Salvi, R., & Ding, D. (2013). In- (2010). Ringing ears: The neuroscience of tinni-
sensitivity of the audiogram to carboplatin in- tus. J. Neurosci., 30, 14972–14979.
duced inner hair cell loss in chinchillas. Hear- Rosing, S. N., Schmidt, J. H., Wedderkopp, N., &
ing Research, 302, 113–120. Baguley, D. M. (2016). Prevalence of tinnitus
Maison, S. F., Usubuchi, H., & Liberman, M. C. and hyperacusis in children and adolescents: a
(2013). Efferent feedback minimizes cochlear systematic review. BMJ Open, 6, e010596.
neuropathy from moderate noise exposure. Ryugo, D. K. (1992). The auditory nerve: Peripheral
J. Neurosci., 33, 5542–5552. innervation, cell body morphology, and central
Makary, C. A., Shin, J., Kujawa, S. G., Liberman, projections. In D. B. Webster, A. N. Popper, &
M. C., & Merchant, S. N. (2011). Age-related R. R. Fay (Eds.), The mammalian auditory pathway,
primary cochlear neuronal degeneration in hu- neuroanatomy (pp. 23–65). New York: Springer.
man temporal bones. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol., Sammeth, C. A., Preves, D. A., & Brandy, W. T.
12, 711–717. (2000). Hyperacusis: Case studies and evaluation
Meddis, R., Lecluyse, W., Clark, N. R., Jurgens, T., of electronic loudness suppression devices as a
Tan, C. M., Panda, M. R., & Brown, G. J. treatment approach. Scand. Audiol., 29, 28–36.
(2013). A computer model of the auditory pe- Schaette, R., & Kempter, R. (2006). Develop-
riphery and its application to the study of hear- ment of tinnitus-related neuronal hyperactivity
ing. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol., 787, 11–20. through homeostatic plasticity after hearing
5. Peripheral Mechanisms of Decreased Sound Tolerance 75

loss: A computational model. Eur. J. Neurosci., Tagoe, T., Barker, M., Jones, A., Allcock, N., &
23, 3124–3138. Hamann, M. (2014). Auditory nerve perinodal
Schmiedt, R. A., & Zwislocki, J. J. (1980). Effects dysmyelination in noise-induced hearing loss.
of hair cell lesions on responses of cochlear J. Neurosci., 34, 2684–2688.
nerve fibers. II. Single- and two-tone intensity Tyler, R. S., Pienkowski, M., Roncancio, E. R., Jun,
functions in relation to tuning curves. J. Neuro- H. J., Brozoski, T., Dauman, N., . . . Moore,
physiol., 43, 1390–1405. B. C. (2014). A review of hyperacusis and future
Sergeyenko, Y., Lall, K., Liberman, M. C., & directions: Part I. Definitions and manifesta-
Kujawa, S. G. (2013). Age-related cochlear tions. Am. J. Audiol., 23, 402–419.
synaptopathy: an early-onset contributor to van Ruijven, M. W., de Groot, J. C., & Smooren-
auditory functional decline. J. Neurosci., 33, burg, G. F. (2004). Time sequence of degener-
13686–13694. ation pattern in the guinea pig cochlea during
Sheldrake, J., Diehl, P. U., & Schaette, R. (2015). cisplatin administration. A quantitative histo-
Audiometric characteristics of hyperacusis pa- logical study. Hear Res., 197, 44–54.
tients. Front. Neurol., 6, 105. Wang, J., Powers, N. L., Hofstetter, P., Trautwein,
Sherlock, L. P., Formby, C. (2005). Estimates of P., Ding, D., & Salvi, R. (1997) Effects of selec­
loudness, loudness discomfort, and the auditory tive inner hair cell loss on auditory nerve fiber
dynamic range: Normative estimates, comparison threshold, tuning and spontaneous and driven
of procedures, and test-retest reliability. J. Am. discharge rate. Hear Res., 107, 67–82.
Acad. Audiol., 16, 85–100. Weisz, C., Glowatzki, E., & Fuchs, P. (2009). The
Smalt, C. J., Heinz, M. G., Strickland, E. A. (2014). postsynaptic function of type II cochlear affer-
Modeling the time-varying and level-dependent ents. Nature, 461, 1126–1129.
effects of the medial olivocochlear reflex in audi- Weisz, C. J., Lehar, M., Hiel, H., Glowatzki, E.,
tory nerve responses. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol., & Fuchs, P. A. (2012). Synaptic transfer from
15, 159–173. outer hair cells to type II afferent fibers in the
Song, Q., Shen, P., Li, X., Shi, L., Liu, L., Wang, rat cochlea. J. Neurosci., 32, 9528–9536.
J., . . . Wang, J. (2016). Coding deficits in hid- Zaugg, T. L., Thielman, E. J., Griest, S., Henry, J. A.
den hearing loss induced by noise: The nature (2016). Subjective reports of trouble tolerating
and impacts. Sci. Rep., 6, 25200. sound in daily life versus loudness discomfort
Soucek, S., Michaels, L., & Frohlich, A. (1986). levels. Am. J. Audiol., 25, 359–363.
Evidence for hair cell degeneration as the pri- Zeng, F. G. (2013). An active loudness model sug-
mary lesion in hearing loss of the elderly. J. Oto- gesting tinnitus as increased central noise and
laryngol., 15, 175–183. hyperacusis as increased nonlinear gain. Hear
Sun, W., Manohar, S., Jayaram, A., Kumaraguru, A., Res., 295, 172–179.
Fu, Q., Li, J., & Allman, B. (2011). Early age Zheng, X. Y., Henderson, D., McFadden, S. L.,
conductive hearing loss causes audiogenic sei- Ding, D. L., Salvi, R. J. (1999). Auditory nerve
zure and hyperacusis behavior. Hear Res., 282, fiber responses following chronic cochlear
178–183. de-efferentation. J. Comp. Neurol., 406, 72–86.
6
Tinnitus and Hyperacusis:
Relationship, Mechanisms,
and Initiating Conditions
Larry E. Roberts, Tanit Ganz Sanchez, and Ian C. Bruce

A university student contacted our laboratory asking whether we might be able to suppress his
tinnitus. He had experienced tinnitus for about 10 years following a sports-related concussion but
it had worsened and was now affecting his studies. His audiogram measured in the laboratory
showed hearing thresholds below 5 dB HL at frequencies to 16 kHz except for a small but notable
notch peaking at 20 dB HL centered at 10 kHz. Tinnitus loudness measured by matching to a
1 kHz sound (where hearing thresholds were 0 dB HL or better) was 50.5 dB SPL. His tinnitus
spectrum stepped up at 3 kHz and plateaued in the range of 6 to 12 kHz, which is typical of
young subjects measured in our laboratory who have clinically normal hearing thresholds. Our
subject also experienced a modest residual inhibition to band-limited noise maskers with center
frequencies in this frequency region. Although he consulted our laboratory for tinnitus, reduced
sound level tolerance also appeared to be a problem. Upon follow-up about a year later his parents
indicated that increased sound sensitivity had led their son to avoid public places, especially
shopping malls which were intolerable and made his tinnitus worse. We were able to contact our
subject 7 years after his initial laboratory session. He reported then that while he still experienced
tinnitus, its intrusiveness and his sensitivity to sound had diminished to the point that quality
of life was no longer affected.

are frequently co-morbid conditions, the na-


Introduction
ture of their relationship is increasingly a topic
of discussion (Nelson & Chen, 2004; Shel-
While tinnitus (chronic ringing of the ears) drake, Diehl, & Schaette, 2015; Pienkow-
and hyperacusis (where ordinary environmen- ski et al., 2014). Population surveys suggest
tal sounds are perceived to be intolerably loud) that tinnitus is the more prevalent condition

77
78 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

affecting approximately 15% of adults (Shar- level dependent (BOLD) responses evoked
gorodsky, Curhan, & Farwell et al., 2010), by a 95-dB noise burst (Gu et al., 2010) and
which is about twice that of hyperacusis (An- with the amplitude of the acoustic startle re-
dersson, Lindvall, Hursti, & Carlbring, 2002). sponse (ASR), an involuntary eye blink that
Consistent with these statistics, it has been increases with physical sound intensity (Knud-
observed that 86% of individuals with hyper- sen & Melcher, 2016). However, while these
acusis report tinnitus but only about 40% of results support the utility of LDL, LGF slope,
tinnitus patients report hyperacusis (Baguley, and ASR as measures of loudness perception,
2003). A limiting factor, however, is that the neither measure uniquely identifies individuals
two phenomena likely have different thresh- seeking treatment for hyperacusis in an otolar-
olds for self-reporting. Chronic tinnitus is a yngology clinic (“clinical” hyperacusis). Shel-
continuous phantom percept that intrudes on drake, Diehl, and Schaette (2015) found that
consciousness while hyperacusis requires the an LDL of ~90 dB SPL was sufficient to iden-
presence of sound for its expression and may tify 90% of individuals within a group of hy-
not be noticed in quiet environments but only peracusis patients, but at a cost of about 50%
when moderate to high level environmental of false positive errors; a more stringent LDL
sounds are present. of ~70 dB SPL identified ~30% of hyperacusis
The possibility of a different threshold patients with no false positives, but at a cost
for reporting hyperacusis suggests that more of ~70% false negative errors. The relationship
objective psychoacoustic measurements are of LDL and the ASR to hyperacusis assessed
needed to establish its presence in a tinnitus by questionnaires is also inconsistent (Knud-
patient. One such measure is to determine the sen & Melcher, 2016; Gu et al., 2010). These
level of a sound at which a subject reports dis- results suggest that while LDL, LGF slope, and
comfort and a preference not to be presented the ASR are sensitive to loudness perception,
with still louder sounds (loudness discomfort abnormal loudness perception may not be the
level, LDL). Another method is to measure only factor that brings an individual to seek
the slope of functions relating the perceived clinical help for hyperacusis. Psychological fac-
loudness of a sound to the physical intensity of tors such as fear of sound and the impact of
that sound (loudness growth functions, LGF). reduced SLT on quality of life may modulate
Both measures require norms based on popu- auditory perceptual responses in quiet envi-
lation data gathered using standardized clinical ronments and increase the likelihood of ascer-
procedures for their interpretation in individ- tainment by a clinic. In this chapter we will
ual cases. While such data are presently lim- focus on hyperacusis defined as reduced SLT
ited, Sherlock and Formby (2005) concluded measured by LDLs, LGFs, ASRs, and similar
from their study of 59 normal hearing adults psychoacoustic methods, recognizing that these
that LDL provides a simple and valid measure measures may not capture all aspects of clini-
of the threshold for discomfort that may be cal hyperacusis.
useful in assessing sound level tolerance (SLT) A further distinction is between hyper-
in clinical populations. LDLs have been found acusis so defined and recruitment. In recruit-
to be about 15 dB lower in hyperacusis suffer- ment, perceived loudness grows rapidly above
ers compared to controls (Sheldrake, Diehl, & the threshold of hearing, which is typically
Schaette, 2015), and the slope of LGFs is sim- in the audiometric hearing loss range. Sub-
ilarly steeper in hyperacusis patients (Noreña sequently the LGF plateaus follow a near-
& Chery-Croze, 2007). LDL also correlates normal course at higher sound intensities.
with cortical metabolic blood oxygenation In hyperacusis, on the other hand, perceived
6. Tinnitus and Hyperacusis: Relationship, Mechanisms, and Initiating Conditions 79

sound intensity exceeds that associated with tual framework whereby central gain changes
normal hearing at higher sound intensities, could lead to tinnitus and/or recruitment or
although at lower intensities loudness percep- hyperacusis, depending on the location(s) and
tion may be more or less normal. Another dif- degree(s) of the gain changes. However, Zeng
ference is that while recruitment is accompa- (2013) did not discuss in detail the neural
nied by audiometric hearing loss, hyperacusis mechanisms potentially involved in tinnitus
can be observed with or without hearing im- and hyperacusis or changes in central gain.
pairment expressed in the clinical audiogram. In the following we first consider the re-
Examples of the difference between hyper­ lationship between hyperacusis and tinnitus
acusis and recruitment in LGFs measured in by examining the time course of their devel-
hyperacusis patients can be found in Noreña opment, and whether they share a common
and Chéry-Croze (2007, their Figure 1). initiating condition, which could be cochlear
In this chapter we distinguish recruit- pathology that may or may not be hidden from
ment from hyperacusis because its mechanism the audiogram. We then discuss the neural
is debated and may be different from that of changes that appear to underlie tinnitus and
hyperacusis. It has been suggested that recruit- hyperacusis from the cochlear nucleus to the
ment is caused by a loss of compressive non- auditory cortex, and the implications of the
linearity on the basilar membrane owing to findings for the treatment of both conditions.
OHC damage or by the spread of excitation
from cochlear dead regions to adjacent regions
with increasing sound intensity (Joris, 2009).
These hypotheses imply that recruitment re- The Time Course of
flects pathology in the cochlear transduction Hyperacusis and Tinnitus
mechanism and thus should be expressed in
Development
the behavior of auditory nerve fibers (ANFs),
but studies have failed to consistently find
such evidence (Joris, 2009; Heinz & Young, One approach to understanding the relation-
2004). Alternatively, recruitment may be ship of tinnitus and hyperacusis (defined as
caused by increases in the gain (input/output reduced SLT) is to compare their development
response) of neurons in central auditory struc- over time in a cohort of participants selected
tures that occur following cochlear injuries for neither condition. Sanchez et al. (2016)
(Cai, Ma, & Young, 2009; Noreña, 2011). In studied the prevalence of both phenomena in a
this alternate explanation, recruitment could sample of 170 adolescents in a private school,
be considered to arise from a degree of central first by a detailed questionnaire that assessed
gain increase that is commensurate with the their risky listening habits and prior experi-
amount of audiometric threshold shift and ence of tinnitus in the natural environment,
roughly normalizes loudness at higher sound and then by psychoacoustic measurements in
levels, whereas hyperacusis is generated by ex- a sound chamber. The audiogram (to 16 kHz),
cessive central gain increases, disproportionate transient evoked and distortion product oto-
with the audiogram. Furthermore, numerous acoustic emissions at several frequencies, and
studies have suggested that central gain in- LDL at four frequencies, were measured for all
creases triggered by cochlear pathology may subjects. When tinnitus was heard in the sound
also lead to aberrant spontaneous neural activ- booth, its pitch and loudness were determined
ity that is perceived as tinnitus. Based on these by audiometric methods. Three groups were
observations, Zeng (2013) proposed a concep- distinguished, consisting of (1) adolescents who
80 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

reported a history of tinnitus on question- contrary, the affected adolescents reported sig­
naire and heard it in the sound booth (Group nificant tendencies to listen at lower volume
Q+B+, 46/170 or 27.1% of the sample), levels and to attend parties and raves for a
(2) adolescents reporting tinnitus by question- shorter period of time than their unaffected
naire but did not hear it in the booth (Group classmates, suggesting that they tended to be
Q+B-, 47/170 cases or 27.6% of the sample), comparatively more protective of their hearing.
or (3) adolescents who reported no experience Fifty-four of the 170 subjects tested by Sanchez
of tinnitus at all (Group Q-B-, 74/170 cases, et al. (2016) were available for retest one year
43.5% of the sample). Hearing thresholds and later. On retest 8/54 (14%) of teens qualified
otoacoustic emissions were normal and did as Q+B+ cases, and 6/54 of these cases (11%)
not discriminate among the three groups (Fig­ were repeat Q+B+ cases. The latter percentage
ure 6–1, A–C), but LDL did so, being reduced (11%) may be a conservative estimate of prev-
by 11.3 dB (p < 0.0001) only for Group alence of persistent tinnitus in adolescents that
Q+B+ where tinnitus was reported by both takes into account the possibility that in the
assessment methods (Figure 6–1D). The tin- absence of continued excessive sound expo-
nitus percepts of these adolescents (loudness sure tinnitus may resolve. However, the LDLs
and pitch matching) were congruent with of the affected teens were again reduced com-
those of adult chronic tinnitus sufferers. These pared to other teens by an amount (17.2 dB,
findings suggest that tinnitus and reduced p < 0.0002) that was similar to Group Q+B+
SLT follow a similar course of development in the initial test (see Figure 6–1D), while their
over time. It should also be noticed that LDL audiograms and otoacoustic emissions (not
was reduced for all frequencies tested (0.5 to shown) remained normal and were highly re-
4 kHz), unlike tinnitus frequencies measured peatable. A further result was that, while a clear
in the sound booth which were concentrated group difference was observed in the averaged
above 3 kHz in accordance with other pub- LDL data of Figure 6–1D, approximately half
lished data (Henry, Roberts, Ellingson, & of the adolescents in Group Q+B+ at both
Thielman 2013; Roberts et al., 2008; Schaette tests expressed LDLs > 90 dB SPL which over-
& McAlpine, 2011). LGFs measured in hy- lapped those of adolescents without tinnitus.
peracusis patients (Noreña & Chery-Croze, Thus, reduced LDL and tinnitus while related
2007) and in tinnitus sufferers with normal au- in the group data were not inextricably linked,
diograms (Hébert, Fournier, & Noreña, 2013) in agreement with the results reported in the
are also steepened across frequencies, in agree- introductory section of this chapter.
ment with these LDL data. Further information on the time course
Tinnitus verified by questionnaire and of tinnitus and reduced SLT came from stud-
psychoacoustic measurement (Group Q+B+, ies in which input to auditory pathways was
27.1% of the sample) likely reflects an upper attenuated in normal hearing volunteers who
estimate of the prevalence of verified tinni- wore ear plugs for 1 to 2 weeks. These studies
tus in adolescents because new cases of tinni- found that auditory deprivation could modu-
tus can subside over time (Roberts, 2016). It late SLT and the threshold of the middle ear
is noteworthy that the adolescents in Group stapedius reflex in normal hearing humans
Q+B+ did not appear to have been exposed (Formby, Sherlock, & Gold, 2003; Munro,
to more or higher levels of recreational sound Turtle, & Schaette, 2014; see Brotherton et al.,
than other teens, since risky listening habits 2015 for a review). In one study, loudness per-
were almost universal in the sample. On the ception at several levels of an LGF decreased
Figure 6–1. Audiometric, otoacoustic, and loudness discomfort level measurements in 170
adolescents. Tinnitus was assessed by questionnaire and by psychoacoustic measurements
in a sound booth. Three groups are shown: Q+B+ history of tinnitus reported on question-
naire and verified by psychoacoustic testing in the sound booth (46/170 adolescents); Q+B-
history of tinnitus reported by questionnaire but not heard during psychoacoustic testing
(47/170 adolescents); Q-B- no history or experience of tinnitus (74/170 adolescents). A. Au-
diogram measured from 0.25–16 kHz. B. Transient otoacoustic measurements at six fre-
quencies. C. Distortion product otoacoustic emissions at six frequencies. D. Loudness discom-
fort levels measured at four frequencies. Of the original sample of 170 subjects, 54 subjects
returned for a second measurement one year later (8/54 Q+B+, 20/54 Q+B-, 26/54 Q-B-).
Their data are identified by the symbols and bold broken lines. Their audiograms and oto-
acoustic emissions were unchanged from the first measurement (data not shown). The bars
are ±1 SE (for clarity, only one direction is shown for subjects retested at one year). See text
for details. Source: A–C. From Sanchez et al. (2016). D. Data are from the study of Sanchez
et al. (2016), with new results reported for the follow up.

81
82 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

by ~7 dB in both ears even though only one emulating a mild sensorineural hearing loss.
ear was occluded, while the stapedius reflex At the end of week 11, participants reported a
threshold decreased by ~3 dB in the occluded stable bilateral tinnitus with a measured high-
ear but increased in the control ear by a sim- pitched frequency spectrum aligning with that
ilar amount (Munro, Turtle, & Schaette, reported by chronic tinnitus sufferers with
2014). These changes were observed at 2 kHz (Roberts et al., 2008; Noreña, Micheyl, Chery-
where sound attenuation by the ear plug was Croze, & Collet, 2002) or without (Sanchez
maximal (~25 dB) but also at 0.5 kHz where et al., 2016; Schaette & McAlpine, 2010; Gu,
attenuation was less (~8 dB), suggesting fre- Herrman, Levine, & Melcher, 2012) audio-
quency nonspecific effects. Opposing effects metric hearing loss. Tinnitus resolved when the
between the ears on the reflex threshold may ear plug was removed, indicating that auditory
be explained by evidence (Darrow, Maison, & deprivation was its cause.
Liberman, 2006) that lateral olivocochlear The results of studies employing artificial
efferents tend to balance neural activation sound attenuation techniques are important
between the two ears over a long integra- because they indicate that the development
tion window, which may aid spatial localiza- of tinnitus and reduced SLT follow a similar
tion for transient sounds based on interaural time course. Rescaling of neural input/output
level differences. This mechanism would be functions (increased central gain) commences
expected to yield equivalent loudness judg- at low levels of sound attenuation, which
ments between the occluded and unoccluded might explain why the audiograms of hyper-
ear which was observed; nonetheless a stimu- acusis patients do not necessarily exhibit sub-
lus of given physical intensity was perceived to stantial high frequency hearing loss which is
be louder after unilateral deprivation than be- more common in tinnitus patients. Gu et al.
fore deprivation (Munro, Turtle, & Schaette, (2010) noted how difficult it was to find tin-
2014). Munro et al. interpreted their results nitus patients with measured normal SLT;
to suggest that multiple gain mechanisms cases of reduced SLT without tinnitus were
may be triggered by auditory occlusion. In easier to recruit. However, studies of artificial
an animal study, unilateral ear plugging up- sound attenuation also show that tinnitus and
regulated excitatory glutamatergic receptors reduced SLT can be induced when the cochlea
on fusiform and bushy cells in the cochlear is intact. We can thus conclude that cochlear
nucleus within 24 hours ipsilaterally as well pathology is not necessary for either phenom-
as contralaterally, and downregulated inhibi- enon, although some level of sound attenuation
tory glycinergic synapses on fusiform cells ip- may be necessary. On the other side of the equa-
silaterally, which are further mechanisms that tion, there are many individuals, particularly
could affect gain in central auditory pathways among senior citizens, who have permanent
(Whiting, Moiseff, & Rubio, 2009). high-frequency hearing loss expressed in the
Noting that normal hearing subjects of- audiogram but do not have tinnitus (Roberts
ten report phantom sounds after confinement et al., 2008; Eggermont, 2012). This may apply
in an anechoic chamber, Schaette, Turtle, and to hyperacusis as well, although psychoacous-
Munro (2012) investigated the effects of ear tic data on the topic are lacking. This suggests
plugging on tinnitus. Eighteen volunteers that cochlear hearing damage alone may not
with normal hearing wore a silicone ear plug be sufficient for tinnitus or hyperacusis, either.
for 1 week, which provided an attenuation of Yet the three conditions are undeniably
<10 dB at 0.25 kHz and >30 dB at 3 and 4 kHz related, given the high prevalence of audio-
6. Tinnitus and Hyperacusis: Relationship, Mechanisms, and Initiating Conditions 83

metric hearing loss observed in tinnitus and the cochlear transduction mechanism was
hyperacusis patients compared to the general intact in these young individuals. However,
population. It has been suggested that dispar- the possibility of hidden injuries to ANFs
ities between hearing loss and tinnitus may resulting from the adolescents’ near univer-
reflect a causal role for extracochlear mecha- sal exposure to recreational sound cannot be
nisms in generating chronic tinnitus. Proposed ruled out. ANFs are connected to inner hair
mechanisms include individual differences in cells (IHCs) through synapses containing
the access of tinnitus-related neural activity to ribbon-like structures that govern the release
brain networks for conscious awareness (De of neural transmitters when the IHC is stim-
Ridder, Elgoyhen, Romo, & Langguth, 2011) ulated by sound. ANFs contacting the pillar
or deficiencies in a proposed noise cancella- side of the IHC discharge at very low levels
tion system in the subcallosal brain region of sound (low threshold fibers, LT ANFs) and
that modulates an inhibitory feedback loop therefore determine the threshold of hearing
engaging the reticular nucleus of the thalamus as measured by the audiogram. ANFs con-
(Rauschecker, Leaver, & Mühlau, 2010). These tacting the modiolar side of the IHC, on the
hypotheses are based on functional imaging ev- other hand, are high-threshold fibers (HT
idence for brain changes in these regions in tin- ANFs) that discharge only to sounds exceed-
nitus (Rauschecker, Leaver, & Mühlau, 2010; ing ~50 dB SPL. ANFs differing in threshold
Mühlau et al., 2006). However, the evidence is are one mechanism by which the auditory sys-
not consistent (Melcher, Knudsen, & Levine, tem codes sound intensity varying over orders
2013), and the role of the brain changes in of magnitude using neurons that have a lim-
modulating or generating tinnitus percepts is ited dynamic range. Several studies have now
debated (Eggermont & Roberts, 2015). Al- shown that ribbon synapses on HT ANFs are
ternatively, we discuss in the following section especially vulnerable to damage by noise ex-
whether evidence of a role for hidden hearing posure (Kujawa & Liberman, 2009; Lin, Fur-
loss in tinnitus may clarify the relationship of man, Kujawa, & Liberman, 2011; Furman,
hearing loss to tinnitus and hyperacusis. Kujawa, & Liberman, 2013). This is particu-
larly true for HT ANFs with high frequency
tuning, which notably is where tinnitus fre-
Hearing Loss Is an quencies typically reside. Kujawa and Liberman
(2009) found that ~40% of synaptic ribbons
Initiating Condition
innervating these ANFs in their mouse model
were permanently lost following exposure to
Normal hearing requires a healthy cochlear an octave-band noise for 2 hours at 100 dB
transduction mechanism (outer and inner hair that produced a temporary threshold shift
cells and the physical structures that support (TTS) in the auditory brainstem response
them) and intact ANFs that connect the in- (ABR) audiogram but did not permanently
ner hair cells to the brain. The audiograms elevate audiometric thresholds. In this model,
and otoacoustic emissions observed in the synaptic loss was followed by a delayed loss of
young adolescents of Figure 6–1 expressing 50% of spiral ganglion neurons after 2 years
persistent tinnitus and reduced LDL (Group compared to unexposed control animals. No-
Q+B+) were robust and not discriminably tably, the same level of noise trauma did not
different from those of adolescents not expe- affect ribbon synapses on LT ANFs or lead
riencing these conditions, which suggests that to persisting structural damage to cochlear
84 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

hair cells or their stereocilia. Thus, permanent animals and humans. Hickox and Liberman
cochlear injury induced by noise trauma did (2014) assessed the effect synaptopathic noise
not involve the cochlear transduction mecha- trauma in mice on behavioral startle responses
nism but was preferential for ANFs with high (whole body movement sensed by an acceler-
firing thresholds and high-frequency tuning ometer) evoked by tone bursts of increasing
(Furman, Kujawa, & Liberman, 2013). Sub- level, presented either in quiet or the presence
sequent research in a guinea pig model (Shi of background noise. Behavioral startle re-
et al., 2013) revealed losses of synaptic ribbons sponses are mediated by auditory nerve affer-
of similar magnitude after TTS induced by ents that project to the pontine reticular for-
a 105-dB noise exposure, with greater losses mation, ventral cochlear nucleus (VCN), and
occurring in higher (60 to 70%) compared dorsal cochlea nucleus (DCN), interfacing
to lower (10 to 30%) cochleotopic frequency ultimately with spinal motor neurons evok-
regions in agreement with the frequency pro- ing whole body movement. All of these struc-
file of Kujawa and Liberman (2009) for the tures become hyperactive after noise exposure
mouse. Similar to the mouse, none of these (Kaltenbach, 2011; Eggermont & Roberts,
losses gave a permanent threshold shift (PTS) 2015). In the study of Hickox and Liberman
in the guinea pig model, suggesting that syn- (2014), noise-exposed mice with normal hear-
apses on HT ANFs were predominately af- ing thresholds but verified synaptopathy (hid-
fected. However, synapse counts in the guinea den hearing loss) exhibited startle responses
pig model showed a more robust recovery that were comparable to those of control mice
than in the mouse, returning to control levels when startle responses were evoked in quiet,
in the low frequency cochleotopic region and but were larger than controls when startle re-
recovering to about 80% of control levels in sponses were evoked in background noise. Be-
the high frequency region 1 month after noise cause in the presence of background noise LT
exposure (Shi et al., 2016a). Nonetheless, per- but not HT ANFs would have reached satu-
sisting ribbon loss in the high frequency co- ration, the results revealed changes in central
chleotopic region (20%) was sufficient to re- auditory structures innervated by HT ANFs
duce the compound action potential evoked (e.g., the small cap region of the anterior VCN)
by suprathreshold sounds and to impair tem- as the source of increased behavioral excitabil-
poral processing ability assessed by a forward ity. Consistent with this observation, wave I
masking procedure (Shi et al., 2013). Thus, amplitude of the ABR reflecting the output of
while much remains to be learned about the the cochlea was reduced in synaptopathic mice
effects of noise exposure on ANF synapses compared to controls when evoked by supra-
and dendrites in different species and how threshold sounds; however, wave V amplitude
injury is expressed in peripheral and central which is generated by sources in the auditory
auditory processing (Shi et al., 2016a,b), it is midbrain was increased in exposed compared
apparent that not all forms of cochlear injury to control animals when evoked by sounds
affect hearing thresholds. Hearing loss arising of increasing intensity (Hickox & Liberman,
from HT ANF synaptopathy has been called 2014). Similar findings have been reported for
“hidden hearing loss” because it will not ap- tinnitus patients with clinically normal audio-
pear in the clinical audiogram (Kujawa & Lib­ grams (Schaette & Mc­Alpine, 2011; Gu, Herr-
erman 2009). man, Levine, & Melcher, 2012). In these stud-
Hidden hearing loss does, however, lead ies, ABR wave 1 evoked by clicks >80 dB SPL
to changes in central auditory pathways that was reduced in the patients while wave V was
are expressed as hyperacusis-like behavior in normal or enhanced compared to non-tinnitus
6. Tinnitus and Hyperacusis: Relationship, Mechanisms, and Initiating Conditions 85

controls, although Guest et al. (2016) could not frequency region of the tinnitus subjects. Subse-
confirm a reduced Wave I in tinnitus subjects in quently neural coding in the auditory midbrain
their study. Mechanisms of homeostatic and/or was measured using the “envelope following
Hebb­ian plasticity are believed to underlie these response” (EFR), a response recorded by elec-
effects which reflect increased “central gain” in troencephalography (EEG) that has been shown
auditory pathways. The association of tinnitus to correlate with temporal processing skills
and hyperacusis with reduced LDLs, stapedius in normal hearing listeners (Bharadwaj et al.,
reflex thresholds, and steepened LGFs reported 2015) and to be sensitive to noise-induced ANF
above for subjects with normal audiograms is synaptopathy in animals (Shaheen, Valero, &
also consistent with increased central gain fol- Liberman, 2015). EFRs were measured in back-
lowing hidden hearing loss. Extrapolating to ground noise where HT fibers were expected to
the results of Figure 6–1, tinnitus experienced be required to encode the AM, and also in quiet
by the adolescents with reduced LDLs could where both LT and HT ANFs were expected
similarly signal hidden damage to HT ANFs, to contribute to AM coding. Paul et al. found
particularly when these effects persisted during that subjects without tinnitus whose EFRs were
retest 1 year later. Reversible transient injury to comparatively resistant to the addition of back-
the cochlear transduction mechanism insuffi- ground noise had better AM detection thresh-
cient to elevate hearing thresholds or individ- olds in background noise than subjects whose
ual differences in synaptic repair could apply EFRs were more affected by noise. Simulated
to cases in which tinnitus and altered LDL re- auditory nerve responses using the peripheral
solved prior to retest. model suggested that synaptic losses affecting
While hidden loss affecting HT ANFs HT ANFs alone were sufficient to explain the
appears to be present in tinnitus and hyper­ EFR results of non-tinnitus subjects with poor
acusis without threshold shifts, reduced LT ANF AM coding. In comparison, tinnitus subjects
activity is likely also a factor, given that high had worse AM detection thresholds and exhib-
frequency hearing loss is prevalent in these clin- ited reduced EFRs overall compared to controls,
ical populations. Synaptopathy affecting LT even though thresholds for the test stimuli av-
ANFs can also be hidden from the audiogram, eraged <2 dB HL in both groups. Simulated
since hearing thresholds do not appear to be auditory nerve responses found that in addition
elevated as long as at least ~20% of IHCs remain to severe HT fiber loss, a degree of LT fiber loss
intact (Lobarinas, Salvi, & Ding, 2016). Paul, that would not be expected to affect audiomet-
Bruce, and Roberts (2017) applied cochlear ric thresholds was needed to explain the results
modeling using a well-established model of the of the tinnitus subjects. Thus, while HT syn-
auditory periphery to investigate the putative aptic loss appeared to degrade temporal coding
contribution of both fiber types to individual ability, additional LT fiber losses hidden from
differences in temporal processing ability in the audiogram were necessary to explain tinni-
young adults with and without tinnitus, all of tus (Paul, Bruce, & Roberts, 2017).
whom had normal audiometric hearing. Sub- These observations do not rule out a role
jects were first required to detect the presence for extracochlear mechanisms in cases of tin-
of amplitude modulation (AM) in a 5-kHz nitus and hyperacusis without audiometric
tone embedded in background noise intended hearing loss, or of hearing loss without either
to degrade the contribution of LT fibers, such condition. However, they suggest that hidden
that AM coding was preferentially reliant on hearing losses could in principle be sufficient
HT fibers. The frequency of 5 kHz was cho- to explain many such cases. LT fiber synap-
sen because this frequency was in the tinnitus topathy appears to be especially important,
86 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

in part because loss of these fibers is likely ac- focus to hyperacusis. Events occurring in sub-
companied by damage to the more vulnerable cortical auditory structures converge on the
HT fibers. However, an important functional thalamus and cortex, which can be expected
difference between LT and HT fibers is that to play a role in both phenomena.
LT ANFs exhibit much higher rates of spon-
taneous activity in quiet than do HT fibers.
Accordingly, LT fibers are often referred to as Spotlight on Tinnitus
high-spontaneous rate (HSR) fibers in the lit-
erature, although the designation HSR/LT fi- Neural changes associated with tinnitus have
bers would be more accurate. Injury to HSR/ been studied by exposing animals to putative
LT fibers appears to add a tinnitus deficit to tinnitus-inducing procedures such as salicy-
impaired temporal processing in individuals late administration or to noise trauma which
where hearing thresholds are normal. In the can be scaled to give TTS or PTS (Hickox &
next section we review evidence regarding how Liberman, 2014; Kujawa & Liberman, 2009).
hearing loss (hidden or otherwise) may lead to The presence of tinnitus has subsequently been
tinnitus and hyperacusis. Because tinnitus and evaluated by methods such as gap detection (a
hyperacusis have different frequency profiles, silent gap inserted in a sound delivered prior
their mechanisms must to some extent be dis- to an acoustic startle stimulus will suppress a
tinguishable although they are likely to inter- startle response, unless tinnitus fills the gap)
act by virtue of sharing a common initiating or by making a tinnitus-like sound a cue for
condition. a behavioral response (responding in quiet af-
ter a tinnitus-inducing manipulation signals
tinnitus). Noise exposure producing TTS or
Distinguishable Mechanisms PTS can also enhance sound-driven behavioral
responses suggesting hyperacusis (Hickox &
for Tinnitus and Hyperacusis
Liberman, 2014) although to date the empha-
sis has been on studying tinnitus. Aside from
Experiments on ear plugging discussed above enabling physiological investigations, an ad-
indicate that reducing input to auditory path- vantage of these models is that animals can be
ways by as little as 20 dB SPL can be suffi- divided into those expressing evidence of tin­
cient to trigger an increase in central gain and nitus and those that do not, when their pre­
decrease SLT in humans and animal models, ceding experience has been the same.
even when the cochlea is intact. This indicates It has long been known that noise trauma
that when the initiating condition of cochlear sufficient to damage the cochlear transduction
injuries sufficient to reduce auditory nerve ac- mechanism or auditory nerve synapses reduces
tivity is present, changes in central gain will the spontaneous and driven activity in the au-
occur and thus be present not only in hypera- ditory nerve (Eggermont & Roberts, 2004). In
cusis but also overlap those involved in tinni- response to reduced input from the auditory
tus, although for the reasons discussed above nerve, neurons in central auditory structures
the degree of ANF impairment required for a tend to become hyperactive, reflecting changes
clinically-significant tinnitus may be greater in central gain that restore the responsiveness of
than that needed for hyperacusis-like behavior. neurons in central auditory pathways to their
In the following we first consider the mech- remaining inputs. Hyperactivity is expressed
anisms that may lead to tinnitus and its dis- as an increase in the spontaneous firing rates
tinctive frequency profile, and then shift the (SFRs) of auditory neurons and in their sound-
6. Tinnitus and Hyperacusis: Relationship, Mechanisms, and Initiating Conditions 87

evoked responses, both of which have been re- duced by noise exposure (tinnitus was not
corded at all levels of the auditory pathway in tested). In this research, SFRs increased across
animals subjected to traumatizing noise or to the tonotopic frequency map of primary audi-
ototoxic drugs that selectively destroy IHCs tory cortex below as well as in the hearing loss
(reviewed by Kaltenbach, 2011; Eggermont & frequencies, while increased synchrony was
Roberts, 2015; Shore, Roberts, & Langguth, confined to the hearing loss region (Noreña &
2016; Salvi et al., 2017; Hesse et al., 2016). In­ Eggermont, 2003). Because human tinnitus
creased driven responses are likely to relate to percepts may similarly localize to the hearing
hyperacusis, while increases in the SFRs of au- loss frequencies (Noreña, Micheyl, Chery-
ditory neurons are likely to relate to tinnitus Croze, & Collet, 2002; Roberts et al., 2008),
as it is a persisting phantom percept. Support- aberrant neural synchrony has been proposed
ing this view, SFRs and driven responses have by several authors as the proximal neural cor-
been found to increase in animals that express relate of tinnitus. A neural synchrony code for
behavioral evidence of tinnitus compared to phantom sound is also consistent with the prin-
animals that do not, implicating these phe- ciple that synchronous firing among a group of
nomena as part of the tinnitus phenotype. In- neurons may be used by the brain to detect an
creased SFRs specific to tinnitus animals have acoustic event in the environment using neural
been reported in the ventral (Volger, Robert- elements (ANFs) that are inherently sponta-
son, & Mulders, 2011) and dorsal (Koehler & neously active (Eggermont, 1990).
Shore, 2013) cochlear nucleus, the inferior One unanswered question is how in-
colliculus (IC) (Middleton et al. 2011), and creased synchrony and SFRs associated with
in the auditory cortex (Basura, Kohler, & tinnitus develop following noise-induced TTS
Shore, 2015), reflecting propagation of in- and PTS. Wu, Martel, and Shore (2016) ob-
creased SFRs up the auditory pathway or mech- served that while SFR and synchrony were
anisms operating locally in these structures. increased in their tinnitus animals, changes in
A further neural correlate observed in an- synchrony and SFRs were poorly correlated
imals expressing tinnitus behavior compared in the data, in part because synchrony be-
to animals that do not is an increase in the tween long-distance unit pairs with low cross-
cross-correlated (synchronous) activity of neu- correlation strengths were increased specifi-
rons within the DCN and higher auditory cally in the tinnitus animals and corresponded
structures. In DCN increased synchrony fol- more closely with their tinnitus frequencies.
lowing noise-induced TTS was seen among One factor affecting synchronous activity in
neurons connected primarily by short physical a tonotopic map could be down regulation
and tonotopic distances but also by longer dis- of glycinergic inhibition, which has been
tances, giving tinnitus a spectral profile situated observed in the DCN of tinnitus animals
about 1 octave above the exposure band which (Wang et al., 2009) as well as after cochlear
is where TTS was most strongly expressed (Wu, ablation in the guinea pig (Potashner, Suneja,
Martel, & Shore, 2016). While SFRs also in- & Benson, 2000). In this respect, it may be
creased in this frequency region, SFRs have instructive to consider what the functional
been observed to increase below the exposure role of this inhibition might be in the DCN,
frequencies as well (Dehmel et al., 2012), sug- and how its loss might generate tinnitus-like
gesting a difference in the frequency profile of behavior after PTS or TTS.
tinnitus-related synchrony compared to that of Fusiform cells in the DCN are the first site
SFRs. These results concur with those reported of cross-modal auditory and somatosensory
in an early study of cats with hearing loss in- integration in auditory pathways (Oertel &
88 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

Young, 2004; Zeng et al., 2009). Inputs from in this scenario, since cartwheel cells are driven
the auditory nerve contact the basal dendrites by parallel fiber and not ANF activity. Plasticity
of fusiform cells, which are then targeted af- unleashed on parallel fibers may explain why
ter one synaptic delay by strong feed-forward glutamate transporters in somatosensory path-
inhibition from glycinergic vertical cells con- ways are upregulated when the auditory nerve
tacted by the same ANF (Sedlacek & Bre- is cut (Zeng et al., 2009) and why up to 80%
nowitz, 2014). In contrast, somatosensory in­­ of chronic tinnitus sufferers are able to modu-
formation from the head and neck region is late their tinnitus by clenching their jaws and
conveyed by parallel fibers to the apical den- similar movements of the head and neck re-
drites of fusiform cells, which are similarly gion (Levine, Abel, & Cheng, 2003; Sanchez
targeted by feed-forward inhibition on their et al., 2002).
cell bodies (from cartwheel cells driven by If the mechanism just described is one
parallel fibers in this case) after one synaptic contributing to hypersynchrony in the DCN,
delay (Roberts & Trussell, 2010; Mancilla & loss of LT ANF inputs would be especially
Manis, 2009). Koehler & Shore (2013) found patholytic. This is because in normal hearing
that the timing rules of spike-timing depen- the high rates of spontaneous firing of these
dent plasticity (STDP) induced by paired robust ANFs may be sufficient to constrain
bimodal auditory-somatosensory stimulation STDP on parallel fiber synapses preventing
were changed in animals giving behavioral maladaptive plasticity. In contrast, HT ANFs
evidence of tinnitus, such that potentiation exhibit lower rates of spontaneous activity such
dominated over suppression for a wider range that their loss would have a smaller effect, al-
of bimodal intervals on parallel fiber synapses though these ANFs are important for coding
(Wu, Martel, & Shore, 2016). One mecha- suprathreshold sounds (Paul, Bruce, & Rob-
nism leading to this change could be a loss of erts, 2107; Bharadwaj et al., 2014). Consistent
feedforward inhibition from ANFs via vertical with a role for LT ANFs in hypersynchrony,
cells onto fusiform cells, which may in the in- behavioral evidence of tinnitus in animals has
tact brain gate STDP on the parallel fibers pro- been found to correlate with the magnitude of
tecting synapses that convey temporally conver- PTS (Bauer, Brozoski, & Myers, 2007) with a
gent auditory and somatosensory inputs to the similar trend (r = 0.539, p = 0.10) following
neuron from down-regulation while other syn­ TTS (Wu, Martel, & Shore, 2016) although
apses are weakened. If feedforward inhibition the findings are not consistent on this point
driven from the auditory pathway is diminished (Li, Kalappa, & Tzounopoulos, 2015).
or lost owing to deafferentation, STDP could It is recognized, however, that other
be unleashed on synapses connecting parallel mechanisms could be at work in the DCN or
fibers to fusiform apical dendrites, leading to elsewhere in addition to (or instead of) this
increased synchronous activity among fusiform process. Altered GABAergic inhibition has
cells and to tinnitus. Diminished feedforward been demonstrated in tissue slices taken from
inhibition (Ma & Young, 2009) does not nec- the DCN of mice giving behavioral evidence
essarily require a downregulation in inhibitory of tinnitus, although the role of this form of
neurotransmitter but may simply be caused by inhibition in tinnitus behavior is not known
reduced input from ANFs to the DCN neural (Middleton et al., 2011). The DCN is targeted
circuitry following deafferentation (Zheng et al., by a dense serotonergic input that increases the
2009). Integration among multiple somato- probability of spikes evoked in fusiform cells
sensory inputs gated by feedforward inhibition by parallel fiber stimulation but not by stim-
from cartwheel cells would continue unchecked ulation of the auditory nerve (Tang &Trussell,
6. Tinnitus and Hyperacusis: Relationship, Mechanisms, and Initiating Conditions 89

2015, 2017). Cholinergic neuromodulators are both of which have been reported in tinnitus
also known to act via endocannabinoid signal- patients (Weisz et al., 2007; Wienbruch et al.,
ing to convert long-term potentiation (LTP) to 2006). One important aspect of cortical and
long-term depression (LTD) at DCN parallel subcortical HSP is how it interacts with Heb-
fiber synapses (Zhao & Tzounopoulos, 2011). bian plasticity such as STDP. When the model
Blockade of cholinergic muscarinic receptors of Chrostowski et al. (2011) was extended in
by atropine infused into the fusiform cell layer Chrostowski (2012) to include STDP, it was
of the DCN decreased the spontaneous firing found that changes in cortical connectivity
rates of fusiform cells and increased their syn- initiated by HSP after deafferentation became
chrony (Stefanescu & Shore, 2017), revealing consolidated and magnified through STDP.
effects of cholinergic modulation that altered Another possible interaction between HSP
the rules of STDP in the direction of those seen and STDP can occur due to the failure of
in tinnitus animals. Changes in intrinsic mem- HSP to appropriately regulate STDP, one of
brane conductance are a further consideration HSP’s main functions in addition to setting
and have been reported in the fusiform cells overall levels of activity (Turrigiano & Nelson,
of mice giving behavioral evidence of tinnitus 2008). In the study of Rüttiger et al. (2013),
(Li, Choi, & Tzounopoulos, 2013). Overall, it sound-exposed rats were more likely to develop
seems likely that neural correlates of tinnitus behavioral evidence of tinnitus if they exhibited
are generated by multiple processes involving substantial ANF deafferentation and reduced
reduced inhibition (Wang et al., 2009; Mid- expression of Arc, a gene involved in HSP. If
dleton et al., 2011), altered synaptic plasticity HSP fails, then unconstrained STDP could
(Koehler & Shore, 2013), changes in central lead to destabilization of cortical networks and
gain (Noreña, 2011), and alterations in intrin- increased spontaneous synchrony. We discuss
sic ion channels (Li, Choi, & Tzounopoulos, the role of thalamocortical pathways in tinnitus
2013) that occur consequent on deafferenta- and hyperacusis further, in a later section below.
tion of auditory pathways.
Finally, the feedforward circuitry seen in
the DCN is observed at other levels of the brain Spotlight on Hyperacusis
including the primary auditory cortex and the
cerebellum, where changes could be generated While STDP occurring after TTS/PTS and
independently or in parallel with subcortical its effects conveyed to the auditory cortex
mechanisms. Tinnitus-related activation has may give tinnitus its distinctive frequency
been reported in these structures in electro- profile, other changes taking place in audi-
physiological recordings and functional imag- tory pathways following hearing impairment
ing of tinnitus in animal models (Yang et al., may relate more closely to hyperacusis. In
2011; Basura, Koehler, & Shore, 2013; Bauer, principle, interrelated mechanisms are likely
Wisner, Sybert, & Brozoski, 2013) and human to contribute.
tinnitus patients (Gu et al., 2010; Lanting, One mechanism involves forms of HSP
Woźniak, van Dijk, & Langers, 2016). Com- that adjust the input/output functions (gain)
putational studies have shown how changes in of neurons in central auditory pathways to
the auditory cortex due to homeostatic plas- maintain neuron firing rates within their dy-
ticity (HSP) following deafferentation could namic ranges in different acoustic environ-
generate synchronous activity and map reor­ ments or when acoustic input is diminished
ganization in this structure (Dominguez, Becker, by hearing injuries (Pozo & Goda, 2010). In
Bruce, & Read, 2006; Chrostowski et al., 2011) one example, Qui, Salvi, Ding, and Burkard
90 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

et al. (2000) recorded the response evoked by level functions seen in higher auditory struc-
a sound in the auditory nerve, the IC, and au- tures. Interestingly, primary-like neurons in
ditory cortex, of chinchillas in which ~30% of VCN showed opposite effects suggesting that
IHCs had been destroyed by an ototoxic drug neural changes related to hyperacusis may be
(carboplatin). Input/output functions were selective for specific neuron types. Steepened
reduced for the auditory nerve response (com- rate-level functions have also been recorded
pound action potential) reflecting supra­ from the DCN of guinea pigs 2 weeks after
threshold hearing impairment, but were near- a TTS induced by noise exposure (Dehmel
normal in the IC and normal or supranormal et al., 2012). Although the neuron types con-
in the auditory cortex, revealing an increase in tributing to this effect were not specified, slope
gain as the recording site ascended the auditory was steepened over a wide frequency range and
pathway (also see Syka, Rybalko, & Popelář, were steeper compared to unexposed controls
1994). HSP could increase gain by increasing in neurons with center frequencies below as
presynaptic neural transmitter release, modify- well as in the range where tinnitus behavior
ing receptors in the post-synaptic membrane of was expressed in the animals (Dehmel et al.,
the affected neurons, or by modifying the in- 2012). These observations suggest that the
trinsic response of the neuron to its inputs, or mechanisms underlying increased central gain
all three mechanisms (Noreña & Farley, 2013). may be distinguishable from those underlying
Whiting, Moiseff, and Rubio (2009) found that tinnitus on the basis of their frequency profile.
synapses on bushy cell somata in the VCN and In addition to its expression in many
the basal dendrites of fusiform cells in DCN levels of the auditory pathway, gain enhance-
upregulated their excitatory AMPA receptor ment occurs at different time scales and even
subunits while inhibitory synapses reduced in structures outside of classical auditory path-
their expression of glycine subunits within ways (Auerbach, Rodrigues, & Salvi, 2014).
24 hours after monaural ear-plugging. Synapses Computational studies suggest that a homeo-
on the apical dendrites of fusiform cells were static mechanism that maintains the average
not affected, which distinguishes these plastic rate of firing in a network of neurons can in­
modifications from those produced by STDP crease the spontaneous activity of the network
in animals expressing behavioral evidence of as well as restore driven responses of the net-
tinnitus. The modifications appeared to reflect work (Schaette & Kempter, 2006; Domin-
a mechanism that balances the response of au- guez et al., 2006; Schaette & McAlpine, 2011;
ditory pathways to asymmetric inputs from Chrostowski et al., 2011). However, supra-
the ear to support sound localization based on normal driven responses are needed for hyper­
interaural level differences. In contrast, other acusis. Descending projections from the audi-
changes in the VCN described by Cai, Ma, tory cortex and other structures are a further
and Young (2009) may be more closely related factor that can modulate rate-level functions
to hyperacusis. Non-primary like neurons in underlying hyperacusis (Popelár, Nwabueze-
this structure (those exhibiting chopper re- Ogbo, & Syka et al., 2003). Asokan, Williamson,
sponse profiles) showed steepened rate-level and Polley (2017) found that feedback from
functions and elevated discharge rates at high cortico-collicular neurons to the IC scaled with
sound intensities after noise trauma. Notably, sound intensity and increased above baseline
the frequency response of the affected neurons following TTS, revealing hyperacusis-like be-
was also broadened at high sound intensities. havior in both structures. Neuromodulators
Output from these VCN neurons distributes (Coomber et al., 2015) and interactions with
up the auditory pathway and may alter rate- glial cells (Stellwagen & Malenka, 2006) are
6. Tinnitus and Hyperacusis: Relationship, Mechanisms, and Initiating Conditions 91

also involved in HSP and might spread the Caspary, 2012; Salvi et al., 2017) of animals
bandwidth of its effects. Overall one can say exhibiting TTS or PTS after noise trauma,
that while HSP is a key process in hyperacusis, and at the level of the auditory cortex in hu-
many questions remain as to its specific mech- man tinnitus sufferers (Sedley et al., 2015a).
anisms and multiple sites of action. In the study by Llano et al. (2012) noise-
If HSP is involved in the development exposed mice showed a 68% increase in the
of hyperacusis, it must by definition fail to amplitude of the cortical response evoked by
produce correct neural activity levels for mod- electrical stimulation of thalamocortical affer-
erate to high level sounds. One possibility is ents compared to controls, which correlated
that HSP tries to correctly increase central gain with behavioral evidence of tinnitus. Tinnitus
to compensate for deafferentation but sub- animals also exhibited reduced sensitivity to
sequently enables STDP to create increased GABAergic blockade revealing diminished in-
neural connectivity and too much overall gain hibitory tone, consistent with increased driven
(Chrostowski, 2012). Another potential mech- responses. Notably, the decrease in GABA
anism is that HSP works to normalize neural sensitivity was greater at cortical sites distant
activity for low level sounds but in doing so from the site of stimulation, suggesting that
applies too much gain for moderate to high inhibition conveyed by cortico-cortical pro-
level sounds. A hint that this could be occur- jections was affected. Similarly, Chen, Radzi-
ring in some hyperacusis patients is the fact won, Auerbach, and Salvi (2017) observed
that some individuals experience a reduction increased rate/level functions (hyperacusis) in
in hyperacusis through the use of a low-level the auditory cortex of noise-exposed rats at to-
sound generator. This and other sound ther- notopic frequencies below the noise exposure
apies for hyperacusis will be discussed further band (16 to 20 kHz) revealing hyperactivity
in a later section of this chapter. An alternative in this region but shallow functions suggesting
explanation for hyperacusis development in hypoactivity at high frequencies. This pattern
some individuals may be a failure of HSP to of hyper- and hypo-responsiveness resembles
regulate STDP, which has been demonstrated responses evoked from primary auditory cor-
to also produce tinnitus in an animal model tex in human tinnitus sufferers (Roberts et al.
(Rüttiger et al., 2013). If this mechanism is 2015). In this study the auditory steady-state
widespread in the human hyperacusis patient response evoked in the primary auditory cor-
population, it might explain why tinnitus is tex of tinnitus subjects by 5 kHz 40-Hz AM
so prevalent in this population, whereas there sound (5 kHz in the tinnitus and hearing loss
are multiple mechanisms described earlier that region of the subjects) was smaller than that
could lead to abnormal spontaneous and syn- observed in hearing-level-matched controls
chronous activity (tinnitus) without abnormal suggesting hypoactivity and reduced temporal
sound-driven activity (hyperacusis). coding in the 5 kHz region of the tinnitus sub-
Notwithstanding these possibilities, it jects. However, the results were reversed when
should be noted that loss of input from deaffer- the responses were evoked at 500 Hz, suggest-
entation could yield a net decrease in inhibition ing hyperactivity in the 500 Hz region of the
in cortical networks without plasticity neces- tinnitus group. One explanation of these find-
sarily being involved. Although its mechanism ings could be that hypoactivity in the tinnitus
is unclear, reduced GABAergic inhibition has region caused by hearing loss reduced lateral
been observed in VCN (Godfrey et al., 2015), inhibition of distant frequencies in the corti-
DCN (Wang et al., 2009), IC (Middleton et al., cal tonotopic map, such that neurons tuned to
2011), and auditory cortex (Llano, Turner, & more distant frequencies became hypersensitive.
92 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

A mechanism of this type could account for could be responsible for generating low-
the steepened LGFs seen below the audiomet- frequency (<4 Hz) delta and theta oscillations
ric cut-off frequency seen in the hyperacusis which have been recorded from the auditory
patients of Noreña and Chery-Croze (2007). cortex and several other brain regions from
It should be noted that overall hypoactivity in indwelling electrodes in a tinnitus patient
a frequency band reflecting peripheral cochlear (Sedley et al., 2015b) and may reflect disin-
impairment is not necessarily incompatible hibited interlaminar processing and synaptic
with localized synchronous activity giving a rescaling in the affected regions (Carracedo
tinnitus percept in that band. Alternatively, et al., 2013). There is evidence that increased
disinhibition of cortical networks may involve slow wave activity is associated with high fre-
distinctive events in the thalamus that have quency oscillations in primary auditory cortex
been associated with tinnitus and hyperacusis. that appear to relate to tinnitus percepts (Lli-
nas et al., 2005). High frequency oscillations
of cortical origin in the gamma range (~40 to
Convergence in the 70 Hz) have been found to correlate with tin-
Thalamus and Cortex nitus perception in tinnitus sufferers (Weisz
et al., 2007), although in the study of Sedley
One might expect that decreased inhibition et al. (2015b) gamma activity while present in
consequent on decreased auditory nerve activ- the primary auditory cortex of their patient
ity would be transmitted to the medial genic- appeared to be more closely related to tinnitus
ulate body (MGB) of the thalamus through suppression by forward masking than to the
the cochlear nucleus (VCN/DCN) and IC. At tinnitus percept itself. It was suggested that
the level of the MGB, however, there is little gamma activity is generated by neural net-
evidence of tinnitus-related decreases in GABA­ works as they update current sensory state in
ergic neurotransmission. Instead, Sametsky accordance with predictive coding models of
et al. (2015) observed an increase in tonic tinnitus (Sedley et al., 2016; Roberts, Husain, &
GABAergic inhibition in a subset of thalamic Eggermont, 2013; De Ridder, Vanneste, & Free­
projection neurons that was specific to animals man, 2014) and auditory perception (Winkler,
showing behavioral evidence of tinnitus. This Denham, & Nelkin, 2009).
effect requires an initiating condition, which Overall, current evidence suggests that
could be tinnitus-related neural synchrony slow-wave activity generated by deafferentation
inherited from subcortical pathways. Hyper- of MGB neurons disinhibits processing over
polarization also switched the affected MGB wide cortical areas potentially contributing to
neurons into a burst firing mode and enhanced hyperacusis and broadening its frequency pro-
their driven responses, the latter of which may file (Caspary & Llano, 2016). Faster gamma
potentially relate to hyperacusis (Langers, van oscillations recorded from the tinnitus region
Dijk, Schoenmaker, & Backes, 2007). The of the primary auditory cortex appear to reflect
tonic inhibition producing these effects was the processing of tinnitus-related neural activ-
mediated by extra-synaptic GABAA receptors, ity occurring there. Similar oscillatory changes
which is consistent with a role for this inhibi- (described as thalamocortical dysrhythmia by
tion in exerting a nonspecific modulatory ef- Llinás et al., 2005) have been observed in other
fect on cortical processing (Brickley & Mody, disconnection syndromes and may thus seem
2012). Caspary and Llano (2016) and others pathological, but this may not necessarily be
(Weisz et al., 2007) have proposed that the the case. In normal functioning, low frequency
switch to bursting activity in MGB neurons bursting activity distributed to the brain by
6. Tinnitus and Hyperacusis: Relationship, Mechanisms, and Initiating Conditions 93

thalamic neurons could be a teaching signal group that received low frequency sounds did
that integrates information in sensory path- not experience suppression of their tinnitus
ways with that represented in brain regions un- (Tass et al., 2012). In contrast, sounds cov-
derlying memory, emotion, and consciousness ering the tinnitus frequencies (3 to 12 kHz)
in order to support adaptive behavior. If this do interact with tinnitus neural activity in-
hypothesis is correct, the oscillatory dynam- ducing a degree of RI (Roberts et al., 2008;
ics and brain network activity seen in tinni- Terry, Jones, Davis, & Slater, 1983) particu-
tus (Sedley et al., 2015b; Husain & Schmidt, larly when bandwidth of the frequency match
2014) could be a prolongation of dynamics is close (Sedley et al., 2015b). Sound thera-
that occur during normal information process- pies utilizing sounds in the tinnitus frequency
ing on a much shorter time scale. In tinnitus range have been motivated by various princi-
the dynamics are prolonged, although possibly ples. One approach presents the constituent
at an adapted level, because they are driven by frequencies of complex sounds at variable time
persisting maladaptive changes that occur in au­ delays, aiming to fragment the oscillatory neu-
ditory pathways following deafferentation. ral activities believed to underlie tinnitus by
resetting their phases (Tass et al., 2012). Other
methods have engineered music to conform to
Implications for Treating the measured tinnitus spectrum (Li, Bao, &
Chrostowski, 2016) or the audiograms (Davis,
Hyperacusis and Tinnitus
Wilde, Steed, & Hanley, 2008), or to distrib-
ute lateral inhibition into the tinnitus region
Growing knowledge about the neural correlates by notching sounds closely around the dom-
of tinnitus and hyperacusis and the role of neu- inant tinnitus pitch (Stracke, Okamoto, &
ral plasticity provide a rationale for sound ther­ Pantev, 2010). Patients typically receive these
apies aimed at these conditions (see Chapters 14 treatments on the order of an hour or two per
and 15 for further discussion of this topic). day over a treatment period of a few weeks.
We briefly consider two approaches, one using Modest reductions in tinnitus loudness (mea-
passive or attended exposure to engineered en- sured by visual analog scales) and improved
vironmental sounds to suppress to tinnitus quality of life (assessed by questionnaires) have
and hyperacusis neural activity, and another been reported, with ~20 to 30% of patients
that exploits the timing rules of auditory- achieving improvements deemed to be clin-
somatosensory STDP to suppress this activity. ically meaningful in some studies. However,
no particular method has presented strong
evidence for improvement in a majority of
Exposure to patients using these acute treatment methods,
Environmental Sound particularly in relation to control groups (for
reviews of tinnitus see Hobson, Chisholm, &
While in principle experience with low fre- El Refaie, 2010; Roberts & Bosnyak, 2010;
quency sounds might be expected to distrib- Hoare, Kowalkowski, Kang, & Hall, 2011; for
ute lateral inhibition into the tinnitus fre- a review of SLT, see Fournier, Schönwiesner, &
quency region, such sounds give poor or no Hébert, 2014).
residual inhibition (RI) (Roberts et al., 2008) There is evidence, however, that long-term
indicating little interaction with neural activ- exposure to low-level, complex sounds cover-
ity occurring in the tinnitus region. This may ing the tinnitus frequency region can be ben-
explain why in one study of sound therapy a eficial for tinnitus and hyperacusis. Noreña
94 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

and Eggermont (2005, 2006) found that proved for sounds below as well as above the
rearing cats in complex background sound cut-off frequency in the audiogram, consistent
24 hours per day for several weeks after a sin- with the broad frequency profile often observed
gle noise trauma prevented tonotopic map in hyperacusis patients. Improvement persisted
reorganization and suppressed putative neural for 1 month after the end of therapy and exhib-
correlates of tinnitus otherwise induced by ited a gradual return to baseline subsequently.
acoustic trauma. These benefits were observed The mechanisms underlying suppression of
when the background environment covered the neural responsiveness over such a wide range
hearing loss frequencies but not when sounds of exposure durations are presently not well
were presented below these frequencies. Be- understood (Malmierca, Anderson, & Antunes,
cause improvement was also seen in the ABR 2014; Wehr & Zador, 2005). Forms of HSP or
audiogram, the therapeutic environment ap- stimulus-specific habituation are believed to be
peared to have supported repair of the cochlear involved.
transduction mechanism. Other findings sug-
gest that potential benefits may not be limited
to the auditory periphery. Passive exposure Bimodal Auditory-Somatosensory
to complex low-level sound profoundly sup- Treatments for Tinnitus
pressed the responsivity of cortical neurons to
the exposure frequencies when delivered to nor- A different and more recent approach has been
mal hearing cats 24 hours per day over weeks to use combined auditory-somatosensory stim-
and months (Noreña, Gourévitch, Aizawa, & ulation in an attempt to suppress tinnitus. Ini-
Eggermont, 2006; Pienkowski & Eggermont, tially this approach was motivated by evidence
2011). Concurrently, responsivity increased that somatosensory, auditory, and visual path-
outside of the exposed frequency band reflect- ways interact in multisensory regions of the
ing reduced lateral inhibition delivered from the brain (Hamilton et al., 2016; Oertel & Young,
exposed region. Roberts et al. (2015) reported 2004) and by a belief that modulation of deep
similar on- and off-frequency effects in EEG brain structures by multimodal stimulation
responses evoked from primary auditory cortex might alter the neural substrate of tinnitus
in human tinnitus sufferers, when the tinnitus (Offutt, Ryan, Konop, & Lim, 2014). More
subjects experienced deep RI after exposure to recently, the studies of STDP in the DCN de-
a masker overlapping their tinnitus percepts. scribed above have established a scientific foun-
These and other results (Galazuk, Voytenko, & dation for this approach (Tzounopoulos, Rubio,
Longenecker 2017) suggest that forward sup- Keen, & Trussell, 2007; Zhao & Tzounopou-
pression of neural responses underlying tinni- los, 2011; Dehmel et al., 2012; Koehler &
tus may scale over an extraordinarily wide range Shore, 2013; Wu, Martel, & Shore, 2016). Al-
of exposure durations (from seconds to several though the timing rules of STDP induced by
weeks), engaging mechanisms that could be paired auditory-somatosensory stimulation are
beneficial for tinnitus. Although to date this altered in animals expressing neural correlates
has not been attempted for tinnitus, it has been of tinnitus, some bimodal timing orders and in-
attempted for hyperacusis. Noreña and Chery- tervals depress these correlates, presumably by
Croze (2007) found that listening for 15 weeks inducing LTD. Marks et al. (2018) found that
to a just-audible complex high frequency sound auditory and somatosensory stimulation paired
for a few hours a day improved SLT by as much at one such interval for 20 minutes daily for
as ~15 dB in hyperacusis patients. SLT was im- 25 sessions suppressed neural and behavioral
6. Tinnitus and Hyperacusis: Relationship, Mechanisms, and Initiating Conditions 95

evidence of tinnitus in their animal model that may or may not be hidden from the clini-
while three control procedures (auditory alone, cal audiogram. Injury to ANFs exhibiting low
somatosensory alone, or no stimulation) did thresholds and high rates of spontaneous fir-
not. Remarkably, the same bimodal therapy ing may be particularly important for tinnitus,
applied to tinnitus patients (30 min/day) in which can develop even when the extent of
a blinded cross-over design (four weeks of bi- damage to such fibers is not sufficient to affect
modal treatment followed by auditory alone or hearing thresholds. If the high spontaneous
vice versa, separated by wash-out of the same activity of these ANFs is sufficiently dimin-
duration) progressively reduced tinnitus mea- ished or lost, maladaptive hypersynchronous
sured by psychoacoustic loudness-matching neural activity driven by STDP or enabled by
(Marks et al., 2018). Tinnitus loudness de- HSP mechanisms may be unleashed in the
creased after bimodal but not unimodal treat- affected frequency regions of central auditory
ment to an extent (12.2 dB) that gave tinnitus structures generating tinnitus and giving it its
elimination in two patients. Tinnitus distress distinctive frequency profile. Tinnitus-related
assessed by questionnaire also decreased signifi- neural activity also appears to hyperpolarize
cantly but only when preceded by a bimodal a subset of neurons in the thalamic medial
treatment series. During bimodal treatment geniculate nucleus, switching these neurons
somatosensory stimulation was presented at a into a burst firing mode and generating low-
level that was generally imperceptible, which frequency oscillations that have been recorded
suggests that auditory stimulation alone was over wide brain areas in tinnitus patients.
an effective control procedure for non-specific Burst firing and slow oscillations may serve
treatment effects. Because tinnitus reduction under normal conditions to integrate auditory
accumulated gradually over four weeks it must information with information contained in
have persisted on the order of days, although other brain regions to support adaptive behav-
tinnitus loudness returned to baseline within ior. However, in tinnitus (and other discon-
the first week of the wash-out period. nection syndromes) low frequency oscillations
Further investigations will be needed to may persist at some level of strength, driven
confirm the present findings, assess their pre- by aberrant signals inherited from subcortical
dicted dependence on specific bimodal inter- pathways.
vals, and determine whether additional expo- These neural changes may also occur in
sure will prolong its benefits. At present it is hyperacusis patients, most of whom experience
not known whether bimodal therapy will mod- tinnitus. However, the frequency profile of
ify SLT, which may involve additional path- hyperacusis is broader than that of tinnitus,
ways and different plasticity mechanisms. appearing at frequencies where tinnitus is ex-
perienced but also well below this frequency
range implying that some of its mechanisms
are different. HSP mechanisms activated by de-
Summary and Conclusion
afferentation are believed to amplify the input/
output functions of the affected neurons in or-
We have discussed evidence suggesting that der to maintain their responding and support
tinnitus and hyperacusis measured as reduced auditory functions such as sound localization
SLT follow a similar time course of develop- based on interaural level differences. However,
ment and share a common initiating condi- in so doing these mechanisms may also amplify
tion, which is cochlear hearing impairment driven responses to moderate and higher-level
96 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

sounds, giving rise to hyperacusis. The broad- Eavey, 2010). However, it is notable that the
ened frequency range of hyperacusis may be a majority of adolescents examined by Sanchez
consequence of several possible mechanisms et al. (2016) did not experience tinnitus or al-
including the frequency-nonspecific effects of tered SLT during psychoacoustic assessment,
altered HSP on central gain and STDP, neuro- even though their listening habits appeared to
modulators released by tinnitus neural activity, be as risky as those of the adolescents who were
reduced lateral inhibition conveyed from the affected. Why this was so is another impor­
tinnitus region, or disinhibition of cortical net­ tant area for study. Individual differences in
works by low-frequency bursting activity in the responsivity of olivocochlear efferents is one
thalamic neurons. Areas outside of the cortical factor that could modulate driven responses in
tonotopic tinnitus frequency region appear to the auditory pathway (Kujawa & Liberman,
be disinhibited by these or other mechanisms, 1999), potentially explaining why some ado-
which may implicate cortical processing as one lescents with persistent tinnitus reported LDLs
source of a broadened frequency profile for similar to those of adolescents without tinnitus
hyperacusis. during psychoacoustic testing. Prior exposure
Novel treatments are being evaluated to to levels of background sound that are not syn-
suppress or renormalize the maladaptive neu- aptopathic have also been reported to reduce
roplastic changes believed to underlie tinni- cochlear injuries arising from subsequent noise
tus and hyperacusis. One approach involves trauma in animal models (Canlon, Borg, &
exposure to background sounds covering the Flock, 1988; Canlon & Fransson 1995; Roy
region of hearing impairment to suppress these et al., 2016). The relationship of noise exposure
changes, while another delivers paired auditory- history to tinnitus and hyperacusis appears to
somatosensory stimulation using an order and be complex and modulated by individual dif-
timing interval believed to induce LTD in au- ference factors that are not well understood.
ditory pathways. Because cochlear pathology
is likely a triggering factor in tinnitus and hy-
peracusis, periodic retraining may be required
to prevent the recurrence of maladaptive plas-
Abbreviations
ticity in central auditory structures. A chal-
lenge for the future is to determine whether
these procedures (which potentially draw on ABR Auditory brainstem response
different plasticity mechanisms) can aid in- AM Amplitude modulation
dividuals such as the young tinnitus and hy- ANF Auditory nerve fiber
peracusis sufferer whose history prefaced this ASR Acoustic startle response
chapter. Of course, a preferable solution would BOLD Blood oxygenation level
be to prevent hearing injuries altogether, or at dependent (f MRI imaging)
least detect them early in order to avoid accel- CN Cochlear nucleus (VCN+DCN)
erating age-related declines in hearing (Rob- DCN Dorsal cochlear nucleus
erts, Martin, & Bosnyak, 2010). The presence DPOAE Distortion product otoacoustic
of persisting tinnitus and reduced SLT affect- emissions
ing more than 14% of the adolescents in the EEG Electroencephalography
study of Sanchez et al. (2016) is a potential EFR Envelope following response
early warning sign of cochlear injuries that HT High threshold
may be hidden from the audiogram (Harri- HSP Homeostatic plasticity
son, 2008; Shargorodsky, Curhan, Curhan, & HSR High spontaneous rate
6. Tinnitus and Hyperacusis: Relationship, Mechanisms, and Initiating Conditions 97

IC Inferior colliculus hyperacusis. Frontiers in Neurology, 5(October),


LDL Loudness discomfort level 206. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2014.00206.
LGF Loudness growth function Baguley, D. M. (2003). Hyperacusis. Journal of the
LTD Long-term depression Royal Society of Medicine, 96(December), 582–
LTP Long-term potentiation 585. https://doi.org/10.1258/jrsm.96.12.582.
Basura, G. J., Koehler, S. D., & Shore, S. E. (2015).
MGB Medial geniculate body
Bimodal stimulus timing dependent plasticity
PTS Permanent threshold shift in primary auditory cortex is altered after noise
RI Residual inhibition exposure with and without tinnitus. Journal of
SLT Sound level tolerance Neurophysiology, jn.00319.2015. https://doi.org
SFR Spontaneous firing rate /10.1152/jn.00319.2015.
STDP Spike-timing-dependent Bauer, C. A., Brozoski, T. J., & Myers, K. (2007).
plasticity Primary afferent dendrite degeneration as a
TEOAE Transient otoacoustic emissions cause of tinnitus. Journal of Neuroscience Re-
TTS Temporary threshold shift search, 85, 1489–1498.
VCN Ventral cochlear nucleus Bauer, C. A., Wisner, K. W., Baizer, J. S., & Bro-
zoski, T. J. (2013). Tinnitus, unipolar brush
Acknowledgments. The authors acknowl- cells, and cerebellar glutamatergic function in
an animal model. PLoS ONE, 8(6). https://doi
edge the following institutions for financial
.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064726.
and in-kind support. In Canada (LER, ICB): Bharadwaj, H. M., Masud, S., Mehraei, G., Ver-
The Natural Sciences and Engineering Re- hulst, S., & Shinn-Cunningham, B. G. (2015).
search Council of Canada. In Brazil (TGS): Individual differences reveal correlates of hid-
Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de den hearing deficits. Journal of Neuroscience,
São Paulo, Aché, Biosom, Sancout Electronic 35(5), 2161–2172. https://doi.org/10.1523
Technology, and Fundação Companhia Ener­ /JNEUROSCI.3915-14.2015.
gética de São Paulo. Brickley, S. G., & Mody, I. (2012). Extrasynaptic
GABA A receptors: Their function in the CNS
and implications for disease. Neuron, 73(1),
23–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011
References .12.012.
Brotherton, H., Plack, C. J., Maslin, M., Schaette, R.,
& Munro, K. J. (2015). Pump up the volume:
Andersson, G., Lindvall, N., Hursti, T., & Carl- Could excessive neural gain explain tinnitus and
bring P. (2002). Hypersensitivity to sound hyperacusis? Audiology and Neurotology, 20(4),
(hyperacusis): A prevalence study conducted via 273–282. https://doi.org/10.1159/000430459.
the Internet and post. International Journal of Cai, S., Ma, W. L. D., & Young, E. D. (2009). Encod-
Audiology. ing intensity in ventral cochlear nucleus follow-
Asokan, M., Williamson, R. S., & Polley, D. B. ing acoustic trauma: Implications for loudness
(2017). Changes in corticocollicular feedback recruitment. JARO — Journal of the Association for
fol­lowing cochlear synaptopathy. Presented at the Research in Otolaryngology, 10(1), 5–22. https://
Annual Meeting of the Association for Research doi.org/10.1007/s10162-008-0142-y.
in Otolaryngology, Baltimore, MD (PS 631). Canlon, B., Borg, E., & Flock, Å. (1988). Protec-
Atencio, C. A., & Schreiner, C. E. (2010). Co- tion against noise trauma by pre-exposure to a
lumnar connectivity and laminar processing in low level acoustic stimulus. Hearing Research,
cat primary auditory cortex. PLoS ONE, 5(3). 34(2), 197–200. https://doi.org/10.1016/0378
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0009521. -5955(88)90107-4.
Auerbach, B. D., Rodrigues, P. V., & Salvi, R. J. Canlon, B., & Fransson, A. (1995). Morpholog-
(2014). Central gain control in tinnitus and ical and functional preservation of the outer
98 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

hair cells from noise trauma by sound condi- tinnitus-related hyperactivity? Journal of Neuro-
tioning. Hearing Research, 84, 112–124. science, 32(5), 1660–1671. https://doi.org/10
Carracedo, L. M., Kjeldsen, H., Cunnington, L., Jen- .1523/JNEUROSCI.4608-11.2012.
kins, A., Schofield, I., Cunningham, M. O., . . . De Ridder, D., Elgoyhen, A. B., Romo, R., & Lang-
Whittington, M. A. (2013). A neocortical delta guth, B. (2011). Phantom percepts: Tinnitus
rhythm facilitates reciprocal interlaminar inter- and pain as persisting aversive memory networks.
actions via nested theta rhythms. Journal of Neu- Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 8075–8080.
roscience, 33(26), 10750–10761. https://doi.org De Ridder, D., Vanneste, S., & Freeman, W.
/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0735-13.2013. (2014). The Bayesian brain: Phantom percepts
Caspary, D. M., & Llano, D. A. (2016). Auditory resolve sensory uncertainty. Neuroscience and
thalamic circuits and GABAA receptor func- Biobehavioral Reviews 44, 4–15.
tion: Putative mechanisms in tinnitus pathology. Diehl, P. U., & Schaette, R. (2015). Abnormal au-
Hearing Research. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares ditory gain in hyperacusis: Investigation with
.2016.08.009. a computational model. Frontiers in Neurology,
Chen, G.-D., Radziwon, K., Auerbach, B. D., & 6(JUN), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur
Salvi, R. (2017). Noise-induced hyperacusis/ .2015.00157.
recruitment in rats and the underlying hyper- Dominguez, M., Becker, S., Bruce, I., & Read, H.
activity in the central auditory areas. Presented (2006). A spiking neuron model of cortical cor-
at the Annual Meeting of the Association for relates of sensorineural hearing loss: Spontane­
Research in Otolaryngology, Baltimore, MD ous firing, synchrony, and tinnitus. Neural Com-
(PD 23). putation, 18(12), 2942–2958. https://doi.org
Chrostowski, M. (2012). Cortical Plasticity and /10.1162/neco.2006.18.12.2942.
Tinnitus. Ph.D. thesis, McMaster University. Eggermont, J. J. (1990). On the pathophysiology
http://hdl.handle.net/11375/12585. of tinnitus: A review and a peripheral model.
Chrostowski, M., Yang, L., Wilson, H. R., Bruce, Hearing Research, 48, 111–124.
I. C., & Becker, S. (2011). Can homeostatic Eggermont, J. J. (2012). The neuroscience of tinni­
plasticity in deafferented primary auditory cor- tus. Oxford, U.K.: Oxford University Press.
tex lead to travelling waves of excitation? Journal Eggermont, J. J., & Roberts, L. E. (2004). The neu-
of Computational Neuroscience, 30(2), 279–299. roscience of tinnitus. Trends in Neurosciences,
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10827-010-0256-1. 7(11), 676–682. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins
Coomber, B., Kowalkowski, V. L., Berger, J. I., .2004.08.010.
Palmer, A. R., & Wallace, M. N. (2015). Mod- Eggermont, J. J., & Roberts, L. E. (2015). Tinni-
ulating central gain in tinnitus: Changes in ni- tus: Animal models and findings in humans.
tric oxide synthase in the ventral cochlear nu- Cell and Tissue Research, 361(1), 311–336.
cleus. Frontiers in Neurology, 6(MAR). https:// https://doi.org/10.1007/s00441-014-1992-8.
doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2015.00053. Formby, C., Sherlock, L. P., & Gold, S. L. (2003).
Darrow, K. N., Maison, S. F., & Liberman, M. C. Adaptive plasticity of loudness induced by
(2006). Cochlear efferent feedback balances in- chronic attenuation and enhancement of the
teraural sensitivity. Nature Neuroscience, 9(12), acoustic background. Journal of the Acoustical
1474–1476. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1807. Society of America, 114(1), 55–58. https://doi
Davis, P. B., Wilde, R. A., Steed, L. G., & Hanley, .org/10.1121/1.1582860.
P. J. (2008). Treatment of tinnitus with a cus- Fournier, P., Schönwiesner, M., & Hébert, S. (2014).
tomized acoustic neural stimulus: A controlled Loudness modulation after transient and perma-
clinical study. Ear, Nose and Throat Journal, 87, nent hearing loss: Implications for tinnitus and
330–339. hyperacusis. Neuroscience, 283, 64–77. https://
Dehmel, S., Pradhan, S., Koehler, S., Bledsoe, S., doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.08.007.
& Shore, S. (2012). Noise overexposure alters Furman, A. C., Kujawa, S. G., & Liberman, M. C.
long-term somatosensory-auditory processing (2013). Noise-induced cochlear neuropathy is
in the dorsal cochlear nucleus—Possible basis for selective for fibers with low spontaneous rates.
6. Tinnitus and Hyperacusis: Relationship, Mechanisms, and Initiating Conditions 99

Journal of Neurophysiology, 110(3), 577–86. Heinz, M. G., & Young, E. D. (2004). Response
https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00164.2013. growth with sound level in auditory-nerve fi-
Galazyuk, A. V., Voytenko, S. V., & Longenecker, bers after noise-induced hearing loss. Journal of
R. J. (2017). Long-lasting forward suppression Neurophysiology, 91(2), 784–795. https://doi
of spontaneous firing in auditory neurons: Im- .org/10.1152/jn.00776.2003.
plication to the residual inhibition of tinnitus. Henry, J. A., Roberts, L. E., Ellingson, R. M., &
Journal of the Association for Research in Otolaryn- Thielman, E. J. (2013). Computer-automated
gology, 18(2), 343–353. https://doi.org/10.1007 tinnitus assessment: Noise-band matching,
/s10162-016-0601-9. maskability, and residual inhibition. Journal
Godfrey, D. A., Chen, K., Godfrey, M. A., Lee, of the American Academy of Audiology, 24(6),
A. C., Crass, S. P., Shipp, D., . . . Robinson, 486–504. https://doi.org/10.3766/jaaa.24.6.5.
K. T. (2015). Cochlear ablation effects on Hesse, L. L., Bakay, W., Ong, H. C., Anderson, L.,
amino acid levels in the chinchilla cochlear nu- Ashmore, J., McAlpine, D., . . . Schaette, R.
cleus. Neuroscience, 297, 137–159. https://doi (2016). Non-monotonic relation between noise
.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.03.055. exposure severity and neuronal hyperactivity in
Gu, J. W., Halpin, C. F., Nam, E.-C., Levine, the auditory midbrain. Frontiers in Neurology,
R. A., & Melcher, J. R. (2010). Tinnitus, di- 7(AUG), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur
minished sound-level tolerance, and elevated .2016.00133.
auditory activity in humans with clinically nor- Hickox, A. E., & Liberman, M. C. (2014). Is
mal hearing sensitivity. Journal of Neurophysiol- noise-induced cochlear neuropathy key to the
ogy (September 2010), jn.00226.2010. https:// generation of hyperacusis or tinnitus? Journal
doi.org/10.1152/jn.00226.2010. of Neurophysiology, 111(3), 552–564.
Gu, J. W., Herrmann, B. S., Levine, R. A., & Mel­ Hoare, D. J., Kowalkowski, V. L., Kang, S., & Hall,
cher, J. R. (2012). Brainstem auditory evoked po­ D. A. (2011). Systematic review and meta-
tentials suggest a role for the ventral cochlear nu- analyses of randomized controlled trials ex-
cleus in tinnitus. JARO — Journal of the Association amining tinnitus management. Laryngoscope,
for Research in Otolaryngology, 13(6), 819–833. 121(7), 1555–1564. https://doi.org/10.1002
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10162-012-0344-1. /lary.21825.
Guest, H., Munro, K. J., Prendergast, G., Howe, S., Hobson, J., Chisholm, E., & El Refaie, A. (2012).
& Plack, C. J. (2016). Tinnitus with a normal Sound therapy (masking) in the management
audiogram: Relation to noise exposure but no of tinnitus in adults. Cochrane Database of Sys-
evidence for cochlear synaptopathy. Hearing tematic Reviews, doi:10.1002/14651858.CD00
Re­search. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2016 6371.pub3.
.12.002. Husain, F.T., & Schmidt, S.A. (2014). Using rest-
Hamilton, C., D’Arcy, S., Pearlmutter, B. A., Cris­ ing state functional connectivity to unravel net-
pino, G., Lalor, E. C., & Conlon, B. J. (2016). works of tinnitus. Hearing Research, 307, 153–
An investigation of feasibility and safety of bi- 162.
modal stimulation for the treatment of tinnitus: Joris, P. X. (2009). Recruitment of neurons and
An open-label pilot study. Neuromodulation: Tech- loudness: Commentary on “encoding intensity
nology at the Neural Interface, 19(8), 832–837. in ventral cochlear nucleus following acoustic
https://doi.org/10.1111/ner.12452. trauma: Implications for loudness recruitment”
Harrison, R. (2008). Noise-induced hearing loss in by Cai et al. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. doi:
children: A “less than silent” environmental dan- 10.1007/s10162-008-0142-y. JARO—Journal
ger. Paediatric Child Health, 13(5), 377–382. of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology,
Hébert, S., Fournier, P., & Noreña, A. (2013). 10(1), 1–4. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10162-009
The auditory sensitivity is increased in tinnitus -0156-0.
ears. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(6), 2356–2364. Kaltenbach, J. A. (2011). Tinnitus: Models and
https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3461 mechanisms. Hearing Research, 276(1–2), 52–60.
-12.2013. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2010.12.003.
100 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

Knudson, I. M., & Melcher, J. R. (2016). Elevated Li, S., Kalappa, B. I., & Tzounopoulo, T. (2015).
acoustic startle responses in humans: relation- Noise-induced plasticity of KCNQ2/3 and
ship to reduced loudness discomfort level, but HCN channels underlies vulnerability and re-
not self-report of hyperacusis. JARO — Journal silience to tinnitus. eLife, 4(August2015), 1–23.
of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology, https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.0724.
17(3), 223–235. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10162 Lin, H. W., Furman, A. C., Kujawa, S. G., &
-016-0555-y. Liberman, M. C. (2011). Primary neural degen-
Koehler, S. D., & Shore, S. E. (2013). Stimulus eration in the guinea pig cochlea after reversible
timing-dependent plasticity in dorsal cochlear noise-induced threshold shift. JARO — Journal of
nucleus is altered in tinnitus. Journal of Neuro- the Association for Research in Otolaryngology,
science, 33(50), 19647–19656. https://doi.org 12(5), 605–616. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10162
/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2788-13.2013. -011-0277-0.
Kujawa, S. G., & Liberman, M. C. (2009). Adding Llano, D. A., Turner, J., & Caspary, D. M. (2012).
insult to injury: Cochlear nerve degeneration Diminished cortical inhibition in an aging
after “temporary” noise-induced hearing loss. mouse model of chronic tinnitus. Journal of
Journal of Neuroscience, 29(45), 14077. https:// Neuroscience, 32(46), 16141–16148. https://
doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2845-09.2009. doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2499-12.2012.
Kujawa, S. G., & Liberman, M. C. (1999). Long- Llinas, R., Urbano, F. J., Leznik, E., Ramirez, R. R., &
term sound conditioning enhances cochlear Van Marle, H. J. (2005). Rhythmic and dys-
sensitivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 82(2), rhythmic thalamocortical dynamics: GABA sys-
863–73. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm tems and the edge effect. Trends in Neurosciences.
.nih.gov/pubmed/10444683. 28, 325–333.
Langers, D. R. M., van Dijk, P., Schoenmaker, Lobarinas, E., Salvi, R., & Ding, D. (2016). Se-
E. S., & Backes, W. H. (2007). fMRI activa- lective inner hair cell dysfunction in chinchillas
tion in relation to sound intensity and loud- impairs hearing-in-noise in the absence of outer
ness. NeuroImage, 35(2), 709–718. https://doi hair cell loss. JARO — Journal of the Association
.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.12.013. for Research in Otolaryngology, 17(2), 89–101.
Lanting, C., Woźniak, A., van Dijk, P., & Langers, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10162-015-0550-8.
D. R. (2016). Tinnitus- and task-related differ- Ma, W. L. D., & Young, E. D. (2006). Dorsal co­
ences in resting-state networks. Adv Exp Med chlear nucleus response properties following
Biol. 894, 175–187. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319 acoustic trauma: Response maps and spontane­
-25474-6_19. ous activity. Hearing Research, 216–217, 176–
Levine, R. A., Abel, M., & Cheng, H. (2003). CNS 188. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2006.03.011.
somatosensory–auditory interactions elicit or Malmierca, M. S., Anderson, L. A., & Antunes,
modulate tinnitus. Exp. Brain Res. 153, 643–648. F. M. (2015). The cortical modulation of
Li, S. A., Bao, L., & Chrostowski, M. (2016). In- stimulus-specific adaptation in the auditory
vestigating the effects of a personalized, spec- midbrain and thalamus: A potential neuronal
trally altered music-based sound therapy on correlate for predictive coding. Frontiers in Sys-
treating tinnitus: A blinded, randomized con- tems Neuroscience, 9(March), 19. https://doi.org
trolled trial. Audiology and Neurotology, 296– /10.3389/fnsys.2015.00019.
304. https://doi.org/10.1159/000450745. Mancilla, J. G., & Manis, P. B. (2009). Two dis-
Li, S., Choi, V., & Tzounopoulos, T. (2013). tinct types of inhibition mediated by cartwheel
Pathogenic plasticity of Kv7. 2/3 channel activ- cells in the dorsal cochlear nucleus. Journal of
ity is essential for the induction of tinnitus. Pro- Neurophysiology, 102(May 2009), 1287–1295.
ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.91272.2008.
110, 9980–9985. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas Marks, K., Martel, D. T., Wu, C., Basura, G. J.,
.1216671110/-/DCSupplemental.www.pnas Roberts, L. E., Schvartz-Leyzac, K. C., & Shore,
.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1302770110. S. E. (2018). Auditory-somatosensory bimodal
6. Tinnitus and Hyperacusis: Relationship, Mechanisms, and Initiating Conditions 101

stimulation desynchronizes brain circuitry to Noreña, A. J., & Eggermont, J. J. (2006). En-
reduce tinnitus in guinea pigs and humans. Sci­­ riched acoustic environment after noise trauma
ence Translational Medicine, 10, eaal3175. abolishes neural signs of tinnitus. Neuroreport,
Melcher, J. R., Knudson, I. M., & Levine, R. A. 17(6), 559–563. https://doi.org/10.1097/0000
(2013). Subcallosal brain structure: Correlation 1756-200604240-00001.
with hearing threshold at supra-clinical frequen- Noreña, A. J., & Farley, B. J. (2013). Tinnitus-
cies (>8 kHz), but not with tinnitus. Hearing related neural activity: Theories of generation,
Research, 295, 79–86. https://doi.org/10.1016 propagation, and centralization. Hearing Res.,
/j.heares.2012.03.013. 295, 161–171.
Middleton, J. W., Kiritani, T., Pedersen, C., Turner, Noreña, A. J., Gourévitch, B., Aizawa, N., & Egger-
J. G., Shepherd, G. M. G., & Tzounopoulos, T. mont, J. J. (2006). Spectrally enhanced acous-
(2011). Mice with behavioral evidence of tinni- tic environment disrupts frequency representa-
tus exhibit dorsal cochlear nucleus hyperactivity tion in cat auditory cortex. Nature Neuroscience,
because of decreased GABAergic inhibition. Pro- 9(7), 932–939. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1720.
ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the Noreña, A. J. (2011). An integrative model of tin-
United States of America, 108(18), 7601–7606. nitus based on a central gain controlling neural
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1100223108. sensitivity. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Re­
Mühlau, M., Rauschecker, J. P., Oestreicher, E., views, 35(5), 1089–1109. https://doi.org/10.1016
Gaser, C., Röttinger, M., Wohlschläger, A. M., . . . /j.neubiorev.2010.11.003.
Sander, D. (2006). Structural brain changes in Noreña, A. J., & Chery-Croze, S. (2007). Enriched
tinnitus. Cerebral Cortex, 16(9), 1283–1288. acoustic environment rescales auditory sensitiv-
https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhj070. ity. Neuroreport, 18(12), 1251–1255. https://
Munro, K. J., Turtle, C., & Schaette, R. (2014). doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282202c35.
Plasticity and modified loudness following short- Oertel, D., & Young, E. D. (2004). What’s a cer-
term unilateral deprivation: Evidence of multiple ebellar circuit doing in the auditory system?
gain mechanisms within the auditory system. Trends in Neurosciences, 27(2), 104–110. https://
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 135(1), doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2003.12.001.
315–322. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4835715. Offutt, S. J., Ryan, K. J., Konop, A. E., & Lim,
Nelson, J. J., & Chen, K. (2004). The relationship of H. H. (2014). Suppression and facilitation of
tinnitus, hyperacusis, and hearing loss. Ear Nose auditory neurons through coordinated acous-
and Throat Journal, 83, 472–476. tic and midbrain stimulation: Investigating a
Noreña, A., Micheyl, C., Chery-Croze, S., & Col- deep brain stimulator for tinnitus. Journal of
let, L. (2002). Psychoacoustic characterization Neural Engineering, Dec; 11(6), 066001. doi: 10
of the tinnitus spectrum: Implications for the .1088/1741-2560/11/6/066001.
underlying mechanisms of tinnitus. Audiology Paul, B. T., Bruce, I. C., & Roberts, L. E. (2017).
and Neurootology, 7, 358–369. Evidence that hidden hearing loss underlies
Noreña, A. J., & Eggermont, J. J. (2003). Changes amplitude modulation encoding deficits in in-
in spontaneous neural activity immediately af- dividuals with and without tinnitus. Hearing
ter an acoustic trauma: Implications for neu- Research, 344, 170–182. https://doi.org/10.1016
ral correlates of tinnitus. Hearing Research, 183 /j.heares.2016.11.010.
(1–2), 137–153. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0378 Pienkowski, M., & Eggermont, J. J. (2011). Cor-
-5955(03)00225-9. tical tonotopic map plasticity and behavior. Neu­
Noreña, A. J. & Eggermont, J. J. (2005). En- roscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(10),
riched acoustic environment after noise trauma 2117–2128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev
reduces hearing loss and prevents cortical map .2011.02.002
reorganization. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(3), Pienkowski M., Tyler R. S., Roncancio, E. R., Jun,
699–705. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI H. J., Brozoski, T., Dauman, N., Coelho C. B.,
.2226-04.2005. Andersson, G., Keiner, A. J., Cacace, A. T.,
102 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

Martin, N., & Moore, B. C. J. (2014). A review Roberts, L. E., Husain, F. T., & Eggermont, J. J.
of hyperacusis and future directions: Part II: (2013). Role of attention in the generation and
Measurement, mechanisms, and treatment. modulation of tinnitus. Neuroscience and Biobe-
American Journal of Audiology, 23, 424–436. havioral Reviews, 37(8), 1754–1773. https://
Popelář, J., Nwabueze-Ogbo, F. C., & Syka, J. doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.07.007.
(2003). Changes in neuronal activity of the infe- Roberts, L. E., Moffat, G., Baumann, M., Ward,
rior colliculus in rat after temporal inactivation L. M., & Bosnyak, D. J. (2008). Residual inhi-
of the auditory cortex. Physiological Research, bition functions overlap tinnitus spectra and the
52(5), 615–628. region of auditory threshold shift. JARO — Jour-
Potashner S. J., Suneja, S. K., & Benson, C. G. nal of the Association for Research in Otolaryn-
(2000). Altered glycinergic synaptic activities gology, 9(4), 417–435. https://doi.org/10.1007
in guinea pig brain stem auditory nuclei after /s10162-008-0136-9.
unilateral cochlear ablation. Hearing Research Roberts, M. T., & Trussell, L. O. (2010). Mo-
147(2000), 125–136. lecular layer inhibitory interneurons provide
Pozo, K., & Goda, Y. (2010). Unraveling mecha- feedforward and lateral inhibition in the dorsal
nisms of homeostatic synaptic plasticity. Neu- cochlear nucleus. Journal of Neurophysiology,
ron, 66(3), 337–351. https://doi.org/10.1016 104(5), 2462–2473. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn
/j.neuron.2010.04.028. .00312.2010.
Qiu, C., Salvi, R., Ding, D., & Burkard, R. (2000). Roy, S., Ryals, M. M., Van Den Bruele, A. B.,
Inner hair cell loss leads to enhanced response Fitzgerald, T. S., & Cunningham, L. L. (2013).
amplitudes in auditory cortex of unanesthetized Sound preconditioning therapy inhibits oto-
chinchillas: Evidence for increased system gain. toxic hearing loss in mice. Journal of Clinical
Hearing Research, 139(1–2), 153–171. https:// Investigation, 123(11), 4945–4949. https://doi
doi.org/10.1016/S0378-5955(99)00171-9. .org/10.1172/JCI71353.
Rauschecker, J. P., Leaver, A. M., & Mühlau, M. Rüttiger, L., Singer, W., Panford-Walsh, R., Matsu-
(2010). Tuning out the noise: Limbic-auditory moto, M., Lee, S. C., et al. (2013). The reduced
interactions in tinnitus. Neuron, 66(6), 819– cochlear output and the failure to adapt the cen-
826. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.04 tral auditory response causes tinnitus in noise
.032. exposed rats. PLoS ONE 8(3), e57247. https://
Roberts, L. E., & Bosnyak, D. J. (2010). Auditory doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0057247.
training in tinnitus. In Møller, A., Kleinjung, T., Salvi, R., Sun, W., Ding, D., Chen, G.-D., Lobari-
Langguth, B., and De Ridder, D. (Eds.). Textbook nas, E., Wang, J., . . . Auerbach, B. D. (2017).
of Tinnitus (pp. 563–574). Humana-Springer. Inner hair cell loss disrupts hearing and cochlear
Roberts, L. E., Martin, W. H., & Bosnyak, D. J. function leading to sensory deprivation and en-
(2010). The prevention of tinnitus and noise- hanced central auditory gain. Frontiers in Neu-
induced hearing loss. In Møller, A., Kleinjung, T., roscience, 10(January), 1–14. https://doi.org/10
Langguth, B., and De Ridder, D. (Eds.). Textbook .3389/fnins.2016.00621.
of Tinnitus (pp. 563–574). Humana-Springer. Sametsky, E. A., Turner, J. G., Larsen, D., Ling, L.,
Roberts, L. E. (2016). Mechanism and time course & Caspary, D. M. (2015). Enhanced GABA
of tinnitus associated with hearing impairment. A-mediated tonic inhibition in auditory thal-
In D. Baguley and M. Fagelson (Eds.). Tinnitus: amus of rats with behavioral evidence of tin-
Clinical and research perspectives (pp. 13–33). nitus. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(25), 9369–
Plymouth, U.K.: Plural Press. 9380. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5054
Roberts, L. E., Bosnyak, D. J., Bruce, I. C., Gan- -14.2015.
der, P. E., & Paul, B. T. (2015). Evidence for Sanchez, T. G., Moraes, F., Casseb, J., Cota, J., Freire,
differential modulation of primary and non- K., & Roberts, L. E. (2016). Tinnitus is asso-
primary auditory cortex by forward masking in ciated with reduced sound level tolerance in
tinnitus. Hearing Research, 327, 9–27. adolescents with normal audiograms and oto-
6. Tinnitus and Hyperacusis: Relationship, Mechanisms, and Initiating Conditions 103

acoustic emissions. Scientific Reports, 6( January), athy with envelope following responses. JARO —
27109. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep27109. Journal of the Association for Research in Otolaryn-
Sanchez, T. G., Yupanque Guerra, G. C., Lorenzi, gology, 16(6), 727–745. https://doi.org/10.1007
M. C., Brandão, A. L., & Bento, R. F. (2002). /s10162-015-0539-3.
The influence of voluntary muscle contractions Shargorodsky, J., Curhan, G. C., & Farwell, W. R.
upon the onset and modulation of tinnitus. (2010). Prevalence and characteristics of tin-
Audiology and Neuro-Otology, 7(6), 370–375. nitus among U.S. adults. American Journal of
https://doi.org/10.1159/000066155. Medicine, 123(8), 711–718. https://doi.org/10
Schaette, R., & Kempter, R. (2006). Develop- .1016/j.amjmed.2010.02.015.
ment of tinnitus-related neuronal hyperactiv- Shargorodsky, J., Curhan, S. G., Curhan, G. C., &
ity through homeostatic plasticity after hearing Eavey, R. (2010). Change in prevalence of hear-
loss: a computational model. Eur. J. Neurosci., ing loss in U.S. adolescents. JAMA: The Journal of
23, 3124–3138. the American Medical Association, 304(7), 772–
Schaette, R., & McAlpine, D. (2011). Tinnitus 778. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yped.2010.12.008.
with a normal audiogram: Physiological ev- Sheldrake, J., Diehl, P. U., & Schaette, R. (2015).
idence for hidden hearing loss and computa- Audiometric characteristics of hyperacusis pa­
tional model, 31(38), 13452–13457. https:// tients. Frontiers in Neurology, 6(May), 1–7.
doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2156-11.2011. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2015.00105.
Schaette, R., Turtle, C., & Munro, K. J. (2012). Sherlock, L. P., & Formby, C. (2005). Estimates
Reversible induction of phantom auditory of loudness, loudness discomfort, and the au-
sensations through simulated unilateral hear- ditory dynamic range: Normative estimates,
ing loss. PLoS ONE, 7(6), 1–6. https://doi comparison of procedures, and test-retest reli-
.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0035238. ability. Journal of the American Academy of Au-
Sedlacek, M., & Brenowitz, S. D. (2014). Cell- diology 16, 85–100. doi:10.3766/jaaa.16.2.4.
type specific short-term plasticity at auditory Shi, L., Chang, Y., Li, X., Aiken, S. J., Liu, L., &
nerve synapses controls feed-forward inhibition Wang, J. (2016a). Coding deficits in noise-
in the dorsal cochlear nucleus, 8( July), 1–12. induced hidden hearing loss may stem from
https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2014.00078. incomplete repair of ribbon synapses in the co-
Sedley, W., Friston, K. J., Gander, P. E., Kumar, S., chlea. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 10(May), 1–6.
Griffiths, T. D., Shargorodsky, J., et al. (2016). https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2016.00231.
An integrative tinnitus model based on sensory Shi, L., Chang, Y., Li, X., Aiken, S., Liu, L., &
precision. Trends in Neurosciences, 39(12), 799– Wang, J. (2016b). Cochlear synaptopathy and
812. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2016.10.004. noise-induced hidden hearing loss. Neural Plas-
Sedley, W., Parikh, J., Edden, R. A. E., Tait, V., ticity. https://doi.org/10.1155/2016/6143164.
Blamire, A., & Griffiths, T. D. (2015a). Hu- Shi, L., Liu, L., He, T., Guo, X., Yu, Z., Yin, S., &
man auditory cortex neurochemistry reflects Wang, J. (2013). Ribbon synapse plasticity in
the presence and severity of tinnitus. Journal of the cochleae of guinea pigs after noise-induced
Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society silent damage. PLoS ONE, 8(12). https://doi
for Neuroscience, 35(44), 14822–14828. https:// .org/10.1371/journal.pone.0081566.
doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2695-15.2015. Shore, S. E., Roberts, L. E., & Langguth, B. (2016).
Sedley, W., Gander, P. E., Kumar, S., Oya, H., Ko- Maladaptive plasticity in tinnitus—triggers,
vach, C. K., Nourski, K. V., . . . Griffiths, T. D. mechanisms and treatment. Nature Reviews Neu-
(2015b). Intracranial mapping of a cortical tin- rology, 12(3), 150–160. https://doi.org/10.1038
nitus system using residual inhibition. Current /nrneurol.2016.12.
Biology, 25(9), 1208–1214. https://doi.org/10 Stefanescu, R. A., & Shore, S. E. (2017). Mus-
.1016/j.cub.2015.02.075. carinic acetylcholine receptors control base-
Shaheen, L. A., Valero, M. D., & Liberman, M. C. line activity and Hebbian stimulus timing-
(2015). Towards a diagnosis of cochlear neurop­ dependent plasticity in fusiform cells of the
104 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

dorsal cochlear nucleus. Journal of Neurophys- (2009). Plasticity at glycinergic synapses in


iology, 117, 1229–1238. dorsal cochlear nucleus of rats with behavioral
Stellwagen, D., & Malenka, R. C. (2006). Synap­ evidence of tinnitus. Neuroscience, 164(2), 747–
tic scaling mediated by glial TNF-alpha. Na- 759. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009
ture, 440(7087), 1054–1059. https://doi.org/10 .08.026.
.1038/nature04671. Wehr, M., & Zador, A. M. (2005). Synaptic mech-
Stracke, H., Okamoto, H., & Pantev, C. (2010). anisms of forward suppression in rat auditory
Customized notched music training reduces cortex. Neuron, 47(3), 437–445. https://doi.org
tinnitus loudness. Communicative and Integra- /10.1016/j.neuron.2005.06.009.
tive Biology, 3(3), 274–277. Weisz, N., Muller, S., Schlee, W., Dohrmann, K.,
Syka, J., Rybalko, N., & Popelář, J. (1994). En- Hartmann, T., & Elbert, T. (2007). The neural
hancement of the auditory cortex evoked re- code of auditory phantom perception. Journal
sponses in awake guinea pigs after noise expo- of Neuroscience, 27(6), 1479–1484. https://doi
sure. Hearing Research, 78(2), 158–168. https:// .org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3711-06.2007.
doi.org/10.1016/0378-5955(94)90021-3. Whiting, B., Moiseff, A., & Rubio, M. E. (2009).
Tang, Z.-Q., & Trussell, L. (2017). 5-HT differ- Cochlear nucleus neurons redistribute synap-
entially modulates the output of auditory and tic AMPA and glycine receptors in response to
multisensory inputs onto fusiform cells in the monaural conductive hearing loss. Neuroscience,
dorsal cochlear nucleus. Presented at the An- 163(4), 1264–1276. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
nual Meeting of the Association for Research .neuroscience.2009.07.049.
in Otolaryngology, Baltimore, MD (PS 270). Wienbruch, C., Paul, I., Weisz, N., Elbert, T., &
Tass, P. A., Adamchic, I., Freund, H. J., Von Stack- Roberts, L. E. (2006). Frequency organization
elberg, T., & Hauptmann, C. (2012). Counter­ of the 40-Hz auditory steady-state response in
acting tinnitus by acoustic coordinated reset normal hearing and in tinnitus. NeuroImage,
neuromodulation. Restorative Neurology and Neu­ 33(1), 180–194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuro
roscience, 30(2), 137–159. https://doi.org/10 image.2006.06.023.
.3233/RNN-2012-110218. Winkler, I., Denham, S. L., & Nelken, I. (2009).
Terry, A. M. P., Jones, D. M., Davis, B. R., & Modeling the auditory scene: predictive regu-
Slater, R. (1983). Parametric studies of tinnitus larity representations and perceptual objects.
masking and residual inhibition. Br. J. Audiol., Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(12), 532–540.
17, 245–256. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.09.003.
Turrigiano, G. G., & Nelson, S. B. (2008). Hebb Wu, C., Martel, D. T., & Shore, S. E. (2016). In-
and homeostasis in neuronal plasticity. Current creased synchrony and bursting of dorsal co-
Opinion in Neurobiology, 10, 358–364. http:// chlear nucleus fusiform cells correlate with tin-
dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00091-X. nitus. Journal of Neuroscience, 36(6), 2068–2073.
Tzounopoulos, T., Rubio, M. E., Keen, J. E., & https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3960-15
Trussell, L. O. (2007). Coactivation of pre- and .2016.
postsynaptic signaling mechanisms determines Yang, S., Weiner, B. D., Zhang, L. S., Cho, S.-J., &
cell-specific spike-timing-dependent plasticity. Bao, S. (2011). Homeostatic plasticity drives tin­
Neuron, 54(2), 291–301. https://doi.org/10.1016 nitus perception in an animal model. Proceed­
/j.neuron.2007.03.026. ings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
Vogler, D. P., Robertson, D., & Mulders, W. H. A. M. United States of America, 108(36), 14974–14979.
(2011). Hyperactivity in the ventral cochlear https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1107998108.
nucleus after cochlear trauma. Journal of Neu- Zeng, C., Nannapaneni, N., Zhou, J., Hughes, L. F.,
roscience: The Official Journal of the Society for & Shore, S. (2009). Cochlear damage changes
Neuroscience, 31(18), 6639–6645. https://doi the distribution of vesicular glutamate trans-
.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6538-10.2011. porters associated with auditory and nonaudi-
Wang, H., Brozoski, T. J., Turner, J. G., Ling, L., tory inputs to the cochlear nucleus. The Journal
Parrish, J. L., Hughes, L. F., & Caspary, D. M. of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society
6. Tinnitus and Hyperacusis: Relationship, Mechanisms, and Initiating Conditions 105

for Neuroscience, 29(13), 4210–4217. https:// ence, 31(9), 3158–3168. https://doi.org/10.1523


doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0208-09.2009. /JNEUROSCI.5303-10.2011.
Zeng, F. G. (2013). An active loudness model sug- Zheng, X., Giang, A., Vetsis, S., Bruce, I. C., &
gesting tinnitus as increased central noise and Voigt H. F. (2009). A computational modeling
hyperacusis as increased nonlinear gain. Hearing study of the effects of acoustic trauma on the
Research, 295(May), 172–179. https://doi.org dorsal cochlear nucleus. In: Vander Sloten, J.,
/10.1016/j.heares.2012.05.009. Verdonck, P., Nyssen, M., & Haueisen, J. (Eds.).
Zhao, Y., & Tzounopoulos, T. (2011). Physiolog­ical 4th European Conference of the International
activation of cholinergic inputs controls associa­ Federation for Medical and Biological Engineer-
tive synaptic plasticity via modulation of en- ing. IFMBE Proceedings, vol. 22. Berlin, Hei-
docannabinoid signaling. Journal of Neurosci­ delberg: Springer.
7
Hyperacusis:
Medical Diagnoses and
Associated Syndromes
Don McFerran

tive, and it is quite possible, indeed probable,


Introduction
that multiple mechanisms are at play.
The taxonomy of disorders of sound tol­
As our knowledge of disorders of sound tol­ erance is still unsatisfactory with at least two
erance increases it has become apparent that competing classifications (see also Chapter 1).
there is a family of conditions rather than a Jastreboff and Jastreboff (2002) divided disor­
single entity: this chapter deliberates on con­ ders of sound tolerance into four subgroups:
ditions as disparate as the exaggerated startle recruitment or abnormal loudness growth, at­
reflex of a child with Cri du Chat syndrome, tributed entirely to peripheral mechanisms
the heightened awareness of internal body secondary to outer hair cell loss in the cochlea;
sounds seen in Superior Semicircular Canal hyperacusis, defined as discomfort caused by
Dehiscence, supranormal hearing thresholds exposure to a sound that would not evoke a
following spinal anesthesia, and the auditory similar reaction in an average listener; miso­
pain reported in Acoustic Shock Syndrome phonia, defined as dislike or hatred of specific
or Meniere’s disease. Yet all have been termed sounds due to enhanced reactivity of the limbic
hyperacusis. There is no consensus on how to and autonomic systems; phonophobia, defined
assess disorders of sound tolerance: hyperacusis as a subsection of misophonia when fear is the
tests used by studies in this chapter include test­ dominant evoked emotion. Tyler et al. (2014)
ing the startle reflex, measuring loudness dis­ proposed a different classification comprised of
comfort levels, measuring audiometric thresh­ loudness hyperacusis, annoyance hyperacusis,
olds, patient report, parental report, structured pain hyperacusis, and fear hyperacusis. Nei­
interviews, and hyperacusis questionnaires. The ther of these classifications seems fully fit for
pathophysiological mechanisms are still puta­ purpose and there are situations that do not

107
108 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

seem to sit easily within either system. Formby, et al. (2014) has been reproduced accurately
Sherlock, Hawley, and Gold (2017) have shown by a patient information website, Hyperacusis
that sound tolerance can be improved in the Focus (2017). Unfortunately, the website in­
case of recruitment which should not be pos­ cludes no qualitative assessment of the copied
sible if it was entirely due to outer hair cell information—it is presented as established fact.
damage. Misophonia has been redefined by A search of the medical literature produced a
some workers to describe a group of patients long list of conditions with a possible link to
who experience anger, disgust, and anxiety in disorders of sound tolerance (Table 7–1).
response to their trigger sounds which are typi­ The evidence for a link between the con­
cally sounds such as chewing, lip-smacking, or ditions in Table 7–1 and disorders of sound
whistling. These patients display avoidant or tolerance is explored in more detail in the
aggressive behavior in response to the trigger following text. For many of these conditions
sounds and there have been calls to redefine the evidence suggesting a link is based on case
misophonia as a specific psychiatric condition reports or small case series. To date there have
(Schröder, Vulink, & Denys, 2013). been very few population-based studies on
There would appear to be, potentially, hyperacusis (Fabijanska, Rogowski, Bartnik,
considerable overlap between the categories & Skarzynski, 1999; Andersson, Lindvall,
proposed by Tyler et al. (2014): Is someone Hursti, & Carlbring, 2002; Paulin, Anders­
who has pain hyperacusis not also annoyed? son, & Nordin, 2016) and only one that
In any case, the question of subtypes of hyper­ has attempted to explore the potential co-
acusis is somewhat academic in the context of morbidities of impaired sound tolerance with
medical conditions associated with sound tol­ any degree of scientific rigor. Paulin et al.
erance disorders—in most of the studies dis­ (2016) used data supplied by participants in
cussed in this chapter no attempt was made the Västerbotten Environmental Health Study.
to subtype the associated sound tolerance dis­ This is an ongoing series of different studies
order(s). Generally, in the following text, the conducted on a sample of the population of
word hyperacusis is used as an umbrella term Västerbotten, a county of northern Sweden
to denote any impairment of sound tolerance. with a population of approximately 260,000.
Some older works used the word to signify A random sample of 8600 adults (age range
better than normal hearing thresholds. 18 to 79) was invited to participate. Of these,
Most review articles regarding hyper­ 8520 could be contacted and 3406 ultimately
acusis and other disorders of sound tolerance agreed to participate. In this subsection of
include lists or tables of medical conditions the study, participants were asked whether
associated with sound intolerance (Marriage & they had “been diagnosed with sound intol­
Barnes, 1995; Katzenell & Segal, 2001; Bagu­ erance by a physician” and whether they had
ley, 2003; Tyler et al., 2014; Pienkowski et al., a “hard time tolerating everyday sounds that
2014). Many such publications are now easily you believe most of the people can tolerate.”
available on the Internet, which means that There were 66 people who met the criterion
it is a simple process to cut and paste lists of for physician-diagnosed hyperacusis and 313
hyperacusis-associated medical conditions onto for self-reported hyperacusis. The responses
other information disseminating platforms. from these groups were compared with those
There­fore, the information is sometimes pre­ of a control group of 2995 people and data
sented to the public in an uncritical fashion. regarding a series of health conditions was
For example, the table of potentially associated collected for both the hyperacusis and con­
medical conditions described by Pienkowski trol groups. Odds ratios for the presence of
Table 7–1. Conditions with a suggested association with hyperacusis. The best level of evidence supporting each potential associa-
tion is denoted in the right-hand column. Source material regarding each condition is discussed in the text.

Disease Classification Disease or Condition Best Level of Evidence


Genetic Williams syndrome Controlled case series
GM2 gangliosidoses Tay-Sachs disease Uncontrolled case series
Sandhoff disease Uncontrolled case series
GM1 gangliosidosis, type 2 Case reports
Cri du Chat syndrome Uncontrolled case series
Otological Hearing loss Population study
Tinnitus Population study
Associated with failure of the Bell’s palsy Uncontrolled case series
stapedial reflex due to facial
nerve dysfunction
Ramsay Hunt syndrome Uncontrolled case series

109
Associated with failure of the Post-stapedectomy Uncontrolled case series
stapedial reflex due to direct
dysfunction of the stapedius
muscle
Specific otological conditions Superior canal dehiscence Uncontrolled case series
syndrome (SCDS)
Lateral semicircular canal Uncontrolled case series
dysplasia
Meniere’s disease Uncontrolled case series
Acoustic shock syndrome Uncontrolled case series
Following surgical Uncontrolled case series
correction of conductive
hearing loss: tympanoplasty,
stapedectomy.
Perilymph fistula Uncontrolled case series
Table 7–1. (continued )

Disease Classification Disease or Condition Best Level of Evidence


Neurological Migraine Population study
Other headaches Uncontrolled case series
Myasthenia gravis Uncontrolled case series
Multiple sclerosis Case reports
Spina bifida Controlled case series
Complex regional pain syndrome Population study
Chronic fatigue syndrome Population study
Cerebrovascular accidents Case reports
Neuropsychological Head injury Controlled case series

110
Posttraumatic stress disorder Population study
Neurodevelopmental Autism spectrum Controlled experimental study
Learning disability and stuttering Controlled experimental study
Attention deficit hyperactivity Population study
disorder
Psychological Depression Population study
Anxiety Population study
Panic disorder Population study
Obsessive compulsive disorder Uncontrolled case series
Eating disorder Uncontrolled case series
Endocrine Adrenocortical insufficiency Addison’s disease Controlled case series
Pan-hypopituitarism Controlled case series
Hyperthyroidism None
Growth hormone deficiency Controlled case series
Metabolic Pyridoxine deficiency None
Magnesium deficiency Animal research
Autoimmune Cogan’s syndrome Case report
Systemic lupus erythematosus Controlled case series
Musculoskeletal Back, joint, and muscle pain Population study
Temporomandibular disorder Controlled case series
(TMD)
Fibromyalgia Population study
Vascular Carotid aneurysm None
Middle cerebral artery aneurysm Case report
Gastrointestinal Irritable bowel syndrome Population study
Pharmacological Drug withdrawal Benzodiazepine Randomized controlled trial
Monoamine oxidase Uncontrolled experimental
inhibitor (MOAI) study

111
Drug side effects Case reports
Infective Lyme disease Case reports
Creutzfeldt Jacob disease Case reports
Neurosyphilis Case reports
Typhoid fever None
Typhus Case reports
Iatrogenic Spinal anaesthesia None
Environmental Multiple chemical sensitivity Population study
intolerance Non-specific building-related Population study
symptoms
Electromagnetic hypersensitivity Population study
112 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

hyperacusis in association with any of the our knowledge but as the authors freely ad­
health conditions were calculated. This was mitted, the study did have potential weak­
the first large-scale population-based study nesses: only 40% of the initial random sam­
to explore the medical conditions associated ple agreed to participate and nonparticipation
with hyperacusis and added considerably to was skewed toward those of younger age; this

Table 7–2. Prevalence and odds ratios (adjusted for age, sex, and level of education) for
participants in the population-based study undertaken by Paulin et al. (2016). Confidence
intervals (CI) in brackets.

Self-Reported Physician-Diagnosed
Hyperacusis n = 313 Hyperacusis n = 66
Medical Prevalence Adjusted Odds Prevalence Adjusted Odds
Condition (%) Ratio (95% CI) (%) Ratio (95% CI)
Post-traumatic 4.8 12.80 12.1 44.68
stress disorder (5.65–29.00) (16.52–120.87)
Generalized 5.1 11.50 10.6 31.69
anxiety disorder (5.40–24.51) (11.83–84.87)
Chronic fatigue 3.2 11.33 10.6 34.31
syndrome (4.48–28.00) (12.40–94.96)
ADHD 2.6 10.19
(3.73–27.83)
Tinnitus 24.0 8.63 43.9 16.55
(6.15–12.10) (9.43–29.04)
Exhaustion 14.7 5.05 18.2 7.12
syndrome (3.44–7.43) (3.61–14.03)
Depression 17.3 4.91 22.7 8.89
(3.44–7.01) (4.72–16.72)
Hearing 29.4 4.71 59.1 12.23
impairment (3.47–6.40) (6.89–21.70)
Panic disorder 3.5 2.57
(1.28–5.14)
Back/joint/ 23.4 2.47 39.4 3.84
muscle disorder (1.83–3.34) (2.26–6.52)
Fibromyalgia 4.2 2.19 10.6 5.08
(1.16–4.10) (2.17–11.87)
Irritable bowel 4.8 2.10 9.1 4.34
syndrome (1.17–3.77) (1.78–10.60)
Migraine 6.7 1.49 12.1 3.21
(0.92–2.42) (1.48–6.98)
7. Hyperacusis: Medical Diagnoses and Associated Syndromes 113

study relied on self-report of both hyperacusis Raveh, Apter, & Attias, 2006). The preva­
and medical conditions; no audiological as­ lence of hyperacusis in children with Wil­
sessment was conducted and there were there­ liams syndrome was reported to be as high as
fore no data on hearing threshold or loudness 95% (Klein et al., 1990). In a comprehensive
discomfort levels; the study made no attempt review, Attias, Raveh, Ben-Naftali, Zarchi,
to distinguish between different subtypes of and Gothelf (2008) identified various sound-
hyperacusis and made no attempt to deter­ related symptoms in Williams syndrome: au­
mine the impact of any hyperacusis. Despite ditory fascination defined as above normal
these limitations the study delivered some attraction to certain sounds; phonophobia;
very useful information. Overall, hyperacusis hyperacusis; strong attraction to music. Attias
was found to be more common in women, (2013) also supplied a critique on the under­
more common with advancing age, and pos­ lying pathophysiology of Williams syndrome:
sibly more common with higher levels of edu­ the genes coded by the missing portion of
cation. The overall point prevalence of hyper­ chromosome are known, leading to spec­
acusis in the study was approximately 1 in 10 ulation that the sound tolerance issues may
which agrees with several previous prevalence be linked to the elastin gene (ELN), general
studies (Fabijanska et al., 1999; Andersson transcription factor Iii gene (GTF 21), or do­
et al., 2002). Co-morbidities included hearing main kinase one gene (LMK1). In addition,
impairment, tinnitus, back/joint/muscle dis­ the majority of children with Williams syn­
orders, and various psychiatric conditions and drome have absent stapedial reflexes (Attias
functional somatic syndromes (Table 7–2). et al., 2008) and therefore could develop some
of their sound tolerance issues in a similar way
to other patients with abnormalities of the
acoustic reflex.
Individual Conditions
Associated with Disorders
of Sound Tolerance GM2 Gangliosidoses: Tay-Sachs,
Sandhoff, and GM2 Gangliosidosis
AB Variant
Genetic Conditions
Tay-Sachs disease is a rare autosomal recessive
Williams Syndrome genetic disorder caused by mutations in the
HEXA gene on chromosome 15. Sandhoff
Williams syndrome or Williams Beuren syn­ disease is a rare autosomal recessive genetic
drome is a genetic condition caused by dele­ disorder caused by mutations in the HEXB
tion of a portion of chromosome 7. Affected gene on chromosome 5: it is clinically indis­
individuals have endocrine abnormalities, mild- tinguishable from Tay-Sachs disease. These
to-moderate cognitive impairment, and in­ inherited defects lead to an accumulation of
creased risk of cardiovascular disease. Typ­ a molecule called GM2 ganglioside in the
ically, they have characteristic facial features brain. Affected babies appear to develop nor­
which are often described as elfin. Several mally initially but as the ganglioside accumu­
studies have identified abnormal sound toler­ lates, a variety of neurological problems ap­
ance in individuals with Williams syndrome pear and most affected individuals die in early
(Klein, Armstrong, Greer, & Brown, 1990; childhood. Milder juvenile and adult forms
Nigam & Samuel, 1994; Gothelf, Farber, of the condition can occur. Developmental
114 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

delay or regression is the usual mode of presen­ like cry in infancy. It is caused by partial de­
tation. Hyperacusis has been described (Gor­ letion of the short arm of chromosome 5.
don et al., 1988; Arisoy, Ozden, Ciliv, & Ozalp Cornish and Pigram (1996) explored the de­
1995; Karimzadeh et al., 2014) with Karimza­ velopmental and behavioral characteristics of
deh et al. (2014) reporting it in 7 (39%) of a 27 children (13 boys and 14 girls, mean age
series of 18 patients, comprised of 9 who had 8.3 years) with Cri du Chat syndrome. During
Tay-Sachs disease and 9 who had Sandhoff dis­ a structured interview with the families, the
ease. GM2 gangliosidosis AB variant disease is researchers completed a Society for the Study
an extremely rare autosomal recessive genetic of Behavioural Phenotypes questionnaire for
disorder that is clinically similar to Tay-Sachs each child. In 81.4% of the study group hy­
and Sandhoff diseases, caused by a mutation persensitivity to sensory stimuli including
of the GM2A gene on chromosome 5. Hyper­ sound was reported. The study was uncon­
acusis in association with AB variant disease has trolled and no robust evidence regarding the
been reported in this condition (Sheth et al., state of the auditory periphery was presented.
2016). The effect of Tay-Sachs on the auditory
periphery remains unclear.
Otological Conditions

GM1 Gangliosidosis Hearing Loss and Tinnitus

GM1 gangliosidosis is a rare inherited lyso­ In the population-based study undertaken by


somal storage disorder that progressively de­ Paulin et al. (2016) hearing loss and tinni­
stroys neurons in the central nervous system; tus were respectively the most common and
however, its effect on the cochlea is undeter­ second most common conditions co-morbid
mined. The disease is subdivided into three with hyperacusis. The association of tinnitus
types based on the age of onset of the con­ and hyperacusis tallies with previous work:
dition: early infantile (type 1); late infantile among people presenting with a primary
(type 2); and adult (type 3). Although the complaint of tinnitus, figures for the propor­
types differ in severity, their features overlap tion with co-morbid hyperacusis vary from
significantly. GM1 gangliosidosis is caused 40% (Fabijanska et al., 1999) to 63% (An­
by mutations in the GLB1 gene on chromo­ dersson, Vretblad, Larsen, & Lyttkens, 2001).
some 3 and is inherited in an autosomal re­ When people whose primary complaint is
cessive manner. Two siblings presented with hyperacusis are considered, 86% have co-
developmental arrest, gait disturbance, de­ morbid tinnitus (Anari, Axelsson, Eliasson, &
mentia, epilepsy, and hyperacusis in the sec­ Magnusson, 1999). The position regarding an
ond year of life and after investigation were association of hearing loss and hyperacusis
diagnosed with GM1 gangliosidosis type 2 has been less clear. Nelson and Chen (2004)
(Gascon, Ozand, & Erwin, 1992). argued that tinnitus, hyperacusis, and hearing
loss share a common pathophysiology but in
a population-based study Hannula Bloigu,
Cri du Chat Syndrome Majamaa, Sorri, and Mäki-Torkko (2011)
reported no statistically significant relation­
Cri du Chat syndrome is a congenital con­ ship between hyperacusis and average hearing
dition characterized by learning disability, threshold loss. However, this study revealed
spasticity, abnormal facial features, and a cat- that self-reported hearing impairment was far
7. Hyperacusis: Medical Diagnoses and Associated Syndromes 115

greater than hearing impairment defined by system rather than because of loss of the stape­
audiometric measurement in the range of 0.5 dial reflex. There are other suggestions that the
to 4 kHz. Self-reported hearing difficulties role of the stapedial reflex in the generation of
including tinnitus and hyperacusis seemed hyperacusis is not clear-cut: although it might
predictive of hearing impairment at high fre­ appear obvious that impairment of the acoustic
quencies (4 to 8 kHz). The epidemiological reflex could contribute to increased sound sen­
study described by Paulin et al. (2016) has sitivity there are several studies that contradict
methodological weaknesses and its reliance this supposition. Citron and Adour (1978) de­
on self-reported hearing loss was a shortcom­ scribed 48 consecutive patients with Bell’s palsy
ing. The term “hearing loss” covers a wide and reported that hyperacusis was unrelated to
range of different medical conditions. Nev­ stapedial muscle paralysis. Liriano Magalhães,
ertheless, this study did at least suggest that Barros, José, and Fukuda (2004) reported on
there is an association between hearing loss 18 patients with Bell’s palsy. All but one had
and hyperacusis. impaired stapedial reflexes but only one com­
plained of hyperacusis. Loudness discomfort
levels were performed in this study and were
Stapedial Reflex Dysfunction slightly reduced on the affected side. The au­
thors concluded that sound sensitivity was
The stapedius muscle contracts involuntarily in slightly increased but not enough to generate
response to high-intensity sound, stiffening the clinical symptoms in a majority of patients. A
ossicular chain and decreasing sound transmis­ cohort study by de Seta et al. (2014) reported
sion to the cochlea. Failure of this stapedial, or on 269 patients with Bell’s palsy. Hyperacusis
acoustic reflex would therefore be expected to was an uncommon symptom in this cohort—
allow more sound energy through the conduc­ the results were presented graphically, and an
tive mechanism to the cochlea and thereby in­ exact figure cannot be quoted but is approxi­
crease the range of sound levels that produce mately 5%, which is comparable to the preva­
discomfort in affected individuals. The reflex lence of hyperacusis in the general population.
involves the inner ear, auditory nerve, brain­
stem, facial nerve, stapedius muscle, and ossic­
ular chain: pathology of any component of this Superior Semicircular
arc can disrupt the reflex. Hyperacusis in associ­ Canal Dehiscence
ation with stapedial reflex dysfunction has been
reported with idiopathic or Bell’s palsy (Adour Superior semicircular canal dehiscence (SSCD)
& Wingerd, 1974; McCandless & Goering, is a condition first described by Minor, Solo­
1974; McCandless & Schumacher, 1979), her­ mon, Zinreich, and Zee (1998) in which the
pes zoster associated palsy or Ramsay Hunt syn­ bone overlying the superior semicircular canal,
drome (Wayman, Pham, Byl, & Adour, 1990), separating the canal from the middle cranial
myasthenia gravis (considered separately: see fossa, is deficient, creating a so-called third
below), and after surgical division of the ten­ window into the labyrinth. Patients may report
don of the stapedius muscle during sta­pedec­ increased awareness or sensitivity to bone con­
tomy (Mathisen 1969; McCandless & Goe­ duction sounds such as the sound of their own
ring, 1974). However, Mathisen (1969) re­ported voice or internal body sounds including eye
that stapedectomy-associated hyperacusis tends movements or heartbeat. This has been called
to improve with time suggesting that the hyper­ conductive hyperacusis and needs to be care­
acusis might arise within the central auditory fully contrasted to the more typical hyperacusis
116 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

triggered by external sound sources (see Chap­ Neck Foundation, Inc., 1995). For 6 of their
ter 1 for concerns regarding the terminology). 102 patients hyperacusis was the most trouble­
Other common symptoms include dizziness some symptom of their Meniere’s and presence
and sound-induced vertigo, or Tullio phenom­ of hyperacusis was associated with increased
enon. In addition to hypersensitivity to internal tinnitus severity. Although loudness recruit­
sounds, a case series has been published describ­ ment commonly accompanies Meniere’s, it
ing increased sensitivity to external sounds (Sal­ is not clear from the currently available evi­
iba et al., 2014). dence whether hyperacusis is more common
in Meniere’s than other cases of sensorineural
hearing loss and/or tinnitus.
Lateral Semicircular
Canal Dysplasia
Acoustic Shock
Modugno and Brandolini (2014) reported
three patients who presented with symptoms Toward the end of the twentieth century there
suggestive of superior semicircular canal de­ was a series of occupational health reports
hiscence including hyperacusis to external from call centers describing a cluster of audio­
sounds. CT scanning showed no evidence of vestibular symptoms seen after employees had
bony dehiscence but revealed that all three had been subjected to unexpected sound through
abnormal, dilated lateral semicircular canals. their telephone headsets. The causative sounds
were not necessarily at a level that would be
expected to harm the auditory system and the
Meniere’s Disease
common factor of the sounds was their very
short rise-time or suddenness. The condition
Meniere’s disease is a condition characterized
became known as acoustic shock syndrome or
by episodic attacks of vertigo, hearing loss,
simply acoustic shock (McFerran, 2016a). Pain,
tinnitus, and aural fullness. The exact patho­
tinnitus, perturbation of balance and hyper­
physiology remains unknown but both post­
acusis or auditory hypervigilance were the most
mortem studies and high-resolution MRI
commonly reported symptoms. Although the
scans have shown an association with endo­
condition remains enigmatic, Westcott et al.
lymphatic hydrops—an expansion of the en­
(2013) has suggested that the condition might
dolymphatic compartment of the inner ear.
be caused by abnormal contraction of the ten­
Several studies have reported hyperacusis in
sor tympani muscle. Although tonic tensor
Meniere’s. Hadj-Djilani and Gerster (1984)
tympani syndrome could explain some symp­
reported a retrospective study of 33 cases
toms such as pain and aural fullness, it is less
of Meniere’s: three out of four patients who
clear to see how such pathology could result in
were tested had a pain threshold to sound
hyperacusis.
of less than 100 dB HL. Herraiz, Tapia, and
Plaza (2006) reported on 102 patients with
Meniere’s who had been referred to a tinni­ Surgical Correction of
tus clinic. These patients met the diagnostic Conductive Hearing Loss
criteria for definite Meniere’s as described in
the American Academy of Otolaryngology Although it is common to hear anecdotal re­
Head and Neck Surgery guidelines (Ameri­ ports of excessively sensitive hearing after ven­
can Academy of Otolaryngology—Head and tilation tube or grommet insertion there is
7. Hyperacusis: Medical Diagnoses and Associated Syndromes 117

very little written in the medical literature light, odors, and sound. For those who ex­
regarding this. In a study of acoustic reflexes perience impaired sound sensitivity this dis­
in relation to audio sensitivity, Gordon (1986) comfort can occur during the prodrome or
reported a 9-year-old boy who developed the pain phase. There is conflicting evidence
sound sensitivity after grommet insertion. as to whether migraineurs have increased
Other surgical procedures that can improve sound sensitivity interictally. Woodhouse and
hearing include tympanoplasty and stapedec­ Drummond (1993) suggested this is not the
tomy. There is a single case report of hyper­ case and that patients experience excessive
acusis arising after tympanoplasty, necessitat­ loudness only when experiencing a migraine.
ing the use of a sound attenuating ear plug Vingen, Sand, and Stovner (1999), using dif­
(Stange, Mir-Salim, & Berghaus, 2001). Hyper­ ferent methodology, stated that migraineurs
acusis following stapedectomy is discussed have increased sensitivity even when headache
above. free. Sound hypersensitivity worsens during
the headache period but is not related to the
Perilymph Fistula presence or absence of migrainous aura.
Paulin et al. (2016) demonstrated that
Perilymph fistula refers to an abnormal con­ migraine is a risk factor for hyperacusis though
nection between the inner ear and the middle the odds ratio was relatively modest (1.49 for
ear. It has been reported following ear surgery, self-reported hyperacusis; 3.21 for physician-
head injury, and barotrauma. More contro­ diagnosed hyperacusis). For a common com­
versially, it has been suggested that the condi­ ponent symptom of a common condition
tion may arise spontaneously (Althaus, 1977). there has been remarkably little work done to
Symptoms are similar to other inner ear pa­ characterize the nature of the altered sound
thologies such as Meniere’s disease. A series of sensitivity that people with migraine experi­
40 patients who had received a diagnosis of ence. The increased sound sensitivity is gener­
idiopathic perilymph fistula was reported by ally called phonophobia; using the alternative
Fukaya and Nomura (1988) and in this series classification of hyperacusis proposed by Tyler
62.5% of patients presented with hyperacusis. et al. (2014) it has been labeled a form of fear
Given the skepticism regarding the concept of hyperacusis. Vingen et al. (1999) defined mi­
spontaneous perilymph fistula and the lack of grainous phonophobia as “hypersensitivity to
other substantiating evidence, these reports auditory stimuli, involving a feeling of discom­
must be interpreted with caution. fort or pain.” There was no mention of fear or
phobia in this description. Using audiological
definitions of sound tolerance disorders (Jas­
treboff & Jastreboff, 2002), this definition of
Neurological Conditions migrainous phonophobia would probably be
best regarded as hyperacusis or, in the alterna­
Migraine tive classification of Tyler et al. (2104), pain
hyperacusis. A non-scientific straw poll by the
Migraine affects 14.70% of the global popu­ author of this chapter suggested that people
lation (Vos et al., 2010). Classically, sufferers with migraine find that sounds seem louder
experience episodic headache affecting one than normal and they become irritated or an­
half of the head that may be accompanied by noyed by sound during the headache period.
nausea, vomiting, and increased sensitivity to None of the people questioned described a
118 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

fear of sound. This is clearly an area that needs system and the potential for involvement of
exploration. the auditory system has long been recognized
(Von Leden & Horton, 1948). Cases of hy­
Other Forms of Headache peracusis in association with multiple sclero­
sis have been reported (Weber, Pfadenhauer,
Increased sensitivity to sound has been re­ Stöhr, & Rösler, 2002).
ported in association with other types of
headache. Patients with cluster headache have Spina Bifida
been reported to have increased sound sensi­
tivity during the active phase of their condi­ Spina bifida is a developmental defect in
tion but not when the condition is inactive which babies are born with incomplete clo­
(Vingen, Pareja, & Stovner, 1998). Patients sure of the spinal column. Neural tissue may
with tension-type and cervicogenic headache be exposed or may be covered by soft tissue.
have also been observed with increased sen­ In the cases where neural tissue is exposed
sitivity to sound, though in these conditions there is often associated hydrocephalus. Both
the sensitivity is elevated even during the genetic and nongenetic factors are thought
headache-free period (Vingen et al., 1999). to contribute to the etiology of the condi­
tion. Hyperacusis has been reported in chil­
Myasthenia Gravis dren with spina bifida. In a controlled case se­
ries Oen, Begeer, Staal-Schreinemachers, and
Myasthenia gravis is a rare autoimmune dis­ Tijmstra (1997) used oral interview of parents
ease affecting nerve synapses, leading to skel­ and a nondisclosed questionnaire to inves­
etal muscle weakness. The stapedius muscle tigate 50 babies with spina bifida, reporting
is vulnerable to this pathology, and Laurian, hyperreactivity to sound in 50%.
Laurian, Sadov, Strauss, and Kalmanovitz
(1983) showed abnormalities of the stapedial Complex Regional Pain Syndrome
reflex in myasthenic patients with raised sta­
pedial reflex thresholds and decreased ampli­ Complex regional pain syndrome is a form of
tudes. No clinical correlation to loudness intol­ chronic pain that usually follows injury. Some
erance was made in this study. Morioka, Neff, affected patients develop movement disorders
Boisseranc, Hartman, and Cantrell (1976) including tremor, myoclonus, or dystonia. De
reported a series of six patients who had my­ Klaver et al. (2007) evaluated the audiological
asthenia. Hyperacusis was noted together with function of 40 patients with complex regional
raised stapedial reflex thresholds and both fac­ pain syndrome related dystonia. Peripheral au­
tors improved after treatment. ditory function was normal, and 38% reported
hyperacusis.
Multiple Sclerosis
Chronic Fatigue Syndrome
Multiple sclerosis is a demyelinating condition
of the central nervous system. The insulating Chronic fatigue syndrome or myalgic enceph­
myelin sheaths of nerve fibers are damaged, alomyelitis (ME) is a condition characterized
resulting in impaired conduction of neural by persistent severe fatigue and a variety of
signals. Plaques of demyelination can occur other symptoms including memory and con­
at multiple locations in the central nervous centration issues, joint and muscle pains, and
7. Hyperacusis: Medical Diagnoses and Associated Syndromes 119

headaches. Although various hypotheses exist, person is exposed to a traumatic event. Crite­
the exact pathophysiology remains unclear. ria for making a diagnosis of PTSD are com­
There have been previous suggestions that plex and include exposure to a traumatic event,
hyperacusis is more common in this condi­ intrusive thoughts, flashbacks, feeling of iso­
tion (Behan & Bakheit, 1991). The population- lation, hypervigilance, heightened startle re­
based study by Paulin et al. (2016) confirmed action, concentration problems, and insom­
this association (Table 7–2). nia. Fagelson (2007) reported PTSD in U.S.
armed forces Veterans who were referred to a
Cerebrovascular Disease tinnitus clinic. Approximately 34% of 300 pa­
tients met the criteria for PTSD, were twice
Hyperacusis has been reported occurring rarely as likely as patients without PTSD to report
after both ischemic and hemorrhagic stroke sound tolerance problems, and three times
(Fukutake & Hattori, 1998; Sand, Biller, Cor­ as likely as patients without PTSD to report
bett, Adams, & Dunn, 1986). sound-evoked exacerbation of tinnitus. The
large population-based study undertaken in
Sweden, by Paulin et al. (2016), confirmed
Neuropsychological this co-morbidity (Table 7–2).
Conditions
Neurodevelopmental
Head Injury Conditions

Waddell and Gronwall (1984) studied 9 con­ Autism Spectrum Disorders


secutive closed head injury patients, assessing
their sensitivity to sound and light. Measures Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) comprise
were compared to controls matched for age, a series of neurodevelopmental disorders in
sex, race, and socioeconomic status. Head which affected individuals display persistent
injury patients were statistically more sensi­ deficits in social communication and interac­
tive to light than the controls. Eight patients tion and display restricted repetitive patterns
participated in the sound sensitivity tests: of behavior, interests, or activities. Sensory
4/8 reported that they were more sensitive to hypersensitivity has long been recognized in
sound and 2/8 reported that they were much ASD (Baron Cohen, Ashwin, Ashwin, Tav­
more sensitive. The sound intensity tolerated assoli, & Chakrabarti, 2009) and there are
by the head injury patients was lower than many anecdotal reports describing individual
in the control group (82 dB compared to experiences of sound intolerance in autism
94 dB) but the difference did not reach statis­ (Tavassoli, 2013). The current Diagnostic and
tical significance. Chapter 9 offers a detailed Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.
summary regarding the association between (DSM-5) (American Psychiatric Association,
traumatic brain injury and sound tolerance 2013) has added both hyperreactivity and
issues. hyporeactivity to sensory input to the diag­
nostic criteria for ASD. Although individuals
Post-traumatic Stress Disorder with so-called high functioning ASD can de­
scribe their reactions to sensory stimuli, there
Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a men­ are many autistic people who cannot interact
tal health condition that can develop after a with researchers in this fashion. This makes
120 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

research in the field difficult and is reflected sound intolerance but not meeting the defi­
in the sometimes apparently contradictory nition of audiosensitivity; tolerant to noise.
nature of studies. Beers, McBoyle, Kakande, Stapedial reflex abnormalities were reported
Dar Santos, and Kozak (2014) conducted a more commonly in the audiosensitive group
systematic review and concluded that there than the other two groups. MacCulloch and
is no conclusive evidence that children with Eaton (1971) studied 44 children who had
ASD are at increased risk of peripheral hear­ been referred because of stutter. There were 35
ing loss. Matsuzaki et al. (2014) performed boys and 9 girls with a mean age of 12.5 years.
a magnetoencephalography (MEG) study on An equal group of non-stuttering children was
21 boys with ASD, 12 of whom with auditory used as a control group. Loudness discomfort
hypersensitivity and 9 without; 15 normally levels were tested and were found to be lower
developing boys were used as a control group. in the stuttering group than the non-stuttering
Increased activity was seen in the group with controls. There was no report as to whether
autism and auditory hypersensitivity but in any of the test subjects were distressed by noise.
neither of the other groups. The authors spec­
ulated that auditory hypersensitivity in autism Attention Deficit
was caused by reduced inhibitory processing. Hyperactivity Disorder
Khalfa et al. (2004) performed a range of au­
diological tests on 11 children with autism Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
and 11 without. The autistic children were (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental condition
found to have a smaller auditory dynamic characterized by deficits in attention, organiza­
range and increased perception of loudness. tion, activity levels, and impulse control (Ram­
Rosenhall, Nordin, Sandström, Ahlsén, and tekkar 2017). As part of a large population-
Gillberg (1999) compared 199 children with based study of hyperacusis in Sweden, Paulin
autism (153 boys, 46 girls) against a control et al. (2013) asked participants about vari­
group finding hyperacusis in 18.0% of the au­ ous health conditions including ADHD. El­
tistic subjects versus 0% in the control group. evated odds ratios supported the hypothesis
Danesh et al. (2015) studied hyperacusis in that ADHD is co-morbid with hyperacusis
individuals with Asperger’s syndrome finding though the number of people self-reporting
increased prevalence in the Asperger’s group ADHD was very small (8 individuals) and this
compared to both the general population and suggested association should be interpreted
individuals with other forms of autism. carefully.

Learning Disability and Stuttering


Psychological Conditions
Gordon (1986) described a study of 93 chil­
dren with learning disability comprising 73 Hypersensitivity to sound has long been rec­
boys and 20 girls with a mean age of 9.4 years. ognized as having co-morbidity with several
Many, though the exact number was unspeci­ mental health disorders including depression
fied, had dyslexia. The children were allocated and anxiety (Stansfeld, 1992; Jüris, Andersson,
by parental report to one of three groups: au­ Larsen, & Ekselius, 2013). Data from a large
diosensitive defined as needing to turn the vol­ population-based study supports this assertion
ume down on a TV, radio, or record player; (Paulin, 2016) (Table 7–2). This study also
noise intolerant defined as having any form of demonstrated a co-morbidity between hyper­
7. Hyperacusis: Medical Diagnoses and Associated Syndromes 121

acusis and panic disorder. Obsessive compul­ (Pienowski et al., 2014). Laurian et al. (1983)
sive disorders and eating disorders have been showed abnormalities of the stapedial reflex
described in case reports and case series of pa­ in patients with hyperthyroidism, with raised
tients with misophonia (McFerran, 2016b). stapedial reflex thresholds and decreased am­
plitudes. No clinical correlation was made
in this study and, in particular, there was no
mention of hyperacusis.
Endocrine Conditions

Adrenocortical Insufficiency: Growth Hormone Deficiency


Addison’s Disease and
Pan-Hypopituitarism Growth hormone deficiency is a condition in
which there is a complete or partial absence
Adrenocortical insufficiency is a condition of growth hormone in the body. The condi­
in which the adrenal glands fail to produce tion most commonly presents in childhood,
adequate quantities of steroids, particularly has a genetic basis, and results in the child
glucocorticoids. This can occur because of failing to grow normally. It can also appear
primary failure of the adrenal cortex due to in adulthood most commonly secondary to
autoimmune mechanisms in Addison’s dis­ tumors of the pituitary gland. Prado-Barreto
ease. Alternatively, it can arise because the pitu­ et al. (2014) performed audiological evalua­
itary gland is not producing the necessary hor­ tions on 26 individuals with isolated growth
mones to control the adrenal gland. A study hormone deficiency and 25 normal controls.
examined the auditory function of four pa­ Misophonia and dizziness were significantly
tients with Addison’s disease and four patients more common in the isolated growth hor­
with adrenocortical insufficiency secondary mone deficiency group and peripheral audi­
to pan-hypopituitarism (Henkin, McGlone, tory function was also worse in this group.
Daly, & Bartter, 1968). Hearing thresholds of
the study group were compared against 25 nor­
mal controls. The patients with adrenocortical Metabolic Conditions
insufficiency were found to have better hearing
sensitivity across the audiometric range com­ Pyridoxine Deficiency
pared to the unaffected controls. The patients’
hearing thresholds returned to normal after Pyridoxine, the most common form of vita­
treatment with prednisolone. The paper did min B6, is a water-soluble vitamin that acts
not mention whether any of the participants as a co-factor in a variety of the body’s meta­
with adrenocortical insufficiency had clinical bolic processes. Deficiency of this enzyme can
symptoms (i.e., loudness tolerance problems) result in anemia and peripheral neuropathy.
related to their increased auditory sensitivity. Marriage and Barnes (1995) mention pyri­
doxine deficiency as a condition co-occurring
with hyperacusis. This assertion was based on
Hyperthyroidism a personal communication (Oppe, 1992, per­
sonal communication, cited in Marriage &
Hyperthyroidism, or overactivity of the thy­ Barnes, 1995) and a thorough literature search
roid gland, has been included in a table of has failed to reveal any more compelling evi­
clin­ical conditions associated with hyperacusis dence. Despite the weakness of the evidence,
122 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

the putative link continues to be cited in print ifestations of the condition in 35 consecu­
(Pienkowski et al., 2014) and online. tive patients with SLE, contrasted with 30
healthy age and sex matched patients. In the
SLE group 9 patients (25.7%) reported hy­
Magnesium Deficiency peracusis. In the subgroup that had SLE and
hearing loss, 4/10 reported hyperacusis. Un­
Cevette, Vormann, and Franz (2003) in a re­ fortunately, the presence or absence of hyper­
view paper examined the evidence regarding acusis in the control group was not revealed.
magnesium in the auditory system, reporting The authors discussed the use of ototoxic
auditory hyperexcitability in magnesium defi­ drugs in the management of SLE: 65.7% of
ciency. These observations were based on ani­ the study group received chloroquine and all
mal research in rodents, specifically rats (Kruse, but two of the patients who had hearing loss
Orent, & McCollum, 1932), guinea pigs (Ce­ had received this drug. All four patients who
vette, Franz, Brey, & Robinette, 1989), and had both hearing loss and hyperacusis had re­
mice (Bac et al., 1994). Magnesium is also ceived chloroquine. It therefore remains un­
being investigated as a potential otoprotectant clear whether SLE is a risk factor for develop­
(Le Prell, Hughes, & Miller, 2007). Despite ing hyperacusis or whether the symptom is a
these pieces of research highlighting magne­ consequence of hearing loss caused by the med­
sium’s important role in the auditory system, ical treatment.
there is no robust evidence that magnesium
deficiency causes hyperacusis in humans.
Musculoskeletal Conditions
Autoimmune Conditions
Back, Joint, and Muscle Pain
Cogan’s Syndrome
As part of the large population-based study
in Sweden, Paulin et al. (2013) asked partici­
Cogan’s syndrome is an autoimmune condi­
pants about various health conditions includ­
tion characterized by interstitial keratitis of
ing disorders of the back, joints, and muscles.
the eye together with vestibular and auditory
The prevalence of joint and muscle disorders
dysfunction. A literature search revealed a sin­
was 39.4% among participants with physician-
gle case report of hyperacusis in association
diagnosed hyperacusis and 23.4% among
with Cogan’s syndrome (Schuknecht & Na­
those who self-reported hyperacusis. Elevated
dol, 1994).
odds ratios supported the hypothesis that these
forms of musculoskeletal pain are co-morbid
Systemic Lupus Erythematosus with hyperacusis.

Systemic lupus erythematosus (SLE) is an au­ Temporomandibular Disorder


toimmune disease presenting predominantly
in women that has a wide range of clinical pre­ Temporomandibular disorder (TMD) is a
sentations and can affect many body systems common condition with a variety of symptoms
including the auditory system. Maciaszczyk, related to the jaw joints and muscles of mas­
Durko, Waszczykowska, Erkiert-Polguj, and tication and a huge number of synonyms in­
Pajor (2011) investigated the auditory man­ cluding temporomandibular joint dysfunction,
7. Hyperacusis: Medical Diagnoses and Associated Syndromes 123

craniomandibular dysfunction, myofascial pain has been reported and Geisser et al. (2008)
dysfunction syndrome, Costen syndrome, and conducted a study comparing 30 patients with
many others. There have been suggestions that fibromyalgia to 28 normal controls. Sensitivity
TMD is linked with various auditory system to sound was statistically greater in the fibro­
symptoms including tinnitus, hearing loss, myalgia group and this group also reported
and hyperacusis but there is still debate as to statistically significantly increased sensitivity
whether such coexistence of symptoms is coin­ to everyday sounds. Fibromyalgia was con­
cidental or causal. Rubinstein and Erlandsson sidered by Paulin et al. (2016): the prevalence
(1991) studied 42 patients who had tinnitus was 10.6% among participants with physician-
and craniomandibular dysfunction, compar­ diagnosed hyperacusis and 4.2% among those
ing them to a control group that had tinnitus who self-reported hyperacusis. Elevated odds
alone. A higher prevalence of hyperacusis was ratios supported the hypothesis that fibromy­
reported in the tinnitus and craniomandibu­ algia is co-morbid with hyperacusis.
lar dysfunction group (71.4%) compared to
the tinnitus alone group (58.3%). Hilgenberg,
Saldanha, Cunha, Rubo, and Conti (2012)
Vascular Conditions
studied 100 people with tinnitus and a con­
trol group of 100 people without tinnitus. In
the tinnitus group, 85% met the criteria for Carotid Aneurysm
TMD, compared to 55% in the control group.
Hyperacusis was also more common in the tin­ An aneurysm is a pathological dilatation of
nitus group at 26%, compared to 12% in the the wall of a blood vessel and aneurysms can
controls. Schecklmann, Landgrebe, and Lang­ occur in both the intracranial and extracranial
guth (2014) analyzed data from the Tinnitus portions of the carotid arteries. Carotid aneu­
Research Initiative database which is a large rysms may be asymptomatic or may be associ­
multinational project that collects data on pa­ ated with a variety of neurological symptoms
tients attending specialized tinnitus clinics us­ and signs. Carotid aneurysm has been included
ing a standardized case report form. This data in a table of clinical conditions associated with
collection form includes questions such as, “Do hyperacusis (Pienowski et al., 2014) but there
sounds cause you pain or physical discomfort?” is no source reference included in the paper to
and “Do you suffer from temporomandibular support this assertion.
disorder?” Information on TMD was available
for 1674 patients and demonstrated a signifi­ Middle Cerebral Artery Aneurysm
cantly increased prevalence of TMD in the tin­
nitus and hyperacusis group (26%) compared Aneurysmal swelling of the middle cerebral
to the tinnitus alone group (16%). artery is cited in many publications as a condi­
tion associated with hyperacusis. This is based
on a solitary case report (Khalil, Ogunyemi, &
Fibromyalgia Osborne, 2002): a 35-year-old patient pre­
sented with isolated episodic hyperacusis and
Fibromyalgia is a condition characterized by investigation revealed a middle cerebral artery
chronic pain throughout the body, tenderness aneurysm. This was treated neuro-radiologically
to touch, and fatigue. The majority of affected by Guglielmi detachable coil embolization,
individuals are female, and onset is generally in which resulted in complete resolution of the
middle age. Sensitivity to other sensory stimuli hyperacusis. The authors postulated that the
124 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

aneurysm irritated the nearby auditory cortex and 17 taking a tricyclic antidepressant. Four
thereby generating the hyperacusis. of the phenelzine group developed hyperacu­
sis during drug withdrawal compared to one
of the tricyclic group.
Gastrointestinal Conditions
Drug Side Effects
Irritable Bowel Syndrome
Sürer Adanir, Gizli Çoban, and Özatalay
As part of the large population-based study (2017) reported a case of an 11-year-old boy
in Sweden, Paulin et al. (2016) asked partici­ whose hyperacusis worsened when he was
pants about various health conditions includ­ given risperidone for hyperactivity and be­
ing irritable bowel syndrome. The prevalence havioral problems. The hyperacusis improved
of irritable bowel syndrome was 9.1% among when the drug was stopped but worsened
participants with physician-diagnosed hyper­ again when it was restarted. Hyperacusis is
acusis and 4.8% among those who self-reported mentioned as a potential side effect on the
hyperacusis. Elevated odds ratios (Table 7–2) drug information leaflets of several different
supported the hypothesis that irritable bowel medications: Yang and Agarwal (2011) de­
syndrome is co-morbid with hyperacusis. scribed hyperacusis as a possible side effect of
the antiepileptic drug phenytoin.
Pharmacological Associations
with Hyperacusis
Infective Conditions
Drug Withdrawal
Lyme Disease
Lader (1984) reported intolerance to noise in
6/10 patients who participated in a pilot, un­ Lyme disease is an infectious disease caused by
controlled benzodiazepine withdrawal study. spirochete bacteria of the borrelia genus, trans­
Following this pilot study, a double-blind mitted by ticks. Although deer ticks are the
placebo-controlled study was undertaken in usual vector, the animal reservoir is in small
which 18/24 patients reported hyperacusis. mammals and birds rather than deer. When
Benzodiazepine withdrawal was also men­ humans become infected, the first manifesta­
tioned as a potential cause of intolerance tion is usually an expanding painless circular
to noise in two other publications (Gordon rash called erythema migrans. Flu-like symp­
1981, 1986) but no data was available in these toms may also occur at this stage. The sub­
publications to support the assertions. Hyper­ sequent stages of the disease are divided into
acusis has also been reported as a component early and late disseminated stages, facial nerve
of monoamine oxidase inhibitor (MOAI) with­ palsy can occur during the early disseminated
drawal, following research by Tyrer (1984). Pa­­ phase, potentially resulting in hyperacusis by
tients withdrawing from either phenelzine (a affecting the stapedial reflex. A wide range of
monoamine oxidase inhibitor) or a tricyclic anti­ neurological, neuropsychiatric, and arthritic
depressant drug were studied. Approximately symptoms have been reported during the late
54 patients enrolled in the study with 51 com­ disseminated phase. Hyperacusis in the ab­
pleting, of which 34 were taking phenelzine sence of seventh nerve palsy has been described
7. Hyperacusis: Medical Diagnoses and Associated Syndromes 125

and this association is frequently cited in the hypothesis, this link remains unproven and
hyperacusis literature. Case reports of hyper­ possibly libelous! The only other published
acusis in association with Lyme disease have evidence is a case report of a 42-year-old man
been published (Fallon et al., 1992; Nields, with multiorgan symptoms which included
Fallon, & Jastreboff, 1999) but the numbers tinnitus and phonophobia (Abildgaard, Ot­
are very small, and the suggested co-morbidity tosen, Baggesen, & Vorum, 2014). He was
remains anecdotal rather than proven. found to have antibodies to Treponema pal­
lidum. This is clearly an inadequate level of evi­
dence to prove an association between the two
Creutzfeld Jacob Disease (CJD)
conditions. There is no convincing evidence
that syphilis is more likely to result in hyper­
Merkler, Prasad, Lavi, and Safdieh (2014)
acusis than other causes of hearing loss.
reported a 52-year-old man who presented
with a short history of impaired sound toler­
ance and difficulty discerning noises includ­ Typhoid Fever
ing speech and music. Initial otological and
neurological examination was normal as was Typhoid fever is a bacterial infection caused
audiometry. He went on to develop myalgia, by Salmonella typhi. Symptoms include high
neck pain, dysarthria, and ataxia. He was com­ fever, headache, general weakness, and skin
prehensively investigated and a diagnosis of rash. Late complications include delirium,
CJD was ultimately confirmed on brain bi­ encephalitis, intestinal hemorrhage, intesti­
opsy. He died 21 months after presentation. nal perforation, and septicemia. Typhoid has
A few cases of hearing loss have been reported been included in a table of clinical conditions
in CJD but no other cases of disordered associated with hyperacusis (Pienowski et al.,
sound tolerance. The patient’s description of 2014) but there is no source reference in­
his sound intolerance was that he experienced cluded in the paper to support this assertion.
sounds as jarring and unfamiliar rather than
painful above certain intensities.
Typhus
Syphilis/Neurosyphilis Typhus is a group of infectious diseases caused
by obligate intracellular bacteria of the Rick­
There has long been a suggestion that syph­ ettsia or Orientia genera. Symptoms of infec­
ilis is associated with hyperacusis: Perlman tion include fever, headache, confusion, and
(1938), in a review article, discussed hyper­ a macular skin rash. Hyperacusis has been
acusis occurring in tabes dorsalis, one of the reported in both epidemic typhus caused by
forms of neurosyphilis. Syphilis was mentioned Rickettsia prowazekii (Cowan, 2000) and scrub
as a possible cause of hyperacusis by Gordon typhus caused by Orientia tsutsugumushi (Kaki
(2000), but this suggestion was reached by a & Satyendra, 2016).
rather tortuous and nonscientific argument:
the author noted that many classical music
composers had audiosensitivity and had, or Iatrogenic Conditions
were thought to have had, otosyphilis. He
therefore suggested that the two observations Hyperacusis has been cited as a complica­
could be associated. Although an interesting tion of spinal anesthetic (Pienkowski et al.,
126 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

2014). There is certainly a publication titled


“Hyperacusis after Spinal Anaesthetic” (Gor­ Clinical Implications
don, 1991). This is a letter to the editor of of Medical Conditions
the journal, discussing a previous publication
Associated with Hyperacusis
(Fog, Wang, Sundberg, & Mucchiano, 1990)
in which hearing had been assessed before
and after spinal anesthetic for prostatectomy. Hyperacusis is common, and there are many
In this original study, hearing thresholds im­ proven or suspected co-morbid conditions.
proved slightly in some patients following Because of the wide range of these conditions
spinal anesthesia. Gordon is using the word it would be very difficult to devise a diagnostic
“hyperacusis” to denote supra-normal hear­ algorithm and, given the associated effects on
ing, not hyperacusis as we would currently quality of life expressed across patients, it is
understand. There was no evidence in the re­ doubtful if such a tool would be helpful in any
search undertaken by Fog et al. (1990) that case. In many instances, the co-morbid condi­
the study patients had any negative experi­ tion has been diagnosed long before the sound
ence associated with their improved pure tone intolerance. In most co-morbidities there is no
audiometry. condition-specific treatment for any accompa­
nying hyperacusis. The most commonly asso­
ciated conditions are hearing loss and tinnitus,
Environmental Intolerance and a careful otological history, examination,
and appropriate audiological testing should
Several conditions have been described in reveal these co-morbidities. The other com­
which people display intolerance to various mon association is psychological disease, par­
facets of the environment. These include Mul­ ticularly depression and anxiety states. Being
tiple Chemical Sensitivity in which people mindful of this possible connection and en­
react to very low levels of a variety of odor­ quiring about the patient’s mood during the
ous chemicals, Non-Specific Building-Related process of taking the clinical history is recom­
Symptoms (Sick Building Syndrome), in which mended. Use of a mental health questionnaire
people complain of a variety of symptoms in such as the Hospital Anxiety Depression Scale
particular buildings, and Electromagnetic Hy­ (HADS) (Zigmond & Snaith, 1983) may be
persensitivity in which individuals report appropriate. One disorder that does have spe­
symptoms when exposed to electromagnetic cific management strategies is superior semi­
fields. Palmquist, Claeson, Neely, Stenberg, circular canal dehiscence syndrome, but it is
and Nordin (2014), using data from the a moot point as to whether this can really be
Swedish Västerbotten Environmental Health called a disorder of sound tolerance.
Study, investigated the relation among these Management strategies for decreased
three environmental tolerances and sound in­ sound tolerance are discussed later in this
tolerance. Among 3406 respondents, 21.6% book. There is one interesting medical treat­
reported sensitivity to at least one of the four ment that has recently been reported: Silver­
triggers. This study showed that there was stein, Ojo, Daugherty, Nazarian, and Wazen
statistically significant overlap among all the (2016) described a novel surgical procedure
environmental tolerances for both physician- to use fascia or perichondrium to reinforce
diagnosed and self-reported cases except for the round and oval windows in the middle ear
physician-diagnosed sound intolerance and and reduce the amount of sound energy reach­
electromagnetic field intolerance. ing the inner ear. The authors described the
7. Hyperacusis: Medical Diagnoses and Associated Syndromes 127

outcome for six patients who had undergone Adour, K. K., & Wingerd, J. (1974). Idiopathic
the procedure and reported improved sound facial paralysis (Bell’s palsy): Factors affecting
tolerance and enhanced quality of life in severity and outcome in 446 patients. Neurol­
this small cohort. Although the patients re­ ogy, 24, 1112–1116.
ported no significant change in hearing post- Althaus, S. R. (1977). Spontaneous and traumatic
perilymph fistulas. Laryngoscope, 87, 364–371.
operatively, audiometry showed worse average
American Academy of Otolaryngology—Head and
pure tone thresholds in all but one operated Neck Foundation, Inc. (1995). Committee on
ear. Clearly this treatment is at variance with Hearing and Equilibrium guidelines for the di­
most other hyperacusis management strate­ agnosis and evaluation of therapy in Meniere’s
gies which are designed at improving sound disease. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg., 113,
tolerance rather than blocking sound from 181–185.
the patient’s auditory system. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diag­
nostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
(5th ed.). DSM-5. Arlington, VA: American Psy­
chiatric Publishing.
Anari, M., Axelsson, A., Eliasson, A., & Mag­
Conclusions
nusson, L. (1999). Hypersensitivity to sound–
questionnaire data, audiometry and classifica­
Disorders of sound tolerance have been re­ tion. Scand. Audiol., 28, 219–230.
ported in numerous medical conditions and Andersson, G., Lindvall, N., Hursti, T., & Carl­
syndromes. In many cases the evidence for a bring, P. (2002). Hypersensitivity to sound (hyper­
acusis): A prevalence study conducted via the
link is poor, based on case reports or small
Internet and post. Int. J. Audiol., 41, 545–554.
uncontrolled case series. The best evidence for Andersson, G., Vretblad, P., Larsen, H. C., &
conditions associated with disorders of sound Lyttkens, L. (2001). Longitudinal follow-up of
tolerance includes hearing loss, tinnitus, and tinnitus complaints. Arch. Otolaryngol. Head
neuroses, though even here the evidence is in­ Neck Surg., 127, 175–179.
sufficient to show causality. Population-based Arisoy, A. E., Ozden, S., Ciliv, G., & Ozalp, I.
surveys are emerging, but research is ham­ (1995). Tay-Sachs disease: A case report. Turk.
pered by confusing and inconsistent termi­ J. Pediatr., 37, 51–56.
nology and a lack of both objective and sub­ Attias, J. (2013). New findings on hyperacusis in
jective outcome measures for impaired sound Williams syndrome. ENT & Audiology News.
tolerance. Because of the huge range of con­ 21, 76–78.
ditions that have already been reported in as­ Attias, J., Raveh, E., Ben-Naftali, N. F., Zarchi, O.,
& Gothelf, D. (2008). Hyperactive auditory
sociation with impaired sound tolerance, it is
efferent system and lack of acoustic reflexes in
conceivable that it is a non-specific marker of Williams syndrome. J. Basic Clin. Physiol. Phar­
illness, in which case this avenue of research macol., 19, 193–207.
may turn out to be pointless. Bac, P., Pages, N., Herrenknecht, C., Dewulf, C.,
Binet, P., & Durlach, J. (1994). Effect of var­
ious serotoninergically induced manipulations
on audiogenic seizures in magnesium-deficient
References mice. Magnes. Res., 7, 107–115.
Baguley, D. M. (2003). Hyperacusis. J. R. Soc. Med.,
96, 582–585.
Abildgaard, S. K., Ottosen, S., Baggesen, K. L., & Baron-Cohen, S., Ashwin, E., Ashwin, C., Tav­
Vorum, H. (2014). [Visual symptoms by syph­ assoli, T., & Chakrabarti, B. (2009). Talent in
ilis]. Ugeskr Laeger, 15, 176(25A). autism: Hyper-systemizing, hyper-attention to
128 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

detail and sensory hypersensitivity. Philos. Trans. Fagelson, M. A. (2007). The association between
R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci., 364, 1377–1383. tinnitus and posttraumatic stress disorder. Am.
Beers, A. N., McBoyle, M., Kakande, E., Dar San­ J. Audiol., 16, 107–117.
tos, R. C., & Kozak, F. K. (2014). Autism and Fallon, B. A., Nields, J. A., Burrascano, J. J., Lieg­
peripheral hearing loss: A systematic review. ner, K., DelBene, D., & Liebowitz, M. R.
Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol., 78, 96–101. (1992). The neuropsychiatric manifestations of
Behan, P. O., & Bakheit, A. M. (1991). Clinical Lyme borreliosis. Psychiatr. Q., 63, 95–117.
spectrum of postviral fatigue syndrome. Br. Med. Fog, J., Wang, L. P., Sundberg, A., & Mucchiano, C.
Bull., 47, 793–808. (1990). Hearing loss after spinal anesthesia is re­
Cevette, M. J., Franz, K. B., Brey, R. H., & Rob­ lated to needle size. Anesth. Analg., 70, 517–522.
inette, M. S. (1989). Influence of dietary mag­ Formby, C., Sherlock, L. P., Hawley, M. L., &
nesium on the amplitude of wave V of the au­ Gold, S. L. (2017). A sound therapy-based
ditory brainstem response. Otolaryngol. Head intervention to expand the auditory dynamic
Neck Surg., 101, 537–541. range for loudness among persons with sensori­
Cevette, M. J., Vormann, J., & Franz, K. (2003). neural hearing losses: case evidence showcasing
Magnesium and hearing. J. Am. Acad. Audiol., treatment efficacy. Semin. Hear., 38, 130–150.
14, 202–212. Fukaya, T., & Nomura, Y. (1988). Audiological
Citron, D., 3rd, & Adour, K. K. (1978). Acoustic aspects of idiopathic perilymphatic fistula. Acta
reflex and loudness discomfort in acute facial Otolaryngol. Suppl., 456, 68–73.
paralysis. Arch. Otolaryngol., 104, 303–306. Fukutake, T., & Hattori, T. (1998). Auditory illu­
Cornish, K. M., & Pigram, J. (1996). Develop­ sions caused by a small lesion in the right medial
mental and behavioural characteristics of cri du geniculate body. Neurology, 51, 1469–1471.
chat syndrome. Arch. Dis. Child., 75, 448–450. Gascon, G. G., Ozand, P. T., & Erwin, R. E.
Cowan, G. (2000). Rickettsial diseases: The typhus (1992). GM1 gangliosidosis type 2 in two sib­
group of fevers—a review. Postgrad. Med. J., 76, lings. J. Child. Neurol., 7, Suppl, S41–50.
269–272. Geisser, M. E., Glass, J. M., Rajcevska, L. D., Clauw,
Danesh, A. A., Lang, D., Kaf, W., Andreassen, W. D., D. J., Williams, D. A., Kileny, P. R., & Gracely,
Scott, J., & Eshraghi, A. A. (2015). Tinnitus R. H. (2008). A psychophysical study of au­
and hyperacusis in autism spectrum disorders ditory and pressure sensitivity in patients with
with emphasis on high functioning individuals fibromyalgia and healthy controls. J. Pain, 9,
diagnosed with Asperger’s syndrome. Int. J. Pe­ 417–422.
diatr. Otorhinolaryngol., 79, 1683–1688. Gordon, A. G. (1981). Hyperactive acoustic re­
de Klaver, M. J., van Rijn, M. A., Marinus, J., flexes and perilymphatic hypotension. Arch.
Soede, W., de Laat, J. A., & van Hilten, J. J. Otolaryngol., 107, 774.
(2007). Hyperacusis in patients with complex Gordon, A. G. (1986). Abnormal middle ear mus­
regional pain syndrome related dystonia. J. Neu­ cle reflexes and audiosensitivity. Br. J. Audiol.,
rol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry, 78, 1310–1313. 20, 95–99.
De Seta, D., Mancini, P., Minni, A., Prosperini, L., Gordon, A. G. (1991). Hyperacusis after spinal an­
De Seta, E., Attanasio, G., . . . Filipo, R. (2014). esthesia. Anesth. Analg., 73, 506–507.
Bell’s palsy: symptoms preceding and accompa­ Gordon, A. G. (2000). “Hyperacusis” and origins
nying the facial paresis. Scientific World Journal, of lowered sound tolerance. J. Neuropsychiatry
801971. Clin. Neurosci., 12, 117–119.
Fabijanska, A., Rogowski, M., Bartnik, G., & Gordon, B. A., Gordon, K. E., Hinton, G. G.,
Skarzynski, H. (1999). Epidemiology of tinni­ Cadera, W., Feleki, V., Bayleran, J., & Hecht­
tus and hyperacusis in Poland. In: J. W. P. Ha­ man, P. (1988). Tay-Sachs disease: B1 variant.
zell (Ed.), Proceedings of the Sixth International Pediatr. Neurol., 4, 54–57.
Tinnitus Seminar (pp. 569–571). London, U.K.: Gothelf, D., Farber, N., Raveh, E., Apter, A., &
The Tinnitus and Hyperacusis Centre. Attias, J. (2006). Hyperacusis in Williams syn­
7. Hyperacusis: Medical Diagnoses and Associated Syndromes 129

drome: Characteristics and associated neuro­ Khalfa, S., Bruneau, N., Rogé, B., Georgieff, N.,
audiologic abnormalities. Neurology, 66, 390– Veuillet, E., Adrien, J. L., Barthélémy, C., &
395. Collet, L. (2004). Increased perception of loud­
Hadj-Djilani, A., & Gerster, J. C. (1984). Me­ ness in autism. Hear. Res., 198, 87–92.
niere’s disease and fibrositis syndrome (psycho­ Khalil, S., Ogunyemi, L., & Osborne, J. (2002).
genic rheumatism). Relationship in audiometric Middle cerebral artery aneurysm presenting
and nystagmographic results. Acta Otolaryngol. as isolated hyperacusis. J. Laryngol. Otol., 116,
Suppl., 406, 67–71. 376–378.
Hannula, S., Bloigu, R., Majamaa, K., Sorri, M., & Klein, A. J., Armstrong, B. L., Greer, M. K., &
Mäki-Torkko, E. (2011). Self-reported hearing Brown, F. R., 3rd. (1990). Hyperacusis and
problems among older adults: Prevalence and otitis media in individuals with Williams syn­
comparison to measured hearing impairment. drome. J. Speech Hear. Disord., 55, 339–344.
J. Am. Acad. Audiol., 22, 550–559. Kruse, H. D., Orent, E. R., & McCollum, E. V.
Henkin, R. I., McGlone, R. E., Daly, R., & Bartter, (1932). Studies on magnesium deficiency in ani­
F. C. (1967). Studies on auditory thresholds in mals: I. Symptomatology resulting from magne­
normal man and in patients with adrenal corti­ sium deprivation. J. Biol. Chem., 96, 519–539.
cal insufficiency: The role of adrenal cortical ste­ Lader, M. (1984). Benzodiazepine dependence.
roids. J. Clin. Invest., 46, 429–435. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry, 8,
Herraiz, C., Tapia, M. C., & Plaza, G. (2006). 85–95.
Tinnitus and Meniere’s disease: Characteristics Laurian, N., Laurian, L., Sadov, R., Strauss, M., &
and prognosis in a tinnitus clinic sample. Eur. Kalmanovitz, M. (1983). New clinical applica­
Arch. Otorhinolaryngol., 263, 504–509. tions of the stapedial reflex. J. Laryngol. Otol.,
Hilgenberg, P. B., Saldanha, A. D., Cunha, C. O., 97, 1099–1103.
Rubo, J. H., & Conti, P. C. (2012). Temporo­ Le Prell, C. G., Hughes, L. F., & Miller, J. M.
mandibular disorders, otologic symptoms and (2007). Free radical scavengers vitamins A, C,
depression levels in tinnitus patients. J. Oral. and E plus magnesium reduce noise trauma.
Rehabil., 39, 239–244. Free Radic. Biol. Med., 42, 1454–1463.
Hyperacusis Focus. Retrieved from http://hyper Liriano, R. Y. G., Magalhães, S. L. B., Barros,
acusisfocus.org/research/causes/. F. T., José, R. G., & Fukuda, Y. (2004). Relation
Jastreboff, M. M., & Jastreboff, P. J. (2002). De­ of hyperacusis and peripheral facial paralysis—
creased sound tolerance and tinnitus retraining Bell’s palsy. Revista Brasileira de Otorrinolarin­
therapy. Aust. NZ J. Audiol., 24, 74–84. gologia, 70, 776–779.
Jüris, L., Andersson, G., Larsen, H. C., & Ekse­ MacCulloch, M. J., & Eaton, R. (1971). A note
lius, L. (2013). Psychiatric comorbidity and per­ on reduced auditory pain threshold in 44 stut­
sonality traits in patients with hyperacusis. Int. tering children. Br. J. Disord. Commun., 6,
J. Audiol., 52, 230–235. 148–153.
Kaki, A. R., & Satyendra, S. (2016). Scrub typhus Maciaszczyk, K., Durko, T., Waszczykowska, E.,
with hyperacusis and tinnitus. J. Assoc. Physi­ Erkiert-Polguj, A., & Pajor, A. (2011). Audi­
cians India, 64, 154. tory function in patients with systemic lupus
Karimzadeh, P., Jafari, N., Nejad Biglari, H., Jab­ erythematosus. Auris Nasus Larynx, 38, 26–32.
beh Dari, S., Ahmad Abadi, F., Alaee M. R., . . . Marriage, J., & Barnes, N. M. (1995). Is central
Ghofrani, M. (2014). GM2-gangliosidosis (Sand­ hyperacusis a symptom of 5-hydroxytryptamine
hoff and Tay Sachs disease): Diagnosis and neu­ (5-HT) dysfunction? J. Laryngol. Otol., 109,
roimaging findings (an Iranian pediatric case 915–921.
series). Iran. J. Child. Neurol., 8, 55–60. Mathisen, H. (1969). Phonophobia after stape­
Katzenell, U., & Segal, S. (2001). Hyperacusis: Re­ dectomy. Acta Otolaryngol., 68, 73–77.
view and clinical guidelines. Otol. Neurotol., 22, Matsuzaki, J., Kagitani-Shimono, K., Sugata, H., Hi­
321–326. rata, M., Hanaie, R., Nagatani, F., . . . Taniike, M.
130 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

(2014). Progressively increased M50 responses children with spina bifida; a pilot-study. Eur. J.
to repeated sounds in autism spectrum dis­ Pediatr. Surg., 7 Suppl, 1, 46.
order with auditory hypersensitivity: A mag­ Palmquist, E., Claeson, A. S., Neely, G., Stenberg,
netoencephalographic study. PLoS ONE, 9, B., & Nordin, S. (2014). Overlap in prevalence
e102599. between various types of environmental intoler­
McCandless, G. A., & Goering, D. M. (1974). ance. Int. J. Hyg. Environ. Health, 217, 427–434.
Changes in loudness after stapedectomy. Arch Paulin, J., Andersson, L., & Nordin, S. (2016).
Otolaryngol., 100, 344–350. Characteristics of hyperacusis in the general
McCandless, G. A., & Schumacher, M. H. (1979). population. Noise Health, 18, 178–184.
Auditory dysfunction with facial paralysis. Arch Perlman, H. B. (1938). Hyperacusis. Annals Otol.,
Otolaryngol., 105, 271–274. Rhinol. Laryngol., 47, 947–953.
McFerran, D. (2016a). Acoustic shock. In: D. M. Pienkowski, M., Tyler, R. S., Roncancio, E. R.,
Baguley & M. Fagelson (Eds.), Tinnitus: Clini­ Jun, H. J., Brozoski, T., Dauman, N., . . .
cal and research perspectives (pp. 181–196). San Moore, B. C. (2014). A review of hyperacusis
Diego, CA: Plural. and future directions: Part II. Measurement,
McFerran, D. (2016b). Misophonia and pho­ mechanisms, and treatment. Am. J. Audiol., 23,
nophobia. In: D. M. Baguley & M. Fagelson 420–436.
(Eds.), Tinnitus: Clinical and research perspec­ Prado-Barreto, V. M., Salvatori, R., Santos Júnior,
tives (pp. 245–259). San Diego, CA: Plural. R. C., Brandão-Martins, M. B., Correa, E. A.,
Merkler, A.E., Prasad, M., Lavi, E., & Safdieh, J. Garcez, F. B., . . . Aguiar-Oliveira, M. H.
(2014). Hyperacusis as the initial presentation (2014). Hearing status in adult individuals
of Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease. Neurol. Neuroim­ with lifetime, untreated isolated growth hor­
munol. Neuroinflamm., 1, e32. mone deficiency. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg.,
Minor, L. B., Solomon, D., Zinreich, J. S., & Zee, 150, 464–471.
D. S. (1998). Sound- and/or pressure-induced Ramtekkar, U. P. (2017). DSM-5 changes in at­
vertigo due to bone dehiscence of the supe­ tention deficit hyperactivity disorder and au­
rior semicircular canal. Arch Otolaryngol. Head tism spectrum disorder: Implications for co­
Neck Surg., 124, 249–258. morbid sleep issues. Children (Basel), 4, E62.
Modugno, G. C., & Brandolini, C. (2014). Can Rosenhall, U., Nordin, V., Sandström, M., Ahlsén,
lateral semicircular canal dysplasia play a role G., & Gillberg, C. (1999). Autism and hearing
in the genesis of hyperacusis. Acta Otorhinolar­ loss. J. Autism Dev. Disord., 29, 349–357.
yngol. Ital., 34, 71–74. Rubinstein, B., & Erlandsson, S. I. (1991). A
Morioka, W. T., Neff, P. A., Boisseranc, T. E., stomatognathic analysis of patients with dis­
Hartman, P. W., & Cantrell, R. W. (1976). Au­ abling tinnitus and craniomandibular disorders
diotympanometric findings in myasthenia gra­ (CMD). Br. J. Audiol., 25, 77–83.
vis. Arch Otolaryngol., 102, 211–213. Saliba, I., Maniakas, A., Benamira, L. Z., Nehme, J.,
Nelson, J. J., & Chen, K. (2004). The relationship Benoit, M., & Montreuil-Jacques, V. (2014).
of tinnitus, hyperacusis, and hearing loss. Ear Superior canal dehiscence syndrome: clinical
Nose Throat J., 83, 472–476. manifestations and radiologic correlations. Eur.
Nields, J. A., Fallon, B. A., & Jastreboff, P. J. Arch Otorhinolaryngol., 271, 2905–2914.
(1999). Carbamazepine in the treatment of Sand, J. J., Biller, J., Corbett, J. J., Adams, H. P.
Lyme disease-induced hyperacusis. J. Neuropsy­ Jr., & Dunn, V. (1986). Partial dorsal mesen­
chiatry Clin. Neurosci., 11, 97–99. cephalic hemorrhages: Report of three cases.
Nigam, A., & Samuel, P. R. (1994). Hyperacu­ Neurology, 36, 529–533.
sis and Williams syndrome. J. Laryngol. Otol., Schecklmann, M., Landgrebe, M., Langguth, B., &
108, 494–496. TRI Database Study Group. (2014). Pheno­
Oen, J. M., Begeer, J. H., Staal-Schreinemachers, typic characteristics of hyperacusis in tinnitus.
A. I., & Tijmstra, T. (1997). Hyperacusis in PLoS ONE, 9, e86944.
7. Hyperacusis: Medical Diagnoses and Associated Syndromes 131

Schröder, A., Vulink, N., & Denys, D. (2013). Vingen, J. V., Sand, T., & Stovner, L. J. (1999).
Misophonia: Diagnostic criteria for a new psy­ Sensitivity to various stimuli in primary head­
chiatric disorder. PLoS ONE. 8, e54706. aches: A questionnaire study. Headache, 39,
Schuknecht, H. F., & Nadol, J. B. Jr. (1994). 552–558.
Temporal bone pathology in a case of Cogan’s Von Leden, H., & Horton, B. T. (1949). Auditory
syndrome. Laryngoscope, 104, 1135–1142. nerve in multiple sclerosis. Arch Otolaryngol.,
Sheth, J., Datar, C., Mistri, M., Bhavsar, R., Sheth, 48, 51–57.
F., & Shah, K. (2016). GM2 gangliosidosis AB Vos, T., Flaxman, A. D., Naghavi, M., Lozano, R.,
variant: Novel mutation from India—a case re­ Michaud, C., Ezzati, M. . . . Memish, Z. A.
port with a review. BMC Pediatr., 16, 88. (2012). Years lived with disability (YLDs) for
Silverstein, H., Ojo, R., Daugherty, J., Nazarian, R., 1160 sequelae of 289 diseases and injuries
& Wazen, J. (2016). Minimally invasive sur­ 1990–2010: A systematic analysis for the Global
gery for the treatment of hyperacusis. Otol. Neu­ Burden of Disease Study 2010. Lancet, 380,
rotol., 37, 1482–1488. 2163–2196.
Stange, T., Mir-Salim, P. A., & Berghaus, A. (2001). Waddell, P. A., & Gronwall, D. M. (1984). Sensi­
[Hyperacusis after tympanoplasty]. HNO, 49, tivity to light and sound following minor head
303–306. injury. Acta Neurol. Scand., 69, 270–276.
Stansfeld, S. A. (1992). Noise, noise sensitivity and Wayman, D. M., Pham, H. N., Byl, F. M., &
psychiatric disorder: Epidemiological and psy­ Adour, K. K. (1990). Audiological manifesta­
chophysiological studies. Psychol. Med. Monogr. tions of Ramsay Hunt syndrome. J. Laryngol.
Suppl., 22, 1–44. Otol., 104, 104–108.
Sürer Adanir, A., Gizli Çoban, Ö., & Özatalay, E. Weber, H., Pfadenhauer, K., Stöhr, M., & Rösler,
(2017). Increased hyperacusis with risperidone in A. (2002). Central hyperacusis with phono­
an autistic child. Noro Psikiyatr Ars, 54, 187–188. phobia in multiple sclerosis. Mult. Scler., 8,
Tavassoli, T. (2013). Sound tolerance in autism 505–509.
spectrum disorder. ENT & Audiology News, Westcott, M., Sanchez, T. G., Diges, I., Saba,
21, 74–75. C., Dineen, R., McNeill, C., . . . Sharples, T.
Tyler, R. S., Pienkowski, M., Roncancio, E. R., (2013). Tonic tensor tympani syndrome in tin­
Jun, H. J., Brozoski, T., Dauman, N., . . . nitus and hyperacusis patients: a multi-clinic
Moore, B. C. (2014). A review of hyperacusis prevalence study. Noise Health, 15, 117–128.
and future directions: Part I. Definitions and Woodhouse, A., & Drummond, P. D. (1993).
manifestations. Am. J. Audiol., 23, 402–419. Mechanisms of increased sensitivity to noise
Tyrer, P. (1984). Clinical effects of abrupt with­ and light in migraine headache. Cephalalgia, 13,
drawal from tri-cyclic antidepressants and mono­ 417–421.
amine oxidase inhibitors after long-term treat­ Yang, L., & Agarwal, P. (2011). Systematic drug
ment. J. Affect. Disord., 6, 1–7. repositioning based on clinical side-effects. PLoS
Vingen, J. V., Pareja, J. A., & Stovner, L. J. (1998). ONE, 6, e28025.
Quantitative evaluation of photophobia and Zigmond, A. S., & Snaith, R. P. (1983). The hos­
phonophobia in cluster headache. Cephalalgia, pital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychi­
18, 250–256. atr. Scand., 67, 361–370.
8
Animal Models of
Hyperacusis and Decreased
Sound Tolerance
Jos J. Eggermont

Introduction What Are the Neural


Correlates of Loudness and
Hyperacusis describes an unusual hypersen- Hyperacusis in Humans?
sitivity or discomfort induced by exposure
to sound. It may be a composite of reduced Langers et al. (2007) found in people with
sound level tolerance, which may start at low normal hearing and those with symmetric
levels, and a reduced loudness discomfort level, high-frequency (4 and 8 kHz) hearing loss
which affects moderate to high sound levels. (40 to 70 dB HL) that it was the perceived
Many people with hyperacusis have “clinically loudness of stimuli and not the intensity
normal” audiograms, thereby excluding as pu- level (dB SPL) that related strongly to corti-
tative sources hypersensitive thresholds as well cal activation in these two groups of subjects.
as hearing impairment (Anari et al., 1999). However, the activation measured in the par-
Hyperacusis is accompanied by increased am- ticipants with high-frequency hearing loss
plitude of distortion product otoacoustic emis- increased significantly more with stimulus
sions (DPOAEs) in tinnitus patients without intensity than in the normally hearing sub-
hearing loss (Sztuka et al., 2010), and may be jects. This reflects abnormal growth of loud-
caused by a reduced efferent system action. ness; a disproportionate increase in loudness
Jastreboff and Hazell (1993) described hyper- with stimulus intensity. This demonstrates
acusis as a “manifestation of increased central that fMRI activation at the level of the au-
gain” (Formby et al., 2003; Noreña & Chery- ditory cortex is more closely related to the
Croze, 2007), which may enhance the percep- percept of a stimulus (i.e., loudness) rather
tion of loudness that accompanies stimulation than to its physical characteristics (i.e., inten-
from external signals. sity), and suggests that f MRI activation may

133
134 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

be interpreted as a correlate of the subjective auditory cortex but not in the IC. They also
strength of the stimulus percept. found that salicylate significantly increased the
Rohl and Uppenkamp (2012) used a amplitude of the startle response. The increase
psychophysical scaling procedure for quan- in LFP amplitude and the increased star-
tifying the loudness percept in combination tle reflex suggest that high doses of salicylate
with fMRI. They, as well as Langers et al. increase the gain in the central auditory sys-
(2007), observed an almost linear increase of tem, presumably by down-regulating GABA-
perceived loudness and percent signal change mediated inhibition. Such inhibition provides
(PSC) from baseline in all examined stages of a complimentary mechanism for sustaining
the upper auditory pathway. Across individu- chronic, debilitating hyperacusis and underlies
als, the slope of the underlying growth function the importance of behavior modification, such
for perceived loudness was significantly cor- as patients and physicians monitoring drug in-
related with the slope of the growth function take, in parallel to the specific desensitization
for the PSC in the auditory cortex, but not in techniques for management of hyperacusis
the inferior colliculus (IC) and thalamus. (Noreña & Chery-Croze, 2007).
These two studies suggest that loudness Recent work in mice has shown that
is reflected in the growth of the response- noise exposure resulting in specific ribbon
intensity function in auditory cortex but not synapse damage can cause permanent degen-
in the IC. eration of the auditory nerve despite com-
plete recovery from temporary threshold shift
(TTS; Kujawa & Liberman, 2009). This was
Hyperacusis and suggested as a cause of hyperacusis as reflected
in the enhanced acoustic startle reflex in these
the Startle Response
animals (Hickox & Liberman, 2014). How-
ever, Knudson and Melcher (2016) found
How can hyperacusis be studied in animal that the human equivalent of the startle reflex
models? First, one needs a behavioral indica- (the eye-blink) is not related to hyperacusis
tor for this hypersensitivity. The general ap- but to a reduced loudness discomfort level.
proach taken is to use the magnitude of the The authors distinguished hyperacusis from
startle reflex. Because the inducing sound of reduced loudness discomfort levels in func-
this reflex is very high (mostly 115 dB SPL; tional terms as they asserted the former re-
Turner & Larsen, 2016) one can likely only sulted in abnormal loudness sensation across
measure reduced loudness discomfort levels a subject’s dynamic range of audibility while
(Knudson & Melcher, 2016). Second, to lo- the latter affected only the loudness experi-
cate the neural structures involved, the exam- ence at the upper limit of the dynamic range.
iner often uses the amplitude-level functions
of the local field potential (LFP), or the driven
firing rate of single neurons. Central Gain Changes and
Hyperacusis may be at the basis of obser- Long-Term Non-Traumatic
vations (Ison et al., 2007) that mice with pres-
Noise Exposure
bycusis also show exaggerated acoustic startle
responses (see also Turner & Parrish, 2008;
Sun et al., 2009). Sun et al. (2009) found that The original human study that drove animal
salicylate significantly increased the LFP in the research in directions addressing central gain
8. Animal Models of Hyperacusis and Decreased Sound Tolerance 135

changes in the auditory system was the finding auditory field (AAF) with CF difference
by Formby et al. (2003) that 2-week exposure less than one octave” (p. 2708). One oc-
to a moderate wide band noise (~50 dB SPL) tave corresponds in cat A1 with about 0.75
resulted in a 5 to 6 dB increase in loudness tol- to 1 mm. After long-term exposure to non-
erance thresholds that were attributed to reduc- traumatic sound (Noreña et al., 2006; Pien-
tions in central gain. In contrast, blocking the kowski & Eggermont, 2009) there was a sig-
ear canals for the same period of time resulted nificant change in the correlation distance,
in decreased loudness tolerance that appeared from <1 mm to ~3 mm, and doubling of the
related to an analogous increase in central gain. strength of the spontaneous peak correlation
We (Noreña et al., 2006) exposed nor- (neural synchrony), from ≤0.1 to 0.2 (Fig­
mal hearing adult cats to a 4 to 20 kHz multi- ure 8–3). These increases in correlation strength
frequency sound (termed enhanced acoustic were for neurons with CFs outside the fre-
environment, EAE) for >4 months at 80 dB quency range of the EAE.
SPL, and found that this did not result in au- Thus, increased onset- and average-firing
ditory brainstem response (ABR) threshold rates, increased LFP amplitude, and increased
changes when results were compared to a large neural synchrony may all be electrophysio-
control group. Surprisingly, there was a strong logical correlates of central gain changes pro-
reduction in the primary auditory cortex (A1) voked (somewhat unexpectedly) by stimuli
unit onset responses to tones with frequencies presented at permissible SPLs. Because the
in the 4 to 20 kHz range, and a strong onset- short latency components of stimulus-driven
response enhancement for frequencies <4 kHz LFPs reflect the output of thalamic neurons
and >20 kHz (Figure 8–1, A–C). In most of in the compound excitatory post-synaptic po-
the enhanced response regions, the onset laten- tential of cortical neurons, such gain changes
cies were significantly lengthened compared to are a thalamo-cortical phenomenon.
controls (Figure 8–1D). Similar data were obtained after a 6-week
These changes were also found for av- continuous exposure to steeply filtered 4 to
erage (i.e., more than 100 ms post-stimulus) 20 kHz noise, presented at a level of 68 dB
driven firing rates (Figure 8–2A) and LFP SPL (Pienkowski et al., 2011). Figure 8–4
amplitudes (Figure 8–2B). Thus, whether loud­ shows the frequency dependence of the multi-
ness correlated with onset response or aver- unit (MU) response (left) and LFP amplitude
aged driven firing rates, the findings suggested (right) for 65 dB SPL pure tone-pip stimula-
such changes occurred across a fairly large tion. It is clear that compared to the average
range of stimulus levels, that is, between 10 data in control cats (black) the responses of
and 65 dB SPL. the EAE cats are greatly reduced from about
Neural synchrony in auditory cortex 2.5 to 20 kHz, and greatly enhanced for fre-
may in principle also code for stimulus level. quencies below 2 kHz and above 20 kHz.
Eggermont (2000) found that, “For noise- This occurs both for MU firing rates and LFP
burst stimulation, the onset correlation for amplitudes. Note that when the curves, which
between [cortical] area pairs was independent reflect the 95% confidence levels around the
of stimulus intensity regardless [of ] the dif- mean, do not overlap there is a significant dif-
ference in CF. In contrast, for tone-pip stim- ference at least at the p < 0.05 level. The differ-
ulation a significant dependence on intensity ence between the control and EAE data show
level of the peak correlation strength was evidence for central gain changes, but without
found for pairs involving A1 and/or anterior changes in distortion-product oto-acoustic
Figure 8–1. Averaged poststimulus time histograms across all control and EAE
cats. (a, b) Averaged PSTHs (2-ms time bins) as a function of SPL, over a
100-ms time window, in control and EAE cats. Dashed lines, 10-ms intervals.
Colored bars, mean firing rate. In control cats, the mean response suggested
that the highest sensitivity (lowest thresholds) was to frequencies around
10 kHz and that the largest responses were to frequencies between 2.5 kHz
and 10 kHz. In EAE cats, the most sensitive frequencies were those below
1.25 kHz and above 20 kHz. Note that neural responses in EAE cats were much
more spread out over time compared to those in control cats. (c, d) Graphical
representation of significant differences (Mann-Whitney test) between EAE and
control groups in terms of (c) mean firing rate per frequency-latency bin and
(d) mean latency per frequency-intensity bin. Source: From Noreña et al. (2006).
To view this image in full color visit the Plural Publishing website for the book at
http://pluralpublishing.com/publication_hdsi.htm.

136
8. Animal Models of Hyperacusis and Decreased Sound Tolerance 137

Figure 8–2. Averaged group multiunit firing rates and LFP amplitude as a function of frequency
and intensity. First row, averaged multiunit firing rate; second row, averaged LFP amplitude; third
row, results of statistical tests (Mann-Whitney) for comparisons between control and EAE cats.
Source: From Noreña et al. (2006). To view this image in full color visit the Plural Publishing
website for the book at http://pluralpublishing.com/publication_hdsi.htm.

emissions (DPOAEs) and ABRs (Pienkowski enabled via homeostatic plasticity mecha-
et al., 2011). nisms associated with either permanent or
From the studies reviewed in this sec- very long-lasting changes in the central au-
tion, it is clear that central gain changes can ditory system. These changes could underlie
be induced fairly quickly and that they are hyperacusis in the absence of hearing loss.
138 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

Figure 8–3. Neural synchrony maps in primary auditory cortex. (a, b) Synchrony, defined
here as the peak strength of the corrected cross-correlogram (Methods), as a function of
the position of the recording electrode along the postero-anterior axis (abscissa) and the
distance between the two electrodes involved in the calculation of synchrony (ordinate), in
(a) control and (b) EAE cats. For each electrode pair, positions along the postero-anterior
axis are plotted. Bar colors reflect strength of neural synchrony. In control cats, the strongest
synchrony was found between neighboring electrodes in the array and most correlations
occurred locally. Note the increased synchrony in EAE cats compared to control cats, espe-
cially for larger distances between electrodes. This probably signifies the stronger connec-
tions over large distances (that is, into the reorganized region) made by horizontal fibers. In
these cats, the range of strong correlations is much larger, especially in the –50% to +50%
region, which reflects the entire area with characteristic frequencies <4 kHz but also a sub-
stantial part of the 5 to 20 kHz area. In addition, the area with characteristic frequencies
above 20 kHz (70 to 125%) also showed strongly increased neural synchrony. It is noted
that SFR was significantly increased in the region with putative CFs <4 kHz as well as the
region with CFs >20 kHz. No significant change was found in the 5 to 20 kHz area. Source:
From Noreña et al. (2006). To view this image in full color visit the Plural Publishing website
for the book at http://pluralpublishing.com/publication_hdsi.htm.

Loudness Recruitment and more dominantly, involve the central au-


ditory system.
Is Central
Hyperactivity is seen in central auditory
neurons after sensorineural hearing loss and is
Because hyperacusis often co-occurs with characterized by an abnormally rapid increase
hearing loss, it is important to consider an- of response with stimulus intensity, by a
other well-studied consequence of sensori- heightened maximum response, or both (Salvi
neural hearing loss (SNHL), namely loudness et al., 2000; Wang et al., 2002). This central
recruitment. This was previously thought to hyperactivity is a potential contributor to am-
be a peripheral phenomenon (e.g., Egger- plitude growth of the LFPs, and must affect
mont, 1977; Phillips, 1987) but it may also, patterns of neural activity that code for per-
8. Animal Models of Hyperacusis and Decreased Sound Tolerance 139

Figure 8–4. Population frequency tuning in A1, pooled across four in-
dividual noise-exposed (4 to 20 kHz steeply filtered noise, 68 dB SPL
for 6 weeks) and control cats. Top row: averaged frequency marginals
from control (black) and noise-exposed (red) cats. Curve thickness illus-
trates the Bonferroni-corrected 95% confidence interval for the mean,
dotted lines mark the 4 to 20 kHz noise bandwidth. Left column based
on MU activity, right column based on LFP amplitude. Bottom row:
histogram distributions of response characteristic frequencies (CFs of
frequency-tuning curves) or best frequencies (BFs of spectro-temporal
receptive fields, STRFs) from control (black) and noise-exposed (red)
cats (half-octave bin width). Source: Reprinted with permission from
Pienkowski et al. (2011). To view this image in full color visit the Plural
Publishing website for the book at http://pluralpublishing.com/publication
_hdsi.htm.

ceived loudness. Coincidence-detecting cells the anterior part of the VCN (AVCN) that
in the ventral cochlear nucleus (VCN), which receive convergent ANF inputs are sensitive
receive auditory nerve fiber (ANF) inputs with to the temporal response pattern across the
a narrow region of CFs, can potentially de- inputs (Carney, 1990). In particular, onset
code the spatiotemporal cue for sound level— chopper response types (T-stellate cells) in the
the synchronous firing of a neural population. VCN, which are known to receive convergent
Such spatiotemporal processing could be per- inputs from ANFs with a wide range of CFs
formed at any level of the auditory brainstem and to have wide dynamic ranges (Smith &
wherein cells receive converging inputs from Rhode, 1989), may act as coincidence—that
lower levels. For example, neurons recorded is, synchrony—detectors coding for sound in-
in VCN are typically classified based on tem- tensity. As synapses on inner hair cells (IHCs)
poral discharge patterns in response to tones. and consequently ANFs span a range of differ-
There is physiological evidence that cells in ent thresholds, synchronous activity detected
140 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

from a subset of them could give rise to a chrony across ANFs, Heinz et al. (2005) could
population response related to sound in- not find evidence for any of these mechanisms.
tensity. Note that low-threshold ANFs have It appeared that if the loss was restricted to
high spontaneous firing rates (SFR), and that OHC only, then steeper rate-level functions
high-threshold ANFs have low SFRs. Af- could occur, but if the loss was restricted to
ter acquiring TTS, mice developed a loss of IHCs only, shallower RLFs would be found.
high-threshold ANFs, so that ABR thresholds Because a mix of OHC and IHC loss typically
and DPOAE characteristics were not affected. is found after noise trauma, the effect on rate-
Additionally, the amplitude of the ABR wave level functions may not be significant.
I was reduced at high levels (Kujawa & Liber- Heinz et al. (2005) subsequently sug-
man, 2009), although this finding was later gested that loudness recruitment was central
disputed based on compound action poten- in origin. To explore this, Cai et al. (2008)
tial (CAP) recordings (Bourien et al., 2014). analyzed rate-level functions summed over
various populations of VCN neurons fol-
lowing acoustic trauma and showed that the
Peripheral Correlates responses of non-primary-like types of neu-
of Loudness Recruitment rons, especially chopper neurons, demon-
strated changes in rate responses consistent
Early findings suggested that in human ears with loudness recruitment (as suggested by
with recruitment, the slopes of the CAP-level Carney, 1990), whereas primary-like neurons
functions frequently were steeper than those did not, except in cases where spread of ex-
obtained from control subjects (Eggermont, citation in the population was included. Cai
1977), a result subsequently reported in ani- et al. (2008) suggested that the VCN was the
mal studies of the ANF single-unit rate-level most peripheral stage of the auditory pathway
functions (reviewed in Phillips, 1987). More at which over-excitability of neurons appeared
recent ANF single-unit studies, however, call following acoustic trauma. Moreover, they
these findings into question. again pointed to the synaptic processing in
Liberman and Dodds (1984) reported the non-primary-like, especially the stellate,
that, following acoustic trauma, outer hair cells as the initial locus of central compensa-
cell (OHC) loss is often accompanied by IHC tion for reduced rate responses in the ANFs.
damage, which compromises cochlear trans- An alternate view was offered by the LFP data
duction and lowers the firing rates of ANFs from Wang et al. (2002), however, which put
(Liberman & Kiang, 1984). As a result, the the most peripheral source of recruitment in
slopes of their rate-level functions (RLFs) be- the midbrain. Neural hyperexcitability at the
come shallower on average (Salvi et al., 1983; LFP level is commonly reported in auditory
Heinz & Young, 2004). Additionally, in cats, cortex (AC) and IC (Qiu et al., 2000), but the
the summed RLFs of ANFs following acous- results in cochlear nucleus have been mixed,
tic trauma, measured over either a narrow or with both positive (Saunders et al., 1972) and
wide range of CFs surrounding the stimulus negative (Salvi et al., 1990) findings.
frequency, did not show abnormally steep Summarizing, when considering loud-
slopes (Heinz et al., 2005). ness recruitment, one assumes that changes in
Although loudness recruitment could be the loudness function are mirrored by similar
the result of steeper rate-level functions in changes in neural response growth functions,
ANFs, increased total ANF firing rate due to that is, steeper neural growth functions corre-
spread of excitation, or increased neural syn- spond to steeper loudness growth starting likely
8. Animal Models of Hyperacusis and Decreased Sound Tolerance 141

at the IC. This behavior has been reported in and inhibition. Serotonin-containing fibers in-
the human auditory system through functional nervate inhibitory gamma-aminobutyric acid
imaging at the level of the AC (Langers et al., (GABA) neurons, and in turn may produce
2007). These neural data match the expecta- symptoms such as phantom sounds and the
tion of loudness recruitment qualitatively in excessive auditory gain potentially underlying
the sense that the neural responses of popula- hyperacusis (Hurley & Hall, 2011).
tions are stronger after cochlear damage; how- Sodium salicylate (SS), which is known to
ever, the changes may originate in a variety of increase the central gain (Sun et al., 2009) in-
areas in the auditory CNS. creases 5-HT in IC and AC (Liu et al., 2003).
Wang et al. (2006) subsequently demonstrated
that SS attenuates inhibitory postsynaptic cur-
Interaction between Loudness rents in the auditory cortex, suggesting a po-
Recruitment and Hyperacusis tential mechanism for tinnitus induced by SS
in the central auditory system. This may occur
Interactions between mechanisms causing because SS, through influencing serotonergic
hyper­acusis and loudness recruitment may be modulation of the GABAergic synaptic trans-
demonstrated through effects on firing rate, mission, causes an imbalance between inhibi-
firing synchrony, and LFP amplitudes at ele- tion and excitation (Wang et al., 2008). This
vated SPL. Following acoustic trauma, chop- finding implies that at least a portion of the
per neuron responses in the AVCN (Cai et al., effects of SS and hyperacusis that may result
2008) showed sharply elevated amplitudes and are controlled via modulation of serotonin and
discharge rates at high sound levels, consistent its effects on GABAergic neurotransmission.
with findings related to loudness recruitment. Acoustic trauma is also capable of producing
Such changes may result in over-recruitment, modest but significant decreases in the density
in which intense sounds become too loud for of serotonergic fibers innervating the IC (Pa-
comfortable listening (Sherlock & Formby, pesh & Hurley, 2012), which in turn could
2005; Nelson & Chen, 2004). In contrast, hyper­ decrease inhibitory effects.
acusis patients often cannot stand relatively
soft sounds either and complain that they are
too loud. We must look for a potential neural
correlate that shows strongly increased neural Audiogenic Seizures
responses starting just above threshold, and and Hyperacusis
Noreña et al. (2006) already suggested that a
central gain change may induce this.
A Rodent Model

Audiogenic seizures (AGS) are convulsions


Hyperacusis and Serotonin
brought about by prolonged exposure to high
frequency sound, particularly in rodents and
Serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine, 5-HT) has rabbits and constitute a rodent model of general­
been suggested as a causal factor in hyperacusis ized tonic-clonic epileptic seizures, induced in
(Marriage & Barnes, 1995; Simpson & Da- susceptible animals by high intensity (~110 dB
vies, 2000). Trauma to the cochlea or central SPL) acoustic stimulation (Garcia-Cairasco et al.,
affective dysfunction may alter the serotonergic 1993). Certain strains of mice are either gen­eti­
regulation of the balance between excitation cally susceptible (e.g., DBA/2) or not susceptible
142 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

(e.g., C57BL/6J) to AGS. However, seizure sus­ is also strongly implicated as the other major
ceptibility can be induced in nonsusceptible mechanism in the pathophysiology of AGS.
strains by exposure to intense auditory stim- These neurotransmitter abnormalities result in
ulation during a neonatal sensitive period, excessive firing of IC neurons and act as the
a procedure known as “priming.” As Saunders critical initiation mechanism for triggering
et al. (1972) showed that priming, usually done seizures in response to intense acoustic stimuli
at high sound levels, caused cochlear damage, (Faingold, 2002).
they proposed such exposures as an explanation Repetition of audiogenic seizures (AGS
for the fact that only mice with hearing loss kindling) increases the duration of convul-
exhibited AGS. Additionally, Garcia-Cairasco sive behavior and the emergence of cortical
et al. (1993) reported that mice lacking sero­ epileptiform EEG activity (Feng & Faingold,
to­nin 5-HT2c receptors were extremely sus- 2002). The lateral amygdala (LAMG) is in-
ceptible to AGSs. The onset of susceptibility volved in this expanded network because after
was between two and three months of age. AGS- AGS kindling, LAMG neurons display a sig-
induced immediate early gene expression indi- nificantly increased incidence of onset-only
cated that AGSs were subcortical phenomena patterns (93.3%, at 0.5 Hz), and mean
in auditory circuits. acoustic responsiveness is also significantly in-
Faingold et al. (1988) found that the IC creased (516.2% of control). LAMG neurons
was the most sensitive auditory center to the fire tonically during tonic convulsions and
anticonvulsant drug AP7’s action and that exhibit burst firing during post-tonic clonus.
the imbalance between inhibitory and excit­ Greater acoustically-induced synchronization
atory transmission within the IC might be of LAMG firing, as indicated by elevated re-
crucial in the initiation of AGS in the genet- sponsiveness and increased concentration of
ically epilepsy-prone rat. In about 50% of IC firing near the stimulus onset, may be criti-
neurons of normal rats, high-intensity acous- cal for mediating the behavioral and EEG
tic stimulation produced neuronal firing re- changes induced by AGS kindling. Faingold
ductions compared to lower level stimulation (2004) postulated that AGS qualify as an
thereby resulting in a non-monotonic rate- emergent property, since in AGS the acoustic
level function. stimulus evokes a non-linear output (motor
However, a critical feature of all AGS convulsion), but the identical stimulus evokes
models is the reduction of neural activity in minimal behavioral changes normally. The hi-
the auditory pathways resulting from hear- erarchical neuronal network, underlying AGS
ing loss during development. The initiation in rodents, is initiated in IC and progresses
and propagation of AGS activity relies upon to deep layers of superior colliculus, pontine
hyperexcitability in the auditory system, par- reticular formation, and periaqueductal gray
ticularly the IC wherein bilateral lesions abol- in genetic and ethanol withdrawal-induced
ish AGS (Ross and Coleman, 2000). In AGS AGS. External stimuli can induce emergent
rats, the rate-level functions ceased to be non- properties by producing oscillations that out-
monotonic and instead showed continuing last the stimulus and induce synchronized
increases with level, likely as a result of reduced burst firing in a neuronal population.
GABA activity. A deficit in GABA-mediated Faingold (2004) summarized these ob-
inhibition in IC neurons was shown to be a servations as follows: “In AGS, hyperrespon-
critical mechanism in genetic and induced sive pontine reticular formation (PRF) neu-
forms of AGS. In addition, excessive activation rons receive an exaggerated output from IC
of the excitatory glutamate receptors in the IC and other sites in the AGS network, which
8. Animal Models of Hyperacusis and Decreased Sound Tolerance 143

results in a non-linear output of pontine re- an associated risk factor for hyperacusis and
ticular formation. The synchronized increase tinnitus (Coelho et al., 2007). Related animal
in reticular neuronal responses to sensory stim- experiments suggest that early-age conductive
uli is a critical mechanism of sensory initiation hearing loss may provide an underlying cause.
of generalized seizures. Reticular neuronal re- Sun et al. (2011) demonstrated that early
sponsiveness to sensory stimuli is highly con- conductive hearing loss in rats may not only
strained under normal conditions, resulting cause hyperacusis but also AGS. The authors
in minimal conduction of repetitive stimuli tested the effects of tympanic membrane
via reticular pathways. However, in seizure- (TM) damage at an early age on sound percep-
susceptible states the extensive increases in neu­ tion development in rats. They found that two
ronal responsiveness greatly enhance reticu- weeks after the TM perforation, more than
lar output, synchronizing the firing of many 80% of the rats showed AGS when exposed
neurons that receive reticular input, including to high sound levels (120 dB SPL white noise,
the spinal cord, where the central pattern gen- <1 min). The susceptibility of AGS lasted at
erators for the convulsive behaviors are local- least 16 weeks after the TM damage, even
ized. When the numbers and distribution of after the hearing loss recovered. The TM-
affected neurons reach a critical neural mass, damaged rats also showed significantly en-
the sensory stimulus initiates generalized sei- hanced acoustic startle responses compared
zures” (p. 77). to the rats without TM damage. Sun et al.
(2011) also showed that the AGS can be sup-
pressed by the administration of vigabatrin,
an antiepileptic drug inhibiting catabolism of
Seizures and GABA. Vigabatrin also significantly reduced
Hyperacusis in Humans? the loudness response (Sun et al., 2014). These
results indicated that early-age conductive
AGS also occur in humans and may be in- hearing loss could impair sound tolerance by
duced by early-onset hearing loss or music: reducing GABA inhibition in the IC, an effect
“Musicogenic seizures usually occur in patients possibly related to hyperacusis seen in children
with focal symptomatic or cryptogenic epilep- with otitis media.
sies, but they have been reported in rare genetic
epilepsies such as Dravet syndrome. A single
LGI1 mutation has been described in a girl
with seizures evoked by auditory stimuli. Inter- Type II Auditory Nerve Fibers
estingly, heterozygous knockout (Lgi1+/−) mice
and “Pain” Hyperacusis
show susceptibility to sound-triggered seizures.
Moreover, in Frings and Black Swiss mice, the
spontaneous mutations of MASS1 and JAMS1 Recently, Flores et al. (2015) suggested that
genes, respectively, have been linked to audio- type II cochlear afferents might be involved in
genic seizures” (p. 52) (Italiano et al., 2016). the detection of noise-induced tissue damage.
Recent clinical reports also suggest that early- They implied that such damage could con-
age hearing loss may be related to hyperacu- tribute to a novel form of sensation, termed
sis. Coelho et al. (2007) found that hearing auditory nociception, potentially related to
loss was often reported in children who expe- “pain hyperacusis” (Tyler et al., 2014). Using
rienced hyperacusis and tinnitus (~37%) and immunoreactivity to cFos, Flores et al. (2015)
suggested that the mild hearing loss might be recorded neuronal activation in the brainstem
144 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

of Vglut3–/– mice, in which the type-I afferents is basically a combination of loudness recruit-
were silenced. In these deaf mice, the investi- ment augmented by the increased central gain;
gators found in cochlear nucleus neurons re- these factors cannot easily be disentangled.
sponses to hair-cell damaging noise, but not Evidence for decreased sound tolerance is only
to non-traumatic noise. The responses were clear in animal strains that are prone to AGS.
subsequently shown to originate in the co- The relation to humans is putatively found in
chlea. Flores et al. (2015) concluded that their musicogenic seizures in patients with epilepsy.
findings “imply the existence of an alternative Pain hyperacusis was recently linked to a neu-
neuronal pathway from cochlea to brainstem ral substrate in the type II auditory nerve fibers
that is activated by tissue-damaging noise involving nociception and related avoidance
and does not require glutamate release from behaviors.
IHCs” (p. 606) and does not necessarily en-
code acoustic-only stimulation. Corroboration
of this idea that Type II afferents are nocicep-
tors comes from Liu et al. (2015), who showed References
that type II afferents are activated when outer
hair cells are damaged. This response depends
Anari, M., Axelsson, A., Eliasson, A., & Magnus­
on purinergic receptors, which are ubiquitous
son, L. (1999). Hypersensitivity to sound–
and mediate transport of neurotransmitters questionnaire data, audiometry and classifica-
between cells. The purinergic receptors bind tion. Scandinavian Audiology, 28, 219–230.
ATP released from nearby supporting cells in Bourien, J., Tang, Y., Batrel, C., Huet, A., Lenoir, M.,
response to hair cell damage. Liu et al. reported Ladrech, S., . . . Wang, J. (2014). Contribution
that selective activation of the metabotropic of auditory nerve fibers to compound action
purinergic receptors increased type II afferent potential of the auditory nerve. Journal of Neu­
excitability, a putative mechanism for pain rophysiology, 112, 1025–1039.
hyperacusis. The effect of this activity as part Cai, S., Ma, W.-L. D., & Young, E. D. (2009).
of a nociceptive system could contribute to Encoding intensity in ventral cochlear nucleus
avoidance behaviors displayed by individuals following acoustic trauma: implications for
loudness recruitment. Journal of the Association
with hyperacusis.
for Research in Otolaryngology, 10, 5–22.
Carney, L. H. (1990). Sensitivities of cells in an-
teroventral cochlear nucleus of cat to spatiotem-
poral discharge patterns across primary affer-
ents. Journal of Neurophysiology, 64, 437–456.
Summary
Coelho, C. B., Sanchez, T. G., & Tyler, R. S.
(2007). Hyperacusis, sound annoyance, and
Hyperacusis in animals is indirectly inferred loudness hypersensitivity in children. Progress
from the acoustic startle reflex and from the in Brain Research, 166, 169–178.
Eggermont, J. J. (1977). Electrocochleography
driven firing rate and/or LFP amplitude re-
and recruitment. Annals of Otology Rhinology
corded along the auditory pathway. Steeper
and Laryngology, 86, 138–149.
response-stimulus curves reflect increased cen- Eggermont, J. J. (2000). Sound-induced synchro-
tral gain, which is present in all neural stations nization of neural activity between and within
from the cochlear nucleus to the auditory cor- three auditory cortical areas. Journal of Neuro­
tex. The link between increased central gain physiology, 83, 2708–2722.
and hyperacusis is only by inference, and often Faingold, C. L. (2002). Role of GABA abnormali-
by the occurrence of “over recruitment,” which ties in the inferior colliculus pathophysiology—
8. Animal Models of Hyperacusis and Decreased Sound Tolerance 145

audiogenic seizures. Hearing Research, 168, like exaggeration of the acoustic startle reflex.
223–237. Journal of the Association for Research in Otolar­
Faingold, C. L. (2004). Emergent properties of yngology, 8, 539–550.
CNS neuronal networks as targets for phar- Italiano, D., Striano, P., Russo, E., Leo, A., Spina,
macology: Application to anticonvulsant drug E., Zara, F., . . . Ferlazzo, E. (2016). Genetics
action. Progress in Neurobiology, 72, 55–85. of reflex seizures and epilepsies in humans and
Faingold, C. L., Millan, M. H., Boersma, C. A., & animals. Epilepsy Research, 121, 47–54.
Meldrum, B. S. (1988). Excitant amino ac- Jastreboff, P. J., & Hazell, J. W. P. (1993). A neu-
ids and audiogenic seizures in the genetically rophysiological approach to tinnitus: Clinical
epilepsy-prone rat. I. Afferent seizure initiation implications. British Journal of Audiology, 27,
pathway. Experimental Neurology 99, 678–686. 7–17.
Feng, H.-J., & Faingold, C. L. (2002). Repeated Knudson, I. M., & Melcher, J. R. (2016). Ele-
generalized audiogenic seizures induce plastic vated acoustic startle responses in humans:
changes on acoustically evoked neuronal firing relationship to reduced loudness discomfort
in the amygdala. Brain Research, 932, 61–69. level, but not self-report of hyperacusis. Journal
Flores, E. N., Duggan, A., Madathany, T., Hogan, of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology,
A. K., Márquez, F. G., Kumar, G., . . . García- 17, 223–235.
Añoveros, J. (2015). A non-canonical pathway Kujawa, S. G., & Liberman, M. C. (2009). Add-
from cochlea to brain signals tissue-damaging ing insult to injury: Cochlear nerve degener-
noise. Current Biology, 25, 1–7. ation after “temporary” noise-induced hearing
Formby, C., Sherlock, L. P., & Gold, S. L. (2003). loss. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 14077–14085.
Adaptive plasticity of loudness induced by Langers, D. R. M., van Dijk, P., Schoenmaker,
chronic attenuation and enhancement of the E. S., & Backes, W. H. (2007). fMRI activa-
acoustic background. Journal of the Acoustical tion in relation to sound intensity and loud-
Society of America, 114, 55–58. ness. NeuroImage, 35, 709–718.
Garcia-Cairasco, N., Terra, V. C., & Doretta, M. C. Liberman, M. C., & Dodds, L. W. (1984). Single-
(1993). Midbrain substrates of audiogenic seizures neuron labeling and chronic cochlear pathology.
in rats. Behavioural Brain Research, 58, 57–67. II. Stereocilia damage and alterations of spon-
Heinz, M. G., Issa, J. B., & Young, E. D. (2005). taneous discharge rates. Hearing Research, 16,
Auditory-nerve rate responses are inconsistent 43–53.
with common hypotheses for the neural cor- Liberman, M. C., & Kiang, N. Y. (1984). Single-
relates of loudness recruitment. Journal of the neuron labeling and chronic cochlear pathology.
Association for Research in Otolaryngology, 6, IV. Stereocilia damage and alterations in rate-
91–105. and phase-level functions. Hearing Research, 16,
Heinz, M. G., & Young, E. D. (2004). Response 75–90.
growth with sound level in auditory-nerve fi- Liu, J., Li, X., Wang, L., Dong, Y., Han, H., &
bers after noise-induced hearing loss. Journal of Liu, G. (2003). Effects of salicylate on sero-
Neurophysiology, 91, 784–795. toninergic activities in rat inferior colliculus
Hickox, A. E., & Liberman, M. C. (2014). Is and auditory cortex. Hearing Research, 175,
noise-induced cochlear neuropathy key to the 45–53.
generation of hyperacusis or tinnitus? Journal Liu, C., Glowatzki, E., & Fuchs, P. A. (2015).
of Neurophysiology, 111, 552–564. Unmyelinated type II afferent neurons report
Hurley, L. M., & Hall, I. C. (2011). Context- cochlear damage. Proceedings of the National
dependent modulation of auditory processing Academy of Sciences of the United States of Amer­
by serotonin. Hearing Research, 279, 74–84. ica, 112, 14723–14727.
Ison, J. R., Allen, P. D., & O’Neill, W. E. (2007). Marriage, J., & Barnes, N. M. (1995). Is central
Age-related hearing loss in C57BL/6J mice has hyperacusis a symptom of 5-hydroxytryptamine
both frequency-specific and non-frequency- (5-HT) dysfunction? Journal of Laryngology and
specific components that produce a hyperacusis- Otology, 109, 915–921.
146 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

Nelson, J. J., & Chen, K. (2004). The relationship Salvi, R. J., Wang, J., & Ding, D. (2000). Auditory
of tinnitus, hyperacusis, and hearing loss. Ear plasticity and hyperactivity following cochlear
Nose and Throat Journal, 83, 472–476. damage. Hearing Research, 147, 261–274.
Noreña, A. J., & Chery-Croze, S. (2007). En- Saunders, J. C., Bock, G. R., Chen, C.-S., &
riched acoustic environment rescales auditory Gates, G. R. (1972). The effects of priming for
sensitivity. Neuroreport, 18, 1251–1255. audiogenic seizures on cochlear and behavioral
Noreña, A. J., Gourévitch, B., Aizawa, N., & Egger- responses in BALB/c mice. Experimental Neu­
mont, J. J. (2006). Spectrally enhanced acoustic rology, 36, 426–436.
environment disrupts frequency representation Sherlock, L. P., & Formby, C. (2005). Estimates
in cat auditory cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 9, of loudness, loudness discomfort, and the audi-
932–939. tory dynamic range: normative estimates, com-
Papesh, M. A., & Hurley, L. M. (2012). Plasticity of parison of procedures, and test-retest reliability.
serotonergic innervation of the inferior collicu- Journal of the American Academy of Audiology,
lus in mice following acoustic trauma. Hearing 16, 85–100.
Research, 283, 89–97. Simpson, J. J., & Davies, W. E. (2000). A review
Phillips, D. P. (1987). Stimulus intensity and loud- of evidence in support of a role for 5-HT in
ness recruitment: Neural correlates. Journal of the perception of tinnitus. Hearing Research,
the Acoustical Society of America. 82, 1–12. 145, 1–7.
Pienkowski, M., & Eggermont, J. J. (2009). Re- Smith, P. H., & Rhode, W. S. (1989). Structural
covery from reorganization induced in adult and functional properties distinguish two types
cat primary auditory cortex by a band-limited of multipolar cells in the ventral cochlear nu-
spectrally enhanced acoustic environment. Hear­ cleus. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 282,
ing Research, 257, 24–40. 595–616.
Pienkowski, M., Munguia, R., & Eggermont, J. J. Sun, W., Lu, J., Stolzberg, D., Gray, L., Deng, A.,
(2011). Passive exposure of adult cats to band­ Lobarinas, E., & Salvi, R. J. (2009). Salicylate
limited tone pip ensembles or noise rapidly increases the gain of the central auditory sys-
leads to long-term response suppression in au- tem. Neuroscience, 159, 325–334.
ditory cortex. Hearing Research, 277, 117–126. Sun, W., Manohar, S., Jayaram, A., Kumaraguru, A.,
Qiu, C. X., Salvi, R., Ding, D., & Burkard, R. Fu, Q., Li, J., & Allman, B. (2011). Early
(2000). Inner hair cell loss leads to enhanced age conductive hearing loss causes audiogenic
response amplitudes in auditory cortex of un- seizure and hyperacusis behavior. Hearing Re­
anesthetized chinchillas: Evidence for increased search, 282, 178–183.
system gain. Hearing Research, 139, 153–171. Sun, W., Fu, Q., Zhang, C., Manohar, S., Kumar-
Röhl, M., & Uppenkamp, S. (2012). Neural cod- aguru, A., & Li, J. (2014). Loudness percep-
ing of sound intensity and loudness in the hu- tion affected by early age hearing loss. Hearing
man auditory system. Journal of the Association Research, 313, 18–25.
for Research in Otolaryngology, 13, 369–379. Sztuka, A., Pospiech, L., Gawron, W., & Dudek, K.
Ross, K. C., & Coleman, J. R. (2000). Develop- (2010). DPOAE in estimation of the function
mental and genetic audiogenic seizure models: of the cochlea in tinnitus patients with normal
Behavior and biological substrates. Neurosci­ hearing. Auris Nasus Larynx, 37, 55–60.
ence and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 639–653. Turner, J. G., & Larsen, D. (2016). Effects of
Salvi, R. J., Hamernik, R. P., Henderson, D., & Ah- noise exposure on development of tinnitus and
roon, W. A. (1983). Neural correlates of sensori- hyperacusis: Prevalence rates 12 months after
neural hearing loss. Ear & Hearing, 4, 115–129. exposure in middle-aged rats. Hearing Research,
Salvi, R. J., Saunders, S. S., Gratton, M. A., 334, 30–36.
Arehole, S., & Powers, N. (1990). Enhanced Turner, J. G., & Parrish, J. (2008). Gap detection
evoked response amplitudes in the inferior collic- methods for assessing salicylate-induced tinni-
ulus of the chinchilla fallowing acoustic trauma. tus and hyperacusis in rats. American Journal of
Hearing Research, 50, 245–258. Audiology, 17, S185–192.
8. Animal Models of Hyperacusis and Decreased Sound Tolerance 147

Tyler, R. S., Pienkowski, M., Roncancio, E. R., Jun, Wang, H.-T., Luo, B., Zhou, K.-Q., Xu, T.-L., &
H. J., Brozoski, T., Dauman, N., . . . Moore, Chen, L. (2006). Sodium salicylate reduces in-
B. C. (2014). A review of hyperacusis and fu- hibitory postsynaptic currents in neurons of rat
ture directions: Part I. Definitions and mani- auditory cortex. Hearing Research, 215, 77–83.
festations. American Journal of Audiology, 23, Wang, H.-T., Luo, B., Huang, Y.-N., Zhou, K.-Q., &
402–419. Chen, L. (2008). Sodium salicylate suppresses
Wang, J., Ding, D., & Salvi, R. J. (2002). Func- serotonin-induced enhancement of GABA­ergic
tional reorganization in chinchilla inferior col- spontaneous inhibitory postsynaptic currents in
liculus associated with chronic and acute co- rat inferior colliculus in vitro. Hearing Research,
chlear damage. Hearing Research, 168, 238–249. 236, 42–51.
9
Traumatic Brain Injury
and Auditory Processing
Melissa A. Papesh, Sarah M. Theodoroff,
and Frederick J. Gallun

leading cause of injury (DVBIC, 2016). In


A Brief Background on
fact, blast exposure and resulting TBI are so
Traumatic Brain Injury common among military personnel that TBI
is considered the “signature injury” of recent
Traumatic brain injury (TBI) is a form of ac- military conflicts (Hoge et al., 2008). For a
quired brain injury that occurs when a sudden review on the prevalence and risk factors of
external trauma damages the brain and disrupts auditory-related problems in Veterans and mil-
neurological function. In 2010, approximately itary personnel of the recent conflicts in Iraq
2.5 million patients were seen in emergency and Afghanistan, see Theodoroff et al. (2015).
departments across the United States for TBIs Neurological damage from impact- and
(CDC, 2015). Analysis of these visits reveals blast-related mechanisms results from the
that falls account for the vast majority of TBIs stretching or shearing of the elements com-
in both young children (up to 4 years old) and prising various neural pathways, contusions
in adults ages 45 and older. In other age groups, to the cortical surfaces that collide with the
the causes of TBIs vary considerably and often skull, injury to blood vessels, and the cascade
involve falls, assaults, motor vehicle accidents, or of inflammatory responses to tissue injury that
striking the head against objects (Figure 9–1; can both damage and heal the neural elements
CDC, 2015). Military personnel are also at risk (Taber et al., 2006, 2015; Chafi et al., 2010;
of TBI from exposure to high-intensity blast Tompkins et al., 2013). These effects can dis-
discharges, such as those generated by impro- rupt the functions of specific neural process-
vised explosive devices (IEDs), in addition to ing elements in the brainstem, thalamus, and
the more typical causes of TBI that affect ci- cortex, as well as impair the functioning of
vilians. Since the start of U.S. military oper- synaptic structures and neural connections
ations in Iraq and Afghanistan in 2000, more throughout the entire central nervous system
than 350,000 military service members have (CNS). Additional structures affected include
sustained TBIs, with blast-exposure being the the corpus callosum and other long fiber tracts

149
150 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

Fausti et al., 2009; Nampiaparampil, 2008;


Bergemalm & Lyxell, 2005; Kapoor & Ciuf-
freda, 2002). The occurrence and persistence
of these symptoms is heterogeneous across
patients, even among those with the same eti-
ology and similar severity of neural damage.
Of course, not all blows to the head re-
sult in TBI; diagnosis is based upon clinical
symptoms of neural dysfunction observed dur­
ing and shortly after the injury. Symptoms
indicative of TBI include any period of loss
of or decreased consciousness, retrograde or
Figure 9–1. Primary causes of TBI-related post-traumatic amnesia, neurological deficits
emergency room visits for civilian patients be- such as muscle weakness, loss of balance and
tween the ages of 5 and 64 years. Data were coordination, and sensory loss, as well as any
obtained from hospitals across the United alteration in mental state such as confusion
States between 2001 and 2010. Overall, falls and disorientation (CDC, 2015). The severity
are the leading cause of injury for this age of these symptoms, in combination with struc-
group (31% of TBIs), followed by the head be- tural imaging measures, is used to classify TBIs
ing forcefully struck by or against a solid object as mild, moderate, or severe (Table 9–1). The
(22%), motor vehicle accidents (MVA; 19%),
vast majority of TBIs incurred in both civilian
and assaults (18%). Source: Data obtained
and military populations fall within the mild
from CDC (2015).
classification (also known as a concussion)
which is characterized by brief or no loss of
con­sciousness, amnesia spanning no more than
24 hours, mild neurological symptoms, and
that are essential for coordination and distri- lack of abnormal findings on standard clinical
bution of information throughout the brain imaging tests immediately after injury. Most
(Taber et al., 2006, 2015). Thus, TBI has patients who sustain a mild TBI have a favor-
the capacity to disrupt a wide array of neural able outcome as symptoms resolve within a few
functions including sensory, motor, cogni- weeks post-injury. However, estimates indicate
tive, and emotional processes. Health effects that 10% to 15% of patients who incur a mild
most frequently associated with TBI include TBI will suffer from postconcussion syndrome
cognitive deficits in attention, memory, and (PCS) with symptoms lasting more than a year
executive function; behavioral and emotional after injury (Alves, 1992; Gouvier et al., 1992;
disturbances spanning aggression, depression, Binder et al., 1997). PCS specifically refers to
and impulsivity; psychological injury such as the organic and psychogenic disturbances ob-
post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD); motor served after closed head injuries including the
difficulties frequently affecting balance and subjective physical complaints (e.g., headache,
muscle tone; changes in hearing and vision dizziness), as well as cognitive, emotional, and
including increased sensitivity to light and behavioral changes (WHO, 1992). The like-
noise; and somatic symptoms such as head- lihood of a patient displaying persistent PCS
aches, sleep disturbances, and chronic pain is influenced by many factors such as location
(Verfaellie et al., 2013; Godbolt et al., 2014; and severity of injury, age at injury, and med-
9. Traumatic Brain Injury and Auditory Processing 151

Table 9–1. Symptoms used by medical professionals to classify the


severity of TBI.

TBI Classification
Mild/
Concussion Moderate Severe
Confusion/disorientation: <24 hours >24 hours >24 hours
Loss of consciousness: <30 minutes >30 minutes, >24 hours
<24 hours
Memory loss: <24 hours >24 hours, >7 days
<7 days
Brain imaging: Normal Abnormal Abnormal
Glasgow coma scale: 13–15 9–12 3–8

ical history prior to injury including occur- ever, between 50% and 75% of patients with
rence of previous head injuries (McAllister & previous TBI report auditory difficulties in
Arciniegas, 2002; Munjal et al., 2010; Silver spite of intact auditory periphery and normal
et al., 2011; Silverberg & Iverson, 2011). pure tone hearing sensitivity (Oleksiak et al.,
2012; Saunders et al., 2015; Gallun et al.,
2012a,b, 2016). The difficulties reported by
these patients are consistent with altered pro-
Auditory Consequences of TBI
cessing in the central auditory nervous system
(CANS) and often include increased sensi-
The entire CNS, including the peripheral and tivity to noise, poor speech understanding in
central auditory mechanism, is at risk of dam- competing background noise or reverberation,
age from TBI. The extreme mechanical forces problems understanding on the telephone or
that inflict TBI can produce damage to the when the talker’s face is unseen, difficulty fol-
auditory periphery in the form of tympanic lowing conversations among groups of peo­
membrane rupture, disarticulation of the os- ple, recalling auditory information, and audi­
sicular chain, and/or inner ear damage caused tory fatigue (Ryan & Warden, 2003; Fausti
by tearing of the oval or round window or et al., 2009; Dikmen et al., 2010; Schultz et al.,
fracture of the temporal bone (Darley & Kell- 2011; Papesh & Gallun, 2016; Saunders et al.,
man, 2010). Conductive and sensorineural 2015; Gallun et al., 2016). Current guidelines
hearing losses that result from these types of from the American Speech-Language-Hearing
injuries may be remediated using traditional Association (ASHA; 2005) and the American
otological and audiological methods such as Academy of Audiology (AAA, 2010) agree
surgical repair of structural damage and/or that even mild TBI commonly results in long-
provision of hearing assistive devices (Fausti term CNS damage that may produce auditory
et al., 2009; Darley & Kellman, 2010). How- dysfunction.
152 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

Auditory processing deficits can arise works in response to widespread damage (Al-
from damage specific to the auditory pathway wis et al., 2012, 2013; Greer et al., 2013). The
as well as non-auditory-specific neural damage. resulting functional impairments in cognitive,
Although a characteristic of mild TBI is lack of motor, and sensory systems are frequently ob-
observable structural damage on standard clini- served years after injury, even in cases of mild
cal imaging scans, more advanced imaging tech- and moderate TBI (Draper & Ponsford, 2008;
niques such as magnetic resonance spectroscopy Little et al., 2010; Fischer et al., 2014; Song
(    Jantzen, 2010), diffusion tensor imaging (Be- et al., 2016).
langer et al., 2007), and resting state functional Many of the brain areas listed above are
magnetic resonance imaging (    Johnson et al., intimately involved in the processing of audi-
2012) have recently been employed to examine tory information. The temporal lobe houses
neurological integrity in patients with mild TBI. both the primary auditory cortex, which re-
Data from these studies combined with animal ceives information from the medial genicu-
models of mild TBI have revealed the presence late body of the thalamus and lower auditory
of chronic diffuse axonal injury (DAI) and neu- brainstem centers, and the auditory associa-
ral restructuring in areas including the tempo- tion cortex, which is involved in recognition
ral and frontal cortices (Niogi & Mukherjee, and integration of auditory information with
2010; Johnson et al., 2012), interhemispheric other neural processes. In cases of focal neural
tracts including the corpus callosum (Koliatsos injury, damage to the auditory cortex is asso-
et al., 2011; Davenport et al., 2012; Petrie et al., ciated with a range of functional disabilities
2014; Wright et al., 2016), cortical-subcortical including impaired auditory pattern recog-
tracts (Davenport et al., 2012; Petrie et al., nition (Musiek & Pinheiro, 1987; Musiek
2014), fronto-parietal tracts (Davenport et al., et al., 1990; von Steinbüchel et al., 1999), dich-
2012; Johnson et al., 2012), and the cerebellum otic listening (Musiek, 1983; Mueller, 1987),
(Koliatsos et al., 2011). Additional evidence of and temporal discrimination (Mueller, 1987),
structural damage may include scarring of brain as well as reduced performance on tests of
tissue at junctions between brain parenchyma speech perception in noise and rapidly spoken
and fluids, at junctions between gray and white speech (Bergemalm & Borg, 2005). Damage to
matter (Shively et al., 2016), stunted dendritic interhemispheric tracts including the corpus
outgrowths in GABAergic cortical neurons callosum produce deficits in dichotic listen­
(Carron et al., 2016), mossy fiber sprouting in ing performance (Musiek, 1983). Cortical and
the hippocampus and increases in excitatory subcortical tracts carry auditory information
synapses (Hunt et al., 2011), and excitatory between the cortex and brainstem and coor-
input to cortical pyramidal neurons (    Jin et al., dinate auditory processing with other neural
2011). In the chronic phase of mild TBI, there regions. Damage to these pathways, as well as
is considerable evidence of hyperexcitability in imbalance of excitatory and inhibitory neural
sensory cortical areas (Alwis et al., 2012, 2013; responses in sensory cortices following brain
Carron et al., 2016) largely due to decreased injury, may reduce the ability to encode and
activity of inhibitory neurotransmitters and re- discriminate basic characteristics of acoustic
sulting imbalance of excitatory and inhibitory signals such as changes in the frequency, in-
cortical networks (Yi et al., 2006; Buriticá tensity, and timing of sounds (von Steinbüchel
et al., 2009; Pavlov et al., 2011). This pattern of et al., 1999; Fausti et al., 2009). Such deficits
cortical hyperexcitability is likely a consequence may underlie more complex functional issues
of maladaptive restructuring of cortical net- frequently reported by patients including in-
9. Traumatic Brain Injury and Auditory Processing 153

creased sensitivity to noise and difficulty un- regulation of both auditory stimuli and the re-
derstanding degraded speech. sponses they engender.
Mild TBI produces widespread and du-
rable structural and functional changes that
influence auditory function even when asso-
ciated with damage to non-auditory-specific Sound Intolerance
regions. Additional areas that frequently show
chronic structural and physiological changes It follows that poor pre- and post-attentive
following mild TBI include regions involved regulation of auditory information flow stem-
in cognitive tasks such as the prefrontal cor- ming from damage to auditory and non-
tex and hippocampus (McAllister et al., 1999; auditory pathways likely contributes to sound
Palacios et al., 2013), the limbic system which intolerance in patients with previous TBI.
regulates emotion (Capizzano et al., 2010; The neuropsychological evaluation completed
Zhu et al., 2014), and regions involved in for all Veterans suspected of having sustained
multisensory integration such as the cerebel- a TBI routinely has patients rate their sensi-
lum (Spanos et al., 2007; Potts et al., 2009). tivity to noise on a scale from 1 (none/not a
Current theories of cognitive control indicate problem) to 5 (severe/cannot function with-
that brain centers associated with cognition out help) (DVA, 2007). Between 18 and 32%
modulate incoming sensory information, and of these Veterans report being “bothered by
provide top-down control which sculpts per- noise” even after one year of recovery from TBI
ception and behavioral responses (Mesulam, (Dikmen et al., 2010). Although sensitivity to
2002; Wolf & Koch, 2016). Deficits in higher- noise is commonly reported in patients with
order executive function including memory, previous TBI (Ryan & Warden, 2003; Cice-
attention, and cognitive capacity may impair rone & Kalmar, 1995; Jury & Flynn, 2001),
auditory performance as such deficits reduce the prevalence of this problem has not been
the availability of resources required to pro- well established (Halbauer et al., 2009; Reid
cess auditory stimuli. et al., 2014). This gap in knowledge is due, in
Mental health status is also known to af- part, to the poorly defined nature of sensitivity
fect auditory responses. PTSD and anxiety dis- to noise as well as lack of consistent terminol-
orders amplify acoustic startle reflexes as indi- ogy used to describe and report it (e.g., noise
cated by larger amplitude and lower threshold sensitivity, hyperacusis, phonophobia, miso-
reflexive myogenic responses to unexpected phonia, loudness intolerance). Often, these
auditory stimuli (Morgan et al., 1996; Glover terms are used interchangeably by clinicians
et al., 2011). Similarly, patients with depression and researchers. Tyler et al. (2014) review the
are widely reported to have increased sensitiv- various definitions put forth in the literature
ity to noise and abnormal electrophysiological and suggest a new nomenclature to describe
findings that include significantly steeper cor- various types of hyperacusis, although it is
tical amplitude-intensity functions, increased important to keep in mind that hyperacusis
P200 amplitudes, and prolonged P300 laten- and noise sensitivity may be different types of
cies compared to patients with no signs of de- sound intolerance disorders. Ideally, any clas-
pression (Vandoolaeghe et al., 1998; Gallinat sification system would be informed by objec-
et al., 2000; Gopal et al., 2004). Overall, these tive measures sensitive to differentiating the
examples demonstrate that patients with pre- various types of sound intolerance disorders
vious TBI are at increased risk of poor neural rather than based solely on subjective report. It
154 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

is likely that hyperacusis and noise sensitivity these areas often results in improvements in
are distinct phenomena characterized by dif- other areas. Thus, treatments for TBI-related
ferent patterns of physical and emotional re- cognitive or psychological disorders may in
sponses to sound. For example, when a patient fact improve auditory function (Dundon et al.,
with previous TBI reports “noise sensitivity,” 2015). Unfortunately, there is often a ten-
their statement could indicate any of the fol- dency to attribute auditory dysfunction to
lowing: (a) that high, moderate, or low noise lev­ co-morbidities rather than attempt to specif-
els induce physical sensations of pain; (b) that ically treat the auditory concerns (Oleksiak
the patient has extreme difficulty listening to et al., 2012). While patients are likely to bene­
and understanding speech even in low lev­ fit from treatment of co-morbid disorders,
els of background noise; (c) that background the need for auditory-specific rehabilitation
noise induces emotional responses such as fear should not be discounted; just as improve-
or agitation; or (d) some combination of these ments in co-morbid conditions may result in
symptoms. Unfortunately, the majority of lit- improved auditory processing, improvements
erature available on such sound intolerance in auditory processing may reduce severity of
disorders lacks well-defined self-report metrics co-morbid conditions as well.
and rarely includes rigorous psychophysical Audiological evaluation of patients with
assessment of auditory function (for a rare previous TBI begins with a comprehensive ex-
exception, we refer readers to Ellermier et al., amination including a thorough case history
2001). The validity of LDLs to assess sensitiv- reviewing medical history, previous head inju-
ity to noise has been questioned due to poor ries, and details of the specific difficulties the
sensitivity for diagnosing hyperacusis (Shel- patient is having. As described above, patients
drake et al., 2015), the substantial influence of who are found to have conductive or senso-
methodological differences across clinics and rineural hearing loss are routinely treated via
experimental protocols (Punch et al., 2004), traditional otological and audiological means,
and weak correlations between patients’ LDLs including surgical intervention for repairable
and their self-reported intolerance of sound in structural damage and hearing aids for perma-
everyday life (Zaugg et al., 2016). nent hearing loss. Those who have auditory
difficulties with normal audiometric thresh-
olds should be further evaluated for central
auditory processing disorders (APD). Even
Auditory Evaluation and mild TBIs can produce chronic auditory pro-
cessing deficits that are extremely varied across
Treatment of Patients with TBI
patients. Deficits may include poor auditory
discrimination, decreased auditory temporal
Because dysfunction stemming from TBI is of- processing, sensitivity to noise, difficulty with
ten multifaceted in nature, a multidisciplinary auditory pattern processing, poor dichotic lis-
team approach to assessment and treatment is tening, and abnormally poor auditory perfor-
warranted. A full assessment considering audi- mance in competing acoustic signals or when
tory as well as language, memory, and attention auditory signals are otherwise degraded (Berge-
abilities is required as these higher-order func- malm & Borg, 2005; Bressler et al., 2016;
tions are often affected in patients with TBI. Downie et al., 2002; Dundon et al., 2015;
Due to the interconnected nature of neural Fausti et al., 2009; Gallun et al., 2012, 2016;
processing underlying sensory, cognitive, and Hoover et al., 2015; Mueller et al., 1987). Ta-
emotional regulation, improvements in one of ble 9–2 shows several common clinical tests of
9. Traumatic Brain Injury and Auditory Processing 155

Table 9–2. Rates of abnormal performance by patients with previous TBI on clinical tests
of central auditory processing.

% Abnormal
Central Auditory Processing Tests Performance
Speech in Noise Comprehension
Hearing in Noise Test (HINT); Words in Noise (WIN); Quick
Speech-in-Noise (QuickSIN); Listening in Spatialized Noise; 28–59.2%
SCAN and SCAN-A
Temporal Processing
Gaps-in-Noise (GIN); Adaptive Tests of Temporal Resolution (ATTR) 31–60.7%
Auditory Pattern Recognition
Frequency Patterns Test (FPT); Duration Pattern Test (DPT) 19–64%
Dichotic Listening Tests
Dichotic Digits Test (DDT); Staggered Spondaic Words Test (SSW) 14–64%
Binaural Interaction
Masking Level Difference Test (MLD) 10–20%
Note. Central auditory processing tests are grouped according to the specific auditory processes
which they target. Abnormal percentages were derived from studies that included participants with
at least one TBI or blast exposure incurred from 1 year to 10 years prior to testing. References for
abnormal test performance values include Van Toor et al., 2006; Bergemalm & Lyxell, 2005; Turgeon
et al., 2011; Gallun et al., 2012; Saunders et al., 2015; and Gallun et al., 2016.

central auditory processing grouped according that changes in cortical and cognitive audi-
to auditory tasks which are frequently found to tory evoked potentials (AEP) are often ob­
be deficient in patients with previous TBI. The servable even in mild cases of TBI (Gallun
percentage of abnormal performance for each et al., 2012; Duncan et al., 2011; Folmer et al.,
of these subgroups was derived from published 2011; Lew et al., 2004). Cortical and cognitive
studies of patients with normal pure tone au- AEPs used most often to probe auditory func-
diograms who had sustained a mild TBI be- tion in patients with TBI include the N100,
tween 1 and 10 years prior to testing. P200, N200, mismatch negativity, and P300
Electrophysiological assessments may also responses. In comparison to healthy controls,
aid the evaluation and rehabilitation of au- patients who have suffered TBI most often
ditory concerns following TBI (Fausti et al., demonstrate reduced response amplitudes,
2009; Lew et al., 2007; Folmer et al., 2011). longer latencies, or both (Lew et al., 2004,
While responses generated by the auditory 2007; Gallun et al., 2012; Bressler et al., 2016;
brainstem are generally found to be abnormal Folmer et al., 2011; Elting et al., 2008; Rapp
only in cases of moderate to severe brain dam- et al., 2015; Sun et al., 2015). AEP measures
age (   Jabbari et al., 1987; Folmer et al., 2011; also reveal differences in brain activity when
Bressler et al., 2016), several studies indicate auditory tasks are compared between controls
156 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

and TBI patients. Multiple studies have shown beneficial for TBI patients with APD (Musiek
that although brain-injured patients are often et al., 2004). This technique asks participants
able to perform simple auditory tasks with the to select two different listening materials to
same ability as healthy controls (albeit often be presented to each ear simultaneously. The
with longer response times), the scalp topog- intensity of the dichotic material is initially
raphy of AEP responses clearly demonstrates set so that the better-performing ear receives
differences in the recruitment of various brain a signal at a lower sensation level compared to
regions to perform these tasks (Larson et al., that presented to the poorer ear, thereby mak-
2007; Elting et al., 2008; Gallun et al., 2012; ing it easier for the patient to hear the stimuli
Kaipio et al., 2013). presented to the poorer ear. Over time, the
Due to the heterogeneous nature of audi- level of the stimuli in the two ears is slowly
tory deficits arising from head injury, a patient/ adjusted with the goal of achieving similar
deficit-specific approach to treatment is war- levels of intensity and performance for both
ranted (Bellis, 2002). Treatment strategies are ears. A benefit to this type of intervention is
often comprised of a combination of auditory that patients can choose their own training
training and general management interven- materials and times for training sessions.
tions. With auditory training, the hope is to Assistive devices for hearing have also
elicit experience-based neural plasticity within proven helpful to many patients with normal
the auditory pathway designed to sharpen hearing thresholds and APD (Johnston et al.,
specific auditory functions (Ross-Swain et al., 2009; Bamiou et al., 2006). Such devices pri-
2007; Chermak & Musiek, 2002). In patients oritize improving the relative levels of speech
with previous TBIs, such retraining targets and background noise, the so-called signal-
encoding and regulation of auditory informa- to-noise ratio. A recent study examined the
tion (Murphy et al., 2011). Training is most efficacy of administering a personal ear-level
effective during heightened states of neural frequency modulation (FM) system to young
plasticity, such as during youth and soon after and middle-aged patients with normal hear­
the brain injury has been incurred (Chermak & ing thresholds who had suffered mild TBI
Musiek, 2002). In order for training to pro- within the past 10 years (Saunders et al., 2014).
duce optimal results, it must focus on a specific Study participants found numerous ways to
deficit and provide adequate challenge before use the FM systems to benefit them through-
progressing to more difficult tasks. The trainee out the day, and usage remained high through-
must be motivated and actively participate to out the 8-week study duration. For example,
the best of their abilities; to do so, they must several participants were students who reported
commit to the training exercises on a regular difficulty hearing in classroom environments
schedule (Chermak & Musiek, 2002). While and expressed great benefit from being able to
some auditory training programs are available place the FM microphone near or on the in-
commercially, the majority of these are specif- structor. This likely had the effect of both im-
ically designed for pediatric populations (Ba- proving the signal-to-noise ratio of the instruc-
miou et al., 2006) and do not target specific tor’s voice as well as reducing the deleterious
TBI-related deficits. effects of reverberation in classrooms.
Less-structured training programs, such Individuals also reported benefit of the
as dichotic interaural intensity training for pa- FM system in the workplace. One participant
tients who have one ear that performs signifi- was required to attend weekly team meetings
cantly better than the other, may also prove at which co-workers gathered around a large
9. Traumatic Brain Injury and Auditory Processing 157

table to discuss relevant topics. He expressed need for rehabilitation and changes in com-
difficulty following the conversation as it munication strategies. Counseling should also
switched among talkers. However, by placing include factors such as environmental modifi-
his FM microphone either in the center of the cations to improve listening, encouraging the
table or by passing it to various coworkers to patient to make active requests of communica-
speak into, he was able to keep up with the tion partners to facilitate communication, and
conversation and not miss out on important encouraging patients to use additional strategies
information. Another patient was employed at such as communicating important messages in
a canine kennel and had great difficulty hear- writing as opposed to verbally.
ing her co-workers in the reverberant kennel
area, even when the dogs were being relatively
quiet. By placing the FM microphone near the
Research Priorities
front desk, her co-workers were able to com-
municate with her effectively, thereby reducing
stress and improving job satisfaction. Usage of Although significant advancements have been
the FM systems was so successful that many made regarding the long-term consequences
participants requested to keep their devices at of TBI on the auditory system, a considerable
the conclusion of the study. number of questions remain unanswered. For
Similar positive outcomes have been re- example, it is presently unclear whether the
ported when assistive devices are provided for etiology of head injury is important when de-
pediatric patients with APD (Johnston et al., termining either the type or extent of auditory
2009). Other interventions likely to improve dysfunction that a patient may experience. It
auditory performance in normally hearing is currently unknown whether a TBI resulting
patients with APD concerns now include low- from a fall is likely to cause the same types
amplification hearing aids, as many employ of auditory impairments as those resulting
algorithms designed to improve the signal-to- from vehicle accidents or exposure to high-
noise ratio in various listening environments intensity blast waves. A better understanding
and thus provide a less effortful listening ex- of how different traumatic insults to the brain
perience for users (Stach et al., 1991; Pichora- could impair auditory function may improve
Fuller, 2003). Although many audiology clin- our ability to detect and rehabilitate auditory
ics provide FM systems or low-amplification conditions. This work will require increased
hearing aids to normally hearing patients with application of advanced imaging techniques
APD concerns, the long-term benefits from in patients with TBIs of varying etiologies, as
such interventions are not yet known. well as long-term studies comparing the au-
Lastly, all patients with previous TBI and ditory behavioral and physiological outcomes
auditory processing difficulties should receive of diverse types of TBI.
communication counseling (Bellis, 2002; Ba- Another essential question that remains
miou et al., 2006). Validating auditory com- unanswered is why some individuals will sus-
plaints in this population, and explaining that tain lasting auditory consequences from head
auditory issues can persist in spite of “normal” injury while others will have full recovery of
hearing thresholds, can substantially reduce the auditory function. Several factors are likely to
social and emotional burden of these problems contribute to such individual variability in-
on patients as such counseling helps convince cluding the age at which the injury occurs,
both the patient and their support group of the presence of preexisting behavioral, physical,
158 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

or neurological conditions, genetic factors, telephone, problems paying attention to peo­


socioeconomic factors, and life history. Addi- ple speaking, and confusing similar-sounding
tional large-scale studies are needed to tease words. Comprehensive audiological assessment
out the impact of these various factors. revealed normal pure tone thresholds (Fig­
ure 9–2A), normal functioning on immittance
measures, and present distortion product oto-
acoustic emissions from 750 to 8000 Hz in
Case Study
both the right and left ears. Word recognition
scores were 100% in both ears when presented
A male Veteran (MV) recently presented to an in quiet. MV was subsequently referred to the
audiology clinic in the US at the age of 52 stat- VA’s auditory processing disorder clinic. Here,
ing that he had noticed a substantial decrease in he was tested on the SCAN-A (including all
his hearing ability following his military service subtests), Gaps-in-Noise test, the pitch pat­
as well as the onset of constant tinnitus. MV tern test, Words-in-Noise, QuickSIN, Dich­
served in the Navy and Army National Guard otic Digits, and the Staggered Spondaic Words
between 1977 and 2009. He served many do- test. Results indicated performance within the
mestic and international service missions, in- normal range on all subtests of the SCAN-A,
cluding deployments to Egypt, Cuba, and Iraq. the Words-in-Noise test, the QuickSIN, Dich-
He was exposed to three bomb blasts during his otic Digits, and the Staggered Spondaic Words
time in Iraq, with the most severe re­­sulting from test. However, his performance on the Gaps-
an improvised explosive device in 2008, approx- in-Noise test revealed poor temporal acuity in
imately 6 years prior to presenting in the VA both ears (thresholds of 8 and 10 ms in the
audiology clinic. Bodily damage incurred from right and left ears, respectively), as well as poor
the blast included a TBI, ruptured discs and ver- auditory pattern recognition on the Pitch Pat-
tebrae throughout his spine, broken nose, per- tern Test (52% and 80% correct in the right
manent damage to his right arm and knee, and and left ears, respectively). Additional testing
loss of teeth. In spite of his injuries, he managed indicated minimal benefit from spatial separa-
to rescue three comrades from a burning vehi- tion between sound sources, and electrophysi-
cle, for which he was awarded a Purple Heart. ological measurement of the P300 in response
At the time he was seen at the VA audiology to a tonal oddball paradigm revealed minimal
clinic, he had previously undergone cognitive response to a 1000 Hz rare tone embedded in a
rehabilitation treatment through a polytrauma series of 500 Hz tones (Figure 9–2B).
clinic, as well as treatment from speech lan- MV was subsequently diagnosed with an
guage pathology services for concerns related auditory processing disorder stemming from
to cognitive difficulties. His additional medical poor temporal processing. His reduced tempo-
diagnoses included PTSD, anxiety, obstructive ral acuity likely accounted for poor recognition
sleep apnea, chronic headaches, type 2 diabe- of auditory temporal patterns as well as lack
tes mellitus, sensitivity to light, colitis, chronic of the benefit that most listeners receive from
knee and back pain, hyperlipidemia, weakness spatial separation between talkers of interest
and numbness of the right arm, and coronary and competing background sounds. The tem-
heart disease. An intake interview revealed that poral smearing of sounds resulting from poor
his primary auditory complaints included dif- temporal acuity was likely responsible for his
ficulty hearing in noise and in the presence of reported difficulties hearing in complex lis-
multiple talkers, difficulty understanding on the tening environments and on the telephone as
9. Traumatic Brain Injury and Auditory Processing 159

Figure 9–2. A. Pure tone thresholds obtained in the right and


left ears of patient MV. B. Responses to the rare stimulus during
a P300 oddball paradigm during which 80% of trials consisted of
a 500-Hz tone (frequent stimulus) and 20% of trials consisted of a
1000-Hz tone (rare stimulus). The neural response of patient MV
is shown in comparison to an age- and hearing-matched control
participant.

well as his confusion of various word sounds. the intensity of speech without commensu-
To remediate these issues, his audiologist pre- rate increases in the intensity of competing
scribed a two-pronged approach including the sounds. This effect would putatively assist in
use of low-amplification hearing aids with an difficult listening conditions. Communication
FM streaming system, as well as counseling on strategies discussed included counseling on the
environmental modifications and communica- types of environments that were most condu-
tion strategies to improve signal-to-noise ratios cive to listening, making his communication
during listening. The low-amplification hear- partners aware of his hearing issues and ask-
ing aids, including directional microphones ing them to get his attention before speaking
and the FM system would, in practice, increase to him, conversing in well-lit areas where his
160 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

communication partner’s face could be easily with more robust and synchronous neural fir-
seen, and recommendations to schedule im- ing in response to auditory stimuli (Dallos &
portant meetings earlier in the day when he was Cheatham, 1976; Billings et al., 2009). Thus,
well rested and less likely to be fatigued. it is conceivable that MV’s temporal processing
Eight weeks later, MV was seen for a issues were somewhat ameliorated by the slight
follow-up visit. Data logging indicated that he increase in signal levels provided by the mild
was consistently wearing his hearing aids an amplification of the hearing aid as well as the
average of 8 hours per day and he described improved signal-to-noise ratio.
them to be “perfect.” Although it is often
assumed that patients with normal hearing
thresholds and PTSD are poor candidates for
Summary
low-amplification hearing aids due to their
increased startle response, MV stated that he
felt his hearing aids provided him a better TBI, regardless of etiology, can result in a host
sense of his surroundings which reduced his of chronic neurological disorders affecting au-
overall level of anxiety and propensity to be dition as well as cognition, emotion, motor
startled. His responses on International Out- control, and other sensory systems. For this
come Inventory for Hearing Aids (IOI-HA) reason, audiologists are important members of
and the Client Oriented Scale of Improvement the multidisciplinary team needed for the ap-
(COSI) revealed significant improvements in propriate assessment and rehabilitation of af-
his function in background noise and while fected patients. Even when auditory pure tone
on the telephone, and he reported less fatigue thresholds remain within the clinically normal
at the end of the day because it was “easier to range, a significant proportion of patients with
hear.” His family had also noted positive im- previous head injuries report chronic auditory
provements, not only in his communication problems. Common complaints include tinni-
abilities but also regarding reduced frustration. tus, difficulty understanding speech in compet-
Two years later, MV is still wearing his hearing ing background noise and on the telephone,
aids regularly. difficulty with rapid speech, problems attend-
MV’s perceived benefit from hearing ing to and recalling speech, and hypersensi-
aids likely stems from multiple factors. First, tivity to noise and other types of sound. The
the noise reduction algorithms and directional specific auditory processing deficits that under-
microphones employed by the hearing as well lie such concerns can vary considerably across
as use of the FM system probably resulted in patients even when the cause and severity of
a more favorable signal-to-noise ratio, thus re- TBI is similar. For this reason, patients with
ducing MV’s listening effort. Second, the non- auditory complaints should receive thorough
linear fast-acting compression characteristics examinations of auditory processing abilities
of modern hearing aids, which favor amplifi- in addition to careful probing of the specific
cation of low-level signals compared to higher situations and instances that are problematic.
level signals, have been shown to facilitate dis- Rehabilitation efforts should focus on improv-
crimination of speech signals from noise back- ing specific deficits and may include auditory
ground and to improve listeners’ ability to “lis- training programs (when appropriate) and pro-
ten in the dips” of fluctuating background noise vision of assistive listening devices such as FM
(Gatehouse et al., 2003). Lastly, multiple lines systems and/or low-amplification hearing aids.
of evidence suggest that higher signal levels and Communication counselling should be pro-
lower levels of background noise are associated vided to all patients both to help them better
9. Traumatic Brain Injury and Auditory Processing 161

understand the nature of their hearing deficits, Bergemalm, P. O., & Lyxell, B. (2005). Appear-
and to empower them to actively participate in ances are deceptive—long-term cognitive and
improving their communication abilities. central auditory sequelae from closed head in-
jury. International Journal of Audiology, 44(1),
39–49.
Billings, C. J., Tremblay, K. L., Stecker, G. C., &
References Tolin, W. M. (2009). Human evoked cortical
activity to signal-to-noise ratio and absolute sig­
nal level. Hearing Research, 254(1), 15–24.
AAA. (2010). Taskforce to establish practice guide- Binder, L. M., Rohling, M. L., & Larrabee, G. J.
lines for the diagnosis, treatment and manage- (1997). A review of mild head trauma. Part I:
ment of children and adults with central audi- Meta-analytic review of neuropsychological stud­
tory processing disorder. Retrieved Aug, 2010, ies. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuro­
from http://www.audiology.org/resources/docu psychology,19, 421–431.
mentlibrary/Pages/CentralAuditoryProcessing Bressler, S., Goldberg, H., & Shinn-Cunningham, B.
Disorder.aspx. (2016). Sensory coding and cognitive processing
Alves, W. M. (1992). Natural history of postcon- of sound in Veterans with blast exposure. Hear­
cussive signs and symptoms. Physical Medicine ing Research. In press.
and Rehabilitation, 6, 21–32. Buritica, E., Villamil, L., Guzman, F., et al. (2009).
Alwis, D. S., Yan, E. B., Morganti-Kossmann, Changes in calcium-binding protein expression
M. C., & Rajan, R. (2012). Sensory cortex un- in human cortical contusion tissue. Journal of
derpinnings of traumatic brain injury deficits. Neurotrauma, 26(12), 2145–2155.
PLoS One, 7(12), e52169. Capizzano, A. A., Jorge, R. E., & Robinson, R. G.
Alwis, D. S., Johnstone, V., Yan, E., & Rajan, R. (2010). Limbic metabolic abnormalities in re-
(2013). Diffuse traumatic brain injury and the mote traumatic brain injury and correlation with
sensory brain. Clinical and Experimental Phar­ psychiatric morbidity and social functioning. The
macology and Physiology, 40(7), 473–483. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosci­
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. ences, 22(4), 370–377.
(2005). (Central) Auditory Processing Disor- Carron, S. F., Alwis, D. S., & Rajan, R. (2016).
ders [Technical Report]. Working Group on Traumatic brain injury and neuronal function-
Auditory Processing Disorders. Rockville, MD: ality changes in sensory cortex. Frontiers in Sys­
ASHA. tems Neuroscience, 10, 47.
Bamiou, D. E., Campbell, N., & Sirimanna, T. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.
(2006). Management of auditory processing (2015). Report to Congress on Traumatic Brain
disorders. Audiological Medicine, 4(1), 46–56. Injury in the United States: Epidemiology and
Belanger, H. G., Vanderploeg, R. D., Curtiss, G., & Rehabilitation. National Center for Injury Pre­
Warden, D. L. (2007). Recent neuroimaging vention and Control; Division of Uninten-
techniques in mild traumatic brain injury, Jour­ tional Injury Prevention. Atlanta, GA.
nal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, Chafi, M. S., Karami, G., & Ziejewski, M. (2010).
19(1), 5–20. Biomechanical assessment of brain dynamic re-
Bellis, T. J. (2002). Developing deficit-specific sponses due to blast pressure waves. Annals of
intervention plans for individuals with audi- Biomedical Engineering, 38(2), 490–504.
tory processing disorders. Seminars in Hearing, Chermak, G. D., & Musiek, F. E. (2002). Auditory
23(4), 287–296. training: principles and approaches for remedi-
Bergemalm, P. O., & Borg, E. (2005). Peripheral ating and managing auditory processing disor-
and central audiological sequelae of closed head ders. Seminars in Hearing, 23(04), 297–308.
injury: function, activity, participation and qual- Cicerone, K. D., & Kalmar, K. (1995). Persistent
ity of life. Audiological Medicine, 3(3), 185–198. postconcussion syndrome: The structure of
162 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

subjective complaints after mTBI. Journal of Ellemeier, W., Eigenstetter, M., Zimmer, K.
Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 10, 1–17. (2001). Psychoacoustic correlates of individual
Dallos, P., & Cheatham, M. A. (1976). Compound noise sensitivity, Journal of the Acoustical Society
action potential (AP) tuning curves. The Jour­ of America, 109(4), 1461–1473.
nal of the Acoustical Society of America, 59(3), Elting, J. W., Maurits, N., van Weerden, T., Spik-
591–597. man, J., De Keyser, J., & van der Naalt, J.
Darley, D. S., & Kellman, R. M. (2010). Otologic (2008). P300 analysis techniques in cognitive im­
considerations of blast injury. Disaster medicine pairment after brain injury: Comparison with
and public health preparedness, 4(02), 145–152. neuropsychological and imaging data. Brain In­
Davenport, N. D., Lim, K. O., Armstrong, M. T. jury, 22(11), 870–881.
et al. (2012). Diffuse and spatially variable white Fausti, S. A., Wilmington, D. J., Gallun, F. J., Myers,
matter disruptions are associated with blast- P. J., & Henry, J. A. (2009). Auditory and ves-
related mild traumatic brain injury. Neuroimage, tibular dysfunction associated with blast-related
59(3), 2017–2024. traumatic brain injury. Journal of Rehabilitation Re­
Department of Veterans Affairs. (2007). Screening search and Development 46(6), 797–810.
and evaluation of possible traumatic brain in­ Fischer, B. L., Parsons, M., Durgerian, S. et al.
jury in Operation Enduring Freedom (OEF) and (2014). Neural activation during response in-
Operation Iraqi Freedom (OIF) Veterans. Wash- hibition differentiates blast from mechanical
ington (DC): Department of Veterans Affairs, causes of mild to moderate traumatic brain in-
Veterans Health Administration. jury. Journal of Neurotrauma, 31(2), 169–179.
Dikmen, S., Machamer, J., Fann, J. R., & Tem- Folmer, R. L., Billings, C. J., Diedesch-Rouse, A. C.
kin, N. R. (2010). Rates of symptom reporting et al. (2011). Electrophysiological assessments of
following traumatic brain injury. Journal of the cognition and sensory processing in TBI: Appli-
International Neuropsychological Society, 16(3), cations for diagnosis, prognosis and rehabilita-
401–411. tion. International Journal of Psychophysics, 82(1),
Defense and Veterans Brain Injury Center. (2016). 4–15.
DoD TBI Worldwide Numbers since 2000. Re- Gallinat, J., Bottlender, R., Juckel, G., Munke-
trieved from http://dvbic.dcoe.mil/dod-world Puchner, A., Stotz, G., Kuss, H. J., . . . Hegerl, U.
wide-numbers-tbi. (2000). The loudness dependency of the audi-
Downie, A. L., Jakobson, L. S., Frisk, V., & Ush- tory evoked N1/P2-component as a predictor
ycky, I. (2002). Auditory temporal processing of the acute SSRI response in depression. Psy­
deficits in children with periventricular brain in­ chopharmacology, 148(4), 404–411.
jury. Brain and Language, 80(2), 208–225. Gallun, F., Lewis, M., Folmer, R., Hutter, M., Pa-
Draper, K., & Ponsford, J. (2008). Cognitive func- pesh, M., Belding, H., and Leek, M. (2016).
tioning ten years following traumatic brain in- Chronic effects of exposure to high-intensity
jury and rehabilitation. Neuropsychology, 22(5), blasts: Results of tests of central auditory pro-
618. cessing. Journal of Rehabilitation Research and
Duncan, C. C., Summers, A. C., Perla, E. J., Co- Development, 53(6), 705–720.
burn, K. L., & Mirsky, A. F. (2011). Evaluation Gallun, F. J., Diedesch, A. M., Kubli, L., Walden, T.,
of traumatic brain injury: Brain potentials in Folmer, R. L., Lewis, M. S., . . . Leek, M. R.
diagnosis, function, and prognosis. Interna­ (2012a). Performance on tests of central audi-
tional Journal of Psychophysiology, 82(1), 24–40. tory processing by individuals exposed to high-
Dundon, N. M., Dockree, S. P., Buckley, V., Mer- intensity blasts. Journal of Rehabilitation Research
riman, N., Carton, M., Clarke, S., . . . Dock- and Development, 49(7), 1005–1024.
ree, P. M. (2015). Impaired auditory selective Gallun, F. J., Lewis, M. S., Folmer, R. L., Diedesch,
attention ameliorated by cognitive training with A. C., Kubli, L. R., McDermott, D. J., . . . Leek,
graded exposure to noise in patients with trau- M. R. (2012b). Implications of blast exposure
matic brain injury. Neuropsychologia, 75, 74–87. for central auditory function: a review. Journal of
9. Traumatic Brain Injury and Auditory Processing 163

Rehabilitation Research and Development, 49(7), Hunt, R. F., Scheff, S. W., & Smith, B. N. (2011).
1059–1074. Synaptic reorganization of inhibitory hilar in-
Glover, E. M., Phifer, J. E., Crain, D. F., Nor- terneuron circuitry after traumatic brain in-
rholm, S. D., Davis, M., Bradley, B., . . . & jury in mice. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(18),
Jovanovic, T. (2011). Tools for translational 6880–6890.
neuroscience: PTSD is associated with height- Jabbari, B., Mueller, H. G., & Howard, D. K.
ened fear responses using acoustic startle but (1987). Auditory brainstem response findings
not skin conductance measures. Depression and in the late phase of head injury. Seminars in
Anxiety, 28(12), 1058–1066. Hearing, 8(3), 261–265.
Godbolt, A. K., Cancelliere, C., Hincapié, C. A., Jantzen, K. J. (2010). Functional magnetic reso-
Marras, C., Boyle, E., Kristman, V. L., . . . & nance imaging of mild traumatic brain injury.
Cassidy, J. D. (2014). Systematic review of the Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 25(4),
risk of dementia and chronic cognitive im­ 256–266.
pairment after mild traumatic brain injury: Re­ Jin, X., Huguenard, J. R., & Prince, D. A. (2011).
sults of the International Collaboration on Mild Reorganization of inhibitory synaptic circuits
Traumatic Brain Injury Prognosis. Archives of in rodent chronically injured epileptogenic neo­
Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 95(3), cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 21(5), 1094–1104.
S245–S256. Johnson, B., Zhang, K., Gay, M., Horovitz, S.,
Gopal, K. V., Bishop, C. E., & Carney, L. (2004). Hallett, M., Sebastianelli, W., & Slobounov, S.
Auditory measures in clinically depressed in­ (2012). Alteration of brain default network in
dividuals. II. Auditory evoked potentials and be­ subacute phase of injury in concussed individ-
havioral speech tests. International Journal of Au­ uals: Resting-state fMRI study. Neuroimage,
diology, 43(9), 499–505. 59(1), 511–518.
Gouvier, W. D., Cubic, B., Jones, G., Brantley, P., Johnston, K. N., John, A. B., Kreisman, N. V.,
& Cutlip, Q. (1992). Postconcussion symp- Hall III, J. W., Crandell, C. C., Johnston,
toms and daily stress in normal and head- K. N., . . . & Crandell, C. C. (2009). Multiple
injured college populations. Archives of Clinical benefits of personal FM system use by children
Neuropsychology, 7, 193–211. with auditory processing disorder (APD). Inter­
Greer, J. E., Hånell, A., McGinn, M. J., & national Journal of Audiology, 48(6), 371–383.
Povlishock, J. T. (2013). Mild traumatic brain Jury, M. A., & Flynn, M. C. (2001). Auditory and
injury in the mouse induces axotomy primarily vestibular sequelae to traumatic brain injury:
within the axon initial segment. Acta Neuro­ A pilot study. New Zealand Medical Journal,
pathologica, 126(1), 59–74. 114(1134), 286–288.
Halbauer, J. D., Ashford, J. W., Zeitzer, J. M., Kaipio, M. L., Cheour, M., Öhman, J., Salonen,
et al. (2009). Neuropsychiatric diagnosis and O., & Näätänen, R. (2013). Mismatch nega-
management of chronic sequelae of war-related tivity abnormality in traumatic brain injury
mild to moderate traumatic brain injury. Jour­ without macroscopic lesions on conventional
nal of Rehabilitation Research and Development, MRI. Neuroreport, 24(8), 440–444.
46(6), 757–796. Kapoor, N., & Ciuffreda, K. J. (2002). Vision distur-
Hoge, C. W., McGurk, D., Thomas, J. L., et al. bances following traumatic brain injury. Current
(2008). Mild traumatic brain injury in U.S. Treatment Options in Neurology, 4(4), 271–280.
soldiers returning from Iraq. New England Jour­ Katz, J., & Smith, P. S. (1991). The staggered
nal of Medicine, 358(5), 453–463. spondaic word test. Annals of the New York
Hoover, E., Souza, P., & Gallun, F. (2015). Com- Academy of Sciences, 620(1), 233–251.
peting views on abnormal auditory results after Keith, R. W. (1995). Development and standard-
mild traumatic brain injury. SIG 6, Perspectives ization of SCAN-A: test of auditory processing
on Hearing and Hearing Disorders: Research and disorders in adolescents and adults. Journal of the
Diagnostics, 19(1), 12–21. American Academy of Audiology, 6, 286–286.
164 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

Killion, M. C., Niquette, P. A., Gudmundsen, G. I., Mueller, H. G. (1987). The staggered spondaic
Revit, L. J., & Banerjee, S. (2004). Development word test: Practical use. Seminars in Hearing,
of a quick speech-in-noise test for measuring 8(3), 267–277.
signal-to-noise ratio loss in normal-hearing Munjal, S. K., Panda, N. K., & Pathak, A. (2010).
and hearing-impaired listeners. The Journal of Relationship between severity of traumatic
the Acoustical Society of America, 116(4), 2395– brain injury (TBI) and extent of auditory dys-
2405. function. Brain Injury, 24(3), 525–532.
Koliatsos, V. E., Cernak, I., Xu, L. et al. (2011). Murphy, C. F. B., Fillippini, R., Palma, D., Zalc-
A mouse model of blast injury to brain: initial man, T. E., Lima, J. P., & Schochat, E. (2011).
pathological, neuropathological, and behav- Auditory training and cognitive functioning
ioral characterization. Journal of Neuropathol­ in adult with traumatic brain injury. Clinics,
ogy and Experimental Neurology, 70(5), 399– 66(4), 713–715.
416. Musiek, F. E. (1983). Assessment of central audi-
Larson, M. J., Kaufman, D. A., Schmalfuss, I. M., tory dysfunction: The dichotic digit test revis-
& Perlstein, W. M. (2007). Performance monitor­ ited. Ear and Hearing, 4(2), 79–83.
ing, error processing, and evaluative control fol­ Musiek, F. E., & Pinheiro, M. L. (1987). Fre-
lowing severe TBI. Journal of the International quency patterns in cochlear, brainstem, and ce­
Neuropsychological Society, 13(06), 961–971. rebral lesions. Audiology, 26(2), 79–88.
Lew, H. L., Lee, E. H., Pan, S. S., & Date, E. S. Musiek, F. E., Baran, J. A., & Pinheiro, M. L.
(2004). Electrophysiologic abnormalities of (1990). Duration pattern recognition in nor-
auditory and visual information processing in mal subjects and patients with cerebral and co-
patients with traumatic brain injury. American chlear lesions. Audiology, 29(6), 304–313.
Journal of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, Musiek, F. E. (1994). Frequency (pitch) and dura-
83(6), 428–433. tion pattern tests. Journal of the American Acad­
Lew, H. L., Jerger, J. F., Guillory, S. B., & Henry, emy of Audiology, 5, 265–265.
J. A. (2007) Auditory dysfunction in traumatic Musiek, F. E., Baran, J. A., & Shinn, J. (2004). As-
brain injury. Journal of Rehabilitation Research sessment and remediation of an auditory pro-
and Development, 44(7), 921–928. cessing disorder associated with head trauma.
Little, D. M., Kraus, M. F., Joseph, J. et al. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology,
(2010). Thalamic integrity underlies executive 15(2), 117–132.
dysfunction in traumatic brain injury. Neurol­ Musiek, F. E., Shinn, J. B., Jirsa, R., et al. (2005). GIN
ogy, 74(7), 558–564. (Gaps-In-Noise) test performance in subjects
McAllister, T. W., Saykin, A. J., Flashman, L. A. with confirmed central auditory nervous system
et al. (1999). Brain activation during working involvement. Ear and Hearing, 26(6), 608–618.
memory 1 month after mild traumatic brain in- Nampiaparampil, D. E. (2008). Prevalence of
jury: A functional MRI study. Neurology, 53(6), chronic pain after traumatic brain injury: A
1300–1300. systematic review. Journal of the American Med­
McAllister, T. W., and Arciniegas, D. (2002). Eval- ical Association, 300(6), 711–719.
uation and treatment of postconcussive symp- Niogi, S. N., & Mukherjee, P. (2010). Diffusion
toms. NeuroRehabilitation, 17(4), 265–283. tensor imaging of mild traumatic brain injury.
Mesulam, M. M. (2002). The human frontal Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 25(4),
lobes: Transcending the default mode through 241–255.
contingent encoding. Principles of Frontal Lobe Nölle, C., Todt, I., Seidl, R., & Ernst, A. (2004).
Function, 8–30. Pathophysiological changes of the central au-
Morgan, C. A., Grillon, C., Southwick, S. M., ditory pathway after blunt trauma of the head.
Davis, M., & Charney, D. S. (1996). Exagger- Journal of Neurotrauma, 21(3), 251–258.
ated acoustic startle reflex in Gulf War Veter­ Oleksiak, M., St Andre, J. R., Caughlan, C. M., &
ans with posttraumatic stress disorder. American Steiner, M. (2012). Audiological issues and hear-
Journal of Psychiatry, 153(1), 64–68. ing loss among Veterans with mild trau­matic
9. Traumatic Brain Injury and Auditory Processing 165

brain injury. Journal of Rehabilitation Research Saunders, G. H., Frederick, M. T., Chisolm, T. H.,
and Development, 49(7), 995–1004. Silverman, S., Arnold, M., & Myers, P. (2014).
Palacios, E. M., Sala-Llonch, R., Junque, C., et al. Use of a frequency-modulated system for Vet-
(2013). Long-term declarative memory deficits erans with blast exposure, perceived hearing
in diffuse TBI: correlations with cortical thick- problems, and normal hearing sensitivity. Sem­
ness, white matter integrity and hippocampal inars in Hearing, 35(03), 227–238.
volume. Cortex, 49(3), 646–657. Saunders, G. H., Frederick, M. T., Arnold, M.,
Papesh, M., & Gallun, F. (2015). Auditory im- et al. (2015). Auditory difficulties in blast-
pacts of blast exposure. Hearing Health & Tech­ exposed Veterans with clinically normal hearing.
nology Matters, Pathways. Journal of Rehabilitation Research and Develop­
Pavlov, I., Huusko, N., Drexel, M. et al. (2011). ment, 52(3), 343.
Progressive loss of phasic, but not tonic, GABA Schultz, B. A., Cifu, D. X., McNamee, S., Nich-
A receptor-mediated inhibition in dentate ols, M., & Carne, W. (2011). Assessment and
granule cells in a model of post-traumatic epi- treatment of common persistent sequelae fol-
lepsy in rats. Neuroscience, 194, 208–219. lowing blast induced mild traumatic brain in-
Petrie, E. C., Cross, D. J., Yarnykh, V. L. et al. jury. NeuroRehabilitation, 28(4), 309–320.
(2014). Neuroimaging, behavioral, and psycho- Sheldrake, J., Diehl, P. U., & Schaette, R. (2015).
logical sequelae of repetitive combined blast/ Audiometric characteristics of hyperacusis pa-
impact mild traumatic brain injury in Iraq and tients. Frontiers in Neurology, May 15, 6 105.
Afghanistan war Veterans. Journal of Neuro­ Shively, S. B., Horkayne-Szakaly, I., Jones, R. V.
trauma, 31(5), 425–436. et al. (2016). Characterisation of interface as-
Pichora-Fuller, M. K. (2003). Cognitive aging and troglial scarring in the human brain after blast
auditory information processing. International exposure: A post-mortem case series. The Lancet
Journal of Audiology, 42, 2S26–2S32. Neurology.
Potts, M. B., Adwanikar, H., & Noble-Haeusslein, Silver, J. M., McAllister, T. W., and Yudofsky,
L. J. (2009). Models of traumatic cerebellar in- S. C. (2011). Textbook of Traumatic Brain Injury
jury. The Cerebellum, 8(3), 211–221. (2nd ed.). Washington, D.C.: American Psy-
Punch, J., Joseph, A., & Rakerd, B. (2004). Most chiatric Publishing Inc.
comfortable and uncomfortable loudness lev- Silverberg, N. D., & Iverson, G. L. (2011). Eti-
els: six decades of research. American Journal of ology of the post-concussion syndrome: Phys-
Audiology, 13, 144–157. iogenesis and psychogenesis revisited. Neuro­
Rapp, P. E., Keyser, D. O., Albano, A., Hernan- Rehabilitation, 29(4), 317–329.
dez, R., Gibson, D. B., Zambon, R. A., . . . & Song, H., Cui, J., Simonyi, A. et al. (2016). Link-
Nichols, A. S. (2015). Traumatic brain injury ing blast physics to biological outcomes in
detection using electrophysiological methods. mild traumatic brain injury: narrative review
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 11. and preliminary report of an open-field blast
Reid, M. W., Miller, K. J., Lange, R. T. et al. model. Behavioural Brain Research.
(2014). A multisite study of the relationships Spanos, G. K., Wilde, E. A., Bigler, E. D., Cleavinger,
between blast exposures and symptom report- H. B., Fearing, M. A., Levin, H. S., . . . Hunter,
ing in a post-deployment active duty military J. V. (2007). Cerebellar atrophy after moderate-
population with mild traumatic brain injury. to-severe pediatric traumatic brain injury. Amer­
Journal of Neurotrauma, 31(23), 1899–1906. ican Journal of Neuroradiology, 28(3), 537–542.
Ross-Swain, D. (2007). The effects of auditory Stach, B. A., Loiselle, L. H., & Jerger, J. F. (1991).
stimulation on auditory processing disorder: A Special hearing aid considerations in elderly
summary of the findings. The International Jour­ patients with auditory processing disorders. Ear
nal of Listening, 21(2), 140–155. and Hearing, 12(6), 131S–138S.
Ryan, L. M., & Warden, D. L. (2003). Post con- Sun, H. Y., Li, Q., Chen, X. P., & Tao, L. Y.
cussion syndrome. International Review of Psy­ (2015). Mismatch negativity, social cogni-
chiatry, 15(4), 310–316. tion, and functional outcomes in patients after
166 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

traumatic brain injury. Neural Regeneration Re­ Verma, A., Anand, V., & Verma, N. P. (2007). Sleep
search, 10(4), 618. disorders in chronic traumatic brain injury. Jour­
Taber, K., Warden, D., & Hurley, R. (2006). nal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, 3(4), 357–362.
Blast-related traumatic brain injury: what is von Steinbüchel, N., Wittmann, M., Strasbur­
known? Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical ger, H., & Szelag, E. (1999). Auditory temporal-
Neurosciences, 18(2), 141–145. order judgement is impaired in patients with
Taber, K. H., Hurley, R. A., Haswell, C. C. et al. cortical lesions in posterior regions of the
(2015). White matter compromise in Veterans left hemisphere. Neuroscience Letters, 264(1),
exposed to primary blast forces. Journal of Head 168–171.
Trauma Rehabilitation, 30(1), E15. Wilson, R. H., & Weakley, D. G. (2005). The
Theodoroff, S. M., Lewis, M. S., Folmer, R. L., 500 Hz masking-level difference and word recog­
Henry, J. A., & Carlson, K. F. (2015). Hearing nition in multitalker babble for 40- to 89-year-
impairment and tinnitus: prevalence, risk fac- old listeners with symmetrical sensorineural
tors, and outcomes in U.S. Service Members and hearing loss. Journal of the American Academy of
Veterans deployed to the Iraq and Afghanistan Audiology, 16(6), 367–382.
wars. Epidemiologic Reviews, 37(1), 71–85. Wilson, R. H., Carnell, C. S., & Cleghorn, A. L.
Tompkins, P., Tesiram, Y., Lerner, M. et al. (2013). (2007). The Words-in-Noise (WIN) test with
Brain injury: Neuro-inflammation, cognitive def­ multitalker babble and speech-spectrum noise
icit, and magnetic resonance imaging in a model maskers. Journal of the American Academy of Au­
of blast induced traumatic brain injury. Jour­ diology, 18(6), 522–529.
nal of Neurotrauma, 30(22), 1888–1897. Wolf, J. A., & Koch, P. F. (2016). Disruption of
Turgeon, C., Champoux, F., Lepore, F., Leclerc, S., network synchrony and cognitive dysfunction
& Ellemberg, D. (2011). Auditory processing after traumatic brain injury. Frontiers in Systems
after sport-related concussions. Ear and Hearing, Neuroscience, 10.
32(5), 667–670. World Health Organisation. (1992). International
Tyler, R. S., Pienkowsi, M., Roncancio, E. R., Jun, Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related
H. J., Brozoski, T., Dauman, N., . . . Moore, Health Problems, 10th Revision (ICD-10). Ge-
B. C. J. (2014). A review of hyperacusis and neva: WHO.
future directions: Part I. Definitions and Man­ Wright, D. K., Trezise, J., Kamnaksh, A. et al.
ifestations. American Journal of Audiology, 23(4), (2016). Behavioral, blood, and magnetic reso-
402–419. nance imaging biomarkers of experimental mild
Van Toor, T., Neijenhuis, K., Snik, A., & Blok- traumatic brain injury. Scientific Reports, 6.
horst, M. (2006). Evaluation of auditory pro- Yi, J. H., & Hazell, A. S. (2006). Excitotoxic
cessing disorders after whiplash injury. Audio­ mechanisms and the role of astrocytic gluta-
logical Medicine, 4(4), 191–201. mate transporters in traumatic brain injury.
Vandoolaeghe, E., van Hunsel, F., Nuyten, D., & Neurochemistry International, 48(5), 394–403.
Maes, M. (1998). Auditory event related po- Zaugg, T. L., Thielman, E. J., Griest, S., & Henry,
tentials in major depression: Prolonged P300 J. A. (2016). Subjective reports of trouble tol-
latency and increased P200 amplitude. Journal erating sound in daily life versus loudness dis-
of Affective Disorders, 48(2), 105–113. comfort levels. American Journal of Audiology,
Verfaellie, M., Lafleche, G., Spiro, A., Tun, C., & 25(4), 359–363.
Bousquet, K. (2013). Chronic postconcussion Zhu, Y., Li, Z., Bai, L., Tao, Y., Sun, C., Li, M., . . .
symptoms and functional outcomes in OEF/ Zhang, M. (2014). Loss of microstructural in-
OIF Veterans with self-report of blast exposure. tegrity in the limbic-subcortical networks for
Journal of the International Neuropsychological acute symptomatic traumatic brain injury. Bio­
Society, 19(01), 1–10. Medical Research International.
10
Psychological Aspects and
Management of Hyperacusis
Gerhard Andersson

served among persons across all ranges of hear-


Background
ing sensitivity and not only in the group with
no marked hearing loss (a common definition
In spite of the prevalence of decreased sound in hyperacusis research). This chapter will focus
tolerance in its more severe form––called hy- on three aspects of decreased sound tolerance/
peracusis––most research on the psychological hyperacusis. First, we will examine some of the
mechanisms behind sound tolerance has been associated non-auditory problems that might
focused on noise sensitivity and the psycho- influence a patient’s experience with sound.
logical consequences of noise in the general Second, we will propose potential psycho­
population (Baguley & Andersson, 2007; logical mechanisms that might be involved in
Stansfeld, 1992). It is an established fact that disorders of sound tolerance, as would be con-
there is a substantial overlap between decreased sistent with a biopsychosocial understanding
sound tolerance and a range of medical and of the problem. Finally, a cognitive-behavioral
psychological conditions (Tyler et al., 2014), treatment (CBT) approach will be described
for example, migraine, post-traumatic stress and in addition the initial evidence in favor of
disorder, and tinnitus, just to mention a few exposure-based CBT for hyperacusis. The chap­
examples (Baguley & Andersson, 2007) (for ter will conclude with a case and a discussion
further information see Chapters 1, 7, and 11 about future developments.
in this book). While audiologists and associ-
ated professionals are well acquainted with the
phenomena of loudness recruitment and its Associated Problems
role when fitting hearing aids, there is by no
means a clear-cut difference between how pa-
tients with and without significant hearing loss There are a range of auditory conditions/disor-
experience sounds in terms of decreased sound ders associated with decreased sound tolerance
tolerance. Symptoms of hyperacusis can be ob­ (see Chapters 2, 3, 7, and 9 in this book). It is

167
168 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

important to note that patients may not easily were interviewed using the mini-international
distinguish between different categories of au- neuropsychiatric interview (Sheehan et al.,
ditory problems that for them are perceived as 1998), and the Swedish Universities Scales of
a whole. Tinnitus is an example. Patients with Personality (Gustavsson et al., 2000). Results
decreased sound tolerance and tinnitus can showed that as many as 56% of the patients
experience a strong link between sound expo- had at least one psychiatric disorder, and 47%
sure and reactive tinnitus; this sensitivity can had an anxiety disorder. In addition, person-
be linked to both annoyance and expectations ality traits related to neuroticism were over-
of tinnitus becoming more disturbing during represented relative to the general popula­
or following unwanted sounds. Additionally, tion. Using the same participants in another
hearing loss may cause a noisy social situation report, significant correlations between loud-
to be experienced as uncomfortable when it is ness discomfort levels (LDL) and symptoms of
hard to follow conversations or when loudness anxiety and depression were observed (   Jüris,
distresses the patient. In this example, there Ekselius, Andersson, & Larsen, 2013). In an­
may be at least three reasons for a patient to other study (Blaesing & Kroener-Herwig,
avoid a situation, with only one being directly 2012), 56 tinnitus patients with/without hyper­
related to noise sensitivity: first, the sounds in acusis and 30 healthy controls were compared.
the setting may be experienced as too loud, Results showed that participants with hyper-
second, exposure to those sounds may be as- acusis tolerated significantly shorter exposure
sociated with tinnitus exacerbation (at least to a pure tone than non-hyperacusic subjects.
temporarily), and third, the situation is diffi­ Self-reported avoidance behaviors were signif­
cult from a listening and communicative per- icantly correlated with distress attributed to
spective as the patient risks missing out on hyperacusis (r = 0.81), and with anxiety rat-
conversations, feeling embarrassed, or being ings. Other studies confirmed the role of per-
overwhelmed by the ambient noise level. The sonality in hyperacusis (Villaume & Has­son,
point made here is that different problems are 2017).
not easily distinguished, and patients can, for The biological underpinnings of the link
example, infer that “tinnitus is making it hard between hyperacusis and anxiety/depression
to hear” or that “sounds I cannot bear make it has been discussed for a long time (Marriage
hard for me to communicate.” & Barnes, 1995), and it is a common find-
The second category of associated prob- ing that persons with hyperacusis also suffer
lems is in the psychiatric domain. Again, this from self-reported psychiatric problems such
is mentioned elsewhere (Tyler et al., 2014) as post-traumatic stress disorder, chronic fa-
and in this book (Chapters 9, 13, and 15), tigue syndrome, generalized anxiety disorder,
but different categories of psychiatric prob- and depression (Paulin, Andersson, & Nordin,
lems have been linked with decreased sound 2016).
tolerance (e.g., mood and anxiety disorders). We alluded to the social aspects of
In two studies, Jüris and co-workers (2013 hyperacusis when referring to different reasons
a,b) investigated links between hyperacusis to avoid noisy situations. To the best of our
and psychiatric problems. Psychiatric morbid- knowledge, although many self-help networks
ity and personality traits were focused in one and resources are available, there is a lack of
study, along with different sociodemographic literature addressing the shared aspects of hy-
and clinical characteristics (   Jüris, Larsen, An­ peracusis, for example, occupationally, in ed-
ders­son, & Ekselius, 2013). The study included ucational settings, in family life, and not the
62 adult patients (mean age 40 years), who least what many people regard as a favorite ac-
10. Psychological Aspects and Management of Hyperacusis 169

tivity for relaxation––listening to music. Conse- reaction, for example, the sound of rain falling;
quences of disordered sound tolerance at work an individual may not give the sound much
are often reported by patients in the clinic and thought until they survive a hurricane. The
mentioned in the literature (Pienkowski et al., formerly neutral stimuli (rain falling) becomes
2014), although this topic requires greater at- paired with the reaction even in the absence of
tention as well. A valuable early attempt was an actual hurricane (Schwartz, Wasserman, &
a qualitative study (Trulsson, Johansson, Jans- Robbins, 2002). To be more specific, the per-
son, Wiberg, & Hallberg, 2003), in which son may start to fear and feel uncomfortable in
21 patients with persisting hypersensitivity to a situation in which he or she has experienced
sounds after an acute head trauma were inter- loud or threatening storm-related sounds even
viewed. The authors reported that the patients when the situation poses no danger. This no-
became psychosocially vulnerable and suffered tion assumes that not all sounds lead to pain-
from a variety of symptoms including hyper­ ful reactions and that in the life of a person
sensitivity to sounds, difficulties with concen- with hyperacusis there are some sounds that
trating and remembering, increased anxiety, and are tolerated well and others that are experi-
sensitivity to stress. enced as pain or perhaps as a unique sensa-
In sum, sound tolerance problems may tion of discomfort that, while not acute pain,
include several associated issues with the clini- would be extremely annoying and upsetting. A
cal observation that hyperacusis, when severe, related condition is misophonia in which the
rarely appears as a single symptom/problem, reaction is mainly that of annoyance (often
and that it can have severe consequences in when confronted with everyday sounds such as
daily life. other people eating) (   Jastreboff & Jastreboff,
2006; Taylor, 2017). Interestingly, from first
being observed and named in the field of au-
diology, misophonia is now being recognized
Psychological Mechanisms
in psychiatry (see Chapter 1). The different
emotions involved in hyperacusis are not well
In this section psychological mechanisms will investigated but in addition to irritation and
be proposed, bearing in mind that there are being uncomfortable there is pain and even
few studies on the psychology of hyperacusis. fear (Baguley & Andersson, 2007). The con-
Avoidance is an inherent feature of hyperacusis ditioned fear reaction can reach the level of a
and it is important to consider a patient’s avoid- panic attack with strong anxiety, shortness of
ance strategies to understand and manage their breath, sweating, and ultimately avoidance and
sound tolerance problem. Classical conditioning reduced quality of life.
is likely to occur as certain sounds and situa­ A second psychological perspective to
tions become associated with, or “conditioned” consider concerns cognitions, including beliefs
to, negative reactions (both bodily and cogni- and information processing regarding the po-
tively). Patients in clinic often report experiences tential danger of sounds. Fears associated with
of negative conditioning, for example, when anxiety and pain may involve catastrophic cog­
they feel compelled to leave a situation because of nitions. In the pain field the “fear-avoidance”
sounds and subsequently report that they avoid model has been very influential for the un-
that situation (for a similar discussion regarding derstanding and management of chronic pain
tinnitus, see Andersson, Hesser, & McKenna, (Vlaeyen & Linton, 2000). Briefly, the model
2016). Briefly, in classical conditioning a neu­­ proposes that individuals develop problems as
tral stimulus becomes paired with a negative a result of avoidant behavior based on fear.
170 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

In the case of hyperacusis, the avoidance of A third psychological perspective con-


everyday sounds (and perhaps more impor- cerns individual differences and the observa­
tantly certain situations) is assumed to be in- tion that some people report they are, and al­
fluenced by inappropriate catastrophic beliefs. ways have been, more sensitive to sounds than
In addition to fear and to the patient consid- their friends and family members. This is well
ering avoiding a situation, it is also important known in relation to neuropsychiatric condi-
to consider how actual exposure to sounds is tions such as autism spectrum disorders (Khalfa
experienced during the activity and reflected et al., 2004), but also in the general population
upon afterward. In my clinical work, hyper­ it is known that there are differences in sound
acusis patients report that they endure bother- sensitivity (Stansfeld, 1992). Thus, in clinic it
some sounds (e.g., at a dinner party) but then is fairly common that patients tell us that they
subsequently regret that decision and tend to always have been sensitive to sounds but that
focus on negative aspects of the experience it has worsened (sometimes in association with
rather than the fact that they managed and a noise trauma or negative life event). Stress
might even (at least partly) have enjoyed the vulnerability and stress reactions may also play
situation as it occurred. Selective attention to a role, with patients reporting that they can-
threat is common in anxiety disorders and the not handle stressful work conditions. Stress
tendency to interpret situations in a negative receives some attention in the clinical litera-
manner typifies persons with mood disorders. ture on hyperacusis and there is ongoing work
It is important to note that selective attention linking stress responses to hyperacusis (Chen
to threatening sounds and negative interpre- et al., 2017).
tations should not be viewed as necessarily In sum, a psychological understanding of
false (a common misunderstanding of cogni- hyperacusis needs to consider a biopsychoso-
tive therapy) and that the patient just needs cial approach stressing the importance of audi-
to be informed that the situation is not dan- tory dysfunction and its impact on social and
gerous and that it is better to think positively. psychological function. To date the psycholog-
Instead, the patient may benefit from learning ical understanding has concerned behavioral
more about the ways that thoughts can influ- mechanisms and the related role of cognitions.
ence behavior (the classical cognitive model) It can also be important to consider personal-
and that avoidance can be negative in the long ity, individual differences, and finally the role
run. Other reactions sometimes seen in clinic of stress when a patient is confronted by or
include grief and feelings of bereavement. For anticipating sounds that provoke pain or neg-
example, a musician who has spent a large ative emotions. The next section will outline
part of his life learning music and having it as how hyperacusis can be managed using tech-
a profession understandably suffers much loss niques derived from cognitive behavior ther-
and sadness with the prospect of not being able apy (CBT).
to enjoy music or continue in his profession.
The negative emotional responses also exert
an influence on quality of life as many social Treatment Based on Exposure
and occupational activities are loud and/or
and Cognitive Therapy
noisy. Patients sometimes report that hyper­
acusis has had a major impact on their social life
and that they feel very sad about the perceived There is no published manual on how to con-
need to alter activities that were once routine. duct CBT for hyperacusis but there are de-
10. Psychological Aspects and Management of Hyperacusis 171

scriptions in a book (Baguley & Andersson, goals are briefly discussed at the first meeting,
2007) and in a controlled trial (   Jüris, Anders- the more substantial work starts with the first
son, Larsen, & Ekselius, 2014), from which treatment session (usually one week later). In
the current description is derived. In addi- the first treatment session a CBT-model for
tion, I have worked with hyperacusis patients hyperacusis is outlined, informed by the initial
since the early 1990s and the current outline assessments and the daily diary. A CBT ses-
is informed by what I have learned from my sion always starts with agenda setting and the
patients. first point is to discuss the homework (in this
The first step in the CBT management case, diary keeping). When developing a CBT
of hyperacusis consists of a clinical interview/ model together with the patient, cognitions
case formulation and the use of screening and consequences associated with the patient’s
measures for hyperacusis and anxiety/depres- negative reporting and experiences are covered.
sion. The hyperacusis questionnaire (Khalfa More specific goals, which need to be realistic
et al., 2002) and the Hospital Anxiety and De- and not overly optimistic, are set (McKenna,
pression Scale – HADS (Zigmond & Snaith, 1987). We then provide a rationale and de-
1983) may be used in addition to a structured scriptions for applied relaxation strategies. The
interview. It is important to cover history, as- approach is divided into four steps (sessions)
sociated medical problems, avoidance, use of starting with tense and relax (that’s 1), relax
hearing protection, and the influences of the without tensing first (2), controlled breathing
condition on the patient’s quality of life. More- (3), and then using a cue-word for rapid re­
over, it is important to determine the ways in laxation (with or without disturbing sounds)
which the patient and his or her significant (4). Applied relaxation may support a variety
others view the problem. In association with of interventions but should not be used as a
formal assessments it is reasonable to adopt a “safety behavior” or as a way to avoid a feared
motivational interviewing stance from the start (imagined) catastrophe (McKenna, Handscomb,
(Miller & Rollnick, 2010). In practice, such an Hoare, & Hall, 2014). It can, however, be used
interviewing approach affirms that the patient as a coping technique that supports patients as
needs to make a decision to confront the prob- they confront feared situations. Applied relax-
lems and be willing to spend time with the ation is a standard component in the Swedish
ensuing exercises and interventions. A motiva- approach to CBT for tinnitus (Andersson &
tional stance can include discussion about the Kaldo, 2006) that is presented in treatment
pros and cons of avoidance and hearing pro- sessions and then further practiced in the form
tection. While important information should of homework.
be conveyed, such as the experimental find- Regardless of the patient’s use of applied
ings that protection can increase sensitivity relaxation, the main component of CBT for
in normal hearing individuals (Formby, Sher­ hyperacusis is exposure. The idea of working
lock, & Gold, 2003), the therapist should show with exposure is not new (Scott, 1993), and
empathy and understanding when prepar­ing indeed early work by Vernon (1987), tinnitus
the patient for treatment. I usually introduce retraining (   Jastreboff & Hazell, 2004) and
home registrations, or maintaining a diary, at Formby et al.’s (2003) investigation includes
the first meeting in case the patient decides to exposure to sounds. We present two ways to
continue clinic contact. Patients maintaining a work with exposure. First, the patient begins
diary aids the clinician’s understanding of how by not avoiding sound in general, which can
problems affect the patient’s daily life. While be described as “basic training.” This can be
172 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

accomplished with the help of soft music may hold negative beliefs even when exposure
that should not be viewed as unbearable in has worked. Therefore, it may not be wise to
any way. A metaphor of exercise in general promise that the sensitivity/annoyance will re-
can be useful in that listening practice may duce dramatically, but rather that the patient
be considered a form of everyday physical ex- will be able to manage situations that do not
ercise. The main challenge for the patient will pose a danger to hearing status. While the ex­­
remain exposure to feared or disliked sounds posures should not be to sounds that can dam-
(intense exposure). Such exposures should be age hearing, sometimes the patient is uncertain
gradual and the patient well prepared. It may regarding risks and may need assurance and
be that the patient cannot remain for long in guidance; a form of risk assessment. The coun-
the situation; we have found it can be more seling can be supplemented with text descrip-
effective for the patient to stay longer in a less tions/handouts for the patient to keep.
feared/annoying situation. I usually make two In terms of exposure and avoidance our
plans when giving exposure as homework. clinic focuses on behavioral activation, which
One task should be less difficult, provid- is an approach to the treatment of depression
ing the patient a chance to excel before moving (Martell, Dimidjian, & Herman-Dunn, 2010).
onto a second more difficult exposure. Some- If the patient avoids situations and has become
times patients come up with their own exer- passive, then a focus on activation in general
cises and surprise the clinician. While expo- (not only in terms of exposure to sounds) can
sure also can be completed in session, CBT be useful. Behavioral activation involves values,
requires that the patient practice in real life; or things that the patient believes are impor­
most of the expo­sure is done as homework. tant that, in the end, influence quality of life.
It is crucial to discuss the exposure outcomes This approach is about more than being active
in subsequent sessions. Here, methods from in general (Kanter, Busch, & Rusch, 2009).
cognitive therapy can be useful as the patient Social interactions are inherently involved in

Table 10–1. Overview of CBT for hyperacusis.

First assessment History, motivational interviewing, questionnaires


Session 1 CBT model for treatment, setting goals, applied relaxation 1,
homework
Session 2 CBT model for treatment, applied relaxation 2, exposure,
homework
Session 3 Risk assessment regarding sounds, applied relaxation 3,
exposure, homework
Session 4 Applied relaxation 4, exposure, homework
Session 5 Behavioral activation applied relaxation 4, exposure, homework
Session 6 Behavioral activation relapse prevention, summary, and
evaluation
Source: Based on Jüris et al. (2014).
10. Psychological Aspects and Management of Hyperacusis 173

the activation phase and in exposure; however, favor of the CBT group on all measures ex-
a challenge to the treatment may center on the cept for the HADS anxiety scale. Effects were
patient’s use of hearing protection in certain en- moderate to large (mean standardized differ-
vironments. The patient is encouraged to stop ences approximately 0.65) on the primary out­
protecting the ears from everyday sounds and comes and small to large on the other measures.
plan the exposure without the help of hearing Treatment results were largely maintained after
protection. However, in our experience, there 12 months. In comparison with tinnitus (Hes-
are situations in which use of hearing protec- ser, Weise, Zetterqvist Westin, & Andersson,
tion is the only way for the patient to be able 2011), there was far less evidence in favor of
to attend an event, such as a concert, hence it is CBT (and indeed other treatment approaches
also important to help the patient to be flexible as well), but the findings from this trial were
and not adhere in all situations to fixed rules. promising.
One example could be going to the cinema; Relatedly, a recent open trial on the use
the endeavor has value but may be daunting of CBT for misophonia was published (Schro-
for patients only recently introduced to the der, Vulink, van Loon, & Denys, 2017), with
objectives of desensitization. The last session a sample of 90 patients. Results showed that
ends with a summary and evaluation of goals. 48% of the patients demonstrated a signifi-
A follow-up session is scheduled approximately cant reduction of misophonia symptoms fol­
two months later. A potential CBT program lowing the CBT protocol. Another impor­
targeting hyperacusis appears in Table 10–1. tant outcome was that the authors found that
severity of misophonia and the presence of
disgust were positive predictors of treatment
Research on response.
In sum, there is a need for more research
Psychological Treatment
on hyperacusis and related conditions, but
if taken together with the fact that CBT is
There are to date very few studies on the treat- an evidence-based approach for anxiety and
ment of hyperacusis using CBT. One early mood disorders (Hofmann, Asnaani, Vonk,
case study showed promising outcomes (Scott, Sawyer, & Fang, 2012), referral to a quali-
1993), and in several texts on CBT for tinni- fied CBT therapist should be considered for
tus, hyperacusis treatment has been included patients with hyperacusis, in particular when
as part of a tailored approach to tinnitus man- avoidance and anxiety play a major role.
agement (Andersson, 2002). To my knowledge
there is only one controlled trial on CBT for
hyperacusis (Jüris et al., 2014). In that trial 60
Case Example
patients with hyperacusis were randomized to
CBT or to a waiting list control group. As out-
come measures we used the Loudness Discom- Ingrid worked as preschool teacher, but had
fort Level test, the Hyperacusis Questionnaire, a history of burnout syndrome (depression
the HADS, the Quality of Life Inventory mixed with strong anxiety) and had been on
(Frisch, Cornell, Villanueva, & Retzlaff, 1992), sick-leave for three months when returning
and the Tampa Scale of Kinesiophobia ( Miller, to work. Previously she had enjoyed her work
Kori, & Todd, 1991), adapted for hyperacusis. with small children but increasingly (she was
We found significant between-group effects in now 53 years old) experienced that she reacted
174 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

strongly to loud sounds and this was even work-related exposures. As she hoped to con-
worse now that she had returned to work. tinue her work a decision was made to only
She wore hearing protection on the bus to gradually remove hearing protection in that
work and also with the children. This was, setting. When planning for sound exposures
however, problematic as she needed to hear it became clear that she most of all wanted to
what the children said. She also experienced return to the choir again. She did so gradually
tinnitus but did not consider that as a major and brought the hearing protection with her.
problem. She was referred to a psychologist To her great surprise she managed to return to
via the ENT audiological physician for a first the choir and found that she benefitted from
assessment session. During the interview it the predictable nature of the sounds she expe-
became clear that she had experienced some rienced in that setting. During therapy there
sensitivity to sounds all her life, but also that was one setback when she had to leave a family
she had enjoyed singing in the church choir, gathering at which there was a band playing
which she unfortunately discontinued after her live music. When discussing this event, it be-
sick-leave period. The more severe symptoms came clear that she had not been alone doing
of hyperacusis began before the sick-leave and this and that other people as well had thought
she believed they could be related to a spe- it was too loud for them. When finishing the
cific event that included loud sounds at work therapy, she felt she had made great progress
when a child accidently turned on a radio with but was also aware that she needed more time
the volume at a high level. She was not fully and practice to maintain the achievement and
convinced that this episode caused the sound that some sensitivity would likely remain. At
tolerance problem, but she was now skeptical work she managed to get some alternative tasks
about her work and doubted if she would be and she also benefited from a change in policy
able to continue. At home all was very calm as when the preschool devoted work more with
she had a husband and two grown-up children outdoor activities (which for her was much eas­
who did not live at home. She usually rested in ier to handle).
silence after work and the weekends tended to
be very calm as well. In fact, she avoided noisy
situations including parties and cinemas, and
increasingly used hearing protection. The case
Discussion
formulation clearly showed that things were
going in the wrong direction; she understood
that the avoidance of sounds was not a reason- Hyperacusis can be a severely debilitating
able response, but she still doubted her ability condition to handle for patients and hard to
to handle her work. She and her therapist de- treat for clinicians. In addition, patients may
cided to try the CBT approach and the treat- differ in their response to sounds and envi-
ment started as described above. She responded ronments substantially. For example, treating
positively to the relaxation and had no imme- a child who refuses to go to school because of
diate problems introducing soft sounds in her noise sensitivity is very different from treating
life at home, as she liked music. The gradual a musician who wants to work but just cannot
exposure was more difficult at work as she continue. Despite differences in management,
could not easily control the kids at all times. In relaxation and gradual exposure are important
response, the therapist and she came up with to consider for most cases, and as with other
alternative solutions to gradually increase the medical problems a biopsychosocial under-
10. Psychological Aspects and Management of Hyperacusis 175

standing facilitates and should inform assess- Blaesing, L., & Kroener-Herwig, B. (2012).
ment, treatment, and research. Our experi- Self-reported and behavioral sound avoidance
ence affirms that hyperacusis patients should in tinnitus and hyperacusis subjects, and asso-
be seen in the audiology clinic and preferably ciation with anxiety ratings. International Jour­
within the setting of multidisciplinary teams. nal of Audiology, 51, 611–617. doi:10.3109/14
992027.2012.664290.
A common objection against CBT is that there
Chen, Y. C., Chen, G. D., Auerbach, B. D., Mano-
are few trained CBT clinicians with an inter- har, S., Radziwon, K., & Salvi, R. (2017).
est in audiology. One potential complement Tinnitus and hyperacusis: Contributions of para­
could be to develop Internet interventions for flocculus, reticular formation and stress. Hearing
hyperacusis, as was done for tinnitus and which Research, 349, 208–222. doi:10.1016/j.heares
can be supported by audiologists with basic .2017.03.005.
knowledge about CBT (Beukes, Baguley, Allen, Formby, C., Sherlock, L. P., & Gold, S. L. (2003).
Manchaiah, & Andersson, in press). On the Adaptive plasticity of loudness induced by
other hand, there will most likely always be pa- chronic attenuation and enhancement of the
tients in need of specialized face-to-face CBT acoustic background. Journal of the Acoustical So­
for their hyperacusis, and there remains a need ciety of America, 114, 55–58.
for more treatment research on this often ne- Frisch, M. B., Cornell, J., Villanueva, M., & Retz­
laff, P. J. (1992). Clinical validation of the
glected condition.
Quality Of Life Inventory: A measure of life
satisfaction for use in treatment planning and
outcome assessment. Psychological Assessment, 4,
92–101. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.4.1.92.
References Gustavsson, J. P., Bergman, H., Edman, G., Ekse-
lius, L., von Knorring, L., & Linder, J. (2000).
Swedish universities Scales of Personality (SSP):
Andersson, G. (2002). Psychological aspects of tin- Construction, internal consistency and norma-
nitus and the application of cognitive-behavioral tive data. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 102,
therapy. Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 977– 217–225.
990. doi:10.1016/S0272-7358(01)00124-6. Hesser, H., Weise, C., Zetterqvist Westin, V., &
Andersson, G., Hesser, H., & McKenna, L. (2016). Andersson, G. (2011). A systematic review and
Psychological mechanisms and tinnitus. In meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials
D. M. Baguley & M. Fagelson (Eds.), Tinnitus. of cognitive-behavioral therapy for tinnitus dis-
Clinical and research perspectives (pp. 63–73). tress. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 545–553.
San Diego, CA: Plural. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.12.006.
Andersson, G., & Kaldo, V. (2006). Cognitive- Hofmann, S. G., Asnaani, A., Vonk, I. J., Saw-
behavioral therapy with applied relaxation. In yer, A. T., & Fang, A. (2012). The efficacy of
R. S. Tyler (Ed.), Tinnitus treatment. Clinical cognitive behavioral therapy: A review of
protocols (pp. 96–115). New York, NY: Thieme. meta-analyses. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 36,
Baguley, D. M., & Andersson, G. (2007). Hyper­ 427–440. doi:10.1007/s10608-012-9476-1.
acusis: Mechanisms, diagnosis, and therapies. San Jastreboff, P. J., & Hazell, J. (2004). Tinnitus re­
Diego, CA: Plural. training therapy: Implementing the neurophysio­
Beukes, E. W., Baguley, D. M., Allen, P. M., Man- logical model. Cambridge: Cambridge Univer-
chaiah, V., & Andersson, G. (In press). Audiol- sity Press.
ogist guided Internet-based cognitive behaviour Jastreboff, P. J., & Jastreboff, M. M. (2006). Tin-
therapy for adults with tinnitus in the United nitus retraining therapy: A different view on
Kingdom: A randomised controlled trial. Ear tinnitus. ORL; Journal for Oto-Rhino-Laryngology
and Hearing. and Its Related Specialties, 68, 23–29.
176 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

Jüris, L., Andersson, G., Larsen, H.-C., & Ekse- population. Noise & Health, 18, 178–184. doi:10
lius, L. (2014). Cognitive behavioural therapy .4103/1463-1741.189244.
for hyperacusis: A randomized controlled trial. Pienkowski, M., Tyler, R. S., Roncancio, E. R.,
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 54, 30–37. doi: Jun, H. J., Brozoski, T., Dauman, N., . . .
10.1016/j.brat.2014.01.001. Moore, B. C. (2014). A review of hyper­
Jüris, L., Ekselius, L., Andersson, G., & Larsen, acusis and future directions: Part II. Measure-
H.-C. (2013). The hyperacusis questionnaire, ment, mechanisms, and treatment. Am J Au­
loudness discomfort levels, and the hospital anx- diol, 23, 420–436. doi:10.1044/2014_aja-13
iety and depression scale. A cross-sectional study. -0037.
Hearing, Balance and Communication, 11, 72–79. Schroder, A. E., Vulink, N. C., van Loon, A. J., &
Jüris, L., Larsen, H.-C., Andersson, G., & Ekse- Denys, D. A. (2017). Cognitive behavioral
lius, L. (2013). Psychiatric comorbidity and therapy is effective in misophonia: An open
personality factors in patients with hyperacu- trial. Journal of Affective Disorders, 217, 289–
sis. International Journal of Audiology, 52, 230– 294. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2017.04.017.
235. doi:10.3109/14992027.2012.743043. Schwartz, B., Wasserman, E. A., & Robbins, S. J.
Kanter, J. W., Busch, A. M., & Rusch, L. C. (2009). (2002). Psychology of learning and behavior
Behavioral activation. London, U.K.: Routledge. (5th ed.). New York, NY: Norton.
Khalfa, S., Bruneau, N., Roge, B., Georgieff, N., Scott, B. (1993). A graded exposure treatment for
Veuillet, E., Adrien, J. L., Barthelemy, C., & patients with hyperacusis, Paper presented at the
Collet, L. (2004). Increased perception of loud- 19th Annual Convention of the Association for
ness in autism. Hear. Res., 198, 87–92. Behavior Analysis, Chicago, May.
Khalfa, S., Dubal, S., Veuillet, E., Perez-Sdiaz, F., Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H.,
Jouvent, R., & Collet, L. (2002). Psychometric Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., . . . Dunbar,
normalisation of a Hyperacusis Questionnaire. G. C. (1998). The Mini-International Neuro-
ORL, 64, 436–442. psychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The develop-
Marriage, J., & Barnes, N. M. (1995). Is central ment and validation of a structured diagnostic
hyperacusis a symptom of 5-hydroxytryptamine psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-
(5-HT) dysfunction? The Journal of Laryngology 10. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59 Suppl
and Otology, 109, 915–921. 20, 22–33.
Martell, C. R., Dimidjian, S., & Herman-Dunn, R. Stansfeld, S. A. (1992). Noise, noise sensitivity
(2010). Behavioral activation for depression. A and psychiatric disorder: Epidemiological and
clinician’s guide. New York, NY: Guilford Press. psychophysiological studies. Psychological Med­
McKenna, L. (1987). Goal planning in audiolog- icine, Monograph Supplement 22, 1–44.
ical rehabilitation. British Journal of Audiology, Taylor, S. (2017). Misophonia: A new mental
21, 5–11. disorder? Medical Hypotheses, 103, 109–117.
McKenna, L., Handscomb, L., Hoare, D. J., & doi:10.1016/j.mehy.2017.05.003.
Hall, D. A. (2014). A scientific cognitive- Trulsson, U., Johansson, M., Jansson, G., Wiberg,
behavioral model of tinnitus: novel conceptu- A., & Hallberg, L. R. (2003). Struggling for a
alizations of tinnitus distress. Front. Neurol., 5, new self: In-depth interviews with 21 patients
196. doi:10.3389/fneur.2014.00196. with hyperacusis after an acute head trauma.
Miller, R. P., Kori, S. H., & Todd, D. D. (1991). J Health Psychol, 8(4), 403–412. doi:10.1177
The Tampa Scale: A measure of kinisophobia. /13591053030084001.
The Clinical Journal of Pain, 7, 51–52. Tyler, R. S., Pienkowski, M., Roncancio, E. R.,
Miller, W. R., & Rollnick, S. (2010). Motivational Jun, H. J., Brozoski, T., Dauman, N., . . .
interviewing (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Moore, B. C. (2014). A review of hyperacusis
Press. and future directions: Part I. Definitions and
Paulin, J., Andersson, L., & Nordin, S. (2016). manifestations. American Journal of Audiology,
Characteristics of hyperacusis in the general 23, 402–419. doi:10.1044/2014_aja-14-0010.
10. Psychological Aspects and Management of Hyperacusis 177

Vernon, J. A. (1987). Pathophysiology of tinnitus: A Vlaeyen, J. W., & Linton, S. J. (2000). Fear-
special case—Hyperacusis and a proposed treat- avoidance and its consequences in chronic mus-
ment. American Journal of Otology, 8, 201–202. culoskeletal pain: A state of the art. Pain, 85,
Villaume, K., & Hasson, D. (2017). Health-relevant 317–332.
personality is associated with sensitivity to sound Zigmond, A. S., & Snaith, R. P. (1983). The hos-
(hyperacusis). Scandinavian Journal of Psychol­ pital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychi­
ogy, 58, 158–169. doi:10.1111/sjop.12350. atrica Scandinavia, 67, 361–370.
SECTION III
Auditory Disorders:
Manifestations
11
Reflections on the
Association between
Hyperacusis and Tinnitus
David M. Baguley

Introduction Epidemiology

In the minds of many clinicians and research­ One aspect that hyperacusis and tinnitus
ers, hyperacusis and tinnitus are inextrica­ do share is that estimates of the prevalence
bly linked (Hazell & Sheldrake, 1992). As I and recognized severity of each symptom are
will discuss, patients with these symptoms of­ very strongly influenced by the question asked
ten follow similar referral pathways, and there (Nemholt-Rosing, 2016a). This adversely af­
is an undoubted and well-documented co- fects the interpretation of research findings
incidence of these experiences. This may not for each symptom alone, and when they are
indicate direct association, nor common mech­ considered together, the problem is com­
anisms, however, and it may be that the el­ pounded. However, there is a consensus that
ements of divergence between tinnitus and many patients presenting with a primary com­
hyperacusis are sufficient for them to be con­ plaint of tinnitus have experienced decreased
sidered as separate entities, although with some sound tolerance (DST), and vice versa. What
neurobiological overlap. These and other is­ is largely missing from the literature, however,
sues will be considered below; the present state are data indicating how many patients with a
of knowledge likely ensures that there are at primary complaint of one of these symptoms
least as many questions as answers. have bothersome experiences of the other. Also

181
182 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

lacking are indications that these symptoms to what extent this was due to time, treatment
are somehow coupled in time of onset, or effects, or the way in which hyperacusis was
whether and to what degree they fluctuate defined to participants.
over time (either in synchrony or recipro­ In a substantial series of 4993 adult pa­
cally). These unanswered questions present a tients with tinnitus, Hiller and Goebel (2005)
problem when it comes to determination of reported that hyperacusis (definition not given)
the association or mutually reinforcing ele­ was present in 7.3%, a significant propor­
ments between the symptoms. tion of whom reported high levels of tinni­
As the literature on tinnitus is more ex­ tus loudness and annoyance. This finding was
tensive than that on hyperacusis, an initial re­ echoed by Fagelson (2007), who reported
view could consider the experiences of hyper­ that patients with trauma histories who rated
acusis in patients with a primary complaint of their tinnitus as severe were more likely than
tinnitus. Jack Vernon (1987) reported on a large patients who did not suffer trauma to report
series of patients undergoing clinical care for severe co-occurring sound tolerance issues.
troublesome tinnitus, and while his study was More recent research has provided data
ground-breaking in that it considered decreased that is more nuanced. Scheckelman et al.
sound tolerance at all, he noted: (2014) reported a series of 2333 adult patients,
1713 of whom presented with a primary com­
In our Tinnitus Clinic, where more than plaint of tinnitus. Asking the question, “Do
4,000 patients have been seen, hyperacusis sounds cause you pain or physical discomfort?”
has been seen only four times. produced positive answers in 935 patients
(55%), although 620 patients either did not
Vernon defined hyperacusis as “unusual toler­ answer the question or replied, “I don’t know,”
ance to ordinary environmental sounds,” and and therefore the rate could conceivably be
one reflection might be that incorporation of higher. Those patients deemed to have hyper­
the word unusual into a definition was an in­ acusis had better hearing thresholds than those
dication of an assumption that the symptom, without, and were younger. The two groups
or its trigger, was rare. As more attention was did not differ in gender nor in any of the tinni­
given to symptoms of reduced sound toler­ tus characteristics analyzed. Patients reporting
ance, it became clear that such symptoms were both tinnitus and sound induced discomfort/
more common in people with tinnitus than pain were more likely to be undergoing psy­
Vernon had indicated. Initial reports of the chiatric treatment, and had higher scores on
prevalence of hyperacusis in patients present­ the Tinnitus Handicap Inventory, the Beck
ing with troublesome tinnitus included figures Depression Inventory, and lower self-reported
of 40% (   Jastreboff, Gray & Gold, 1996; Bart­ quality of life. The authors reflected that pa­
nik, Fabijanska, & Rogowski, 1999) and 60% tients with both tinnitus and sound induced
(Andersson et al., 2001). In a study aiming to discomfort/pain were experiencing a form of
validate the Multiple-activity Scale for Hy­ hypervigilance to their symptoms, and that
peracusis, Dauman and Boscau-Faure (2005) these co-incident auditory symptoms repre­
recruited a series of 249 adult patients with sented a specific subtype of tinnitus.
tinnitus, reporting that 79% also had hy­ Yang et al. (2015) reported a consecu­
peracusis. It was noted that with treatment the tive series of 207 adult patients seen for trou­
hyperacusis impact decreased more frequently blesome tinnitus in whom the tinnitus was
than that of tinnitus, although it was unclear unilateral in 105 (50.7%) and bilateral in
11. Reflections on the Association between Hyperacusis and Tinnitus 183

102 (49.3%). The authors did not report the tinnitus and hyperacusis were more likely to
question used to differentiate the groups, nor indicate that the sound tolerance issues out­
did they discuss the experiences of patients in weighed the problems associated with tinnitus.
whom the experience of tinnitus was either In that chart review, 62% of the patients with
central (filling the head) or the source ap­ both tinnitus and hyperacusis reported the
peared to be located externally. It was reported sound tolerance as a more severe impairment.
that of the patients with unilateral tinnitus, Some other data is available regarding
5 (4.8%) experienced hyperacusis, as did 13 experiences of tinnitus and hyperacusis in the
of those with bilateral tinnitus (12.7%): this general population. In a conference proceed­
difference was not statistically significant. The ings, Fabijanska et al. (1999) reported findings
question used to identify hyperacusis was not from a postal questionnaire study in Poland
given. While this work raised an interesting with 10,349 respondents, of whom 20.1% re­
question regarding the perceived tinnitus “lo­ ported tinnitus, and 15.2% hyperacusis. The
cation” and DST, limited information could questions used were not reported. Of those
be gleaned about the physiological association with tinnitus, the proportion also reporting hy­
between hyperacusis and tinnitus. peracusis approximated 40%. The proportion
A further series of 81 adult tinnitus pa­ with hyperacusis indicating that they had tinni­
tients was described by Degeest et al. (2016). tus was not reported. In a more comprehensive
The data collected included Loudness Dis­ study, originating from Sweden, Paulin et al.
comfort Levels (LDLs), and validated Dutch (2016) contacted 8520 adult individuals, of
versions of the Tinnitus Handicap Inventory whom 3406 (40.0%) agreed to participate. Of
(THI) and the Hyperacusis Questionnaire those, 66 answered affirmatively when asked,
(HQ). Statistically significant correlations were “Have you been diagnosed with sound intoler­
found between THI scores and LDL mea­ ance by a physician?”
sures (albeit weak, r = 0.265), and HQ scores A further 313 (9.2%) answered affirma­
(r = 0.729). The mean HQ score in the group tively to the question, “Do you have a hard time
was 18.5 (s.d. 10.1) but the authors did not tolerating everyday sounds that you believe
report how many patients met the HQ cri­ most other people can tolerate?” and were re­
teria for clinical significance. The association garded as self-diagnosing with hyperacusis. Of
between the HQ and THI scores is of interest; those with a physician diagnosis, 29 (43.9%)
causality aside, it may be a result of general reported tinnitus (tinnitus question not given),
distress driving both tinnitus and hyperacusis while of the self-diagnosed, 75 (24.0%) reported
measures rather than a specific physiological tinnitus. If it were the case that a person seek­
association between the symptoms. ing advice from a physician about DST had
Less attention has been paid to patients more severe problems than a person who self-
with a primary complaint of hyperacusis, and diagnosed, then this could be an indication that
whether they experience tinnitus. Anari et al. tinnitus is more prevalent when hyperacusis is
(1999) reported data from a series of 100 adult more severe, but other factors could well be at
patients with a primary compliant of “hyper­ work here, such as listening demands, and his­
sensitivity to sounds.” noting that 86% of tory of noise exposure.
them “suffer from tinnitus,” although formal Thus, there are fundamental problems
evaluation of severity or impact was not re­ in the interpretation of the published data re­
ported. Fagelson (2007) reported that patients garding the co-incidence of hyperacusis and
with trauma histories who experienced both tinnitus in adults. These challenges are even
184 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

more evident when one considers children and and being described as bothersome. Of these,
adolescents. A systematic review (Nemholt- 21/218 (9.6%) reported hyperacusis. As with
Rosing et al., 2016a) of tinnitus and hyper­ adults, the experience of tinnitus in persons
acusis in these populations identified 25 pa­ with hyperacusis is more common than that
pers examining tinnitus and/or hyperacusis of hyperacusis in persons with tinnitus.
in childhood (see also Chapter 13), but con­ In summary, the literature concerning
cluded that the variability in definitions and the co-incidence of hyperacusis and tinnitus
criteria for both hyperacusis and tinnitus were is complex and problematic. Variability in def­
so great that meaningful meta-analysis was initions and criteria is marked, and little at­
not possible. Nevertheless, the co-incidence of tention has been paid to severity or impact of
hyperacusis and tinnitus in childhood was ad­ either tinnitus or hyperacusis as a secondary
dressed by Baguley et al. (2012) who considered condition to the other. While it is possible to
the pediatric referrals of 4 well-established tin­ note that these symptoms commonly occur
nitus clinics in Europe in the year 2009. Dur­ together, it is not possible to make other in­
ing that time 88 children and adolescents were ferences about the possible causality or associa­
treated for a primary complaint of tinnitus, tions that exist between the two symptoms, or
and in 34 (38%) hyperacusis (not defined) their relation over time. It can be noted, how­
was also reported. ever, that it is entirely possible to have hyper­
A series (Nemholt-Rosing, 2016b) of 81 acusis or tinnitus without the other symptom,
young children and adolescents was referred and by no means is it an obligatory experience
to secondary care for tinnitus and/or hyper­ to have both. Having reflected on the epide­
acusis in Southern Denmark in the period miology of hyperacusis and tinnitus, atten­
June 2014–February 2015. Of these, 9 cases tion can now be directed toward when and
(12.8%) had a primary complaint of hyper­ how these symptoms converge, and then how
acusis, and both tinnitus and hyperacusis were they diverge.
the cause of complaint in 11 cases (15.7%).
An epidemiological study (Hall et al.,
2016) considered the prevalence of hyperacusis
Convergence
and its relationship with tinnitus in a cohort of
11-year-old children in the United Kingdom
who had enrolled in a longitudinal study en­ It has been mentioned above that even when
titled Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and the symptoms do not co-occur, in most health
Children (ALSPAC). Of the participating chil­ systems patients will be seen in the same clin­
dren, 261/7092 (3.7%) reported hyperacusis, de­ ics, and by the same professional groups, al­
fined by the question, “Do you ever experience though this will vary country by country. An­
over-sensitivity or distress to particular sounds?” other area of convergence is the relation of
Tinnitus of any severity or type was hyperacusis, and of tinnitus, to psychological
reported by 109 (41.7%) of these children. issues such as anxiety, fear, distress, and de­
As discussed in Chapter 13, risk factors for pression. Chapter 10 contains some discus­
hyperacusis included higher maternal educa­ sion of the mutual reinforcement between
tion, male gender, and a readmission to the hyperacusis and anxiety, as well as tinnitus ex­
hospital in the first 4 weeks of life. Regarding periences that can also be strongly influenced
tinnitus experiences (Humphriss et al., 2016), by psychological state. It would make sense,
218/7092 (3.1%) children reported clinically- therefore, that when hyperacusis and tinnitus
significant tinnitus, defined by persistence occur together, they too would be influenced
11. Reflections on the Association between Hyperacusis and Tinnitus 185

in some form of coupling with a psychological the modulation of tinnitus. Further work is
state, although empirical evidence of this is needed in this area.
sparse to date. The clinical implication is that Tinnitus can be an intermittent or tran­
just as tinnitus therapy consisting of audiology- sient experience, although it can also be unre­
based counseling and sound therapy can have mittingly consistent, and this can vary both
increased efficacy if augmented with elements between individuals, and in a single individ­
of psychological therapy (Cima et al., 2012), ual’s day to day experience. The severity of
such combined interventions are likely to be tinnitus can also vary, and emotional/psycho­
of great value in cases of co-occurring hyper­ logical state is one factor that influences this.
acusis and tinnitus. Hyperacusis, however, appears to be a more
consistent phenomenon, in that once it begins
it is persistent and consistent, and in general,
while it may influence emotions, it does not
vary with emotional state. One reported excep­
Divergence
tion to this is the psychological stress associated
with the anticipation of pain or distress associ­
There are several ways in which the experience ated with an impending sound event (Lupien
of hyperacusis, and of tinnitus, diverge from et al., 2009). Imagine an adult waiting for a
each other. The first is that while unilateral or telephone call, sitting with their cell phone on
strongly lateralized tinnitus is common, com­ the table in front of them: the call to come will
prising 47% of the patient population, lateral­ bring some important news, perhaps of exam­
ized or unilateral hyperacusis is not common ination results, or the findings of an impor­
(Axelsson, 1995), although robust empirical tant medical investigation. As the person waits,
data in that regard is not yet available. When their anticipation increases their auditory gain,
DST is unilateral, the finding is most usually so that when the phone rings, they startle, even
associated with a unilateral injury, such as an though that was the very sound that they were
acoustic shock (McFerran & Baguley, 2007) or expecting. Other examples include an anxious
a specific lesion. Boucher and colleagues (2013) mother listening for her sick child to cough
described a three-patient series each with a le­ during the night, or a householder waiting for
sion of the insular cortex in whom a presenting their noisy neighbor to return home and begin
symptom was unilateral hyperacusis. playing their intense and intrusive music.
Second, while the somatic modulation Finally, hyperacusis seems to be related
of tinnitus is common, this is not the case for to some conditions that have little or nothing
hyperacusis. In the author’s clinical experience to do with tinnitus, such as misophonia, Wil­
this is very rarely volunteered by patients, and liams syndrome, and autistic spectrum disor­
on questioning, is not commonly reported ei­ ders. Misophonia, which may correspond to
ther. It has been proposed (Sanchez, 2016) annoyance hyperacusis in Tyler et al.’s (2014)
that the somatic modulation of tinnitus in­ categorization scheme, is a condition where
volves interaction between the somatosensory specific sounds evoke disgust or rage (Mc­
systems and the auditory system within the Ferran, 2016). Common triggers include eating
dorsal cochlear nucleus, and if further inves­ sounds, throat clearing, or audible breathing.
tigation of the somatic modulation of hyper­ The behavioral consequence is avoidance, and
acusis confirms its low prevalence rate, then as the most common age of onset coincides with
the implication might be that hyperacusis in­ adolescence, the family/social consequences can
volves pathways extrinsic to those involved in be catastrophic. Functional imaging studies of
186 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

misophonia (Kumar et al., 2017) have indi­ Abnormal tolerance of sound is a no­
cated abnormal functional connectivity be­ table feature of autistic spectrum disorders
tween the anterior insular cortex and emotion (ASD; Rosenhall et al., 1999). Professor Tem­
processing centers such as the amygdala, and ple Grandin has written about her auditory
that this may be a disorder in the patient’s eval­ experiences in the context of her ASD:
uation of the salience or meaning of the trig­
ger sounds. As such, misophonia differs sub­ My hearing is like having a hearing aid with
stantially from hyperacusis, which is a general the volume control stuck on “super loud.” It
decreased tolerance to sound likely related to is like an open microphone that picks up ev­
a disorder of auditory gain; however, the emo­ erything. I have two choices: turn the mike
tional aspects of hyperacusis would indicate on and get deluged with sound, or shut it
that abnormal functional connectivity between off . . . . . . . I can’t modulate incoming audi­
sound perception and emotion may be an el­ tory stimulation. (Grandin, 1992)
ement of its neurobiological substrate. While
this could also be proposed as being the case in While it is widely known that people with
some cases of tinnitus, hyperacusis and miso­ ASD have unusual auditory experiences, sur­
phonia appear related in some ways that do not prisingly little data regarding severity or patient
map onto tinnitus. profiles is available. Potentially this could be as­
Williams syndrome (WS) is a neurode­ cribed to the challenges of researching purely
velopmental disorder, associated with abnor­ subjective phenomena in people with reduced
malities on chromosome 7 (see Chapter 7). abilities to communicate emotional or qualita­
People with WS have distinctive elfin-like fa­cial tive experiences. In a study investigating tin­
features, developmental delay, cardiac prob­lems, nitus and hyperacusis in persons with ASD,
with a behavioral phenotype of and inappro­ Danesh et al. (2015) reported data from 55
priate, or readily-displayed trust of strang­ individuals with a diagnosis of Asperger’s syn­
ers, perhaps viewed as excessive friendliness. drome. Hyperacusis was reported by 69%
Sound tolerance issues can be the first feature of the series, although the mean score of the
of WS noticed by the person’s family, and Hyperacusis Questionnaire (HQ) was 21 (of
have been said to involve over 90% of people a potential maximum of 42), which did not
with a diagnosis of WS (Klein et al., 1990; Van indicate a severe problem; perhaps the result
Borsel et al., 1997; Levitin et al., 2005). In­ was indicative of a mismatch between the
vestigation of such patients using the HQ did subjective experiences and those aspects of
not indicate a severe hyperacusis (Blomberg hyperacusis that the HQ aimed to measure
et al., 2006), however, the extent to which the (see Chapter 2). The prevalence of tinnitus
HQ derives meaningful data in the WS pop­ was 35%, with a mean THI score of 23 (of a
ulation is unknown. A model of hyperactivity potential maximum of 100); while this score
in the auditory system of persons with WS would exceed that of the general population,
was proposed (Zarchi et al., 2010) and some it was not indicative of severe tinnitus. The
auditory evoked potential data supported this prevalence of both tinnitus and hyper­
model (Zarchi et al., 2015). To the author’s acusis was 31%. Some caution must be advised
knowledge, there is no published data regard­ when interpreting these results: the age range
ing tinnitus in persons with WS, and it may of the participants was 4 to 42 years (i.e., per­
well be that WS is a condition characterized haps too wide to be meaningful), the response
by reduced sound tolerance but without any rate was only 16%, and the extent to which
associated tinnitus. the THI and HQ provided meaningful data
11. Reflections on the Association between Hyperacusis and Tinnitus 187

in this population, given both the diagnosis • When hyperacusis and tinnitus are co-
and the young age of some participants, was incident, which responds more quickly
uncertain. and effectively to treatment?
These reflections on areas of divergence
between hyperacusis and tinnitus may have It is our hope that colleagues in the auditory
some implications. There are some conditions community, researchers, clinicians, and pa­
in which sound tolerance issues are pathogno­ tients, might identify areas of interest, and un­
monic, but that do not seem to involve tin­ dertake controlled trials to further our knowl­
nitus. There are some aspects of hyperacusis edge. There is much to do in that regard!
that have markedly different characteristics to
tinnitus, including the characteristic lack of
lateralization and of somatic modulation, and
the relentless consistency of hyperacusis rather References
than the labile nature often ascribed to tinni­
tus. Does this mean that hyperacusis and tinni­
Anari, M., Axelsson, A., Eliasson, A., & Mag­
tus are essentially separate phenomena? Given nusson, L. (1999). Hypersensitivity to sound:
the present sparse data we have, a definitive an­ Questionnaire data, audiometry, and classifi­
swer is not yet possible. However, while there cation. Scandinavian Audiology, 28, 219–230.
is some neurobiological overlap, it does seem Baguley, D. M., Bartnik, G., Kleinjung, T., Savas­
that there is sufficient divergence that the tra­ tano, M., & Hough, E. A. (2013). Trouble­
ditional grouping of hyperacusis with tinnitus some tinnitus in childhood and adolescence:
can be called into question. An analogy to use Data from expert centres. Int. J. Ped. Otorhino-
with patients might be to describe hyper­ laryngol., 77(2), 246–251.
acusis and tinnitus as cousins rather than sib­ Blomberg, S., Rosander, M., and Andersson, G.
lings: related, yet with substantial, distinctive, (2006). Fears, hyperacusis and musicality in
Williams syndrome. Res. Dev. Disabil., Nov-
and im­portant differences.
Dec, 27(6), 668–680.
Boucher, O., Turgeon, C., Champoux, S., Ménard,
L., Rouleau, I., Lassonde, M., . . . Nguyen,
Unanswered Questions D. K. (2015). Hyperacusis following unilateral
damage to the insular cortex: A three-case re­
port. Brain Res. 5, 1606, 102–12
As with several other chapters in this book, Cima, R. F., Maes, I. H., Joore, M. A., Scheyen, D. J.,
the number of unanswered questions identi­ El Refaie, A., Baguley, D. M., . . . Vlaeyen, J. W.
fied is considerable. Some of these are offered (2012). Specialised treatment based on cog­
below: nitive behaviour therapy versus usual care for
tinnitus: A randomised controlled trial. Lancet,
• What is the prevalence of hyperacusis 379(9830), 1951–1959.
Danesh, A. A., Lang, D., Kaf, W., Andreassen, W. D.,
and tinnitus as a secondary symptom to
Scott, J., & Eshraghi, A. A. (2015). Tinnitus
the other in adults and children?
and hyperacusis in autism spectrum disorders
• What is the severity of hyperacusis and with emphasis on high functioning individu­
tinnitus as a secondary symptom to the als diagnosed with Asperger’s Syndrome. Int. J.
other in adults and children? Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol., Oct, 79(10), 1683–
• When hyperacusis and tinnitus are 1688.
co-incident, which is precursive of the Dauman, R., & Bouscau-Faure, F. (2005). Assess­
other, and what is the natural history? ment and amelioration of hyperacusis in tinnitus
188 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

patients. Acta Otolaryngologica, 125(5), 503– Levitin, D. J., Cole, K., Lincoln, A., & Bellugi, U.
509. (2005). Aversion, awareness, and attraction: In­
Degeest, S., Corthals, P., Dhooge, I., & Keppler, H. vestigating claims of hyperacusis in the Wil­
(2016). The impact of tinnitus characteristics liams syndrome phenotype. J. Child Psychol.
and associated variables on tinnitus-related Psychiatry, May, 46(5), 514–523.
handicap. J. Laryngol. Otol., 130(1), 25–31. Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R., &
Fabijanska, A., Rogowski, M., Bartnik, G., & Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout
Skarzynski, H. (1999). Epidemiology of tinni­ the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cogni­
tus and hyperacusis in Oland. In Hazell, J. W. P. tion. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), 434–
(Ed.). Proceedings of Sixth International Tinnitus 445.
Seminar (pp. 569–571). London: The Tinnitus McFerran, D. J. (2016). Misophonia and pho­
and Hyperacusis Centre. nophobia. In Baguley, D. M., & Fagelson, M.
Fagelson, M. A. (2007). The association between (Eds.). Tinnitus: Clinical and research perspec-
tinnitus and posttraumatic stress disorder. Am. tives (pp. 254–260). San Diego, CA: Plural.
J. Aud., (16), 107–117. McFerran, D. J., & Baguley, D. M. (2007). Acous­
Grandin, T. (1992). An inside view of autism. In tic shock. J. Laryngol. Otol., 121(4), 301–305.
Scopler, E. & Meribov, G. B. (Eds.). High func- Nemholt-Rosing, S., Hvass Schmidt, J., Wedder­
tioning individuals with autism (pp. 105–126). kopp, N., & Baguley, D. M. (2016a). Prevalence
New York, NY: Springer. of tinnitus and hyperacusis in children and
Hall, A. J., Humphriss, R., Baguley, D. M., Parker, M., adolescents: A systematic review. BMJ Open 6,
& Steer, C. D. (2016). Prevalence and risk fac­ 1–20.
tors for reduced sound tolerance (hyperacusis) Nemholt-Rosing, S., Kapandais, A., Schmidt, J. H.,
in children. International Journal of Audiology, & Baguley, D. M. (2016b). Demographic data,
55(3), 135–141. referral patterns and interventions used for chil­
Hiller, W., & Goebel, G. (2006). Factors influ­ dren and adolescents with tinnitus and hyper­
encing tinnitus loudness and annoyance. Arch acusis in Southern Denmark. Int. J. Ped. Otorhi-
Otolaryngol. Haed Neck Surg., 132(12), 1323– nolaryngol., 89, 112–120.
1330. Paulin, J., Andersson, L., & Nordin, S. (2016).
Humphriss, R., Hall, A. J., & Baguley, D. M. (2016). Characteristics of hyperacusis in the general
Prevalence and characteristics of tinnitus in population. Noise & Health, 18(3), 178–184.
11-year-old children. Int. J. Audiol., 55(3), 142– Rosenhall, U., Nordin, V., Sandstrom, M., Ahlsen,
148. G., & Gillberg, C. (1999). Autism and hearing
Jang, C., Jung, J., Kim, S. H., Byun, J. Y., Park, loss. J. Autism Devel. Dis., 29(5), 349–357.
M. S., & Yeo, S. G. (2015). Comparison of Sanchez, T. G. (2016). Clinical aspects of somatic
clinical characteristics in patients with bilateral modulation of tinnitus. In Baguley, D. M.,
and unilateral tinnitus. Acta Oto-Laryngologica Fagelson, M. (Eds.). Tinnitus: Clinical and re-
135(11), 1128–1131. search perspectives (pp. 111–127). San Diego,
Jastreboff, P. J., Gray, W. C., & Gold, S. L. (1996) CA: Plural.
Neurophysiological approach to tinnitus pa­ Scheckleman, M., Landgrebe, M., Langguth, B., &
tients. Am. J. Otol., 17(2), 236–240. TRI Database Study Group. (2015). Phenotypic
Klein, A. J., Armstrong, B. L., Greer, M. K., & characteristics of hyperacusis in tinnitus. PLOS
Brown, F. R. 3rd. (1990). Hyperacusis and otitis One, 9(1): e86944.
media in individuals with Williams syndrome. Tyler, R. S., Pienkowski, M., Roncancio, E. R., Jun,
J. Speech Hear. Disord., May, 55(2), 339–344. H. J., Brozoski, T., Dauman, N., . . . Moore,
Kumar, S., Tansley-Hancock, O., Sedley, W., B. C. (2014). A review of hyperacusis and fu­
Winston, J. S., Callaghan, M. F., Allen, M., . . . ture directions: Part I. Definitions and manifes­
Griffiths, T. D. (2017). The brain basis for tations. American Journal of Audiology, 23(4),
misophonia. Curr. Biol., 27(4), 527–533. 402–419.
11. Reflections on the Association between Hyperacusis and Tinnitus 189

Van Borsel, J., Curfs, L. M., Fryns, J. P. (1997). Zarchi, O., Avni, C., Attias, J., Frisch, A., Car­mel,
Hyperacusis in Williams syndrome: A sample M., Michaelovsky, E., . . . Gothelf, D. (2015).
survey study. Genet. Couns., 8(2), 121–126. Hyperactive auditory processing in Williams
Vernon, J. A. (1987). Pathophysiology of tinnitus: syndrome: Evidence from auditory evoked po­
A special case––hyperacusis and a proposed tentials. Psychophysiology, 52(6), 782–789.
treatment. Am. J. Otol., 8(3), 201–202.
Zarchi, O., Attias, J., & Gothelf, D. (2010). Audi­
tory and visual processing in Williams syndrome.
Isr. J. Psychiatry Relat. Sci., 47(2), 125–131.
12
Diplacusis
Marc Fagelson

auralis refers to the observation that a single


Introduction
tone may be perceived as multiple tones or
as a noise, in the stimulated ear. Such obser-
Diplacusis occurs when a listener perceives at vations may be made by patients with pro-
least two distinctly different pitches as a pure found unilateral hearing loss; the “better” ear
tone signal or musical note or chord is pre- produces the aberrant perception without con-
sented simultaneously to both ears. Musicians tribution from the “poorer” ear. Diplacusis bin-
appear more sensitive to the effects of dipla- auralis is the most common form of the disor-
cusis, likely because they can recognize when der in which a single tone audible to both ears
notes in a song are not in tune (i.e., dissonance is perceived as two distinct tones or as a noisy
or roughness is present when it should not be) signal. The two ears may appear to hear the
with a greater degree of resolution and accuracy tone(s) with a slightly different timing as well.
than other listeners. The term “diplacusis bin- Diplacusis dysharmonia (occasionally diplacu-
auralis” is attributed to Shambaugh Sr. (1907; sis harmonica) is a rarely used term, and, similar
cited by Albers & Wilson, 1968) and several to diplacusis binauralis, relates to distinct tones
case studies over the years describe challenges or musical notes being perceived when only one
to musicians’ performances and listening expe- note is presented.
riences. It is implicated in the effectiveness of Prevalence of diplacusis remains unknown
cochlear implant fittings and mapping (Reiss and is difficult to determine definitively. Colin
et al., 2007, 2011), although diplacusis is di- et al. (2016) indicate that prevalence among
agnosed and reported far less often than other patients with hearing loss exceeds that of normal-
manifestations of disordered sound tolerance hearing individuals, and that asymmetric hear-
( Jastreboff & Jastreboff, 2015). The needs of ing losses most often accompany the unusual
these diverse patient groups dictate that dipla- pitch event. Small study samples, few patient
cusis merits consideration from clinicians and reports, and difficulty measuring the pitch
researchers working with patients who require disparities are among the reasons prevalence
audiologic rehabilitation. data remain lacking. A search for primary care
At least three forms of diplacusis receive data indicates that 83 cases with diplacusis as
attention in the literature. Diplacusis mon- the primary diagnosis are on record for 2009 in

191
192 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

the United States (2013; ProQuest Document the effectiveness of cochlear implants as well.
#1267703683). The diplacusis phenomenon remains incom-
In an analysis of musicians with histo- pletely understood, however, a few recent cases
ries of noise exposure, Jansen et al. (2009) highlight the challenges associated with clinical
compared prevalence of various auditory im- management of such patients. In this chapter
pairments including hearing loss, hyperacusis, we will review causes and manifestations of dis-
diplacusis, and others. Of 241 musicians sam- ordered pitch perception, as well as review two
pled, 190 (79%) reported hyperacusis, or issues cases of patients with varying degrees of hearing
with loudness tolerance, 121 (51%) reported loss and diplacusis.
tinnitus, 57 (24%) reported “distortion” of mu-
sical tones, and 17 (7%) experienced diplacu-
sis. The experience of distortion may be a result
of processing impairment related to diplacusis Review of Pitch
(i.e., the roughness produced by two different
Perception Mechanisms
and unresolved notes), but in any case, the mu-
sician reports suggested that diplacusis was far
less common than hyperacusis and tinnitus. In The sensation of pitch is derived from two
this study, prevalence of diplacusis was about complimentary but distinct processing events.
10% that of hyperacusis. First, an environmental sound produces dis-
Most of us understand pitch to be the placement of the eardrum, middle ear bones,
psychological correlate of a sound’s frequency. and ultimately the sensory surface of the inner
In some cases, the terms are inaccurately used ear, the basilar membrane. Because the basilar
interchangeably; this mistake is understandable membrane displays different resonance fre-
as, in general, the frequency of a sound is propor- quencies at specific locations along its length,
tional to the pitch perception it creates. How- the perception of pitch is related to, and gen-
ever, the situation is not so simple. For example, erated by, the displacement pattern on the bas­
tangible pitch perceptions can arise through ilar membrane. The place theory of hearing
manipulations of complex sounds’ spectra that emerges from our understanding of cochlear
eliminate energy in the cochlear frequency re- traveling waves and displacement patterns, and
gion from which the pitch perception would the place theory may be pictured as analogous
appear to arise, forming the so-called “residue” to the relation between pitch and place along
or “virtual” pitch (e.g., Terhardt, 1974; Pantev the length of a piano keyboard. Consider that
et al., 1996). In other cases, the sensation of displacement of the keyboard surface on the
pitch may be altered following onset of hearing instrument’s left side produces low-frequency
loss, particularly single-sided loss, to the point notes that are progressively higher in pitch and
that a pure tone signal can produce a pitch that frequency as one moves from the left side of the
differs across a listener’s ears. Musicians experi- keyboard to the right side. The basilar mem-
encing this unusual event can on occasion map brane is similarly organized or mapped with
the pitch anomalies, and state with confidence respect to frequency of vibration such that the
the notes or range of notes that contribute to the left side of the piano corresponds to the api-
disordered pitch sensation. The realization that cal region of the cochlea while the right-hand
perceived pitch may not be accurate can drive a side of the piano corresponds to the basal re-
musician away from playing, and can limit the gion. Therefore, an environmental sound gen-
enjoyment one experiences listening to music. erates inner ear activity at the location along
Analogous pitch-related disorders can influence the membrane that resonates when exposed to
12. Diplacusis 193

the spectral content of the sound; the cochlea 250 Hz) not only produced the pitch sensa-
converts frequency to place, and initiates the tion of 250 Hz, it produced brain activity in
process by which the central mechanism prop- the 250 Hz region of the auditory cortex de-
agates a neurally encoded version of the sound. spite the omission of energy at that frequency
The neural representation of the sound, under from the sound’s spectrum.
normal conditions, emphasizes the energy as- Although frequency and intensity were
sociated with the most substantial physical dis- long considered independent physical attributes
placements of the membrane, thereby resulting of a sound, stimulus intensity influenced pitch
in a salient pitch perception. perception in a variety of experiments. Basilar
The place theory provided a clear and membrane recordings (Rhode, 1971) suggested
easily understood explanation for pitch per- that basilar membrane displacement patterns
ception during many routine listening tasks; at high signal levels were displaced in the basal-
however, some interesting contradictions to ward, or high-frequency direction, with concur-
its basic assumptions were identified. For ex- rent change in an observer’s rating of the pitch.
ample, Plomp (1967), Schouten et al. (1962), Presumably, in an individual with symmetric
and Terhardt (1974) identified spectral com- hearing (or hearing loss), the intensity effect
ponents of complex stimuli that produced would be approximately equal in both ears,
the sensation of pitch even when the stimu- minimizing the likelihood of diplacusis occur-
lus presented to listeners lacked energy at the ring. However, in cases of asymmetric loss,
frequency corresponding to the pitch. A har- or unilateral hearing loss, the influence of
monic complex of 800, 1000, and 1200 Hz such basilar membrane shifts on the ensuing
would produce a pitch sensation correspond- pitch event would be less predictable; an expe-
ing to a 200-Hz sinusoid despite the absence rienced listener (i.e., musician) could detect
of 200 Hz in the presented signal. This pitch pitch differences that would subsequently
experience arose from a stimulus that lacked reduce the consonance of musical passages,
energy at the cochlear place associated with particularly those with substantial intensity
the pitch frequency, and the pitch was termed fluctuations.
a “residue” of the harmonic complex. Because Additional investigators, such as Gold-
no displacement occurred at the 200-Hz place, stein (1973), concluded that pitch perception
the listener extracted pitch by using a mech- was drawn from temporal information related
anism other than the place information de- to the periodicity of the complex waveform.
scribed above. A pitch perception that was The aforementioned 800 to 1000 to 1200 Hz
assumed to rely upon displacement along the complex had a period of 5 ms, and the wave-
basilar membrane at a specific location arose form would complete a cycle at the same rate
without such activation; a “virtual pitch.” as a 200-Hz sinusoid. A temporal theory of
Plomp, Terhardt, and others ultimately re- pitch suggested that a central auditory pro-
ported that the highest common factor in the cessing mechanism locked to a specific phase,
harmonic train would correspond in most or moment in the waveform’s cycle, could be
cases to the perceived pitch. Indeed, Pantev responsible for the pitch perception in the ab-
et al. (1996) demonstrated that the virtual sence of sound energy at the pitch frequency.
pitch event produced activation in the same Limitations on the rate at which neurons
cortical area that would have been stimulated fire was found to limit the frequency range
by a sound with the matching spectral con- through which temporal mechanism would
tent. That is, a sequence of 8 tones at integer contribute to pitch identity (Palmer & Russell,
multiples of 250 Hz (but that did not include 1986). Taken together, the place theory and
194 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

the temporal theory offered reasonable expla- that a process of fusing the two tones had oc-
nations for most pitch events perceived by curred. This idea was supported by the obser-
normal-hearing listeners. vation that listeners appeared to “average” the
As with the place theory, the temporal two tones; the pitch of the emergent single
theory alone could not explain the various “fused” tone resided somewhere between the
pitch experiences and disorders that emerged stimulus frequencies. Because different co-
in clinical and laboratory testing. Ward (1963) chlear traveling wave displacements occurred
specified diplacusis as evidence that the wave- during the aforementioned two-tone stimu-
form periodicity alone could not explain pitch lation, a central mechanism was involved in
perception. If one sound was presented to both the fusion process. One way to conceptualize
ears, then both ears must be receiving iden­ the fusion experiments would be to consider
tically periodic signals. The temporal theory an analogous experiment in the tactile system.
specified that the rates of neural discharge sig- Two pins placed near each other on the fin-
nal pitch; however, if the same signal was pre- gertip may be indistinguishable from one pin,
sented to both ears, then the neural discharge until the two pins are spaced further apart. At
rates must be identical. The observation that some distance, two pins can be distinguished
not all patients heard the same tone in both from one pin—there is a critical distance of
ears affirmed that the temporal theory alone separation across the fingertip at which the
could not explain all pitch-related phenom- stimulation produced by two pins cannot be
ena. Further, the likelihood that peripheral loss fused into one.
could influence the pitch mechanisms further In a sense, this central mechanism mini-
limited the putative contribution of a central, mized the likelihood of diplacusis occurring as
temporal processing system. it reduced the relative contribution classically
Not surprisingly, subsequent findings sup- associated with pitch arising from the location
ported the notion that pitch extraction relied of maximum cochlear stimulation. Recall that
upon the complimentary action of peripheral diplacusis manifested as the perception of dif-
and central mechanisms. Van den Brink and ferent tones in the two ears despite the presen-
colleagues (1974, 1976) performed a number tation of the same tone to each. Van den Brink
of experiments in which test subjects were pre- et al. (1976) summarized this situation by stat-
sented, simultaneously, different tones in each ing that observers with “normal” hearing did
ear (e.g., 1000 Hz in the left ear, and a tone of not typically notice diplacusis even though all
variable frequency in the right ear). When the observers, regardless of hearing loss severity, had
variable tone frequency equaled or was within diplacusis “to some extent” (Van den Brink
0.2 to 0.4% of the fixed tone (in this example, et al., 1976; p. 1472). This statement reiterated
within 20 to 40Hz of the 1000-Hz standard; Licklider’s (1951) assertion that “a moderate de-
although substantial variability was found be- gree of binaural pitch disparity is the rule rather
tween subjects) then the participant heard a than the exception” (Licklider, 1951; p. 1033).
single tone. When the frequency difference was Therefore, subtle changes affecting the central
increased, the “fusion” into a single pitch heard processor, such as those that would impair the
in the two tones was no longer possible for fusion mechanism, could allow for more fre-
the listener, and the percept changed to that quent perceptions of diplacusis in an affected
of hear­ing two tones simultaneously. individual, particularly when stimulated by
The fact that two different tones would environmental events that produced strong
be heard as one suggested that frequency dif- pitch perceptions, such as music. If true, then
ferences were not always heard as such, and this situation would be most likely in cases of
12. Diplacusis 195

asymmetric hearing loss of cochlear origin that nation with the various constrictions or other
disrupted basilar membrane mechanics, or for physical modifications to the flow of air dis-
individuals, such as musicians, with substantial placed by the instrument’s sounding. Because
experience involving pitch judgment. of these physical differences, a musical note,
say the concert A of 440 Hz, will sound dif-
ferent when played on a violin compared to a
trumpet. Although the two instruments play
the same note, it sounds different because of
Pitch and Musical Tones
the instruments’ physical differences. Timbral
differences exist in the presence of pitch simi-
Perception of musical intervals, or the listener’s larities (or differences) and may be influenced,
ability to evaluate the frequency difference be- as with other perceptual events, by disorders of
tween notes, highlights the listener’s coincident pitch perception.
use of at least the place and timing mecha- It is worth noting that many patients
nisms; the reader would be safe in assuming reporting diplacusis are musicians, or music
that the listener’s experience with sound and enthusiasts. Schubert (1957) and others have
music would also influence matters. First, the commented that the pitch sensation associated
pitch comparison between a note on a piano with diplacusis might not be evident, or obvi-
and the frequency of the note reveals a mono- ous, to an individual without musical training;
tonic change upward as one moves toward they might not report the problem as it does
the right side of a piano keyboard. This rela- not interfere with their livelihood or related
tion produces a sensation termed tone height recreational activities. This situation will be
(Bachem, 1950). Bachem also reports a dif- illustrated in cases reviewed at the end of the
ferent pitch experience related to different oc­­ chapter. The experience of diplacusis, and the
taves of musical notes. For example, a C in influence it exerts on the enjoyment of music,
one octave retains the pitch of a C even when bears consideration and begs the question: if
played at appropriate frequencies in higher or a person experiences diplacusis from an early
lower registers. A note of C one octave below age, is it possible that music never “sounds
middle C could be matched to the same pitch right” to that individual, and would that per-
as middle C even though the frequency of the son be one who states that there is no partic-
former would be approximately ½ that of the ular form of music that they enjoy, and that,
latter. The consistency of pitch across octaves indeed, they have never been interested in lis-
of a musical note, or its pitch class, is termed tening to music?
tone chroma. Therefore, a note with frequency
of 220 Hz would have the same tone chroma
as one of 440 Hz, however, the 440-Hz tone
would have a greater tone height.
Causes of Diplacusis
Other aspects of tone perception rely
upon more complex processing that gives rise
to a tone’s quality, or its timbre. Timbre is asso- The reliance on mechanisms both peripheral
ciated with the harmonics and spectral compo- (place theory, contributing primarily to tone
nents produced by the physical characteristics, height) and central (temporal theory, contrib-
such as length and diameter of a string (as in uting primarily to tone chroma) suggests that
a harp, piano, or guitar), an open tube (as in pitch perception could be impaired via injury
a brass or woodwind instrument) in combi- or disruption to several diverse and seemingly
196 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

unrelated elements of the auditory pathway. fork tests, bone-conduction testing, and med-
Cochlear hearing loss could give rise to changes ical examination ruled out allergy, infection,
in, or losses of, audibility that would preclude circulatory system disorder, as well as alcohol
pitch perception (i.e., Gaeth & Norris, 1965). and nicotine abuse. Because the patient was a
Neural damage could interfere with timing musician, he was able to identify the specific
cues or synchronous neural activity necessary pitch region that was affected; his complaints
for pitch extraction that relies upon temporal arose from difficulties tuning his instrument,
processing, or phase-locking in the analysis of an act that put him at odds with his colleagues
a sound’s spectrum. In order to make sense of in the orchestra.
patient complaints regarding pitch anomalies, Given the patient’s medical history, Schu­
such as diplacusis, it is important to consider bert (1957) speculated labyrinthine edema,
changes to auditory function that have the po- or fluid retention in the inner ear, was at the
tential to impair the pitch perception process. source of the patient’s reported problems. He
The first consideration will be cases in which a used an intravenous glucose administration
single tone is perceived as noticeably different as an agent to promote dehydration in the
in the two ears. cochlea twice daily. Diplacusis “disappeared
but recurred a few days after treatment was
stopped. This repeated itself several times”
(p. 4) as Schubert managed the patient’s case.
Schubert’s direct treatment of the cochlea’s
Cochlear Damage condition, and the ensuing diplacusis fluctu-
ations experienced by the patient, suggested
Early mentions of diplacusis were related to cochlear site of lesion for the diplacusis phe-
patients with unilateral or asymmetric senso- nomenon. Schubert’s intervention, in retro-
rineural hearing loss. Schubert (1957) sum- spect, resembled the common use of steroids
marized much of the earlier work related to for sudden hearing losses of cochlear origin
investigations of pitch-related impairments as believed to arise from inflammatory processes
he reported that its causes included, among as would occur in a case of inner ear autoim-
other insults to auditory mechanism infection, mune disorder.
head trauma (he specified “accidents or war in- The association between inflammatory
juries”), atrophy of the brain, and otosclerosis processes and diplacusis also was discussed
surgery. Schubert also conveyed that the liter- by George Shambaugh Sr. (1920; cited in Al-
ature regarding diplacusis did not specify the bers & Wilson, 1968) and by George Sham-
mechanisms by which the unusual perception baugh, Jr. (1940; 1959). In his 1959 text,
arose. Shambaugh, Jr. discussed the influence of lab-
Schubert (1957) presented two cases yrithitis on cochlear function. He indicated
upon which he built an argument for a co- that the edema associated with an inner ear
chlear/labyrinthine source of the inaccurate infection would affect primarily perception
pitch event. In the first case, he described a of high frequency sounds and could result in
37-year-old male patient, a musician, with uni- both distorted hearing and diplacusis, echo-
lateral hearing loss and the presence of diplacu- ing Schubert’s (1957) work.
sis in the right ear. The loss was determined to Other investigators speculated regarding
be sensorineural, and the presence of diplacusis the importance of cochlear mechanisms to the
of 3-week duration prior to Schubert’s exam- development of diplacusis as they discussed
ination. Differential diagnosis included tuning cases similar to those reported by Schubert.
12. Diplacusis 197

Gaeth and Norris (1965) described five patients To clarify, an example of this experimen-
with unilateral sensorineural hearing loss who tal approach could be described as follows.
reported diplacusis-like sound experiences. As First, a patient with unilateral hearing loss was
with prior investigators, Gaeth and Norris re- presented a tone in the good ear, and then the
ported that pitch variances were most prevalent patient adjusted a tone in the poorer ear until
in the region of hearing loss. The general find- it matched the pitch of the tone in the better
ings they reported and reviewed indicated that, ear. The two frequencies that were matched in
in fatigued (i.e., ears displaying temporary pitch by the listener were compared, and in
threshold shift), or damaged ears, patients expe- the vast majority of cases, the listener ascribed
rienced an upward shift in pitch, and distorted a higher-frequency tone in the poor ear as one
quality of hearing. They also noted, however, that matched the frequency in the better ear.
that an individual with normal hearing (one In this example, it might be that a 2000-Hz
in their study, several historically) exhibited tone in the better ear was equal in pitch to a
similar anomalous findings. 2200-Hz tone in the poorer ear. When the
Gaeth and Norris (1965) tested patients same person was stimulated in both ears by
in monaural (either the better ear or the poorer the same tone, and then asked to match the
ear) and binaural listening conditions. After pitch of the tone, the participant matched the
test subjects completed practice trials in which tone to an intermediate frequency. In this ex-
all stimuli were presented to normal-hearing ample, a 2000-Hz tone presented to both ears
ears, their pitch matches were assessed in other, might be matched to a comparison tone of
more challenging conditions and the deviation 2100 Hz.
in Hz from the initial perceived pitch in the Although cochlear damage with related
better ear was measured. Pitch matches in the hearing impairment, and diplacusis often co-
participants’ poor ears were less consistent, and occurred in clinical reports, their relation was
revealed that impaired ears perceived stimuli not readily determined. Obviously not all in-
to be of higher pitch than nonimpaired ears. dividuals with hearing loss would experience
For example, the poor ear pitch matches using diplacusis, and on occasion, diplacusis ap-
stimuli presented to one ear at a time (mon- peared in individuals with normal hearing
aurally) were to frequencies consistently higher (Gaeth & Norris, 1965). Knight (2004) ex-
than those in normal ears when the two were amined a case of sudden unilateral hearing loss
compared. When test subjects with unilateral in which the patient complained of diplacusis,
hearing loss were assessed using stimuli pre- and was treated after one week’s time with
sented to both ears simulta­neously (binaurally) anti-inflammatory steroids. He compared the
the results indicated processing more similar to, patient’s threshold change to their otoacous-
but not reaching that of normal hearing ears; tic emission (OAE) characteristics with addi-
listeners provided intermediate pitch values tional consideration of the diplacusis reports.
that settled between the normal and abnormal The patient displayed a slight decrease in low
matches. When stimuli levels were increased frequency sensitivity that did not reach clinical
from 40 dB (as above) to 80 dB, the pitch significance (i.e., the thresholds changed, but re­
matches in hearing-impaired ears improved, mained in the normal range and returned to pre-
that is, pitch was matched to tone frequen- shift thresholds within three weeks of onset), a
cies that were closer to the reference stimulus mild threshold shift at 8kHz that recovered as
than when tested at the lower stimulus level; well, but over a longer time frame, and a severe
nevertheless, the pitch was still perceived as a threshold shift at 12 kHz that did not return to
higher frequency than the stimulus. normal; in sum, thresholds over time returned
198 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

to normal at frequencies through 8 kHz, but sudden hearing loss resulting from cochlear
remained elevated at 12 kHz. Note that base- damage may resolve over time as the cochlea’s
line data were not available, therefore thresh- function improves.
olds that improved over time to the normal
range may not have returned to preloss values.
The patient’s reports of diplacusis wors-
ened during the first week the hearing loss was Central Mechanisms
present even though the hearing thresholds
did not worsen during that time. Diplacusis Cochlear dysfunction provided a reasonable
was reportedly most pronounced in higher fre- explanation for perception of diplacusis asso-
quencies, associated by Knight with the pos- ciated with hearing loss, particularly in those
sibility that infection spread from the middle cases when the loss was temporary, asymmet-
ear into the basal (high-frequency) region of ric, and the diplacusis experience decreased as
the cochlea. His measure of OAEs linked the hearing improved. However, it was also the
cochlea’s health to the reported changes in case that individuals with normal hearing
sensitivity. Emissions provoked by broadband could experience pitch anomalies. Further,
signals (transient OAEs) displayed different pitch processing that resulted in diplacusis or
recovery time courses from recovery associ- other undesirable auditory outcomes was of-
ated with pure tone thresholds and diplacusis. ten a concern for cochlear implant recipients
Therefore, the OAE behavior was presumably and their audiologists. In a sense, the binau-
related to mechanisms different from those that ral fusion described by Van den Brink et al.
produced the diplacusis perception. However, (1976) might not be available, or may require
Knight also used frequency-specific stimuli fa- training and experience, in order to be of ben-
cilitating measures of the OAE’s fine frequency efit to some CI recipients.
structure; results of these measures were related Reiss and colleagues (2007, 2016) demon-
over time and in the frequency region of inter- strated that CI patients displayed changes in
est, to patient reports of diplacusis. Although pitch over time with CI use, and that well-
the amplitude of the OAEs was not correlated established mechanisms of pitch extraction,
with patient reports, the spectral energy of the such as those described by van den Brink et al.
emission fine structure, once the emissions re- (1976) could not adequately explain pitch pro-
appeared late in the first week following the cessing and changes as demonstrated by such
sudden hearing loss, changed in conjunction patients. Reiss et al. (2007) tracked changes in
with the patient’s reports of anomalous pitch monaural CI users’ perceived pitch and found
experiences. Knight indicated that the emis- that experience with the implant influenced
sion behavior, coupled to the patient’s experi- pitch judgments over time. Three of five sub-
ence, was evidence that cochlear traveling wave jects who compared electrical stimuli to acous-
mechanics had been altered, and that traveling tic (in their better ear) displayed decreases in
wave peaks in the cochlea were displaced to- pitch matches of up to two octaves within 2 to
ward the base of the organ when the hearing 5 years of implant use. It should be noted that
loss was still present. A basalward displacement the changes in pitch were not always accompa-
of the cochlear traveling wave would account nied by similarly substantial changes in word
for several aspects of diplacusis reported pre- recognition ability. The investigators indicated
viously with a shift in pitch upward the most that mechanisms of peripheral processing that
obvious example. As depicted in cases at the contributed to patients’ pitch and frequency
end of this chapter, diplacusis associated with resolution, as well as intensity cues, could not
12. Diplacusis 199

on their own explain the observed changes in


patient performance.
Management of Diplacusis
Perhaps a more dramatic piece of evidence
supporting a dissociation between pitch per- Management of diplacusis must consider the
ceptions with CIs was reported by Reiss et al. roles to pitch perception provided by both
(2011) as a case study. A patient with profound cochlear (i.e., place) and neural (i.e., timing)
high frequency hearing loss was implanted with systems. Given that some degree of diplacusis
a short, 10-mm electrode. Two years later, the is demonstrated by listeners who do not com-
patient’s residual hearing in both ears was lost plain about the symptom, it is clear that the
to an autoimmune disorder; the patient was phenomenon is, at least in its least objection-
implanted in the other ear with a full-length, able form, a by-product of normal auditory
24-mm electrode. The differences in electrode function. The well-established effects of lis-
length required distinct frequency allocations tening experience, as described by Burns, Van
across electrode sites because the two electrodes den Brink, Reiss, and colleagues, provide hope
terminated at substantially different cochlear lo- that the diplacusis perceptions, although dis-
cations. The patient was asked to compare the turbing and perhaps career-changing, may not
pitch of binaurally-presented electrical stimuli. be permanent.
Although the electrodes stimulating auditory Because the majority of patients with di-
nerves were of different length, their putative placusis display hearing loss, specifically asym-
frequency coordinates in the cochlea allowed for metric high-frequency hearing loss, a hearing
predictions regarding pitch evaluations. Within aid fit to the poorer ear should improve the sit-
three years after implantation of the second uation by restoring audibility in the damaged
electrode, the investigators found a “large mis- frequency region. Unfortunately, hearing aids
match” between the cochlear locations assumed do not restore normal function, they merely
to be associated with the stimulus frequency and amplify environmental sounds, and patients
the resulting pitch sensation. That is, electrode (such as the one examined below in case #1)
pairs across the two ears that stimulated nerve report that the devices do not normalize pitch
fibers synapsing at substantially different cochlear experiences. Although hearing aids may facili-
regions ended up producing similar, matched, tate communication and localization of sound,
pitches in the two ears. The lack of correspon- they do not appear to reconcile the interaural
dence between cochlear place and pitch sug- differences in processing that generate two dis-
gested that CI recipients developed, from ex­­ tinct pitch perceptions. This shortcoming of
perience, adaptation strategies that relied upon hearing aid fitting related to diplacusis com-
central mechanisms in order to minimize or plaints affects individuals regardless of their
reconcile pitch/spectral differences between ears. listening experience; musicians and non-
Reiss et al. (2011) raised the interesting pos- musicians stand to receive similar benefit from
sibility that central mechanisms may “realign” hearing aid fitting, at least with regard to di-
disparate inputs, or abnormal pitch experi­ placusis complaints.
ences, despite gross differences in cochlear ar- Musicians and music enthusiasts have
eas and fiber populations stimulated. Indeed, a different set of listening experiences, and
the authors did not assert that the patient’s ad- clearly a more personal relationship to music,
aptation had normalized pitch absolutely, but than do non-musically inclined individuals. A
rather had developed the means to provide the person’s relationship with music may be pro-
kind of binaural fusion between the two ears found and cherished. Compromising or dis-
discussed by van den Brink et al. (1976). torting that source of succor and enjoyment
200 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

has the potential to ruin a life. Musicians piano player who has several recordings to his
faced with giving up their occupation often credit. Mr. C was first seen at our clinic in Au-
use brute force—memorization of muscle ac- gust 2015, at which time he was 81 years old.
tion that produces a pitch when singing, for He served in the military, however, he reported
example—in order to maintain their ability his hearing as normal upon discharge. The pa-
to perform or share music with peers. Such tient’s medical history includes type II diabetes
an adjustment is admirable, but frustrating to which he indicated is controlled effectively.
have to maintain, and many musicians curtail The patient first noticed hearing loss in
their activities as a result. 2003, more pronounced in the left ear, and
This situation is regrettable, particularly he completed a fitting with personal ampli-
when we consider the importance of experi- fication. He used a standard hearing aid (no
ence in the judgment of pitch. Age of hearing fitting information was available) monaurally
loss onset, and duration of hearing aid use, in- for several years. In 2009 he was fit with a
crease the likelihood that listeners will display new device at a Veteran’s hospital; however,
binaural fusion across a wide range of frequen- throughout this time period, the patient was
cies (Reiss et al., 2017). This finding extends able to play, record, and perform music as
to Reiss and colleagues’ work with patients a soloist as well as with different groups. In
who receive CIs in that fusion of two differ- 2013, he was fit with a second hearing aid. It
ent tones presented to the ears occurs across a was shortly after that time that he began to
broad frequency range. Experience with de- notice changes in his hearing and in his abil-
vices, whether acoustic or electric, accesses or ity to process pitch.
exploits plasticity in the auditory system that First, in the spring of 2014, on a Sunday
fuse pitch experiences for some listeners. Un- night, he realized that he was struggling to
fortunately, the range of frequencies through distinguish musical notes. The notes sounded
which fusion would occur in CI patients may “fuzzy” and “noisy” but did not retain their
be far greater than would be found with nor- pitch. However, if he played a note, one re-
mal hearing listeners. Additionally, what is portedly sustained for 30 to 45 seconds, the
not clear at present is whether musicians or tonality would change, and the sensation of
music lovers can exploit the fusion event to pitch would become more salient. He noticed
extract a pitch sensation that is accurate, or that the severity of this problem changed day
that improves the listeners’ ability to practice, to day; there was no steady improvement,
perform, and share music. Indeed, Reiss et al. rather steps forward and backward over the
(2016) reiterated previous reports that such course of several months.
fusion may actually reduce word recognition He set about mapping the changes in
ability and lead to errors of absolute pitch pitch, and his findings were consistent with
judgment. those reported by Schubert (1957) and oth-
ers. Specifically, he observed that in the third
octave above middle C, the note G was heard
as A (that is, a full tone higher in pitch), and
Cases
the sensation was obvious in the left ear.
Other pitches in the same octave were simi-
Two cases in which individuals with back- larly affected, but not to that degree. In the
grounds in music are reviewed below. The next octave up, the pitch sensation returned
first case involves a musician and composer, a to normal for all notes. He assured us that
12. Diplacusis 201

his hearing had not changed since the time bothersome music (i.e., avoid avoidance), and
he noticed the pitch anomaly provided results rather to gravitate, over time, to passages that
from a recent audiogram. he anticipated would be difficult.
Our time in the clinic focused on assess- It is now two years from our initial con-
ing specific pitch events rather than monitor- tact. The patient reports his situation is less dire,
ing hearing. Upon completion of his intake but he is not back to where he was prior to the
interview, we asked whether the patient would changes in hearing. He notes improvements
be willing to spend some time in the audio not just with listening and playing music, but
suite “playing around.” He brought with him a also with sound localization. He remains bin-
harmonica and was eager to experiment. First, aurally amplified, and still has a substantial
we tried to determine whether the presence of asymmetric hearing loss. It is likely that this pa-
a low-level masking noise presented to each ear tient suffered from a sudden hearing loss some-
in turn, and then bilaterally (both narrowband time in the spring, 2014, that exacerbated his
and white noise were used) would eliminate pre-existing loss. Perhaps the pre-existing loss,
or modify the pitch perception; it did not. gradual in nature, had allowed for the patient
During this testing, the patient stated that his to adjust unconsciously over time and to main-
difficulties were most pronounced when lis- tain reasonably accurate pitch judgments. The
tening to chords, or when trying to ascertain sudden change in 2014 likely did not recover
an interval separating notes. He also reported fully, hence the patient’s prior ability to adjust
that he heard a minor third on occasion when and manage the loss could not be repeated,
sounding a single note, for example, when he producing the observed and durable changes
played an E, he also heard a minor third, or a in pitch processing. His case remains open and
G, mixed in with the note he was playing. The he will continue to be followed.
patient indicated that this distortion occurred The second case centers on an individual
on occasion, but not at all times. (the chapter’s author, MF) with little musical
Upon interviewing the patient, it was training, but a rich experience throughout his
clear that he was avoiding listening to and lifetime listening to and playing music. He
playing music that sounded abnormal and un- hesitates to call himself a musician, rather, he
pleasant to him, and he also reported that not plays bass. Until 2012, he had normal hearing
all music produced the same response. How- bilaterally, but at about that time, he observed
ever, he also admitted that he likely would not that he often could not hear the monitor tone
have noticed this event were it not for his ex- from the audiometer through headphones at
perience with music. This final point was used 6 to 8 kHz in only the right ear. Measures
as a way to help the patient shape his listening at that time revealed a mild, high frequency,
experiences and environment (see Chapter 15 asymmetric hearing loss. On March 15, 2016,
for a sample listening protocol). that situation changed.
He was encouraged to find music that On the previous evening, the ear felt full,
posed the minimum challenge to his enjoy- but not alarmingly so. During the night, how-
ment, and to try to increase his inventory of ever, with the left ear facing the pillow, the pa-
musical selections over time. Our intent was tient was startled when the family dog jumped
to facilitate the reintegration of music as a on the bed. Normally, this event would be
prime motivator and positive experience for prefaced by the old dog getting a running start,
this patient. He understood the logic of min- scrambling across the floor, signaling the jump
imizing his attempts to shield himself from to come. However, on this night, the dog�s nails
202 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

on the floor were inaudible, hence the landing numerous times, including instances when
on the bed alarming. MF sang along to the music with the poor
Upon waking the next morning, it was ear plugged, then switched to plug the good
clear that the right ear had experienced a sub- ear, with the result that he observed the key in
stantial decrease in sensitivity. Running water, which he was singing was flat, or a lower pitch
voices, the car, everything sounded foreign than the music. The process worked in re-
and unclear. MF was reminded of complaints verse as well; he could plug the good ear, sing
he had heard in the clinic, of patients who along, and when switching to plug the bad
stated that some voices sounded as though ear, his singing was now a full tone sharp as
they were being spoken through cellophane; heard in the good ear. The change in pitch was
scratchy, tinny, crinkled, and distorted. While immediate upon switching plugged ears, and
these patient impressions had, in such clinical when the maneuver was performed during
encounters, been difficult to imagine, their ve­ an instrumental section prominently featur-
racity was now apparent. ing guitar, the sound of the guitar changed
A hearing test later that morning revealed as though the guitarist was using a so-called
a sensorineural hearing loss in the right ear with “whammy bar,” which is a metal arm attached
thresholds 10 to 30 dB worse than previously; to the bridge of the instrument that increases
thresholds at 4, 6, and 8 kHz were 55, 60, and or releases tension on the strings, thereby rais-
65 dB, respectively. As a patient, MF was seen ing or lowering pitch, when it is pulled up or
later that day in a local ENT physician’s office, pressed down, respectively.
and after an exam was prescribed prednisone. The sensation produced by sound during
The balance of the day was spent at work, as the next 48 hours can be described in many
the auditory event was not accompanied by ways, and it was clear that a naïve patient could
vertigo, nor any other symptoms. Note that easily catastrophize the event. It was also clear
MF worked with the patient described above that a person with a gratifying relationship
prior to the sudden hearing loss onset. with music would feel the change in hearing
Following MF’s brief experience with as a personal loss, perhaps a loss that com-
the patient described in case 1, he decided to pelled grieving, anger, or self-recrimination.
experiment by listening to familiar music as a Such were the patient narratives recalled by
means to assess the effect of the sudden hear- MF in the past; of course, at this point, such
ing loss on music appreciation. He picked an patient reports took on new meaning. Over
album that contained clear and simple vocal the next few days, MF experimented with
harmonies, but also distorted guitars in order music, listening and playing. By the third day
to assess the effect of the hearing loss on the post-onset, hearing improved, and the sensa-
ability to resolve chords and accurately pro- tion of diplacusis abated. Although the hear-
cess complex and distorted signals. The mu- ing did not return completely, the pitch sen-
sic, as all sound at that time, sounded unusual sation normalized.
although it remained familiar. Most striking, The two cases illustrated common find-
however, was the observation that when MF ings in patients with diplacusis: asymmetric
plugged his “good” ear, the pitch of the record- hearing loss, sudden onset hearing loss, and
ing immediately changed, as though someone an awareness of the disorder that was likely re­­
had modulated the frequency of all instru- lated to a person’s listening experience. It is
ments and voices upward by approximately also likely that the patient who is able to coex-
a full tone. This observation was repeated ist with the unusual sounds, and continue to
12. Diplacusis 203

accept exposures to such sounds, has the po- Knight, R. D. (2004). Diplacusis, hearing thresh-
tential to experience improvement. Such ad- old and otoacoustic emissions in an episode of
vice, in lieu of an evidence-based cure, should sudden, unilateral cochlear hearing loss. Inter­
be an important element of counseling and national Journal of Audiology, 43, 45–53.
management. Licklider, J. C. R. (1951). Basic correlate of the
auditory stimulus. In S. S. Stevens (Ed.). Hand­
book of experimental psychology. New York, NY:
John Wiley & Sons.
References Palmer, A. R., & Russell, I. J. (1986). Phase-
locking in the cochlear nerve of the guinea pig
and its relation to the receptor potential of in-
Albers, G. D., & Wilson, W. H. (1968). Dipla- ner hair cells. Hearing Research, 24, 1–15.
cusis. I. Historical review. Arch Otolaryngol., Pantev, C., Elbert, T., Ross, B., Eulitz, C., & Ter-
87(6), 601–603. hardt, E. (1996). Binaural fusion and the rep-
Bachem, A. (1950). Tone height and tone chroma resentation of virtual pitch in the human audi-
as two different pitch qualities. Acta Psycholog­ tory cortex. Hearing Research, 100, 164–170.
ica, 7, 80–88. Plomp, R. (1967). Pitch of complex tones. J. Acou.
Colin, D., Micheyl, C., Girod, A., Truy, E., & Soc. Am., 41, 1526–1533.
Gallégo, S. (2016). Binaural diplacusis and its Reiss, L. A. J., Turner, C. W., Erenberg, S. R., &
relationship with hearing-threshold asymme- Gantz, B. J. (2007). Changes in pitch with a
try. PLoS ONE, 11(8), e0159975. doi:10.1371 cochlear implant over time. J. Assoc. Res. Otolar­
/journal.pone.0159975. yngol., 8(2), 241–257.
Gaeth, J. H., & Norris, T. W. (1965). Diplacusis Reiss, L. A. J., Lowder, M. W., Karsten, S. A.,
in unilateral high-frequency hearing losses. J. Turner, C. W., & Gantz, B. J. (2011). Effects of
Speech Hear. Res., 8, 63–75. extreme tonotopic mismatches between bilat-
Gfeller, K., Turner, C., Mehr, M., Woodworth, G., eral cochlear implants on electric pitch percep-
Fearn, R., Knutson, J. F., . . . Stordahl, J. (2002). tion: a case study. Ear Hear., 32(4), 536–540.
Recognition of familiar melodies by adult co- doi:10.1097/AUD.0b013e31820c81b0
chlear implant recipients and normal-hearing Reiss, L. A. J., Eggleston, J. L., Walker, E. P., &
adults. Cochlear Implants International, 3(1), Oh, Y. (2016). Two ears are not always better
29–53. doi:10.1002/cii.50. than one: Mandatory vowel fusion across spec-
Goldstein, J. L. (1973). An optimum processor the- trally mismatched ears in hearing-impaired lis-
ory for the central formation of the pitch of com- teners. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryng., 17, 342–356.
plex tones. J. Acou. Soc. Am., 54, 1496–1516. doi: 10.1007/s10162-016-0570-z
https://search.proquest.com/docview/878064162 Reiss, L. A. J., Shayman, C. S., Walker, E. P., Ben-
/fulltext/150F97DE184D49C0PQ/1?accountid nett, K. O., Fowler, J. R., Hartling, C. L., . . .
=10771. Oh, Y. (2017). Binaural pitch fusion: Compar-
Jansen, E. J. M., Helleman, H. W., Dreschler, W. A., isons of normal-hearing and hearing-impaired
and de Laat, J. A. P. M. (2009). Noise induced listeners. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 141(3), 1909–1920.
hearing loss and other hearing complaints Rhode, W. S. (1971). Observations of the vibra-
among musicians of symphony orchestras. Int. tion of the basilar membrane in squirrel mon-
Arch Occup. Environ. Health, 82, 153–164. keys using the Mossbaurer technique. J. Acoust.
Jastreboff, P. J., & Jastreboff, M. M. (2015). De- Soc. Am., 49, 1218–1231.
creased sound tolerance: Hyperacusis, misopho- Schouten, J. F., Ritsma, R. J., & Cardozo, B. L.
nia, diplacousis, and polyacousis. In G. G. Ce- (1962). Pitch of the residue. J. Acou. Soc. Am.,
lesia & G. Hickok (Eds.). Handbook of clinical 34, 1418–1424.
neurology; the human auditory system; Vol. 129 Schubert, K. (1957). “On pathological pitch per-
(3rd Series). Edinburgh, U.K.: Elsevier. ception,” in Translations of the Beltone Institute
204 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

for Hearing Research, edited by C. Lightfoot. van den Brink, G. (1974). Monotic and dichotic
Beltone Institute for Hearing Research; Chi- pitch matchings with complex sounds. In E.
cago, IL Vol. 5, pp. 1–12. Zwicker & E. Terhardt (Eds.). Facts and mod­
Shambaugh, G. E. Jr. (1940). Diplacusis: A local- els in hearing (pp. 178–188). Berlin, Germany:
izing symptom of disease of the organ of Corti. Springer.
Arch. Otol., 31, 160. van den Brink, G., Sintnicolaas, K., & van Stam,
Shambaugh, G. E. Jr. (1959). Surgery of the ear. W. S. (2017). Dichotic pitch fusion. J. Acoust.
Philadelphia, PA: WB Saunders. Soc. Am., 59, 1471–1476.
Terhardt, E. (1974). Pitch, consonance, and har- Ward, W. D. (1963). Diplacusis and auditory the-
mony. J. Acou. Soc. Am., 55, 1061–1069. ory. J. Acou. Soc. Am., 35, 1746–1747.
SECTION IV
Management
13
Increased Sound
Sensitivity in Children
Veronica Kennedy, Claire Benton, and Rosie Kentish

of a sound, and the second, misophonia, was


Introduction
described as an abnormally strong reaction to
a sound with a specific pattern and/or mean­
Increased sensitivity to sounds, often termed ing to an individual. Klein (1990) also used
hyperacusis, is a common experience for many a definition describing the reaction to sound
children. For most children affected, this is a rather than the perception of sound as, “consis­
transient problem lasting for the duration of tently exaggerated or inappropriate responses
the sounds, for others it can be a distressing to sounds that are neither threatening nor un­
experience which limits the child’s activities comfortably loud to a typical person.”
and those of the child’s family. There is cur­ The lack of a consensus about a defini­
rently no consensus on the definition of hy­ tion is likely a reflection of the heterogeneity
peracusis. Nevertheless, different writers have of the symptoms associated with hyperacusis.
converged on similar terminology, such as “an Phillips and Carr (1998) proposed that distur­
unusual (low) loudness discomfort level asso­ bances in loudness perception could arise from
ciated with annoyance from normal sounds” a variety of physical and psychological sources,
(Coelho, Sanchez, & Tyler 2007), “an abnor­ and that the differences in underlying patho­
mal lowered tolerance to sound” (Baguley & physiology would be reflected behaviorally.
Andersson, 2007), an “increased sensitivity to The above definitions imply that hyper­
sound in levels that would not trouble a nor­ acusis arises from a disordered process, whereas
mal individual” (Katzenell & Segal, 2001), or in the case of young children, sensitivity to
simply “a heightened awareness of sounds” sounds may be an element of developmental
(Phillips & Carr, 1998). Jastreboff and Jas­ or maturational experience which normalizes
treboff (2014) proposed two components of as the child gets older. Misophonia, loudness
what they termed decreased sound tolerance recruitment, which is an abnormally steep
(DST): the first, hyperacusis, was described as a loudness growth for given increments in stim­
negative reaction to the physical characteristics ulus intensity, and is pathognomic of cochlear

207
208 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

hearing loss (Phillips & Carr, 1998) and sound- to the developmental status of the child. De­
provoked seizures (Tibussek, 2006) are ex­ velopmental problems such as ASD tend to be
plored in Chapter 8. In this chapter we will more common in males and children thus af­
consider the effects of hyperacusis on children fected may not be proportionately represented
by defining hyperacusis as an increased sensi­ in a mainstream school study.
tivity to routinely experienced sounds resulting
in distress to the child or an adverse impact on
the activities of the child.
Associated Conditions

Prevalence of
While hyperacusis can occur in isolation, many
Hyperacusis in Children
associated conditions have been reported in
pediatric and adult populations. While adults
While the prevalence within a general school- demonstrate a high association between tinni­
aged population is reported as between 3.2 tus and hyperacusis, less information is avail­
and 17.1%, the prevalence can be much able in the pediatric population. Hall et al.
higher, up to 95%, in specific populations. (2015) reported that 41.9% of the children
This variation may be explained by the ways with hyperacusis also had tinnitus. Coelho
in which children were questioned about their et al. (2007) reported 50% of children with
experiences, the definitions used by the inves­ hyperacusis had tinnitus, while tinnitus was
tigators, the setting of the studies, the number present in 17.8% of the children without hyper­
of children involved, the age of the studied acusis. In a study on children with tinnitus,
population, study tools, and associated condi­ Baguley et al. (2013) reported that 39% had
tions (Table 13–1). For example, in a study on hyperacusis. Hearing loss has been associated
children with Williams syndrome, Klein, Arm­ with hyperacusis including conductive hearing
strong, and Greer (1990) reported a prevalence loss due to otitis media with effusion (OME)
of hyperacusis of 95%. Other studies investi­ (Coelho et al., 2007; Myne & Kennedy, 2017),
gating children with autism spectrum disorder and high frequency hearing loss seen in Wil­
(ASD) reported prevalence that varied between liams syndrome (Klein et al., 1990; Miani et al.,
18% and 50% (Rosenhall et al., 1999; Hall 2001; Levitin et al., 2005) or after exposure to
et al., 2015). loud music (Rodrigues et al., 2015; Landälv,
Different age ranges have been used across Malmström, & Widén, 2013). Acoustic trauma
the studies of children with hyperacusis. In the (Remenschneider et al., 2014) and semicircular
studies across the entire pediatric age range canal dehiscence (Thabet, 2014) have also been
(0 to 19 years), the commonest age of presen­ reported.
tation was 3 to 4 years (Settal, 2009; Myne & Additional medical conditions associated
Kennedy, 2017). There was no consistent re­ with hyperacusis are summarized in Chapter 7
ported gender effect. In some studies (Coelho and include migraine (Tkachuk et al., 2003),
et al., 2007; Widen & Erlandsson, 2004), fe­ Bell’s palsy (Lee et al., 2013), Lowe syndrome
males comprised the majority of children with (Recker et al., 2015), and Laron syndrome (At­
hyperacusis. Conversely, Hall et al. (2015), tias et al., 2012). Associated neurodevelopmen­
Myne and Kennedy (2017), and Settal (2009) tal conditions include ASD (Gomes, Rotta,
reported a higher proportion of males with hy­ Pedroso, Sleifer, & Danesi, 2004; Hall et al.,
peracusis. Some of these differences may relate 2015; Khalfa et al., 1999), Fragile X syndrome
Table 13–1. Overview of studies of hyperacusis in children.

No of
Children (with
Hyperacusis/ Associated
Prevalence Age Range in Study) Study Tools Conditions

Coelho Bothered by 5–12 years; 16/499 Interview (child), Tinnitus, otitis


et al. sound; 42% questionnaire media with
(2007) Hyperacusis (parents) effusion
3.2% otoscopy,
tympanometry,
Phonophobia audiometry,
9% LDLs
Khalfa 63% 9–17 years 11/11 LDLs Autistic spectrum
et al. (2004) disorder
Klein et al. 95% 1–28 years 65/65 Questionnaire to Williams syn­
(1990) Mean age: parents drome
8 years
Hall et al. 3.68% 11 years 261/7093 Interview (child), In the 29 children
(2015) audiometry, with Autistic
tympanometry, spectrum
acoustic reflexes, disorder, 41.4%
otoacoustic had hyperacusis
emissions (OAE)
Myne & – 0–19 years 61 children Interview, Autistic spectrum
Kennedy with hyper­ audiometry, disorder, cere­­
(2017) acusis tympanometry, bral palsy, ADHD,
oto-acoustic glue ear
emissions
Oen et al. 50% 0–6 months 25/50 Interview, Spina bifida
(1997) Control: 19 questionnaire

Rosenhall 18% 8 years 111/57 ABR, audiometry Autistic spectrum


(1999) disorder
Sattar – 0–16 years 100 children Interview, Autistic spectrum
(2009) Peak age with hyper­ audiometry, disorder, ADHD,
3–4 years acusis and no tympanometry, meningitis,
hearing loss oto-acoustic migraine,
emissions perinatal
problems related
to prematurity
Widen & 17.1% 13–19 years 220/1285 Questionnaire Tinnitus
Erlandsson
(2004)

209
210 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

(Mott & Sun, 2014), ADHD (Mangeot et al., old child who reports needing to frequently
2001), and spina bifida (Oen, Begeer, Staal- monitor the clock in class so as not to be sur­
Schreinemachers, & Tijmstra, 1997). Hyper­ prised by the school bell.
acusis has also been reported with problems The child’s reactions to sounds can have
associated with prematurity including cerebral a substantial impact on the child’s activity and
palsy and hydrocephalus (Sattar, 2009; Myne & the activities of the family (Carter, Ben-Sasson,
Kennedy, 2017). & Briggs-Gowan, 2011). Household chores
such as vacuuming or shopping may be de­
ferred until the child is absent. Outings may
be avoided or limited because of the child’s re­
Presenting Symptoms
fusal to use a public bathroom in case someone
switches on the hand dryer. The refusal to use
Most children disturbed by sounds respond a public bathroom can in turn lead to wetting.
by covering their ears for the duration of the The avoidance behavior may influence the
sound. Others can get quite distressed, re­ child’s development of social skills and friend­
port pain in the ears, cry, or run away from ships as playgrounds and cafeterias are avoided
the sound, heedless of danger (Gomes et al., (Stiegler & Davis, 2010; Myne & Kennedy,
2004; Sattar, 2009; Myne & Kennedy, 2017). 2017). In a misguided attempt to help, ear de­
In some cases, a phobic response occurs and fenders are sometimes provided to these chil­
the child seeks to avoid situations in which the dren by parents or teachers (Stiegler & Davis,
distressing sound may be present, often with 2010; Hall et al., 2015; Myne & Kennedy,
marked disruption to their family’s day-to-day 2017) who are unaware of the negative im­
life (Klein et al., 1990; Stiegler & Davis, 2010; pact that this can have on central auditory
Hall et al., 2015; Myne & Kennedy, 2017). gain (see Chapters 6 and 8).
The troublesome sounds include children
shout­ing or screaming, alarms, sirens, hand or
hair dryers, vacuum cleaners, and traffic (Co­
elho et al., 2007; Myne & Kennedy, 2017). Categorizing Hyperacusis
Hall et al. (2015) reported 42.9% of the chil­
in Children
dren with hyperacusis displayed avoidance
behaviors. Similarly, Stansfield (1992) inves­
tigated highly noise-sensitive and low noise- Based on the different presentations of hyper­
sensitive women and observed that individuals acusis in children, we propose the following
who reported greater sensitivity tended to pay categories of possible mechanisms:
more attention to sounds, were more likely to
perceive sounds as threatening, and reported • Immature but normally developing
less control over potentially disturbing situa­ auditory system
tions. There are similarities with findings seen • Temporary auditory deprivation
in the pediatric population, particularly those • Disorder within the auditory system
children with ASD. While the child is gener­ • Disorder of sensory processing
ally not averse to making noise, it is the un­
predictability of a sound made by others or The categories are not exclusive as, in some
the lack of control over a sound that can cause children, there may be a combination of mech­
distress. As an example, consider the 8-year- anisms across categories, for example, hyper­
13. Increased Sound Sensitivity in Children 211

acusis in a child with Williams syndrome and volved with emotional reactions and the amyg­
otitis media with effusion. dala controls the fight or flight response, this
pathway may explain the severity with which
a child sometimes responds to aversive sounds.
Immature but Normally The involvement of these pathways in routine
Developing Auditory System auditory processing diminishes with increas­
ing age through childhood.
Although hyperacusis is commonly seen in
young children, its presence and impact typ­
ically decrease over time. This suggests either Temporary
habituation to the troublesome sounds or an Auditory Deprivation
auditory developmental process that requires
maturation and experience. Moore (2002) re­ Reduced sound input, even from a temporary
ported that myelination of the thalamic fibers cause, can lead to an increase in central audi­
of the thalamocortical auditory system pro­ tory gain as spontaneous and sound-evoked fir­
gresses from ages 1 to 4 years old with fur­ ing rates increase (Pienkowski et al., 2014). In
ther axonal myelination through to ages 5 to children, the most common cause of tempo­
10 years. The maturation process also con­ rary hearing loss is otitis media with effusion.
tributes to improvements in auditory sensi­ While some children and their parents express
tivity thresholds. Fior and Bolzonello (1982) relief if the child is less sensitive to sounds
reported maturation of intensity discrimina­ during a period of active infection, there may
tion through at least 10 years of age. Trehub, be an increase in hyperacusis once the condi­
Schneider, and Endman (1980) reported de­ tion resolves. Increased central auditory gain
velopment as a gradual process throughout may result from pathology-related deprivation
childhood with maturation in high frequency that appears to “turn up” the subjective loud­
regions (4 and 10 kHz) before age 5 years but ness of sounds. Nigam and Samuel (1994)
after 10 years of age for 1 kHz. Another devel­ reported that 47% of children complained of
opmental mechanism may be the maturation hyperacusis after ventilation tubes were inserted
of sensory gating, the process the brain uses to to treat chronic otitis media with effusion. The
inhibit “distracting,” or non-relevant auditory hyperacusis was of varying degrees and lasted
information (Davies, Chang, & Gavin, 2009) between 2 and 40 days. Temporary auditory
which occurs between ages 7 and 13 years. deprivation may also lead to hyperacusis by
During this age range, there is also progressive other mechanisms. Based on a study of young
maturation of the frontal cortex resulting in a rats, Sun et al. (2011) proposed that conduc­
decrease in susceptibility to interference from tive hearing loss may impair sound tolerance by
irrelevant environmental stimuli (Marshall, Bar- reducing GABA-related inhibition in the infe­
Haim, & Fox, 2004). rior colliculus. Therefore, changes in loudness
Møller and Rollins (2001) studied the tolerance may be viewed as a manifestation of
nonclassical auditory pathways involved in reduced central inhibition in addition to the
loudness perception in children. These path­ aforementioned central gain. Deprivation can
ways respond to somatosensory as well as au­ also influence the processing of sounds in
ditory stimuli and project to different regions the auditory brainstem and temporarily lower
of the brain including the limbic system and acoustic reflex thresholds (Formby, Sherlock, &
the amygdala. Because the limbic system is in­ Gold, 2003; Munro, Turtle, & Schaette, 2014).
212 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

that the neurotransmitter serotonin (5HT)


Disorder Within the may be implicated in hyperacusis as this neu­
Auditory System rotransmitter inhibits modulation of central
responses to sensory stimuli. It must also be
In addition to transient changes in hearing noted, however, that both increased and de­
sensitivity, disorders involving the peripheral creased serotonin levels have been noted to
and/or central auditory systems can play a produce a heightened startle response in an­
role in hyperacusis. Peripheral dysfunction in­ imals (Fletcher et al., 2001).
cludes, for example, the high frequency hear­
ing loss seen in children with Williams syn­
drome (Gothelf et al., 2006). In response to Disorder of
the peripheral auditory dysfunction, the cen­ Sensory Processing
tral auditory system processing may change in
ways consistent with increased auditory gain Hyperacusis may also be related to sensory
and/or decreased inhibition. As increased processing disorders involving modalities
central gain is also thought to be one of the other than hearing. Problems in the sensory
mechanisms for tinnitus, such processing gating system, perhaps associated with de­
changes may explain the co-morbidity of layed maturation, can lead to difficulties with
hyperacusis and tinnitus (Coelho et al., 2007; selective inhibition of irrelevant or distracting
Baguley et al., 2013). Other studies have information (Davies et al., 2009). This in turn
found the stapedial reflexes to be deficient or can result in the processing of inappropriate
of reduced amplitude (Klein et al., 1990; Kat­ sensory stimuli or difficulty with integrating
zenell & Segal, 2001; Gothelf et al., 2006), or sensory stimuli. Hall et al. (2017) reported
that hyperacusis may be due to impairment of that many children with hyperacusis also had
the auditory efferent system (Phillips & Carr, other sensory sensitivities. Sensitivity to light/
1998; Attias et al., 2008). colors was noted in 8.4% while sensitivity
Regarding the central auditory system, to touch, smell, taste, and pain were 6.5%,
Matsuzakiet et al. (2012) proposed that the 5.4%, 3.8%, and 3.5%, respectively. Sensory
primary auditory cortex was the focal site processing or modulation disorders have been
promoting sensitivity to sounds in children widely reported in children particularly those
with ASD. Using a magnetoencephalographic with developmental disabilities. Ben-Sasson
assessment, they reported that children diag­ and Carter (2009) noted that sensory over-
nosed with ASD had significantly delayed responsivity (SOR), a sensory modulation
M50/M100 peak latencies suggesting neuro­ disorder manifested by intense behavioral re­
logical immaturity or functional abnormali­ sponses, interfered with routine activities in 5
ties. Funabiki, Murai, and Toichi (2012) used to 16% of school age children. SOR was also
changes in blood oxygenated hemoglobin described in children with a range of neuro­
(OxyHb) measured using fMRI in the pre­ developmental conditions (Adamson, O’Hare,
frontal and temporal cortices to determine and Graham, 2006; Mangeot et al., 2001; Rog­
a hyperacusis locus. They proposed that the ers, Hepburn, & Wehner, 2003). The children
affected areas were not in the auditory but with SOR may experience a “sensory overload”
in the prefrontal region, a finding that could effect in situations where there are a number
account for the selective listening (or inatten­ of sensory stimuli such as the auditory, visual,
tion) to some sounds and sensitivity to oth­ tactile, olfactory stimuli of a children’s party.
ers. Marriage and Barnes (1995) proposed Parents of children with SOR reported many
13. Increased Sound Sensitivity in Children 213

restrictions in family life (Carter et al., 2011). It is also important to note the use of safety
Møller and Rollins (2001) suggested that fail­ behaviors, such as avoidance of situations or
ure of maturation of the non-classical auditory the use of ear defenders, as these are often un­
pathways may account for some of the sensory helpful in the long run. Background informa­
symptoms seen in children with developmen­ tion should include audiological/otological
tal disorders. history, medical history (medical conditions,
developmental disorders), and educational
history (progress, additional academic sup­
port accessed or needed).
Assessment

It is important to conduct a thorough inter­


Audiological Assessment
view with the child and family to elicit the
main reason(s) they are seeking help. The key,
however, is doing this in a manner that en­ Each child should have a full audiological as­
courages the child to engage in the consulta­ sessment to determine their hearing thresh­
tion (Tinnitus in Children Practice Guidance, olds and middle ear status using age- and
2015). Information gathering should include ability-appropriate techniques in a suitable
details about the troublesome noises, the sit­ pediatric setting. Children with hyperacusis
uations the child finds troublesome, reactions may be anxious about testing, so clinicians
of the child and family, and the impact on should be prepared to allow plenty of time.
the child and family. Although there is no Because the audiology test suite may seem
validated hyperacusis questionnaire for chil­ foreign or unnerving, pediatric patients ben­
dren, an estimation of the level of severity can efit from clear instructions and reassurance
be obtained by applying the following de­ to support their confidence regarding the test
scriptors to the response to the troublesome protocol. Creating a friendly environment for
sounds: the child and family should improve the reli­
ability of test results.
• Level 1: Dislike of sounds Determining uncomfortable loudness lev­
• Level 2: Distress caused by sounds els (ULLs) in the assessment of hyperacusis
• Level 3: Fear of sounds with in adults is commonplace; however, in their
anticipatory anxiety or avoidance review, Fackrell, Fearnley, Hoare, and Sereda
of situations where the troublesome (2015) conclude that the evidence for their use
sounds may be present is mixed. The test-retest reliability of ULLs is
questionable in adults (Baguley & Andersson,
It is important to determine whether the 2007), and it is likely to be more so in chil­
child experiences other sensory problems, as dren. Because the management strategies for a
well as to identify coping strategies that have child with hyperacusis are based on the impact
been helpful or unhelpful. If the family mem­ of their symptoms, such as the “distress,” “dis­
bers have already accommodated the child’s like,” and “fear” indicated above, establishing
challenges, then it is important to note the ULLs may add little to the management plan.
degree to which their routine has been re­ It also seems somewhat unfair at best, and
stricted or has changed. It may be relevant to cruel at worst, to ask a child, who has come to
explore the hyperacusis in other settings such the clinic because of sound-provoked distress,
as school or during extracurricular activities. to sit and listen to loud sounds; the role of the
214 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

audiologist relies at least in part on the practi­ defenders, a careful consideration of the child’s
tioner’s ability to build a relationship of trust activities and interests is needed to reduce
with that child. their use while minimizing the child’s anxiety.
However, when hyperacusis is related to very
loud sounds such as class music lessons (Ro­
drigues et al., 2015), school discos, or other
noisy events, advice on noise risk reduction
Management Strategies
should be provided, and, in these situations, it
may be appropriate to use ear defenders.
Following assessment of auditory status and It is important that the strategies set for
the behavioral impacts of hyperacusis, the any child are achievable for that child and fam­
management plan to be implemented will de­ ily and are consistently applied both at home
pend on the age and the developmental stage and at school or nursery. This often involves
and/or cognitive ability of the child as well as a multidisciplinary approach to ensure that
the impact of hyperacusis on the child and all caregivers are using similar strategies. The
family. The initial step should be to provide a aim is to improve a child’s confidence around
clear explanation of hyperacusis. The frame­ the sounds that cause distress and to give the
work of the Child Friendly Tinnitus Model child a sense of control over the situation. In
(Emond & Kentish, 2013) can be adapted to the case of a young child or a child with devel­
hyperacusis and used as a basis for counsel­ opmental delay, it can be particularly helpful
ing and management. For some families, an to involve the child in the activities related
explanation may be all that is needed; this can to the sound, for example, leading the class
include exploring how anxiety and the child’s in starting and stopping a singing activity, or
sound sensitivity may reinforce one another ringing the school bell, as this can help main­
and lead to negative emotions and avoidance tain a sense of control. Another strategy is to
behaviors. Part of the management process is divert the child’s attention from the sound to
to enable the child and/or family to develop something that might be of interest, perhaps
expectations and strategies that are meaning­ the reason for the sound, or a characteristic of
ful to them. Usually reassurances can be given the sound (“That’s the bell for lunch. I won­
that the level of sounds to which the child is der what’s for lunch? What do you think it
sensitive will not harm the child’s hearing. will be?” “Do you think that siren is from an
This can allay both child and parental anxiety. ambulance or police car? Let’s see.”). In sit­
Although the use of ear defenders or ear uations where multiple sensory stimuli can
plugs to reduce the volume of sounds seems be overwhelming, such as a birthday party,
to be commonsense to those seeing a child the child may be helped to focus attention
distressed by loud sounds, this strategy should on something manageable (“Let’s find John
be actively discouraged. Evidence shows that and play with him, can you see him?”). Older
the ensuing auditory deprivation, although children may be able to engage in rationaliz­
well-intentioned, can actually sensitize the au­ ing the impact of sounds, such as the necessity
ditory system (Munro & Blount, 2009). It can for some sounds (e.g., alarms) to be loud even
be difficult to explain to families that a strat­ if they are unpleasant. When possible, engage
egy that offers apparent instant relief to their the child’s cooperation in the development
child is possibly exacerbating the problem. If a of coping strategies that will meaningful and
child has become reliant on the wearing of ear effective for that child; such interactions can
13. Increased Sound Sensitivity in Children 215

sometimes uncover great imaginative skills on option of sound/wideband noise generators


the part of the child. (WNG). Despite the lack of unequivocal sup­
porting evidence, the use of WNG in adults
with hyperacusis is common and often ben­
Relaxation Strategies eficial to the patient. The use of WNGs may
improve the child’s ability to tolerate the prob­
for Children
lematic sounds by modifying auditory system
activity associated with hyperacusis. Formby
Relaxation techniques can support coping et al. (2003) reported improved sound tolerance
strate­gies and enable a child to change their after 2 weeks of exposure to a moderate (50 dB
behavior around the sounds causing distress. SPL) level of noise. The exact mechanisms at
Such techniques can be useful in situations in work in that research were not clear; however,
which these sounds are anticipated as well as to the use of such devices may help improve a
help children to become calm when startled. child’s sense of control over a situation, such as
Relaxation can be a useful adjunct in support­ school, where the option of moving away from
ing desensitization strategies. There are a vari­ the sound source might not possible.
ety of resources that may help develop age or WNGs are worn either at ear level or
ability appropriate relaxation techniques, and behind-the-ear, similar to hearing aids, and
different families will have different preferences are widely available. It is important to fit
as to what suits them. It is important to note them with a large vent, an open ear mold, or
that children and families will need support in with a slim tube, open-fit, as the child needs
the monitoring of using these techniques to access to all external sounds. Binaural fitting
ensure they are successful. Breathing exercises is recommended as the hyperacusis is rarely at­
can be carried out in a variety of situations, tributed to just one ear. It is also important to
and, once learned, can be useful in reducing ensure the child can still hear normal speech
symptoms associated with anxiety. For exam­ while wearing the devices. The volume of the
ple, for younger children, the simple exercise WNG should be set at a just audible level and if
of blowing bubbles can be used, whereas older the child is able to manage it, the volume con­
children can, with practice, learn diaphragm or trol can be activated. There are no published
deep breathing techniques. Again, a variety of guidelines describing how a child should use
resources are available to help with this. the WNG, however, support can be requested
from the audiologist or the companies provid­
ing the devices.
Sound Therapy/Wide
Band Sound Generators
Case Example
A formal desensitization approach can be dif­
ficult for some children and families. Desensi­ James was 6 years old when he first came to
tization may be impaired because of difficulty the clinic; he was having difficulty tolerating
engaging with strategies that address anxiety noise in school. He had a complex medical
or because the child is affected by a substan­ history and high levels of anxiety. After trying
tial developmental or language delay. In such some behavioral techniques, it was decided
cases, clinicians may find it helpful to try the James needed further support as his anxiety
216 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

around school remained difficult to manage. for fears and anxiety in children (Davis et al.,
He was fitted with bilateral WNGs on slim 2011).
tubes, with the wide band noise set at an out­ With children, any intervention approach
put of 55 dB SPL. He wore them all day at must take into consideration developmental
school and found his ability to cope much factors. Typically, younger children require an
improved. In particular, when there was approach focused more upon behaviors and
building work near his classroom, he was still their management than older children who are
able to comfortably take part in lessons. His better able to manage a combined cognitive be­
parents felt that the WNGs gave James a sense havioral approach. Parental support is essential,
that he could do something for himself. This as is the involvement of a child’s school when
helped him manage his hyperacusis and in hyperacusis is experienced in that context. Ba­
turn improved his confidence in situations he guley and Andersson (2007) describe a CBT
found difficult. approach for patients reporting hyperacusis.
This includes identifying safety behaviors, re­
laxation methods, advice regarding sound and
Psychological Approaches to sound levels, graded exposure to everyday
sounds, and goal setting. In CBT, the cognitive
Hyperacusis Management
component involves identifying and challeng­
ing thoughts and responses, for example, that
Management approaches for adults with hyper­ the feared sound is associated with danger.
acusis typically include components of educa­ From a behavioral perspective, specific
tion, sound therapy, and counseling. In a recent fears and phobias are often maintained because
randomized, controlled trial, cognitive behavior of an avoidance of exposure to the feared stim­
therapy (CBT) was found to be a promising in­ uli. As a result, there is no opportunity for a
tervention ( Jüris, Andersson, Larsen, & Ekse­ child to learn that, over time, their fear can
lius, 2014). To date, psychological approaches diminish if they do not avoid or try to escape
to hyperacusis management in children have the sounds, and that the outcomes they dread
not been evaluated, but clinically, both adults often do not come true or are not as terrible
and children with hyperacusis often describe as imagined. Fear-provoked avoidance occurs
similar responses to sound: annoyance, distress, when a patient does not enter a situation at all
fear, sensations of pain, belief that sound expo­ or enters the situation but chooses not to expe­
sure will harm them, and avoidance of sounds rience it fully. Exposure therapy is often con­
and contexts in which offending sounds occur. sidered an important therapeutic ingredient in
Given the similarities in presentation between CBT to address fears and anxiety. Such ther­
adults and children, it is reasonable to assume apy can take many forms but most commonly
that as part of a multidisciplinary approach to involves gradually exposing individuals to a
the management of hyperacusis, CBT will ben­ hierarchy of feared stimuli, starting with the
efit children and adolescents too, particularly less threatening ones and progressing to more
when there is an element of sound-related fear challenging ones until the stimuli are well toler­
or anxiety. CBT has been shown to be effective ated. This approach, employing “small manage­
in reducing a range of psychological difficul­ able steps,” is referred to in self-efficacy training
ties, including chronic pain in children and as “mastery experience” through which a patient
adolescents (Ecclestone, Morley, Wiliams, Yorke, becomes more confident, following small victo­
& Mastroyannopoulou, 2002; Velleman, Stal­ ries, that the aversive situation can be managed
lard, & Richardson, 2010) and as a treatment acceptably. Imaginal exposure combines the use
13. Increased Sound Sensitivity in Children 217

of fear-evoking images and thoughts with in- simple relaxation techniques and other
vivo exposure to actual feared stimuli in real coping strategies, such as identifying
life. In systematic desensitization the exposure and maintaining positive thoughts that
is paired with relaxation or another physiologic can be used when exposed to the feared
state that is incompatible with fear in order to stimuli (sound). Younger children
decrease the fear response. Some children, and/ often rely upon the use of imaginary
or their parents, report apprehension that the characters or “superheroes” to help
troublesome sound will damage hearing, and them manage their anxiety or combat
they cannot tolerate such fear or the pain caused fears during exposure (Bouchard,
by offending sounds. These thoughts can be Mendlowitz, Coles, & Franklin, 2004).
challenged by gradually and carefully expos­ It is essential that relaxation techniques
ing the child to mildly troublesome or feared or other coping strategies are fully
sounds to confront the inaccurate beliefs as the mastered before the child is exposed to
child finds the sounds can be tolerated. As the the troublesome sounds.
child learns to tolerate sounds, it builds con­
Step three: A graded, step-by-step
fidence in the ability to tolerate other sounds
exposure to the sound or feared stimuli.
the child fears or finds painful. This newfound
This may be accomplished first through
self-confidence is empowering and an impor­
imaging the feared stimuli, looking at
tant tool in the process of change.
pictures, listening to the sound on the
In systematic desensitization with chil­
computer at gradually increasing levels
dren, therapy involves a number of steps:
and, finally, in-vivo exposure to the real
sound while the child utilizes relaxation
Step one: Identifying a hierarchy of and other coping strategies to help
feared sounds, with those most easily them manage fear and anxiety. Again,
tolerated at the bottom and those most it is important for the child to be
disliked or feared at the top. Parent, involved wherever possible in drawing
therapist, and child develop this up the steps, starting with ones that
together, and the younger the child, they identify as easily manageable and
the greater the amount of guidance can succeed at.
required. Children often understand
and like this approach when it is
In practice, these techniques can be ex­
presented as a ladder, with the most
tremely effective in cases when sound hyper­
easily tolerated sounds placed on the
sensitivity is combined with a high level of fear
lower rungs, and those most disliked
and anxiety. The exposure technique for use
and feared toward the top. When
with children is described in detail by Bou­
children are asked to rate sounds again
chard et al. (2004). They stress the importance
during the course of treatment or at
of making the treatment fun and playful, es­
the end of treatment, this image also
tablishing a strong rapport with the child, en­
provides a simple and useful pre- and
suring that the child is motivated to undertake
post-treatment measure, with progress
this challenging work, and rewarded for prog­
identified as sounds move down the
ress made. Directly observing a sound exposure
ladder.
gives important information not only about
Step two: Learning relaxation and the child’s response, but also about the par­
coping strategies. Children are taught ent’s behavior; the latter may unintentionally
218 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

convey fear themselves, or reinforce avoidance


behavior. Desensitization can fail if relaxation Peter’s Ladder

or coping strategies are not firmly established;


if the initial goal is too difficult, then the result­ first
appointment
last
appointment

ing failure may make the child reluctant to par­


ticipate further. Progressing through the hier­ School bells 5 5
archy of goals too fast and before each step has
been mastered also risks the child’s anxiety be­
coming overwhelming. Bouchard et al. (2004) Kitchen equipment 4 4

stress that practice and repetition of each step


Lawn mower
are the keys to success. In each situation, the Hand dryer
Hoover 3 3 School bells

child should be able to persevere long enough


that their feared beliefs can be tested out and
evaluated. Finally, it is important to remember 2 2

that there are some sounds such as school bells


that will remain intolerably painful for a child Hand dryer
Lawn mower
1 1
with hyperacusis and that repeated exposure
Kitchen equipment

to these situations would be unethical. In such


0 0 Hoover
cases, alternative ways of supporting the child
must be established. © Medical Illustration RNTNEH 020 3456 5103 Unique Code: 33205

Figure 13–1. Peter’s ladder. Source: © Med­


ical Illustration RNTNEH 020 3456 5103
Unique Code: 33205. Reprinted with permis­
Case Example
sion from University College London Hospitals
NHS Foundation Trust.
Nine-year-old Peter described pain and fear
in response to a range of sounds including
“hoovers,” lawnmowers, kitchen food proces­
sors, and school bells. His mother made sure while his mother hoovered for 5 minutes and
that he was not exposed to these sounds at gradually moving downstairs and finally into
home and at school he wore ear defenders for the room where his mother was “hoovering.”
much of the day, and sometimes avoided going To acknowledge his work passing each step,
to school altogether. If he did hear any of these Peter was given his chosen reward, picture
sounds he would “go into meltdown” because he cards of his favorite football team players. Pe­
believed these sounds would damage his hear­ ter was taught some simple relaxation tech­
ing. Peter and his therapist drew up a ladder or niques and some coping thoughts (“I can do
hierarchy of sounds (Figure 13–1) from those this,” “I’m brave,” “Remember, the sound
that he would find easiest to tolerate to those won’t damage my ears”) were written onto
he found most difficult. Hoovers were placed small cards which he kept in his pocket as a re­
at the bottom of his ladder and we agreed to minder. Peter and his mother practiced every
start with this. The first desensitization goal day, and after two weeks, Peter had not only
was then broken down into smaller steps, with overcome his fear of hoovers, but was able
Peter first looking at the hoover when not to do the hoovering for his mother. Peter and
working, then standing upstairs in the house his mother then went on to apply the same
13. Increased Sound Sensitivity in Children 219

techniques to lawnmowers and kitchen appli­


ances. At school, and with support from his
References
teachers, Peter was able to tolerate the sound
of the school bell without his headphones, pro­ Adamson, A., O’Hare, A., & Graham, C. (2006).
vided he was given notice of when the school Impairments in sensory modulation in children
bell was to go off and he was not standing too with autistic spectrum disorder. Br. J. Occup.
close to it. Ther., 69(8), 57–364.
Anari, M., Axelsson, A., Eliasson, A., & Magnus­
son, L. (1999). Hypersensitivity to sound. Ques­
tionnaire data, audiometry, and classification.
Conclusion Scand. Audiol., 28, 219–230.
Attias, J., Raveh, E., Ben-Naftali, N. F., Zarchi, O.,
Hyperacusis can be distressing for children & Gothelf, D. (2008). Hyperactive auditory
efferent system and lack of acoustic reflexes in
and their families. There is a need for pro­
Williams syndrome. J. Basic Clin. Physiol. Phar-
spective studies in children to explore under­ macol., 19(3–4), 193–207.
lying mechanisms and effective management Attias, J., Zarchi, O., Nageris, B. I., & Laron, Z.
techniques. However, despite the lack of vali­ (2012). Cochlear hearing loss in patients with
dated assessment tools or outcome measures, Laron syndrome. European Archives of Oto-Rhino-
there is a lot that can be done to help children Laryngology, 269(2), 461–466.
who are distressed by sounds. An awareness Baguley, D., & Andersson, G. (2007). Hyper­
of the different categories of hyperacusis can acusis: Mechanisms, diagnosis, and therapies. San
guide discussions on prognosis and manage­ Diego, CA: Plural.
ment plans. Children who have an immature Baguley, D. M., Bartnik, G., Kleinjung, T., Savas­
but normally-developing auditory system are tano, M., & Hough, E. A. (2013). Troublesome
likely to “outgrow” the problem. Those with tinnitus in childhood and adolescence: Data
from expert centres. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhino­
wider sensory issues are likely to have a more
laryngol., 77(2), 248–251.
persistent course and require a multimodal Ben-Sasson, A., Carter, A. S., & Briggs-Gowan, M. J.
approach including sound therapy, behavioral (2009). Sensory over-responsivity in elemen­
and/or desensitization strategies. It is impor­ tary school: Prevalence and social-emotional
tant to have a holistic approach to manage­ correlates. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol., 37(5), 705–
ment, selecting aims that are acceptable and 716.
achievable with a consistent approach to be Bouchard, S., Mendlowitz, S. L., Coles, M.E., &
used across all the child’s environments, such Franklin, M. (2004). Considerations in the use
as home and school. It is also important to of exposure with children. Cognitive and Behav­
engage the child and family in the manage­ ioural Practice, 11, 56–65.
ment process. While children may present British Society of Audiology. (2015). Tinnitus in
children: Practice guidance. Retrieved from http://
with a range of hyperacusis-related problems,
www.thebsa.org.uk/.
after appropriate explanations and guidance,
Carter, A. S., Ben-Sasson, A., & Briggs-Gowan,
it is often the children who come up with M. J. (2011). Sensory over-responsivity, psycho­
the most successful strategies. Being mindful pathology, and family impairment in school-
of the concerns of the child and family and aged children. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psy-
incorporating strategies meaningful to them chiatry., 50(12), 1210–1219.
is important in the development of an effec­ Coelho, C. B., Sanchez, T. G., & Tyler, R. S.
tive management plan. (2007). Hyperacusis, sound annoyance, and
220 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

loudness hypersensitivity in children. Prog. Gothelf, D., Farber, N., Raveh, E., Apter, A., &
Brain Res., 166, 169–178. Attias, J. (2006). Hyperacusis in Williams syn­
Davies, P. L., Chang, W., & Gavin, W. J. (2009). drome: Characteristics and associated neuroau­
Maturation of sensory gating performance in diologic abnormalities. Neurology, 66(3), 390–
children with and without sensory processing 395.
disorders. Int. J. Psychophysiol., 72(2), 187–197. Hall, A., Humphriss, R., Baguley, D. M., Parker,
Davis, E., May, A., & Whiting, S. E. (2011). M., & Steer, C. D. (2016). Prevalence and risk
Evidenced-based treatment of anxiety and pho­ factors for reduced sound tolerance (hyper­
bia in children and adolescents: current status acusis) in children. Int. J. Audiol., 55(3), 135–141.
and effects on the emotional response. Clinical Jastreboff, P. J., & Jastreboff, M. M. (2014). Treat­
Psychology Review, 31592–31602. ments for decreased sound tolerance (hyper­
Ecclestone, C., Morley, S., Wiliams, A., York, L., acusis and misophonia). Semin. Hear., 35(2),
& Mastroyannopoulou, K. (2002). Systematic 105–120.
review of randomised controlled trials of psy­ Jüris, L., Andersson, G., Larsen, H. C., Ekselius, L.
chological therapy for chronic pain in children (2014). Cognitive behaviour therapy for hyper­
and adolescents, with subset meta-analysis for acusis: A randomized controlled trial. Behav.
pain relief. Pain, 99, 157–165. Res. Ther., 54, 30–37. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2014
Emond, A., & Kentish, R. (2013). Tinnitus coun­ .01.001. Epub 2014 Jan 24.
selling with children. Audacity, (2), 26–29. Katzenell, U., & Segal, S. (2001). Hyperacusis:
Fackrell, K., Fearnley, C., Hoare, D. J., Sereda, M. Review and clinical guidelines. Otol. Neurotol.,
(2015). Hyperacusis questionnaire as a tool for 22, 321–327.
measuring hypersensitivity to sound in a tinni­ Khalfa, S., Bruneau, N., Rogé, B., Georgieff, N.,
tus research population. BioMed Research Inter- Veuillet, E., Adrien, J. L. . . . Collet, L. (2004).
national, 290425, 1–12. Increased perception of loudness in autism.
Fior, R., & Bolzonello, P. (1988). An investigation Hearing Research, 198(1–2), 87–92.
on the maturation of hearing abilities in chil­ Klein, A. J., Armstrong, B. L., Greer, M. K., &
dren. Ear and Hearing, 8(6), 347–349. Brown, F. R. III. (1990). Hyperacusis and otitis
Fletcher, P. J., Zoë, F. S., Azampanah, A., & Sills, media in individuals with Williams syndrome.
T. L. (2001). Reduced brain serotonin activity J. Speech Hear. Disord., 55(2), 339–344.
disrupts prepulse inhibition of the acoustic star­ Landälv, D., Malmström, L., & Widén, S. E.
tle reflex: Effects of 5,7-dihydroxytryptamine (2013). Adolescents’ reported hearing symp­
and p-chlorophenylalanine. Neuropsychopharma- toms and attitudes toward loud music. Noise &
cology, 24, 399–409. doi:10.1016/S0893-133X Health, 15(66), 347–354.
(00)00215-3. Lee, H. Y., Byun, J. Y., Park, M. S., & Yeo, S. G.
Formby, C., Sherlock, L. P., & Gold, S. L. (2003). (2013). Effect of aging on the prognosis of
Adaptive plasticity of loudness induced by Bell’s palsy. Otology & Neurotolog., 34(4), 766–
chronic attenuation and enhancement of the 770.
acoustic background. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 114(1), Levitin, D. J., Cole, K., Lincoln, A., & Bellugi, U.
55–58. (2005). Aversion, awareness, and attraction: In­
Funabiki, Y., Murai, T., & Toichi, M. (2012). Cor­ vestigating claims of hyperacusis in the Wil­
tical activation during attention to sound in liams syndrome phenotype. J. Child Psychol.
autism spectrum disorders. Research in Devel- Psychiatry., 46(5), 514–523.
opmental Disabilities, 33(2), 518–524. Mangeot, S. D., Miller, L. J., McIntosh, D. N.,
Gomes, E., Rotta, N. T., Pedroso, F. S., Sleifer, P., & McGrath-Clarke, J., Simon, J., Hagerman, R. J.,
Danesi, M. C. (2004). Auditory hypersensitiv­ & Goldson, E. (2001). Sensory modula­tion
ity in children and teenagers with autistic spec­ dysfunction in children with attention-deficit-
trum disorder. Arq. Neuropsychiatr., 62(3–B), hyperactivity disorder. Dev. Med. Child Neurol.,
797–801. 43(6):, 399–406.
13. Increased Sound Sensitivity in Children 221

Marriage, J., & Barnes, N. M. (1995). Is central Orekhova, E. V., Stroganova, T. A., Prokofyev,
hyperacusis a symptom of 5-HT dysfunction? A. O., Nygren, G., Gillberg, C., & Elam, M.
J. Laryngol. Otol., 109, 915–921. (2008). Sensory gating in young children with
Marshall, P. J., Bar-Haim, Y., & Fox, N. A. autism: Relation to age, IQ, and EEG gamma
(2004). The development of P50 suppression oscillations. Neurosci. Lett., 434(2), 218–223.
in the auditory event-related potential. Int. J. Pienkowski, M., Tyler, R. S., Roncancio, E. R., Jun,
Psychophysiol., 51(2), 135–141. H. J., Brozoski, T., Dauman, N., . . . Moore,
Matsuzaki, J., Kagitani-Shimono, K., Goto, T., B. C. (2014). A review of hyperacusis and fu­
Sanefuji, W., Yamamoto, T., Sakai, S., . . . ture directions: Part II. Measurement, mecha­
Taniike, M. (2012). Differential responses of nisms, and treatment. Am. J. Audiol., 23(4),
primary auditory cortex in autistic spectrum 420–436.
disorder with auditory hypersensitivity. Neuro­ Phillips, D. P., & Carr, M. M. (1998). Disturbances
report, 23(2), 113–118. of loudness perception. J. Am. Acad. Audiol., 9,
Miani, C., Passon, P., Bracale, A. M., Barotti, A., & 371–379.
Panzolli, N. (2001). Treatment of hyperacusis Recker, F., Zaniew, M., Bockenhauer, D., Migli­
in Williams syndrome with bilateral conduc­ etti, N., Bokenkamp, A., Moczulska, A., . . .
tive hearing loss. Eur. Arch Otorhinolaryngol., Ludwig, M. (2015). Characterization of 28
258(7), 341–344. novel patients expands the mutational and
Miller, E. E., Grosberg, B. M., Crystal, S. C., & phenotypic spectrum of Lowe syndrome. Pedi-
Robbins, M. S. (2015). Auditory hallucinations atric Nephrology, 30(6), 931–943.
associated with migraine: Case series and litera­ Remenschneider, A. K., Lookabaugh, S., Ali­
ture review. Cephalalgia, 35(10), 923–930. phas, A., Brodsky, J. R., Devaiah, A. K., Da­
Moore, D. R. (2002). Auditory development and gher, W., . . . Quesnel, A. M. (2014). Oto­
the role of experience. Br. Med. Bull., 63(1), logic outcomes after blast injury: the Boston
171–181. Marathon experience. Otology & Neurotolog.,
Mott, B., & Sun, W. (2014). Firing property of 35(10),1825–1834.
inferior colliculus neurons affected by FMR1 Rodrigues, M. A., Amorim, M., Silva, M. V.,
gene mutation. Journal of Otology, 9(2), 86–90. Neves, P., Sousa, A., & Inacio, O. (2015). Sound
Munro, K. J., & Blount, J. (2009). Adaptive plas­ levels and risk perceptions of music students
ticity in brainstem of adult listeners following during classes. Journal of Toxicology & Environ-
earplug-induced deprivation. Journal of Acous- mental Health Part A, 78(13–14), 825–839.
tical Society of America, 126(2), 568–571. Rogers, S. J., Hepburn, S., & Wehner, E. (2003).
Munro, K. J., Turtle, C., & Schaette, R. (2014). Parent reports of sensory symptoms in tod­
Plasticity and modified loudness following dlers with autism and those with other develop­
short-term unilateral deprivation: Evidence of mental disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord., 33(6),
multiple gain mechanisms within the auditory 631–642.
system. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 135(1), 315–322. Rosenhall, U., Nordin, V., Sandstrom, M., Ahlsen,
Myne, S., & Kennedy, V. (2017). Clinical profile G., & Gillberg, C. (1999). Autism and hearing
of children with hyperacusis. Annual Confer­ loss. J. Autism Dev. Disord., 29(5), 349–357.
ence of British Association of Audiovestibular Sattar, N. (2009). A study of hyperacusis in 100
Physicians, Chesham, England. normally-hearing children. Arch Dis. Child,
Nigam, A., & Samuel. P. R. (1994). Hyperacusis 84(Suppl 1), A98 (G251).
and Williams syndrome. J. Laryngol. Otol., 108(6), Stansfeld, S. A. (1992). Noise, noise sensitivity and
494–496. psychiatric disorder: epidemiological and psy­
Oen, J. M., Begeer, J. H., Staal-Schreinemachers, chophysiological studies. Psychol. Med. Monogr.
A. I., & Tijmstra, T. (1997). Hyperacusis in Suppl., 22, 1–44.
children with spina bifida; a pilot-study. Eur. J. Stiegler, L., & Davis, R. (2010). Understanding
Pediatr. Surg., 7(Suppl.1), 46. sound sensitivity in individuals with autism
222 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

spectrum disorders. Focus on Autism and Other Tkachuk, G. A., Cottrell, C. K., Gibson, J. S.,
Developmental Disabilities, 25(2), 67–75. O’Donnell, F. J., & Holroyd, K. A. (2003).
Sun, W., Manohar, S., Jayaram, A., Kumaraguru, A., Factors associated with migraine-related quality
Fu, Q., Li, J., & Allman, B. (2011). Early age of life and disability in adolescents: A prelimi­
conductive hearing loss causes audiogenic sei­ nary investigation. Headache, 43(9), 950–955.
zure and hyperacusis behaviour. Hearing Re- Trehub, S. E., Schneider, B. A., & Endman, M.
search, 282(1–2), 178–183. (1980). Developmental changes in infants’
Thabet, E. M. (2014). Ocular vestibular evoked sensitivity to octave-band noises. J. Exp. Child
myogenic potentials n10 response in autism Psychol., 29, 282–293.
spectrum disorders children with auditory hy­ Velleman, S., Stallard, P., & Richardson, T. (2010).
persensitivity: an indicator of semicircular canal A review and mata-analysis of computrized cog­
dehiscence. Eur. Arch Otorhinolaryngol., 271(5), nitive behaviour therapy for the treatment of
1283–1288. pain in children and adolescents. Child: Care,
Tibussek, D., Wohlrab, G., Boltshauser, E., & Health and Development 36, 4, 465–472.
Schmitt, B. (2006). Proven startle-provoked Widen, S. E., & Erlandsson, S. I. (2004). Self-
epileptic seizures in childhood. Semiologic and reported tinnitus and noise sensitivity among
Electrophysiologic Variability Epilepsia, 47(6), adolescents in Sweden. Noise Health, 7, 29–40.
1050–1058.
14
Hearing Aids for Decreased
Sound Tolerance and
Minimal Hearing Loss:
Gain Without Pain
Grant D. Searchfield and Caroline Selvaratnam

ing loss, and how compression processing in


Introduction
hearing aids can compensate for this (Dillon,
1996; Kates, 2010). Recruitment is not the
Hearing loss accompanied by decreased sound focus of the chapter; instead we concentrate
tolerance (DST) can be managed by a patient- on hearing aid fitting with minimal hearing
centered approach to provide hearing aid “gain loss and DST related to lowered discomfort
without pain.” Compared to the typical hear- levels or intolerance of sound. Throughout
ing aid fitting protocol, several additional steps this chapter we will address our rationale with
are needed to fit hearing aids to the average reference to existing literature where pos-
DST patient. For patients with hearing loss, sible, but given the limited evidence in this
tinnitus, and DST, the first step is to manage field many of our recommendations should
the DST, then the hearing loss, and then tin- be viewed as opinion that is based upon an
nitus. Fitting hearing aids to this population is accumulation of anecdotal and clinical expe-
an iterative process that requires patience and a rience. We will present several case studies to
willingness to take small backward steps when illustrate our philosophy in managing DST.
needed to secure long-term goals. Much of the past focus on minimal and
Elsewhere in this book the types of DST mild hearing loss has been in the pediatric
and potential mechanisms have been de- population because of concerns on its effects
scribed. Audiologists will be familiar with the on language development (McKay, Gravel,
issue of auditory recruitment, the abnormal & Tharpe, 2008). In adults, mild hearing
loudness growth that often accompanies hear- loss can lead to poorer quality of life (Dalten

223
224 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

et al., 2003). Even mild hearing loss influences the person with DST to tackle their sensitiv-
the ability to filter sounds (Baker & Rosen, ities and fears.
2002; Moore & Glasberg, 2011), detect gaps
(Leigh-Paffenroth & Elangovan, 2011; Neijen-
huis, Tschur, & Snik, 2004), and localize sounds
(Goverts, Houtgast, & van Beek, 2000), thereby A Protocol for Fitting
creating issues impairing hearing in back-
Minimal-Mild Hearing Loss
ground noise (Helfer & Wilber, 1990; Pekkar-
Accompanied by DST
inen, Salmivalli, & Suonpaa, 1990). There is
concern that even mild hearing loss can con-
tribute to deficits in cognition that occur with Our philosophy to manage hearing loss and
aging (Tun, McCoy, & Wingfield, 2009; Stew- tinnitus with DST has not changed a great
art & Wingfield, 2009). Increasing evidence deal over the last decade (Searchfield 2006,
also suggests that changes in hearing can occur 2016). The overarching principle is to reduce
without noticeable changes in the audiogram; an individual’s sense of sound isolation cre-
such hidden hearing loss appears to arise from ated either by avoidance behaviors or the pres-
damage to auditory nerve synapses and fibers ence of hearing loss. We address this principle
(Liberman & Kujawa, 2014), and leads to through counseling and the fitting of hear-
poorer temporal coding of sound and possibly ing aids. In order to accommodate reduced
DST (Plack, Barker, & Prendergast, 2014). loudness tolerance, extra caution must be
In addition to DST, tinnitus is a common applied in selecting appropriate hearing aids
co-morbidity accompanying minimal hearing and their settings. Optimal amplification of
loss. There is overlap in the presentation of speech sounds may have to be sacrificed ini-
DST and tinnitus, suggesting some common tially with preference given instead to comfort
mechanisms (Schecklmann, Landgrebe, Lang- and amplification of soft sounds. If necessary,
guth, & Group, 2014) (see Chapters 5, 6, and gain is reduced across inputs and is gradually
8). Similar to tinnitus, DST is associated with increased as sound tolerance permits. When
hearing loss (Nelson & Chen, 2004) although a patient cannot tolerate any amplification,
it can also occur with normal hearing or mini- combination instruments can be used; in such
mal reduction in audiometric thresholds (Gu, cases, low level broadband sound may be used
Halpin, Nam, Levine, & Melcher, 2010). One for desensitization (Sandlin & Olsson, 1999),
common approach to managing tinnitus is the gradually withdrawn, and eventually replaced
fitting of hearing aids (Shekhawat, Searchfield, by amplified sounds. The fitting has to be a
& Stinear, 2013), and in the last decade we progressive, but measured, process that takes
have reported a great deal regarding the ways the individual’s needs into account. It is rec-
in which hearing aids can be best used for tin- ommended that the aids are first used in quiet
nitus management (Searchfield, 2015). DST environments, with exposure increasing over
presents an even greater challenge to hearing time (Herraiz & Diges, 2010).
aid fitting. Another layer of complexity is cre-
ated by the audiological goal of amplifying
sound as it conflicts with patients’ beliefs that
less sound is needed. We have the technology A Step-Wise Approach
to address these conflicting goals, but the pa-
tient must buy-in to a process that they may We employ an adaptation-based protocol in
fear. Counseling is very important to enable which the individual’s needs are assessed and a
14. Hearing Aids for Decreased Sound Tolerance and Minimal Hearing Loss 225

management plan created that varies accord- iii. Is it always the same sound?
ing to the person’s progress. It usually consists iv. Do you have an idea why these sounds
of the following elements: cause discomfort?
v. Is it only when that sound is very loud or
• Clinical history taking does it make you feel uncomfortable even
• DST assessment if it is not loud?
• Needs assessment, goal setting, and
counseling These questions enable us to evaluate sound
• Weaning from earplugs related effects for tuning of hearing aids. In
• Hearing aid selection particular we want to know if the effects are
• Fitting primarily related to the loudness of sounds, or
• Transition to tolerance if the DST is more related to intolerance or
annoyance to sounds (whether loud or not)
(Tyler et al., 2014).
1. Clinical History Taking
C. Situations and environments—psychosocial
Along with standard audiological history tak- context
ing, it is important to understand the DST
problem and how it affects the person in as i. Where are you when the discomfort
much fine-grain detail as possible. Here are occurs?
some examples of questions to ask and how ii. Does the same sound always cause dis­
they might inform the hearing aid fitting pro- comfort?
cess accompanying DST. iii. Does the problem occur at a particular
time of day?
A. Reaction effect iv. How do your partner/family/friends re-
act to the same sound?
i. What effect does the sound have on you? v. Are they supportive?
ii. Is it painful or irritating? vi. Does it change if you are busy or having
iii. Does the effect persist even after the sound fun?
stops? vii. Does it change if you are experiencing
stress?
With these questions we provide an opportu-
nity for the patient to express details regarding In a family-centered context these questions
their DST. We hope to gain an understanding are also important for the patients’ significant
of effects and how quickly any management others to answer. Clinicians need to know the
plan might proceed. A longer intervention effects that the DST has on family and friends
time course might be planned when DST ef- as they try to determine the degree to which
fects are dramatic. The nature of the impact the complaint is sensory versus behavioral.
should assist in decisions on referrals to other When there is evidence of a strong psycho-
professions (e.g., psychology). logical component to the DST, counseling
takes on a greater importance than it would
B. The sounds causing discomfort in less complicated cases. As with tinnitus,
audiologists need to establish when psycho-
i. Do all sounds cause discomfort? logical referral is required to either augment
ii. What are the specific sounds? or substitute for audiology-based counseling.
226 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

D. Coping strategy 2. DST Assessment

i. What do you do when the sounds occur? While some researchers have reported posi-
ii. Do you wear earplugs or earmuffs? How tive correlations between loudness discomfort
often? measures and hyperacusis complaint (Gold-
iii. Do you limit yourself or your activities stein & Shulman, 1996) others have not
socially, and has this changed? (Meeus, Spaepen, de Ridder, & van de Heyning,
2009). Methods of obtaining loudness mea-
It is important to identify existing coping sures and their description vary (Mueller &
strategies, and then provide expert advice Bentler, 2005). Terms used for the intensity of
regarding whether they are helpful for long- sound that patients report as being too loud
term reductions in DST and hearing aid ac- or creating discomfort include: Threshold of
ceptance. We should recognize and work with Discomfort, Loudness Discomfort Level (LDL),
the strategies the person has developed; if they and Uncomfortable Loudness Level (UCL;
are maladaptive and unhelpful for recovery of Mueller & Bentler, 2005). In this chapter we
sound tolerance then the clinician must find will use UCL to encapsulate all these mea-
a gentle and preferably evidence-based way sures. The overall RMS of the signal appears
of turning behaviors in a positive direction. to determine the UCL rather than its spec-
For example, lecturing a patient who is reli- trum, but that being said UCL at 750, 1500,
ant on earmuffs that they must immediately and 3000 Hz may be the most appropriate
stop using them will likely create a barrier to frequencies to set hearing aid maximum out-
further progress. Instead the effects of sensory put to a pure-tone sweep (Bentler & Nelson,
deprivation need to be explained and support 2001) or 1500 Hz for NBN (Keidser, Bentler,
provided to “wean” patients from their per- & Kiessling, 2010).
ceived, but often inaccurate need to use hear- Keidser et al. (2010) importantly noted
ing protection. that patients react differently to laboratory
stimuli compared to real-world sounds. In
E. Pleasant sounds a similar manner to the context of tinnitus
(Searchfield, 2014), sound, personality, and
i. What are your favorite sounds? mood are all likely contributors to discomfort
ii. Do you listen to music? (Keidser et al., 2010). Some clinicians are very
iii. Is your work/home environment quiet? reluctant to measure UCL due to the fear that
iv. How easy would it be to introduce enjoy- it may distress the patient and alienate them
able sounds to your lifestyle? from the clinician. We believe that the benefits
of obtaining the measure outweigh the risks
The goal here is to identify sounds that may (see hearing aid fitting), however, we approach
be used in the rehabilitation process. The pa- these measurements with great caution and
tient is encouraged to increase exposure to forewarning of the process. We always begin
“enjoyable” sounds. The spectral differences near threshold of the test signal, very gradu-
between tolerable and intolerable sounds can ally increase the sound, and stop immediately
be estimated to indicate the contribution of when indicated by patient feedback. Testing
frequency-specific sensitivity to the DST. With is not continued if the patient is distressed.
multiple channel hearing aids, it is possible to UCLs are not measured if the patient finds
alter level-dependent gain at DST frequencies. standard audiometry uncomfortable.
14. Hearing Aids for Decreased Sound Tolerance and Minimal Hearing Loss 227

A different measure of the effects of noise signal:noise is important for management of


is the Acceptable Noise Level (ANL) (Na- both APD and DST as we describe below.
belek, Freyaldenhoven, Tampas, Burchfiel, & There are several questionnaire-based in-
Muenchen, 2006). The ANL is the difference dices of hyperacusis’s effects on quality of life
(in dB) between the most comfortable level (Fackrell, Fearnley, Hoare, & Sereda, 2015;
(MCL) of a speech stimulus and the highest Khalfa et al., 2002). These questionnaires tend
background noise level (BNL) acceptable to not to have been psychometrically validated to
the patient listening to the speech. One ver- the same extent as their tinnitus equivalents.
sion of the ANL is available commercially The problem with all questionnaires is their
through Frye Electronics (http://www.frye ability to be relevant to the individual under-
.com/wp/anl/). The ANL is not a typical DST going assessment. For identifying the needs of
measure as it relates less to loudness discom- patients and measuring the outcomes of DST
fort than to the annoyance of sounds (Bees, therapy in our clinic (see also Chapter 4) we
2016). Patients who are more accepting of tend to prefer a version of the Client Orien-
background noise have smaller ANLs (their tated Scale of Improvement (COSI) adapted
MCL and BNLs are closer) than those less for delineation of the patient’s objectives as-
accepting of noise. In a small group, Levy sociated with tinnitus and/or DST (Dillon,
et al. (2011) found ANLs were larger (less James, & Ginis, 1987).
acceptance) in a group with various forms of
DST than in a normal hearing group (Levy, 3. Needs Assessment, Goal
Peck, & Balachandran, 2011). Importantly, Setting, and Counseling
in the context of this chapter, the ANL can
predict success with hearing aids, as those Due to the heterogeneous presentation of
more accepting of noise are more likely to be DST, identifying individual’s needs and then
successful hearing aid users (Nabelek et al., turning those needs into goals for therapy is
2006). very important. The modified COSI can ad-
There is some preliminary evidence, dress both patient needs and help the clinician
in children, that the ANL is related to some set realistic goals for therapy. Based on the his-
measures used in the assessment of auditory tory we ask the patient their main problems, we
processing disorder (APD) (Sheehan, Hough- list those, and then seek to place them in or-
ton, & Searchfield, 2016). We have also found der of priority. This is a two-way process (and
that many persons referred to our clinic for will be more comprehensive if family can be
DST also have symptoms of APD and fail encouraged to participate). The patient’s goal
some APD tests. We now routinely under- priorities can be used to create an “effect-
take APD testing when patients present with timeline” whereby the clinician can go through
DST. Their complaints of intolerance to sound each goal and explain which is achievable first
seem related to an inability to cope in social and why. New goals can emanate from this
environments when surrounded by compet- conversation and should be noted.
ing sound. A common description is that their Counseling should be undertaken when-
brain “crashes” and they need to escape from ever hearing aids are fitted, but the need is
the sound. The division between DST and heightened with DST. Psychoeducation, or the
APD can be blurred so a differential diagnosis use of psychological therapies, is very impor­
to understand the full nature of the patient’s tant in this process. Materials developed for tin-
complaint is worthwhile. Improvement in nitus have been modified for DST, for example,
228 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

Hyperacusis Activities Treatment (Pienkowski tients, Vernon (2002) instructed use of a


et al., 2014) emerged from Tinnitus Activities sound-level-meter (SLM) to determine when
Treatment (Tyler, Gogel, & Gehringer, 2007). sound was damaging, and only use of ear-
Detailed counseling suggestions are made plugs when it was (Vernon, 2002). SLM apps
elsewhere in this book. To be successful the pa- are now available for smartphones (many ex-
tient must trust the clinician, who is essentially amples are available from iTunes and google
recommending an intervention that patients play). If used appropriately, patients can chal-
fear and that they may see as the opposite from lenge themselves to experience higher levels
their needs (i.e., increasing sound not decreas- of sound, but care needs to be taken so that
ing it). Encourage patients to become familiar obsessive use of SLMs does not replace one
with dangerous versus safe sound levels (http:// maladaptive coping strategy with another.
dangerousdecibels.org/). Consistency in patient-
clinician contact is extremely important; ide- B. Nonlinear or active earplugs
ally the patient will see the same clinician over
many visits, and failing this a consistent mes- The use of nonlinear or active plugs to pro-
sage and protocol should be used within a clinic vide level-dependent attenuation can assist
to avoid confusion and doubt. Patients need to weaning off earplugs. These earplugs provide
be able to understand how hearing aids work, greater attenuation as sound levels increase
identify the sounds and situations that are most (Dancer & Hamery, 1998), they are often
troublesome, develop strategies to manage the marketed for shooters or motorbike riders and
challenging situations as they take responsibil- consist of a small acoustic filter in a silicon
ity for their own rehabilitation. It is important plug either available off-the-shelf or in cus-
for the clinician to understand the psycholog- tom versions. More expensive electronic active
ical state of the patient and learn their exist- earplugs are also available (e.g., http://www
ing coping strategies in order to create a safe .etymotic.com/consumer/hearing-protection
environment for the reintroduction of sound .html). Patients feel safer being provided with a
(Westcott, 2002). substitute for their usual earplugs, given these
provide primarily high intensity attenuation
they are an interim step toward recovery of
4. Wean from Earplugs sound sensitivity.

Some DST patients develop an unhealthy de­ C. Reduce hours of use


pendence on hearing protection. A not too
un­common sight in our waiting room is a per­ Each increase in hours experienced without
son reading a magazine while wearing earmuffs. protection should be seen as an achievement
Reducing this reliance on ear protection must and celebrated with the patient.
be managed with empathy, it should not be
rushed; overcoming this barrier to adaptation
is a key to future success. 5. Hearing Aid Selection

A. Instruct appropriate use of protection Special hearing aids for DST featuring low
compression thresholds, low maximum out-
Hearing protection use is appropriate when puts, and no venting were available in the
sound is damaging. Patients need to be given 1990s (Preves, Sammeth, Cutting, & Wood-
the tools to understand this. For some pa- ruff, 1995). These aids were primarily active
14. Hearing Aids for Decreased Sound Tolerance and Minimal Hearing Loss 229

electronic earplugs. They only provided suf- be raised and expansion should be activated.
ficient amplification to reduce insertion-loss Expansion reduces the gain below the com-
created by plugging the ear. These aids would pression knee point; greater expansion results
be of minimal benefit in correcting for hear- in less soft sound. The dynamic characteristics
ing loss, and no longer appear to be available; of compression desirable for DST would in-
however, the ideal hearing aid for DST would clude a fast attack-time (so that the aids react
have a similar range of compression thresh- quickly to increases in sound) that must be bal-
olds and flexibility in selection of maximum anced against Dillon’s (2012) suggestion that
output. Such settings would enable the user moderate release times provide more comfort.
to begin using the aid at levels of amplifica-
tion that, while suboptimal for speech recog- B. Noise Reduction
nition, could provide “safe” amplification as
patients transition to normal prescribed tar- Automatic noise reduction is unlikely to im-
gets. Here we list some of the features we pre- prove speech intelligibility but it can improve
fer in hearing aids for DST, and provide brief comfort and reduce listening effort (Dillon,
rationales. 2012). Initially, high levels of noise reduction
may be needed for DST. Noise reduction has
A. Flexible output and compression settings been shown to improve the ANL by 1.1 dB
(Wu & Stangl, 2013). As sound tolerance im-
The maximum output (maximum power out- proves, noise reduction can be reduced. Less
put [MPO] or output sound pressure level noise reduction should result in better sound
[OSPL]) is the highest level that the hearing quality and environmental awareness.
aid can produce. Output-controlled compres-
sion is designed so that a compression thresh- C. Directional and remote microphones
old below the patient’s UCL should prevent
discomfort. Appropriate output limiting is an Improving the signal-to-noise ratio through
important determinate of hearing aid success directional microphone settings, and especially
in general (Mueller & Bentler, 2005), and wireless microphones close to the sound of in-
even more so when an individual’s UCLs are terest, will improve effective communication.
lower than normal. But it should not be as- When combined with listening strategies, for
sumed that lowering the maximum output is example, the patient orienting their attention
the best or only solution. It is important to away from noise sources, use of wireless mi-
remember that manipulations to maximum crophones will reduce the influence of back-
output will only affect high-level sounds. DST ground noise relative to that of the signal.
may occur to sounds lower than this setting Wireless microphones become even more im-
can control. Wide dynamic range compres- portant when DST accompanies APD. Direc-
sion (WDRC) is an input compression scheme tional microphone settings improve the ANL
with low compression threshold (40 to 50 dB by 2.8 dB (Wu & Stangl, 2013).
SPL) and low compression ratios (1.5 to 3:1).
WDRC reduces gain as sound level increases. D. Transient impulse control
It is important to note that WDRC is designed
to provide gain to soft sounds and increases Transient impulse control is available in some
(worsens) the ANL by 1.5 dB (Wu & Stangl, hearing aids; this feature detects sounds that
2013). If soft sounds are a problem for the lis- increase more rapidly in intensity than speech,
tener, then the compression threshold should and does not amplify them (Hayes, 2006).
230 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

E. Datalogging purpose, and there are websites with material


suitable for desensitization (e.g., www.tinnitus
Datalogging does not directly change the tunes.com).
sound perceived by persons with DST. In-
stead, datalogging provides an insight to the H. Volume Control
use patterns of the aids. The information ob-
tained through datalogging can guide adjust- The volume control is a simple feature that has
ments in fitting and supplement the patient’s become somewhat sidelined in today’s world
self-report of hearing aid use. of automatic adjustment; however, a volume
control on the aids or remote can empower the
F. Domes versus earmolds user. If the audiologist is concerned that the
user will just turn the volume to minimum and
Persons with minimal-mild hearing loss should leave it there, avoiding sound, compliance can
be able to use open-fit aids. This style of aid be checked with datalogging. A volume con-
will minimize occlusion; however, they allow trol minimizes the likelihood of patients hav-
essentially unimpeded passage of environmen- ing to persevere with uncomfortable sounds.
tal sounds. Persons accustomed to earplugs
may be best served by use of custom earmolds, I. Programs
and then progress to domes with time. Select-
A-Vent variable diameter venting can be used Most manufacturers offer multiple memories
to block and then progressively open the vents in their aids that are designed for different pur-
to allow more environment sound to pass into poses (e.g., listening in quiet, comfort, music).
the ear. From open ear molds the user can tran- Different prescriptions of amplification can be
sition to open-fit domes. trialed in different programs. A winning pre-
scription can be determined on the basis of
G. Combi devices and Bluetooth self-report and datalogging. We also suggest
that a program be created that has several de-
If hearing aids with low amplification settings grees less amplification than the standard pro-
are not tolerated, then it is best that the focus on gram. This so-called “safe program” is intended
restoring audibility be delayed until acceptable for loud environments where conventionally
sound tolerance is achieved. Use of constant amplified ambient sounds would be intolera-
low-level broadband noise can be beneficial for ble. The safe program would include most of
those patients who fear unexpected amplified the features described above such as: strong
sounds. Combination devices are ideal for pro- noise reduction, expansion, a high compression
viding such desensitizing sound as the sound ratio, directionality, and low maximum out-
generator can be phased out with amplifica- put. This setting would serve as the emergency
tion introduced in time. This strategy avoids program to enable the user to feel safe while
the expense of purchasing a dedicated sound providing some environmental awareness and
therapy device and then changing to hearing sound stimulation. The safe program provides
aids. Bluetooth-featured hearing aids allow the more gain than the next recommendation.
users to stream music or other sounds from
their portable music players or smartphones J. Off button
direct to the aids. Many hearing aid manufac-
turers have mobile applications (apps) for tin- The reason and use of this should be self-
nitus sound therapy that can be used for this explanatory.
14. Hearing Aids for Decreased Sound Tolerance and Minimal Hearing Loss 231

6. Fitting suggest different targets for soft-loud sounds


for different hearing losses (Figure 14–1).
We use hearing aid prescriptions and real-ear The estimated UCLs based on the audiogram
measures to inform our fittings for patients may be grossly inappropriate in cases of DST.
with DST. However unlike normal hearing The prescriptions allow for the substitution
aid fittings, meeting targets created by the of measured UCLs instead of average values.
prescription is not the initial goal, it is the We believe that the measurement of UCLs is
long-term goal. It is important to start with invaluable for this purpose, although as stated
a clear strategy to select and verify initial set- previously their measurement must be under-
tings for target gain and output. Hearing aids taken with caution to avoid discomfort.
use level-dependent amplification strategies Hearing aid fitting based on loudness
based on prescription of gain to make speech growth measures have also been used in the
audible and maximum output to avoid dis- past (e.g., LGOB; Allen, Hall, & Jeng, 1990)
comfort (Dillon, 1996; Kates, 2010). Com- but time pressures in clinic and the putative
pression is used to compensate for recruit- adequacy of threshold based estimations of
ment (Dillon, 1996; Kates, 2010). Hearing UCLs means threshold-based prescriptions
prescriptive procedures (e.g., NAL-NL1; Byrne, dominate audiology. In the DST population,
Dillon, Ching, Katsch, & Keidser, 2001; loudness growth measures can assist in iden-
NL2; Keidser, Dillon, Flax, Ching, & Brewer, tifying dynamic range, modify the prescrip-
2011; DSL V5; Polonenko et al., 2010) and tion of the maximum output levels, and assist
their implementation in hearing aids are well the clinician in selecting reasonable estimates
established and intended to manage the loss of suitable gain for soft, medium, and loud
of sensitivity to low intensity sounds in pa- inputs. Aided loudness validation (such as
tients with normal UCLs. Compression and RESR) may be more valuable than unaided
its benefit in cases of recruitment is covered UCL measures (Mackersie, 2007), however
in many standard hearing aid textbooks (Dil- any measurements undertaken for a person
lon, 2012). with DST must either consider a priori the
Prescription procedures and default set- patient’s UCL or start the measurements at
tings in hearing aids have not been specifi- a low level with gradual increases. The clini-
cally developed for the less-frequent situation cian must be able to stop measurements rap-
of abnormal discomfort levels or annoyance idly to avoid discomfort and risk alienation of
of sounds. To achieve the goals of hearing aid the patient. If the client has adverse reaction
fitting with DST, the clinician must under- to even low-level sounds, it is possible to use
stand the technology and how to find “work- coupler measures incorporating RECD mea-
arounds” to achieve the fitting goals. Prescrip- sures as a substitute for UCLs (Dillon, 2012).
tive procedures utilize target gain and UCL Audiologists need to be careful when us-
values derived from average predictions of ing the manufacturer’s software to adjust anti-
ideal settings based on the audiogram (Storey, feedback features. Feedback tests can cre-
Dillon, Yeend, & Wigney, 1998). Verification ate SPLs in the ear canal of 110 to 115 dB
occurs through real-ear measures using dif- (Sankowsky-Rothe, Blau, Schepker, & Doclo,
ferent intensities of sound (Dillon, 2012). As 2015), far in excess of what many people with
DST invariably results in lower preferred lev- DST will accept. If feedback occurs with the
els of amplification than would be typical, the aids (the feedback can be uncomfortable in
prescribed targets become end goals rather its own right), audiologists should try manual
than initial targets. The different prescriptions adjustment of the manufacturer’s feedback
Frequency (Hz)
250 500 1000 1500 2000 3000 4000 6000 8000
0

20

Threshold (dB HL)


40

60

80

100

120

Figure 14–1. A. Audiogram from right ear of a patient. B. SPL-O-


gram, audiogram thresholds converted to SPL, UCLs estimated for
the audiogram using NAL-Nl1, NAL-NL2, and DSL v 5 Adult, and
measured UCL. C. Input-output function for the audiogram (4 kHz)
based on the three prescription procedures, the UCL for the pre-
scriptions based on the audiogram (same for all three), and the ac-
tual UCL. The prescriptions provide varying targets depending on in-
put. The average UCL estimate by the different prescriptions is very
similar, but much higher than the actual UCL in this case. The RESR
is the real ear saturation response for the example hearing aid.
Table 14–1. Recommended changes to address patient complaints related to comfort.

Problem Description
Background Voices too Booming Too loud Sound Dishes Loud Sounds
noise too loud painful too loud sounds uncomfortable
loud too loud
Potential Solultion
1 Decrease Decrease Decrease Decrease Decrease Increase Decrease Decrease

233
LF gain overall gain LF gain overall gain MPO HF CR MPO MPO
2 Increase Decrease Decrease Decrease Increase Decrease Increase Decrease
LF CR LF gain overall gain LF gain HF CR MPO HF CR overall gain
3 Raise Increase Decrease Decrease Decrease Decrease Increase
LF CT LF CR MPO MPO overall gain HF gain LD CR
4 Decrease
overall gain
Source: Based on Jenstad, Van Tasell, & Ewert (2003).
234 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

settings, or traditional methods of reducing very low maximum output, high amounts of
feedback such as less venting, or softer and compression, and low gain, the hearing aid
tighter earmolds. We would strongly recom- will provide a narrowed dynamic range that
mend not performing these feedback tests un- may create distortion and minimal benefits
til the users tolerance to sound has normalized, in hearing (Formby et al., 2015). This very
and even then performing the tests should be conservative setting of hearing aids must be
a last resort; be ready to hit the pause button. considered temporary and will need to be
Multichannel hearing aids have many param- modified as rapidly as the individual’s toler-
eters that can be adjusted to improve comfort ance will allow. The limitation of an initial
(Table 14–1). conservative fitting strategy on the audibility
of speech needs to be clearly articulated to
7. Transition to Tolerance patients. They need to understand the initial
setting is just a stepping-stone to more amplifi-
Counseling is critical for enabling the person cation. If the patient does not have a clear un-
with DST to develop improved tolerance to derstanding of the process and/or the benefit
sound. Clinicians can center their approach of audibility at far less than optimum levels,
around supporting goals established with then the patient may reject the hearing aids.
the COSI. In addition to increasing passive
sound exposure through generated and am-
plified sound there is also emerging evidence
that auditory training used with tinnitus Case Examples
(Searchfield, Morrison-Low, & Wise, 2007)
may improve acceptance of noise as measured
Case One (M)
by the ANL. Following a 3-week sound iden-
tification and localization task (“AOIL” avail-
able commercially from www.tinnitustunes. This 10-year-old child presented with severe
com) Morrison-Low (2006) found a 2.4 dB reaction and distress to sound (curling into a
decrease (improvement) in the ANL. A cross- ball on the ground and bursting into tears).
over study comparing LDLs and ANLs mea- The problem started one year prior to testing
sured in 40 participants with normal hear­ as a result of girls in his class screaming in his
ing, prior to and after 15 days of training and ear, and initially his strong reaction was only
15 days of no training (control) also found a observed when he heard sounds that would
significant training-specific decrease (improve­ be considered potentially damaging to his
ment) in ANLs, but no change in LDLs was hearing. Over the space of a year, M started to
observed (Bees, Guan, Alsarrage, & Search- become defensive to sound both within and
field, 2017). outside the classroom and was reacting aver-
sively to sounds that others did not consider
loud. There were no concerns with regards to
hearing with M’s mother reporting that he
Risk of Conservative had always exhibited “super” hearing for soft
sounds. In the classroom M had struggled
Hearing Aid Fitting
learning to read and write.
The assessment process encompassed both
Under-amplification carries risks of rejection loudness tolerance measures and APD tests.
due to the absence of benefit to hearing. With Thresholds were within normal limits bilater-
14. Hearing Aids for Decreased Sound Tolerance and Minimal Hearing Loss 235

ally although there was a notch in otoacoustic Speech needed to be whispered and the radio/
emissions at 4000 Hz, bilaterally. M became TV were set to a level that was below audi-
very distressed when asked to do loudness bility for everyone else. G did work part time
tolerance testing and as such his UCLs were in the local fire station doing data logging but
quite limited (20 dB above threshold). Most could only cope with this if the station was
comfortable levels (MCL) to sentences were empty and there were no sirens. He was wear-
substantially higher (70 dB above threshold), ing off-the-shelf earplugs when he was exposed
the UCL/MCL difference indicative of a sig- to car noise but not in quiet settings.
nificant emotional response to sound. In APD Testing revealed a mild high frequency
testing M exhibited delays in his ability to pro- sensorineural loss in both ears with UCL’s
cess temporal and tonal aspects of sound. 30 dBHL above threshold for low frequency
Dangerous decibel (http://dangerous sounds and 20 dBHL above threshold for high
decibels.org/) counseling was carried out with frequency sounds. APD testing was not com-
M and contact was made with the school to pleted because the neuropsychological assess-
recommend that they participate in a danger- ment had already confirmed a significant cog-
ous decibel program. The recommendation nitive impairment.
was based on the school’s suggestion that M Nonlinear plugs replaced the off-the-shelf
ignore the behavior of his classmates rather ear defenders for use in noisy settings and G
than treating the incidents as an assault. Sound was counseled regarding the impact of con-
files were provided for use in quiet settings to sistent exposure to environments with lim-
reduce his awareness of external sounds, and ited sound. Bilateral open fit mini BTE aids
by doing so facilitate control over his antici- were fitted for use in quiet environments with
patory behavior. An APD program was also the long-term goal being consistent hearing
designed to help develop M’s temporal and aid use in all settings. Noise suppression was
tonal processing. set to maximum and given his visual issues,
M was reassessed after 6 months. He microphones were set to provide a low level
was still more reactive to loud sounds than of directionality. NAL 2 was chosen because
his peers but no longer anticipated discom- of the level of compression and initially the
fort and simply covered his ears when un- aid was set to 70% of the prescriptive target.
expected loud sounds occurred. Significant Closed domes were attached to the aid to try
changes had been seen with his reading and to reduce the amount of incidental non-
writing, with APD tests showing that M was processed sound reaching the ear. An FM sys-
approaching normative levels. tem was provided for use in group or noise
environments to support speech audibility.
Counseling was required at every appoint-
Case Two (G) ment to keep G positive and on task.
Over the space of 6 months G gradually
This 61-year-old man experienced a traumatic increased his aid use to 6 hours per day. Over
brain injury 9 years ago that resulted in sig- this time his tolerance of both expected and
nificant light and sound sensitivity, balance unexpected sounds (in quiet) improved to the
issues, lack of peripheral vision and changes in point that he was able to listen to the radio/
memory and concentration. G reported that TV at a level that was audible for others. G
sudden sound was more uncomfortable than rejected the FM because the level of sound
steady-state noise but that all sound negatively appeared louder than the level of the aid (de-
affected his ability to concentrate on a task. spite verification in the test box showing that
236 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

the gain was similar) and as such he found determined by intraoperative measures. Cur-
that no improvement had occurred with his rent levels were initially reduced so that sound
ability to tolerate sound in more dynamic en- was just audible and over the space of nine
vironments. G continued to use ear protection months settings were increased until the map
appropriately. was at an appropriate level. At the same time
counseling was provided for appropriate ear
plug use and S was encouraged to stop using
Case Three (S) the plugs when he was in a quiet setting with
long-term goals being to restrict ear plug use
S is a 54-year-old man who experienced a sud- except when S was exposed to loud noise. In
den hearing loss in his right ear 3 years ago. the home setting S was asked to use a speaker
Since the sudden loss he has suffered from de- rather than ear buds when listening to sound
bilitating tinnitus, hyperacusis, and panic at- files so that he was less isolated, and that he
tacks that left him feeling very isolated. Prior move out of the office for his relaxation time
to the incident he was an avid musician and when it was quiet enough for him to feel safe.
played the guitar in a band. S was still able to Referral to a counselor was arranged to help
work but at all times wore an earplug in his with his anxiety and addiction problems.
good ear. When at home he locked himself in Nine months after the CI was switched
his office and used ear buds with white noise on, S reported that tinnitus was no longer an
to reduce his awareness of external sounds. S issue other than when he slept. He was work-
was taking the maximum dose of anti-anxiety ing with a psychologist, reduced his consump-
medication allowed and was self-medicating tion of alcohol, but still required a maximum
with alcohol to help him cope. S came to the dose of anxiety medication to function. Over-
clinic for assessment just prior to having a co- all sensitivity to sound improved but was still
chlear implant inserted into his worse ear. At an issue. S is now more confident interacting
this point S was not in the care of a mental with people in a quiet setting and does not
health specialist. isolate himself in his office at the end of the
Hearing testing revealed that S had no re- day. He has gone back to playing the guitar
sidual hearing in the right ear and a mild high independently and is hopeful that he will be
frequency sensorineural loss in the left. The able to play with friends in the future.
tinnitus assessment identified that his tinnitus
was localized to the right while all sensitivity
issues were localized to the left. UCL’s were re-
duced and revealed a dynamic range of approx-
Summary
imately 50 dB for frequencies up to 6000 Hz.
At 6000 Hz and above sounds were uncom-
fortable 5 dB above threshold. Initial recom- In a book chapter on the management of tin-
mendations were that the cochlear implant be nitus with hearing aids published a little over a
delayed so that earplug use could be reduced decade ago (Searchfield, 2006), the challenge
first, but given that S had already tried numer- in writing about tinnitus was similar to that
ous treatments in the past he felt the need to we faced here writing about DST. There was
proceed with the implant as soon as possible. little empirical evidence to support recom-
The cochlear implant was successfully mendations. While we do have evidence to
inserted into the right ear with settings being support the benefits of hearing aids for the use
14. Hearing Aids for Decreased Sound Tolerance and Minimal Hearing Loss 237

of tinnitus relief (Searchfield, 2015), we now Dillon, H. (2012). Hearing aids (2nd ed.). Sydney:
need similar evidence for DST. Clinical expe- Boomerang Press.
rience supports the recommendations made in Dillon, H., James, A., & Ginis, J. (1987). Client
this chapter; we firmly believe that with care, oriented scale of improvement (COSI) and its
patience, and patient motivation, hearing aid relationship to several other measures of ben-
efit and satisfaction provided by hearing aids.
gain can over time be achieved with little or no
J. Am. Acad. Audiol., 8, 27–43.
pain in cases of minimal hearing loss and DST. Fackrell, K., Fearnley, C., Hoare, D. J., & Sereda, M.
(2015). Hyperacusis questionnaire as a tool
for measuring hypersensitivity to sound in a
References tinnitus research population. Biomed. Res. Int.,
290425. doi:10.1155/2015/290425.
Formby, C., Hawley, M. L., Sherlock, L. P., Gold, S.,
Allen, J. B., Hall, J. L., & Jeng, P. S. (1990). Loud- Payne, J., Brooks, R., . . . Siegle, G. R. (2015).
ness growth in 1/2-octave bands (LGOB)–a pro- A sound therapy-based intervention to expand
cedure for the assessment of loudness. J. Acoust. the auditory dynamic range for loudness among
Soc. Am., 88(2), 745–753. Retrieved from http:// persons with sensorineural hearing losses: A
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2212299. randomized placebo-controlled clinical trial.
Baker, R. J., & Rosen, S. (2002). Auditory filter Semin. Hear, 36(2), 77–110. doi:10.1055/s-0035
nonlinearity in mild/moderate hearing impair- -1546958.
ment. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 111(3), 1330–1339. Goldstein, B., & Shulman, A. (1996). Tinnitus—
Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov Hyperacusis and the loudness discomfort level
/pubmed/11931310. test—A preliminary report. Int. Tinnitus J., 2, 83–
Bees, K., Guan, D., Alsarrage, N., & Searchfield, G. 89. Retrieved from file:///C|/Documents%20
(2017). The effects of auditory object identifica- and%20Settings/msan020/Local%20Settings
tion and localization (AOIL) training on noise /Application%20Data/Quosa/Data/My%20
acceptance and loudness discomfort in persons Citations/e3rsbvhnkoekv3n3iejgeqa744.qpw.
with normal hearing. Speech, Lang & Hear, 1–8. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi
Bentler, R. A., & Nelson, J. A. (2001). Effect of ?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Citation
spectral shaping and content on loudness dis- &list_uids=10753346.
comfort. J. Am. Acad. Audiol., 12(9), 462–470. Goverts, S. T., Houtgast, T., & van Beek, H. H.
Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov (2000). The precedence effect for lateralization
/pubmed/11699817. at low sensation levels. Hear. Res., 148(1–2),
Byrne, D., Dillon, H., Ching, T., Katsch, R., & 88–94. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih
Keidser, G. (2001). NAL-NL1 procedure for .gov/pubmed/10978827.
fitting nonlinear hearing aids: Characteristics Gu, J. W., Halpin, C. F., Nam, E. C., Levine, R. A.,
and comparisons with other procedures. J. Am. & Melcher, J. R. (2010). Tinnitus, diminished
Acad. Audiol., 12(1), 37–51. Retrieved from sound-level tolerance, and elevated auditory
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/112 activity in humans with clinically normal hear-
14977. ing sensitivity. J. Neurophysiol., 104(6), 3361–
Dancer, A. L., & Hamery, P. J. F. (1998) A new non- 3370. doi:10.1152/jn.00226.2010.
linear earplug for use in high-level impulse noise Hayes, D. (2006). Transient impulse control for
environment. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 103(2878). hearing aids. The Hearing Review.
Dillon, H. (1996). Compression? Yes, but for low Helfer, K. S., & Wilber, L. A. (1990). Hearing
or high frequencies, for low or high intensities, loss, aging, and speech perception in reverbera-
and with what response times? Ear Hear, 17(4), tion and noise. J. Speech Hear. Res., 33(1), 149–
287–307. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm 155. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih
.nih.gov/pubmed/8862967. .gov/pubmed/2314073.
238 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

Herraiz, C., & Diges, I. (2010). Tinnitus and unilateral hearing loss. Trends Amplif., 12(1),
hyperacusis/phonophobia. In A. R. Moller, B. 43–54. doi:10.1177/1084713807313570.
Langguth, D. De Ridder, & T. Kleinjung (Eds.), Meeus, O., Spaepen, M., de Ridder, D., & van
Textbook of tinnius. New York, NY: Springer. de Heyning, P. (2009). Correlation between
Jenstad, L. M., Van Tasell, D. J., & Ewert, C. (2003). hyperacusis measurements in tinnitus patients
Hearing aid troubleshooting based on patients’ [poster abstract]. From clinical practice to basic
descriptions. J. Am. Acad. Audiol., 14(7), 347– neuroscience and back: An international con-
360. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih ference on tinnitus: Third Tinnitus Research Ini-
.gov/pubmed/14620609. tiative Meeting, Stresa, Italy. June 24–26, 2009.
Kates, J. M. (2010). Understanding compression: (pp. 59). Stresa, Italy: TRI.
Modeling the effects of dynamic-range com- Moore, B. C., & Glasberg, B. R. (2011). The
pression in hearing aids. Int. J. Audiol., 49(6), effect of hearing loss on the resolution of par-
395–409. doi:10.3109/14992020903426256. tials and fundamental frequency discrimina-
Keidser, G., Bentler, R., & Kiessling, J. (2010). tion. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 130(5), 2891–2901.
A multi-site evaluation of a proposed test for doi:10.1121/1.3640852.
verifying hearing aid maximum output. Int. J. Mueller, H. G., & Bentler, R. A. (2005). Fitting
Audiol., 49(1), 14–23. doi:10.3109/14992020 hearing aids using clinical measures of loudness
903160876. discomfort levels: An evidence-based review of
Keidser, G., Dillon, H., Flax, M., Ching, T., & effectiveness. Journal of the American Academy
Brewer, S. (2011). The NAL-NL2 prescription of Audiology, 16(7), 461–472.
procedure. Audiol. Res., 1(1), e24. doi:10.4081 Nabelek, A. K., Freyaldenhoven, M. C., Tampas,
/audiores.2011.e24. J. W., Burchfiel, S. B., & Muenchen, R. A. (2006).
Khalfa, S., Dubal, S., Veuillet, E., Perez-Diaz, F., Acceptable noise level as a predictor of hearing
Jouvent, R., & Collet, L. (2002). Psychometric aid use. Journal of the American Academy of Au-
normalization of a hyperacusis questionnaire. diology, 17(9), 626–639. Retrieved from http://
ORL J. Otorhinolaryngol. Relat. Spec., 64(6), 436– www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17039765.
442. doi:67570. Neijenhuis, K., Tschur, H., & Snik, A. (2004).
Leigh-Paffenroth, E. D., & Elangovan, S. (2011). The effect of mild hearing impairment on au-
Temporal processing in low-frequency channels: ditory processing tests. J. Am. Acad. Audiol.,
effects of age and hearing loss in middle-aged 15(1), 6–16. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi
listeners. J. Am. Acad. Audiol., 22(7), 393–404. .nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15030098.
doi:10.3766/jaaa.22.7.2. Nelson, J. J., & Chen, K. (2004). The relationship
Levy, M., Peck, T., & Balachandran, R. (2011). of tinnitus, hyperacusis, and hearing loss. Ear
Acceptable noise levels in hyperacusic individ- Nose Throat J., 83(7), 472–476. Retrieved from
uals. Powerpoint presentation. file:///C|/Documents%20and%20Settings/msan
Liberman, M. C., & Kujawa, S. G. (2014). Hot top- 020/Local%20Settings/Application%20Data
ics—Hidden hearing loss: Permanent cochlear- /Quosa/Data/My%20Citations/os2s4l7bmojkp
nerve degeneration after temporary noise- 6qbdv5t3lbmig.qpw.
induced threshold shift. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi
135(4), 2311–2311. doi:doi:http://dx.doi.org/10 ?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Citation
.1121/1.4877618. &list_uids=15372918.
Mackersie, C. L. (2007). Hearing aid maximum Pekkarinen, E., Salmivalli, A., & Suonpaa, J.
output and loudness discomfort: Are unaided (1990). Effect of noise on word discrimination
loudness measures needed? J. Am. Acad. Audiol., by subjects with impaired hearing, compared
18(6), 504–514. Retrieved from http://www with those with normal hearing. Scand. Audiol.,
.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17849638. 19(1), 31–36. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi
McKay, S., Gravel, J. S., & Tharpe, A. M. (2008). .nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2336538.
Amplification considerations for children with Pienkowski, M., Tyler, R. S., Roncancio, E. R., Jun,
minimal or mild bilateral hearing loss and H. J., Brozoski, T., Dauman, N., . . . Moore, B. C.
14. Hearing Aids for Decreased Sound Tolerance and Minimal Hearing Loss 239

(2014). A review of hyperacusis and future di- processing disorder. Speech, Language and Hear-
rections: Part II. Measurement, mechanisms, ing, 19(3), 180–188. doi:10.1080/2050571X
and treatment. Am. J. Audiol., 23(4), 420–436. .2016.1182310.
doi:10.1044/2014_AJA-13-0037. Shekhawat, G. S., Searchfield, G. D., & Stinear,
Plack, C. J., Barker, D., & Prendergast, G. (2014). C. M. (2013). Role of hearing AIDS in tinni-
Perceptual consequences of “hidden” hearing tus intervention: A scoping review. J. Am. Acad.
loss. Trends Hear., 18. doi:10.1177/233121651 Audiol., 24(8), 747–762. doi:10.3766/jaaa.24
4550621. .8.11.
Polonenko, M. J., Scollie, S. D., Moodie, S., See- Stewart, R., & Wingfield, A. (2009). Hearing loss
wald, R. C., Laurnagaray, D., Shantz, J., & Rich­ and cognitive effort in older adults’ report ac-
ards, A. (2010). Fit to targets, preferred listen- curacy for verbal materials. J. Am. Acad. Au-
ing levels, and self-reported outcomes for the diol., 20(2), 147–154. Retrieved from http://
DSL v5.0 a hearing aid prescription for adults. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19927677.
Int. J. Audiol., 49(8), 550–560. doi:10.3109 Storey, L., Dillon, H., Yeend, I., & Wigney, D.
/14992021003713122. (1998). The National Acoustic Laboratories’
Preves, D., Sammeth, C., Cutting, M., & Wood- procedure for selecting the saturation sound
ruff, B. (1995). Experimental hearing device pressure level of hearing aids: Experimental val-
for hyperacusis. Hearing Instruments, December, idation. Ear Hear., 19(4), 267–279. Retrieved
34, 37–40. from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/97
Sandlin, R. E., & Olsson, R. J. (1999). Evaluation 28722.
and selection of maskers and other devices used Tun, P. A., McCoy, S., & Wingfield, A. (2009).
in the treatment of tinnitus and hyperacusis. Aging, hearing acuity, and the attentional costs
Trends in Amplification, 4, 6–26. of effortful listening. Psychol. Aging, 24(3), 761–
Sankowsky-Rothe, T., Blau, M., Schepker, H., & 766. doi:10.1037/a0014802.
Doclo, S. (2015). Reciprocal measurement of Tyler, R. S., Gogel, S. A., & Gehringer, A. K.
acoustic feedback paths in hearing aids. J. Acoust. (2007). Tinnitus activities treatment. Prog. Brain
Soc. Am., 188(4), EL399–EL404. Res., 166, 425–434. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(07)
Schecklmann, M., Landgrebe, M., Langguth, B., & 66041-5.
Group, T. R. I. D. S. (2014). Phenotypic char- Tyler, R. S., Pienkowski, M., Roncancio, E. R.,
acteristics of hyperacusis in tinnitus. PLoS ONE, Jun, H. J., Brozoski, T., Dauman, N., . . .
9(1), e86944. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0086944. Moore, B. C. (2014). A review of hyperacusis
Searchfield, G. D. (2006). Hearing aids and tinni- and future directions: Part I. Definitions and
tus. In T. R (Ed.), Tinnitus protocols. New York: manifestations. Am. J. Audiol., 23(4), 402–
Thieme. 419. doi:10.1044/2014_AJA-14-0010.
Searchfield, G. D. (2014). Tinnitus what and Vernon, J. A. (2002). Hyperacusis: Testing, treat-
where: An ecological framework. Front. Neu- ments and a possible mechanism. Australian
rol., 5, 271. doi:10.3389/fneur.2014.00271. and New Zealand Journal of Audiology, 24(2),
Searchfield, G. D. (2016). Hearing aids for tinni- 68–73.
tus. In D. M. Baguley & M. Fagelson (Eds.), Westcott, M. (2002). Case study. Management of
Tinnitus: Clinical and research perspectives. San hyperacusis associated with post-traumatic stress
Diego, CA: Plural. disorder. In P. R. (Ed.), Proceedings of the Seventh
Searchfield, G. D., Morrison-Low, J., & Wise, K. International Tinnitus Seminar (pp. 280–285).
(2007). Object identification and attention train- Perth: University of Western Australia.
ing for treating tinnitus. Prog. Brain Res., 166, Wu, Y. H., & Stangl, E. (2013). The effect of hear-
441–460. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(07)66043-9. ing aid signal-processing schemes on acceptable
Sheehan, G., Houghton, J., & Searchfield, G. D. noise levels: perception and prediction. Ear Hear.,
(2016). Acceptability of background noise 34(3), 333–341. doi:10.1097/AUD.0b013e318
amongst children diagnosed with an auditory 27417d4.
15
Hyperacusis Management:
A Patient’s Perspective
Rob Littwin

I took it easy for a couple days at home


Onset
and rested my ears. Then I went to the gym
to work out when very few people were there.
I am a musician. In 2004, I developed hyper- The background music, which I had never
acusis and severe ear pain. A year later, I de- paid attention to before, seemed uncharacter-
veloped tinnitus. I created my own approach istically loud and high-pitched. For the first
to address these challenges which enabled me time, I felt continual pain, numbness, and pres­
to re-establish my tolerance of sound, amelio- sure in my left ear.
rate my ear pain, and manage tinnitus. When the ear pain and other symptoms
In April 2004, I visited a friend who didn’t go away over the next day or two, I made
owned and ran a small restaurant/grocery with an appointment with a highly regarded neuro-
his wife in a suburb of Atlanta. My friend had tologist. My otologic examination, which took
moved a number of wooden tables and chairs place one week after I had been exposed to the
outside, behind the restaurant, and was using a pressure washer, was normal. My audiogram
pressure washer to clean them. I stood outside demonstrated normal hearing. One of the
with him for 30 to 40 minutes watching the tests administered to me showed I had acous-
color and grain of the wood reveal itself. While tic reflex decay in my left ear. In light of this
it was very loud—both of us were standing result, the doctor believed there was a 40 to
several feet from the pressure washer without 50% chance I could have an acoustic neuroma
hearing protection—I didn’t realize anything and he ordered an MRI scan. He prescribed
was wrong until I was driving home that after- prednisone and Trental (pentoxifylline) and
noon and put on a CD. The music sounded said he would not treat me further until the
sharper, distorted, tinny, high-pitched, and un- MRI results came back. I had the MRI a day or
comfortably loud. It was also somewhat pain- two later. My doctor failed to tell me how criti-
ful to hear. I shut off the CD. I tried listening cally important it is to wear hearing protection
to it again a few minutes later with the same during an MRI. This is particularly true for in-
results. dividuals with decreased sound tolerance. As a
241
242 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

result, I wore no hearing protection and none posed one week later to the extremely loud
was offered to me. sound of an MRI—the auditory version of
The experience was painful, but I with- double-dipping—my sensitivity to sound be­
stood more than 45 minutes of intensely loud came very severe and the ear pain was frequently
sound because I was concerned my symptoms excruciating. Normal, everyday sounds were
might be explained by an acoustic neuroma. I considerably louder to me and were often pain­
also badly needed my doctor’s continued help. ful to hear.
But from that moment forward, following the I had two very different kinds of ear pain.
MRI, my symptoms were exacerbated and my One type of pain was immediate and occurred
life changed. At home that evening, I turned uniformly upon the presentation of sounds
on the television. A big-voiced contestant on that exceeded my tolerance––the aural equiva-
a popular show at the time was singing, but lent of placing one’s hand on a hot stove. Many
rather than take pleasure in the performance, normal sounds that I would not have charac-
or even regard it with critical distance, the ex- terized as remotely loud prior to exposure to
perience was like being physically assaulted by the pressure washer and MRI felt physically
sound and I quickly turned off the TV. I was painful or uncomfortable to hear in both ears
stunned. for as long as the sound lasted. These sounds
I called my doctor a few days later. When included high-frequency or resonant adult
I told him how much worse I was after the voices, a crying baby, the voices or laughter of
MRI and questioned him about it, he was non- young children, a party, the percussive sound
chalant. He didn’t believe exposure to an MRI of a glass being placed on a counter, a dog
could worsen my symptoms. Perhaps I misread barking, a bird whistling, silverware dropped
it, but he seemed more invested in defending in the sink, the clatter of dishes being placed
his professional acumen than in supporting his in the dishwasher, a garbage disposal or vac-
very worried patient. I fired him. uum cleaner, a piano, sax, trumpet, flute, vio-
Much later, after I took the reins of my lin, or drums, my own singing, some flushing
own recovery by educating myself about my toilets, a leaf blower, a dentist’s drill, shopping
challenges and doing something about them, malls, grocery stores, movie theaters, most res­
I read the following in a book about tinnitus taurants, announcements over a loudspeaker,
and hyperacusis (Jastreboff & Hazell, 2004, a car horn, squealing brakes, and the sounds of
p. 79) that proved invaluable: “All patients be- road noise when driving.
ing referred for magnetic resonance imaging The other type of ear pain consisted of
must be advised about the need for hearing unremitting, continuous waves of sharp, stab-
protection, as this examination involves quite bing pain in my left ear that felt as if a nail
high levels of sound. In those with severe de- or an ice pick was repeatedly driven deep into
creased sound tolerance, it should be consid- my ear. These bouts of severe pain lasted for
ered that the scan is delayed until significant days, weeks, or months. Unlike the other type
improvement is noted.” Other researchers have of ear pain, which was immediate and would
described the importance of wearing properly last for as long as the given sound was present,
inserted earplugs during an MRI to preserve I would continue to feel the second type of ear
cochlear function (Radomskij et al., 2002). pain even when I was no longer exposed to the
In hindsight, I regard my decreased sound or the setting in which I had heard it.
sound tolerance and accompanying ear pain Since the MRI, I had been feeling hope-
following exposure to the pressure washer as less about my prospects and I was in a kind
moderate at best. However, after being ex- of mourning. I felt powerless, lethargic, and
15. Hyperacusis Management: A Patient’s Perspective 243

prodigiously sorry for myself. I was practi- frequencies so the music wasn’t physically pain-
cally looking at my life in retrospective terms, ful to hear. We kept the volume extremely low.
watching it recede. The sound level was still uncomfortable to hear
Prior to developing hyperacusis, I had at times, but being back in the studio doing
been working on a long and satisfying proj- what I loved gave me access to something in
ect in a recording studio where I sang, played myself that I missed and needed. On my first
keyboards, co-wrote horn and string arrange- day back, we worked for two or three hours.
ments, produced the sessions, and worked with Returning to my recording project pro-
some seriously fine jazz musicians. I couldn’t vided a huge psychological boost at a time
imagine ever being able to tolerate working in when I needed to be reminded how important
a recording studio again, much less sing, or lis- it is to look forward to things. I didn’t begin
ten to or write music at home. I was very upset desensitization therapy for eight more months,
with the doctor who had treated me, but I real- but in a sense my recovery began that day. Over
ized soon enough that I had to try to let those the coming months I was able to continue
feelings go. Feeling anger and regret was to face working on the project, albeit very quietly.
in the wrong direction, looking back at some- For the first few months, hyperacusis
thing that had already happened and expecting had defined me. But I found it was possible,
a different result. I needed to look ahead. indeed essential, to take hold of a few things
Fear can paralyze us or it can be a catalyst in my life that were meaningful to me and get
for making changes. Four months after I de- busy pursuing them. None of this came easily
veloped hyperacusis, I woke up one morning or quickly.
and realized as much as I missed experienc- If a recording studio was one of the few
ing sound the way I used to, I missed myself places I could control sound in my environ-
even more. I could no longer locate myself. I ment, I had little or no control in other set-
hadn’t listened to music for months. On this tings. I remember trying to watch the movie
morning, I resolved no matter how scared and Million Dollar Baby when it was released early
overwhelmed I felt, I would book some time in the following year. Even the narration by the
at the recording studio. soft-spoken Morgan Freeman was so loud and
The session consisted of my engineer and painful to me that I had to leave the theater.
me doing a lot of close listening to tracks we I also worked as an independent con-
had previously recorded and creating “comp” tractor, as a business analyst, and I was often
(short for composite) tracks from them. I required to be onsite. The sounds of some co-
didn’t think I would last more than two min- worker’s voices were like knives, and the par-
utes. I discovered I had far more control in a ticular sound quality of some male and female
recording studio than in other settings. We voices was physically very painful to hear. A
tend to think of recording studios as loud en- printer near my office made a loud, piercing
vironments, and they can be, but I learned to beep each time it printed a page. It too was
work smart, carefully, and very quietly when painful to hear. Some of my symptoms were
I returned to the studio. Instead of using surprising. For months I felt aural fullness, as
the large speakers I was accustomed to, we if a tiny balloon had been inflated inside my
used much smaller speakers, a comparatively ear, and left there. I had difficulty with pro-
comical-looking delivery system consider- cessed sound and in settings with a lot of echo.
ing all the great-sounding equipment at hand A conversation on the telephone lasting for
that I was unable to tolerate at the time. We more than a couple minutes would result in
also adjusted the mix by taking off some high my feeling the presence of something in my
244 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

ear that didn’t belong there and lowered my painful, but it is axiomatic that painful sound
tolerance of sound for several days. As a work- will seek out a hyperacusic. I got out in the world
around, I placed a small piece of duct tape over to the best extent I could, despite many setbacks
the receiver to help modulate the volume and and daily exposure to innumerable sounds that
frequency of the sound, making it less painful were too loud for me and caused pain or were
to hear. uncomfortable to hear. Several years later, an ex-
The highly processed sound of voices perienced clinician told me my decision to con-
coming through a speakerphone during con- tinue to go to work was an important step to
ference calls was both immediately painful take. To be honest, it was a decision I made out
to hear and resulted in my feeling continual of necessity. I needed to earn a living. When I
waves of pain in my left ear. During an emer- had several setbacks during which my sensitivity
gency at work, I sat in a conference room for to sound was exacerbated, I explained my situ-
eight hours with several co-workers, as well as ation to my manager and to a few co-workers;
others who called into a conference bridge for my colleagues could not have been more gra-
a call that lasted into the middle of the night. cious or understanding. I was encouraged to
Within an hour, I began to feel sharp pain in know that I had help and support at work.
my left ear. Over the next two months, the Just as I encountered many sounds at
pain did not let up for a minute. work that were either physically painful or un-
comfortably loud to hear, the same held true
in many settings outside work. With hyper­
acusis, I noticed that many background sounds
Diagnosis
became foreground sounds. The beeping pro-
duced by the price scanners at the checkout in
During this time I was diagnosed with hyper- the grocery store where I shopped was pain-
acusis at the Department of Otolaryngology fully loud and exacerbated my sensitivity to
at the Emory School of Medicine. The doctor sound for several days. For a while, driving
who examined me was the fifth doctor to do was overwhelmingly loud. Many places I fre-
so, a frustrating experience many hyper­acusic quented were too loud and painful for me to
patients share in our search to obtain an ac- even consider. Living with hyperacusis was a
curate diagnosis. It gave me some hope to at- tug of war between engagement and avoid-
tach a name to what I was experiencing and ance. I put myself in settings I could handle
the doctor was extremely solicitous and kind. and avoided environments that were too much
He encouraged me to look into a program at for me, even as I gently tried to push the aural
Emory University called Tinnitus Retraining envelope. I didn’t look for a perfect situation;
Therapy (TRT), an intervention used to man- I looked for environments that were tolerable.
age tinnitus and hyperacusis. He advised me For example, I found and could enjoy a com-
to wear hearing protection for several weeks at paratively quiet restaurant where the acoustics
work to let my constant ear pain gradually di- were acceptable and echo was minimal.
minish. When it did, I weaned myself off hear- When I was diagnosed with hyperacusis,
ing protection, as my doctor had suggested, and I invested time in learning as much as I could
no longer used earplugs as a routine practice. about the condition and my options so that I
Despite hyperacusis developing and per- could make an informed decision about how
sisting, I decided to continue working. I spent to proceed. I learned that hyperacusis is not
every day around sound, rather than purposely well known and is often misunderstood. Many
avoiding it. I didn’t seek out sound that was hearing healthcare clinicians are tentative or
15. Hyperacusis Management: A Patient’s Perspective 245

apprehensive with respect to its diagnosis and hear the sound, and for this reason I chose not
treatment. There is an abundance of confu- to pursue TRT.
sion regarding even basic information, such Dan Malcore, who founded and mod-
as terminology and definitions, ranging from erates The Hyperacusis Network (http://www
the short-lived hyperacusis dolorosa (Mathisen, .hyperacusis.net)—an Internet health support
1969)—which speaks to the emotional impact board and information website dedicated to
and attendant distress and sorrow of hyper­ individuals with hyperacusis and other forms
acusis sufferers—to the dismissive view that of decreased sound tolerance—graciously sent
hyperacusis is an “exaggerated or inappropri- me a copy of the pink noise Dr. Vernon used
ate” response to sounds that “a typical person” with moderate success to treat hyperacusic pa-
would regard as neither a threat nor uncom- tients. Pink noise is generally perceived as less
fortably loud (Klein et al., 1990). Studies on shrill than white noise. The idea of using pink
the prevalence of hyperacusis in the general noise to help re-establish sound tolerance was
population have drawn widely different con- suggested to Dr. Vernon by a patient whose
clusions (Andersson et al., 2002; Rubinstein experience with pink noise affirmed that this
et al., 1996). The mechanisms that may explain form of broadband noise would be easier to
the emergence of hyperacusis are hypothetical. tolerate than white noise.
When hyperacusis is described in the media, Unfortunately, after auditioning the pink
the reports are typically short on fact and long noise through headphones for just 10 seconds,
on hyperbole, and often employ the language I felt so profoundly dizzy I could barely stand
of catastrophe. up. When a mastering engineer and I examined
During this period, I improved my un- the pink noise presentation at the studio we
derstanding by reading Dr. Jastreboff and found it was very poorly made. Using high-end
Mr. Hazell’s book on Tinnitus Retraining equipment to provide a visual representation
Therapy (TRT), the approach recommended of the pink noise in real time, we discovered it
by the doctor who had diagnosed me. For contained an uncountable number of artifacts
many TRT patients, therapy included the use throughout the entire track. These artifacts re-
of wearable noise generators (WNGs). These sembled a series of fence posts, each causing
devices emitted broadband noise and were ini- a momentary increase in volume. As a result,
tially intended to foster habituation in patients the presentation repeatedly jumped from a
suffering from tinnitus. The devices were also steady volume to a much higher volume and
used by individuals with hyperacusis as part of back again. The spikes occurred over so short
a strategy to help them become less sensitive to a period, a listener wouldn’t be able to hear it
sound. I also learned that a highly respected cli- as a volume increase, but the overall effect to
nician, Dr. Jack Vernon, was using broadband the listener was that the sound was harsh. We
pink noise to treat his hyperacusis patients. also discovered the pink noise was unfiltered
A company that produced well-regarded and had a bandwidth of 1 Hz to 22,050 Hz,
WNGs for use in TRT made available a read- a considerably higher high end than the more
out of its broadband noise presentation on its moderate, filtered presentation Dr. Vernon
website, and I worked with two colleagues in described in his excellent book (Vernon &
the recording studio to replicate the presen- Sanders, 2001). Although we subsequently
tation. However, even when I played it very created a filtered pink noise presentation that
softly, I was unable to tolerate the broadband matched the characteristics described in Dr. Ver­
noise presentation used in TRT without ex- non’s book—200 Hz to 6 kHz—I found this
periencing immediate discomfort. It hurt to presentation painful to hear as well.
246 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

I was at a crossroads. I knew I needed help, noise could be produced in a recording stu-
but it appeared I couldn’t tolerate the commer- dio that contained substantially more of the
cially available options. A year had passed. I low-frequency energy I could tolerate. This
had put myself out in the world from the start gave the broadband noise a more rounded,
and did the best I could despite daily exposure pleasant sound, making it easy for me to use.
to many sounds that were too loud for me and In terms of volume and time spent using
caused pain or discomfort. Most environmental broadband noise, I chose to not adopt Dr. Ver-
sound was uniformly louder, as if someone had non’s protocol to work with broadband noise
reached into my auditory system and turned the at the loudest volume that was comfortable.
gain way up. Many sounds produced discom- I was mindful that Jastreboff and Hazell had
fort, and those that exceeded my tolerance in a written some particularly severe cases of hyper­
given frequency range and volume were imme- acusis could be permanently worsened by
diately painful to hear. In the preceding year, my using broadband pink noise at a high volume.
sound tolerance had barely improved. Instead, I gradually increased the volume to
a level that did not cause annoyance or dis-
comfort. More recently, Jastreboff has recom-
mended (2014) hyperacusic patients in TRT
Treatment
use broadband noise at about 9 dB SL (i.e.,
9 decibels above one’s threshold for hearing
I decided to create a pink noise presentation I the noise). Although I ultimately worked with
could tolerate, one that caused me no pain, dis- the volume set higher than the TRT level,
comfort, or annoyance. Working in the studio, I could clearly hear external sound and was
we produced several trial versions of broad- rarely aware of the broadband noise. I worked
band noise before I found one that was well- up to using the pink noise CD for eight hours
suited for me. I was able to tolerate broadband a day, and achieved better results when using
pink noise with a low-end frequency of 30 Hz it for eight consecutive hours rather than di-
and a high-end frequency of just 3 kHz. My viding the time spent listening into smaller
experience with broadband noise is atypical in portions. I used the CD for three months, at
this regard. It is unusual for a patient to have a a very low volume in the first month, slightly
difficult time tolerating unfiltered pink noise increasing the volume the next month, and
or the broadband noise presentations of in-ear increasing it a little more in the third month.
wearable noise generators. For this reason, my I found it helpful to set and forget the volume
approach to manage hyperacusis may or may at the beginning of the month and not change
not be effective or suitable for others. it until the following month.
We made a pink noise CD for my use I used good, very comfortable head-
and although its frequency content differed phones, capable of accurately producing high-
from that recommended by Dr. Vernon, and end, midrange, and low-end frequency sound.
was considerably less ambitious than the To ensure I could clearly hear sound in my
broadband noise used in TRT, I was thrilled immediate environment, I used open air head-
to find an acceptable and therapeutic sound. phones for which the headphone pads lay
By creating broadband noise in the studio, I flat against the ears rather than fully enclos­
also had an advantage; the commercial WNGs ing them.
were limited with respect to the low-end fre- I developed a program to manage my
quencies they could emit, whereas broadband hyperacusis using a series of graduated broad-
15. Hyperacusis Management: A Patient’s Perspective 247

band pink noise presentations created in a re- problem. Most companies keep their large serv-
cording studio. Each presentation was slightly ers in a closet or in an enclosed area, but this
more ambitious than the previous one. Every company had placed a large server in an open
three months, I increased the high-frequency area where my manager and a co-worker sat. The
energy of the presentation and started again at server emitted a relatively high-pitched sound
the lower volume. In addition to using broad­ I measured at 65 dB—the only time I ever used
band noise during waking hours, I used a table- a sound meter to measure anything. At the
top sound machine at bedtime so that I did time, my LDLs showed my sound tolerance
not sleep in silence. at 6 kHz and above was still low. Exposure to
Four or five months after I began ther- the high-frequency sound of the server hurt to
apy, now working with broadband noise with hear and made me more sensitive to sound; ev-
a low end of 30 Hz and a high end of 4 kHz, erything sounded louder to me. I decided to
I noticed a small improvement in my sound leave the contract. Later, I was compelled to
tolerance. Over time, I would extend the leave another contract when exposure to sound
high end to 5 kHz, 6 kHz, 6.5 kHz, 7.5 kHz, on site exacerbated my sound tolerance for
8.5 kHz, and above. By working in this way, days. My progress was slow and fitful.
the broadband noise that I was unable to tol- A similar problem occurred on another
erate at first became comfortable to use over contract at a time when the U.S. economy
time. I hired an experienced, smart, supportive was terrible and contract positions, like all
audiologist to periodically administer an audio- jobs, were hard to come by. Ever since I devel-
logical test called a Loudness Discomfort Level oped hyperacusis, I continued to have diffi­
(LDL) test to me. The test can help a hearing culty tolerating the heavily-processed sound
healthcare clinician determine the nature and of speakerphones on conference calls. Even at
extent of a person’s decreased sound tolerance. moderate sound levels, someone’s voice over
When I was initially diagnosed with hyper­ a speakerphone physically hurt to hear in the
acusis, my LDLs were in the mid-30 to mid- moment and each call caused me to feel con-
40s decibel range, consistent with my experi- tinual stabbing ear pain. Since I participated
encing discomfort at low conversational levels. in multiple calls per day, I was in non-stop
In an LDL test, sound is presented pain for the entire contract—seven-and-a-
through headphones using a clinical audiom- half months. I felt waves of sharp pain in my
eter to a patient seated in an audiometric test left ear, whether I was sitting in a quiet room,
suite. The test is used to identify the sound driving, working, waking up, or trying to fall
level that distinguishes a presented tone as loud asleep. After the contract ended and my ear
from one that is uncomfortably loud. When a pain had time to resolve, I found that if I used
tone reaches a volume that is uncomfortable to broadband noise as a buffer while on a con-
hear, the patient signals the clinician to move ference call, I could tolerate speakerphones
on to the next frequency. Tones at each new without ear pain. While I was glad to find a
frequency are initially presented at a consider- workaround, I thought of it as an auditory
ably lower volume. I was always tested twice in Band-Aid® but not a solution.
each ear and my audiologist used the second Until that time, a major part of my ther-
set of readings. We used the LDL test to mea- apy consisted of using sound whose spectrum
sure my progress and fine-tune my treatment. was progressively widened to improve my tol-
I had started a new contract as a busi- erance of sound. In an LDL test administered
ness analyst and immediately encountered a after I had been working for three months
248 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

with broadband noise with a high-end fre- tude, which can be very helpful to treat hyper­
quency of 8.5 kHz, my tolerance of sound at acusis, were absent in the broadband noise
8 kHz and above had improved, but less than I had used to this point. We replicated the
I would have liked. It was taking longer for characteristics of the commercially available
me to reestablish my sensitivity to sound in presentation in the studio by applying filters
high-end frequencies. and a narrow band-pass. We purposely used
There are two ways to customize broad- a high end of 9.5 kHz, slightly more ambi-
band noise for use in the desensitization ther- tious than the high-end frequency used by the
apy I employed. One is to extend the high-end aforementioned WNGs (Figure 15–1).
frequency, and I had been using this approach It had taken me a long time, but by
since I started therapy. The other approach is to working with progressively more challeng-
increase the amplitude of the broadband noise ing broadband noise, I was eventually able to
presentation at specific frequencies or frequency tolerate and use a presentation with spectral
ranges. This approach was used by companies characteristics that were comparable to those
that made wearable noise generators, and it was used in TRT. A new LDL test administered
used in TRT to treat tinnitus and hyperacusis. I to me around this time indicated my sound
adopted the latter approach as I proceeded with tolerance at 8 kHz and 12 kHz had increased
desensitization therapy to manage hyperacusis. dramatically.
An important difference between the Later, we would create a broadband white
broadband noise emitted by a well-regarded noise presentation modeled on the characteris-
wearable noise generator and the filtered pink tics of a WNG (Figure 15–2). Experienced cli-
noise I had been using to this point was that nicians report filtered broadband white noise is
the WNG contained a 10 dB increase in a bit more effective for hyperacusis treatment
amplitude at 6 kHz and a smaller increase in than broadband pink noise, provided the pa-
amplitude at 2.8 kHz. These boosts in ampli- tient can tolerate it.

Figure 15–1. Broadband noise modeled on a wearable noise generator presentation with em-
phasis at 2.8 kHz and 6 kHz.
15. Hyperacusis Management: A Patient’s Perspective 249

Figure 15–2. White noise from 30 Hz to 22,050 Hz modeled on a wearable noise generator
presentation with six small increases in amplitude. We purposely increased the low end to round
out the sound.

enormously powerful. Recognizing the feel-


Emotions
ings in oneself and finding a way to address
them is difficult but essential.
The central auditory system is a master de- One of the greatest barriers to success in
tective. When we consider a sound to be im- treating individuals with severe hyperacusis is
portant, particularly if we regard the sound the failure to acknowledge and address the role
as a threat, our auditory system works hard of the emotions in their response to sound.
to monitor it. The auditory system is also an Different terms—phonophobia (Phillips &
effective discriminator. When a sound is no Carr, 1998), misophonia ( Jastreboff & Jastre-
longer perceived to be of importance, our boff, 2002), fear hyperacusis, and annoyance
hearing system disregards it. In this way, the hyperacusis (Tyler et al., 2014)—have been
hearing system is selective about the sound to used to distinguish this aspect of decreased
which it attends, which is why we can hear sound tolerance. Although audiologists and
our name being called across a noisy, crowded hearing scientists are not in agreement on the
room or wake up to the sound of a crying utility of these terms, each is an attempt to
baby, but sleep through a thunderstorm. describe the emotional response that accom-
A hyperacusic regards many sounds as panies hyperacusis like an uninvited dinner
a threat, hence of value and requiring atten- guest who has an inordinately large appetite.
tion. While it is reasonable to respond with Since starting therapy, I regarded hyper­
prudence to sound that is physically painful acusis as a physiological problem that had to do
to hear, inevitably, for the hyperacusic this re- with the way the brain processes sound. While
sults in developing strong and adverse feelings I was aware of the emotional impact hyper­
about sound. These feelings, and the resultant acusis exerted on me every day, I assumed all
hypervigilance to or avoidance of sounds, are my challenges with decreased sound tolerance
250 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

would be resolved by working with broadband (e.g., Westcott, 2016) have preferred the term
noise, avoiding the overuse of hearing protec- Tonic Tensor Tympani Syndrome (TTTS).
tion around normal sound, and consistently Hyperacusic patients who report aural full-
exposing myself to different external settings. ness or pressure, ear pain, and other symptoms
However, I was wrong. The strategies I de- consistent with the involvement of the tensor
ployed, similar to those used in clinics around tympani muscle are described by West­cott and
the world to improve LDLs, had not helped Jastreboff. For Westcott (2016, p. 145) these
me resolve my continual ear pain. Like other symptoms were reported by patients “with an
hyperacusics, I was concerned that the pain was anxiety-based need to protect the health of their
a symptom of irreparable damage to my audi- ears or their hearing.” For Jastreboff (2014,
tory system. For this reason, I maintained an p. 111), the “over-activation of the autonomic
abiding, if unfamiliar sense of caution around nervous system may, in turn, activate the tensor
sounds and in settings that could provoke tympani muscle resulting in tensor tympani syn-
these symptoms. For a long time, I didn’t un- drome (e.g., fullness, pulsation, pain in the ear).”
derstand the line connecting my ear pain and The symptoms of Tensor Tympani Syn-
aural fullness on one hand, to the fear, anxiety, drome have also been reported by individuals
and distress I experienced around some specific exposed to an unexpected loud sound, typi­
sounds and settings on the other. cally with a short rise time that produces a
so-called acoustic shock, as well as by patients
with temporomandibular disorders (TMD).
Ear Pain Patuzzi, Milhinch, and Doyle (2000) proposed
that symptoms of acoustic shock injury may
The tensor tympani and stapedius muscles re- be attributed to an excessive startle reflex of the
duce the power at which sound reaches the tensor tympani muscle. Ramirez et al. (2007,
inner ear. The contraction of the tensor tym- p. 97) wrote that emotional stress may play an
pani muscle is regarded as a startle reflex. For important role in patients with TMD, noting
some individuals, tensor tympani muscle ac- that ear pain “can be felt by tym­panic mem-
tivity may increase, and the muscle may even- brane tension due to constant tensor tympani
tually be fixed in a state of tonic contraction. muscle contracture.”
Klochoff (1979) speculated that the over- Regarding therapy for Tensor Tympani
activity of the tensor tympani is associated Syndrome, Klochoff (1979) indicated the most
with several symptoms often reported by important step is for clinicians to reassure pa-
patients. These symptoms include episodic tients that the symptoms relate to “psychoso-
ear fullness; otalgia (i.e., ear pain); tinnitus; matic dysfunction due to stress” and are not a
dysacusis consisting of murmurs, clicks, and sign of disease. For patients in intractable pain,
distortion; tension headaches; and vertigo. Klochoff recommended physical relaxation ther­­
Klochoff and Westerberg (1971) reported that apy and medication such as diazepam. West-
increased mental stress appeared to be the com- cott has reported that broadband noise desen-
mon thread running through nearly every case sitization therapy combined with Cognitive
of spontaneous tonic tensor tympani muscle Be­­havioral Therapy, or referral to a physiother­­a­
activity in the ear, which they called the tonic pist or pain specialist are helpful to some hyper­­
tensor phenomenon. Due to the prevalence acusic patients with ear pain and other symp-
of these symptoms, Klochoff (1979) wrote, toms of TTTS. As a last resort, some patients
“it seems justified to talk of a Tensor Tympani have considered surgery to section the stapedius
Syndrome.” For several years, some authors and/or tensor tympani muscle tendons. In a
15. Hyperacusis Management: A Patient’s Perspective 251

systematic review of 21 articles of mostly case a recording studio. The music protocol en-
reports or case series, based on the poor quality abled me to give myself control over sound in
of the studies Bhimrao et al. (2012) re­­ported a different setting, feel less tentative around
there is weak evidence to support surgical ten­ sound, and slowly prove to myself, con-
otomy of the middle ear muscles in the treat- sciously and unconsciously, that I could let
ment of middle ear myoclonus—of which Ten- go of the feelings and beliefs I had developed
sor Tympani Syndrome is a subtype. about the dangers of sound. Exposure to nor-
mal sound could not hurt me.
The music protocol helped me com-
pletely resolve the continual bouts of severe ear
Music Protocol
pain I felt and helped me to further improve
my LDLs. I have used the protocol twice: once
I had reached a point in therapy at which I to manage hyperacusis and severe ear pain and
needed to push the aural envelope a little fur- again when I was exposed to a loud, sudden
ther. Broadband noise has no dynamics; it is sound at work that clinicians characterized as
static, with no loud and soft sections, whereas an acoustic shock. Following the acoustic in-
everyday life is filled with such dynamics. Lis- cident, I felt earache, aural fullness, pressure,
tening to music in a deliberate, controlled, and tingling, and slight pain—comparatively mild
purposeful way would expose me to sound that symptoms. Over time, these symptoms de-
varies considerably over time. I hoped it would creased and then fully resolved as I continued
be an effective approach to supplement the use to use the music protocol week after week.
of broadband noise and enable me to further The protocol consists of three phases. In
improve my tolerance of high-frequency sound phase 1, the baseline setting is the lowest pos-
and address my challenges with severe ear pain. sible volume the listener can hear. In phase 2,
With these goals in mind, I created a protocol for the baseline setting is the most comfortable
listening to music. The last thing I wanted was volume the listener chooses. In phase 3, the
to create something that felt like more “work” so baseline setting is the loudest volume the lis-
I made it fun. I walk on a treadmill every day for tener can tolerate without being uncomfort-
at least 45 minutes and I thought if I used that able. See Tables 15–1, 15–2, and 15–3.
time to listen to music, I could exercise and con- Phase 1 should be used for nine weeks,
centrate on enjoying music at the same time. phase 2 for 18 weeks, and phase 3 for 12 weeks;
By pairing sound with an enjoyable ac- however, individual needs and differences may
tivity, the music protocol helped me re- dictate that modifications are indicated.
evaluate the emotional response to sound that
accompanied my hyperacusis and to address • Every day, listen to an entire CD (i.e.,
my anxiety and stress about sound in certain between 40 and 75 minutes per session
settings. I thought it was important to move depending on the recording used in
toward my anxiety rather than away from it. that day’s session).
That is why one of the conditions I set for • Choose any type of music, regardless
myself was to listen to music through head- of style and dynamic range, from
phones. I needed to prove to myself that even Beethoven to Hendrix, Miles Davis to
with sound right up against my ears, there Judy Collins, rap to country music.
was nothing to be concerned about. The key is to play music one enjoys and
Until that time, I had been able to work wants to hear.
with music in the controlled environment of • Actively listen to the music.
252 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

• It is very important to introduce the music much better fidelity, although they are
as directly and consistently as possible to far more expensive.
one’s auditory system. Therefore, listen • If listening to a certain style of music
to the music via headphones rather than feels too ambitious during the first
through external speakers. couple of months, it is fine to listen to
• The only requirement about the any style of music that is relaxing.
headphones is for the music to sound • If the music hurts or is uncomfortable
pleasing when played through them. at any time, lower the volume. If the
Noise cancellation headphones can be music continues to hurt, stop and play
used, if preferred. something else. The music should never
• Use CDs, LPs, or files that have been be physically painful or uncomfortable.
downloaded in the WAV or AIFF • For the first few weeks, the volume will
format. be extremely low. It may seem that no
• Do not use MP3 files, as their sound progress is being made, but during this
quality may be degraded and less time one is learning to treat sound as
pleasant. nonthreatening.
• Do not use the typical ear buds • Do not use broadband noise during a
that come with iPods. There are listening session.
commercially available ear buds with • Use the music protocol for nine months.

Table 15–1. Phase 1.

Week Instructions
1 Each day of the week, set the volume of the CD so it is at the lowest possible
volume that can be heard (i.e., at one’s threshold for hearing sound). Listen to
the entire CD. By design, the listener will strain to hear the music. Relax and
pay attention.
2 Each day of the week, set the volume of the CD so it is at the lowest possible
volume that can be heard. Then, increase the volume to a level that is slightly
louder. Listen to the entire CD. If the music feels painful at any time, even
at this low volume, slightly lower the volume so that it is at the threshold for
hearing sound.
3 Each day of the week, set the volume of the CD so it is at the lowest possible
volume that can be heard. Then, increase the volume to a level that is slightly
louder. Then, increase the volume slightly more. Listen to the entire CD. If the
music feels painful at any time, slightly lower the volume. If the listener feels
worried or uncomfortable, try to hang in there. If these feelings persist, slightly
lower the volume so that it is at the threshold for hearing sound.
4–9 Listen to an entire CD each day of the week. Set the volume to a level that
is slightly louder than the previous week. For example, during week 4 the
volume should be slightly louder than it was for week 3, in week 5 the volume
should be slightly louder than it was for week 4, and so on. Listen to the entire
CD. If the music feels painful at any time, slightly lower the volume. If the
listener feels worried or uncomfortable, try to hang in there. If these feelings
persist, slightly lower the volume a little more.
Table 15–2. Phase 2.

Week Instructions
10 Each day of the week, set the volume of the CD so it is at the most
comfortable volume. Listen to the entire CD. If the volume is set to a level
that is lower than the volume used in the previous week, it is fine. Relax, pay
attention, and enjoy the music. If the music feels painful at any time, or if the
listener feels worried or uncomfortable, slightly lower the volume.
11 Each day of the week, set the volume of the CD so it is at the most
comfortable volume. Then, increase the volume to a level that is slightly
louder. Listen to the entire CD. If the music feels painful at any time, or if the
listener feels worried or uncomfortable, slightly lower the volume.
12 Each day of the week, set the volume of the CD so it is at the most
comfortable volume. Then, increase the volume to a level that is slightly
louder. Then, slightly increase the volume a bit more. Listen to the entire
CD. If the music feels painful at any time, or if the listener feels worried or
uncomfortable, slightly lower the volume.
13 Repeat the steps described in week 10.
14 Repeat the steps described in week 11.
15 Repeat the steps described in week 12.
16 Repeat the steps described in week 10.
17 Repeat the steps described in week 11.
18 Repeat the steps described in week 12.
19 Repeat the steps described in week 10.
20 Repeat the steps described in week 11.
21 Repeat the steps described in week 12.
22 Repeat the steps described in week 10.
23 Repeat the steps described in week 11.
24 Repeat the steps described in week 12.
25 Repeat the steps described in week 10.
26 Repeat the steps described in week 11.
27 Repeat the steps described in week 12. By now, individuals may find the
volume at which one is most comfortable listening to music is a little louder
than it was during the first several weeks of using the music protocol. This is
to be expected and indeed is a positive sign.

253
Table 15–3. Phase 3.

Week Instructions
28 Each day of the week, set the volume of the CD so it is at the most
comfortable volume. Listen to the entire CD. If the volume is set to a level
that is lower than the volume used in the previous week, it is fine. Relax,
pay attention, and enjoy the music. If the music feels painful at any time,
or if the listener feels worried or uncomfortable, slightly lower the volume.
At the end of the session, choose any song on the CD. Set the volume
so it is uncomfortably loud and then lower the volume until it is no longer
uncomfortable to hear. Each day during the week, listen to one song at the
end of the session at this volume. If the chosen volume is louder than normal,
it is fine provided it is neither painful nor uncomfortable. As always, the listener
should lower the volume anytime he or she feels uncomfortable or afraid.
29 Each day of the week, set the volume of the CD so it is at the most
comfortable volume. Then, increase the volume to a level that is slightly
louder. Listen to the entire CD. At the end of the session, choose two songs on
the CD. Set the volume so it is uncomfortably loud and then lower the volume
until it is no longer uncomfortable to hear. For each day during the week, listen
to two songs at the end of the session at this volume. If the chosen volume is
louder than normal, it is fine provided it is neither painful nor uncomfortable.
As always, the listener should lower the volume anytime he or she feels
uncomfortable or afraid.
30 Each day of the week, set the volume of the CD so it is at the most
comfortable volume. Then, increase the volume to a level that is slightly
louder. Then, slightly increase the volume a bit more. Listen to the entire CD.
At the end of the session, choose three songs on the CD. Set the volume
so it is uncomfortably loud and then lower the volume until it is no longer
uncomfortable to hear. For each day during the week, listen to three songs
at the end of the session at this volume. If the chosen volume is louder than
normal, it is fine provided it is neither painful nor uncomfortable. As always,
the listener should lower the volume anytime he or she feels uncomfortable or
afraid.
31 Repeat the steps described in week 28.
32 Repeat the steps described in week 29.
33 Repeat the steps described in week 30.
34 Repeat the steps described in week 28.
35 Repeat the steps described in week 29.
36 Repeat the steps described in week 30.
37 Repeat the steps described in week 28.
38 Repeat the steps described in week 29.
39 Repeat the steps described in week 30.

254
15. Hyperacusis Management: A Patient’s Perspective 255

After using the music protocol, work-


ing with broader spectrum noise presenta- Intuition
tions that contained increases in amplitude
at certain frequencies (similar to the broad- A bat and a ball cost $1.10. The bat costs
band noise used in TRT), and continuing to $1.00 more than the ball. How much does
put myself out in the world, the last time my the ball cost? ____ cents
LDLs were administered, they were in the
90 dB HL to 100 dB HL range, which hear-
ing healthcare clinicians regard as within the When this puzzle was presented to students
range of normal LDLs for non-hyperacusic at Harvard, MIT, and Princeton, as part of
patients (Knobel & Sanchez, 2006). a Cognitive Reflection Test, more than half
There is a lot we do not know about gave the intuitive answer, 10 cents (Frederick,
hyperacusis. Rather than focus on the un- 2005). When students at less selective col-
knowns, I chose to focus on what could be leges and universities tried to solve the puzzle,
done, on what I could do to help myself. No more than 80% reached the same conclusion.
matter how frustrated I was by repeated set- This answer, which comes easily and immedi-
backs, or the number of adjustments I made ately to mind, is wrong. (The correct answer
to my treatment or in my thinking, I focused is 5 cents.) Notably, when students answered
on getting better. At times, improvements correctly, in many cases they considered the
were hard to come by because hyperacusis intuitive answer first before crossing it out
could be unpredictable. One afternoon, after and solving the puzzle.
working quietly in the recording studio, my The bat-and-ball puzzle demonstrates
sound tolerance collapsed for days. Therapy two cognitive processes at work––one pro-
did not progress in a straight line, and expo- cess is performed quickly, automatically, and
sure to normal sound could repeatedly cause effortlessly, with little conscious thought; the
sound in general to be louder for days or lon- other process is performed more slowly, reflec-
ger. Still, I did not allow “I can’t recover from tively, and deliberately. The processes, named
this” to be part of my vocabulary. System 1 and System 2 (Stanovich & West,
I have come a long way in re-establishing 2000), are the focus of many studies in cogni-
my sound tolerance. There are many activi- tive and social psychology, and have enormous
ties in which I can engage that I never imag- relevance for sufferers of tinnitus, hyperacusis,
ined I would be able to do again. I can go and ear pain.
to movies, many restaurants, and even some An intuitive answer can be regarded as
clubs without fear or concern. While I have the first thought that comes to mind. Just as
not recovered 100% of my sound tolerance, the bat-and-ball puzzle evokes an intuitive
my LDLs are once again in the normal range response, the same can be said of a patient’s
and I function normally, comfortably, and response to tinnitus and hyperacusis. I had
with pleasure in many aural settings. My developed tinnitus a year after I developed
sound tolerance continues to improve as I ex- hyperacusis and, at first, I was miserable. Like
pose my auditory system to a broader range many other tinnitus sufferers, I focused on
of sound. The continual ear pain I felt and how loud and inexorable tinnitus was. Tinni-
the sense of aural fullness have completely tus was inimitable, unlike any other sound, and
resolved. I was constantly aware of it.
256 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

If my tinnitus could be likened to an op- assured “inner expert” began to pontificate


era composed by a thousand hyperactive mos- about tinnitus, I short-circuited his commen-
quitos trapped in a blender set to the highest tary by silently reminding myself I didn’t know
speed, then my thinking about tinnitus could what I was talking about. I ended up having a
be reasonably compared to the accompany- very productive session in the studio.
ing libretto. When I acquired tinnitus, my An individual’s intuitive thinking about
mind kept up a running commentary about tinnitus can have a profound negative influence
it and I drew unremitting, dire conclusions on one’s attempts to manage the condition;
about my prospects. For example, I thought less attention has been paid to how a person
life as I knew it was over, I would never be thinks about hyperacusis (McKenna, Baguley,
happy again, and I would never feel differ- & McFerran, 2010). Human beings are pre-
ently. These beliefs and judgments, the first disposed to making intuitive, quick judgments
thoughts that had entered my mind on a sub- about sensory experiences, and sound is no
ject I knew nothing about—I had never dis- exception. An ambulance runs its siren, and
cussed tinnitus with a doctor or read anything anyone driving nearby automatically reacts,
about it—were arrived at with assurance and essentially without thinking. Indeed, the siren
I subscribed to each one. sound is composed in a manner that ensures
Over time, I realized my thoughts about an automatic reaction in anyone within hear-
tinnitus were contributing to and energizing ing distance. When individuals develop hyper­
how bad I felt. I was used to trusting my own acusis, they continue to make reflexive judg-
thinking, but on the subject of tinnitus, I de- ments about sound. Unfortunately, routine
cided to take a counterintuitive approach: I sounds of moderate level can cause them to feel
accepted that whatever I thought about tin- endangered, and the patients may respond au-
nitus was probably wrong and rather than tomatically to these sounds in a manner resem-
endorse the thought, I would dismiss it. This bling a prior, and normal, response to a con-
realization was liberating. The key to manag- siderably louder sound such as a siren. Thus,
ing tinnitus had less to do with the sound of the impact of everyday and undamaging sound
tinnitus and a great deal to do with my think- levels on people with hyperacusis changes dra-
ing about tinnitus. matically and the meaning they assign to their
People often make decisions and base sound experiences changes as well.
their actions about intrusive tinnitus on lit- Those who experience a sensation of
tle more than intuitive thinking. When the physical pain or discomfort when exposed to
first thing that comes to mind seems plausi- moderate sound levels develop a strong accom-
ble, the thought is endorsed, even if it isn’t panying emotional response to such sounds
in one’s best interest and when following the that may, in turn, influence their beliefs about
first thing that comes to mind serves to make sound and the decisions they make. These
one miserable. We can’t shut off that part of emotions are consistent with the reactions of
ourselves, but we don’t have to take these individuals who struggle with intrusive tinni-
thoughts seriously or pursue them. tus who also may believe that many routine
I have a vivid memory of working in the and innocuous environmental sounds are dan-
recording studio the day after I had this realiza- gerous and can further worsen their condition.
tion. I had a lot of close listening to do and was When individuals with hyperacusis are
concerned I wouldn’t be able to concentrate. exposed to sound that is perceived as pain-
For a while during the session, when my overly fully loud, a series of interconnected ideas,
15. Hyperacusis Management: A Patient’s Perspective 257

emotions, and behaviors are evoked. Exposure hear, but it is important for patients with
to a painful sound triggers surprise, fear, and hyperacusis to distinguish between sound that
anxiety, perhaps evoking memories of similar is loud enough to cause physical damage and
exposures, which, in turn, trigger a tendency to sound that they perceive feels that way.
avoid the painful sound, thereby intensifying
the emotions to which this behavior is linked,
and further reinforcing the affected individu-
al’s inaccurate analysis regarding the dangers
Getting Help
of sound. This self-reinforcing, interconnected
series of events, called associative activation,
is a feature of System 1 that occurs automati- Hyperacusis is manageable; even individuals
cally, and is notable for its coherence and in- with severely decreased sound tolerance can
ternal consistency, despite potential inaccura- make progress, if they work at it. As a first
cies. Thoughts, emotions, beliefs, and actions step, it is very important to be evaluated and
that are “activated by the associative context” diagnosed by a neurotologist, otoneurologist,
are given considerably more weight than they or audiologist who is experienced in success-
deserve, whereas other highly relevant infor- fully diagnosing and treating hyperacusis. It
mation, such as others in the environment dis- is also important to explore whether a medi-
playing no discomfort at the sound’s presence, cal condition explains the presence of hyper­
is underweighted or ignored (Morewedge & acusis. If there is a medical explanation, a
Kahneman, 2010; Kahneman, 2011). medical intervention may be possible.
For these reasons, hyperacusics are not Every day, individuals with hyperacusis
well-served by making intuitive, automatic are well-advised to engage in an activity that
judgments about sound, and the choices they gets them out of their homes and to do so
make as a result of these judgments (e.g., the without hearing protection so that they begin
overuse of hearing protection, disinclination to associate safety with environments other
to seek treatment, a preference for silence and than their own home. The auditory system
seclusion, etc.) may not be in their best in- thrives on sound, and this is as true for indi-
terest. Modifying automatic thinking about viduals with hyperacusis as it is for people who
sound is difficult because it is natural and essen- do not have hyperacusis.
tial for people to trust their judgments about
what they hear. Moreover, mood influences the
ability to think intuitively (Bolte et al., 2003);
when a hyperacusic feels uncomfortable, anx­­ Loudness Discomfort Levels
ious, or fearful around sound, it can have a
strong impact on intuitive performance. While there is general agreement among
A patient’s belief that sound is damaging hearing healthcare professionals that LDLs are
is persuasive; the belief may compel a patient decreased in patients with hyperacusis, there is
to modify lifestyle and routine activities. That no international consensus on how to admin­
said, there is no evidence to support the belief ister or evaluate LDLs. At a minimum, individ­
that a hyperacusic’s auditory system is more uals should be tested at .5 kHz, 1 kHz, 2 kHz,
susceptible to damage than someone who 3 kHz, 4 kHz, 6 kHz, and 8 kHz. A frequency-
does not have hyperacusis. A sound may be specific LDL test that begins at .5 kHz and
very uncomfortable or physically painful to tests up to 12 kHz is ideal.
258 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

The LDL test (also referred to as a test one’s home, it is important to be willing to
to measure uncomfortable loudness levels, or travel to a clinician who can help.
ULLs) is regarded as risky by some patients I was approached several years ago by
and clinicians. However, if the test is properly Dan Malcore to create an original recording
administered and the directions are clearly of unfiltered pink noise for The Hyperacusis
stated and followed, then no one who takes Network. I made the Network’s pink noise
an LDL test is subjected to a painful sound; CD with two colleagues, Larry Anthony and
it is a loudness discomfort level test and not a Marty Kearns, and donated it to The Hyper-
loudness pain test. There is also a significant acusis Network. The pink noise CD is about
difference between a sound that is uncomfort- 74 minutes in length and the recorded sound
able to hear and one that is dangerous to hear. covers a range from 30 Hz to 22.05 kHz. By
Sounds of dangerous levels are not presented placing one’s listening device in “repeat” mode,
to patients during an LDL test when the cli- individuals can offer themselves vir­tually un­
nician and patient communicate effectively. interrupted, unfiltered broadband noise. The
The LDL test can also offer hope to patients file can be downloaded to an iPod, but should
as therapy proceeds because it gives measur- be converted to the WAV or AIFF audio for-
able proof of progress and provides valuable mats. The iPod supports both formats.
infor­mation about frequency regions in which
pro­g­ress occurs more slowly.
Background Sound

Desensitization Therapy Playing gentle, neutral, relaxing background


sound at bedtime and at other times (e.g.,
reading at home, working in the office) is an
The use of broadband noise for the purposes
important part of desensitization therapy for
of desensitization has enabled many hyper­
hyperacusis. The background sound should
acusics to significantly improve their tolerance
be set to a low volume so that it isn’t intru-
of sound. As indicated above, white noise may
sive and doesn’t keep one awake at bedtime. A
be more effective than pink noise; however,
table-top sound machine or a recording with
some hyperacusic patients find white noise
nature sounds (e.g., forest, rain, ocean, etc.)
too harsh. In those cases, a pink noise presen-
are effective options.
tation will be more easily accepted, and hence
considerably more effective; few patients will
use broadband noise that is painful or un-
comfortable, but most patients will accept a Hearing Protection
neutral signal they can easily ignore. For best
results, desensitization therapy should be done When sound becomes shocking, loud, or
with the guidance of a knowledgeable hearing painful, it is logical for people with hyper-
healthcare professional who has successfully acusis to want to protect themselves from
diagnosed and treated other patients who have it. Common sense tells us that if a car horn
hyperacusis. The clinician should be conver- blows unexpectedly in a parking lot, and pro-
sant with the options available to manage duces a sensation of immediate pain in a pass-
hyperacusis and help the patient determine the erby, then the sound can be avoided the next
best course for his or her needs. If it isn’t pos- time by wearing earplugs in parking lots. If
sible to locate a knowledgeable clinician near street noise is uncomfortable or painful to
15. Hyperacusis Management: A Patient’s Perspective 259

hear, logic dictates discomfort can be avoided one’s individual ears, musician’s earplugs are
by wearing hearing protection when walking also much more expensive than store-bought
down the street. When these beliefs are ex- earplugs.
tended, hyperacusics may wear hearing protec-
tion in anticipation of loud sound regardless of
the probability that such sounds may not occur to Travel
limit what these individuals regard as the risk
of further auditory system damage. Noise cancellation headphones are excellent
The use of hearing protection is seduc- at blocking out the low-frequency rumble
tive because it allows a hyperacusic to feel of airplane engines and are ideal for a hyper­
less pain and discomfort around sound and acusic when flying. Care should be taken to
provides a measure of control. Although it is select a well-reviewed and reputable product.
counterintuitive, hyperacusis is improved not Individuals with hyperacusis may find it help-
by playing defense against sound but through ful to request a seat that is closer to the front
purposeful and consistent exposure to sound. of the plane and away from the engines.
Overuse of hearing protection is detrimental
to individuals with hyperacusis and typically
makes one more, not less, sensitive to sound.
When Formby et al. (2003) investigated the
Support Groups
impact of hearing protection versus the use
of wearable noise generators on loudness per-
ception for individuals with no hyperacusis, Some kind, thoughtful, and generous people
they concluded that subjects who used hear- contribute to Internet patient support groups
ing protection became more likely to display in which hyperacusis and other forms of de-
reduced LDLs over time, while subjects who creased sound tolerance are discussed. There
used in-ear noise generators displayed increases is also a lot of misinformation about hyper­
in LDLs during the same period. acusis in the patient community; in particu-
Hyperacusics and non-hyperacusics alike lar, that hyperacusis is caused by ear damage
should wear hearing protection in settings and exposure to normal sound is dangerous
when ambient sound levels require individuals for hyperacusics. Substantial negative coun-
to speak loudly in order to communicate with seling and counterproductive thinking occur
someone standing next to them. Earplugs on these sites.
purchased in a pharmacy or grocery store pro- A newcomer to a support site will soon
vide hearing protection that attenuates some discover that accurate information competes
frequencies more than others, which is why for space alongside inaccurate information.
sound is muffled or appears unnatural when Thus, one will read about viable treatments
using store-bought earplugs. Foam earplugs for hyperacusis, such as broadband pink noise
with a Noise Reduction Rating (NRR) of 32 therapy and Tinnitus Retraining Therapy
or 33 provide excellent protection; the NRR (TRT), but one will also read about the over-
is listed on the package. Musician’s earplugs use of earplugs. The best way for a newcomer
may provide less protection; however, they to distinguish practical, worthwhile informa-
attenuate sound more evenly across a wide tion about hyperacusis from happy talk and
range of frequencies. As a result, environmen- misinformation is to discuss all suggestions
tal sounds, and in particular music, will sound one learns about on the Internet with one’s
natural. Because they are custom-fitted for audiologist or doctor.
260 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

Ineffective, insufficient, or dangerous damage to the auditory system, frequency and


treatments for hyperacusis are often endorsed probability are assessed “by the ease with which
by support groups. These include the appli- instances or occurrences can be brought to
cation of low level laser therapy (LLLT) to mind.” Because these topics are often discussed
treat hyperacusis when numerous placebo- on hyperacusis support boards, instances
controlled studies have failed to show efficacy may be recalled more easily, and hence seem
(Klienjung, 2011); a surgical procedure for more numerous to participants than they re-
round and oval window reinforcement that ally are. This is known as the availability heu-
results in minor improvement in LDLs (Sil- ristic (Tversky & Kahneman, 1973, 1974).
verstein et al., 2015); and the use of danger- The representativeness heuristic (Kahne­
ous anti-epileptic drugs to treat hyperacusis man & Tversky, 1972, 1974) is used to make
based on an anecdotal report on the Internet. an intuitive judgment about the probability
The reception these approaches have received of an event “by the degree to which it is sim-
on support sites may be indicative of the mere ilar in essential properties to its parent popu-
exposure effect (Zajonc, 1968), in which re- lation.” In support settings, ear pain is often
peatedly exposing an individual to a stimulus incorrectly assessed as physical damage to the
creates a positive attitude for it. auditory system because the symptoms are
Support groups can be frequented by in- similar to (i.e., representative of ) an individu-
dividuals who have chosen to not get the help al’s mental model of physical damage.
they need to manage hyperacusis. Some never The affect heuristic (Slovic et al., 2002)
get out of the starting blocks, while others is frequently observed on hyperacusis support
become frustrated when setbacks occur and boards, on which people’s judgments and
decide to avoid sound rather than to engage choices are strongly influenced by their emo-
it. This is understandable, as people can be tions. One’s evaluation of sound and the de-
powerfully motivated to protect themselves cisions a person makes to approach or avoid
when they feel threatened. It also explains it are guided by a reliance on feelings rather
why experienced hearing healthcare clinicians than on more deliberate, analytical thinking.
generally advise patients with hyperacusis to Slovic et al. (2002) suggest that “affect may
limit the time spent on support sites or avoid serve as a cue for many important judgments.”
them altogether during treatment.
Three heuristics are frequently encoun-
tered on Internet support settings. A heuris-
tic is a mental short-cut individuals use when
Clinicians
making intuitive judgments about uncertain
events. Heuristics help people to find “ad-
equate, though often imperfect, answers to When desensitization therapy is not helpful
difficult questions” by substituting a question to patients, or not helpful enough, in some
that is easier to answer (Kahneman, 2011). cases it is because the emotions associated with
When participants on a hyperacusis sup- decreased sound tolerance were not identi-
port board evaluate the frequency or prob- fied or not considered an important element
ability of an event, such as the number of of intervention. When patients are asked to
cases in which hyperacusis has not improved consider that there may be an emotional com-
with proper intervention, or the chances that ponent to hyperacusis or ear pain, they some-
exposure to moderate sound levels has caused times dismiss it or view it as an insult. They
15. Hyperacusis Management: A Patient’s Perspective 261

may wonder why it isn’t clear to others that If prospective hearing healthcare clini-
they are describing a purely physical challenge cians take a “by-the-numbers” or “cookbook”
and their responses to it are self-evident and approach to treating hyperacusis, most likely
natural. It is important for clinicians to be it won’t fulfill them and it certainly won’t help
sympathetic to patients in this regard, while at their patients. On the other hand, if clini-
the same time help patients acknowledge the cians place a value on listening to patients,
strong feelings that unavoidably develop about empathizing with and being responsive to
the experience. them, and respectful of them, and are in-
The physiological responses associated vested in making a hard-earned difference in
with hyperacusis are so powerful that it may people’s lives, a significant number of patients
be difficult for patients to understand and with these challenges will benefit from their
attribute the impact of hyperacusis on one’s guidance.
beliefs and decisions. A hyperacusic’s beliefs
about sound can be deep-rooted and often
difficult to change. Hence, it is important for
clinicians to uncover a hyperacusic patient’s
References
beliefs and behaviors regarding exposure to nor-
mal sound. These beliefs, which patients may
be resistant or reluctant to share or challenge, Andersson, G., Lindvall, N., Hursti, T., & Carl-
can influence the decisions patients make with bring, P. (2002). Hypersensitivity to sound
regard to exposing themselves to sound. (hyperacusis). A prevalence study conducted via
Patients should discuss with their provid- the Internet and post. International Journal of
ers any concerns they may have that exposure to Audiology, 41, 545–554.
normal sound can cause permanent damage to Bhimrao, S., Masterson, L., & Baguley, D. (2012).
Systematic review of management strategies for
their auditory system. These questions are not
middle ear myoclonus. Otolaryngology—Head
trivial and a full discussion will likely uncover
and Neck Surgery, 146, 698–706.
fears and concerns about offending sounds Bolte, A., Goschke, T., & Kuhl, J. (2003). Emo-
that are worth exploring. Patients may inter- tion and intuition: Effects of positive and neg-
pret the pain or discomfort they feel around ative mood on implicit judgment of semantic
normal sound to be evidence of damage to the coherence. Psychological Science, 14, 416–421.
auditory system, and they may strongly be- Formby, C., Sherlock, L. P., & Gold, S. C. (2003).
lieve continued exposure to sound will further Adaptive plasticity of loudness induced by
damage their hearing. Cognitive Behavioral chronic attenuation and enhancement of the
Therapy (CBT) may be helpful in challenging acoustic background. Journal of the Acoustical
catastrophic thinking about decreased sound Society of America, 114, 55–58.
tolerance; however, in my view, CBT should Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and de-
cision making. Journal of Economic Perspectives,
be undertaken in concert with a desensitiza-
1(4), 25–42.
tion program that involves the use of broad-
Jastreboff, M. M. & Jastreboff, P. J. (2002). De-
band noise to help re-establish one’s tolerance creased sound tolerance and Tinnitus Retrain-
of sound. In a randomized controlled trial on ing Therapy (TRT). The Australian and New
the use of CBT to treat hyperacusis ( Jüris et al., Zealand Journal of Audiology, 24(2), 74–84.
2014), LDL tests were only administered up to Jastreboff, P. J. & Hazell, J. (2004). Tinnitus re-
4 kHz and improvement in LDLs was less than training therapy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
10 dB in both ears. University Press.
262 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

Jastreboff, P. J. & Jastreboff, M. M. (2014). Treat- handset users. Neuro-otological Society of Aus-
ments for decreased sound tolerance (hyper­ tralia Annual Conference, Melbourne.
acusis and misophonia). Seminars in Hearing, Phillips, D. P. & Carr, M. M. (1998). Distur-
35(2), 205–120. bances of loudness perception. Journal of the
Jüris, L., Andersson, G., Larsen, H., & Ekselius, L. American Academy of Audiology, 9, 371–379.
(2014). Cognitive behaviour therapy for hyper- Radomskij, P., Schmidt, M. A., Heron, C. W., &
acusis: A randomized controlled trial. Behaviour Prasher, D. (2002). Effect of MRI noise on co-
Research and Therapy, 54, 30–37. chlear function. The Lancet, 359(9316), 1485–
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking fast and slow. 1486.
New York, NY: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. Ramirez, L. M., Ballesteros, L. E., & Sandoval,
Kahneman, D. & Tversky, A. (1972). Subjective G. P. (2007). Tensor tympani muscle: Strange
probability: A judgment of representativeness. chewing muscle. Medicina Oral Patologia Oral y
Cognitive Psychology, 3, 430–454. Cirugia Bucal, 12, E96–E100.
Klein, A. J., Armstrong, B. L., Greer, M. K., & Rubinstein, B., Ahlqwist, M., & Bengtsson, C.
Brown III, F. R. (1990). Hyperacusis and otitis (1996). Hyperacusis, headache, temporoman-
media in individuals with Williams syndrome. dibular disorders and amalgam fillings––an
Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55, epidemiological study. In Reich, G. E. & Ver-
339–344. non, J. A. (Eds.), Proceedings of the Fifth Inter-
Klienjung, T. (2011). Low-level laser therapy. In national Tinnitus Seminar (pp. 657–658). Port-
Moller, A., Langguth, B., De Ridder, D., & Klein- land, OR: American Tinnitus Association.
jung, T. (Eds.), Textbook of tinnitus (pp. 749– Silverstein, H., Wu, Y., & Hagan, S. (2015).
752). New York, NY: Springer. Round and oval window reinforcement for the
Klochoff, I., Lindholm, L., & Westerberg, C. E. treatment of hyperacusis. American Journal of
(1971). Spontaneous impedance fluctuation–– Otolaryngology––Head and Neck Medicine and
a “tensor tympani syndrome” with special ref- Surgery, 36, 158–162.
erence to tension headache. Nordisk Medicine, Slovic, P., Finucane, M., Peters, E., & Mac­
85, 577. Gregor, D. (2002). The affect heuristic. In Gi-
Klochoff, I. (1979). Impedance fluctuation and lovich, T., Griffin, D., & Kahneman, D. (Eds.),
a “tensor tympani syndrome.” In R. Penha & Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive
P. Pizarro (Eds.), Proceedings of the Fourth In- judgment (pp. 397–420). New York: Cambridge
ternational Symposium on Acoustic Impedance University Press.
Measurements (pp. 69–76). Lisbon, Portugal: Stanovich, K. & West, R. (2000). Individual dif-
Universidad Nova de Lisboa. ferences in reasoning: Implications for the ra-
Knobel, K. & Sanchez, T. (2006). Loudness dis- tionality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences,
comfort level in normal hearing individuals. 23, 645–726.
Pró-Fono Revista de Atualizacão Cientifica, 18(1), Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability:
31–40. A heuristic for judging frequency and probabil-
Mathisen, H. (1969). Phonophobia after stape- ity. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 207–232.
dectomy. Acta Oto-Laryngologica (Stockholm), Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment
68, 73–77. under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Sci-
McKenna, L., Baguley, D., & McFerran, D. (2010). ence, 185(4157), 1124–1131.
Living with tinnitus and hyperacusis. London, Tyler, R., Pienkowski, M., Roncancio, E., Jun, H.,
U.K.: Sheldon Press. Brozoski, T., Dauman, N., Coelho, C., Anders-
Morewedge, C. & Kahneman, D. (2010). Associa- son, G., Keiner, A., Cacace, A., Martin, N., &
tive processes in intuitive judgment. Trends in Moore, B. (2014). A review of hyperacusis and
Cognitive Sciences, 14(10), 435–440. future directions: Part 1. Definitions and man-
Patuzzi, R., Milhinch, J., & Doyle, J. (2000). ifestations. American Journal of Audiology, 23,
Acute aural trauma in telephone headset and 402–419.
15. Hyperacusis Management: A Patient’s Perspective 263

Vernon, J. A. & Sanders, B. (2001). Tinnitus ques- and research perspectives (pp. 145–162). San Di-
tions and answers (pp. 132–139). Allyn and ego, CA: Plural.
Bacon. Zajonc, R. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere ex-
Westcott, M. (2016). Middle ear myoclonus and posure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
tonic tensor tympani syndrome. In Baguley, ogy, 9, 1–27.
D. M. & Fagelson, M. (Eds.), Tinnitus: Clinical
16
Hyperacusis:
Past, Present, and Future
Marc Fagelson and David M. Baguley

attention. Issues may include the fact that au-


Introduction
diologists and otologists have been extremely
busy diagnosing and managing hearing loss
Despite patient experiences with hyperacusis (and tinnitus and imbalance) and so have had
and other disorders of sound tolerance re- insufficient time to dedicate to reduced sound
ported for hundreds of years, the conditions tolerance. Also, it may be the case that in pre-
have taken a long time to attract any signifi- vious generations the rigours of everyday life
cant scientific, clinical, or public interest. As were such that decreased sound tolerance was
indicated in Chapter 3, there have been spo- neither a rehabilitative imperative nor a pri-
radic mentions in various categories of liter- ority. Although it may be a modern conceit
ature, but it was only in the early 2000s that that we consider ourselves to have an easier
patient narratives and therapeutic options re- life than previous generations, a patient’s sen-
ceived any substantial attention. One reason sory experiences, both positive and negative,
for the unfortunate lack of interest was that remain by-products of the ubiquitous media,
many current auditory scientists, and audi- entertainment, and occupational advances we
ology or otology clinicians, completed their expect and routinely employ. While emerging
training with nary a mention of decreased technology, modern medical care, and food
sound tolerance. That this situation is im- availability are all great blessings in the pres-
proving is due to a concerted effort, and the ent day, access to these is by no means pre-
pioneering interest of several individuals, dictable, hence the stress imposed by the en-
whose names populate the reference lists of vironment upon individuals can be difficult
each chapter, and who contributed chapters to ascertain. Further, a professional working
to this book. week can presently be longer than at other
It is difficult to know why a symptom times in history, challenging both patients
that can be severe, and is apparently prevalent with sensory disruptions, and the clinicians
in the modern world, has attracted so little charged with addressing their needs. It may

265
266 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

be that hyperacusis is a modern disorder, rela- and hyperacusisreasearch.com, and various


tively rare in previous times, but derived now decreased sound tolerance Facebook commu-
from twenty-first century lifestyles whose qual- nities, indicates that a public conversation is
ity is challenged by the proliferation of stress- underway, and this is unlikely to decrease over
related disorders: without a time machine, or time––as with the noises with which we share
the adventitious discovery of some legacy data the world, the proliferation of patients with
this cannot be ascertained. concerns and the networks that can serve them
The unique and evolving listening re- are rather more likely to increase.
quirements and environmental-sound chal-
lenges faced by many individuals likely contrib-
utes to the problem. Most of us are confronted
numerous times in any day by sounds that are
Where We Are Going
loud, incessant, or annoying; ironically, it is
also the case that sounds appearing as negative
to one person will be equally pleasing to the Several improvements must be made with re-
next. The experience of sitting in traffic lis- spect to the clinical assessment of disordered
tening to another driver’s music is common; sound tolerance. Greater consensus than cur-
if the music is enjoyed, then the two drivers rently exists regarding measures of loudness in
may share a smile and nod of the head, if the particular must be developed and implemented
music is not enjoyed, then windows are raised if we are to distinguish auditory pathologies,
along with associated anger. Alarms and sirens particularly those to which other clinical tests
are intended to remind us of tasks or warn us are insensitive, from one another. The repli-
of approaching danger, and these sounds are cability of such measures must be addressed,
intended to be the center of attention, they and improvements would facilitate audiolo-
are designed for maximum detectability (Pat- gists maximizing the benefit of hearing aids
terson, 1990). It is no surprise that some indi- for patients with sound tolerance issues, and to
viduals find the sounds irritating; indeed, this detect changes over time, perhaps as linked to
irritating nature is intentional and contributes intervention, for the patients served.
to the difficulty one has ignoring them. If indi- The proliferation of smartphone appli-
viduals with normal tolerance limits find such cations to measure hearing, along with the in-
sounds intrusive, then we, as clinicians, must creased availability of over the counter hearing
consider the powerful effect the sounds would aids and personal sound amplification prod-
have on an individual with hypersensitivity or ucts suggests that patients who would in the
a low tolerance. While not all environmental past have accessed audiologic rehabilitation
sounds serve the purpose of a warning signal, through an audiologist may in the near future
the tendency over time appears to favor the try to manage their hearing health care needs
public use of attention-grabbing sound, not on their own. The probability is high, how-
just for caution, but for advertising and recre- ever, that these individuals will seek services
ation. The ubiquity and obnoxiousness of such to maximize the benefit provided by their ge-
noise compels many people to avoid situations neric devices at some point. This statement is
and places. not intended as an indictment of self-fitting
It is only recently that people with amplification, and audiologists will likely be
hyperacusis are finding, or being allowed, their compelled to walk a fine line between dismiss-
voice in public. The success and popularity ing the self-fitting process and patients who
of communities based around hyperacusis.net, practice it altogether with offering support and
16. Hyperacusis: Past, Present, and Future 267

assistance to those patients (who may have re- patients may find themselves asking an audi-
jected the audiologists’ help initially). Clearly, ologist or hearing instrument specialist about
as with any clinical endeavor, many things can fitting parameters that could be adjusted to
go wrong with a patient testing their own hear- improve the device’s effectiveness. Here it will
ing and self-fitting a hearing aid. The situation be essential for the audiologist to advise and
is exacerbated by the lack of orientation and support the patient even in the possible cir-
counselling that comprise an essential element cumstance that the patient declined hearing
of audiologic rehabilitation, but that would aids from the audiologist in the past.
not be available to such patients, unless an au- Hearing health care for patients with dis­
diologist with an interest in rehabilitation steps orders of sound tolerance must incorporate
up to help. Of particular concern for such pa- effective counseling and strategies to ease a
tients would be the possibility that their assis- patient’s adjustment to amplification. All pa­
tive device’s effectiveness would be minimized tients receiving amplification benefit from
because of an obscure and difficult-to-detect such counseling, although some patients ad-
sound tolerance problem. just to personal amplification relatively easily.
As indicated in Chapter 2, reliable mea- Our concern is for the patient who, with the
sures of loudness tolerance remain a challenge best of intentions, acquires a product that
for routine clinical practice; for a self-fit hear- does not initially suit their needs. Audiolo-
ing aid, a patient may be unaware of loudness gists will adhere to their professional respon-
tolerance issues, have no accurate or replicable sibilities and provide care and information in
test to obtain such information, and therefore these cases, or they will be brushed aside as the
endure a fitting with a device whose benefit patient either gives up on the device or seeks
will be limited from the start. A patient expe- help elsewhere. Clearly, the interests of patient
riencing such problems will have options, of and provider are most reasonably met when
course. They can return the device, or, because the audiologist helps the patient obtain their
its price was modest to begin with, they can money’s worth, even if the device’s purchase is
use the device sparingly without experiencing not from the audiologist. While this situation
undue remorse that might accompany the may be most common in the United States, it
purchase of something expensive, but little serves as a reminder that it may be our coun-
used. Limiting use of amplification, especially seling and communication skills, our ability
for a person with loudness tolerance issues, to troubleshoot fitting problems, and our
may exert a negative influence on desensiti- understanding of technology and its interface
zation to various sound levels, as described in with our patients that confirms our value as
Chapters 7 and 9. Some degree of desensiti- an essential element of the patient’s hearing
zation would be expected to occur following health care. This book’s intent is to improve
routine exposures to amplified sound; ideally, the clinician’s understanding of the varieties
such exposures would, over time, ameliorate and manifestations of disordered sound toler-
some of the loudness tolerance problems. Un- ance as one way to improve the audiologist’s
fortunately, the patient can wear the device, ability to provide information, counseling, and
but at output levels too low to provide ade- adaptation strategies for patients experiencing
quate amplification or consistent benefit; in difficulty with amplification.
such cases, patients would be likely to mini- Hearing protection devices present sim-
mize device use as well. ilar challenges in that their use requires care-
Another option would focus on seek- ful consideration of the patient’s motivation,
ing help from a professional. In this instance, strategies, and rationale for protecting hearing.
268 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

Patients are clearly in need of accurate infor- Cochlear implantation influences a pa-
mation regarding options, benefits, and lim- tient in many ways, and it may be that train-
itations related to hearing protectors. As noise ing derived from various psychophysical test
levels and warning signals in the environment strategies can improve outcomes with the de-
increase, we can be certain that a proportion vices. The pitch changes over time reported
of the population will experience aversive by Reiss and colleagues (2011, 2016) and in
responses during routine occupational and Chapter 12 are reminders that, even in dras-
recreational activities. If patients are willing tically modified peripheral systems, the same
to test their own hearing to purchase ampli- rules of central adaptation and compensation
fication devices, then it is certain that they that apply to normal-hearing individuals may
will continue to make their own decisions be accessed by cochlear implant users, hence
regarding hearing protection. As discussed in the potential value in honing assessments that
Chapters 3, 6, 7, and 8, hearing protection are sensitive to such change.
can be overused and exacerbate the negative Hyperacusis will likely receive more at-
sound experience that triggers their use in the tention in the lab and literature in the future.
first place. This situation should be avoidable Therefore, its use as a term may require more
in most cases; however, providers must have specificity and care than is exercised at present,
an understanding of the neural mechanisms at a concept advanced by several investigators, Ty-
work in order to counsel effectively when pa- ler et al. (2014) for example, and as discussed
tients experience loudness tolerance issues that in Chapter 3. In particular, the use of hyper­
restrict dynamic range and, by extension, ac- acusis to connote extreme sensitivity regarding
tivities the patient might routinely carry out. absolute threshold should be reconsidered. One
Another issue relates to the various pa- disorder in particular, superior semicircular
tient groups that experience frequent expo- canal dehiscence (SSCD), has as the typical
sures to some of the most toxic environmen- clinical sign of unusually low bone-conduction
tal sounds. Emergency medical technicians, thresholds, particularly in the low frequen-
firefighters, officers of the law, and military cies. The thresholds often appear as clinically
populations have unique exposures that re- relevant air-bone gaps despite normal hearing
quire hearing protection; however, the rigors via air conduction testing. This situation, de-
of the jobs and frequent need to communi- scribed in Chapter 6, has been termed “con-
cate may contraindicate routine use of pro- ductive hyperacusis” (e.g., Minor, 2005), in
tective devices or earplugs. Little is known which the hyperacusis label indicates better
regarding the prevalence of workers whose than average sensitivity rather than what we
sound intolerance compels them to retire or would suggest as the preferred use, connoting
find new employment rather than continuing loudness intolerance. Perhaps hypersensitivity
work in these important areas. An additional or hypersensitive bone-conduction thresholds
consideration relates to the risk members of could be applied to patients’ results during
these populations face regarding concussion SSCD testing; the possibility that a patient with
or traumatic brain injury (TBI). Patients recov- SSCD could experience both conductive hyper­
ering from TBI often experience hypersensitiv- acusis, meaning extremely sensitive bone-
ities to a variety of sensory inputs, particularly conduction thresholds, and loudness hyper­
visual, auditory, and olfactory inputs, whose acusis simultaneously should be motivation
effects are exacerbated when unexpected, or enough to clarify terminology and usage.
when serving as reminders of traumatic expo- One example of an opportunity to im-
sures (Alwis et al., 2012). prove matters would relate to the development
16. Hyperacusis: Past, Present, and Future 269

of a clinic practice guideline for the manage- behavioral, and clinical science. While the fact
ment of patients with disordered sound toler- that the research questions are so plentiful,
ance. The American Academy of Otolaryngol- and so broad is an indication that our knowl-
ogy offers dozens of well-crafted and thorough edge regarding hyperacusis really is sparse, it is
practice guidelines covering specialty areas from also an indication that this is an area in which
pediatric middle ear management to benign research opportunities abound.
paroxysmal positional vertigo, to tinnitus. At Unanswered research questions in disor-
present, no guideline is offered for disorders of dered sound tolerance (modified from Bagu-
sound tolerance, although hyperacusis is men- ley and Hoare, in press):
tioned in the guidelines for tinnitus (Tunkel
et al., 2014) and Bell’s palsy (Baugh et al., 2013). • What is the prevalence of hyperacusis
It is possible that the physicians and prac- in adults and children?
titioners who contribute to practice guide- • What are the risk factors associated
line development are confounded by patient with hyperacusis?
complaints of disordered sound tolerance. The • What is the natural history of
literature regarding sound-evoked pain, for ex- hyperacusis?
ample, as summarized in Chapter 9, is in its • How is “pain hyperacusis” perceived?
infancy and may be years from translation into • What mechanisms are involved in
clinical practice. In this regard, the current hyperacusis?
state of disordered sound tolerance manage- • What is the relation between
ment resembles that associated with tinnitus hyperacusis and tinnitus?
management some 10 to 15 years ago; a vari- • Can a questionnaire be developed
ety of clinical approaches with minimal sup- that accurately measures the impact
porting evidence applied to a distressed popu- of hyperacusis, and can be used as a
lation of patients who struggle to get answers treatment outcome measure?
or assistance from their traditional medical • What treatments, alone or in
sources. Professionals in our field must com- combination, are effective for
mit to improving the situation, and the devel- hyperacusis?
opment of a practice guideline would compel
communication between physicians, audiolo- A multidisciplinary approach to hyperacusis re-
gists, and psychologists with the ultimate goal search would be valuable, as the tinnitus field
of standardizing terminology, diagnoses, and has benefited from an additional linkage (Hall
components of patient care. et al., 2013), drawing patients into the discus-
sion about tinnitus research through a formal
process led by the James Lind Alliance in the
United Kingdom (http://www.jla.nihr.ac.uk
Research Priorities
/priority-setting-partnerships/tinnitus/). The re­
sult of the process was a list of research prior-
From the issues raised above it will be clear ities in tinnitus, with the aim of increasing the
that there are many opportunities for research traction of funding applications for tinnitus re-
into hyperacusis. A summary of those research search, with independent verification that the
questions deemed to be priorities was pro- topic chosen was of both scientific and pub­
posed by Baguley and Hoare (in press), and lic interest. A similar program is underway for
can be found in modified form in the bulleted hyperacusis, again involving the James Lind Al-
list below. It will be seen that these span basic, liance, and led by Dr. Kathryn Fackrell in the
270 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

United Kingdom. Additionally, the growing


dialogue between patient communities such as
References
those based around the Hyperacusis Network
and Hyperacusis Research and the scientific and Alwis, D. S., Yan, E. B., Morganti-Kossmann,
clinical communities is welcome. M.-C., & Rajan, R. Sensory cortex underpin-
Two things are missing, however. The nings of traumatic brain injury deficits. PLoS
first is the absence of any formal patient orga- ONE 7(12), e52169. https://doi.org/10.1371
nization, such as exist for tinnitus (the Amer- /journal.pone.0052169.
ican and British Tinnitus Associations, for Baguley, D. M., & Hoare, D. J. Hyperacusis: major
example): these perform an invaluable role in research questions. HNO, in press
Baugh, R. F., Basura, G. J., Ishii, L. E., Schwartz,
patient advice, support, and advocacy, and in
S. R., . . . Vaughan, W. (2013). Clinical prac-
fundraising for research. While these tinnitus tice guideline: Bell’s palsy. Otolaryngology—
associations acknowledge that there is some Head and Neck Surgery, 149, 3 suppl, S1–S27.
overlap between tinnitus and hyperacusis, nei- Hall, D., Mohamad, N., Firkins, L., Fenton, M., &
ther has sought a significant role with hyper­ Stockdale, D. (2013). Identifying and priori-
acusis patients, and there is no indication that tizing unmet research questions for people with
this is likely to develop. tinnitus: The James Lind Alliance Tinnitus Pri-
The second missing element is that of a ority Setting Partnership. Clinical Investigation,
substantial funding stream, such as is avail- 3, 1, 21–28, doi: 10.4155/cli.12.129.
able for hearing loss, and becoming more Minor, L. B. (2005). Clinical manifestations of
available for tinnitus. The absence of research superior semicircular canal dehiscence. The La-
ryngoscope, 115, 1717–1727. doi:10.1097/01
resources will hamper progress in hyperacusis
.mlg.0000178324.55729.b7.
until this is remedied.
Patterson, R. (1990). Auditory warning sounds
in the work environment. Philos. Trans. R. Soc.
Lond. B Biol. Sci., 327(1241), 485–492.
Closing Words Reiss, L. A. J., Lowder, M. W., Karsten, S. A.,
Turner, C. W., & Gantz, B. J. (2011). Effects of
extreme tonotopic mismatches between bilat-
While opportunities are evident for progress eral cochlear implants on electric pitch percep-
in understanding the mechanisms of hyper- tion: a case study. Ear Hear., 32(4), 536–540.
acusis, the natural history, and in developing doi:10.1097/AUD.0b013e31820c81b0.
optimal treatments, some significant con- Reiss, L. A. J., Eggleston, J. L., Walker, E. P., &
straints are also apparent. It is our hope that Oh, Y. (2016). Two ears are not always better
in editing this book we will have contributed than one: mandatory vowel fusion across spec-
trally mismatched ears in hearing-impaired lis-
toward the embedding of hyperacusis into
teners. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryng., 17, 342–356.
the mainstream of audiology, and of auditory doi: 10.1007/s10162-016-0570-z.
neuroscience, which is essential if the field is Tunkel, D. E., Bauer, C. A., Sun, G. H., et al.
to develop. Patients with disorders of sound (2014). Clinical practice guideline: Tinnitus.
tolerance are numerous, and the burden on Otolaryngology—Head and Neck Surgery, 151,
them as individuals, their families, and upon S1–S40. doi: 10.1177/0194599814545325.
the societies in which they live, is substantial. Tyler, R. S., Pienkowski, M., Roncancio, E. R., . . . ,
If the reader is motivated to contribute to the Moore, B. C. J. (2014). A review of hyperacusis
development of this fascinating and important and future directions: Part I. Definitions and
field, then we will have succeeded in our aims. manifestations. Am. J. Aud., (23), 402–419.
Index

Note: Page numbers in bold reference non-text material.

A Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, 110,


112, 120
AAF. See Anterior auditory field Audiogenic seizures, 141–143
ABR. See Auditory brainstem response Audiometers, 5
Acceptable noise level, 227 Auditory brainstem response, 27, 83
Acoustic reflex, 68 Auditory evoked potentials, 155–156
Acoustic reflex threshold testing, 25 Auditory nerve fibers
Acoustic shock syndrome, 107, 109, 116 efferent feedback failure effects on, 68–69
Acoustic startle response, 134, 144 high-spontaneous rate, 86
Addison’s disease, 110, 121 high-threshold, 83–85
Adolescents. See also Children inner nerve cells and, 83
hyperacusis in, 38, 96 low-threshold, 83, 85, 88, 140
tinnitus in, 96 in tinnitus, 86
Adrenocortical insufficiency, 110, 121 type I fibers, 63–68
Adult Sensitivity Questionnaire, 53 type II, 63, 71–72, 143–144
Affect heuristic, 260 Auditory neurons, 86–87
AGS. See Audiogenic seizures Auditory processing deficits, 152, 156
ALSPAC. See Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents Auditory Processing Disorders, 168, 227
and Children Auditory steady-state response, 27
American Academy of Otolaryngology, 269 Auditory system disorders, 212
American Tinnitus Association, 270 Autism spectrum disorders, 119–120, 186, 212
A-MISO-S. See Amsterdam Misophonia Scale Autoimmune conditions, 111, 122
Amsterdam Misophonia Scale, 54 Avoidance, 169–170, 216
ANF. See Auditory Nerve Fibers Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and
ANL. See Acceptable noise level Children, 38
Annoyance hyperacusis
definition of, 12
description of, 107
B
misophonia and, 185 Background sound, 258
Anterior auditory field, 135 Back/joint/muscle disorder, 112, 122
Anxiety, 22, 168, 184 Basilar membrane, 6, 64, 65, 192
APD. See Auditory Processing Disorders Bat-and-ball puzzle, 255
Applied relaxation, 171, 174 Behavioral activation, 172
ART. See Acoustic reflex threshold Bell’s palsy, 115, 269
ASD. See Autism spectrum disorders Benzodiazepine withdrawal, 111, 124
ASQ. See Adult Sensitivity Questionnaire Bimodal auditory-somatosensory treatments, for
ASSR. See Auditory steady-state response tinnitus and hyperacusis, 94–95

271
272 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

Bluetooth, 230 systematic desensitization for, 217


British Tinnitus Association, 270 temporary auditory deprivation as cause of,
Broadband noise, 248, 248, 258 211
with tinnitus, 184
wideband noise generators for, 215
C immature but normally developing auditory
CANS. See Central auditory nervous system system in, 211
Carotid aneurysm, 111, 123 loudness perception in, 211
CBT. See Cognitive-behavioral treatment otitis media with effusion in, 211
Central auditory nervous system, 151 sensory overresponsivity in, 212
Central auditory processing tests, 155 temporary auditory deprivation in, 211
Central auditory system tinnitus in, 184
in children, 212 Chronic fatigue syndrome, 112, 118–119
description of, 249 CJD. See Creutzfeldt Jacob disease
hyperactivity in, 138 Classical conditioning, 169
peripheral damage effects on, 70 Client Orientated Scale of Improvement, 227
Central gain, 79, 134–137, 144, 212 Clinical practice guidelines, 269
Cerebrovascular disease, 119 Cluster headache, 118
Cervicogenic headache, 118 Cochlear damage
Child Friendly Tinnitus Model, 214 diplacusis caused by, 196–198
Children. See also Adolescents hearing impairment secondary to, 197
acceptable noise level in, 227 noise trauma as cause of, 84
auditory system disorders in, 212 type I auditory nerve fibers affected by,
decreased sound tolerance in, 26–27 66–68
hyperacusis in Cochlear excitation pattern, 64
age ranges for, 208 Cochlear filter bank models, 64
assessment of, 213–214 Cochlear hair cells
audiological assessment of, 213–214 inner. See Inner hair cells
auditory system disorder as cause of, 212 noise- or age-related damage to, 66–68
case examples of, 215–216, 218–219 outer. See Outer hair cells
categorization of, 210–213 Cochlear hearing loss, 7
causes of, 210–213 Cochlear implantation, 198–199, 268
cognitive-behavioral therapy for, 216 Cochlear synaptopathy, 23, 66, 68, 85
conditions associated with, 208, 210 Cogan’s syndrome, 111, 122
description of, 38–39, 207–208 Cognitive-behavioral therapy, 167, 170–175,
desensitization approaches for, 216–218 172, 216, 250, 261
immature but normally developing auditory Complex regional pain syndrome, 118
system as cause of, 211 Conductive decreased sound tolerance, 16
management of, 214–215 Conductive hearing loss
prevalence of, 208 surgical correction of, 116–117
psychological approaches, 216–218 traumatic brain injury and, 151
relaxation strategies for, 215 Conductive hyperacusis, 115, 268
sensory processing disorder as cause of, Contralateral masking, 26
212–213 Cortical tracts, 152
sound therapy for, 215 COSI. See Client Orientated Scale of
studies of, 208, 209 Improvement
summary of, 219 Creutzfeldt Jacob disease, 111, 125
symptoms of, 210 Cri du Chat syndrome, 107, 109, 114
Index 273

prevalence of, 17, 61


D
problems associated with, 167–169
DAI. See Diffuse axonal injury psychological conditions associated with, 167
DCN. See Dorsal cochlear nucleus questionnaires for, 26, 47–54
Decreased sound tolerance somatic complaints associated with, 21
activities of daily living affected by, 21 sounds considered problematic for patients
anxiety effects on, 22 with, 19
assessment of stress effects on, 22
acoustic reflex threshold tests for, 25 subcategories of, 20
case history used in, 18–22 sudden-onset, 19
electrophysiological tests for, 27 tinnitus and, 16–19, 22, 28, 55, 168
future developments in, 27 uncomfortable loudness level measurements of,
in hearing aid fitting protocol, 226–227 43–47, 54–55
immittance, 25 Depression
loudness discomfort levels for, 24, 61 decreased sound tolerance and, 22
otoacoustic emissions, 25–26 hyperacusis and, 110, 112, 120, 168
pure tone audiometry for, 23 Desensitization therapy, 215–218, 248, 258,
questionnaires used in, 26, 47–54 260, 267
rationale for, 15–18 Diary, 171
audiometric measures of, 43–47 Diffuse axonal injury, 152
auditory conditions/disorders associated with, Diplacusis
167–169 case studies of, 200–203
avoidance behaviors, 21 causes of, 195–199
background on, 167 central mechanisms of, 198–199
behaviors associated with, 15 cochlear damage as cause of, 196–198
case examples of, 27–30 definition of, 191
case history for, 18–22 hearing loss associated with, 199
in children, 26–27 inflammatory disorders and, 196
clinical features of, 62 management of, 199–200
conductive, 16 in musicians, 195
definition of, 15 prevalence of, 191–192
depression and, 22 Diplacusis binauralis, 191
diagnostic significance of, 16 Diplacusis dysharmonia, 191
hearing aids for. See Hearing aids Diplacusis monauralis, 191
hearing loss and, 23 Directional microphones, 229
hearing protection for, 21–22 Distortion product otoacoustic emissions, 25,
hyperacusis. See Hyperacusis 133, 135, 137, 140
identification of, reasons for, 17–18 Dorsal cochlear nucleus
literary depictions of, 7 description of, 84
loudness discomfort levels for, 24, 61, 71 feedforward circuitry in, 89
loudness perception changes with, 61 fusiform cells in, 87, 90
loudness scaling, 46 hypersynchrony in, 88
misophonia. See Misophonia DPOAEs. See Distortion product otoacoustic
most comfortable listening level and, 46 emissions
novel surgical strategy for, 126–127 Dravet syndrome, 143
onset of, 19 Drug side effects, 111, 124
otological conditions with, 16 Drug withdrawal, 111, 124
patient’s motivation and readiness to deal with, 21 DST. See Decreased sound tolerance
274 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

GÜF. See German Questionnaire on


E Hypersensitivity to Sound
Ear defenders, 214
Ear pain, 62–64, 143–144, 250–251
H
Earplugs
for children with hyperacusis, 214 HADS. See Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale
loudness tolerance changes associated with, 8 Head injury, 119
nonlinear, 228 Headaches
tinnitus affected by, 82 cervicogenic, 118
weaning patients from, 228 cluster, 118
Elastin gene, 113 migraine, 112, 117–118
Electromagnetic hypersensitivity, 111, 126 tension-type, 118
Emotions, 249–251 Hearing
Endocrine conditions, 110, 121 physiology of, 83
Enhanced acoustic environment, 135, 136 place theory of, 192–194
“Envelope following response,” 85 Hearing aids
Environmental intolerance, 111, 126 case examples of, 234–236
Environmental sounds clinical history taking for, 225–226
pitch perception mechanisms, 192 combination devices, 230
tinnitus and hyperacusis comorbidity treated conservative fitting of, 234
with low levels of, 93–94 counseling for, 227–228
toxic, 268 datalogging, 230
Exhaustion syndrome, 112 description of, 223–224
Exposure, 170–171, 174, 217 diplacusis managed with, 199
directional microphones, 229
fitting protocol for, 17, 224–234
F goal setting for, 227–228
Fear hyperacusis, 12, 107 needs assessment for, 227–228
Fear-avoidance model, 169 noise reduction with, 229
Fear-provoked avoidance, 216 open-fit, 230
Fibromyalgia, 111–112, 123 output sound pressure level for, 229
Fight or flight response, 211 programs for, 230
Frequency modulation systems, 156 psychoeducation, 227
rate of return for, 17
remote microphones, 229
G selection of, 228–230
GABAA receptors, 92 self-fitting of, 267
GAD-7. See Generalized Anxiety Disorder-7 transient impulse control, 229
Gamma oscillations, 92 transition to tolerance, 234
Generalized anxiety disorder, 112 Hearing impairment, 112
Generalized Anxiety Disorder-7, 22 Hearing loss
Genetic disorders, 109, 113–114 conductive
German Questionnaire on Hypersensitivity to surgical correction of, 116–117
Sound, 52–53 traumatic brain injury as cause of, 151
Glycinergic inhibition, 87–88 decreased sound tolerance and, 23
GM1 gangliosidoses, 109, 114 in diplacusis, 199
GM2 gangliosidoses, 109, 113–114 hearing aid fitting protocol for, 224–234
Growth hormone deficiency, 110, 121 “hidden,” 84
Index 275

hyperacusis and hearing loss and, 66, 138


description of, 66, 114–115, 138 hearing thresholds in, 36
tinnitus and, 83–86 homeostatic plasticity in, 89–91, 95
sensorineural, 138, 151, 197 loudness recruitment and, 141
tinnitus and, 224 mechanisms of, 89–92
Hearing protection model of, 12
by decreased sound tolerance patients, 21–22 multidisciplinary approach to, 269
description of, 6, 267–268 neural correlates of, 133–134
in hyperacusis patients, 7 overlapping of, 108
indications for, 228, 258–259 patient’s perspective on, 241–263
loudness tolerance reduced with, 8 physiological responses to, 261
overuse of, 259, 268 prevalence of, 61, 113, 208
Hebbian plasticity, 89 psychological mechanisms of, 169–170,
Heuristics, 260 184
HEXA gene, 113 quality of life effects, 227
HEXB gene, 113 recruitment versus, 78–79
High-spontaneous rate auditory nerve fibers, 86 salicylate-induced, 24
Homeostatic plasticity, 89–91, 95, 137 serotonin and, 141
Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale, 22, 26, time course of development for, 79–83
126, 171 tinnitus and
HQ. See Hyperacusis Questionnaire in adolescents, 96
HSP. See Homeostatic plasticity bimodal auditory-somatosensory treatments
5-Hydroxytryptamine. See Serotonin for, 94–95
Hyperacusis in children, 184, 208
in adolescents, 38 convergence between, 184–185
animal models of, 133–147 description of, 114–115
assessment of, 107 divergence between, 185–187
avoidance strategies in, 169–170 environmental sound exposure for, 93–94
in children. See Children, hyperacusis in epidemiology of, 181–184
classification of, 12–13, 107–108 extracochlear mechanisms in, 85
clinical practice guidelines for, 269 hearing loss as initiating condition in,
cognitive-behavioral treatment for, 167, 83–86
170–175, 172, 216, 261 literature regarding, 183–184
comorbidities associated with, 109–112, loudness discomfort levels in, 78
114–127 loudness growth functions in, 78
conditions associated with, 108, 109–112, mechanisms of, 89–92
208, 210 overview of, 77–78
conference reports on, 34 prevalence of, 78, 112, 182–183, 208
definitions of, 9, 34, 78, 182, 207–208 summary of, 95–96
depression and, 110, 112, 120, 168 time course of development for, 79–83
description of, 7–10, 133 treatment of, 93–95
diagnosis of, 244–246 treatment of, 246–248, 248–249
diseases and disorders associated with, 108, types of, 12–13, 107–108
109–112, 208, 210 word origin of, 8–9
distress associated with, 13 Hyperacusis dolorosa, 9
epidemiology of, 34–39 Hyperacusis Focus website, 108
exposure for, 170–171, 174 Hyperacusis gain, 70
frequency range of, 96 Hyperacusis Network, 256
276 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

Hyperacusis Questionnaire, 26, 47–51, 171, 173, description of, 226, 247, 257–258
186 earplugs effect on, 8
Hyperthyroidism, 110, 121 hyperacusis level and, 45, 78
opting of testing for, 27
techniques for establishing, 24
I tinnitus and, 16
Iatrogenic conditions, 111, 125–126 Loudness growth functions, 78
IEDs. See Improvised explosive devices Loudness growth pattern, 6
Immittance, 25 Loudness hyperacusis
Improvised explosive devices, 149 definition of, 12
Infective conditions, 111, 124–126 description of, 107
Inner ear, 68 peripheral mechanisms of, 64–65, 70–71
Inner hair cells Loudness perception
description of, 6, 66–67, 139 in children, 211
outer hair cells and, 140 description of, 64
Inner nerve cells, 83 Loudness recruitment. See also Recruitment
Internal Gain, 7 auditory nerve fibers in, 140
Intuition, 255–257 central hyperactivity, 138–139
Irritable bowel syndrome, 112, 124 cochlear hearing loss as cause of, 7
description of, 5–6
hyperacusis and, 141
J natural history of, 33
James Lind Alliance, 269 peripheral correlates of, 140–141
prevalence of, 33
Loudness scaling, 46
L Loudness tolerance
Labyrinthine edema, 196 behavioral measures of, 24
Lateral amygdala, 142 determination of, 23
Lateral olivocochlear bundle earplugs and, 8
description of, 68 hearing protection for, 8
feedback from, 69 measurement of, 24
Lateral semicircular canal dehiscence, 109, 116 measures of, 6
LDLs. See Loudness discomfort levels upper limit of, 6
Learning disability, 110, 120 Low level laser therapy, 260
LFP. See Local field potentiation LTD. See Long-term depression
LLLT. See Low level laser therapy LTP. See Long-term potentiation
Local field potentiation, 134–135, 137, 138 Lyme disease, 111, 124–125
Long-term depression, 89
Long-term potentiation, 89
Loudness
M
behavioral measures of, 23–25 Magnesium deficiency, 111, 122
neural correlates of, 133–134 MAQ. See Misophonia Assessment Questionnaire
Loudness Discomfort Level test, 173, 257–258 MASH. See Multiple-Activity Scale for Hyperacusis
Loudness discomfort levels. See ULL Maslach burnout inventory general survey, 50
assessment of, 36–37, 81 MBI-GS. See Maslach burnout inventory general
audiometric measures of, 43–47 survey
in Bell’s palsy patients, 115 MCL. See Most comfortable listening level
decreased sound tolerance assessments using, MCR. See Misophonia Coping Responses
24, 61, 71 Medial geniculate body, 92
Index 277

Medial olivocochlear bundle Neuromodulators, 90


description of, 68 Neurosyphilis, 111, 125
inner ear feedback from, 69 Noise
Meniere’s disease, 107, 109, 116 broadband, 248, 248, 258
Mental health disorders. See also Depression headphones for travel
screening tools for, 22 hearing aids for reduction of, 229
sound tolerance and, 22 pink, 246, 256
MER. See Misophonia Emotional Response survey sensitivity to, in children, 38
Metabolic conditions, 111, 121–122 temporary threshold shift caused by, 83, 86
MGB. See Medial geniculate body Noise cancellation headphones for travel, 259
Middle cerebral artery aneurysm, 111, 123–124 Nonlinear earplugs, 228
Middle ear reflex, 68 Non-specific building-related symptoms, 111, 126
Middle latency response, 27
Migraine, 112, 117–118
O
Military Personnel, 149
Misophonia OAEs. See Otoacoustic emissions
behaviors associated with, 108 Open-fit hearing aids, 230
characteristics of, 40 OSPL. See Output sound pressure level
cognitive-behavioral therapy for, 173 Otitis media with effusion, 211
definition of, 107–108, 169, 185 Otoacoustic emissions
description of, 10–11, 19 decreased sound tolerance assessments using,
epidemiology of, 39–41 25–26
hyperacusis versus, 185–186 diplacusis evaluations, 197–198
natural history of, 40 distortion product, 25, 133, 135, 137, 140
psychological mechanisms of, 169 transient evoked, 26
questionnaires for, 53–54 Outer hair cells
Misophonia Assessment Questionnaire, 54 description of, 5, 33, 63
Misophonia Coping Responses, 54 function of, 66
Misophonia Emotional Response survey, 54 inner hair cells and, 140
Misophonia Questionnaire, 53–54 medial olivocochlear bundle feedback to, 68
MLR. See Middle latency response Output sound pressure level, 229
Monoamine oxidase inhibitor withdrawal, 111,
124
P
Most comfortable listening level, 46, 227
Motivational interviewing, 171 Pain hyperacusis
MQ. See Misophonia Questionnaire animal model of, 72
Multiple chemical sensitivity, 111, 126 definition of, 12, 20
Multiple sclerosis, 118 description of, 107, 117
Multiple-Activity Scale for Hyperacusis, 51–52, 182 peripheral mechanism for, 62–64
Musculoskeletal conditions, 111, 122–123 symptoms of, 20
Music protocol, 251–255, 252–254 type II auditory nerve fibers and, 143–144
Musical tones, 195 Pain threshold, 62
Musicogenic seizures, 143 Pan-hypopituitarism, 110, 121
Myasthenia gravis, 118 Panic disorder, 112, 121
Patient Health Questionnaire-9, 22
Paul of Aegina, 9
N PCS. See Postconcussion syndrome
Neural synchrony, 135, 138 Perilymph fistula, 117
Neurodevelopmental conditions, 110, 119–120 Permanent threshold shift, 87
278 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

Phenelzine, 124 hyperacusis versus, 78–79


Phonophobia improvements in, 108
definition of, 107, 117 Relaxation, 171, 174, 215
description of, 10–11 Remote microphones, 229
migrainous, 117 Representativeness heuristic, 260
PHQ-9. See Patient Health Questionnaire-9 Research, 269–270
Pink noise, 246, 256 Residual inhibition, 93
Pitch Retigabine, 72
musical tones and, 195 Robinson, Ruby, 7
place theory, 192–194
temporal theory of, 193–194
S
Pitch perception
impairments in, 195 Sandhoff disease, 109, 113
mechanisms of, 192–195 Scandinavia, 35
stimulus intensity effects on, 193 SCD. See Superior canal dehiscence
Plasticity Seizures, audiogenic, 141–143
homeostatic, 89–91, 95, 137 Selective sound sensitivity syndrome, 19
spike-timing dependent, 88–89, 91, 94 Self-fit hearing aid, 267
Poland, 34, 183 Sensitivity
Pontine reticular formation, 142 behavioral threshold of, 23
Postconcussion syndrome, 150 noise, in children, 38
Post-traumatic stress disorder, 112, 119, 150, 153 Sensorineural hearing loss, 138, 151, 197
Primary auditory cortex, 138, 152 Sensory gating, 211
Psychoeducation, 227 Sensory overresponsivity, 212
Psychological conditions, 110, 120–121 Sensory processing disorder, 212–213
PTS. See Permanent threshold shift Serotonin, 141, 212
Pure tone audiometry, for decreased sound Sick building syndrome, 111, 126
tolerance assessments, 23, 36 SLE. See Systemic lupus erythematosus
Pyridoxine deficiency, 111, 121–122 SLM. See Sound-level-meter
Smartphones, 266
Sodium salicylate, 141
Q SOR. See Sensory overresponsivity
Quality of life, 227 Sound intolerance, 153–154
Quality of Life Inventory, 173 Sound level tolerance
Questionnaires assessment of, 78
decreased sound tolerance assessment uses of, reduced. See Hyperacusis
26, 47–54 Sound Sensitive Tinnitus Index, 53
German Questionnaire on Hypersensitivity to Sound therapy, 18, 215
Sound, 52–53 Sound tolerance
Hyperacusis, 26, 47–51, 171, 173, 186 audiometric measures of, 43–47
Misophonia, 53–54 most comfortable listening level and, 46
Multiple-Activity Scale for Hyperacusis, 51–52 Sound tolerance disorders
quality criteria for evaluating, 48–49 classification of, 107
hearing health care for, 267
history of, 3–5
R mental health disorders and, 22
Ramsay Hunt syndrome, 109, 115 taxonomy of, 107
Recruitment. See also Loudness recruitment Sound Tolerance Interview and Questionnaire
causes of, 79 Instrument, 26
Index 279

Sound-level-meter, 228 Tensor tympani muscle, 250


Spike-timing dependent plasticity, 88–89, 91, 94 Tensor tympani syndrome, 250
Spina bifida, 118 TEOAE. See Transient evoked otoacoustic
Spinal anesthetic, 125–126 emissions
SPL-O-gram, 232 Thalamocortical dysrhythmia, 92
Spontaneous firing rates of auditory neurons, Thalamus, 92
86–87 THI. See Tinnitus Handicap Inventory
SSCD. See Superior semicircular canal dehiscence THQ. See Tinnitus Handicap Questionnaire
SSSS. See Selective sound sensitivity syndrome Tick-borne disease. See Lyme disease
SSTI. See Sound Sensitive Tinnitus Index Timbre, 195
Stapedectomy, 115 Tinnitus
Stapedius muscle, 115, 250 assessment of, 81
Stapedius reflex. See Acoustic reflex auditory nerve fiber impairment in, 86
abnormalities of, 121 case example of, 27–28
description of, 82, 113 in children, 39, 184
dysfunction of, 109, 115 cortical tonotopic, 96
stuttering and, 120 craniomandibular dysfunction and, 123
Startle response/reflex, 134, 250 decreased sound tolerance and, 16–19, 22, 28,
STDP. See Spike-timing dependent plasticity 55, 168
STIQI. See Sound Tolerance Interview and ear plugging effects on, 82
Questionnaire Instrument gap detection assessment of, 86
Stress, 22 hearing loss and, 224
Stuttering, 110, 120 hyperacusis and
Subcortical tracts, 152 in adolescents, 96
Superior canal dehiscence bimodal auditory-somatosensory treatments
case example of, 28–30, 29 for, 94–95
computed tomography of, 29 in children, 184, 208
description of, 16, 107 convergence between, 184–185
Superior semicircular canal dehiscence, 115–116, description of, 114–115
268 divergence between, 185–187
Support groups, 259–260 environmental sound exposure for, 93–94
Sweden, 34 epidemiology of, 181–184
Syphilis, 111, 125 extracochlear mechanisms in, 85
Systematic desensitization, 216–217 hearing loss as initiating condition in, 83–86
Systemic lupus erythematosus, 111, 122 literature regarding, 182–184
loudness discomfort levels in, 78
loudness growth functions in, 78
T
mechanisms of, 86–89
Tampa Scale of Kinesophobia, 173 overview of, 77–78
Tay-Sachs disease, 109, 113–114 prevalence of, 78, 112, 182–183, 208
TBI. See Traumatic brain injury summary of, 95–96
Temporal theory, of pitch, 193–194 time course of development for, 79–83
Temporary auditory deprivation, 211 treatment of, 93–95
Temporary threshold shift intermittent or transient nature of, 185
murine models of, 134 intuitive thinking about, 256
noise trauma as cause of, 83, 86 mechanisms of, 86–89
Temporomandibular disorder/dysfunction, 20, neural changes associated with, 86–87
111, 122–123 organizations for, 270
Tension-type headache, 118 prevalence of, 78
280 Hyperacusis and Disorders of Sound Intolerance: Clinical and Research Perspectives

Tinnitus (continued ) TRT. See Tinnitus Retraining Therapy


psychological conditions and, 184 TSCHQ. See Tinnitus Sample Case History
somatic modulation of, 185 Questionnaire
sound therapy for, 18 T-stellate cells, 139
time course of development for, 79–83 TTS. See Temporary threshold shift
uncomfortable loudness levels and, 44 TTTS. See Tonic tensor tympani syndrome
Tinnitus Handicap Inventory, 183 Tullio phenomenon, 16, 116
Tinnitus Handicap Questionnaire, 50 Turnbull, Laurence, 9
Tinnitus Questionnaire, 51 Type I auditory nerve fibers
Tinnitus Retraining Therapy, 244–245, 248, cochlear damage effects on, 66–68
255, 259 description of, 63–64, 65
Tinnitus Sample Case History Questionnaire, 55 Type II auditory nerve fibers, 20, 63, 71–72,
TMD. See Temporomandibular disorder/ 143–144
dysfunction Typhoid fever, 111, 125
Tone height, 195 Typhus, 111, 125
Tonic tensor phenomenon, 250
Tonic tensor tympani syndrome, 20, 116, 250
U
TQ. See Tinnitus Questionnaire
Transient evoked otoacoustic emissions, 26 ULLs. See Uncomfortable loudness levels
Traumatic brain injury Uncomfortable loudness levels. See LDL
auditory consequences of, 151–154 in children, 213, 235
auditory cortex damage secondary to, 152 measurement of, 43–47, 54–55, 213, 226, 231
auditory evaluation in, 154–157
auditory evoked potentials in, 155–156
auditory processing deficits secondary to, 152,
V
160 Vascular conditions, 111, 123–124
auditory processing disorders secondary to, VCN. See Ventral cochlear nucleus
154, 156–157 Ventral cochlear nucleus
background on, 149–151 description of, 84, 90, 139
case study of, 158–160 GABAergic inhibition in, 91
causes of, 150 Vernon, Jack, 9
central nervous system damage caused by, 151 Veterans, 149, 153
classification of, 150, 151 Vigabatrin, 143
conductive hearing loss caused by, 151
cortical hyperexcitability in, 152
W
definition of, 149
description of, 26 WDRC. See Wide dynamic range compression
diffuse axonal injury associated with, 152 White noise, 249
electrophysiological assessments in, 155 Wide dynamic range compression, 229
health effects of, 150 Wideband noise generators, 215, 248
mild, 151, 152–153 Williams syndrome, 109, 113, 186, 211–212
in military personnel, 149, 158–160 WNG. See Wideband noise generators
postconcussion syndrome secondary to, 150 Woman in White, The, 4
research priorities in, 157–158
sensory hypersensitivity associated with, 268
Y
sound intolerance secondary to, 153–154
symptoms of, 150, 160 Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale, 54
treatment of, 154–157 Y-BOCS. See Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive
Treponema pallidum, 125 Scale

You might also like