You are on page 1of 5

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2016, 49, 15 NUMBER 4 (SUMMER)

ARRANGING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS AND THE DISTRIBUTION


OF REINFORCERS: A BRIEF REVIEW OF PREFERENCE AND
PERFORMANCE OUTCOMES
JOHN C. WARD-HORNER
EVERGREEN CENTER

MIRELA CENGHER
QUEENS COLLEGE AND THE GRADUATE CENTER, CUNY

ROBERT K. ROSS
BEACON ABA SERVICES, INC.

AND

DANIEL M. FIENUP
QUEENS COLLEGE AND THE GRADUATE CENTER, CUNY

Recent research has demonstrated that some participants prefer to complete a larger series of
responses in exchange for a longer duration of reinforcer access, rather than completing fewer tasks
associated with smaller, but more frequent, reinforcer access. This review provides a summary of this
line of research, examines variables contributing to participant preference and performance under dif-
ferent response-reinforcer arrangements, and discusses limitations and areas for future research.
Key words: continuous, discontinuous, preference, performance, response-reinforcer
arrangements

Recent research indicates that some partici- participant completed math problems and con-
pants with disabilities prefer to complete an sumed reinforcers. In one arrangement, research-
entire series of responses followed by continuous ers provided 3 min of reinforcer access after
reinforcer access, rather than dividing the completion of each worksheet (discontinuous
response-reinforcer components into smaller arrangement1). In the other, researchers pro-
units. For instance, Fienup, Ahlers, and Pace vided 18 min of reinforcer access after com-
(2011) provided a participant with a choice pletion of all six worksheets (continuous
between two arrangements, both of which arrangement1). The participant preferred the
included six 20-problem math worksheets and a continuous arrangement despite the require-
total of 18 min of reinforcer access. Researchers ment of completing 120 math problems
presented the participant with cards (initial before accessing the reinforcer.
link stimuli) displaying the associated response-
1
reinforcer arrangement, and selection of one The research literature has been inconsistent in the use
of labels for different response-reinforcer arrangements:
card led to a terminal link in which the The terms uent, accumulated, and continuous have been
used to describe the completion of all response require-
We thank Joe Vedora for his feedback on early revi- ments prior to continuous reinforcer access, and the terms
sions of this manuscript. disuent, distributed, and discontinuous have been used to
Correspondence can be sent to John C. Ward-Horner, describe completion and consumption of several smaller
Evergreen Center, 345 Fortune Blvd., Milford, MA response-reinforcer units. We used the terms continuous
01757 (e-mail: jward-horner@evergreenctr.org). arrangements and discontinuous arrangements to refer to the
doi: 10.1002/jaba.350 different response-reinforcer arrangements.

2016 Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior


1
2 JOHN C. WARD-HORNER et al.

Table 1
Procedural and Qualitative Variables That May Inuence Preference and Performance

Reinforcer Characteristics Response Characteristics Participant Characteristics Instructions


Type of reinforcer (edible, social, activity) Acquisition vs. maintenance Age/developmental level Rules regarding
contingencies
Continuity of access Free vs. restricted operant Cognitive functioning Contingency-shaped
(higher/lower) behavior
Empirically derived, positive (edible, social, Small vs. large Negatively reinforced
activity) vs. negative reinforcers Easy vs. difcult problem behavior
High vs. low preference stimuli High vs. low preference response Self control vs. impulsive
Long vs. short exchange delay
Sufcient duration to complete the activity
Conditioned reinforcers (tokens vs. no
tokens)
Components of token systems:
-Token-production schedule
-Exchange-production schedule
-Token-exchange schedule

The outcome of Fienup et al. (2011) raises time to progress through pivotal stages of the
important questions regarding the variables that activity (e.g., reading an entire book chapter
inuence preference for different response- vs. reading a few paragraphs), whereas the value
reinforcer arrangements. For example, clearer of edible reinforcers may be less dependent
understanding of these variables may lead to upon continuity of access. In their second
the selection of different response-reinforcer experiment, the researchers compared preference
arrangements for different types of tasks and for continuous and discontinuous arrangements
reinforcers (e.g., acquisition vs. maintenance when the reinforcer was an activity vs. an edible.
tasks; activity-based vs. edible reinforcers). There Three participants preferred the continuous
is a growing body of applied research examining arrangement regardless of type of reinforcer and
variables that may affect preference for and one participant was indifferent when the rein-
performance under various response-reinforcer forcer was an edible. For all participants, task
arrangements (Bukala, Yao Hu, Lee, Ward-Hor- duration was shorter in the continuous arrange-
ner, & Fienup, 2015; DeLeon et al., 2014; ment when the reinforcer was an activity,
Kocher, Howard, & Fienup, 2015; Ward-Hor- whereas task duration was shorter in the discon-
ner, Pittenger, Pace, & Fienup, 2014). This tinuous arrangement for two of the four partici-
paper provides a review of these studies and dis- pants when the reinforcer was a food item2.
cusses variables (see Table 1) that likely inuence Bukala et al. (2015) extended this line of
preference and performance. research by evaluating different aspects of par-
ticipant performance under continuous and dis-
continuous arrangements. In comparison to
Prior Research DeLeon et al. (2014), Bukala et al. measured
DeLeon et al. (2014) examined whether overall session and transition durations in
reinforcer type inuenced preference for and
performance under continuous and discontinu- 2
ous arrangements. DeLeon et al. hypothesized DeLeon et al. (2014) also included an evaluation of
the effects of tokens and delays to exchanging tokens, but
that longer-duration access to activity reinfor- the presence and absence of tokens did not affect prefer-
cers may increase their value by allowing ample ence and the effects of exchange delays were inconsistent.
RESPONSE-REINFORCER ARRANGEMENTS 3

addition to task duration. Overall session dura- and performance. Therefore, Kocher et al.
tion was the total time to complete tasks, con- (2015) extended the aforementioned studies by
sume reinforcers, and transition between tasks evaluating preference for and performance under
and reinforcers. Task duration was the time continuous and discontinuous arrangements
between the rst instruction and completion of with skill acquisition tasks. The researchers
the last response, and transition duration was found that (a) preference was mixed with one
the remaining time after task duration and rein- participant preferring each arrangement,
forcement time was subtracted from overall ses- respectively, and one participant demonstrating
sion duration. Overall session duration was no differentiated preference, (b) the number of
found to be shorter during the continuous sessions to mastery did not differ for two of
arrangement for two of the three participants, three participants, and (c) overall session dura-
with shorter transition durations for one partic- tion was consistently shorter for the continuous
ipant and with shorter task durations for the arrangement for all three participants. Never-
other participant. Bukala et al. also found that theless, it is unclear whether Kocher et al.s
all participants preferred the continuous preference outcome is the result of skill acqui-
arrangement. sition tasks or some other variable, as the
Ward-Horner et al. (2014) attempted to study did not directly compare preference for
clarify the role of continuity of reinforcer arrangements when the skills were acquisition
access by directly manipulating the overall vs. maintenance tasks. Future research could
amount of reinforcement (i.e., the magnitude directly examine the effects of acquisition and
of continuity of access) in the continuous maintenance tasks on preference by manipu-
arrangement, while holding all aspects of the lating the type of task across experimental
discontinuous arrangement constant. Speci- phases.
cally, for one participant, the researchers Collectively, the aforementioned studies sug-
decreased the duration of reinforcer access for gest that (a) participants generally prefer continu-
the continuous arrangement, so that it was ous arrangements, (b) continuous arrangements
80%, 60%, or 20% of the overall duration are generally more efcient than discontinuous
available for the discontinuous arrangement. arrangements, (c) reinforcer type inuences per-
Ward-Horner et al. found that the participant formance, but not necessarily preference, (d) task
preferred the continuous arrangement when type may inuence preference but not perfor-
the magnitude of reinforcement equaled 100% mance, and (e) reinforcer continuity alone may
or 80% of the magnitude of the discontinuous not be sufcient to inuence preference. Never-
arrangement; however, when the magnitude theless, the mechanisms driving preference and
was reduced to 60% or 20%, preference performance are unclear. First, it is unclear
switched to the discontinuous arrangement. whether continuity of access inuenced the value
This outcome suggests that greater continuity of the different types of reinforcers by allowing
of reinforcer access alone may be insufcient sufcient time to reach pivotal stages in the activ-
to inuence preference. ity (i.e., completing an activity ensures that a
The preference and performance outcomes pivotal stage was reached). For instance, DeLeon
described thus far are based on responses already et al. (2014) reported that three of the four
in the participants repertoire (i.e., maintenance participants were unable to complete the entire
tasks). Because maintenance tasks do not require activity reinforcer within a session (i.e., two
learning, the less frequent reinforcer delivery exposure trials and ve choice trials per session),
inherent to continuous arrangements may have and the participant who was able to complete the
been a critical variable inuencing preference activity was unable to complete it during a single
4 JOHN C. WARD-HORNER et al.

choice trial. In addition, although Ward-Horner Variables that May Inuence Preference and
et al. (2014) directly manipulated continuity of Performance
reinforcer access, the researchers did not directly Table 1 lists variables that may inuence
manipulate the extent to which the participant preference for and performance under response-
could complete the activity. reinforcer manipulations. Some of these variables
Second, it is unclear whether continuity of may inuence preference, performance, or both,
access serves as a motivating operation or a and each of these variables may or may not affect
manipulation of reinforcer magnitude. Tradi- preference and performance in similar ways.
tionally, continuity of access has been equa- Consideration of the variables in Table 1 may
ted to reinforcer magnitude (e.g., Bonem & help clarify the conditions under which continu-
Crossman, 1988; Hoch, McComas, Johnson, ous and discontinuous arrangements inuence
Faranda, & Guenther, 2002), but more participants preference and performance, and
recent research suggests that continuity of each, in isolation or combination represent fertile
access may function as a motivating operation grounds for future research. Next, we briey
(Steinhilber & Johnson, 2007). For instance, highlight two variables: (a) response characteris-
Steinhilber and Johnson (2007) demonstrated tics and (b) participant characteristics.
that participants preference for activity reinfor- Task difculty may affect preference and,
cers was dependent upon duration of access. specically, participants who generally prefer
Therefore, continuity of access may simply continuous arrangements may switch prefer-
change the reinforcing value of an activity regard- ence as the task difculty increases. Difcult
less of the particular stage of the activity, or it tasks may be more aversive (Lerman, Addi-
might inuence preference and performance by son, & Kodak, 2006), require greater response
way of reinforcer magnitude. effort, and result in longer delays to reinforce-
Finally, an important question that has yet ment. Therefore, as responses become more
to be addressed is whether preference is difcult, more frequent reinforcer access and
determined by the arrangement of responses escape from work (i.e., discontinuous arrange-
(i.e., continuous and discontinuous task ments) might be preferred. Other response
completion), distribution of reinforcers, or a characteristics, such as large vs. small response
combination of these variables (DeLeon requirements, may affect preference and perfor-
et al. 2014; Ward-Horner et al. 2014). The mance for similar reasons (e.g., greater delay to
most direct approach for examining this reinforcement and increased response effort).
question is to hold either the arrangement of Future research could clarify the role of
responses or distribution of reinforcers con- response characteristics by examining their
stant while manipulating the values of the effects on preference and performance for
other variable. For instance, to investigate response-reinforcer arrangements across experi-
the effects of the arrangement of responses, mental conditions. Nevertheless, it is important
researchers might arrange the discontinuous to note that manipulation of variables such as
condition so that a delay occurs between small vs. large tasks and easy vs. difcult tasks
each set of responses, instead of brief rein- may result in excessively long session durations,
forcer access, and continuous reinforcer which might evoke challenging behavior, par-
access is provided following the completion ticularly with continuous arrangements.
of all tasks. DeLeon et al. (2014) recom- Participant characteristics may also affect
mended an analogous procedure for examin- outcomes. The outcomes of the ve studies
ing the effects of reinforcer distribution. (Bukala et al., 2015; DeLeon et al., 2014;
RESPONSE-REINFORCER ARRANGEMENTS 5

Fienup et al., 2011; Kocher et al., 2015; Ward- of tasks completed and types of reinforcers con-
Horner et al., 2014) come from a fairly homog- sumed. Future research will certainly illuminate
enous group of participants. There were 12 the relative contribution of response characteris-
participants across the ve studies and their tics, reinforcement characteristics, and interac-
ages ranged from 13 to 20 years old. All partici- tion between responses and reinforcers.
pants followed one or two-step instructions,
nine participants communicated vocally, and
three participants relied primarily on non-vocal REFERENCES
modes of communication (i.e., gestures, sign Bonem M., & Crossman, E. K. (1988). Elucidating the
language, augmentative communication devices, effects of reinforcement magnitude. Psychological Bulletin,
104,348362.doi:10.1037/0033-2909.104.3.348
or a combination of non-vocal modes of com- Bukala, M., Hu, M. Y., Lee, R., Ward-Horner, J. C., &
munication). The extent to which participant Fienup, D. M. (2015). The effects of work schedules
characteristics inuenced the outcomes of these on performance and preference in participants with
autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 48, 215
studies is unclear. 220. doi:10.1002/jaba.188
There are several ways that future research DeLeon, I. G., Chase, J. A., Frank-Crawford, M. A.,
might clarify the effects of participant character- Carreau-Webster, A. B., Triggs, M. M.,
istics. First, researchers might attempt to repli- Bullock, C. E., & Jennett, H. K. (2014). Distributed
and accumulated reinforcement arrangements: Evalua-
cate this line of research with younger tions of efcacy and preference. Journal of Applied
participants and participants with more pro- Behavior Analysis, 47, 293313. doi:10.1002/jaba.116
found intellectual disabilities who may respond Fienup, D. M., Ahlers, A. A., & Pace, G. (2011). Prefer-
ence for uent versus disuent work schedules. Jour-
more impulsively and therefore prefer discontin- nal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44, 847858.
uous arrangements. Second, the research to date doi:10.1901/jaba.2011.44-847
has examined positively reinforced daily living Hoch, H., McComas, J. J., Johnson, L.,
skills and academic behavior; researchers might Faranda, N., & Guenther, S. L. (2002). The effects
of magnitude and quality of reinforcement on
examine whether participants with challenging choice responding during play activities. Journal
behavior maintained by negative reinforcement of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 171181.
perform similarly under continuous and discon- doi:10.1901/jaba.2002.35-171
Kocher, C. P., Howard, M. R., & Fienup, D. M. (2015).
tinuous arrangements. For instance, Lerman The effects of work-reinforcer schedules on skill acqui-
et al. (2006) demonstrated that two participants sition for children with Autism. Behavior Modication,
whose problem behavior was negatively rein- 69, 600621. doi:10.1177/0145445515583246
Lerman, D. C., Addison, L. R., & Kodak, T. (2006). A
forced responded impulsively by choosing to preliminary analysis of self-control with aversive
delay work completion when provided the events: The effects of task magnitude and delay on
choice of completing less work sooner or more the choices of children with autism. Journal of Applied
work later, suggesting that these participants Behavior Analysis, 39, 227232. doi:10.1901/
jaba.2006.90-05
would prefer discontinuous arrangements. Steinhilber, J., & Johnson, C. (2007). The effects of brief
and extended stimulus availability on preference.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, 767772.
doi:10.1901/jaba.2007.767-772
Summary Ward-Horner, J. C., Pittenger, A., Pace, G., &
Additional research on response-reinforcer Fienup, D. M. (2014). Effects of reinforcer magni-
arrangements is needed to better understand the tude and distribution on preference for work sche-
dules. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 47,
variables contributing to preference and perfor- 623627. doi:10.1002/jaba.133
mance. Consideration of the variables in
Table 1 may help disentangle these contributing
Received February 9, 2015
factors, which might lead to assessments and Final acceptance July 1, 2016
teaching procedures that t best with the types Action Editor, John Borrero

You might also like