You are on page 1of 24

Values, schemas, and norms in the culture-behavior nexus: A situated dynamics

framework
Author(s): Kwok Leung and Michael W Morris
Source: Journal of International Business Studies , December 2015, Vol. 46, No. 9, Special
Issue: What Is Culture and How Do We Measure It? (December 2015), pp. 1028-1050
Published by: Published by: Palgrave Macmillan Journals on behalf of Academy of
International Business.

Stable URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/43653782

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms

Palgrave Macmillan Journals is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access
to Journal of International Business Studies

This content downloaded from


194.145.103.190 on Thu, 02 May 2024 15:08:20 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
^¿C Journal of International Business Studies (201 5) 46, I 028- 1 050
© 201 5 Academy of International Business All rights reserved 0047-2506
www.jibs.net

Values, schémas, and norms in the culture-


behavior nexus: A situated dynamics framework

Abstract
Kwok Leung1 and
International business (IB) research has predominantly relied on value constructs
Michael W Morris2
to account for the influence of societal culture, notably Hofstede's cultural
dimensions. While parsimonious, the value approach's assumptions about
1 Department of Management, Chinese University
the consensus of values within nations, and the generality and stability of cul-
of Hong Kong, S hati h, NT, Hong Kong, China;
tural patterns of behavior are increasingly challenged. We review two promising
2 Columbia Business School, Columbia University,
New York, USA alternatives - the constructive approach centering on schémas and the
intersubjectivist approach centering on norms - and the evidence that demon-
Correspondence: strates their usefulness in accounting for international differences in the behavior
K Leung, Department of Management, of managers, employees, and consumers. We propose a situated dynamics
Chinese University of Hong Kong, Shatin, NT, framework, specifying the role of values, schémas, and norms in accounting for
Hong Kong, China. cultural differences, and delineating conditions under which each causal
Tel: +00 852 3943 9541
mechanism is operative. Values play a more important role in accounting for
cultural differences in weak situations where fewer constraints are perceived;
schémas play a more important role when situational cues increase their
accessibility and relevance; and norms play a more important role when social
evaluation is salient. Directions for future research based on this integrative
framework and its implications for the measurement of culture and application
in IB are discussed.
journal of International Business Studies (201 5) 46, 1 028- 1 050. doi: 1 0. 1 057/jibs.20 1 4.66

Keywords: cultural values; cross-cultural research/measurement issues; comparative


management; cross-cultural management; cultural dimensions; culture

INTRODUCTION
A longstanding conceptualization of international differences in th
workplace and consumer behavior centers on values: A society
socializes its members into distinctive value priorities, and individu
are driven by their internalized cultural value orientations to beha
in the ways that are characteristic of the society. The most influen
value framework in international business (IB) research is that of
Hofstede (1980), which scores countries on several value dimensions.
Hofstede's (1980) dimensions are parsimonious and broadly encom-
passing; they have proved useful for organizing research on cultural
differences in a wide range of business behaviors (Taras, Kirkman, &
Steel, 2010), including modes of foreign investment (Kogut & Singh,
1988), consumer behaviors such as online shopping (Lim, Leung, Sia,
Received: 1 December 201 3
Revised: 24 September 2014
& Lee, 2004), and sourcing services from different countries (Peeters,
Accepted: 29 September 2014 Dehon, & Garcia-Prieto, 2014). At the same time, many researchers
have identified empirical patterns that do not fit the assumptions of
Online publication date: 18 December 2014

This content downloaded from


194.145.103.190 on Thu, 02 May 2024 15:08:20 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
1029

the value approach, and have called for studying


Third, we provide suggestions on how culture
alternative mechanisms of cultural influence (e.g.,
should be measured from a situated perspective and
Kirkman, Lowe, & Gibson, 2006). how this situated framework can be applied in the
In this article we review the limitations of the IB context. Measurement of culture should not be
value account, that people in different nations
narrowly confined to values and needs to expand
to alter-
pursue different ends, and then introduce two cover schémas and norms, and we provide illu-
native approaches: The constructivist account thatto show how it can be done. To illustrate
strations
the or
culture influences behavior through the schémas utility of this framework, we discuss the new
cognitive lenses people use to make sense ofinsights
ambig- it offers to IB research. We show how the
uous information, and the intersubjectivist app- framework provides a new perspective on
situated
roach that culture influences behavior through
international mergers, and suggest interesting
the social norms that direct typical or appropriate
research topics that go beyond value differences for
behavior in a given situation. While these different
a recent phenomenon, "reverse foreign direct invest-
approaches have been developed in separate resea-
ment^ from emerging economies into the West.
rch traditions, recent findings increasingly point to
connections and parallels in the functioning of VALUES
values, schémas, and norms that give rise From
to cul-
survey ratings of work-related values by IBM
tural differences. Drawing on these different employees
streams in a large number of countries, Hofstede
(1980) distilled four dimensions of culture based on
of research, we propose a situated dynamics frame-
work to integrate these three explanatory mechan- country means, such as Individualism-Collectivism
isms and identify the conditions under which and each
Power Distance. Insights about a fifth dimension,
mechanism explains cultural patterns in behavior. Confucian Dynamism or short-term/long-term orien-
Our article makes three major contributions to came from an independent investigation of
tation,
theory and methodology concerning the culture- Chinese values around the world (Chinese Culture
behavior nexus. First, while there is clear evidence
Connection, 1987). Conceptual and methodological
that the value approach fails to account forrefinements
a wide of value dimensions came in the work of
range of cultural influences, a systematic attemptSchwartz (1994) and in the GLOBE project (House,
Hanges,
to address its limitations is still lacking. In the past Javidan, Dorfman, & Gupta, 2004). These
two decades, research on schémas and norms has value frameworks provided a great research advance
substantiated their validity as alternative explanatory over accounts of culture that merely described differ-
mechanisms. For instance, schema research eluci- ences in behaviors (e.g., Hall, 1959), in that they
dates how and when bicultural individuals effort- provided theoretical accounts of cultural differences
lessly mesh with a cultural setting as opposed to
in organizational behaviors, such as cultural differ-
acting in contrast to its expectations. Norm research ences in reactions to leaders (Kirkman, Chen, Farh,
elucidates how newcomers to a culture, such as Chen, & Lowe, 2009). The value approach is based on
expatriates and visitors, can act in a culturally appro-a trait perspective on behavior, in which the obser-
priate manner without undergoing socialization intovable phenotype is a syndrome of organizational
its values. As we shall see, these and many other
behaviors and the underlying genotype is a set of
phenomena important to IB can be better understood
value priorities (e.g., Triandis, 1995). Despite the vast
by moving beyond the traditional focus on values.
influence of the value approach, however, research
Cultural phenomena inadequately explained byincreasingly reveals patterns that are inconsistent
values can be addressed by a broadened view that
with some of its major assumptions.
considers the influence of schémas and norms. The first issue concerns consensus. Hofstede (1980)
Second, IB and other behavioral fields need a
defined culture as "collective programming ... that
comprehensive framework mapping the influ- distinguishes one group or category of people from
others" (25). Value theorists using country-level
ence of culture on behavior. We integrate theories
about values, schémas, and norms to develop a scores to predict the behavior of individuals from
situated dynamics framework that provides a fuller the same country assume that the average values are
theory of culture and behavior. This framework broadly representative, an assumption that Tung
posits the conditions under which each of these and Verbeke (2010) have critiqued. Value heterogene-
three key mechanisms operates by identifying the ity within a culture would be expected from ethnic
situational factors that catalyze or inhibit the rele- and regional subcultures (Baskerville, 2003) as well
vant psychological processes. as from social roles, personality, and other

Journal of International Business Studies

This content downloaded from


194.145.103.190 on Thu, 02 May 2024 15:08:20 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
1030

individual differences (McSweeney, 2002). Anshock"


exten-from the mismatch between self and envi-
ronment, and even "reverse culture shock" upon
sive review reveals that individualistic and collecti-
returning home (Sussman, 2000). Although many
vistic values vary greatly within countries and much
immigrants and expatriates struggle with cultural
less across countries than traditionally portrayed
(Oyserman, Coon, & Kemmelmeier, 2002). Inadjustment,
a learning a new culture may not be
inherently stressful. A large-scale study of immigrant
recent analysis of three multicountry value data sets,
Fischer and Schwartz (2011) found much greater adolescents in many societies by Berry, Phinney, Sam,
and Vedder (2006) found that immigrants were no
variation within than between countries, contradict-
ing the assertion of Hofstede (1980) that valuesworse are than their non-immigrant peers in psycho-
like shared mental software that distinguishes logical one well-being and school adjustment. Not only
people from other cultural groups. do immigrants pick up new cultures quickly, many
become bicultural - meaning that they identify
A second issue is the generality of cultural patterns
across situations. Hofstede (1980) proposed that with and operate proficiently in both their heritage
value orientation would give rise to "a broad ten- culture and the host culture. Research reveals that
dency to prefer certain states of affairs over others"biculturals switch between cultural frames through
(19). Some broad patterns of this sort have been automatic subconsicous processes in response to
identified: In high power-distance societies with situational cues of cultural expectations (Hong,
more hierarchical values, many kinds of judgments Morris, Chiù, & Benet-Martínez, 2000). As globaliza-
and decisions show deference to authority (Kirkman tion processes give more people exposure to multiple
et al., 2009). While awareness of these broad ten- cultural traditions, conceptions of unitary/static cul-
dencies is useful, Osland and Bird (2000) argued tural predispositions become less relevant to IB
that these depictions are but "sophisticated stereo- research, and theories about how plural, dynamic
types" that fail to capture situational nuances. Some cultural proficiencies that surface situationally become
cultural tendencies vary dramatically from one situa- more important.
tion to the next - Japanese consumers are tradition- The value approach also predicts stability in coun-
alists about kitchen appliances but technophiles try-level behavioral patterns over the long duration.
Hofstede (1993: 92) asserted that "National cultures
about their toilets. No general value, like traditional-
ism, could account for this duality in Japanese change only very slowly if at all." Schwartz (2006)
preferences; the forces underlying these tendencies suggested that country-level value orientations may
must be context-specific. persist across centuries, as each generation passes on
A third issue is the stability of cultural patterns,its values in the way they socialize the next genera-
at the micro-level in the behavior of individuals tion. Yet some cultural patterns of behavior change
across similar occasions and at the macro-level in dramatically. Even measures of values show greater
a society's behavioral patterns across the individualism
long in China today than a generation ago
duration. The value approach predicts stability. (e.g., Ralston, Egri, Stewart, Terpstra, & Kaicheng,
If individuals' values are molded by early cultural 1999). Gould and Grein (2009) noted that cultures
socialization, then individual differences in procliv-are not closed systems - transformations can arise
ity toward culturally typical decisions and behaviors through intercultural interactions as many networks
should be fairly fixed over time. Yet research revealstranscend national boundaries. Morris, Chiù, and
that the degree to which individuals exemplify Liu (2014) proposed polyculturalist research pro-
cultural patterns varies dramatically as a function grams,of premised on a network conception of culture.
changing conditions such as mood (Ashton-James, Boyd and Richerson (2005) modeled cultural evolu-
Maddux, Galinsky, & Chartrand, 2009), cognitive tion as a process in which new practices spread
load (Knowles, Morris, Chiù, & Hong, 2001),through and a population as a function of social norm-
whether reasons must be given for one's choice related processes such as imitation of the average
(Briley, Morris, & Simonson, 2000). Whether orpeer not or emulation of the successful. The ways in
individuals exhibit culturally typical decisions andcultures change as a function of internal and
which
actions also depends on their recent experiences external shock, and new thoughts and practices
(Savani, Morris, Naidu, Kumar, & Berlia, 2011). diffuse through societies cannot be well-understood
The assumption of the value approach that in cul-
terms of trait like value orientations, but they can
tural characteristics are stable and almost immutable be explained in terms of social norms.
is exemplified by the notion that expatriates and To conclude, the value approach to culture, spear-
immigrants necessarily go through extended "culture headed by Hofstede (1980), has led to significant

Journal of International Business Studies

This content downloaded from


194.145.103.190 on Thu, 02 May 2024 15:08:20 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
1031

progress in IB research, but many international


comparative study of social judgment, Morris and
Peng
differences in behavior do not map onto patterns of (1994) found that, compared with Ameri-
values (Fischer & Smith, 2003; Kirkman et al.,cans,
2006;Chinese attributed individual behaviors
Lamoreaux & Morling, 2012; Taras et al., 2010). less to the internal properties of the actors and
In a prominent recent example, the GLOBE more studyto the factors in the social environment
found, contrary to expectations, negative associa- (relationships, groups, and norms). Subsequent
research found that when explaining the out-
tions between cultural values and cultural practices
comes
(for attempts to resolve this puzzle, see Taras, Steel, & of a group or an organization, Chinese
Kirkman, 2010). There have been repeated calls to
emphasized the collective actor's internal proper-
develop alternative conceptual frameworks of cul-
ties rather than its environment. Thus both
ture (e.g., Fang, 2010; Leung, Bhagat, Buchan,cultures
Erez, show a tendency to make internal, dispo
& Gibson, 2005). Kirkman et al. (2006: 313) summed sitional attributions, but differ in the unit the
it up by concluding that "perhaps the time is rightconstrue as the agent or actor (Menon, Morris,
for a move beyond Culture's Consequences" and & Hong, 1999). Westerners habitually con-
Chiù,
that the field needs to "look beyond this paradigm strue individuals as actors and rarely groups,
to break new ground." whereas East Asians are relatively more likely t
Two alternative approaches have emerged inconstrue cul- groups as actors.
tural psychology over the last two decades. They The constructivist (schema) approach differs from
posit different psychological mechanisms of cultural
the trait (value) approach greatly with regard t
influence, namely, schema activation and norm predictions about the generality and stability o
salience, and can account for cultural differences cultural patterns of behavior because schémas exe
that cannot be handled by the value approach. We an influence on behavior only at moments when
review these two streams of research below to set the they are activated, or put into use as a filter for one
stage for developing a novel, situated approach to information processing. A person's legacy of cu
culture. tural schémas is often compared by constructivis
with a toolbox containing an assortment of impl
SCHEMAS ments: Some are on top of the stack from recent us
Constructivist accounts trace cultural differences towhile others are rusting at the bottom of the bo
the cognitive lenses or templates that guide oursome fit the current problem and others do no
interpretations, expectancies, and responses. People some are tools that one feels comfortable using and
with the same value priorities may exhibit different others are not (Swidler, 1986). The activation of
judgments and behaviors as a function of the sché- schema depends on three factors: accessibility
mas conferred by their cultures. Constructivists applicability, and judged appropriateness (Higgin
emphasize that people impose culturally conferred 1996).
cognitive structures to make sense of stimuli and Accessibility is the key to several constructi
problems; without schémas they could not make vist hypotheses about the situation-dependenc
sense of experience or organize complex sequences of cultural behaviors. Constructs that have been
of actions. We look at the world through cultural activated recently remain highly accessible and
lenses; we act coherently and coordinate with others thereby more likely to be used (Bruner, 1956;
by following cultural scripts. Higgins, Bargh, & Lombardi, 1985). Cultural sché-
While the constructivist approach to culture dates mas can be activated by situational cues in two ways,
to the 1920s Russian school of Vygotsky (1962/ direct semantic priming and indirect associative
1986) and Luria (1976), it gained new life in the priming. Schemas for individualism/collectivism,
1990s as experimental social psychologists began independence/interdependence, and individuation/
investigating cultural differences in social cognition contextualization can be primed directly through
biases. Markus and Kitayama (1991) analyzed East- prior tasks that engage them. Trafimo w, Triandis,
West differences in self-construals, arguing that East and Goto (1991) asked participants to read a story
Asians show less of a self-enhancing bias, which about an individualistic or collectivistic warrior and
results from the independent self-concept fostered this manipulation affected their subsequent self-
by Western cultures. East Asians are also less prone descriptions; the individualistic story elicited more
to the "fundamental attribution error" of reading references to personal dispositions, and the collecti-
behavior as reflective of a person's characteristics vistic story more references to relationships and
while overlooking situational constraints. In a group memberships. Oishi, Wyer, and Colcombe

Journal of International Business Studies

This content downloaded from


194.145.103.190 on Thu, 02 May 2024 15:08:20 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
'

1032

(2000) presented individualism- or collectivism- clear, firm answers rather than complex, ambiguous,
relevant words subliminally and found an expected or provisional solutions. Chiù, Morris, Hong, and
shift in attributions for negative outcomesMenon consis-(2000) studied situationally induced NFCC
tent with the priming manipulation. through varying the degree of time pressure in a task,
Cultural schémas can be primed indirectly by that time pressure accentuated dispositional
finding
images, sounds, or even smells and tastes reminis- inference solely in the group-actor condition for
cent of a culture, even though these stimuli have
Chinese perceivers and solely in the individual-actor
no semantic connection to the schémas. Cultural condition for American perceivers. These results sug-
icons - images that symbolize central characteristics gest that the motive for cognitive closure created by
of a culture - elicit culturally typically thoughts time pressure
and accentuates the influence of the chroni-
behaviors. Hong et al. (2000) found that exposing cally accessible agency schema of a given culture, that
bicultural Hong Kong students to images of Western is, the group agency schema for Chinese and the
vs Chinese landmarks (The White House vs The individual agency schema for Americans.
Forbidden City) induced, respectively, more disposi- The influence of motivations on people's use of
tional and more contextual attributions. Exposure to schémas is further illustrated by the individual dif-
a Chinese face or iconic images such as a Chinese ferences in the ways biculturals respond to priming.
vase increased the accessibility of Chinese language Benet-Martínez, Leu, Lee, and Morris (2002) intro-
structures as opposed to English language struc- duced the dimension of bicultural identity integra-
tures (Zhang, Morris, Cheng, & Yap, 2013). In tion, which distinguishes biculturals who experience
another study, bicultural students from Hong Kong their dual identities as compatible vs conflicting.
were presented with either Chinese (e.g., kung fu), While compatible biculturals react in a way consis-
American (e.g., football), or culturally neutral pictu- tent with a primed culture, conflicted biculturals are
res, and then confronted with a prisoners' dilemma more likely to respond to cultural cues in a contrast-
game involving either ingroup or outgroup counter- ing manner, inhibiting responses characteristic of
parts (Wong & Hong, 2005). Participants cooperated the primed culture and enacting patterns of the
more with an ingroup member after being primed culture that was not primed. Conflicted biculturals
with Chinese images. The prime triggered the rela- frame-switch contrastively even when cultures are
tional schémas from their Chinese heritage and they primed subliminally, indicating processes that are
played the game in a more Chinese way. reflexive and automatic (Mok & Morris, 2013).
Another set of constructivist hypotheses focuses Recent evidence suggests that conflicted biculturals
on applicability. An activated schema may show feel a need to defend the non-primed cultural iden-
little effect on a behavior if it has no relevance for tity (Mok & Morris, 2009, 2013).
the behavior. For instance, the individual agency Research on determinants of schema accessibility,
schema that is more accessible to Americans fits applicability, and appropriateness has produced
events with an individual actor, but not events insights with for two important questions: "How does
a group actor. In a similar vein, the group agency matter?" and "When does culture matter?"
culture
schema that is more accessible to East Asians are (Gibson, Maznevski, & Kirkman, 2009).
applicable to events with a group actor, but not to
events involving individuals. Activated schemes
influence behaviors to the extent that they are
Evaluation of the Constructivist Approach
relevant to them (Menon et al., 1999). An overall evaluation of the constructivist (schema)
vs the trait (value) approach must begin with the
Appropriateness is a final gate on schema activation.
acknowledgment
For example, many people have a strong desire not that constructivist accounts are
to make judgments based on gender and ethnic far less parsimonious; they posit many variables that
stereo-
interact
types, so even if those stereotypes are triggered in to determine cultural patterns of behavior.
their minds, they vigilantly resist using them,However,
some- the constructivist approach can explain
why many
times overcompensating in the opposite direction as cultural patterns are situation-specific
a result. Motivations to conform culturally or not
rather than context-general, and offer an account for
may similarly moderate whether cultural the schémas
instability and malleability of cultural patterns.
direct thoughts and behaviors. A great deal of It provides a more nuanced picture of how a person's
recent research concerns an epistemic motive, need temporary motivations or life experiences, such
for cognitive closure (NFCC, Kruglanski, Webster, & as working in a multicultural context and living
Klem, 1993), which refers to the desire for quick, abroad, change their likelihood of expressing

Journal of International Business Studies

This content downloaded from


194.145.103.190 on Thu, 02 May 2024 15:08:20 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
1033

behavioral patterns characteristic of their heritage


the proximal mechanisms for cultural differences in
culture. the individual's subjective beliefs. Another app-
The constructivist approach also provides a better roach, sometimes termed the "intersubjective"
account of multiple cultural legacies that affect the approach, locates the source of cultural influence in
same individual. Trait accounts portray biculturals as the surrounding group and the individual's per-
hybrids whose values are midway between the ceptions of it. Social psychologists have long found
means of the two respective cultures, or compart- that people assimilate their thoughts to the group
mentalizers who endorse one set of cultural values norm, because peers' responses are an influential
for work and another for home (e.g., Phinneysource & of information (Sherif, 1936) and because
Devich-Navarro, 1997). In contrast, constructivistthey seek to avoid negative judgments by others
research elucidates the flexibility of biculturals (Asch,
to 1956). In the theory of reasoned action,
switch between different cultural frames for the perceived normative beliefs are key to predicting
same problem (Hong et al., 2000). The constructivist
a wide range of behaviors (Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975).
approach helps us understand the mutability, malle-Cialdini, Kallgren, and Reno (1991) distinguished
ability, and multiplicity of cultural influence on
between descriptive norms, what ingroup mem-
individual behavior. bers commonly do in a situation, and injunctive
A critic might fairly ask whether constructivistnorms, what they approve of. People adhere to
descriptive
accounts predict more instability than truly exists, as norms because they provide easy
some broad differences between societies can be default solutions and because they enable coordi-
traced back centuries if not millennia. If cultural nating with other group members. Injunctive
patterns of individual behavior are not temporally norms are moralized, imbued with oughtness, so
stable, how can we account for the macro-level people adhere to injunctive norms out of moral
societal stability? Part of the answer is that endur- emotions such as shame at wrongdoing. Moral
ing societal structures and institutions evoke theemotions also drive us to punish others whom we
behavioral pattern typical of a culture. Kitayama observe violating injunctive norms, even if the
and colleagues (e.g., Kitayama, Markus, Matsumoto, punishment costs us personally.
& Norasakkunkit, 1997; Kitayama, Mesquita, & Not just differences in business etiquette (bowing,
Karasawa, 2006; Morling, Kitayama, & Miyamoto, kissing, or shaking hands) but also differences
2002) sampled the interpersonal situations that in judgment and decision making can arise from
American and Japanese students frequently experience norm-following. Norenzayan and colleagues
and then presented these situations to new partici- (Buchtel & Norenzayan, 2008; Norenzayan, Smith,
pants in both countries to gauge how these situations Kim, & Nisbett, 2002) found that Westerners' reli-
affected them. Results showed an effect of situation- ance on formal logic and East Asians' reliance on
culture: For both groups American-sourced situa-intuitive association in tasks such as syllogism pro-
tions were more likely to evoke feelings of blems do not reflect different capacities, but differ-
self-enhancement and efficacy, whereas Japanese- ent notions of what is regarded as reasonable or wise,
sourced situations were more likely to evoke feelings that is, injunctive norms. Yamagishi, Hashimoto,
of self-criticism and relatedness to others. US and and Schug (2008) reexamined cultural differences
in the decision to select the unique option from
Japanese sociocultural settings appear to afford differ-
ent modes of experiencing the self. Furthermore, a choice set, which Kim and Markus (1999) pre-
results also showed an effect of participant-culture, sented as an expression of individualistic values or
as Americans were generally more likely to exhibit an independent self-concept. Yamagishi et al. found
self-enhancement, and Japanese, self-criticism. Each that both Japanese and Americans took the unique
group has a default interpretation, reflecting chroni-
option if they were last to choose (and thus did not
cally accessible schémas that they fall back on deprive
in others), but that both groups declined the
ambiguous situations. One mechanism for sustaining unique option when it was salient that they might
these default schémas appears to be reinforcement, deprive others, and it was only in the ambiguous
condition where the two cultures differed in the
that is, the situationscape in different societies rewards
different cognitive habits (e.g., Savani et al., 2011).expected manner. Yamagishi et al. further showed
that Japanese participants favored uniqueness more
NORMS in private than when they were observed, and
The value and schema approaches may be labeled
declined uniqueness after experiencing being mon-
"subjective culture" approaches in that theyitored
locateby other people, supporting the account that

Journal of International Business Studies

This content downloaded from


194.145.103.190 on Thu, 02 May 2024 15:08:20 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
'

1034

the cultural difference arises from more salient


Korean than American tendency to understand an
norms against self-oriented behavior in Japan. outcome contextually rather than blaming a focal
Japanese and Americans hold similar values individual
in this was driven not by cultural differences in
personal collectivistic values, but by cultural dif-
context, but behave differently in response to differ-
ent norm enforcement incentives. ferences in a perceived descriptive norm of collectiv-
While some research on culture and norms shows
ism. Likewise, cultural differences in the perception
of the
that individuals are affected by objective norms, harm resulting from rights or duty violations
average beliefs or behaviors in their group were
(e.g.,more consistent with a descriptive norm than
Becker et al., 2014), most research examines ahowcultural value account. Zou, Tam, Morris, Lee, Lau,
individuals are affected by subjective norms,and
whatChiù (2009) found that perceived descriptive
norms mediated several cultural differences in social
they perceive to be typical in their group. These
perceptions may not be veridical because they cognitive biases. For example, differences between
Americans and Polish in responsiveness to requests
are based on others' observable public behaviors,
were
which imperfectly reflect private beliefs. This cannot driven by personal collectivism, but by a
lead to the whole group misperceiving itself, a
perceived cultural norm of collectivism. Differences
case of "pluralistic ignorance" that works tobetween
per- Americans and Hong Kong Chinese in
petuate cultural patterns of behavior. Hirai (2000)
dispositional attribution were found not to be driven
found that while Japanese participants on average
by personal beliefs, but by perceived beliefs of typical
endorsed individualist values, they perceivedcultural
that members. Overall, these studies indicate
"typical Japanese people" hold collectivist values,
that country differences in cognitive biases are not
and this perceived norm guides their public so much expressions of divergent inner values and
beha-
vior, thereby perpetuating the perceived collecti-
beliefs across cultures as they are accommodations to
vist norm. different perceived cultural norms.
The GLOBE project (House et al., 2004) measured Chiù, Gelfand, Yamagishi, Shteynberg, and Wan
both values about workplace practices ("should (2010) delineated several mechanisms through
be" measures) and perceptions of typical prac- which perceived descriptive norms influence beha-
tices ("as is" measures), finding unexpectedly that viors. In addition to serving epistemic and coordina-
these measures of cultural values and perceived tion functions with others, norms also provide a
descriptive norms exhibit low and sometimes nega- basis for self-identity and positive self-regard. Wan,
tive correlations. Similarly, Fischer et al. (2009) Chiù, Tam, Lee, Lau, and Peng (2007) assessed the
measured individualism-collectivism as perceptions norms that existed within a community or society
of typical ingroup behaviors. Four dimensions were through calculating the "intersubjective consensus"
identified, and only one dimension, independent based on individual perceptions of these norms.
vs interdependent conceptualization of the self, The beliefs/values that most members assume most
showed a high correlation with the individualism- other members share, which because of pluralistic
collectivism dimension of Hofstede (1980), and the ignorance do not amount to the same as the average
institutional collectivism dimension of GLOBE. This of personal beliefs and values. They found that
identification with a culture was best predicted from
descriptive norm approach captures some ways that
societies differ on the dimension of individualism-
the match of personal values to the intersubjective
collectivism not captured by previous approaches
consensus in the culture, not the match of personal
based on personal values. Researchers looking values
at to the average personal values. A similar set of
values find a similar pattern of results: Fischer
findings is reported by Wan, Chiù, Peng, and Tam
(2007).
(2006) measured personal endorsement and percei-
ved ingroup endorsement across a wide set of values
and found only modest relationships between theseEvaluation of the Intersubjective Approach
two types of value endorsements. Compared with the other two approaches, norm-
based accounts do not assume a consensus of personal
Increasingly, research finds that cultural differ-
beliefs or values in a culture; they assume more
ences in judgment patterns are carried more by
sharedness in perceptions of the group's norms -
perceived descriptive norms than by personal beliefs
or values. A familiar cultural difference is that East assumptions about what a typical group member
Asian societies place less blame on individuals believes, does, and expects. Perceived cultural norms
and emphasize duty rather than rights. Shteynberg, vary across countries and can account for cultural dif-
Gelfand, and Kim (2009) found that the greater ferences in a range of social cognitions and behaviors.

Journal of International Business Studies

This content downloaded from


194.145.103.190 on Thu, 02 May 2024 15:08:20 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Aj/.
1035

Like schema-based accounts, norm-based accounts Au, Huang, Kurman, Niit, & Niit, 2007). Kurman and
serve well to explain situation-specific culturalRonen-Eilon
differ- (2004) asked two immigrant groups
ences because norms are representations ofintypical
Israel to report their personal axioms as well as
responses to specific situations. their perception of those of the average Israeli.
Norm accounts may serve better than schema Immigrant adaptation was better predicted by accu-
accounts to explain the stability and persistence
rate perception of the average Israeli than by simi-
of cultural patterns of social behavior. Individuals
larity of self to the average Israeli. As with the
aforementioned
within a nation may vary greatly in their values but findings, these results highlight
that adaptation in a cultural group depends on
agree largely in their perception of societal norms,
and by dint of this shared perception they intersubjective beliefs, beliefs about what the mem-
would exhibit similar patterns of conduct, at leastbers of the group endorse.
when in public (e.g., Yamagishi et al., 2008), there-
by perpetuating the culture's behavioral patterns. COMPARISON OF THE THREE APPROACHES
Likewise, the stability of beliefs may come from the We have reviewed accounts of cultural differences in
role of intersubjective perceptions in communi- terms of values, schémas, and norms, and shown
cation. When rumors are spread or organizationalthat each account has some strengths relative to the
stories repeated, the content evolves slightly with other two. Accounts of cultural differences based
each retelling. However, the change is not random on values are parsimonious but limited in explaining
variation; the content grows closer to the con- situational variation, mutability over time, and multi-
ventions and stereotypes in the culture (Kashima, plicity of cultural legacies. Constructivist research
2000, 2014). Communicators emphasize the ideas on schema activation introduced methods such
that they perceive their audience to share, and as priming that can explain situational variation
through this process stories grow more culturally behavior and biculturalism, but this approach h
conventional. difficulty in fully accounting for why cultura
Norm accounts also elucidate how newcomers patterns persist. Intersubjective accounts focusi
to a culture learn to coordinate their behavior with
on perceived norms and beliefs are newest and leas
others, without necessarily being socialized into well-developed,
new but this research program has be
successful in identifying individual-level mediato
values. More accurate perceptions of a host culture's
normative value priorities correlate with having
of country differences in patterns of judgment
better interpersonal interactions with locals (Li &
decisions, and behaviors. Norm-based accounts
also offer clues about how to reconcile individual
Hong, 2001). Biculturals can be thought of as having
well-calibrated assumptions about the normsmalleability
for with societal persistence, micro-level
a situation in each of the cultures they know. fluctuation with macro-level stability. For a summary
Compared with monocultural Euro Americans, of the differences among these three approaches, see
bicultural Chinese are more accurate in estimating Table 1.
the extent to which Chinese and Americans differ
in their decision making, specifically in the weight-TOWARD AN INTEGRATED FRAMEWORK
ing of gains vs losses (Leung, Lee, & Chiù, 2013).
The three psychological mechanisms - values, sché-
As a result, bicultural Chinese are more able to tailor
mas, and norms - are usually pitted against each
persuasive messages toward targets from eachother cul- as rival accounts of how culture influences
individual behavior. However, they may be com-
ture that resonate with their biases. The advantages
that biculturals bring to IB can be understood in
plementary; different explanatory mechanisms
often have incremental utility in accounting for
terms of knowledge of multiple norms and resulting
flexibility and competence in tailoring their strate-
cultural differences (e.g., Bond, Leung, Au, Tong, &
gies for intercultural interaction. Chemonges-Nielson, 2004). As we have seen that
value-based accounts, which have dominated IB
Another kind of social belief, more general than
behavioral norms, are social axioms, principles research on culture, are incomplete, a framework
that underlie people's understanding of their social integrating schémas and norms provides a more
world and life goals, such as the belief that effort comprehensive account of international differences.
produces rewards (Leung & Bond, 2004, 2009). We develop the situated dynamics framework below,
Social axioms are not self-descriptive and show in which each of the mechanisms - values, schémas,
low correlations with values and personality traits and norms - depends on situational factors in
(e.g., Chen, Fok, Bond, & Matsumoto, 2006; Leung, different ways. The different roles of the situation in

Journal of International Business Studies

This content downloaded from


194.145.103.190 on Thu, 02 May 2024 15:08:20 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
-&ř
1036

Table 1 Values, schémas, and norms as mediators of cultural influence

Values Schemas Norms

Nature Individual differences Knowledge structures Social perceptions


Locus Self Mind x environment interaction Social perceiver x context interaction
Theorized mechanism Preferences drive behavior Accessible schémas guide Descriptive norm
of cultural influence construal influence
Injunctive norms - no
influence

Consensus Shared within a culture Shared within a given context Vary as a function of social groups
Generality/situationality Suppression of preference Activated by situational cues, Salience affected by social context,
in "strong" as opposed to such as priming such as implicit or explicit
"weak" situations sanctioning
Cultural stability Micro and macro stability Micro instability; Micro instability;
Macro stability as a result of Macro stability as a result of
chronically accessible schémas perceived sharedness that sustains
conventionalization and
sacralization of norms

Cultural multiplicity Biculturals have middling or Biculturals have two networks of Biculturals have two norm
compartmentalized values schémas representations

moderating cultural patterns are the main execution."


thrust of Mischel and Shoda (1995, 2010) further
this integrative approach. developed this view of the "situated person" by
theorizing about why some features of situations
The Role of the Situation affect most people in the same way whereas other
features of situations reliably affect some individuals
The trait approach to personality assumes that people
should exhibit the same behavioral patterns across and not others. The variation is due to individual
differences in construal of situations, which give
situations. Contrary to this prediction, Mischel (1968)
rise to different expectations, goals, and affects, and
showed that cross-situational consistency is low across
hence different behaviors. This argument is crystal-
a wide variety of studies. This predicament also con-
fronts the trait approach to culture (Mendoza-Dentonlized in the construct of situational strength in the
& Mischel, 2007), which assumes that cultural diffe-organizational context, with situations perceived to
rences in behaviors should correspond to cultural be "strong" overriding the influence of individual
differences in values, an assertion contradicted differences
by (Meyer, Dalai, & Hermida, 2010). For
example,
considerable empirical findings (e.g., Kirkman et al., an assembly line involves strong norms
2006). Just as with personality traits, the role of and as a result workers behave more or less alike
cultural values in directing behaviors varies acrossregardless of their personalities and cultural values.
situations (e.g., Bond, 2013; Chiù, Ng, & Au, 2013). Whereas the construct of situational strength
To address the fleeting relationship between per- lumps together several ways that situations con-
sonality traits and behaviors, Mischel (1973) pro- strain behavior, distinguishing these different ways
posed a cognitive social learning perspectiveisof useful for integrating values, schémas, and norms
in accounting for cultural differences. The situated
personality, later developed into the cognitive affec-
tive personality system (Mischel & Shoda, 1995).dynamics framework that we propose resembles a
The key proposition is that people do not exist moderated
in mediation model, with the three psycho-
a social vacuum and are always in some sort logical
of mechanisms mediating the influence of
situation, which Mischel (1973: 276) conceptualized culture on individual behavior, and different types
as a major determinant of behavior: "psychological of situational characteristics moderating the salience
'situations' and 'treatments' are powerful to the of each mechanism. We draw on theorizing about
degree that they lead all persons to construe the the situated person (e.g., Mischel & Shoda, 1995,
particular events the same way, induce uniform 2010) to identify the situaitonal characteristics
expectancies regarding the most appropriate res- moderate the importance of values. We adapt
that
ponse pattern, provide adequate incentives for the the situated inference model of Loersch and Payne
performance of that response pattern, and instill (2011)
the to shed light on the situational moderators
for
skills necessary for its satisfactory construction and schémas. We ground the intersubjective

Journal of International Business Studies

This content downloaded from


194.145.103.190 on Thu, 02 May 2024 15:08:20 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
^c.
1037

Social adaptation signals


- privacy
- identifiability
- expected future interaction
- cues about meanings and
interpretations

Cues to moral or ethical


dimensions

Cultural
differences in
Cultural

Social evaluation
- presence of others
- collective identity
- wealth

Ambiguity
- unfamiliar situations with cues

about typical behaviors

Cues of specific norms

Behavioral tasks with social


implications

Cultural tightness

Figure 1 The situated dynamics framework.

approach in the focus theory of normative conduct,


cultural values in directing behavior. Milgram (1963)
which identifies situational cues for the operation
asked whether Americans who hold fairly egalitarian
cultural values would comply with an authority's
of norms (e.g., Cialdini et al., 1991; Goldstein &
Cialdini, 2010). For a schematic representationrequest
of that they harm another person. Despite their
our situated dynamic approach to understanding values, Americans followed the instructions of author-
cultural influence on behavior, see Figure 1. ities for fear of their negative social evaluation.
Switching away from the actions corresponding to
When Values Prevail one's cultural values can be adaptive. For instance, in
dispute resolution decisions, Americans generally
Absence of social adaptation signals favor adjudication procedures, and Chinese gene-
rally
A particularly salient dimension of the situation is prefer procedures that involve compromises,
such as mediation. These differences in behavior
the behavior of the other people present. The press
correspond to differences in prioritization of the
of other people can overwhelm the force of internal

Journal of International Business Studies

This content downloaded from


194.145.103.190 on Thu, 02 May 2024 15:08:20 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
*

1038

Ethical dimensions
value of competition. However, in strong situations,
this cultural difference in behavior vanishes. Both Some choice situations are imbued with moral con-
notations
cultural groups prefer adjudication when facing a or implications, and they evoke deli-
beration
counterpart low in agreeableness and high in emo- based on one's values. Personal values play
a strong
tional stability (Morris, Leung, & Iyengar, 2004). The causal role in driving this type of behavior
despite that the situation may activate schémas
situation of a stubborn, volatile counterpart signals
that mediation would not be a wise choice and and norms that deviate from these values. Fischer
hence overrides the influence of cultural values on (2006) examined how personal values and perceived
the decision. ingroup values (norms) predict an array of self-
An implication of this argument is that values reported behaviors across different cultures. Social
would shape behavior more in situations lacking conformity tasks such as resolving a conflict were
strong signals of social adaptation. This holdspredicted in by norms more than personal values,
private as opposed to public situations (Kim, Chiù, whereas ethical tasks such as objecting to prejudice
Peng, Cai, & Tov, 2010), such as making anonymous were predicted by personal values more than by
suggestions to management or posting anonymous perceived group norms. Conformity tasks afford
comments on a website. It also holds in public questions about the appropriate behavior, which
situations where there is low identifiability such as a invoke knowledge of norms, whereas ethical tasks
collective work task where it is hard to identify the invoke representations of one's value commitments.
action of an individual (e.g., Brickner, Harkins, & Similarly, Fischer et al. (2009) found that perceived
Ostrom, 1986). Lower expected future interaction individualism-collectivism of one's culture predic-
with the other people present reduces their influ- ted tradition, conformity, and socially oriented
ence; signals of their preferences would be regarded behaviors, even after controlling for personal indi-
less if one does not expect to see them again, mean- vidualism-collectivism, but did not predict self-
ing that one's personal inclinations will direct beha- direction and stimulation-oriented behaviors.

vior (e.g., Heide & Miner, 1992). Another class of


situations lacking strong social adaptation signals Schemas and Situated Inferences
are those in which one cannot read the signals. The activation of schémas as an explanatory mecha-
A German executive just arrived in Vietnam to be nism for cultural differences depends on situational
the general manager of a factory is likely to experi- factors in different ways. The situated inference
ence a lack of clear signals of her subordinates' model of priming (Loersch & Payne, 2011) distin-
preferences to which she is accustomed. People tend guishes several distinct situational factors that acti-
vate knowledge structures to influence behavior.
to construe these unfamiliar situations in abstract
terms and rely on values to guide their behaviorsSituations prior to a task may make certain schémas
accessible. According to this model, cues in prior
(e.g., Torelli & Kaikati, 2009; Vallacher & Wegner,
situations do not directly determine people's deci-
1989). Value differences tend to drive cultural differ-
ences in behaviors in these situations. sions; they merely elevate the accessibility of all
Variation in adherence to cultural values cannot be mental structures associated with the cues.
Another important influence comes from the task
solely accounted for by the influence of social adap-
situation.
tation signals. For instance, Americans who hold Different tasks evoke or afford different
individualistic values dislike team-based incentives
kinds of mental questions, which then select for the
at work but tend to favor team sports. Is this corresponding
because types of schémas. The task of inter-
preting an ambiguous stimulus evokes construal
the work context involves weaker social adapta-
tion signals than sports? That seems unlikely,questions
as co- (what kind of person, object, or event
workers and managers are important role models is this?)
and and the corresponding types of schémas
sources of social evaluation. The alignment of (stereotypes,
work- object concepts, and event scripts).
Tasks that demand action evoke conduct questions
place preference and cultural values also reflects
(What will I do?) and schémas that guide behavior.
institutions that shape work behavior (e.g., Ashforth
& Anand, 2003). Individualistic work behavior is Tasks that call for self-expression evoke identity
reinforced by procedures of performance evaluationquestions (What do I want or care about?) and evoke
and criteria for promotions. An account of valuesself-related schémas. This role of the task situation
moderated by situational strength only provides elaborates
a the construct of applicability in past schema
partial answer to understanding situational variabi-
research. To illustrate this process in the IB context,
lity in cultural patterns of behavior. consider a Westerner assigned to work in Japan.

Journal of International Business Studies

This content downloaded from


194.145.103.190 on Thu, 02 May 2024 15:08:20 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
1039

Regarding the construal question, she maydisapproving


need to stare or a shake of the head, can be
potent
ñnd out whether Japanese customers like their pro-cues of social approval. An important pro-
position
ducts, and schémas about customer preferences andis that social norms guide behaviors only
Japanese consumer culture are relevant in when
this they
taskare in focus or salient (Cialdini et al., 1991).
Generally
situation. Regarding how to behave, she may need to speaking, norms become salient when
find out how to deal with a customer complaint.
there are cues about social identity and group mem-
bership, such as in a situation where one may be
Schemas about complaint handling and interaction
with Japanese are relevant. Finally, the self-identity
judged by others (Cialdini et al., 1991). Recent work
question is illustrated by a situation in which shethat situational cues signaling a collective
has shown
is accused of wrongdoing. Schemas about theidentity
impor- can accentuate the salience of both injunc-
tance of clearing one's name and protecting tive self-
and descriptive norms (White & Simpson, 2013).
interest would affect her reactions. The awareness that one is part of a group brings
The most novel aspect of the situated inferencenorms in focus. The contrary also holds: When indi-
model concerns the perceived "ownership" over the
viduals are in a situation where they are less depen-
accessibility of a schema. When a person metacogni- dent on others, the influence of norms declines.
tively construes the accessibility of an idea as coming An example is wealth, which signals a sense of self-
from her intrinsic response to a task or problem, shesufficiency and autonomy. People primed with
feels ownership of the accessibility. When a person money felt that social influence attempts threatened
interprets the accessibility of an idea as spillover fromtheir autonomy (Liu, Smeesters, & Vohs, 2012; Vohs,
a prior experience or cue, she would not feel owner- Mead, & Goode, 2008), which suggsts that wealthy
ship. The role of ownership explains why subtle cuespeople have a stronger sense of autonomy and self-
often influence behavior more than blatant cues - sufficiency, and are less influenced by norms. This
blatant cues produce accessibility but the personargument does extends to the macro level, as Inglehart and
not feel ownership of that accessibility. People try (2005) concluded from extensive survey data
Welzel
not to be influenced by accessibility that they dothat not the influence of norms is in decline in post-
"own" and in the process often overcompensate, industrialized, affluent societies.
producing a contrastive response to blatant cues. For Different types of situations make different kinds of
example, Shih, Ambady, Richeson, Fuj ita, and Gray norms salient. Injunctive norms become particularly
(2002) found that subtle primes of Asian identity salient
led in situations with cues about social evalua-
Asian Americans to perform better on a mathematics tion. The mere presence of peers during a task acti-
quiz, whereas blatant primes led them to do worse. vates injunctive norms against depriving others in
Ownership may also explain the responses of bicul- the minds of Japanese students making choices
tural individuals who experience identity conflict. (Yamagishi et al., 2008). Other situations have clearer
Because these conflicted biculturals tend to feel dis- evaluative implications, such as making a presenta-
identified with one or both of their cultures, they tion
are to one's work team. Still others are explicitly
less likely to feel ownership of accessible cultural
evaluative, such as a job interview. These types of
schémas following exposure to cultural primes (Zou,
situations are strong situations that evoke compliance
Morris, & Benet-Martinez, 2008). with social norms. In general, the higher the social
evaluative pressure, the more salient are norms, and
Norms under Focus of Attention the more they influence individual behavior.
Injunctive norms may be absent in some situa-
The focus theory of normative conduct posits that
descriptive norms provide information on what tions and people turn to descriptive norms for
behavior is typical, and injunctive norms provide
guidance. Following what most people do is a safe
bet for an acceptable response when there are no
information on what behavior is socially approved.
Descriptive norms are based on the observed beha-
cues about proper and appropriate actions. In unfa-
miliar situations where there is little information
viors of others and social learning (Kashima, Pearson,
and knowledge about proper behaviors, but the
& Pearson, 2013), whereas injunctive norms are based
on social reactions to one's behaviors (e.g., Schultz,
behaviors of others people are observable, descrip-
Nolan, Cialdini, Goldstein, & Griskevicius, 2007). tive norms function as heuristics to guide judgments
Explicit, evaluative feedback conveys the extent toand actions (Asch, 1956; Sherif, 1936). A Chinese
which a behavior is approved, such as the feed- manager who just arrives at France is likely to follow
back managers provide to their subordinates in per- the styles of her French colleagues in interacting
formance appraisal. Implicit feedback, such as a with her French superior to avoid social disapproval.

Journal of International Business Studies

This content downloaded from


194.145.103.190 on Thu, 02 May 2024 15:08:20 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
■¿&T ***
***

1040

Specific situational cues can make certainsituated


norms processes are differentially activated to
inform
salient, and the activation can occur through future research.
auto-
matic associations (Aarts & Dijksterhuis, 2003).
Our For
analysis is guided by the premise of the
situated
instance, the library context cues people to be quietinference model that different task situa-
because of the learned association of libraries with
tions evoke different mental questions (Loersch &
the quietness norm. Kwan, Chiù, and Leung (2014)
Payne, 2011). This model posits that a task situation
found that priming American participants with
affords three types of questions in the mind of a
social actor: Motivation, construal, and behavior.
President Bush increased their perception of Amer-
icans' preference for iconic American brands, a accomplishment is a key concern in the organi-
Task
specific descriptive norm. Savani, Morris, and zational context, and this tripartite typology of task
Naidu (2012) found that priming Indian partici- situations is highly relevant in this context. We
pants with their boss induced them to choose more hypothesize that different types of tasks cue differ-
practical workshops and to feel guilt and shame ent if
constructs because of the mental questions they
they did not, indicating the activation of an injunc-
evoke. Tasks that involve ethical and identity issues,
tive norm. such as whether one should knowingly sell defected
The salience of social norms varies across coun- products to customers, are motivational in nature
tries. Gelfand et al. (2011) measured the tightnessandof likely to evoke values, which align judgments
norms across cultures by asking respondents andtoactions with self-view. Construal tasks that
report their perception of the typical behaviors of the interpretation of meanings and impli-
require
the members of their societies. In tight societies,cations, such as determining what product features
will appeal to a certain type of customers, likely
there is strict compliance with norms and deviations
from norms are sanctioned, whereas in loose socie- invoke schémas, which guide sense-making and
ties, norms are weak and violation of norms is inferential activities. Behavioral tasks that involve
tolerated. In tight cultures such as Pakistan andthe choice of an appropriate social behavior, such as
Malaysia, people report that many situations have whether one should interact with customers in
clear norms and norm violation is sanctioned, a formal or casual manner, likely evoke norms for
whereas in loose cultures such as the Netherlands guidance. This analysis based on the afforded que-
stions
and Brazil people are less likely to report this type of of task situations provides a parsimonious
situation (Gelfand et al., 2011). People in tight scheme to predict the type of construct that is
cultures are chronically aware that their actions activated
are in a situation and the theoretical basis to
being evaluated, so they are prevention-oriented, develop a general model of situated processes in
cautious, and dutiful; they exhibit more impulse future work.
control and more self-monitoring. People in loose A different direction for theoretical development is
cultures do not engage in such self-regulation, so
to address the interplay of the three types of con-
norms in general exert less influence on behavior. structs. Values as guiding principles of life may
activate schémas and norms. It is well-known that
Different situational variables influence the impor-
tance of each of the three mechanisms that trans- motives and goals, which are under the influence of
mit the influence of culture. Table 2 provides a
values, may activate schémas (e.g., Cohen, 1979).
summary of the moderator variables that influence In the organizational context, Harris (1994) pro-
the importance of values, schémas, and norms posed in a that an organizational culture, with values as
given situation. a key component, may activate schémas because of
the associations between the organizational culture
and certain schémas developed through experiences,
Activation of Values, Schemas, and Norms, andsocial learning, and interaction among organiza-
their Interplay tional members. These arguments suggest that cul-
The insights about distinctive cues for different tural values should activate schémas that are learned
kinds of schémas and norms help resolve many to be associated with them. Cultural values may also
promising but unintegrated ideas about how situa- activate norms through a similar process. Kwan et al.
tional cues activate behaviors inconsistent with (2014) argued that values perceived as widely shared
personal values and beliefs. We previouslymay reviewed
activate certain normative expectations. Their
the major situational factors that shape the empirical
salience results suggest that values perceived as
of each explanatory mechanism of cultural influ-
widely shared by Americans may activate a norm of
ence, and the next step is to explore how these iconic American brands.
preferring

Journal of International Business Studies

This content downloaded from


194.145.103.190 on Thu, 02 May 2024 15:08:20 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
•í&r
1041

Table 2 Effects of the situation on the importance of values, schémas, and norms

Values as mediators of the influence of culture

Conditions under which values drive thoughts and behaviors


Cues from social adaptation signaling
Privacy is high, for example, anonymous suggestions
Individual identifiability is low, for example, the presence of a large group of people
Low expected future interaction, for example, negotiation with a one-off customer
Lack of cues about meanings and interpretations, for example, in a new culture
Cues to moral or ethical dimensions, for example, corporate social responsibility decisions

Schemas as mediators of the influence of culture

Conditions under which schémas shape thoughts and behaviors


Accessibility
Cultural cues, such as iconic images, heritage languages or ethnic foods, send excitation through the network of associated
schémas (associative priming), for example, an American flag may elevate the accessibility of a competitive schema
Recent use (direct semantic priming), for example, the recent experience of a competitive negotiation makes a
competitive schema more accessible

Applicability
Fit to a stimulus or problem - classes of task situations elicit types of schémas that fit a problem, for example, the task of
figuring out what kind of interaction is going on in a cocktail party makes schémas about social events applicable

Ownership
Felt ownership of the accessibility of a schema - the absence of cues that a schema's accessibility arose from sources other
than the current task, for example, an executive might distrust the idea of a baseball-themed advertisement if it occurred
to him during the World Series, when all the headlines were about baseball

Norms as mediators of the influence of culture

Conditions under which norms direct thoughts and behaviors


Social evaluation

Presence of others, for example, presentation to customers


Salient collective identity, for example, high interdependence of group members
Wealth reduces the influence of social evaluation

Ambiguity - the absence of personal preference and knowledge about how to behave, for example, an unfamiliar situation
Presence of cues about specific norms - association of situational cues and specific norms based on social learning, for example, a
formal setting cues polite behaviors
Behavioral tasks with social implications, for example, drafting a public announcement
Cultural tightness - shared expectation of compliance with norms within a society, for example, Japan

Schemas and norms may also activate each other, restaurant may activate a schema of relationship
a possibility suggested by the cognitive-affective building for the European, which in turn may
personality system (Mischel & Shoda, 1995). In this activate a social norm of helpfulness. Another possi-
system, cognitive-affective units are activated by bility is that a schema may lead to a specific inter-
the psychological meanings attached to the situa- pretation of the situation and activate a norm
tion. Activated units, such as expectations and associated with this interpretation. Viewing an inter-
beliefs, and encodings (categories and constructs), cultural negotiation as an opportunity for relation-
may activate other units. Following this proposition, ship building may activate a compromise norm.
one type of activated construct, such as a lay theory By the same logic, social norms brought in focus
of the self as agentic, may activate a different type by situational cues may activate schémas. To illu-
of construct, such as a social norm because of their strate this process, consider the large compensation
learned association. Consider a European having gap between expatriate and local employees in multi-
a business lunch with a Chinese colleague in a national operations in China, with expatriates earn-
Japanese restaurant. The ambience of the Japanese ing much more than their local counterparts.

Journal of International Business Studies

This content downloaded from


194.145.103.190 on Thu, 02 May 2024 15:08:20 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
'

1042

This compensation gap is regarded as unfair intracultural


by than intercultural variation in value
locals; however, the feeling of unfairness depends
endorsement (Fischer & Schwartz, 2011). What this
on whether there is an organizational norm endor-
finding implies is still under debate, but sharedness
is that
sing the gap. Leung, Lu, and Lin (2014a) found not aadefining element of culture in the situated
framework.
pro-disparity norm, that is, acceptance of the com- Numerous situations, varying in situa-
pensation gap by local employees, was relatedtionaltostrength,
a exist in any culture, and subgroups
weaker relationship between the compensation may be gapformed based on the types of situations
and perceived distributive injustice. One account
in which people typically function. Assembly line
workers
of this finding is that the pro-disparity norm acti- are in stronger situations than farmers
vates a perspective on the compensation gap thatof the presence of strict rules and close
because
supervision
recognizes expatriates as from a different labor mar- in factories. In a similar vein, collecti-
ket and legitimizes their higher salaries. vism is not at a similar level throughout Japan,
Our knowledge about how values, schémas, but and
varies across prefectures (Yamawaki, 2012).
norms relate to each other is minimal and it is too Variation in the typical situations of different groups
early to offer precise theoretical statements withinabout a culture reduces sharedness, and the situa-
ted
their interplay. This is a priority research area as framework provides a mechanism to account
such
knowledge is key to developing a full account forofsuch variation. Sharedness may depend on the
their joint effects on behavior. heterogeneity of situations within a culture. In a
country like China, where there are significant
IMPLICATIONS OF THE SITUATED DYNAMICS regional differences in wealth and industries, shared-
FRAMEWORK ness should be lower than in countries with high
situational homogeneity, such as Denmark, where
The Conceptualization of Culture most people have similar incomes and work in the
service sector.
We identify three major implications of the situated
dynamics framework for the conceptualization Theoffinal implication is concerned with the static
culture: Context, sharedness, and variation.view Many of culture, perhaps best illustrated by Hofstede's
(1993) assertion that cultural change is very slow.
definitions portray culture with context-free charac-
teristics, such as the well-known definition of Insub-
contrast, the situated framework posits that some
jective culture by Triandis (1972: 4): "a cultural
aspects of culture, namely, schémas and norms, are in
group's characteristic way of perceiving the man
flux with the situation, and coherence and consis-
made part of its environment. The perception tencyof in cultural differences across domains are not
rules and the group's norms, roles, and values are Situations in which cultural groups would
assumed.
act in ways that contradict predictions based on
aspects of the subjective culture." The trait approach
to culture does recognize the role of situational
cultural values are commonplace (e.g., Yamagishi
variables, but they are conceptualized as external et al.,
to 2008). Cultural paradoxes, such as individuali-
and distinct from the construct of culture (Kirkman stic practices in Japan and collectivistic behaviors by
et al., 2006). In contrast, the situated framework Americans, are not enigmatic from the situated
views the situation as an integral part of culture perspective.
because situations are nested within culture, and Cultural theories must explain both cultural stabi-
the influence of culture cannot be fully understood lity and cultural change. Trait-based research has
without considering the situation. On a general focused on stable differences, while constructivists
level, Gelfand et al. (2011) showed that perceivedscrutinize changes across situations in which sché-
situational constraint is a useful construct for diffe-
mas influence behavior. The intersubjective app-
roach may enable an integrated understanding of
rentiating cultures, and that situational constraints
vary across situations within a culture, but some the stability assumed in trait accounts and the
cultures have more constraining situations than dynamism exhibited in the constructivists' expe-
others. riments. Prevalent schémas and norms of a culture
Another common theme in the definition of cul- are formed based on a history of social perception
and learning, and members of the same culture
ture is the foundational role of sharedness (Schwartz,
2014). In the trait approach, members of a culture
assume that these constructs are intersubjectively
valid. Americans would assume that most fellow
are assumed to converge on a trait that characterizes
the culture. Recently, sharedness as a foundation of
Americans subscribe to an agentic account of indivi-
dual behavior and endorse individualistic norms,
culture is cast in doubt by the observation of more

Journal of International Business Studies

This content downloaded from


194.145.103.190 on Thu, 02 May 2024 15:08:20 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
1043

whereas Chinese would assume that most Chinese negotiators use to characterize the behaviors of their
regard collectivistic norms as legitimate. Because of
opponents in intercultural negotiation.
such intersubjective perception, the central schémas
Measures of implicit theories, that is, a layperson's
and norms of a culture would generally change account of an event or phenomenon, can be used to
slowly, steadying their influence on behavior. This accessible schémas. A comprehensive set of
measure
process explains why young people in industrialized,
constructs for an implicit theory is identified, usually
wealthy Asian societies, such as Singapore, exhibit
by a literature review and/or interviews of infor-
collectivist behaviors. The dependence of schémas
mants, and respondents are then asked to indicate
and norms on social perception and learningthe also
usefulness of these constructs in describing or
explains how social change can occur, sometimes
explaining the event or phenomenon in question.
quite dramatically. When the prevalence of Engle
a newand Lord (1997) used this approach to mea-
behavior reaches a tipping point at which manysure implicit theories of leadership by identifying
more people adopt the new behavior, drastic the traits and behaviors seen as prototypical of
changes in schémas and norms and the behavior leaders. An application in the IB context is to use
they promote would ensue. This type of cascade can this method to identify implicit theories about effec-
explain rapid cultural changes despite centuries oftive leaders of multicultural teams. Different cultural
cultural stability, such as the surge in divorce rategroups may ascribe different traits and behaviors to
and materialism in China within a short period ofeffective leaders of multicultural teams.
time. Finally, accessible schémas can be identified by
cognitive or cause maps, which seek to understand
thought processes underlying judgment and deci-
Implications for Measurement of Culture sion making by means of a content analysis of
The situated dynamics framework calls for a careful narratives. Barr, Stimpert, and Huff (1992) used this
analysis of the situation. The notion of situational approach to analyze letters to shareholders to
strength is well-known in management (Meyer et al., understand the reasoning and decision-making
2010), but a standardized scale has been developed processes of top management. An application in
only recently (Meyer et al., 2014). This scale mea- the IB context is that in an international joint
sures individual perception of four hypothesized venture, managers with different cultural back-
dimensions: Clarity and consistency of situational grounds may have different understanding and
cues, and constraints on and consequences of deci- explanations of why certain work problems occur.
sions and behaviors. This scale is useful for identify- Cognitive maps can be examined to identify cul-
ing situations perceived to be low in situational tural differences in the schémas involved, and how
strength, in which cultural values are likely to be these differences influence the way work problems
expressed to influence behavior. are resolved. Different research questions call for
A fuller account of the situation requires the different measurement methods, and some custo-
measurement of schema accessibility and norm sal-mization of the method chosen is usually needed
ience. Cross-cultural research on schémas is primar- for a specific application.
ily experimental in nature and probes their influence The measurement of norms is more developed,
on judgment and behavior by systematical manip- and standardized measures are available. Several
ulations. This approach, while capable of providing studies have developed measures for a range of
causal evidence, is less applicable in the field context general descriptive norms across cultures, especially
where most IB research is conducted. There are well- norms associated with individualism-collectivism
established methods to identify schémas that people
(e.g., Fischer et al., 2009; Gelfand et al., 2011).
use for judgment and decision making, which canAbe whole host of measures have been developed to
adopted in the IB context. In semi-structured elicita-
measure salient norms for specific situations (e.g.,
tion procedures, respondents are asked to list Goldstein
the & Cialdini, 2010), and research has also
constructs that they frequently use for a well-defined
targeted specific norms that are important in the IB
judgment or decision-making task. Wojciszke, context (e.g., Leung et al., 2014a). The major chal-
Bazinska, and Jaworski (1998) used this procedure lenge is that researchers need to identify the salient
to identify a set of personality traits of other people norms in a situation of interest before an appropriate
that respondents regarded as important and drew measure can be identified or developed. What is
their attention. An application in the IB context is to currently lacking is a taxonomy of major norms for
use this method to identify the salient traits that IB research, a major objective for future research.

Journal of International Business Studies

This content downloaded from


194.145.103.190 on Thu, 02 May 2024 15:08:20 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
1044

To sum up, theoretical analysis is needed to To illustrate the new insights offered by the situa-
identify the types of schémas and norms that areted framework beyond those offered by the value
activated by a given situation. The situated frame-approach, we consider the failure of the merger
work delineates the major types of situational factors of Mercedes-Benz and Chrysler. Germany and the
that activate schémas and norms. The many studiesUnited States are similar on value profiles (e.g.,
on schemes and norms reviewed before provide Hofstede, 2001), and the trait approach cannot
illustrations of how such a theoretical analysis canexplain the failure of this merger. In contrast, the
be conducted. situated framework would turn to differences in
schémas and norms between German and American

CONCLUSION executives for answers. A cause of the failure may be


related to cultural differences in schémas. The merger
Implications for IB Research was trumpeted as synergistic, but many German
The trait view of culture as context-free, shared, and
employees construed the working relationship as
stable is dominant in IB research as evidenced abyone-way
the flow of expertise from the German side
(Meiners, 2004). The view of many Germans that
popularity of value dimensions of culture. Cultural
differences are seen as pervasive and enduring, the and
American side brought little expertise to the
their negative effects, such as intercultural conflict,
merger may be one reason why DaimlerChrysler was
hard to overcome. The situated dynamics framework
not able to capture the mass auto market, an original
recognizes that the general pattern of behavior in the merger (Badrtalei & Bates, 2007).
goal of
a culture shows long-term stability, but cultural With regard to the influence of cultural differences
differences may fluctuate across situations because
in norms, American executives perceived the merger
as between "equals," but German executives domi-
the activated schémas and salient norms may differ.
This situated approach to culture provides nated
new the new company and many American exe-
insights for many IB research topics, and we illu- left shortly after the merger (Badrtalei &
cutives
Bates, 2007). A cause of the failure may be that
strate the utility of this new approach by contrasting
American executives did not endorse the hierarchi-
it with the trait approach in two lines of IB research.
cal norm held by German executives in managing
The notion of cultural distance has a long history
in IB research, which is typically measured the by the
way the two cultural groups worked together.
similarity between two cultures' value profiles From a situated dynamics perspective, the
(Kogut Sc Singh, 1988) based on the dimensions of
DaimlerChrysler case suggests that to understand
Hofstede (2001) and GLOBE (House et al., 2004).the
Theperformance of international alliances and
mergers, researchers need to examine the typical
trait approach to culture suggests that collaboration
among people and firms from cultures with a situations
large and identify the relevant schémas and
cultural distance is difficult because pervasive,norms
stable
that shape decision making and intercultural
cultural differences hinder intercultural interaction interaction. Researchers also need to consider the
various types of moderator variables posited in the
(e.g., Manev Sc Stevenson, 2001). However, the gene-
ral thesis of the trait approach that intercultural
situated framework that may shape the salience of
difficulty increases with cultural distance is these
cha-schémas and norms.
The second example is concerned with the glo-
llenged by substantial contradictory findings (e.g.,
Brouthers & Brouthers, 2001). balization of firms, a key IB research area in which
From the perspective of the situated framework, the traditional focus is on the issues confronting
cultural values and hence cultural distance should Western multinationals in emerging markets. There
matter more in weak situations and less in is strong
a recent trend of reverse foreign direct investment
situations. The situated framework anticipatesfrom the
emerging economies into the West, notably
inconsistent findings associated with culturalfromdis-
China (e.g., Child Se Rodrigues, 2005). A high-
tance because in strong situations, situational cues,
profile example is Lenovo, a Chinese multinational
not cultural distance based on personal value that acquired the PC division of IBM in 2005 and
endor-
sements, shape individual behavior through activa- Mobility in 2014. IB theory and research
Motorola
ted schémas and norms. For instance, Nouri, Erez, do not have much to offer for understanding
Rockstuhl, Ang, Leshem-Calif, and Rafaeli (2013) the cultural issues confronting multinationals from
showed that cultural distance mattered less for team developing economies when they enter mature mar-
kets. In the case of Chinese executives sojourning
performance in the presence of a clear task structure,
a feature of a strong situation. in the West, the value approach suggests that

Journal of International Business Studies

This content downloaded from


194.145.103.190 on Thu, 02 May 2024 15:08:20 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
1045

need
researchers need to examine the compatibility of to be sensitive about the situational variables
the
value profile of Chinese executives with the thatvalue
influence individual behavior in a multicultural
profiles of their host cultures. The situatedcontext.frame-If a situation is judged to be weak, cultural
work, however, would call attention to the salient differences in values are important and should be
schémas and norms that bear on their effectiveness the target of intervention effort for improving inter-
in a host culture. One important schema is asso-
cultural collaboration. An example is a multicultural
ciated with the status of China as a developingR&D
eco-team, in which autonomy is encouraged to
nomy with few leading firms, as Western employees
promote creativity (West, Hirst, Richter, & Shipton,
may view their Chinese bosses as high in position 2004). Cultural values are likely to be expressed
power, but low in expert power. These Chinese in this context, and team managers need to recog-
expatriates may be disadvantaged by the negative nize and manage cultural differences in values to
halo around their third world status in the eyes minimize unproductive intercultural conflict.
of their Western subordinates, which may handicap Second, international managers need to under-
their legitimacy as effective leaders (DeRue & stand and address the influence of activated schémas
Ashford, 2010). The situated framework points to and norms in strong situations. Consider the con-
the need to examine the extent to which this clusion of Shenkar (2001) that the symmetrical
schema is used by Westerner employees and its
effects of cultural distance have no clear support,
consequences, and the moderator variables and anthat
example is that German firms operating in
affect the accessibility and utilization of this the United
schemaStates are not in an equivalent situation
in different situations. as American firms operating in Germany. In fact,
A relevant norm that impacts the effectiveness ofGerman executives sojourning in the United States
Chinese executives in the West is concerned with face different challenges than those confronting
American executives in Germany (Selmer, Chiù, &
what are regarded as effective managerial practices.
Shenkar, 2007). The activated schémas and norms
Chinese executives may affirm their cultural heritage
by engaging in managerial behaviors that are are
typical
likely to be different for Germans in the United
in China, or they may accommodate to their host
States and Americans in Germany. To help German
cultures by adopting Western management andprac-
American expatriates adapt to their host cul-
tices (e.g., Bond & Yang, 1982). Their perception
tures, of
it is important to identify what schémas and
the normative expectations of two referentnorms groups facilitate their performance in their specific
are relevant, namely, Chinese and Westerners in Intervention efforts, such as promoting the
context.
adaptive schémas and norms by effective organi-
the firm. They may perceive similar or different
normative expectations from these two groups, zational
and and job design, can be implemented.
the situated framework would call attention to It is also important to identify and strengthen the
examining the impact of these two types ofmoderator
norma- variables that promote the accessibility of
tive perceptions and how moderator variables may
adaptive schémas and norms. For instance, schémas
affect their relative salience. and norms that promote intercultural learning are
In summary, the two examples illustrate how theuseful for intercultural adaptation and can be pro-
situated framework may provide new research moted by creating a learning climate (Edmondson,
1999).
insights beyond those offered by the value approach.
In accounting for cultural differences in behavior,Third, selection and training of expatriates tradi-
researchers need to consider the influence of the tionally target personal characteristics predictive of
situation, the relevance of schémas and norms, and
expatriate success (Leung, Ang, & Tan, 2014b). The
situated framework underscores schémas and norms
the moderator variables that impact their relative
influence. activated in a host culture as important foci for
the selection and training of expatriates. Expatriate
Implications for Practice managers need to acquire the adaptive schémas and
The situated dynamics framework suggests that norms, and one effective strategy is to learn how to
create and maintain situations conducive to their
international managers have to consider the influ-
ence of cultural values and at the same time under- activation. As discussed before, a large compensation
stand the influence of the situation in terms of gap between expatriates from high-income countries
schema accessibility and norm salience. We
andillu-
local employees in developing economies is a
source of dissatisfaction for locals. Expatriates should
strate the practical utility of the situated framework
receive training about strategies and management
by three major applied implications. First, managers

Journal of International Business Studies

This content downloaded from


194.145.103.190 on Thu, 02 May 2024 15:08:20 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
*

1046

practices that promote a pro-disparity normgroup-level


in their constructs, whereas the individually
firms to minimize the strain of the pay gapperceived
on their situation is at the individual level, thus
calling
working relationships with locals (Leung for multilevel theoretical development in
et al.,
2014b). future research (Fischer, 2013).
Fourth, the relative influence of schémas and
Limitations and Directions for Future Research norms in strong situations depends on situational
While the situated dynamics framework provides a
factors, and we posit several types of such moderator
variables. There is the need to develop a compre-
richer description of cultural dynamics than the trait
approach to culture, some of the dynamics need hensive account of the variables that moderate the
further development. We discuss several limitationsinfluence of culture through values, schémas, and
that require attention in future research. First, norms.
we Some moderators may show general moder-
focus on schémas and norms, but other constructsating effects, and others may show situation-specific
may be important. A leading possibility is cultural moderating effects. The identification of both types
can provide important insight into how values,
identity, which is usually not salient, but its effects
on behavior can be substantial once activated (e.g.,
schémas, and norms interact and jointly influence
Verkuyten & Pouliasi, 2002). In a globalizing world,
judgment, decision, and behavior.
some individuals may identify with two or more Finally, we conceptualize culture from a positivist
cultures (Chao & Moon, 2005), and the influence perspective
of as possessing distinct characteristics
that can be measured and even manipulated. We do
cultural identity is especially important and com-
plex for this type of individuals. We need more recognize an interpretative approach that views
research to understand how the activation of cul- culture as an integrative whole and cannot be
described and measured in a reductionist manner
tural identity may account for cultural differences,
and how this construct can be integrated into(e.g., the Gould & Grein, 2009; Shweder & Sullivan,
situated framework. 1993). Interpretative approaches to culture offer
Second, we need to probe the interplay between insight not captured by positivist approaches, and
values, schémas, and norms in influencing behavior. how to leverage such insight to enrich the situated
Values are more likely to be expressed in weak situa- framework has to be pursued in future research.
tions. Chronically accessible schémas and norms Romani and colleagues (e.g., Primecz, Romani, &
may also be influential in these situations, and more Sackmann, 2009; Romani, Primecz, & Topçu, 2011)
work is needed to examine their joint effects. We have provided some directions for the integration of
posit that schémas and norms rise in importance positivist and interpretative perspectives.
with situational strength, but we know little about To conclude, we integrate the trait, constructivist,
how the change in situational strength affects theand intersubjective approaches to develop the situa-
interplay of values, schémas, and norms, a priority ted dynamics framework to offer a rich perspective
area for future research. on culture. This framework can account for empi-
Third, in addition to the objective situation, Bond rical findings and conceptual issues that cannot be
addressed by the trait approach to culture, and
(2013) proposed a distinction between the situa-
tion as consensually perceived by a group and thesuggests important, novel topics for IB research.
situation as individually perceived by a social actor.
This distinction is important as different concep- ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
tualizations of the situation may show differentWe thank the editors, three anonymous reviewers, an
effects (Smith, 2013). In addition, the objective Michael Bond for their constructive comments on ear-
situation and the consensually defined situation are lier drafts of the article.

REFERENCES
Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. 2003. The silence of the library:
Ashton-|ames, C. E., Maddux, W. W., Calinsky, A. D., St Chartrand,
Environment, situational norm, and social behavior, journalT.of
L. 2009. Who I am depends on how I feel: The role of affect in
Personality and Social Psychology, 84(1 ): 1 8-28. the expression of culture. Psychological Science, 20(3): 340-346.
Asch, S. E. 1956. Studies of independence and conformity: Badrtalei, ]., & Bates, D. L. 2007. Effect of organizational cultures
on mergers and acquisitions: The case of DaimlerChrysler.
I. A minority of one against a unanimous majority. Psychological
Monographs, 70(9): 1-70. International Journal of Management, 24(2): 303-31 7.
Ashforth, B. E., & Anand, V. 2003. The normalization of corrup-
Barr, P. S., Stimpert, ). L., & Huff, A. S. 1 992. Cognitive change,
tion in organizations. Research in Organizational Behavior,strategic action, and organizational renewal. Strategic Manage-
25: 1-52. ment journal, 1 3(S1 ): 1 5-36.

Journal of International Business Studies

This content downloaded from


194.145.103.190 on Thu, 02 May 2024 15:08:20 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
1047

Chiù, C.-Y.,
Baskerville, R. F. 2003. Hofstede never studied culture. Accounting ; Ng, S. S.-L., & Au, E. 2013. Culture and social
Organizations and Society, 28(1 ): 1 -1 4. cognition. In D. Carlston (Ed), Oxford handbook of social cogni-
tion : R.,
Becker, M., Vignoles, V. L, Owe, E., Easterbrook, M. )., Brown, 767-785. New York: Oxford University Press.
Cialdini,
Smith, P. B., Bond, M. H., Regalia, C., Manzi, C, Brambilla, M.,R. B., Kallgren, C. A., & Reno, R. R. 1991. A focus
Aldhafri, S., Gonzalez, R., Carrasco, D., Paz Cadena, M.,theory
Lay, S.,of normative conduct: A theoretical refinement
andLI.
Schweiger Gallo, I., Torres, A., Camino, L., Özgen, E., Güner, réévaluation
E., of the role of norms in human behavior. In
M. P.P.,
Yamakoglu, N., Silveira Lemos, F. C., Trujillo, E. V., Balanta, Zanna (Ed), Advances in experimental social psychology,
Macapagal, M. E., Cristina Ferreira, M., Herman, Vol. G., 24:
de201-234. New York: Academic Press.
Sauvage, I., Bourguignon, D., Wang, Q., Fülöp, M., Cohen, C. E. 1979. Observational goals and schema activation:
Harb, C.,
Chybička, A., Mekonnen, K. H., Martin, M., Nizharadze, G.,
A theoretical framework for behavior perception. Journal of
Gavreliuc, A., Buitendach, )., Valk, A., & Koller, S. H. 2014.
Experimental Social Psycholoqy, 1 5(4): 305-329.
Cultural bases for self-evaluation: Seeing oneself positively DeRue, D. in S., & Ashford, S. J. 2010. Who will lead and who
different cultural contexts. Personality and Social Psychology will follow? A social process of leadership identity construction
Bulletin , 40(5): 657-675. in organizations. Academy of Management Review, 35(4):
Benet-Martinez, V., Leu, )., Lee, F., & Morris, M. W. 2002. 627-647.
Negotiating biculturalism: Cultural frame switching in biculturals Edmondson, A. 1 999. Psychological safety and learning behavior
with oppositional versus compatible cultural identities. Journal of in work teams. Administrative Science Quarterly, 44(2): 350-383.
Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33(5): 492-516. Engle, E. M., & Lord, R. G. 1997. Implicit theories, self-schemas,
Berry, J. W., Phinney, J. S., Sam, D. L., & Vedder, P. 2006. and leader-member exchange. Academy of Management Jour-
Immigrant youth: Acculturation, identity, and adaptation. nal, 40(4): 988-1010.
Applied Psychology: An International Review, 55(3): 303-332. Fang, T. 2010. Asian management research needs more self-
Bond, M. H. 201 3. Refining Lewin's formula: A general model for confidence: Reflection on Hofstede (2007) and beyond. Asia
explaining situational influence on individual social behavior. Pacific Journal of Management, 27(1 ): 1 55-1 70.
Asian journal of Social Psychology, 1 6(1 ): 1-15. Fischer, R. 2006. Congruence and functions of personal and
Bond, M. H., Leung, K., Au, A., Tong, K. K., & Chemonges- cultural values: Do my values reflect my culture's values?
Nielson, Z. 2004. Combining social axioms with values Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(1 1 ): 1 41 9-1 431 .
in predicting social behaviors. European Journal of Personality, Fischer, R. 201 3. Situational challenges: Putting biology,
18(3): 177-191. resources and multi-level constraints back into the picture.
Bond, M. H., & Yang, K. S. 1982. Ethnic affirmation versus cross- Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 1 6(1 ): 30-33.
cultural accommodation: The variable impact of questionnaire Fischer, R., Ferreira, M. C., Assmar, E., Redford, P., Harb, C.,
language on Chinese bilinguals from Hong Kong. Journal of Glazer, S., Cheng, B.-S., Jiang, D.-Y., Wong, C. C, Kumar, N.,
Cross-Cultural Psychology, 1 3(2): 1 69-1 85. Kärtner, )., Hofer, )., & Achoui, M. 2009. Individualism -
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. ). 2005. The origin and evolution of collectivism as descriptive norms: Development of a subjective
culture. New York: Oxford University Press. norm approach to culture measurement. Journal of Cross-
Brickner, M. A., Harkins, S. G., & Ostrom, T. M. 1986. Effects of Cultural Psychology, 40(2): 1 87-21 3.
personal involvement: Thought-provoking implications for Fischer, R., & Schwartz, S. H. 201 1 . Whence differences in value
social loafing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, priorities? Individual, cultural, or artifactual sources. Journal of
51(4): 763-769. Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42(7): 1 1 27-1 1 44.
Briley, D. A., Morris, W. W., & Simonson, I. 2000. Reasons asFischer, R., & Smith, P. B. 2003. Reward allocation and culture:
carriers of culture: Dynamic versus dispositional models of A meta-analysis. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34(3):
cultural influence on decision making. Journal of Consumer 251-268.
Research, 27(2): 157-1 78. Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. 1975. Belief, attitude ; intention, and
Brouthers, K. D., & Brouthers, L. E. 2001 . Explaining the national behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA:
cultural distance paradox. Journal of International Business Addison-Wesley.
Studies, 32(1): 177-189. Gelfand, M. )., Raver, j. L., Nishii, L., Leslie, L. M., Lun, )., Lim,
Bruner, ). S. 1 956. You are your constructs. Contemporary Psychol- B. C., Duan, L., Almaliach, A., Ang, S., Arnadottir, )., Aycan, Z.,
ogy, 1 : 355-356. Boehnke, K., Boski, P., Cabecinhas, R., Chan, D., Chhokar, J.,
Buchtel, E. E., & Norenzayan, A. 2008. Which should you use, D'Amato, A., Ferrer, M., Fischlmayr, I. C., Fischer, R., Fülöp, M.,
intuition or logic? Cultural differences in injunctive norms about Georgas, j., Kashima, E. S., Kashima, Y., Kim, K., Lempereur, A.,
reasoning. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 1 1 (4): 264-273. Marquez, P., Othman, R., Overlaet, B., Panagiotopoulou, P.,
Chao, G. T., & Moon, H. 2005. The cultural mosaic: A metatheory Peltzer, K., Perez-Florizno, L. R., Ponomarenko, L., Realo, A.,
for understanding the complexity of culture. Journal of Applied Schei, V., Schmitt, M., Smith, P. B., Soomro, N., Szabo, E.,
Psychology, 90(6): 1 1 28-1 1 40. Taveesin, N., Toyama, M., Van de Vliert, E., Vohra, N., Ward, C.,
Chen, S. X., Fok, H. K., Bond, M. H., & Matsumoto, D. 2006. & Yamaguchi, S. 201 1 . Differences between tight and loose
Personality and beliefs about the world revisited: Expanding the cultures: A 33-nation study. Science, 332(6033): 1 100-1 104.
nomological network of social axioms. Personality and IndividualGibson, C. B., Maznevski, M. L., & Kirkman, B. L. 2009. When
Differences, 41 (2): 201-211. does culture matter? In R. S. Bhagat, & R. M. Steers (Eds),
Child, 1., & Rodrigues, S. B. 2005. The internationalization of Cambridge handbook of culture, organizations, and work : 46-70.
Chinese firms: A case for theoretical extension? Management Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
and Organization Review, 1 (3): 381-41 0. Goldstein, N. j., & Cialdini, R. B. 2010. Managing normative
Chinese Culture Connection. 1987. Chinese values and the influences in organizations. In D. D. Cremer, ). K. Murnighan, &
search for culture-free dimensions of culture. Journal R. of V.
Cross-
Dick (Eds), Social psychology and organizations : 67-85.
Cultural Psychology, 18(2): 143-164. Boca Raton, FL: Taylor & Francis.
Chiù, C.-Y., Gelfand, M. J., Yamagishi, T., Shteynberg, G.,Gould, & Wan,S.C.)., & Grein, A. F. 2009. Think glocally, act glocally: A
2010. Intersubjective culture: The role of intersubjective culture-centric
percep- comment on Leung, Bhagat, Buchan, Erez and
tions in cross-cultural research. Perspectives on Psychological Gibson (2005). Journal of International Business Studies, 40(2):
Science, 5(4): 482^93. 237-254.
Chiù, C.-Y., Morris, M. W., Hong, Y.-Y., & Menon, T. 2000. Hall, E. T. 1 959. The silent language. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.
Motivated
Harris, S.
cultural cognition: The impact of implicit cultural theories onG. 1994. Organizational culture and individual sense-
dispositional attribution varies as a function of need formaking:
closure. A schema-based perspective. Organizational Science,
5(3): 309-321.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(2): 247-259.

Journal of International Business Studies

This content downloaded from


194.145.103.190 on Thu, 02 May 2024 15:08:20 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
1048

Heide, ļ. B & Miner, A. S. 1 992. The shadow of the future: EffectsA. W., Webster, D. M., & Klem, A. 1993. Motivated
Kruglanski,
of anticipated interaction and frequency of contact on resistance
buyer- and openness to persuasion in the presence or
seller cooperation. Academy of Management Journal, 35(2):
absence of prior information. Journal of Personality and Social
265-291 . Psychology, 65(5): 861-876.
Kurman, J., & Ronen-Eilon, C. 2004. Lack of knowledge of a
Higgins, E. T. 1996. Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applic-
ability, and salience. In E. T. Higgins, & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds),
culture's social axioms and adaptation difficulties among immi-
Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles : 1 33-1 68. grants.
New Journal of Cross-Cultura I Psychology, 35(2): 1 92-208.
York: Guilford Press. Kwan, L. Y.-Y., Chiù, C.-y., & Leung, A. K.-y. 2014. Priming
Higgins, E. T., Bargh, ). A., & Lombardi, W. ). 1985. Nature
Bush (vs. Obama) increases liking of American brands: The role
of intersubjectively important values. Social Influence, 9(3):
of priming effects on categorization. Journal of Experimental
206-223.
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 1 1 (1 ): 59-69.
Lamoreaux,
Hirai, M. 2000. Stereotypes about the Japanese: Differences in M., & Morling, B. 2012. Outside the head and
evaluations between "the Japanese" and "myself". Japaneseoutside individualism-collectivism: Further meta-analyses of
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39(2): 1 03-1 1 3. cultural products. Journal of Cross-Cultura I Psychology, 43(2):
299-327.
Hofstede, G. 1 980. Culture's consequences: International differences
in work-related values . Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Leung, K., & Bond, M. H. 2004. Social axioms: A model for social
beliefs in multicultural perspective. In M. P. Zanna (Ed),
Hofstede, G. 1993. Cultural constraints in management theories.
Academy of Management Executive, 7(1 ): 81 -94. Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 36: 119-197.
New York: Academic Press.
Hofstede, G. 2001. Culture's consequences: Comparing values,
behaviors, institutions, and organizations across nations, 2nd K., & Bond, M. H. (Eds) 2009. Psychological aspects of social
Leung,
edn. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. axioms: Understanding global belief systems. New York: Springer.
Hong, Y.-Y., Morris, M. W., Chiù, C.-Y., & Benet-Martinez, V. 2000. K., Bhagat, R. S., Buchán, N. R., Erez, M., & Gibson, C. B.
Leung,
Multicultural minds: A dynamic constructive approach to2005. cul-Culture and international business: Recent advances and
ture and cognition. American Psychologist, 55(7): 709-720. their implications for future research. Journal of International
Business
House, R. J., Hanges, P. J., Javidan, M., Dorfman, P. W., & Gupta, V. Studies, 36(4): 357-378.
(Eds) 2004. Culture, leadership, and organizations: The GLOBE
Leung, K., Au, A., Huang, X., Kurman, J., Niit, T., & Niit, K. K.
study of 62 societies. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 2007. Social axioms and values: A cross-cultural examination.
European Journal of Personality, 21 (2): 91-111.
Inglehart, R., & Welzel, C. 2005. Modernization, cultural change,
and democracy: The human development sequence. NewLeung, York:A. K.-Y., Lee, S.-L., & Chiù, C.-Y. 201 3. Meta-knowledge of
Cambridge University Press. culture promotes cultural competence. Journal of Cross-Cultura I
Kashima, Y. 2000. Maintaining cultural stereotypes in the Psychology,
serial 44(6): 992-1 006.
reproduction of narratives. Personality and Social Psychology Leung, K., Lu, L., & Lin, X. 2014a. Compensation disparity
Bulletin, 26(5): 594-604. between locals and expatriates in China: A multilevel analysis
Kashima, Y. 2014. Meaning, grounding, and the construction of influence of norms. Management International Review,
of the
54(1): 107-128.
social reality. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 1 7(2): 81-95.
Kashima, Y., Pearson, L. J., & Pearson, C. 201 3. The acquisition
Leung,of K., Ang, S., & Tan, M. L. 2014b. Intercultural competence.
perceived descriptive norms as social category learning in social
Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational
networks. Social Networks, 35(4): 711-719. Behavior, 1 : 489-51 9.
Li, Q., & Hong, Y.-Y. 2001. Intergroup perceptual accuracy
Kim, H., & Markus, H. R. 1999. Deviance or uniqueness, harmony
or conformity? A cultural analysis. Journal of Personality and
predicts real-life intergroup interactions. Group Processes Sr
Social Psychology, 77(4): 785-800. Intergroup Relations, 4(4): 341-354.
Kim, Y.-H., Chiù, C.-Y., Peng, S., Cai, H., & Tov, W. 2010.Lim, K. H., Leung, K., Sia, C. L., & Lee, M. K. 2004. Is eCommerce
Explaining East-West differences in the likelihood of makingboundary-less? Effects of individualism-collectivism and uncer-
favorable self-evaluations: The role of evaluation apprehension
tainty avoidance on internet shopping. Journal of International
Business Studies, 35(6): 545-559.
and directness of expression. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psycho-
logy, 41 0): 62-75. Liu, J., Smeesters, D., & Vohs, K. D. 2012. Reminders of money
Kirkman, B. L., Lowe, K. B., & Gibson, C. B. 2006. A quarter
elicit feelings of threat and reactance in response to social
century of Culture's Consequences : A review of empirical
influence. Journal of Consumer Research, 38(6): 1 030-1 046.
research incorporating Hofstede's cultural values framework.
Loersch, C., & Payne, B. K. 201 1 . The situated inference model:
Journal of International Business Studies, 37(3): 285-320. An integrative account of the effects of primes on perception,
Kirkman, B. L., Chen, G., Farh, J. L., Chen, Z. X., & Lowe, behavior,
K. B. and motivation. Perspectives on Psychological Science,
6(3): 234-252.
2009. Individual power distance orientation and follower reac-
tions to transformational leaders: A cross-level, cross-cultural
Luria, A. R. 1 976. The cognitive development: Its cultural and social
examination. Academy of Manaqement Journal, 52(4): 744-764. foundations. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., Matsumoto, H., & Norasakkunkit,Manev,
V. I. M., & Stevenson, W. B. 2001. Nationality, cultural
1997. Individual and collective processes in the construction of
distance, and expatriate status: Effects on the managerial net-
the self: Self-enhancement in the United States and self- work in a multinational enterprise. Journal of International
Business Studies, 32(2): 285-303.
criticism in Japan. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
72(6): 1245-1267. Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. 1991. Culture and the self:
Kitayama, S., Mesquita, B., & Karasawa, M. 2006. CulturalImplications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psycholo-
affordances and emotional experience: Socially engaging and gical Review, 98(2): 224-253.
McSweeney,
disengaging emotions in Japan and the United States. Journal of B. 2002. Hofstede's model of national cultural
Personality and Social Psychology, 91 (5): 890-903. differences and their consequences: A triumph of faith - A
failure of analysis. Human Relation, 55(1 ): 89-1 1 8.
Knowles, E. D., Morris, M. W., Chiù, C. Y., & Hong, Y. Y. 2001.
Culture and the process of person perception: EvidenceMeiners, for J. 2004. Germans view Chrysler as a drag on M-B.
automaticity among East Asians in correcting for situational Automotive News Europe, 9(22): 1 .
Mendoza-Denton, R., & Mischel, W. 2007. Integrating system
influences on behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
27(10): 1344-1356. approaches to culture and personality: The cultural cognitive-
Kogut, B., & Singh, H. 1 988. The effect of national culture on theaffective processing system (C-CAPS). In S. Kitayama, & D. Cohen
choice of entry mode. Journal of International Business Studies,(Eds), Handbook of cultural psychology : 175-195. New York:
19(3): 411-432. Guilford.

Journal of International Business Studies

This content downloaded from


194.145.103.190 on Thu, 02 May 2024 15:08:20 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
"ì&r
1049

Primecz,
Menon, T., Morris, M. W., Chiù, C.-Y., & Hong, Y.-Y. 1 999. H., Romani, L., & Sackmann, S. A. 2009. Cross-cultural
Culture
and the construal of agency: Attribution to individual versus
management research: Contributions from various paradigms.
group dispositions, journal of Personality and Social Psychology, International journal of Cross-Cultural Management, 9(3): 267-274.
76(5): 701-717. Ralston, D. A., Egri, C. P., Stewart, S., Terpstra, R. H., & Kaicheng, Y.
Meyer, R. D., Dalai, R. S., & Hermida, R. 2010. A review and 1 999. Doing business in the 21 st century with the new genera-
synthesis of situational strength in the organizational sciences. tion of Chinese managers: A study of generational shifts in work
Journal of Management, 36(1): 121-140. values in China, journal of International Business Studies, 30(2):
415-428.
Meyer, R. D., Dalai, R. S., José, I. )., Hermida, R., Chen, T. R., Vega,
R. P., Brooks, C. K., & Khare, V. P. 2014. Measuring job-related Romani, L., Primecz, H., & Topçu, K. 201 1 . Paradigm interplay
situational strength and assessing its interactive effects with for theory development: A methodological example with the
personality on voluntary work behavior, journal of Management, Kulturstandard method. Organizational Research Methods,
40(4): 1010-1041. 14(3): 432-455.
Savani, K., Morris, M. W., Naidu, N. V. R., Kumar, S., &
Milgram, S. 1963. Behavioral study of obedience, journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67(4): 371-378. Berlia, N. V. 201 1 . Cultural conditioning: Understanding inter-
Mischel, W. 1968. Personality and assessment. New York: John personal accommodation in India and the United States in
Wiley. terms of the modal characteristics of interpersonal influence
Mischel, W. 1973. Toward a cognitive social learning reconcep- situations, journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1 00(1 ):
tualization of personality. Psycholoqical Review, 80(4): 252-283. 84-1 02.
Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. 1995. A cognitive-affective system Savani, K., Morris, M. W., & Naidu, N. V. R. 2012. Deference in
theory of personality: Reconceptualizing situations, disposi- Indians' decision making: Introjected goals or injunctive norms?
tions, dynamics, and invariance in personality structure. Psycho- journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1 02(4): 685-699.
logical Review, 1 02(2): 246-268. Schultz, P. W., Nolan, J. M., Cialdini, R. B., Goldstein, N. )., &
Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. 2010. The situated person. In Griskevicius, V. 2007. The constructive, destructive, and recon-
B. Mesquita, L. F. Barrett, & E. R. Smith (Eds), The mind in structive power of social norms. Psychological Science, 18(5):
context : 149-1 73. New York: Guilford Press. 429-434.
Mok, A., & Morris, M. W. 2009. Cultural chameleons and icono-Schwartz, S. H. 1994. Beyond individualism/collectivism: New
clasts: Assimilation and reactance to cultural cues in biculturals' cultural dimensions of values. In U. Kim, H. C. Triandis,
expressed personalities as a function of identity conflict, journalÇ. Kâgitçiba§i, S.-C. Choi, & G. Yoon (Eds), Individualism and
of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(4): 884-889. collectivism: Theory, method, and applications, Vol. 18: 85-119.
Mok, A., & Morris, M. W. 2013. Bicultural self-defense in Thousand Oaks. CA: Saae.
consumer contexts: Self-protection motives are the basis for Schwartz, S. H. 2006. A theory of cultural value orientations:
contrast versus assimilation to cultural cues, journal of ConsumerExplication and applications. International Studies in Sociology
Psychology, 23(2): 1 75-1 88. and Social Anthropology, 5(2-3): 1 37-1 82.
Morling, B., Kitayama, S., & Miyamoto, Y. 2002. Cultural practices Schwartz, S. H. 2014. Rethinking the concept and measurement
emphasize influence in the United States and adjustment inof societal culture in light of empirical findings, journal of Cross-
japan. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(3): 31 1-323.Cultural Psychology, 45(1 ): 5-1 3.
Morris, M. W., & Peng, K. 1 994. Culture and cause: American and Selmer, j., Chiù, R. K., & Shenkar, O. 2007. Cultural distance
Chinese attributions for social and physical events, journal of asymmetry in expatriate adjustment. Cross Cultural Manage-
Personality and Social Psychology, 67(6): 949-971 . ment, 14(2): 150-160.
Shenkar, O. 2001. Cultural distance revisited: Towards a more
Morris, M. W., Leung, K., & Iyengar, S. S. 2004. Person percep-
tion in the heat of conflict: Negative trait attributions affectrigorous conceptualization and measurement of cultural differ-
procedural preferences and account for situational and cultural ences. journal of International Business Studies, 32(3): 51 9-535.
differences. Asian journal of Social Psychology, 7(2): 1 27-1 47. Sherif, M. 1936. The psychology of social norms. Oxford: Harper.
Shih, M., Ambady, N., Richeson, j. A., Fujita, K., & Gray, H. M.
Morris, M. W., Chiù, C.-Y., & Liu, Z. 2014. Polycultural Psychology.
Annual Review of Psychology, advance online publication,2002. Stereotype performance boosts: The impact of self-
September 22, doi: 1 0.1 1 46/annurev-psych-01 081 4-01 5001 . relevance and the manner of stereotype activation, journal of
Norenzayan, A., Smith, E. E., Kim, B. )., & Nisbett, R. E. 2002. Personality and Social Psychology, 83(3): 638-647.
Cultural preferences for formal versus intuitive reasoning. Shteynberg, G., Gelfand, M. j., & Kim, K. 2009. Peering into the
Cognitive Science, 26(5): 653-684. "magnum mystérium" of culture: The explanatory power of
Nouri, R., Erez, M., Rockstuhl, T., Ang, T., Leshem-Calif, T., &descriptive norms, journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 40(1):
Rafaeli, A. 201 3. Taking the bite out of culture: The impact of 46-69.
task structure and task type on overcoming impediments to Shweder, R. A., & Sullivan, M. A. 1993. Cultural psychology:
cross-cultural team performance, journal of OrganizationalWho needs it? Annual Review of Psychology, 44: 497-523.
Behavior, 34(6): 739-763. Smith, P. B. 201 3. Through a glass darkly: Viewing the social
Oishi, S., Wyer, jr., R. S., & Colcombe, S. j. 2000. Cultural variationcontexts of our behaviour. Asian journal of Social Psychology,
in the use of current life satisfaction to predict the future, journal16(1): 19-21.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(3): 434-445. Sussman, N. M. 2000. The dynamic nature of cultural identity
Osland, j. S., & Bird, A. 2000. Beyond sophisticated stereotyping:throughout cultural transitions: Why home is not so sweet.
Cultural sensemaking in context. Academy of ManagementPersonality and Social Psychology Review, 4(4): 355-373.
Executive, 14(1): 65-79. Swidler, A. 1986. Culture in action: Symbols and strategies.
Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. 2002. Rethink-American Sociological Review, 51 (2): 273-286.
ing individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical Taras, V., Kirkman, B. L., & Steel, P. 201 0. Examining the impact
assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128(1):of Culture's Consequences : A three-decade, multilevel, meta-
3-72. analytic review of Hofstede's cultural value dimensions, journal
Peeters, C., Dehon, C., & García-Prieto, P. 2014. The attention of Applied Psychology, 95(3): 405-439.
stimulus of cultural differences in global services sourcing. Taras, V., Steel, P., & Kirkman, B. L. 201 0. Negative practice-value
journal of International Business Studies , advance online publi- correlations in the GLOBE data: Unexpected findings, ques-
cation, September 4, 2014; doi:1 0.1 057/jibs.201 4.30. tionnaire limitations and research directions, journal of Interna-
tional Business Studies, 41 (8): 1 330-1 338.
Phinney, J. S., & Devich-Navarro, M. 1 997. Variations in bicultural
identification among African American and Mexican American Torelli, C. j., & Kaikati, A. M. 2009. Values as predictors of
adolescents, journal of Research on Adolescence, 7(1 ): 3-32. judgments and behaviors: The role of abstract and concrete

Journal of International Business Studies

This content downloaded from


194.145.103.190 on Thu, 02 May 2024 15:08:20 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
1050

mindsets. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Yamawaki,


96(1):N. 2012. Within-culture variations of collectivism in
231-247. Japan, journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 43(8): 1191-1 204.
Trafimow, D., Triandis, H. C., & Goto, S. G. 1991. Some tests
Zhang, of
S., Morris, M. W., Cheng, C. Y., & Yap, A. J. 2013.
Heritage-culture
the distinction between the private self and the collective self. images disrupt immigrants' second-language
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(5): 649-655.
processing through triggering first-language interference. Pro-
ceedings
Triandis, H. C. 1972. The analysis of subjective culture. New York:of the National Academy of Sciences, 1 1 0(28):
Wiley. 11272-11277.
Triandis, H. C. 1995. Individualism & collectivism. Boulder, CO:
Zou, X., Morris, M. W., & Be net- M arti nez, V. 2008. Identi
Westview Press. motives and cultural priming: Cultural (dis)identification in
Tung, R. L., & Verbeke, A. 2010. Beyond Hofstede and GLOBE: assimilative and contrastive responses, journal of Experimen
Improving the quality of cross-cultural research, journal of Social Psycholoqy, 44(4): 1151-1159.
International Business Studies, 41 (8): 1 259-1 274. Zou, X., Tam, K.-P., Morris, M. W., Lee, S.-l., Lau, I. Y.-M., & Ch
Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. 1989. Levels of personal C.-Y. 2009. Culture as common sense: Perceived consensus
agency: Individual variation in action identification, journal of versus personal beliefs as mechanisms of cultural influence.
Personality and Social Psychology, 57(4): 660-671 . journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(4): 579-597.
Verkuyten, M., & Pouliasi, K. 2002. Biculturalism among older
children: Cultural frame switching, attributions, self-identification,
and attitudes, journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33(6): 596-609.
Vohs, K. D., Mead, N. L., & Goode, M. R. 2008. Merely activating ABOUT THE AUTHORS
the concept of money changes personal and interpersonal
behavior. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1 7(3): Kwok Leung is Choh-Ming Li Professor of Mana-
208-212.
gement at Chinese University of Hong Kong. His
Vygotsky, L. 1 962/1 986. Thought and language. Cambridge, MA:
The MIT Press. research areas include justice and conflict, social
Wan, C., Chiù, C.-Y., Peng, S., & Tam, K.-P. 2007. Measuring axioms, creativity, international management, and
cultures through intersubjective cultural norms: Implications for cross-cultural research methods. He is a Deputy
predicting relative identification with two or more cultures.
tournai of Cross-Cultural Psycholoqy, 38(2): 21 3-226. Editor-in-Chief of Management and Organization
Wan, C., Chiù, C.-Y., Tam, K.-P., Lee, S.-L., Lau, I. Y.-M., & Peng, S. Review , a past president of International Association
2007. Perceived cultural importance and actual self-importance for Cross-Cultural Psychology, and a fellow of Acad-
of values in cultural identification, journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 92(2): 337-354. emy of International Business, Academy of Intercul-
West, M. A., Hirst, G., Richter, A., & Shipton, H. 2004. Twelve tural Research, and Association for Psychological
steps to heaven: Successfully managing change through deve- Science.
loping innovative teams. European journal of Work and Organi-
zational Psychology, 1 3(2): 269-299.
White, K., & Simpson, B. 201 3. When do (and don't) normative Michael W Morris is the Chavkin-Chang Professor
appeals influence sustainable consumer behaviors? journal of
Marketinq, 77(2): 7 8-95. at Columbia University in the Business School and
Wojciszke, B., Bazinska, R., & Jaworski, M. 1998. On the dom- the Psychology Department. Previously he worked
inance of moral categories in impression formation. Personality at Stanford University and, as a visitor, at universities
and Social Psycholoqy Bulletin, 24(1 2): 1 251 -1 263.
Wong, R. Y.-M., & Hong, Y.-Y. 2005. Dynamic influences ofin China, Japan, Korea, and Spain. He has studied
culture on cooperation in the prisoner's dilemma. Psychologicalcultural differences in judgments, decisions, interac-
Science, 1 6(6): 429-434. tions, and relationships. In recent years his research
Yamagishi, T., Hashimoto, H., & Schug, J. 2008. Preference versus
strategies as explanations for culture-specific behavior. Psychlo- has focused on the psychological dynamics of cultu-
gical Science, 19(6): 579-584. ral influence, learning, adaptation, and change.

Accepted by Bradley Kirkman, Guest Editor, 29 September 2014. This article has been with the authors for two revisions.

Journal of International Business Studies

This content downloaded from


194.145.103.190 on Thu, 02 May 2024 15:08:20 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

You might also like