You are on page 1of 14

R O B E R T A .

L EV I N E

Ethnographic Studies of Childhood: A Historical Overview


ABSTRACT In this article, I briey survey the ethnographic research literature on childhood in the 20th century, beginning with the

social and intellectual contexts for discussions of childhood at the turn of the 20th century. The observations of Bronislaw Malinowski and Margaret Mead in the 1920s were followed by later ethnographers, also describing childhood, some of whom criticized developmental theories; still others were inuenced initially by Freudian and other psychoanalytic theories and later by the suggestions of Edward Sapir for research on the childs acquisition of culture. The Six Cultures Study led by John Whiting at midcentury was followed by diverse trends of the period after 1960including eld studies of infancy, the social and cultural ecology of childrens activities, and language socialization. Ethnographic evidence on hunting and gathering and agricultural peoples was interpreted in evolutionary as well as cultural and psychological terms. The relationship between ethnography and developmental psychology remained problematic. [Keywords: history, ethnography, child development, cultural acquisition, interpersonal relationships]

N THIS ARTICLE, I present an overview of ethnographic research on childhood during the 20th century, including works in biological, linguistic, social, and psychological anthropology. The literature is extensive and interdisciplinary, appearing in journals of psychology, psychiatry, and linguistics, as well as anthropology, and in monographs and volumes of collected papers. It grew during the second half of the century, covering all regions of the world, and in the 1990s there were more ethnographic studies of childhood published in book form than in any previous decade. The aim of this article is to show the extent and diversity of published ethnographic accounts of childhood over the past 80 years,1 providing a bibliography and a basic historical background that might prove useful for future analyses.2 In the interests of brevity, I have limited this account to ethnographies of infancy and childhood in domestic and community settings. That has meant omitting not only theoretical treatises, psychometric studies, and school studies but also other bodies of anthropological research on childhood: studies of immigrant children (e.g., SuarezOrozco and Suarez-Orozco 2001); studies of chronically ill, dying, or disabled children (e.g., Bluebond-Langner 1978, 1996); research on child abuse and other social problems of children in diverse cultures (e.g., Korbin 1981; Montgomery 2001; Montgomery et al. 2003; Scheper-Hughes 1987); the cultural politics literature on childhood (e.g., ScheperHughes 1992; Scheper-Hughes and Sargent 1998; Stephens

1995); and the archaeology of childhood (e.g., Baxter 2005). These bodies of research deserve, and in some cases have had, overviews of their own. An ethnographic study of childhood as used herein is a descriptive account, based on eld observations and interviews, of the lives, activities, and experiences of children in a particular place and time, and of the contextssocial, cultural, institutional, economicthat make sense of their behavior there and then. There would be no need for such descriptive accounts if childhood in ideal and practice were uniform across human populations and historical periods or if the developmental pathways for children were randomly distributed among humans. The ethnography of childhood, then, is based on the premiseconstantly reexamined in empirical researchthat the conditions and shape of childhood tend to vary in central tendency from one population to another, are sensitive to population-specic contexts, and are not comprehensible without detailed knowledge of the socially and culturally organized contexts that give them meaning. HISTORICAL BACKGROUND The recording and publishing of descriptions of the lives, activities, and experience of children did not originate with anthropology, but in the course of the 20th century anthropologists gave such accounts a distinctive stamp

AMERICAN ANTHROPOLOGIST, Vol. 109, Issue 2, pp. 247260, ISSN 0002-7294 online ISSN 1548-1433. C 2007 by the American Anthropological Association. All rights reserved. Please direct all requests for permission to photocopy or reproduce article content through the University of California Presss Rights and Permissions website, http://www.ucpressjournals.com/reprintInfo.asp. DOI: 10.1525/AA.2007.109.2.247.

248

American Anthropologist Vol. 109, No. 2 June 2007 from the home to study at postsecondary institutions, vote, and participate in public life; fertility was to be voluntarily controlled through contraception supported by sex education, so that families could be kept small; and children were to be raised scientically rather than according to the dictates of custom. In this milieu, Sigmund Freuds writings became available (in what he regarded as inadequate translations) and inuential. Freud was hardly an extreme environmentalist, but he was widely interpreted as claiming that young children were not only sentient and passionate beings but also highly vulnerable to parental and other early inuences, for worse as well as for better. By 1920, a Freudian context for the interest in childrens experience, often based on an exaggeration or distortion of Freuds ideas, was becoming widespread in public discourse on childhood. Freuds works drew unprecedented attention to the subjective experience of children and added a public preoccupation with the new and anxious questions: What is the best way to raise a child? What is the normal child? Through what stages of child development do infants become adults? Christian missionaries and colonial administrators of the late 19th and early 20th centuries wrote books describing the customs of the peoples with whom they worked in Oceania, Asia, Africa, and the Americas and included sections (often with photographs) on customs of child rearing that struck them as noteworthy, including adoption, naming and initiation ceremonies, practices of breast feeding and sibling care, and childrens houses. Although these observers were not as deliberate, comprehensive, or disciplined as the anthropologists who followed them, and although they were more likely to make ethnocentric judgments, such writers often had a good command of the local language, having spent long periods among the peoples they wrote about, with many opportunities to observe children. Some of the later missionary ethnographers like Henri Junod (1927), who wrote about the Thonga of Mozambique, were trained in anthropology and published valuable if fragmentary accounts of childrens lives in cultural context. Ethnographies of childhood by missionaries and colonial administrators continued into the 1940s (e.g., Childs 1949; Elwin 1947; Raum 1940). This early ethnographic literature provided the initial evidence for wide cultural variations in childhood environments used by anthropologists in generalizing about human childhood (e.g., Mead 1931; van Gennep 1960). Examination of this literature made it clear that there was divergence among peoples of the world in their concepts of the best way to raise children and of what constitutes normal child development. The stage was set for future confrontations between ethnographic evidence and the concepts of normal child development emerging from theory and research in Western countries. By 1928, professional anthropologists were publishing ethnographic accounts of childhood based on their own eldwork in non-Western communities. Margaret Mead (1928a, 1928b) and Bronislaw Malinowski (1929) were

as research to enhance knowledge of human variation for scientic and policy purposes. From the 1920s onward, anthropologists published ethnographic reports on childhood among human populations across the world, devised and rened observational approaches, and developed and borrowed theoretical frameworks for analyzing data collected in eldwork. Descriptions of customary practices regarding children and their activities appeared in the texts of many cultures, non-Western as well as Western, before the 19th century, including travelers reports on foreign practices that seemed strange (and therefore noteworthy) and clergymens advice to parents in a particular context. The 19th century saw a heightened public interest in children in Western countries, manifested in literary and journalistic representations of children as innocent victims of economic exploitation, the child-saving movement that sought to abolish child labor and provide care for orphans, the laws that made school attendance compulsory, and efforts to create an empirical science that would replace religious doctrine and philosophical speculation in public discourse about children. This public interest had been institutionalizedin school systems, orphanages, and academic and medical specialtiesby the end of the century, when anthropology and other social science elds were taking their modern forms. In the United States and western Europe soon after 1900, there were some fundamental alterations in the conditions of childrens lives: (1) Infant and child mortality began to decline, partly as the result of public health measures (purication of drinking water, pasteurization of milk, etc.) and sanitary practices in the home and other settings; and (2) mass secondary school enrollment extended schooling throughout childhood and into adolescence. The biomedical changes formed the basis of what might be called the medicalization of childcare, as illustrated by the hygiene movement in the United States, which was actively promoted by government and medical authorities (Ewbank and Preston 1990; Preston and Haines 1991). The vast increases in school attendance during the late 19th and early 20th centuries called public attention to the processes of learning and developmental change during childhood and stimulated the emergence of child psychology and educational psychology, led by pioneers such as G. Stanley Hall, Edward L. Thorndike, James Mark Baldwin, and (in France) Alfred Binet. In the United States, questions of learning were made urgent by the problems of educating large numbers of immigrant children. Thus, from about 1880 to 1920, childhood was increasingly formulated in what might be called pediatric and pedagogical terms, and there was public discussion about how science might improve the care and learning of children in the contexts of newly expanded medical and educational institutions. There were also new and radical ideas about the family, women, and child rearing that arose during this period in the Anglo-American world, although their greatest impact would be later. Women were to be freed

LeVine Ethnography of Childhood among the rst. In her brief article Samoan Children at Work and Play in Natural History, Mead presents a number of ways in which childhood in Samoa as she observed it differs from that of children in the United States: Samoan children change their own names, move from one relative to another, live in families of as many as 20, and are spoiled and pampered until the age of ve years old, when they become the caretakers or trainers of younger children. They have to work at gender-specic tasks of increasing difculty as they get older during childhood. (No ages are given.) They have no toys, but they participate in adult life almost from the start and play games in which they imitate adult activities. Yet they are punished if they stand out conspicuously above other children or attempt grown-up tasks above their status. These are themes that would be elaborated in detail in the ethnographies that followed over the 20th century. Like Mead, Malinowski, in The Sexual Life of Savages in North Western Melanesia (1929), was addressing the general public rather than other professional anthropologists, and his whole book was intended to describe Trobriand ideas and practices concerning sexual behavior that contrast with those of the West. But he also sought to show (as he had argued in his earlier book, Sex and Repression in a Savage Society [1927]) that Trobrianders, being matrilineal in social organization, had family dynamics and sexual development contrasting sharply with those Freud claimed to be generically human in his concept of the Oedipus complex. Malinowskis crystalline descriptions and vivid anecdotes were carefully selected to make this point: Trobriand father and mother are of equal status; fathers participate in infant care and take great pride in their infants; children are free and independent; and, although parents sometimes beat their children, Malinowski also observed quite as often children beating their parents (1929:45). He reports the childrens sex games he observed (with no interference by older persons [Malinowski 1929:50]) and those he heard about but was unable to observe. While making his case for a revision of Freuds ideas, he was demonstrating the ethnographers command of language, context, culture, and behavior that would set a standard for future studies. THE PLASTICITY PRINCIPLE AND HUMAN VARIABILITY The precursor to ethnographic research on childhood in U.S. anthropology was Franz Boass anthropometric work on child growth among European immigrants to the United States. Boas had been trained in anthropometric methods in 1883 in the Berlin laboratory of Rudolf Virchow, the pioneer of medical pathology (and physical anthropology), for whom plasticity was a major biological principle, operating at every level from the cell to the organism (Stocking 1968:139, 1974:22). Virchow argued, and Boas later taught, that the plasticity characteristic of biological processes often made it difcult to distinguish between normal and pathological variants, mandating caution in claiming as pathological a variant form that might actually represent an adaptive response to a particular environment. In other words,

249

difference may betoken adaptive variation rather than deciency or defect (Boas 1902; see also reprint in Stocking 1974:3641). Boas brought this perspective to his studies of child growth among immigrants, rst in Worcester, Massachusetts, when he was teaching in G. Stanley Halls Psychology Department at Clark University from 1889 to 1892, later in New York from 1908 to 1910. After the large New York study, Boas (1912) formulated a developmental perspective suggesting not only that human growth is inuenced by environmental factors but also that, given the gradual maturation of the human nervous system, the childs mental makeup must also be affected by the social and geographical environment (1912:217218). Boas left further specication of this perspective to his students Edward Sapir, Ruth Benedict, and Margaret Mead the founders of the culture and personality movement although it is unclear whether he liked what they did with it (Stocking 1992). His silence during the next 30 years on the very cultural psychology of child development to which he had opened the door was no doubt related to his wellknown antagonism to theoretical speculation. But as he was well acquainted with the psychologies of his time, the silence seems to indicate an especially prescient skepticism about psychological theories and methods, many of which were eventually thrown aside by child psychologists themselves in the course of the 20th century. This instability in psychology points to the paradox later anthropologists of childhood would face: Anthropologists are at least partly dependent on developmental knowledge from other disciplines in their assumptions about how children experience their environmentsincluding the environmental features to which they are sensitive and their age-related concepts for understandingand they have often turned to psychology and psychiatry for guidance in making these assumptions plausible. For much of the 20th century, however, this guidance was unreliable, as one developmental theory followed another into the trash heap of history. The Freudian theory of psychosexual stages led some ethnographers to focus their data collection on feeding, toilet training, and emerging sexuality in young children, whereas behavioristic psychology recommended recording the rewards and punishments in the childs environment. When these theories lost credibility, so did the ethnographic accounts based on them as adequate records of childhood experience. The turnover in developmental theories in psychology continued after 1960, as the inuence of Jean Piaget and other cognitive theorists waxed and then waned, and the social-cognitive formulations of Lev Vygotsky and his students and the attachment theory of John Bowlby and Mary Ainsworth introduced a new but not universally accepted focus on social interactions. Thus the chronic instability of dominant theories concerning the childs psychological development during the 20th century posed a quandary for ethnographers of

250

American Anthropologist Vol. 109, No. 2 June 2007 ogy has consistently immunized itself not only to evidence from ethnography and cross-cultural replications that casts doubt on its theories du jour but even to the gross limitations of its empirical base for generalizing about childhood: As of 2000, 88 percent of the primary schoolaged children in the world lived in the less-developed regions (UN Population Division 2005:10) where anthropologists typically work, but most psychological studies of children are conducted in the United States and a few other Western countries. The resulting knowledge decit has not been recognized by the child development research eld, even in the agenda-setting report by a National Academy of Sciences panel on The Science of Early Childhood Development (Shonkoff and Phillips 2000). Thus, relations between anthropology and developmental psychology have been sporadic and often distant. Yet there have been anthropologists throughout the century who addressed their research to issues in child development, and psychologists who have conducted culturally informed contextual research on children in diverse cultures. The eld studies of psychologists such as Wayne Dennis (1940), Patricia M. Greeneld (1966, 2004), Heidi Keller (in press), Barbara Rogoff (2003), Charles Super (Super and Harkness 1985, 1986), and Edward Z. Tronick (Tronick et al. 1992; Tronick et al. 1987) are examples of this latter tendency. ETHNOGRAPHY OF CHILDHOOD AS PART OF ANTHROPOLOGY Anthropologists, regardless of their developmental assumptions, sought to describe child rearing and the activities of children throughout the world. Even Mead (1930) did not intend to limit the anthropology of childhood to disproving the propositions of psychologists. She characterized world cultural variation in child rearing as a laboratory in which thousand-year experiments were being conducted by different peoples. The results were retrievable through ethnographic eldwork in distant places like Melanesia, to be brought back to the Western world for the resolution of issues like whether permissive rearing was advisable for U.S. middle-class children. Although they might have disagreed with the broad sweep of this claim, anthropologists were inspired by Meads metaphor of a laboratory and its mission of learning lessons about childhood from other cultures. Thus, in the 1930s and later, ethnographers sought to add to our knowledge of childhood by publishing their eld observations. In Britain, the students of MalinowskiRaymond Firth (1936), Audrey I. Richards (1932, 1956), Ian Hogbin (1931), C. H. Wedgwood (1938), Phyllis Kaberry (1939), E. E. EvansPritchard (1940, 1953), and Margaret Read (1960, 1968) and those directly inuenced by him like Meyer Fortes (1938, 1949)published accounts of childhood in varying amounts of detail. In his ethnographic classic, We, the Tikopia: Kinship in Primitive Polynesia (1936), Firth described the relational context of childhood and the initiation rituals for boys. In Chisungu (1956), Richards devoted an entire

childhood. If they organized their eldwork around the best knowledge available at a particular moment, they risked having their account of childhood become obsolete and inadequate by the time they published their ndings. Yet they had to make some assumptions about what was worth observing and recording in the environments and activities of children in diverse settings, and it was the developmental psychology of the day that offered attractive if potentially faulty assumptions. The ways in which anthropologists of childhood dealt with this dilemma is taken up in the following sections. CULTURAL CRITIQUE AND ETHNOGRAPHIC EXPLORATION A characteristic response of anthropologists to the claims of developmental psychology and psychiatry was to use ethnographic evidence to prove them culture bound, suggesting that a theory needed to be modied to encompass cultural variation or abandoned as a false generalization about the human species. Malinowski and Mead led the way. Malinowski (1927) proposed that Freuds concept of the Oedipus complex in young children had to be modied for matrilineal societies like that of the Trobriand Islanders, and Mead (1928b) argued that the Samoans she studied did not experience the adolescent turmoil posited by Hall (1904) as a human universal. Thus, before 1930, anthropologists had initiated a critical ethnography of childhood that could play a decisive role in rejecting universal propositions falsiable by a single negative instance (Kaufmann 1944) in the study of child development. Mead (1932) provided another example of this gady role for ethnographic research on childhood when she published an article criticizing Piagets theory of childhood animism on the basis of interviews she conducted with Manus children in Melanesia. Psychologists nevertheless continued to generate developmental propositions claiming universal validity during the rest of the 20th century, even after John Whitings (1954) persuasive case for cross-cultural analysis in the Handbook of Social Psychology, so the gady role has remained relevant, and anthropologists have continued to exercise their veto with evidence from nonWestern cultures. Piagets universalist account of childhood cognitive development, which became inuential among U.S. psychologists after 1963, also came under empirical attack with cross-cultural evidence at that time (e.g., Greeneld 1966; Shweder and LeVine 1975). Other proposed universals of child development that were subject to cultural critique included Lawrence Kohlbergs (1984) cognitively based model of moral development (Shweder et al. 1990), Carol Gilligans (1982) conception of gender differences in development (Miller 1994), and the John Bowlby-Mary Ainsworth attachment theory (Harwood et al. 1995; LeVine and Norman 2001). However powerful these cultural critiques may seem to anthropologists and to some leading psychologists such as Jerome Bruner (1990), developmental psychol-

LeVine Ethnography of Childhood monograph to the description of girls initiation among the Bemba of Zambia. Evans-Pritchard, in The Nuer (1940) and Kinship and Marriage among the Nuer (1953), offered generalizations about Nuer childhood and fatherchild relations based on his eld observations. Read, a student of Malinowski who pursued a career in colonial and international education, published a book-length study, Children of Their Fathers (1960, 1968), of childhood among the Ngoni of Nyasaland/Malawi. Fortes, who had received a Ph.D. in educational psychology before turning to anthropology, produced an unsurpassed observational account of learning and social relationships among Tallensi children in 1938, and then published more about the course of childhood in The Web of Kinship among the Tallensi (1949). These works by a generation of anthropologists inuenced directly by Malinowski leave no doubt that by the 1930s childhood was an established topic of ethnographic description, often in the context of kinship or ritual, sometimes in relation to education or socialization, only occasionally with psychological interpretations. Childhood was part of their anthropology, not a topic borrowed from developmental psychology or other disciplines (although Richards and Fortes knew the child development literature of their time). Future generations of British and U.S. ethnographers would continue to describe the care, relationships, and learning of children, and their social participation, in published reports from Africa (e.g., Gibbs 1965; Goody 1982; Grindal 1972; Leis 1972; Peshkin 1972; Smith 1954), the Americas (e.g., Bolin 2006; De Laguna 1965; Hughes 1974; Lantis 1960), Asia (e.g., Lebra 1976, 1990, 1994; Peak 1991; Schwalb and Schwalb 1996; Stafford 1995), Oceania (e.g., Fajans 1997; Howard 1970; Mead and MacGregor 1951; Ritchie 1957; Toren 1990, 1993; Williams 1969), and Europe (e.g., Mead and Wolfenstein 1954; Norman 1991). THE INFLUENCES OF FREUD, THE NEO-FREUDIANS, AND EDWARD SAPIR Some anthropologists in the rst half of the 20th century conducted ethnographic eldwork focused on the Freudian stages of psychosexual development in early childhood. This began with Geza Roheims attemptsuggested by Freud and funded by his disciple Marie Bonaparteto prove Freud right in claiming the Oedipus complex to be universal and, more urgently, to prove Malinowski (1927) wrong in his claim, based on ethnographic evidence from the Trobriands, that the Oedipus complex takes a distinctive form in matrilineal societies. In 1928, Roheim began a three-year trip that involved eldwork of varying durations among four non-Western peoples, two of them matrilineal. His results were published in 1932. Roheims conclusions were predictably supportive of Freud against Malinowski, but some of his eld methods, including observations of child behavior with dolls to elicit reactions, were innovative then and interesting even now. (His more detailed observations in Australia were not published until 20 years after his death as Children of the Desert [1974].)

251

Erik Eriksons (1950) work among the Yurok Indians of California was also based on Freuds psychosexual stages of child development, although he superimposed on them his famous psychosocial stages. But there was a wider and subtler Freudian inuence on many ethnographers, then and later, who assumed, for example, that breast feeding, weaning, toilet training, and sexual behavior were the most important events of early experience to investigate and describe, whether or not they interpreted them in terms of Freuds concepts of oral, anal, and phallic-genital stages or xations. Melford E. Spiros (1958) comprehensive and richly detailed account of childhood on an Israeli kibbutz, for example, devotes a chapter to the socialization of the oral, anal, and sexual behavior systems following the concepts and terminology of Whiting and Irving Child (1953)and another chapter to the Oedipus complex and sexual identity. Ironically, Whiting (1994:24 25) himself saw the ndings of their 1953 book, Child Training and Personality: A Cross-Cultural Study, as casting doubt on the validity of Freuds psychosexual stages, and in his own later research and writing they were rarely mentioned. Years later, Jean Briggs (1972) criticized Eriksons stages of early childhood from the perspective of her study of Inuit children. For many U.S. anthropologists interested in childhood during the 1930s and 1940sparticipants in the culture and personality movement initiated by Edward Sapir, Ruth Benedict, and Margaret Meadit was the neo-Freudian psychoanalysis (more accurately termed interpersonal) of Harry Stack Sullivan, Karen Horney, and Erich Fromm that supplied its conception of childhood experience. In this conception, there were no libidinal or psychosexual stages, and the focus was on the quality of the childs interpersonal experience with parents and siblings during the early years. Unlike the orthodox Freudians, the neo-Freudians were open to the study of cultural variation, and their theory could be related to social relationships as ethnographers observed them. The pioneering monograph in this vein was Balinese Childhood: A Photographic Analysis by Gregory Bateson and Margaret Mead (1942). Abram Kardinera psychoanalyst who, after graduate study with Boas, was trained as a physician and then was analyzed by Freud in 1921formulated another line of theory and research similar to the neo-Freudians, replacing Freuds biological drives unfolding in the course of early child development with a culturally variable theory of ego development. Kardiner organized an interdisciplinary seminar on culture and personality in New York that ran from 1933 onward, with numerous anthropologists participating. He also published (with the collaboration of Ralph Linton) two inuential volumes, The Individual and His Society (1939) and The Psychological Frontiers of Society (1945), based on the seminar proceedings. The anthropologist Cora DuBois (1944) worked with Kardiner in the seminar and then carried out eldwork among the Alorese in Indonesia that included ethnographic observations of childhood, based in part on Kardiners approach.

252

American Anthropologist Vol. 109, No. 2 June 2007


most ambitious psychological theory ever constructed. [Yet our three-year-olds are not all the same.] Our children are fully developed personalities very early. [We do not quite know how this comes about, but it depends considerably on] the interactions between the child and his early environment up to the age of three. In the childs cosmos, patterns of behavior are understood emotionally, [in terms of a particular constellation of relationships]. Study the child minutely and carefully, from birth until, say, the age of ten, with a view to seeing the order in which culture patterns and parts of patterns appear in his psychic world; study the relevance of these patterns for the development of his personality; and at the end of the suggested period, see how much of the total ofcial culture of the group can be said to have a signicant existence for him. Moreover, what degree of systematization, conscious or unconscious, in the complicating patterns and symbolisms of culture will have been reached by this child? [Sapir 1993:197198; square brackets in original]

In retrospect, these psychiatrically inspired ethnographies (i.e., Bateson and Mead 1942 and DuBois 1944) have a judgmental castBalinese mothers torment their toddlers, Alorese mothers neglect theirsand imply pathological consequences of the culture-specic parental practices they describe. This was consistent with neo-Freudian approaches, in which the mental illness of children was blamed on parents whose practices failed to give their children emotional security. The parents point of view on these practices is not given prominence in the ethnographic accounts. Furthermore, despite the claim of Bateson and Mead that Balinese culture provides therapeutic ritual dramas for the childhood conicts of adults, these ethnographies are not portraits of alternative normal or healthy pathways for child development. At the time, however, they represented a new focus on interpersonal relationships in early childhood, and Bateson and Meads photographs enabled readers to share a microscopic view of childhood social interaction in sequence. Jules Henry and Zunia Henrys (1944) study of sibling rivalry among Pilaga Indian children of the Argentine Gran Chaco, attempting to replicate a doll play experiment devised by the psychoanalyst David Levy (1937), was also focused on interpersonal relations in psychiatric perspective. In this case, however, the ethnographic observers tended to see the Pilaga parents and children with whom they worked as freer of pathogenic sibling rivalryand of remorse and self-punishment after their sibling conictsthan their counterparts in the United States. Henry and Henry concluded that sibling rivalry is as inevitable among the Pilaga as anywhere else, yet the Pilaga childrens lack of remorse is culturally determined. Finally, there is the inuence of Edward Sapir (1884 1939), who never did an ethnographic study of childhood but who proposed research on the childs acquisition of culture to his students and others who carried out such studies. Sapir had a long-standing interest in the psychology of culture and gave a course of lectures on that topic at the University of Chicago from 1926 to 1931 and at Yale until 1937. That course, reconstructed as a book from students lecture notes and published in 1993 by Judith Irvine, contained the following passages:
[Perhaps we can say something more about] the personal world of meanings, [if we consider] the eld of child development. As soon as we set ourselves at the vantagepoint of the culture-acquiring child, [with] the personality denitions and potentials that must never for a moment be lost sight of, and which are destined from the very beginning to interpret, evaluate, and modify every culture pattern, sub-pattern, or assemblage of patterns that it will ever by inuenced by, everything changes. Culture is then not something given but something to be gradually and gropingly discovered. [If we are to understand the transmission of culture, or indeed the whole problem of culture from this developmental point of view,] the time must come when the cosmos of the child of three will be known and dened, not merely referred to. The organized intuitive organization of a three-year-old is far more valid and real than the

These excerpts show that Sapir, prior to 1937, was constructing and advocating a view of childhood that, although inuenced by the interpersonal theory of psychiatry of his friend Harry Stack Sullivan, was focused on meanings, patterns, and organization in the childs experience rather than questions of pathology, emphasized the subjective experience rather than the behavioral conformity of the child, and assumed the child to be an active and denitive decision maker concerning the meanings of culture patterns. This is a subtle conception of cultural acquisition that seems as good a basis for research today as it was 70 years ago. Did anyone pursue these leads so cogently expressed by Sapir in his lectures? Not really, at least for a long while. Many of his graduate students at Yale were linguists with no apparent interest in childhood, but two who took his course were John W. M. Whiting and Beatrice Blyth Whiting, who devoted their careers to the anthropological study of childhood. John Whiting conducted eldwork on the transmission of culture to children among the Kwoma of New Guinea in 193637 and wrote a monograph entitled Becoming a Kwoma (1941) that contains much ethnographic data Sapir would have appreciated. In analyzing the data, however, Whiting followed the lead of John Dollard who, although brought to Yale by Sapir as a kindred spirit, was soon attracted to the (behavioristic) learning theory of the Yale psychologist Clark L. Hull. So Whitings account of the acquisition of culture by Kwoma children, although representing the descriptive and contextual richness of a Malinowskian ethnography (Malinowski was at Yale in 193839 and advised Whiting on the writing of the book), was interpreted (in the last two chapters) not in terms of Sapirs concepts but according to the stimulusresponse theory of learning derived from laboratory experiments with animals. Although not his student, Clyde Kluckhohn was also inuenced by Sapir and in 1939 published an article on the acquisition of culture (the culturalization of the child) in which he made recommendations for data on this process to be collected by eld workers. Like Whiting, however,

LeVine Ethnography of Childhood Kluckhohn was concerned with developing an empirical method for studying how children acquire culture, and his emphasis is on a quantitative approach, illustrated by the sample of 48 children in his Navajo research, which formed the basis for the monograph Children of the People (Leighton and Kluckhohn 1948). There is nothing of Sapirs focus on symbolic meanings and the childs subjective perspective in the article. It would be the 1980s before linguistic anthropologists translated Sapirs vision of the cultural study of childhood into ethnographic research. Yet several ethnographers working after 1950 focused on the subjective experience of the child in terms close to those Sapir was advocating: Hildred Geertz (1959) in Java, William Caudill (1972) in Japan, and Robert I. Levy (1969, 1973:430469) in Tahiti, for example. Thus, from 1928 to 1950, professional anthropologists led by Mead and Malinowski created an anthropology of childhood that was grounded in ethnographic eldwork in diverse cultures and critical of developmental formulations in psychology. They experimented with interpretive frameworks drawn from psychiatry, psychoanalysis, and academic psychology. They constructed concepts to study the childs acquisition of culture. The collection of ethnographic data on childhoodin relation to kinship, religion, the family, the life cycle, and other standard ethnographic topics, as well as psychological developmentbecame part of the ethnographers conventional toolkit, as eld studies were conducted in many parts of the world.3 THE SIX CULTURES STUDY OF SOCIALIZATION AND OTHER WHITING PROJECTS The Six Cultures Study (SCS), launched in 1954 by John Whiting (by then at Harvard) and two psychologists, Irvin L. Child at Yale and William Lambert at Cornell, was an ambitious project in the anthropology of childhood and deserves a separate article dedicated to its history. From the perspective of the present overview, it was a unique effort to harness six ethnographic studies in different parts of the world to collect comparable data, following a single detailed eld manual (Whiting et al. 1966), on childhood in social and cultural context. The six ethnographic studies of the SCS were conducted by trained anthropologists between 1954 and 1957 in a Mixtecan community in Mexico (Romney and Romney 1966); an Ilongot community in the Phillipines (Nydegger and Nydegger 1966); a Rajput community in India (Minturn and Hitchcock 1966); an Okinawan community in Japan (Maretzki and Maretzki 1966); a Gusii community of Kenya (LeVine and LeVine 1966); and the small town of West Acton in eastern Massachusetts (Fischer and Fischer 1966). Apart from the last, all were sedentary agriculturalists; none were hunter-gatherers or pastoral nomads, and none were urban. The studies were published in 1963 as a single volume edited by Beatrice Whiting and then as separate monographs in 1966. The six monographs combined the topics covered in a conventional general ethnography of the timeto estab-

253

lish the contexts of life for families, parents, and children with a special focus on the course of childhood that went beyond description of norms to report the frequency of specic practices in a sample of 24 families. Thus, the SCS described the variability of child rearing within a community as well as its culturally distinctive ideals and practices. Because the ethnographers had carried out naturalistic observations of children between three and 11 years of age, their accounts were enriched by anecdotes of child behavior from their many hours of observation. Each book constituted a record or reconstruction of the routine practices, relationships, settings, and activities that made up the environments of childrenand their interactions with those environmentsfrom birth to adolescence in a particular local community at a particular moment in time. The SCS ethnographies established a new standard for the detailed examination of childhood through ethnographic eldwork. The original plan of the SCS, however, was not simply to contribute toward . . . a more useful ethnography in the study of socialization and enculturation (Whiting et al. 1966:3) but also to test hypotheses relating child rearing to its causes and consequencesthat is, the socioeconomic and cultural factors that constrain and direct child rearing practices, on the one hand, and the psychocultural patterns resulting from childhood experience, on the other hand. This hypothesis-testing program proved problematic, largely because the processes posited by the SCS framework of psychoanalytic behaviorism were not adequately assessed through the data collected. The child interview and projective test designed to assess the psychodynamic outcomes of child rearing were difcult to apply in some of the communities, and the Field Guide provided no alternatives. Furthermore, the SCS used a mother interview to assess child rearing at the individual level (Minturn and Lambert 1964), but such retrospective interview approaches to childhood environments were shown to be unreliable even in the United States (Yarrow et al. 1968). When the Whitings (1975) published the nal report, they narrowed its analysis to relationships between the socioeconomic environments of families and the behavior patterns of children assessed through naturalistic observation, from which valid conclusions could be drawn. So, in the end, the SCS had little to say about the causal inuence of child rearing on personality development and cultural expression. Apart from the monographs, the most enduring (and largely unanticipated) contribution of the SCS may have been the introduction of systematic naturalistic observations of childrenthat is, repeated and aggregated observations of children in their routine behavior settings as a method for recording the interactions of children with their environments in diverse cultures. Beatrice Whiting salvaged the observational data for analysis and developed this method further for the comparative study of childhood in 14 diverse communities she conducted with Carolyn P. Edwards (Whiting and Edwards 1988) years later. While the SCS eldwork was being conducted, John Whiting became interested in male initiation ceremonies and, with his students, launched a program of cross-cultural

254

American Anthropologist Vol. 109, No. 2 June 2007 whom conducted their eldwork in the 1960s. Caudill and Konner were followed by many others. All focused their efforts on parental concepts and practices of infant care, the infants changing social environment, and the infants communicative and other social interactions, but they differed in their approaches, methods, and theoretical frameworks.4 It is noteworthy that the contributors to this literature include biological anthropologists (Konner, Chisholm, and Hewlett), who were often but not always studying hunter-gatherers, and psychologists (Super, Tronick, Morelli, Greeneld, and Keller), as well as social and cultural anthropologists. Super and Sara Harkness (1986) formulated the concept of the developmental niche to integrate diverse analytic perspectives on the observable conditions of infancy. There are rich ethnographic descriptions in some of these publications, whereas in others ethnographic evidence is used primarily as context to interpret quantitative observational data from ecological, cultural, developmental, and evolutionary perspectives. A related new direction for ethnographies of childhood after 1960 concerns the childs social relationships and social participation. These topics had been covered in earlier reports from the eld, but the new research involved closer examination of specic patterns and broader concepts to make sense of them. Adoption, for example, was studied in eastern Oceania (Carroll 1970), and the fostering of children in West Africa (Goody 1982). Thomas Weisner and Ronald Gallimore (1977) summarized the evidence on sibling care giving, suggesting that it was widespread among human societies (although not in contemporary Western ones) and had some advantages for infants and their older siblings who cared for them. Weisner (1984, 1987, 1989) later published more on this subject from his eldwork among the Abaluyia of Kenya and set it in the context of an ecocultural approach to childhood, drawing on the social developmentalism of the Soviet psychologists Lev Vygotsky (1978) and A. N. Leontev (1981) and requiring ethnography for its implementation. In a parallel move, Barbara Rogoff (Rogoff et al. 1993), a developmental psychologist who had done eldwork in a Guatemalan Mayan village over a 30-year period, coined the term guided participation to refer more accurately than earlier terms like rearing, training, and socialization to the way in which children take part from their early years in the social activities of their environments, with guidance from those around them. An extensive example of ethnographic description in this mode is provided by Suzanne Gaskinss (1996, 1999, 2000, and 2003) articles on Yucatec Mayan children. Childrens play also received ethnographic attention during the last quarter of the 20th century, in the studies by David Lancy (1996; see also Lancy and Tindall 1977), Helen Schwartzman (1978), and Lawrence Goldman (1998), as well as in the observations of childrens activities provided by the comparative study of Whiting and Edwards (1988), the ethnography of Gaskins (2003), and in many ethnographies not focused on play.

research on male identity. Its basic hypothesis linked the conditions under which boys were raised in infancy and early childhood to rituals ranging from circumcision to the couvade that socialized them into aggressively masculine adult roles or permitted them to act out female roles. The ideas involved were developed in cross-cultural analyses of published ethnographic data (Burton and Whiting 1961; Whiting 1964; Whiting et al. 1958) and led to the eldwork by Robert L. Munroe and Ruth H. Munroe among the Black Caribs of Belize (Munroe 1980; Munroe et al. 1973, 1981) and to Beatrice Whitings analysis (1965) of protest masculinity among the six cultures of the SCS. Judith Brown (1963) extended the comparative analysis to female initiation rituals. INFANT STUDIES, EARLY SOCIAL INTERACTION, AND GUIDED PARTICIPATION The last four decades of the 20th century saw anthropologistsbiological and linguistic as well as social and psychologicaland some developmental psychologists take new directions in ethnographic studies of childhood. There were no projects approaching the scale of the SCS, but this was an expansive period in which a greater number of academic scholars did more kinds of research in a wider variety of places. (The growth of U.S. universities and of funding for research in the 1960s facilitated this expansion; so did the extension of jet aircraft service to all parts of the world.) Infant research was one of the new directions for ethnography during this period. Even though it had been taken for granted by anthropologists of the culture and personality movement in the 1930s that the childs acquisition of culture began in infancy (e.g., Linton 1936), hardly any postWWII eldwork was focused on that period. By the 1960s, evidence from psychological experiments was showing that shortly after birth human infants were capable of perception, cognition, memory, imitation, and social engagement (Osofsky 1987). This evidence was often interpreted as revealing hard-wired universals of development, but it also indicated a much greater and earlier sensitivity to environmental inuence than had been previously imagined. And the ethologists and behavioral biologists studying imprinting and other aspects of parentoffspring relations in nonhuman species provided models for analyzing how both innate and environmental factors inuence behavioral development (Blurton-Jones 1972). Ethnographic observations on infancy and infant care during this period were carried out by numerous social and biological anthropologists, mostly in sub-Saharan Africa but also in Japan and among the Navajo of New Mexico. The rst studies were those of William Caudill (Caudill 1972; Caudill and Frost 1974; Caudill and Weinstein 1969) among middle-class families in Japan and the United States and Melvin J. Konner (1972, 1976, 1977) among the !Kung San foragers of the Kalahari Desert in Botswana, both of

LeVine Ethnography of Childhood THE ACQUISITION OF LANGUAGE AND CULTURE Sapirs question of how three-year-old children acquire culture began to nd answers many years later through the work of linguistic anthropologists inspired by the sociolinguistics movement (Ervin-Tripp and Slobin 1967; Gumperz and Hymes 1972; Hymes 1974), who began to study the childs acquisition of communicative competence. Between the late 1960s and the early 1990s, child language was established as a signicant focus for the ethnography of childhood. Early eld studies of child language and language socialization in non-Western societies included those of Keith Kernan (1969) in Samoa, Benjamin Blount (1971, 1972, 1975) among the Luo of Kenya, and Sara Harkness (Harkness and Super 1977) among the Kipsigis of Kenya. After Shirley Brice Heaths (1982, 1983) pioneering work on childrens communicative development in North Carolina, Elinor Ochs and Bambi Schieffelin launched an extensive series of publications on language acquisition and language socialization in different cultures (Ochs 1988, 1990; Ochs and Schieffelin 1984; Schieffelin 1990; Schieffelin and Ochs 1986, 1987). Their edited volume Language Socialization across Cultures (1987) featured the studies of numerous other linguists and linguistic anthropologists, and their monographs on, respectively, Samoans (Ochs 1988) and the Kaluli of New Guinea (Schieffelin 1990) showed the depth and value of sociolinguistic eldwork on language socialization. They used electronic recording to shed light on cultural meanings as children construe them and learn them through communicative participation. Their ndings indicate not only that children in the second, third, and fourth years of life acquire fundamental aspects of cultural communication as they learn a rst language (e.g., the conventions of conversing, expressing, and suppressing emotions, as well as giving social signals) but also that this early learning provides symbolic elements for the growing sense of self. During the 1990s, Peggy Miller and colleagues (Miller et al. 1996; Miller et al. 1990; Miller et al. 2001; Miller et al. 1997), particularly Heidi Fung (1999), carried the sociolinguistic approach to the development of the self into the three-year-old childs acquisition of cultural narratives. In analyzing the videotapes of personal storytelling concerning the transgressions of young children in the United States and Taiwan, they provided vivid ethnographic evidence of culture-specic maternal communications and the coconstruction of narrative between mother and child that contribute to the childs sense of shame, self-esteem, and other culturally organized aspects of self. Although it is not possible in this brief article to summarize or illustrate further the ndings from sociolinguistic research on childhood, it has clearly emerged as an important naturalistic strategy for understanding the ways in which communicative interaction during childhood helps shape both cultural acquisition and psychological development. CONCLUSIONS

255

In the 20th century, an anthropology of childhood was created, grew, and diversied, with the ethnographic eld study of children in context as its core empirical approach. This historical overview has noted some major trends and problems without conducting an exhaustive, analytic, or critical review of the literature. Anthropological pioneers like Malinowski, Mead, and Sapir identied ways in which childhood was worthy of ethnographic attention, and the literature grew at an increasing pace from the 1920s to the present. Ethnographers conducted eldwork among huntergatherers and agriculturalists and in some urban settings on virtually every continent. The entire corpus of data is still fragmentary, but there have been ethnographic proles of childhood environments in many different societies, and the past 15 years have seen the publication of an unprecedented number of monographs and other books (e.g., Bolin 2006; Briggs 1998; Goldman 1998; Gottlieb 2004; Hewlett 1991, 1992; Hewlett and Lamb 2005; Kramer 2006; Kurtz 1992; Morton 1996; Norman 1991; Riesman 1992; Seymour 1999).5 In addition, there have been specialized eld studies of infancy, adoption and fostering, childrens relationships, play, and language, as mentioned above. The ethnographic exploration of childhood has taken many different pathways over the past 75 years. Some early studies were guided by Freudian or neo-Freudian premises, but from the beginning ethnographers used the data they had collected in the eld to criticize developmental formulations in psychology, and the cultural critique of developmental theory became an established genre. Developmental psychologys changing theoretical orientations, and its reluctance to take seriously evidence from eld research, posed obstacles to sustained interdisciplinary engagement and made the building of cumulative knowledge difcult. Ethnographers, including biological and linguistic anthropologists, nevertheless constructed approaches to eldwork on childhood and interpreted their ndings in theoretical perspectives that were variously ecological, cultural, evolutionary, and sociolinguistic, as well as developmental. Ethnographers have documented the diversity across human populations in parenting, both in the norms and practices that form childhood environments and in the baseline developmental pathways of children from birth to adolescence. They have described the ways children learn through participation in differing social and symbolic environments and acquire cultural meanings during the early years. The research conducted to date, however, has just begun to realize the potential of anthropologys contribution to the understanding of childhood in the human species. Ethnographic studies of childhood could be improved in their depth and sophistication and even in their technology. The time seems ripe for a new eld manual recommending methods to ethnographers. Mead (Bateson and Mead 1942; Mead and MacGregor 1951) advocated the use of visual records to deepen the ethnographic

256

American Anthropologist Vol. 109, No. 2 June 2007


Baxter, Jane Eva 2005 The Archaeology of Childhood: Children, Gender and Material Culture. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press. Benthall, Jonathan 1992 A Late Developer? The Ethnography of Children. Anthropology Today 8:2335. Blount, Benjamin G. 1971 Socialization and Pre-Linguistic Development among the Luo of Kenya. Southwestern Journal of Anthropology 27:41 50. 1972 Aspects of Luo Socialization. Language in Society 1:236 248. 1975 Studies in Child Language: An Anthropological View. American Anthropologist 77:580600. Bluebond-Langner, Myra 1978 The Private Worlds of Dying Children. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. 1996 In the Shadow of Illness: Parents and Siblings of the Chronically Ill Child. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Blurton-Jones, N. 1972 Ethological Studies of Child Behaviour. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Boas, Franz 1902 Rudolf Virchows Anthropological Work. Science 16(403): 441480. 1912 Instability of Human Types. In Papers on Interracial Problems Communicated to the First Universal Races Congress Held at the University of London, July 2629, 1911. Gustav Spiller, ed. Pp. 99103. Boston: Ginn and Co. Bolin, Inge 2006 Growing Up in a Culture of Respect: Child Rearing in Highland Peru. Austin: University of Texas Press. Briggs, Jean 1972 The Issues of Autonomy and Aggression in the Three-yearold: The Utku Eskimo Case. Seminars in Psychiatry 4:317329. 1998 Inuit Morality Play: The Emotional Education of a ThreeYear-Old. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Brown, Judith K. 1963 A Cross-Cultural Study of Female Initiation Rites. American Anthropologist 65(4):837853. Bruner, Jerome S. 1990 Acts of Meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Burton, Roger V., and John W. M. Whiting 1961 The Absent Father and Cross-Sex Identity. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 7:8595. Carroll, Vern, ed. 1970 Adoption in Eastern Oceania. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press. Caudill, William 1972 Tiny Dramas: Vocal Communication between Mother and Infant in Japanese and American Families. In Mental Health Research in Asia and the Pacic, vol. 2. William Lebra, ed. Pp. 2548. Honolulu: East-West Center Press. Caudill, William, and Helen Weinstein 1969 Maternal Care and Infant Behavior in Japan and America. Psychiatry 32:1243. Caudill, William, and Lois Frost 1974 A Comparison of Maternal Care and Infant Behavior in Japanese-American, American and Japanese Families. In Mental Health Research in Asia and the Pacic, Vol. 3. William Lebra, ed. Pp. 415. Honolulu: East-West Center Press. Childs, Gladwyn Murray 1949 Umbundu Kinship and Character. London: Oxford University Press. Chisholm, James 1983 Navajo Infancy. New York: Aldine. De Laguna, Frederica 1965 Childhood among the Yakutat Tlingit. In Context and Meaning in Cultural Anthropology. Melford E. Spiro, ed. Pp. 323. New York: Free Press. Dennis, Wayne 1940 The Hopi Child. New York: Appleton-Century. DuBois, Cora 1944 The People of Alor. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

understanding of childhood, and although powerful and portable video recording technology is now available, it has only rarely been used outside of linguistic studies. Joseph Tobin and colleagues (1989) showed in Preschool in Three Cultures that videos of child-care practices in one culture shown to parents and teachers of another can elicit not only emotional expressions of shock and disapproval but also a spontaneous explication of indigenous assumptions concerning children that had eluded conventional ethnographic interviewing. Furthermore, despite the global reach of ethnographic research on childhood, there is hardly any area of the world in which coverage could be called adequate and many others to which ethnography of childhood needs to be extended. But ethnographic documentation by itself, however excellent, cannot create an anthropology of childhood of more than marginal signicance to anthropologists (Hirschfeld 2002), or for that matter to other social scientists, without further theory building and cross-cultural comparison (Quinn 2005; Shweder et al. in press). R OBERT A. LE VINE Harvard Graduate School of Education, Cambridge, MA 02138 NOTES
Acknowledgments. This article was written when the author was a fellow of the John Simon Guggenheim Memorial Foundation. 1. The claim that the topic of childhood has been neglected by anthropologists (Benthall 1992; Hardman 1973; Hirschfeld 2002; Schwartzman 2001), without reference to many of the works reviewed here, leaves the impression of a small or declining literature that is inconsistent with the results of this overview. 2. Some of my own interpretations of this literature and more extensive historical accounts of the anthropology of childhood will appear in future publications (LeVine in press a, in press b; LeVine and New in press). 3. The numerous case studies in cultural anthropology, edited by George Spindler and Louise Spindler and published by Holt, Rinehart, and Winston from the 1960s to the 1980s, illustrate this point with their chapters on Growing Up or The Life Cycle, which contain ethnographic data on childhood from a particular society. 4. This literature cannot be adequately reviewed in this article, but some of the major ethnographic projects can be listed: James Chisholm (1983) among the Navajo; Sara Harkness and Charles Super (1983, 1996) among the Kipsigis of Kenya; Ruth H. Munroe and Robert L. Munroe (1972) among the Logoli of Kenya; Mary Martini and John Kirkpatrick (1981) among the Marquesans; Greeneld et al. (1989) among Mayans of Chiapas, Mexico; Robert A. LeVine and colleagues (1988, 1994) among the Gusii of Kenya; Philip Kilbride and Janet Kilbride (1990) among the Ganda of Uganda; Edward Z. Tronick and Gilda Morelli (Tronick et al. 1992; Tronick et al. 1987) among the Efe pygmies of the Congo; Paul Riesman (1992) among the Fulani of Burkina Faso; Barry Hewlett (1991; Hewlett et al. 2000; Hewlett et al. 1998) among the Aka pygmies of Cameroun and the Ngandu of the Central African Republic; Alma Gottlieb (2004) among the Beng of the Ivory Coast; and Heidi Keller (in press) among the Nso of Cameroun and three other cultures. 5. A special issue of Ethos (31[2], June 2003) devoted to The Cultural Construction of Childhood is also worth noting here.

REFERENCES CITED
Bateson, Gregory, and Margaret Mead 1942 Balinese Childhood: A Photographic Analysis. New York: New York Academy of Sciences.

LeVine Ethnography of Childhood


Elwin, Verrier 1947 The Muria and Their Ghotul. Madhya Pradesh: Vanya Prakashan. Erikson, Erik 1950 Childhood and Society. New York: W. W. Norton. Ervin-Tripp, Susan, and Dan Slobin 1967 A Field Manual for Cross-Cultural Study of the Acquisition of Communicative Competence. Berkeley: University of California Press. Evans-Pritchard, E. E. 1940 The Nuer. London: Oxford University Press. 1953 Kinship and Marriage among the Nuer. London: Oxford University Press. Ewbank, Douglas C., and Samuel H. Preston 1990 Personal Health Behavior and the Decline in Infant and Child Mortality: The United States, 19001930. In What We Know about Health Transition: The Cultural, Social and Behavioural Determinants of Health, vol. 1. John C. Caldwell, Salley Findley, Pat Caldwell, Gigi Santow, Wendy Cosford, Jennifer Braid, and Daphne Broers-Freeman, eds. Pp. 116149. Canberra, Australia: The Australian National University. Fajans, Jane 1997 They Make Themselves: Work and Play among the Baining of Papua New Guinea. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Firth, Raymond 1936 We, the Tikopia. New York: American Book. Fischer, John L., and Ann Fischer 1966 The New Englanders of Orchard Town, U.S.A. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Fortes, Meyer 1938 Social and Psychological Aspects of Education in Taleland. London: International African Institute Memorandum XVII. 1949 The Web of Kinship among the Tallensi. London: Oxford University Press. Fung, Heidi 1999 Becoming a Moral Child: The Socialization of Shame among Young Chinese Children. Ethos 27(2):180209. Gaskins, Suzanne 1996 How Mayan Parental Theories Come into Play. In Parents Cultural Belief Systems. Sara Harkness and Charles Super, eds. Pp. 345363. New York: Guilford. 1999 Childrens Daily Lives in a Mayan Village. In Childrens Engagement in the Social World. Artin Goncu, ed. Pp. 2581. New York: Cambridge University Press. 2000 Childrens Daily Activities in a Mayan Village: A Culturally Grounded Description. Cross-Cultural Research 34:375389. 2003 From Corn to Cash: Change and Continuity within Mayan Families. Ethos 31(2):248273. Geertz, Hildred 1959 The Vocabulary of Emotion: A Study of Javanese Socialization Processes. Psychiatry 22:225237. Gibbs, James L., ed. 1965 Peoples of Africa. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Gilligan, Carol F. 1982 In a Different Voice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Goldman, Lawrence R. 1998 Childs Play: Myth, Mimesis and Make-Believe. Oxford: Berg. Goody, Esther N. 1982 Parenthood and Social Reproduction: Fostering and Occupational Roles in West Africa. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Gottlieb, Alma 2004 Afterlife Is Where We Come From: The Culture of Infancy in West Africa. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Greeneld, Patricia M. 1966 On Culture and Conservation. In Studies in Cognitive Growth. Jerome S. Bruner, S. M. Olver, and Patricia M. Greenelds, eds. Pp. 225256, 327334. New York: John Wiley and Sons. 2004 Weaving Generations Together: Evolving Creativity in the Maya of Chiapas. Santa Fe: School of American Research Press. Greeneld, Patricia M., T. B. Brazelton, and C. P. Childs 1989 From Birth to Maturity in Zinacantan: Ontogenesis in

257

Cultural Context. In Ethnographic Encounters in Southern Mesoamerica: Essays in Honor of Evon Zartman Vogt. Victoria Bricker and Gray Gossen, eds. Pp. 177216. Albany: State University of New York at Albany. Grindal, Bruce T. 1972 Growing Up in Two Worlds: Education and Transition among the Sisala of Northern Ghana. Gumperz, John, and Dell Hymes, eds. 1972 Directions in Sociolinguistics: The Ethnography of Communication. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Hall, G. Stanley 1904 Adolescence: Its Psychology and Its Relations to Physiology, Anthropology, Sociology, Sex, Crime, Religion and Education. 2 vols. New York: Appleton. Hardman, Charlotte 1973 Can There Be an Anthropology of Childhood? Journal of the Anthropological Society of Oxford 4:8599. Harkness, Sarah, and Charles Super 1977 Why African Children Are So Hard to Test. In Issues in Cross-Cultural Research, 285. L. L. Adler, ed. Pp. 326331. New York: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1983 The Cultural Construction of Child Development: A Framework for the Socialization of Affect. Ethos 11(4):221 231. Harkness, Sara, and Charles Super, eds. 1996 Parents Cultural Belief Systems. New York: Guilford Press. Harwood, Robin, Joan G. Miller, and Nydia Lucca-Irizarry 1995 Culture and Attachment: Perceptions of the Child in Context. New York: Guilford Press. Heath, Shirley Brice 1982 What No Bedtime Story Means: Narrative Skills at Home and School. Language in Society 11:4976. 1983 Ways with Words: Language, Life, and Work in Communities and Classrooms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Henry, Jules, and Zunia Henry 1944 Doll Play of Pilaga Indian Children. New York: American Orthopsychiatric Association. Hewlett, Barry S. 1991 Intimate Fathers: The Nature and Context of Aka Pygmy Paternal Infant Care. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press. Hewlett, Barry S., ed. 1992 FatherChild Relations: Cultural and Biosocial Contexts. New York: Aldine de Gruyter. Hewlett, Barry S., and Michael Lamb, eds. 2004 Hunter-Gatherer Childhoods: Evolutionary, Developmental, and Cultural Perspectives. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter. Hewlett, Barry S., Michael E. Lamb, Birgit Leyendecker, and Axel Scholmerich 2000 Internal Working Models, Trust, and Sharing among Foragers. Current Anthropology 41:287297. Hewlett, Barry S., Michael E. Lamb, Donald Shannon, Birgit Lyendecker, and Axel Scholmerich 1998 Culture and Early Infancy among Central African Foragers and Farmers. Developmental Psychology 34:653 661. Hirschfeld, Lawrence A. 2002 Why Dont Anthropologists Like Children? American Anthropologist 104(2):611627. Hogbin, H. Ian 1931 Education at Ontong Java, Solomon Islands. American Anthropologist 33(4):601614. Howard, Alan 1970 Learning To Be Rotuman: Enculturation in the South Pacic. New York: Teachers College Press. Hughes, Charles C. 1974 Eskimo Boyhood: An Autobiography in Psychosocial Perspective. Lexington: University of Kentucky Press. Hymes, Dell 1974 Foundations in Sociolinguistics: An Ethnographic Approach. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press. Junod, Henri 1927[1912] Life in a South African Tribe. 2nd edition. London: Macmillan. Kaberry, Phyllis 1939 The Aboriginal Woman. London: G. Routledge and Sons.

258

American Anthropologist Vol. 109, No. 2 June 2007


LeVine, Robert A. In press a Anthropological Foundations of Cultural Psychology. In Handbook of Cultural Psychology. Shinobu Kitayama and Dov Cohen, eds. New York: Guilford. In press b Plasticity and Variation: Cultural Inuences on Parenting and Child Development within and between Human Populations. Pacic Palisades, CA: Foundation for Psychocultural Research. LeVine, Robert A., and Barbara LeVine 1966 Nyansongo: A Gusii Community in Kenya. New York: John Wiley and Sons. LeVine, Robert A., and Karin Norman 2001 The Infants Acquisition of Culture: InfantMother Attachment Re-Examined from an Anthropological Point of View. In The Psychology of Cultural Experience. Carmella Moore and Holly Mathews, eds. Pp. 83104. New York: Cambridge University Press. LeVine, Robert A., and Rebecca S. New, eds. In press Anthropology and Child Development: A CrossCultural Reader. Malden, MA: Blackwell. LeVine, Robert A., Patrice M. Miller, and Mary Maxwell West, eds. 1988 Parenting in Diverse Cultures. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. LeVine, Robert A., Suzanne Dixon, Sarah LeVine, Amy Richman, Constance H. Keefer, P. Herbert Leiderman, and T. Berry Brazelton 1994 Child Care and Culture: Lessons from Africa. New York: Cambridge University Press. Levy, David M. 1937 Sibling Rivalry. Research Monograph No. 2. American Orthopsychiatric Association. Levy, Robert I. 1969 Child Management Structure in Tahitian Families. Journal of the Polynesian Society 78:3543. 1973 Tahitians. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Linton, Ralph 1936 The Study of Man. New York: D. Appleton-Century Co. Malinowski, Bronislaw 1927 Sex and Repression in Savage Society. London: Paul, Trench, Trubner. 1929 The Sexual Life of Savages in North Western Melanesia. London: Routledge. Maretzki, Thomas, and Hatsumi Maretzki 1966 Taira: An Okinawan Village. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Martini, Mary, and John Kirkpatrick 1981 Early Interactions in the Marquesas Islands. In Culture and Early Interactions. Tiffany Field, Anita Sostek, Peter Vietze, and P. Herbert Leiderman, eds. Pp. 189213. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Mead, Margaret 1928a Coming of Age in Samoa. New York: William Morrow. 1928b Samoan Children at Work and Play. Natural History 28:626636. 1930 Growing Up in New Guinea. New York: William Morrow. 1931 The Primitive Child. In A Handbook of Child Psychology. C. Murchison, ed. Pp. 669686. Worcester, MA: Clark University Press. 1932 An Investigation of the Thought of Primitive Children, with Special Reference to Animism. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland 62:173 190. Mead, Margaret, and Frances Cooke MacGregor 1951 Growth and Culture: A Photographic Study of Balinese Childhood. New York: G. P. Putnams Sons. Mead, Margaret, and Martha Wolfenstein, eds. 1954 Childhood in Contemporary Cultures. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Miller, Joan G. 1994 Cultural Diversity in the Morality of Caring. CrossCultural Research 28:339. Miller, Peggy J., Angela R. Wiley, Heidi Fung, and Chung-Hui Liang 1997 Personal Storytelling as a Medium of Socialization in Chinese and American Families. Child Development 68:557 568. Miller, Peggy J., Heidi Fung, and Julie Mintz 1996 Self-Construction through Narrative Practices: A Chinese

Kardiner, Abram 1939 The Individual and His Society. New York: Columbia University Press. 1945 Psychological Frontiers of Society. New York: Columbia University Press. Kaufmann, Felix 1944 Methodology of the Social Sciences. London: Oxford University Press. Keller, Heidi In press Cultures of Infancy. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Kernan, Keith T. 1969 The Acquisition of Language by Samoan Children. Berkeley: University of California Language-Behavior Research Laboratory. Kilbride, Phillip L., and Janet C. Kilbride 1990 Changing Family Life in East Africa: Women and Children at Risk. University Park: Pennsylvania State University Press. Kluckhohn, Clyde 1939 Theoretical Bases for an Empirical Method of Studying the Acquisition of Culture by Individuals. Man 39:98103. Kohlberg, Lawrence A. 1984 The Psychology of Moral Development: The Nature and Validity of Moral Stages. San Francisco: Harper and Row. Konner, Melvin J. 1972 Aspects of the Developmental Ethology of a Foraging People. In Ethological Studies of Child Behaviour. N. BlurtonJones, ed. Pp. 285304. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 1976 Maternal Care, Infant Behavior, and Development among the !Kung. In Kalahari Hunter-Gatherers: Studies of the !Kung San and Their Neighbors. Richard B. Lee and Irven DeVore, eds. Pp. 218245. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. 1977 Infancy among the Kalahari Desert San. In Culture and Infancy: Variations in the Human Experience. P. Herbert Leiderman, Steven Tulkin, and Ann Rosenfeld, eds. Pp. 287328. New York: Academic Press. Korbin, Jill, ed. 1981 Child Abuse in Cross-Cultural Perspective. Berkeley: University of California Press. Kramer, Karen L. 2006 Maya Children: Helpers at the Farm. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Kurtz, Stanley 1992 All the Mothers Are One: Hindu India and the Cultural Reshaping of Psychoanalysis. New York: Columbia University Press. Lancy, David F. 1996 Playing on the Mother-Ground: Cultural Routines for Childrens Development. New York: Guilford. Lancy, David F., and B. Allan Tindall, eds. 1977 The Anthropological Study of Play. West Point, NY: Leisure Press. Lantis, Margaret 1960 Eskimo Childhood and Interpersonal Relationships: Nunivak Biographies and Genealogies. Seattle: University of Washington Press. Lebra, Takie Sugiyama 1976 Japanese Patterns of Behavior. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press. 1990 Socialization of Aristocratic Children by Commoners. Cultural Anthropology 5:78100. 1994 Mother and Child in Japanese Socialization: A JapanU.S. Comparison. In Cross-Cultural Roots of Minority Child Development. Patricia M. Greeneld and R. R. Cocking, eds. Pp. 259 274. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Leighton, Dorothea, and Clyde Kluckhohn 1948 Children of the People: The Navaho Individual and His Development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Leis, Philip E. 1972 Enculturation and Socialization in an Ijaw Village. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Leontev, A. N. 1981 The Problem of Activity in Psychology. In The Concept of Activity in Soviet Psychology. James V. Wertsch, ed. Pp. 3771. Armonk, NY: M. E. Sharpe.

LeVine Ethnography of Childhood


and American Comparison of Early Socialization. Ethos 24(2):237280. Miller, Peggy J., R. Potts, Heidi Fung, Lisa Hoogstra, and Julie Mintz 1990 Narrative Practices and the Social Construction of Self in Childhood. American Ethnologist 17(2):292311. Miller, Peggy J., T. Sandel, Chung-Hui Liang, and Heidi Fung 2001 Narrating Transgressions in Longwood: The Discourses, Meanings and Paradoxes of an American Socializing Practice. Ethos 29(2):159186. Minturn, Leigh, and John Hitchcock 1966 The Rajputs of Khalapur, India. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Minturn, Leigh, and William W. Lambert 1964 Mothers of Six Cultures. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Montgomery, Heather 2001 Modern Babylon? Prostituting Children in Thailand. New York: Bergahn Books. Montgomery, Heather, Rachel Burr, and Martin Woodhead, eds. 2003 Changing Childhoods: Global and Local. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Morton, Helen 1996 Becoming Tongan: An Ethnography of Childhood. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press. Munroe, Robert L. 1980 Male Transvestism and the Couvade: A Psychocultural Analysis. Ethos 8(1):4959. Munroe, Robert L., R. H. Munroe, and J. W. M. Whiting 1973 The Couvade: A Psychological Analysis. Ethos 1(1):30 74. 1981 Male Sex-Role Resolutions. In Handbook of Cross-Cultural Human Development. R. H. Munroe, R. L. Munroe, and B. B. Whiting, eds. Pp. 611632. New York: Garland. Munroe, Ruth H., and R. L. Munroe 1972 Household Density and Infant Care in an East African Society. Journal of Social Psychology 83:313. Norman, Karin 1991 A Sound Family Makes a Sound State: Ideology and Upbringing in a German Village. Stockholm: Department of Social Anthropology, University of Stockholm. Nydegger, William, and Corinne Nydegger 1966 Tarong: An Ilocos Barrio in the Phillipines. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Ochs, Elinor 1988 Culture and Language Development: Language Acquisition and Language Socialization in a Samoan Village. New York: Cambridge University Press. 1990 Indexicality and Socialization. In Cultural Psychology: Essays on Comparative Human Development. James Stigler, Richard A. Shweder, and Gilbert Herdt, eds. Pp. 287308. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Ochs, Elinor, and Bambi Schieffelin 1984 Language Acquisition and Socialization: Three Stories. In Culture Theory: Essays on Mind, Self, and Emotion. Richard A. Shweder and Robert A. LeVine, eds. Pp. 276320. New York: Cambridge University Press. Osofsky, Joy, ed. 1987 Handbook of Infant Development. 2nd ed. New York: Wiley. Peak, Lois 1991 Learning To Go to School in Japan. Berkeley: University of California Press. Peshkin, Alan 1972 Kanuri Schoolchildren. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Preston, Samuel H., and Michael R. Haines 1991 The Fatal Years: Child Mortality in Late NineteenthCentury America. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Quinn, Naomi 2005 Universals of Child Rearing. Anthropological Theory 5:475514. Raum, O. F. 1940 Chaga Childhood. London: Oxford University Press. Read, Margaret 1960 Children of Their Fathers: Growing Up among the Ngoni of Nyasaland. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

259

1968 Children of Their Fathers: Growing Up among the Ngoni of Malawi. Case Studies in Education and Culture. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Richards, Audry I. 1932 Hunger and Work in a Savage Tribe. London: George Routledge and Sons. 1956 Chisungu: A Girls Initiation Ceremony among the Bemba. London: Faber and Faber. Riesman, Paul 1992 First Find Your Child a Good Mother. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press. Ritchie, Jane 1957 Childhood in Rakau: The First Five Years of Life. Wellington: Victoria University. Roheim, Geza 1932 The Psychoanalysis of Primitive Cultural Types. International Journal of Psychoanalysis 13:1233. 1974 Children of the Desert: The Western Tribes of Central Australia. New York: Harper and Row. Rogoff, Barbara 2003 The Cultural Nature of Human Development. New York: Oxford University Press. Rogoff, Barbara, Jayanthi Mistry, Artin Goncu, and Christine Mosier 1993 Guided Participation in Cultural Activity by Toddlers and Caregivers. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 58(236). Romney, A. Kimball, and Romaine Romney 1966 The Mixtecans of Juxtlahuaca, Mexico. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Sapir, Edward 1993 The Psychology of Culture: A Course of Lectures. Judith Irvine, ed. The Hague: Mouton. Schieffelin, Bambi 1990 The Give and Take of Everyday Life: Language Socialization of Kaluli Children. New York: Cambridge University Press. Schieffelin, Bambi, and Elinor Ochs 1986 Language Socialization. In Annual Review of Anthropology 15:163191. Schieffelin, Bambi, and Elinor Ochs, eds. 1987 Language Socialization across Cultures. New York: Cambridge University Press. Schwalb, David W., and Barbara J. Schwalb 1996 Japanese Childrearing: Two Generations of Scholarship. New York: Guilford Press. Schwartzman, Helen 1978 Transformations: The Anthropology of Childrens Play. New York: Plenum Press. 2001 Children and Anthropology: A Century of Studies. In Children and Anthropology: Perspectives for the 21st Century. Helen Schwartzman, ed. Pp. 1537. Westport, CT: Bergin and Garvey. Scheper-Hughes, Nancy 1992 Death without Weeping: The Violence of Everyday Life in Brazil. Berkeley: University of California Press. Scheper-Hughes, Nancy, ed. 1987 Child Survival: Anthropological Perspectives on the Treatment and Maltreatment of Children. Dordrecht, the Netherlands: D. Reidel. Scheper-Hughes, Nancy, and Carolyn F. Sargent, eds. 1998 Small Wars: The Cultural Politics of Childhood. Berkeley: University of California Press. Seymour, Susan 1999 Women, Family and Child Care in India. New York: Cambridge University Press. Shonkoff, Jack, and Deborah Phillips 2000 From Neurons to Neighborhoods: The Science of Early Childhood Development. Washington, DC: National Academies Press. Shweder, Richard A., Jacqueline Goodnow, Giyoo Hatano, Robert A. LeVine, Hazel Markus, and Peggy Miller In press The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities. In Handbook of Child Psychology. 6th edition. William Damon and Richard M. Lerner, eds. New York: John Wiley and Sons.

260

American Anthropologist Vol. 109, No. 2 June 2007


Vygotsky, Lev 1978 Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Wedgwood, C. H. 1938 The Life of Children in Manam. Oceania 9:129. Weisner, Thomas S. 1984 Ecocultural Niches of Middle Childhood: A Cross-Cultural Perspective. In Development during Middle Childhood: The Years from Six to Twelve. W. A. Collins, ed. Pp. 335369. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. 1987 Socialization for Parenthood in Sibling Caretaking Societies. In Parenting across the Lifespan. Jane Lancaster, Alice Rossi, Jeanne Altmann, and Lonnie Sherrod, eds. Pp. 237270. New York: Aldine. 1989 Cultural and Universal Aspects of Social Support for Children: Evidence among the Abaluyia of Kenya. In Childrens Social Networks and Social Supports. Deborah Belle, ed. Pp. 7090. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Weisner, Thomas S., and Ronald Gallimore 1977 My Brothers Keeper: Child and Sibling Caretaking. Current Anthropology 18:169190. Whiting, Beatrice B. 1965 Sex Identity Conict and Physical Violence: A Comparative Study. American Anthropologist 67:123140. Whiting, Beatrice B., ed. 1963 Six Cultures: Studies of Child Rearing. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Whiting, Beatrice B., and Carolyn Edwards 1988 Children of Different Worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Whiting, Beatrice B., and John W. M. Whiting 1975 Children of Six Cultures: A Psycho-Cultural Analysis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Whiting, John W. M. 1941 Becoming a Kwoma. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. 1954 The Cross-Cultural Method. In The Handbook of Social Psychology. Gardner Lindzey, ed. Pp. 524531. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley. 1964 Effects of Climate on Certain Cultural Practices. In Explorations in Cultural Anthropology. Ward Goodenough, ed. Pp. 611632. New York: McGraw-Hill. 1994 Fifty Years as a Behavioral Scientist: Autobiographical Notes. In Culture and Human Development: The Selected Papers of John Whiting. Eleanor Hollenberg Chasdi, ed. Pp. 1444. New York: Cambridge University Press. Whiting, John W. M., and Irvin L. Child 1953 Child Training and Personality: A Cross-Cultural Study. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Whiting, John W. M., Irvin L. Child, and William W. Lambert 1966 Field Guide for a Comparative Study of Socialization. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Whiting, John W. M., R. Kluckhohn, and A. Anthony 1958 The Function of Male Initiation Ceremonies at Puberty. In Readings in Social Psychology. 3rd edition. Eleanor E. Maccoby, T. Newcomb, and E. Hartley, eds. Pp. 359370. New York: Henry Holt. Williams, Thomas R. 1969 A Borneo Childhood: Enculturation in Dusun Society. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Yarrow, Marian R., John D. Campbell, and Roger V. Burton 1968 Child Rearing: An Inquiry into Research and Methods. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Shweder, Richard A., and Robert A. LeVine 1975 Dream Concepts of Hausa Children: A Critique of the Doctrine of Invariant Sequence in Cognitive Development. Ethos 3(2):209230. Shweder, Richard A., M. Mahapatra, and Joan G. Miller 1990 Culture and Moral Development. In Cultural Psychology: Essays on Comparative Human Development. James Stigler, Richard A. Shweder, and Gilbert Herdt, eds. Pp. 130204. New York: Cambridge University Press. Smith, Mary F. 1954 Baba of Karo: A Woman of the Muslim Hausa. London: Faber and Faber. Spiro, Melford E. 1958 Children of the Kibbutz: A Study in Child Training and Personality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Stafford, Charles 1995 The Roads of Chinese Childhood: Learning and Identication in Angang. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Stephens, Sharon, ed. 1995 Children and the Politics of Culture. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Stocking, George S. 1968 Race, Culture, and Evolution: Essays in the History of Anthropology. New York: Free Press. 1974 The Shaping of American Anthropology, 18831911: A Franz Boas Reader. New York: Basic Books. 1992 Polarity and Plurality: Franz Boas as Psychological Anthropologist. In New Directions in Psychological Anthropology. Theodore Schwartz, Geoffrey M. White, and Catherine A. Lutz, eds. Pp. 311323. New York: Cambridge University Press. Suarez-Orozco, Carola, and Marcelo Suarez-Orozco 2001 Children of Immigration. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Super, Charles M., and Sara Harkness 1985 Infants Niche in Rural Kenya and Metropolitan America. In Cross-Cultural Research at Issue. L. L. Adler, ed. Pp. 247255. New York: Academic Press. 1986 The Developmental Niche: A Conceptualization at the Interface of Society and the Individual. International Journal of Behavioral Development 9:545570. Tobin, Joseph, David Wu, and Dana Davidson 1989 Preschool in Three Cultures. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Toren, Christina 1990 Making Sense of Hierarchy: Cognition and Social Process in Fiji. LSE Monograph in Anthropology 61. London: Athlone Press. 1993 Making History: The Signicance of Childhood Cognition for a Comparative Anthropology of Mind. Man (n.s.) 28:461478. Tronick, Edward Z., Gilda Morelli, and P. K. Ivey 1992 The Efe Infant and Toddlers Pattern of Social Relationships: Multiple and Simultaneous. Developmental Psychology 28:568577. Tronick, Edward Z., Gilda A. Morelli, and Steve Winn 1987 Multiple Caretaking of Efe (Pygmy) Infants. American Anthropologist 89(1):96106. UN Population Division 2005 World Population Monitoring 2003: Population, Education and Development. New York: UN Economic and Social Council. van Gennep, Arnold 1960[1908] The Rites of Passage. Michaela Vizedom and Mari Caffee, trans. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

You might also like