You are on page 1of 5

PREVENTIVE MEDICINE 26, 607611 (1997)

ARTICLE NO. PM960124

REVIEW
The Nocebo Phenomenon: Concept, Evidence, and Implications for
Public Health
Robert A. Hahn, Ph.D., M.P.H.1
Division of Prevention Research and Analytic Methods, Epidemiology Program Office, Mailstop D-01, Centers for Disease Control and
Prevention, Atlanta, Georgia 30333

associated emotional states. Two forms of the nocebo


The nocebo hypothesis proposes that expectations effect should be recognized: In the specific form, the
of sickness and the affective states associated with subject expects a particular negative outcome and that
such expectations cause sickness in the expectant. The
outcome consequently occurs; for example, a surgical
nocebo phenomenon is a little-recognized facet of cul-
patient expects to die on the operating table and does
ture that may be responsible for a substantial variety
of pathology throughout the world. However, the ex-
dienot from the surgery itself, but from the expecta-
tent of the phenomenon is not yet known, and evi- tion and associated affect [7,35]. In the generic form,
dence is piecemeal and ambiguous. This paper reviews subjects have vague negative expectationsfor ex-
the concept of nocebo and its association with the pla- ample, they are diffusely pessimisticand their expec-
cebo phenomenon, gives examples of evidence for the tations are realized in terms of symptoms, sickness, or
nocebo phenomenon, and suggests public health im- deathnone of which was specifically expected. Again,
plications. expectation plays a causal role.
Key Words: nocebo; placebo; expectation; etiology. The nocebo phenomenon considered in this review is
distinct from placebo side effects (Fig. 1). Placebo side
effects occur when expectations of healing produce
INTRODUCTION
sickness, i.e., a positive expectation has a negative out-
The nocebo hypothesis proposes that expectations of come. For example, a rash that occurs following admin-
sickness and the affective states associated with such istration of a placebo remedy may be a placebo side
expectations cause sickness in the expectant. Result- effect. Diverse placebo side effects have been docu-
ant pathology may be subjective as well as objective mented; one review reports an incidence of 19% in the
conditions. Some nocebo effects may be transient; oth- subjects of pharmacologic studies [29]. In the nocebo
ers may be chronic or fatal. An extreme form of the phenomenon, however, the subject expects sickness to
nocebo phenomenon was described in Cannons classic be the outcome, i.e., the expectation is a negative one.
paper [7] as voodoo death. Because expectations are Nocebos may also have side effects, i.e., when negative
largely learned from the cultural environment, nocebo expectations produce positive outcomes or outcomes
effects are likely to vary from place to place. other than those expected.
The nocebo phenomenon, first named by Kennedy When Kennedy [17] and Kissel and Barrucand [19]
[17] and then elaborated by Kissel and Barrucand [19], first referred to the nocebo phenomenon, they did not
has not been systematically assessed. In this review, I distinguish placebo side effects from the effects of nega-
formulate a working definition of the nocebo phenom- tive expectations. However, reference to voodoo death,
enon that relates nocebos and placebos, present a for example, as an instance of the placebo phenomenon
range of examples of nocebo phenomena, and draw sev- is etymologically inappropriate. Kennedy and Kissel
eral implications for public health. and Barrucand distinguished placebos from nocebos
only in terms of positive and negative outcomes, not
A WORKING DEFINITION OF THE NOCEBO
PHENOMENON also in terms of expectations. Kennedys examples are
all placebo side effects, and Kissel and Barrucand did
The nocebo effect is the causation of sickness (or not separate examples of placebo side effects from an
death) by expectations of sickness (or death) and by example of nocebo in the sense proposed here: 80% of
hospitalized patients given sugar water and told that it
1
Fax: (404) 639-4463. was an emetic subsequently vomited. What distin-
607

0091-7435/97
608 ROBERT A. HAHN

only the study by Anda et al. [2] uses epidemiologic


methods to control for the confounding effects of other
risk factors. Anda et al. examined the effects of depres-
sion on ischemic heart disease (IHD) incidence and
mortality among a sample of U.S. adults. They exam-
ined persons who were free from heart disease at the
outset of the study and excluded subjects whose initial
depressed affect might have been the consequence of
chronic disease. Depression was assessed from the
General Well-Being Schedule [11]. Anda and col-
leagues found that persons with depressive affect were
1.6 times more likely to have nonfatal IHD and 1.5
times more likely to have fatal IHD than persons who
did not have depressive affect, independent of other
known risk factors for IHD. These researchers also ex-
amined the effects of hopelessness on heart disease in-
FIG. 1. The Placebo Thesis: relations between expectation and cidence and mortality, and found a dose responsea
outcome. Reproduced, by permission of the publisher, from Ref. [14]. critical criterion in the inference of causality. Greater
hopelessness was associated with greater incidence
guishes nocebos is that the subject has negative expec- and mortality.
tations and experiences a negative outcome. Schweiger Considering that an 11.1% prevalence of depressed
and Parducci [31] refer to nocebos as negative place- affect was assessed in the study cohorta sample of
bos. U.S. adultsit can be estimated (as the population
Nocebos are causal in the same way that commonly attributable risk) that approximately 26,000 deaths a
recognized pathogens are, e.g., cigarette smoke of lung year (i.e., more than 5% of U.S. IHD mortality and
cancer, the tubercular bacillus of tuberculosis [15,38]. more than 1% of all U.S. deaths) may be attributable to
That is, nocebos increase the likelihood that the sick- depression, independent of other risk factors. Mortality
ness they refer to will occur, and this effect is not the associated with depressive expectations is an example
result of confounding, i.e., the empirical association of of the generic form of the nocebo phenomenon. The
the hypothesized nocebo with another cause of the con- other examples in this review are of specific nocebo
dition. None of these exposures is a necessary or a suf- phenomena.
ficient cause of the given outcome.
Nosological Categories and Self-Scrutiny
EVIDENCE OF NOCEBO PHENOMENA
In one specific form, cardiac neurosis or cardiopho-
This review of evidence is divided according to the bia, patients are persistently fearful of heart attacks or
source or manner of acquisition of expectations. It be- other cardiac symptoms, and report chest pain, de-
gins with (1) the effects of inner, mental worlds and scribed by physicians as nonspecific. Although these
moves to (2) the effects of nosological categories and patients may not manifest recognized cardiac symp-
self-scrutiny, (3) sociogenic illness, or mass hysteria, toms, there is evidence that belief that one is suscep-
and (4) the deliberate induction of sickness or symp- tible to heart attacks is itself a risk factor for coronary
toms. death. Eaker examined women, 45 to 64 years of age, in
the Framingham Study for the 20-year incidence of
Inner, Mental World myocardial infarction and coronary death [12]. Women
who believed they were more likely than others to suf-
Mood, affect, and some psychiatric conditions are of-
fer a heart attack were 3.7 times as likely to die of
ten associated with negative expectations [1]. For ex-
coronary conditions as were women who believed they
ample, hopelessness is a prominent component of di-
were less likely to die of such symptoms, independent
verse forms of depression. Somatoform disorders such
of commonly recognized risk factors for coronary death
as hypochondriasis and conversion disorder may also
(e.g., smoking, systolic blood pressure, and the ratio of
be associated with expectations of pathology. Some
total to high-density lipoprotein cholesterol).
anxiety disorders, too, may be associated with expec-
tations of pathology. Panic disorder, for example, may Sociogenic Illness
involve a sense of impending doom and a fear of
death [1]. Sickness or symptoms may also occur when one per-
Although several studies indicate an association of son observes or learns of the sickness or symptoms in
negative expectations and affect with psychiatric con- others. Knowledge of sickness in others fosters an ex-
ditions and pathological outcomes [46,13,24,28,36,37], pectation that one may also be subject to the same
NOCEBO: CONCEPT, EVIDENCE, AND IMPLICATIONS 609
condition. Perhaps the best recognized form of conta- the union. They were 2.2 times as likely to be sole
gion by observation are epidemics referred to as socio- breadwinners, 5.6 times as likely to be divorced, and
genic, psychogenic illness, mass hysteria, or, in the 30% more likely to have a child under 6 years of age.
workplace, assembly line hysteria [9]. The outbreak began among women who were socially
Sirois [32] reviewed 78 documented outbreaks of isolated, subsequently spread among women connected
epidemic hysteria reported between 1872 and 1972. by links of close social relations, and finally diffused
Of these, 44% occurred in schools, 22% in towns, and among women less closely connected. The phenomenon
10% in factories. Twenty-eight percent involved fewer analyzed by Kerckhoff and Back might be described as
than 10 persons, 32% involved 1030 persons, and 19% mass somatization.
more than 30 persons; 5% were of unreported magni- The effects of a persons social environment on sick-
tude. (Whereas the largest outbreak noted by Sirois ness or illness behavior need not involve direct per-
involved approximately 200 persons, an outbreak has sonal contact. An association has been found between
been described that involved 949 persons [23]). Only traumatic death or violence in the community environ-
females were involved in 74% of the outbreaks, only ment and subsequent suicide or suicide-like behavior
males in 4% [32]. Outbreaks occurred more commonly [2527]. In this instance, the first victim serves as a
among persons from lower socioeconomic classes and in model with whom others may identify. For example,
periods of uncertainty and social stress. Convulsions when newspaper or television stories about a suicide
were reported in 24% of outbreaks, abnormal move- are released, the rate of suicide may increase in the
ments in 18%, and fainting, globus/cough/laryngismus, following week; the greater the circulation of the news-
and loss of sensation in 11.5% each. Symptomatology paper, the greater the increase [25]. After Marilyn
changed over the 100 years surveyed, from more glo- Monroes suicide in 1962, 197 suicides occurred in the
bus/cough/laryngismus and abnormal movements to United States during the following week12% more
more fainting, nausea, abdominal malaise, and head- than the number expected on the basis of past suicide
aches. patterns [25]. A recent study indicates that teenagers
Colligan and Murphy [8] point out that sociogenic are more susceptible to televised publicity about sui-
outbreaks are commonly associated with a source be- cide and that increases in suicides are greater for girls
lieved to be related to the symptoms, e.g., a strange than for boys [27].
odor or gas, new solvent, or an insect bite. However, Motor vehicle fatalities also follow newspaper stories
sometimes reported symptoms do not fit biomedical of suicide. Phillips [26] calculates that, on average, mo-
knowledge of associations between potential toxins or tor vehicle fatalities increase 9% above the expected
pathogens and pathophysiology. Persons affected often rate in the week following front-page reporting of sui-
have repetitive jobs, are under unusual stress, and/or cides in newspaper stories and that, when the newspa-
have poor relations with superiors. They may be in per has greater than average circulation, the increase
poorer general health and have been absent more often is 19%.
than persons who are not affected. Colligan and Mur-
phy indicate that sociogenic outbreaks in workplace Sickness/Symptoms Induced
settings are substantially underreported.
Sirois [33] estimates that sociogenic outbreaks occur Social psychologists have conducted diverse experi-
in approximately 1 of every 1,000 schools per year in ments that demonstrate the effects of negative sugges-
the province of Quebec. A review of recent school out- tion on the experience of negative symptoms [16,20,
breaks in diverse countries indicates attack rates (i.e., 30,31,34]. In one experiment, 47.5% of asthmatics who
the proportion of persons exposed who experience the were exposed to (normally innocuous) nebulized saline
condition) of 648% [3]. solution and told they were inhaling irritants or aller-
The study of Kerckhoff and Back [18] of the 1962 gens experienced substantially increased airway resis-
June Bug outbreak in a Montana mill is one of the tance and changes in airway resistance and thoracic
few to carefully reconstruct social patterns of the gas volume [22]. Controls who did not have asthma
spread of a sociogenic condition. In the June Bug event, were unaffected by exposure to the same stimulus.
those affected fainted or complained of pain, nausea, or Twelve asthmatic subjects developed full-blown at-
disorientation. Sixty-two (6.4%) of 965 workers were tacks that were relieved by the same saline solution
affected, 59 (95.2%) of them women; all those affected presented therapeutically. (The researchers also refer
worked in dressmaking departments. Persons affected to an asthmatic patient in another study whose allergy
were 70% more likely than controls to believe that the to roses was induced by plastic as well as natural roses,
cause of the outbreak was an insect or other physical indicating that the effect of the rose did not result en-
object. Persons affected were 62% more likely to have tirely from its botanical properties.)
worked overtime at least 2 or 3 times a week than In a follow-up, double-blind experiment, Luparello et
those not affected. Persons affected were less likely to al. [21] randomized asthmatic patients to four condi-
go to a supervisor with a complaint or to be members of tions: Two groups were given a bronchodilator, the
610 ROBERT A. HAHN

other two a bronchoconstrictor; half of the group given dent of other risk factors; that symptoms may spread
each substance was told they were being given a bron- in communities by being witnessed; and that symp-
chodilator, the other half that they were being given a toms may be caused by experimentally induced expec-
bronchoconstrictor. For each substance administered, tations. Further investigations should explore the
expectations induced by misinformation about the sub- ways in which, like the placebo phenomenon, the ex-
stance reduced its physiologic effectiveness by 43% (for pectations of the nocebo phenomenon translate diverse
the bronchoconstrictor) and 49% (for the bronchodila- cultural beliefs into physiological process.
tor). I conclude with two implications of the nocebo phe-
Another study was designed to evaluate a method for nomenon for public health.
the diagnosis of psychogenic seizures, reported to ac- First, the nocebo phenomenon is a side effect of hu-
count for as much as 20% of refractory epilepsy [20]. man culture. A societys culture tells its members how
Lancman and colleagues compared the effect of sugges- the world is divided, interconnected, and known; it
tion on the induction of seizure behavior in patients specifies what is valued and what is not, what is good,
with psychogenic seizures and others with known epi- beautiful, right, wrong, and indifferent; it provides
lepsy. Patients were told that a medicine administered rules of conduct whereby the societys members know
through a skin patch would induce seizures within 30 how to behave and how to judge the behavior of others
sec, and that removal of the patch would end the sei- [14]. One element of cultures has been referred to as an
zure. Of patients with psychogenic seizures, 77% mani- ethnomedicine. A societys ethnomedicine tells soci-
fested seizures when the patch was applied, with etal members what sicknesses there are, how they are
symptoms such as nonresponsiveness, generalized vio- acquired, how manifested, how treated. The nocebo
lent thrashing, and uncoordinated movements; 19% of phenomenon suggests that the categories of an ethno-
these patients reported auras, and 44% showed postic- medicine may not only describe conditions of sickness,
tal confusion and/or sleepiness. None of the patients but may also foster those conditions by establishing
with diagnosed epilepsy manifested seizures. expectations that they may occur. Thus, a cultural sys-
Finally, another study [16], designed to evaluate a tem commonly thought to serve a healing function may
controversial method of food allergy testing, compared also have a contrary outcome, fostering the same pa-
the effect of injecting the food substancesthe test to thologies intended to be healed. The assessment of the
be evaluatedwith the effect of injecting saline diluent extent of this noxious facet of ethnomedicines, includ-
without the substance in question on symptoms that ing our own system of biomedicine, is an important
included itching of the nose, watering or burning eyes, public health challenge.
plugged ears, tight or scratchy throat, nausea, dizzi- Second, and more immediately practical, if commu-
ness, sleepiness, and depression. (Patients with a his- nication about pathological conditions may serve not
tory of anaphylactic reactions or documented cardiac only to describe, but, in a sense, also to foster sickness
irregularity or other severe reactions to their allergies by creating expectations, then we must be cautious in
were excluded.) In this double-blind study, the propor- both public health communications and in clinical
tion of patients who experienced symptoms was not medicine. We need to know more about how health
statistically different in patients given test (27%) and messages affect their audience. Such knowledge may
nocebo diluent injections (24%). Neutralizing injec- enhance our ability to minimize the pathological con-
tions, given to eliminate the reactions, were also sequences of negative messages. The placebo/nocebo
equally effective whether they contained the food sub- phenomenon suggests that it may be healthier to err on
stance orin this casethe diluent placebo. An injec- the side of optimism than on the side of pessimism.
tion becomes a nocebo (or placebo) not because of its
contents, but because of the pessimistic (or optimistic) REFERENCES
expectations of its consumer. 1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical
manual of mental disorders, 3rd ed. Washington: Am. Psychiatr.
DISCUSSION Assoc., 1980.
2. Anda R, Williamson D, Jones D, Macera C, Eaker E, Glassman
I have reviewed a range of studies indicating that A, Marks J. Depressed affect, hopelessness, and the risk of isch-
socially given negative expectations and their emo- emic heart disease in a cohort of U.S. adults. Epidemiology
1993;4(4):28594.
tional associations facilitate their own realization. Be-
liefs can make us sick as well as healthy. The nocebo 3. Arcidiacono S, Brand JI, Coppenger W, Calder RA. Mass socio-
genic illness in a day-care centerFlorida. MMWR 1990;31(18):
phenomenon is a little-recognized facet of culture that 3014.
may be responsible for a substantial variety of pathol- 4. Black DW, Warrack G, Winokur G. Excess mortality among psy-
ogy throughout the world. However, the extent of the chiatric patients. JAMA 1985;253(1):5861.
phenomenon is not yet known, and evidence is piece- 5. Black DW, Warrack G, Winokur G. The Iowa record-linkage
meal and ambiguous. There is evidence that inner, study. II. Excess mortality among patients with functional dis-
mental states affect pathological outcomes, indepen- orders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1985;42:828.
NOCEBO: CONCEPT, EVIDENCE, AND IMPLICATIONS 611
6. Bruce ML, Leaf PJ, Rozal GPM, et al. Psychiatric status and 9 23. Modan B, Tirosh M, Weissenberg E, et al. The Arjenyattah epi-
year mortality data in the New Haven epidemiologic catchment demic: a mass phenomenon: spread and triggering factors. Lan-
area study. Am J Psychiatry 1994;151(5):71621. cet 1983;24/31:14724.
7. Cannon WB. Voodoo death. Am Anthropol 1942;44(2):16981. 24. Newman SC, Bland RC. Mortality in a cohort of patients with
8. Colligan MJ, Murphy LR. Mass psychogenic illness in organiza- schizophrenia: a record linkage study. Can J Psychiatry 1991;
tions: an overview. J Occup Psychol 1979;52:7790. 36:23945.
9. Colligan MJ, Stockton W. The mystery of assembly-line hysteria. 25. Phillips DP. The influence of suggestion on suicide: substantive
Psychol Today 1976;12:93116. and theoretical implications of the Werther effect. Am Soc Rev
10. Conti S, Savron G, Bartolucci G, et al. Cardiac neurosis and 1974;39:34054.
psychopathology. Psychother Psychosom 1989;52:8891. 26. Phillips DP. Motor vehicle fatalities increase just after publi-
11. Dupuy HJ. A concurrent validational study of the NCHS Gen- cized suicide stories. Science 1977;196:14645.
eral Well-Being Schedule. Washington: U.S. Govt Printing Of- 27. Phillips DP, Carstensen LL. Clustering of teenage suicides after
fice, 1977; Vital and Health Statistics, Ser. 2, No. 73, DHEW television news stories about suicide. N Engl J Med 1986;315:
Publication No. (HRA)78-1347. 6859.
12. Eaker E, Pinsky J, Castelli WP. Myocardial infarction and coro- 28. Reich P. Psychological predisposition to life-threatening ar-
nary death among women: psychosocial predictors from a 20- rhythmias. Annu Rev Med 1985;36:397405.
year follow-up of women in the Framingham Study. Am J Epi-
29. Rosenzweig P, Brohier S, Zipfel A. The placebo effect in healthy
demiol 1992;135:85464.
volunteers: influence of experimental conditions on the adverse
13. Friedman HS, Booth-Kewley S. The disease-prone personality: events profile during phase I studies. Clin Pharmacol Ther
a meta-analytic view of the construct. Am Psychol 1987;42(6): 1993;54(5):57883.
53955.
30. Schachter S, Singer JE. Cognitive, social, and physiological de-
14. Hahn RA. Sickness and healing: an anthropological perspective. terminants of emotional state. Psychol Rev 1962;69(5):37999.
New Haven (CT): Yale Univ. Press, 1995.
31. Schweiger A, Parducci A. Nocebo: the psychologic induction of
15. Harris HW, McClement JH. Pulmonary tuberculosis. In
pain. Pavlov J Biol Sci 1981;16:1403.
Hoeprich PD, editor. Infectious diseases. New York: Harper &
Row, 1983:378404. 32. Sirois F. Epidemic hysteria. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1974;51(252):
744.
16. Jewett DL, Fein G, Greenberg MH. A double-blind study of
symptom provocation to determine food sensitivity. N Engl J 33. Sirois F. A propos de la frequence des epidemies d9hysterie.
Med 1990;323(7):42933. Union Med Can 1975;104:1213.
17. Kennedy WP. The nocebo reaction. Med World 1961;91:2035. 34. Sternbach RA. The effects of instructional sets on autonomic
18. Kerckhoff AC, Back KW. The June Bug: a study of hysterical responsivity. Psychophysiology 1964;1(1):6772.
contagion. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1968. 35. Weisman AD, Hackett TP. Predilection to death: death and dy-
19. Kissel P, Barrucand D. Placebos et effet placebo en medecine. ing as a psychiatric problem. Psychosom Med 1961;23(3):232
Paris: Masson, 1964. 56.
20. Lancman ME, Asconape JJ, Craven WJ, Howard G, Penry JK. 36. Weissman MM, Markowitz JS, Ouellette R, Greenwald S, Kahn
Predictive value of induction of psychogenic seizures by sugges- JP. Panic disorder and cardiovascular/cerebrovascular prob-
tion. Ann Neurol 1994;35(3):35961. lems: results from a community survey. Am J Psychiatry 1990;
21. Luparello TJ, Leist N, Lourie CH, Sweet P. The interaction of 147:15048.
psychologic stimuli and pharmacologic agents on airway reac- 37. Wells KB, Stewart A, Hays RD, et al. The functioning and well-
tivity in asthmatic subjects. Psychosom Med 1970;32(5):50913. being of depressed patients. JAMA 1989;262(7):9149.
22. Luparello T, Lyons HA, Bleecker ER, McFadden ER. Influences 38. United States Surgeon General. Reducing the health conse-
of suggestion on airway reactivity in asthmatic subjects. Psycho- quence of smoking. 25 years of progress. Rockville, MD: U.S.
som Med 1968;30:81925. Public Health Service, 1989.

You might also like