You are on page 1of 14

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY ARTICLE

published: 21 May 2012


doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00158

The benets of executive control training and the


implications for language processing
Erika K. Hussey 1,2 * and Jared M. Novick 1,2
1
Program in Neuroscience and Cognitive Science, Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, USA
2
Center for Advanced Study of Language, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, USA

Edited by: Recent psycholinguistics research suggests that the executive function (EF) skill known as
Andriy Myachykov, University of
conict resolution the ability to adjust behavior in the service of resolving among incom-
Glasgow, UK
patible representations is important for several language processing tasks such as lexical
Reviewed by:
Silvia P. Gennari, University of York, UK and syntactic ambiguity resolution, verbal uency, and common-ground assessment. Here,
Marina Bedny, Massachusetts we discuss work showing that various EF skills can be enhanced through consistent prac-
Institute of Technology, USA tice with working-memory tasks that tap these EFs, and, moreover, that improvements on
*Correspondence: the training tasks transfer across domains to novel tasks that may rely on shared underly-
Erika K. Hussey , Program in
ing EFs. These ndings have implications for language processing and could launch new
Neuroscience and Cognitive Science,
Department of Psychology, University research exploring if EF training, within a process-specic framework, could be used as a
of Maryland, 1147 Biology-Psychology remediation tool for improving general language use. Indeed, work in our lab demonstrates
Building, College Park, MD 20916, that EF training that increases conict-resolution processes has selective benets on an
USA.
untrained sentence-processing task requiring syntactic ambiguity resolution, which relies
e-mail: ehussey@umd.edu
on shared conict-resolution functions. Given claims that conict-resolution abilities con-
tribute to a range of linguistic skills, EF training targeting this process could theoretically
yield wider performance gains beyond garden-path recovery. We offer some hypotheses
on the potential benets of EF training as a component of interventions to mitigate general
difculties in language processing. However, there are caveats to consider as well, which
we also address.
Keywords: cognitive training, executive function, conflict resolution, process-specificity, language processing,
ambiguity resolution

INTRODUCTION refrain from turning; revise the more frequent meaning, but cur-
Cognitive control, also called executive function (EF), refers to rent misanalysis, of bill). Such conict resolution functions are
a cluster of mental processes that permit the exible adjustment an essential part of cognitive control (Botvinick et al., 2001) and
of thoughts and actions across domains, allowing individuals to help adapt information-processing strategies so individuals can
adapt to new rules and guide the selection of task-relevant over regulate behavior in view of ever-changing goals, new contexts, or
task-irrelevant information in an environment that varies contin- situation-specic demands.
uously (Miller and Cohen, 2001). As we navigate our surroundings, As many researchers have argued, EFs encompass a collection of
we can frequently rely on a set of highly regularized functions that cognitive processes that help guide goal-directed behavior; that is,
render certain tasks like driving a car or skimming a magazine cognitive control is not a unitary construct but comprises separa-
article relatively automatic. Sometimes, however, new instructions ble components (Norman and Shallice, 1986; Botvinick et al., 2001;
or conicting information compels us to override these reexive Miller and Cohen, 2001). In addition to the conict-resolution
actions and instead consider what might otherwise be a disfavored processes outlined above, other EFs include task-switching, updat-
(or atypical) response. For instance, a resident of Chicago may ing, and information monitoring, each of which can operate over
be in the habit of making a legal right turn on red when driving visual, spatial, or verbal domains (Smith and Jonides, 1999; Miyake
at home, but this routine behavior could result in a costly ticket et al., 2000; Friedman and Miyake, 2004) and thus may be recruited
when she visits New York City, where turning on red is strictly across a variety of tasks including selective attention, decision-
prohibited! Likewise, imagine reading the following sentence upon making, working memory (WM), error monitoring, and language
skimming a magazine: at the restaurant, the interns discussed the bill processing (Botvinick et al., 2001; Thompson-Schill et al., 2005;
before suggesting edits to the senator. One might initially interpret Badre and Wagner, 2007; inter alia). With regard to conict-
the word bill to mean the list of charges incurred for the meal, resolution functions in particular, converging data from neuropsy-
rather than its intended (though less common) interpretation, chological patients and brain-imaging studies of healthy adults
namely a draft piece of legislation. On the surface, both examples suggest that, across a range of WM, attention, and language tasks,
are quite different, but conceivably induce a similar experience: the posterior regions of left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC)
detection of an incompatibility and the ensuing need to rein-in a commonly support the ability to resolve among competing sources
highly familiar, yet currently inappropriate cognitive reaction (e.g., of evidence, regardless of domain (Thompson-Schill et al., 2005).

www.frontiersin.org May 2012 | Volume 3 | Article 158 | 1


Hussey and Novick Cognitive training for language use

In this paper, we discuss how a burgeoning literature demon- in the form of underdetermined conict, in which multiple can-
strates that EFs can be trained through ample practice that such didate representations are equally reasonable and thus compete
abilities are seemingly not xed, but malleable and that per- for selection (Botvinick et al., 2001). Importantly, brain-imaging
formance increases throughout the course of training generalize ndings suggest separable neuroanatomical involvement for rep-
to novel tasks that were not part of the training protocol. Some resentational conict versus response conict (or response selection;
examples of transfer include benets on unpracticed tasks tapping see Milham et al., 2001; Nelson et al., 2003). Our major focus here is
uid intelligence (Jaeggi et al., 2008), working-memory updating on the implications of conict-resolution training at the represen-
(Dahlin et al., 2008; Li et al., 2008), and task-switching (Karbach tational level on particular language-performance measures such
and Kray, 2009) that is to say, transfer benets have been observed as lexical and syntactic ambiguity resolution (in comprehension)
across a range of EF. and verbal uency (in production). Both prepotent and under-
We are especially interested in the implications that these determined representational conicts recruit posterior regions of
training-transfer ndings have for language processing under con- VLPFC (Brodmann areas 44 and 45) across language and memory
ditions of conict, given that domain-general conict-resolution domains, meeting the requirements for a test of process-specicity
and cognitive-control functions have been associated with assorted (see Novick et al., 2010; see also Milham et al., 2001 and Nelson
linguistic abilities including the resolution of lexical (Bilenko et al., et al., 2003, which demonstrate VLPFC recruitment for represen-
2009; Copland et al., 2009; Khanna and Boland, 2010) and syn- tational conict resolution but anterior cingulate recruitment for
tactic ambiguities (Novick et al., 2005), verbal uency (Robinson response-level conict resolution).
et al., 1998; Kan and Thompson-Schill, 2004; Novick et al., 2009; Generally, we believe that considering the mounting evi-
Schnur et al., 2009), and perspective-taking during natural dialog dence showing the effectiveness of various types of EF training
(Brown-Schmidt, 2009; Nilsen and Graham, 2009; for reviews, see in different populations (Klingberg et al., 2005; Westerberg et al.,
Novick et al., 2005; Novick et al., 2010). Thus, in the hypothesis 2007; Jaeggi et al., 2011) there is room to establish new research
section, which details the potential implications of EF training investigating if EF training protocols that focus on selective sub-
and transfer effects on language use, we consider a theory based processes (i.e., representational conict resolution) could be used
on evidence that left VLPFC-supported conict resolution is the successfully as an intervention technique to mitigate problems in
kind of cognitive-control function of principal relevance to these general language use that arise under high-EF (i.e., high-conict)
particular linguistic tasks (see e.g., Novick et al., 2005). We couch demands.
our hypotheses within a process-specic account (see Dahlin et al., Indeed, there is tantalizing evidence supporting process-
2008; Shipstead et al., 2010, 2012), which in the training litera- specic transfer to conict-related language measures, drawn
ture posits that post-intervention, performance increases on novel not from a long-term training paradigm per se, but rather from
tasks largely depends on the extent of overlap between the train- another type of intervention designed to fatigue selective cogni-
ing and transfer measures, both in terms of the shared cognitive tive processes common to WM and language processing tasks.
processes and underlying neural systems needed to complete them. These so-called resource depletion models offer an interesting
That is, if a certain component of EF (e.g., conict resolution) is framework to understand negative transfer to tasks relying on tem-
targeted and improved through training, then transfer measures porarily exhausted EFs shared across ostensibly different domains
relying on common processes should be inuenced accordingly, (Van der Linden et al., 2003; Persson et al., 2007). That is, rather
irrespective of domain. In view of this, we will focus our discus- than boosting general-purpose EFs through long-term practice, as
sion on a few language comprehension and production tasks that is the case with training studies, resource depletion paradigms
t within the VLPFC-mediated process-specic function typically rely on short-term overuse of a particular cognitive process.
referred to as conict resolution. However, in the discussion, we For example, after performing a complex task that places high
acknowledge other brain systems involved in a wider array of EFs, demands on EF capacities, these resources are rendered temporar-
and consider briey the implications for training and the effects on ily unavailable for continued use; therefore, performance decreases
language. on transfer measures that rely on the common worn out EF (Van
As sketched in the driving and reading examples earlier, when der Linden et al., 2003; Persson et al., 2007; see also Snyder et al.,
we talk about conict (or interference), we are referring to condi- 2010 for similar ndings among anxious individuals).
tions that contain the presence of mismatched or incongruent In one study (Persson et al., 2007), conict-resolution abilities
sources of evidence. Specically, conict designates cases in were fatigued through an intensive session of an item-recognition
which current situation-specic demands generate an incompat- task with high conict-resolution demands. In this task, partic-
ibility between how an input stimulus should be characterized ipants indicated whether a probe item (e.g., C) appeared in an
(dubbed representational conict ), given how the input is nor- immediately prior memory set (e.g., r, f, c, l; see Monsell, 1978).
mally considered. Such conict is often called prepotent conict, Frequently, subjects could respond correctly due to familiarity
because individuals must override their dominant (prepotent) alone: familiar probes required a yes response and unfamiliar
biases in support of atypical alternatives (Botvinick et al., 2001). ones a no response. However, relying on familiarity on some
For instance, the Stroop task is a canonical representational con- no trials was prone to error, because they contained a probe
ict task involving the need to countermand a prepotent bias that (e.g., G) that was not among the current memory set (j, p, v, m)
is generated by a lexical representation (which gives rise to an but was among the items in the prior trial (g, k, v, p). Thus, these
automatic reading response), in favor of a perceptual (color) rep- trials required subjects to override a prepotent familiarity bias (and
resentation. A comparable type of representational conict occurs yes response) and instead re-characterize the probe stimulus as

Frontiers in Psychology | Cognition May 2012 | Volume 3 | Article 158 | 2


Hussey and Novick Cognitive training for language use

familiar-but-irrelevant, and respond no. Such recent-no trial task to another (i.e., from an intervention task to a transfer task).
types, when compared to non-recent-no trials (when the probe Transfer from training to untrained assessment tasks cannot be
did not appear in either the current or preceding sets) routinely expected, or explained, without a well-formulated process-specic
recruit left posterior VLPFC (Jonides and Nee, 2006). Impor- theory (Shipstead et al., 2010, 2012). To this end, we also specu-
tant for the current discussion, after subjects completed this task late that the magnitude of transfer effects is contingent upon the
and fatigued the conict-resolution process, they subsequently degree to which a targeted EF contributes to and shares critical
demonstrated selective performance decline on VLPFC-mediated, features with an outcome measure. This is particularly important
high-conict conditions on a verbal uency task, in which they had if, as some researchers suggest, EF is not a unitary construct but is
to generate an associated verb to a given noun (e.g., scissors cut ; comprised of separable, multi-component processes such as con-
high-conict items had many possible associated verbs, like ball ict resolution, updating, and task-switching (Miyake et al., 2000;
kick, throw, catch, bounce, and thus contained underdetermined Persson et al., 2007; Dahlin et al., 2008).
response conict; see Thompson-Schill et al., 1998). This pattern As outlined in this hypothesis and theory piece, we integrate
of negative transfer was not observed for (1) subjects who received the extant training and psycholinguistic literatures to develop
exposure to only low-conict trials during their intensive practice testable hypotheses from an emerging picture within the EF train-
session (i.e., no recent-no trials were present); or (2) individu- ing research. The following section begins with a brief review of
als who practiced a different task before the verb generation task, cognitive training studies demonstrating transfer to novel tasks
namely a stop-signal task that recruits mainly right-hemisphere that are ostensibly different from those practiced during the train-
networks and a different subcomponent of EF (response inhibi- ing regimens, but share specic processing demands. We then
tion; see also Friedman and Miyake, 2004). Together, this suggests turn to research on the role of conict resolution in language
that the process-specicity observed across intervention and trans- use, sketching some hypotheses and implications the training
fer tasks operates on a short time scale, such that as conict ndings have for new work aimed at improving language pro-
resolution is temporarily depleted, other tasks relying on shared cessing under high-EF particularly high-conict-resolution
cognitive and neural resources are affected accordingly. demands. That is, if conict-resolution is malleable (which seems
Although these effects are transient, the selective transfer nd- to be the case given the resource depletion work outlined above),
ings are nonetheless critical: they demonstrate that conict res- we hypothesize that training such processes should also show
olution abilities are at least temporarily malleable, and this mal- transfer to untrained measures of conict resolution within the
leability can subsequently affect language processing under similar linguistic domain, patterning with other training-transfer nd-
conditions of high conict. Consequently, we ask: considering evi- ings. The theory bolstering this claim comes from work (drawn
dence for process-specic transfer, on a short time scale, across from patients, children, and brain-imaging studies of adults)
memory and language tasks that commonly rely on VLPFC- indicating that conict-resolution and cognitive-control mea-
mediated conict-resolution functions, might one observe longer- sures play an important role in language tasks that we outline
term effects on language measures as well, when conict resolution below.
is boosted via extensive practice? That is, can we observe positive
transfer namely, performance increases when individuals con- EXECUTIVE FUNCTION TRAINING AND ITS TRANSFER
sistently train conict-resolution functions over time? We hypoth- ACROSS COGNITIVE DOMAINS: A BRIEF REVIEW
esize that the answer should be yes, given the evidence that other A recent urry of research is devoted to testing if general-purpose
EFs (e.g., task-switching, etc.) are both trainable and transferrable. cognitive abilities can be enhanced through consistent practice
Indeed, work from our lab demonstrates reliable transfer to syn- with WM tasks that recruit brain regions within the cortico-
tactic ambiguity resolution in healthy adults, where individuals striatal network key to executive functioning. Although interven-
who have undergone extensive conict-resolution training fare tions geared toward improving psychological faculties, specically
signicantly better at revising early misinterpretations than their intelligence, were pioneered decades ago (see Feuerstein, 1980),
untrained counterparts (Hussey et al., 2010; Novick et al., sub- Klingberg and colleagues have recently reinstated the notion by
mitted for publication). Additionally, on the basis of the theory training domain-general cognitive abilities as a means to remedi-
that posterior regions of VLPFC support conict resolution across ate populations with diminished WM resources including stroke
domains, such displays of transfer, we hypothesize, might clearly patients (Westerberg et al., 2007), children with attention decit
extend beyond just garden-path recovery, given the putative hyperactivity disorder (Klingberg et al., 2005), and older adults
role of conict-resolution in several other measures of language (Brehmer et al., 2011). Ever since, cognitive training programs
processing. have undergone signicant study, particularly in healthy adults,
Although we outline below some potential benets of conict- to examine whether normally functioning individuals EF abili-
resolution training on language use, we also discuss some caveats ties can be improved, and what generalized outcomes consistent
that should be considered, including individual differences in training might have on everyday performance on non-trained
training success (not everyone responds to training or achieves tasks. To this end, researchers have been investigating questions
similarly high levels, cf. Chein and Morrison, 2010; Jaeggi et al., related to dosage-dependence (does more practice yield more
2011), limitations that may be involved in training special popula- transfer?; Jaeggi et al., 2008), the extent to which training trans-
tions, and the need for explicit linking hypotheses between training fers to untrained but related measures (Li et al., 2008; Karbach
and any expected transfer: namely, there must be a theory that and Kray, 2009; Chein and Morrison, 2010; Morrison and Chein,
bridges the hypothesized underlying cognitive processes from one 2011), if training tasks must adapt to individuals performance

www.frontiersin.org May 2012 | Volume 3 | Article 158 | 3


Hussey and Novick Cognitive training for language use

to be effective (Klingberg et al., 2005; Brehmer et al., 2011), and all require the same basic strategy, namely, information must be
individual differences in training success (Jaeggi et al., 2011). monitored and updated in a predictable fashion.
Here, we focus on the extent to which training generalizes to In addition to the above ndings, Karbach and Kray (2009)
novel tasks. The typical training study is designed as a pre/post observed that increases in task-switching abilities an EF based
longitudinal experiment in which subjects are assessed on some on mental shifting across different goals or rules as a con-
cognitive capacity immediately before and again after an extensive sequence of training generalizes to performance on novel tasks
intervention. In some cases, the intervention comprises practice with similar switching demands. Specically, their training reg-
with a single training task (Dahlin et al., 2008; Jaeggi et al., 2008, imen involved making two-alternative forced-choice judgments
2011; Li et al., 2008), whereas in others, a battery of training tasks about pictures (trees/owers), based on two separate character-
is administered (Klingberg et al., 2002, 2005; Karbach and Kray, istics (e.g., identity vs. color), such that the relevant character-
2009). Regardless, the training tasks are different from those com- istic (or rule) changed predictably across trials. Stimulus types
pleted at the pre/post assessment sessions, with the intervention (sh/birds, trees/owers, sports/music, planes/cars) and response
component typically lasting for several hours distributed over a categories (identity, number, color, and rotation) varied across ses-
few weeks. Upon conclusion of the regimen, trainees return to the sions within the training regimen. An assessment of near-transfer
lab and complete follow-up assessments, namely complementary involved responding to a novel set of stimuli (fruits/vegetables)
versions of the tasks that were done just prior to training, to eval- using number and identity as response categories; compared to
uate whether performance on assessments has reliably improved, a non-switching active-control group, the task-switching trainees
thereby providing evidence for transfer. showed greater posttest improvement in switching costs, i.e., the
Transfer has been documented for untrained tasks that share difference in response time on switch (color followed by identity
obvious features with well-practiced training tasks, an effect some- judgment) vs. non-switch trials.
times referred to as near-transfer. For instance, performance These examples highlight two sources of near-transfer: train-
increases on WM training tasks generalize to structurally simi- ing and outcome measures tap the same underlying EFs (e.g.,
lar (but new) WM assessments (Li et al., 2008; Karbach and Kray, monitoring and updating), and both tasks provoke similar pro-
2009; see below). However, far transfer can also be observed, cessing demands through a shared task structure (task-specic
namely to assessments that appear, on the surface, to be wildly aspects). Consequently, it is difcult to disentangle the source
different from the training tasks completed throughout the inter- of near-transfer effects, as two possibilities may account for any
vention regimen (Kloo and Perner, 2003; Dahlin et al., 2008; Jaeggi observed pre/post changes: (1) the trained EF shared by both tasks
et al., 2008, 2011). This latter form of transfer is possible provided may have been improved, or (2) a task-specic strategy may have
that training and assessment tasks share certain essential underly- been developed. Indeed, in cases of near-transfer, the training and
ing EFs (as well as overlapping neural resources; see Jonides, 2004; transfer tasks need not tap the same underlying EFs, since transfer
Shipstead et al., 2010, 2012). could occur simply with improvements at task-specic aspects of
the paradigm. Near-transfer effects might be unsurprising: prac-
NEAR-TRANSFER OF TRAINING ticing an n-back task improves n-back performance, and therefore
Near-transfer effects emerge when the nature of the processed transfers to other n-back tasks (perhaps regardless of domain);
information including stimulus type, task structure, and likewise, practicing a categorization task-switching task general-
response type is similar across training and assessment tasks izes to a similar task with novel categories. But, the extent to which
(but see Morrison and Chein, 2011 for an alternative denition of these near-transfer effects are driven by the shared EFs across train-
near-transfer). For instance, in one report (Li et al., 2008), trainees ing and assessment tasks, the surface-level features (stimulus or
practiced a spatial 2-back task, during which they had to monitor response characteristics) that are isomorphic between both sets of
the locations of sequentially presented squares on a 3 3 grid and tasks, or through a combination of both factors is unknown.
respond whenever the current location matched the location seen
two trials earlier. Compared to a no-contact control group, trained FAR-TRANSFER OF TRAINING
participants demonstrated post-intervention improvements on Training studies designed to show far-transfer effects help to elu-
a spatial 3-back task, providing evidence for near-transfer to a cidate the role of shared EFs; by design, the surface-level proper-
more difcult, but otherwise identical task. Another type of near- ties stimuli or required responses of the training and assessment
transfer occurs when the type of information (i.e., the stimuli) tasks are quite different. Consequently, contrary to near-transfer
being processed is changed across training and transfer tasks, ndings, far-transfer effects are assumed not to rely heavily on the
while the response-level requirements remain constant, resulting structural (task-specic) similarities across training and assess-
in a structural continuity between both tasks. For example, in the ment tasks, and instead result mostly from improvements on
same study by Li et al. (2008), trainees also improved on numeric underlying EFs important to both the training and assessment
2- and 3-back tasks, where instead of remembering locations on measures (Shipstead et al., 2010). In other words, the goal of far-
a grid, subjects indicated when a serially presented number (0 transfer training is rooted in improvement of specic processes
9) matched the identity of a number presented two (or three) engaged during tasks with dissimilar structures, often spanning
trials previously. The authors argued that transfer to a numeric domains (again, sometimes referred to as process-specic training ).
n-back task provided support for a task-specic response strategy For instance, in one set of studies, subjects practiced a dual
shared across stimulus modalities: Although the spatial 3-back and n-back memory task involving simultaneous updating of shape
numeric n-back tasks differ from the spatial 2-back training task, locations and the identity of heard letters, such that a target was

Frontiers in Psychology | Cognition May 2012 | Volume 3 | Article 158 | 4


Hussey and Novick Cognitive training for language use

dened as an item repeating n-trials previously in either modality Given these training and far-transfer effects for a range of
(Jaeggi et al., 2008). Trainees showed subsequent improvements EFs (e.g., attention control, memory updating), one might also
on Ravens Advanced Progressive Matrices, a transfer task that hypothesize that transfer from general-purpose EF training to
requires participants to select a textured shape from a set of possi- certain tasks of language processing might occur as well. That
ble response items, which ts a sequence of other textured shapes is, the language tasks are not trained per se, but tap particu-
to complete a particular pattern with one absent piece (Jaeggi lar cognitive functions (conict resolution) that may be train-
et al., 2008, 2011). The response and surface-level properties of able through an extensive regimen targeting common processes
n-back and Ravens are distinct, as one task involves monitor- (or neural resources). As hypothesized below, the result could
ing a continuous stream of letters or block locations for familiar be an alleviation of language processing difculty under con-
instances, and the other requires reasoning to identify the missing ditions that place heavy demands on the EF system in healthy,
element that completes a 4 4 matrix containing orderly patterns and perhaps even in special populations. We focus on a select
across rows and columns; thus, to observe transfer, there must few of these language conditions in the following section, con-
be an underlying process common to both tasks that is enhanced centrating specically on a functional-anatomical association
through intensive n-back training. The authors reasoned that this between conict-resolution processes of EF, and regions within
shared process centered around a common need to employ atten- left VLPFC that support them (for an extensive review, see
tional control, such that their training procedure which forced Novick et al., 2010). We sketch how this association is impor-
trainees to practice constant shifting of attention to new stimuli tant for production and comprehension abilities in healthy
facilitated this ability, thereby enabling transfer to Ravens, which adults, young children, and patients with circumscribed VLPFC
similarly involves updating and selection among multiple repre- damage.
sentations (via the control of attention). Importantly, because the
training and transfer measures were characteristically so different,
the authors argued that task-specic elements could not explain THE ROLE OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTION IN LANGUAGE USE:
the observed generalization, effectively ruling out near-transfer as HYPOTHESES AND IMPLICATIONS FOR TRAINING
an explanation for their ndings. Rather, training boosted a part of One priority in psycholinguistics has been to study how non-
the EF system here, multiple-task management and attentional linguistic cognitive abilities contribute to language production
control processes important for a range of cognitive tasks, includ- and comprehension. EF abilities have emerged as a candidate
ing Ravens performance. Indeed, separate work demonstrates that characteristic, dening in part those individuals who can better
n-back and Ravens activate a similar network of neural regions, coordinate rapidly among multiple sources of linguistic (syntactic,
providing additional support for resources common to both tasks semantic) and extra-linguistic (pragmatic, contextual) evidence
(Burgess et al., 2011). across a range of communicative tasks. Given the breadth of
Additional evidence of process-specic training comes from work on various EFs for language, we focus only on the role of
demonstrations of selective far-transfer from an updating task (let- conict-resolution training for a handful of language tasks. As
ter running-span) to a structurally different assessment measure sketched in the introduction, conict resolution refers to the re-
(number n-back) that requires a similar updating EF; critically characterization of information in the face of competing sources of
though, such transfer was not demonstrated on the Stroop task, evidence. Regarding language processing, good conict-resolution
which relies on a separable EF conict resolution (Dahlin et al., skills enable readers and listeners to avoid comprehension errors
2008). During the letter running-span task, participants must in the face of ambiguity (e.g., by consulting top-down evidence to
recall the last four items of a study list that terminates unexpect- override misinterpretations), produce the right word among com-
edly, forcing them to continuously update the correct response peting options, and take an interlocutors perspective when assess-
set from a eeting memory store; similarly, their version of n- ing common-ground information during natural, unscripted dia-
back required subjects to monitor and refresh representations as log (see Novick et al., 2005, 2010). Indeed, patients with circum-
new information is processed and deemed relevant. Running-span scribed damage to left posterior VLPFC consistently underperform
and a standard number n-back task recruit similar striatal regions, on high-conict conditions on non-linguistic tasks such as Stroop
corroborating their underlying reliance on a common EF. Con- and the recent-no task described above (Hamilton and Mar-
trastingly, tasks requiring conict resolution, like Stroop, require tin, 2005). Moreover, this general conict-resolution disorder in
subjects to re-characterize an automatized response (reading) in patients has been tied to their concomitant decits on language
order to promote atypical, but task-relevant information (color tasks that generate similar conict-resolution demands, for exam-
name); such tasks rely on a separable neural prole (compared to ple, when dominant meanings of lexical ambiguities must be
that required for updating tasks) including a network of frontal countermanded (Bedny et al., 2007), when initial interpretations
and parietal regions. Dahlin et al. (2008) demonstrated that train- of syntactic ambiguities must be reprocessed (Novick et al., 2005,
ing on running-span confers benets to assessment measures that 2009), or when object names must be selected among categorical
share updating demands and corresponding neurological proles competitors (Schnur et al., 2009). As such, by training general-
(n-back), while those with little or no such overlap (Stroop) show purpose conict-resolution abilities supported by regions within
negligible improvement. In sum, the amount of far-transfer to VLPFC in healthy adults, we hypothesize that there should be
untrained tasks following intervention depends on the degree systematic improvements in high-conict conditions on language
of overlap among cognitive and neural resources shared by the tasks requiring shared demands for conict resolution. Below, we
training and the transfer tasks. provide examples of when conict-resolution abilities appear to

www.frontiersin.org May 2012 | Volume 3 | Article 158 | 5


Hussey and Novick Cognitive training for language use

interact with particular language processing skills and outline the below) would endow trainees with improved abilities essential to
implications these associations have for process-specic training. re-interpreting garden-path sentences. (Performance adaptation
means that as subjects reached a certain criterion, task difculty
SYNTACTIC AMBIGUITY RESOLUTION increased dynamically in terms of n and the number of lures
Theory present.) Similar to the processing demands of the recent-no task,
Readers and listeners process sentences in real-time, commit- our training task required participants to re-characterize stimu-
ting to an interpretation incrementally as words and phrases are lus representations in real-time. Specically, subjects completed
encountered moment-by-moment (Altmann and Kamide, 1999; a version of n-back during training that contained interference
Tanenhaus, 2007). One consequence of incremental processing is lures, or items that match in target-identity but appeared in non-
temporary ambiguity: the rst analysis individuals assign some- n-positions. For example, in the sequence G-P-K-G, the second G
times turns out wrong. Cognitive control has been tied to individ- would be a target in a 3-back condition because it matches the 3-
uals ability to adjust interpretations when late-arriving evidence back stimulus. However, in the sequence G-P-K-L-G, the second G
signals that their initial analysis was incorrect (Novick et al., 2005). would be alurein a 3-back condition because it matches the stim-
Such cases of conict (the so-called garden-path effect) elicit ulus presented four, not three, items back (Gray et al., 2003; Burgess
temporary processing difculty in reading (Frazier and Rayner, et al., 2011). We argued (as have others) that the familiarity of lure
1982; Staub and Rayner, 2007; inter alia) and confusion during items forces participants to engage conict-resolution functions
spoken comprehension (Tanenhaus et al., 1995). Individuals must to override a familiarity bias and the tendency to respond target
then engage in a process that permits them to revise and capture to familiar representations; instead, subjects must re-characterize
the intended interpretation. familiar letters in non-n locations as non-targets (thus lures are
Evidence for the role of conict-resolution in this recovery akin to recent-no trials in the item-recognition task). Impor-
process comes from populations with underdeveloped or impaired tantly, neuroimaging work (Gray et al., 2003) demonstrates that
cognitive control such as young children (whose PFC development lure trials activate VLPFC resources that are also recruited during
is protracted; see Huttenlocher and Dabholkar, 1997) and patients high-conict language processing tasks. This nding suggests that
with focal damage to left posterior VLPFC. Both populations fail to practicing an n-back task with lures may lead to improvements not
initiate cognitive-control functions across assorted non-syntactic just on that task, but also in resolving competing interpretations
measures (e.g., Stroop, the recent-no, and other analogous tasks; of syntactically ambiguous sentences.
e.g., Hamilton and Martin, 2005; Khanna and Boland, 2010), and To examine process-specic training-related changes in sen-
both groups similarly fail to revise sentence interpretations fol- tence processing, readers eye movements were recorded; we were
lowing early misanalysis (Trueswell et al., 1999; Weighall, 2008; primarily interested in the effect of training on processing dif-
Novick et al., 2009; see also Christianson et al., 2006 for similar pat- culty, particularly in sentence regions that introduced new evi-
terns in older adults). The linking assumption is that the discovery dence signaling an incompatibility with individuals early interpre-
of a misinterpretation deploys conict-resolution to resolve the tations (i.e., disambiguating regions that induce conict). Readers
incompatibility between representations of sentence meaning: the also answered comprehension questions, the responses to which
one initially assigned and the one in need of recovery, similar to the indexed a failure to ultimately override their original misanalysis
controlled processes required to resolve conict during incongru- (Christianson et al., 2006). We found three important patterns:
ent Stroop trials, or interference from familiar but currently irrel- (1) those trainees who responded most to n-back practice
evant items in the recent-no task (Hamilton and Martin, 2005; reected in steady performance gains throughout the regimen
Novick et al., 2005, 2010). Interestingly, healthy adults undergoing demonstrated signicantly improved comprehension accuracy at
functional neuroimaging demonstrate co-localized neural activ- posttest for ambiguous (but not unambiguous) materials, whereas
ity within left posterior VLPFC when performing both syntactic the untrained controls and non-responsive trainees did not; (2)
and non-syntactic tasks requiring conict resolution, corroborat- responsive trainees reading times were reliably faster at posttest,
ing the necessary involvement of shared, domain-general processes acutely in disambiguating regions of ambiguous sentences, but not
presumed from special populations (January et al., 2009; Ye and in other regions, reecting less processing difculty post-training
Zhou, 2009). upon encountering conicting evidence the control group
and non-responders demonstrated no test-retest change; and (3)
Hypothesis trainees performance improvement on n-back-with-lures and
This convergence of ndings suggests an opportunity to allevi- no other training task administered as controls predicted the
ate the processing difculty associated with temporary ambigu- increases they achieved in garden-path recovery.
ities that arise during sentence processing by targeting the EFs The selectivity of these ndings is of particular interest, because
(through training) that appear to be domain-general, i.e., com- trainees exhibited improvements only on the language materi-
mon across certain syntactic and non-syntactic tasks. We tested als where conict-resolution processes are hypothesized to trig-
this hypothesis in a study in which healthy trainees completed ger (unambiguous materials did not involve the need to employ
pre/post reading assessments involving syntactically ambiguous control to revise interpretations, and no test-retest changes in
sentences susceptible to misanalysis (Hussey et al., 2010; Novick accuracy or reading times were found in this condition). Fur-
et al., submitted for publication). We hypothesized that practic- ther, these pre/post improvements were accounted for only by
ing a performance-adaptive non-linguistic task requiring conict- individual training gains on the n-back-with-lures task i.e., a
resolution processes the n-back memory task with lures (see task requiring conict resolution and no other well-practiced

Frontiers in Psychology | Cognition May 2012 | Volume 3 | Article 158 | 6


Hussey and Novick Cognitive training for language use

WM task completed during intervention (participants also trained Hypothesis


on tasks tapping visuo-spatial and verbal WM functions with- Considering the training results observed for syntactic ambiguity
out conict-resolution demands). Importantly, many researchers resolution and therefore assuming that conict resolution is yet
argue (DEsposito and Postle, 1999; Kane and Engle, 2000) that another trainable EF in addition to updating and task-switching
there are some tasks of WM that tap non-mnemonic functions, lexical ambiguity resolution abilities may also be enhanced, hypo-
such as the need to resolve conicting representations, which is a thetically, through conict-resolution training tasks designed to
general-purpose skill necessary for some (not all) WM tasks and target EFs central to overriding dominant biases and implement-
some language tasks like syntactic ambiguity resolution (Novick ing cognitive control (provided the effects are large enough to
et al., 2005). observe improvement; this may be particularly true in clinical
Overall, the patterns are consistent with the idea that the abil- patients). Future research might test whether EF training, with the
ity to recover from misinterpretation can be enhanced by training right tasks, could garner improvements in integration among top-
domain-general EFs common to some tasks of language process- down contextual and lexical sources of evidence, particularly when
ing and some tasks of WM. These ndings indicate that within these latter sources give rise to multiple conicting meanings.
the right framework, and having appropriate linking hypotheses, There are obvious implications for clinical patients with word-
EF training may be a viable way to improve language use under comprehension decits stemming from poor conict-resolution
certain conditions through tests of far-transfer. Open questions abilities.
remain about the trainability of special populations particu-
larly if training VLPFC patients and young children with poor REFERENCE RESOLUTION
conict-resolution skills will result in improved cognitive control, Theory
extending to an enhanced ability to recover from parsing mis- When conversational participants interact, they establish what is
analyses. But the opportunity to test such ideas is ripe. To our known as common ground, or shared beliefs. Brown-Schmidt
knowledge, this study is the rst to investigate the impact of EF (2009) has demonstrated that variations in cognitive-control abil-
training on the processes that commonly contribute to language ities can explain healthy individuals occasional inattentiveness
comprehension. As sketched below, conict-resolution abilities are to common-ground information; that is, objects visually acces-
associated with various other specic language processing tasks, sible only to the listener are occasionally (incorrectly) favored
leaving room to explore the effects of training on language use as a referential interpretation over objects accessible to both
more generally. partners. Specically, individual differences in conict resolution
may determine if a listener can successfully override perspective-
LEXICAL AMBIGUITY RESOLUTION inappropriate interpretations of referential ambiguities uttered by
Theory their partner. As such, conict resolution may predict how easily
Research examining comprehension at the single-word level sug- semantic and pragmatic information is integrated in order to rule
gests a role for conict resolution when the dominant meaning out incorrect interpretations during natural dialogue.
of an ambiguous word (e.g., bill, as the tab issued by a restau- Indeed, a study testing young children corroborates this
rant) must be overridden to retrieve its subordinate meaning account by showing that although 5-year-olds can distinguish
(an outline of a prospective law; Bedny et al., 2007). Questions common versus privileged knowledge during conversation, the
posed in this literature examine whether good conict-resolution preference for their own perspectives assessed by gaze duration
skills enable context-dependent meaning selection, and conversely, to inappropriate privileged-ground alternatives is predicted by
whether poor abilities impair it. Researchers have found that better measures of conict resolution and inhibitory control (Stroop,
conict resolution is related to young childrens contextual sensi- a tapping task, and the bear/dragon puppet task), all of which
tivity: context can be used by kids to countermand dominant, require resolving among conicting representations by overriding
but inappropriate meanings of an ambiguous word; however, the a dominant rule/bias (Nilsen and Graham, 2009). That is, children
use of top-down information is largely dependent on the matu- with poorer cognitive-control demonstrated exaggerated looking
rity of their EF abilities, as indexed by a separate task of conict times to high-conict referential alternatives inaccessible to the
resolution and inhibitory control (Khanna and Boland, 2010). speaker but hidden (or privileged) so that only the listener (the
Correspondingly, neuropsychological patients with poor conict child) can see them (e.g., a small duck when Look at the duck is
resolution show inadequate lexical ambiguity resolution when the uttered and competes with the target that is common knowledge,
subordinate meaning is activated by local contextual information i.e., a large duck). Namely, children with better performance on
(Balota and Faust, 2001; Bedny et al., 2007), suggesting that such high-conict conditions of an inhibitory control task were more
patients have difculty suppressing context-inappropriate mean- likely to override their egocentric view and modify their behavior
ings of ambiguous words (Copland et al., 2009; Vuong and Martin, to be consistent with information shared by both communicative
2011). Finally, across several studies, regions within VLPFC the parties, and did so selectively for high-conict items evidenced
same areas involved in lesion-decit analyses of patients show- by spending less time gazing at inappropriate privileged-ground
ing conict-resolution impairments are active in healthy adults alternatives.
during lexical-decision tasks necessitating resolution of mean- Adults occasionally show similar consideration of perspective-
ing competition, suggesting that VLPFC-mediated EFs trigger to inappropriate interpretations when a speaker utters a referential
resolve increased competition associated with accessing the less ambiguity, failing to be sensitive to common-ground informa-
frequent meaning of an ambiguous word (Bilenko et al., 2009). tion immediately. This behavior is also related to individual

www.frontiersin.org May 2012 | Volume 3 | Article 158 | 7


Hussey and Novick Cognitive training for language use

variation in conict-resolution abilities. For instance, during one training individuals receiving false-belief training improved on
visual-world task (Brown-Schmidt, 2009), participants assisted card-sorting, and those trained on card-sorting showed benets on
the experimenter in revealing the identity of subject-privileged the Sally-Ann task suggesting the presence of a shared object re-
pictures on a display by answering the experimenters questions. description process. Note that a similar card-sorting task resulted
Generally, addressees consulted common-ground information to in transfer totask-switchingmeasures in a report of near-transfer
resolve temporarily ambiguous requests, like, Whats above the highlighted earlier (Karbach and Kray, 2009). Both sets of results
horse with the glasses?, when two horses might be referenced, one point to the malleability of EFs important for perspective tak-
wearing glasses and another wearing shoes. If the item above one ing, namely, object re-description (given by the Kloo and Perner
of the horses (the horse with shoes) was previously grounded, ndings) and task-switching (consistent with Karbach and Krays
then subjects directed their gaze toward the unmentioned tar- work). To this end, task-switching ability is apt to overlap with
get and the horse (with glasses) located below it, as the ambiguity conict resolution (object re-description), as switching between
unfolded. Crucially, however, the degree to which an addressee was multiple rules involves overriding old features and rules in favor of
able to use perspective information to avoid considering inappro- newly relevant ones, a type of information re-characterization that
priate interpretations (i.e., understanding the question to mean is a hallmark of conict resolution. A carefully designed training
the already-revealed object) was determined by his Stroop perfor- regimen for example, by comparing task-switching training with
mance. That is, subjects with better conict-control were quicker conict-resolution training may illuminate the overlapping con-
to resolve referential conict by directing their attention away from tributions of each EF for each false-belief and perspective-taking
grounded items and toward previously unmentioned items. tasks similar to those outlined above.
Although conict-resolution measures account for the individ-
ual differences in perspective-taking ability in children and adults, VERBAL FLUENCY
common-ground assessment likely requires multiple different Theory
kinds of EF (e.g., memory for perspective). However, it is impor- During language production, the ease with which a lexical item
tant to note that the only experimental conditions predicted by is generated depends partly on the degree of competition from
Stroop performance are those that impose high conict-resolution other candidate words. Competition demands are particularly
demands. high when multiple semantically related words are equally plau-
sible contenders for selection (a classic case of underdetermined
Hypothesis representational conict; see above discussion). Items with high
This raises the question: if relevant EF skills can be targeted versus low name-agreement, for instance, present different levels of
and enhanced via conict-resolution training (for instance, using conict during naming tasks, such that low name-agreement items
a training-appropriate version of the Stroop task as in Brown- associated with many alternative labels (e.g., couch/sofa/loveseat)
Schmidt, 2009), would individuals (particularly children) subse- elicit more competition, reected by longer naming latencies, thus
quently be less likely to consider unintended interpretations in requiring the use of VLPFC-mediated conict resolution to select
cases of referential ambiguity? That is, one might hypothesize that among the competing alternatives (Kan and Thompson-Schill,
EF training, within a process-specic conict-resolution frame- 2004; Novick et al., 2009). High name-agreement items (e.g.,
work, will result in a generally sharper ability to promote relevant images that invoke a single label, like apple), by contrast, have fewer
sources of information like context and pragmatics, and sup- alternative labels to choose from, rendering them easier to access
press currently irrelevant ones (e.g., ones privileged perspective) and produce, and thus, less dependent on conict-resolution
through top-down control. processes. Furthermore, selection costs are compounded when
Indeed, there is indirect yet tantalizing support for this. Work by cases of high-competition (low name-agreement) are crossed
Kloo and Perner (2003) provides evidence for far-transfer across with increased retrieval demands (e.g., low association-strength
structurally dissimilar tasks of information re-characterization between a cue and its most accessible response), such that items
within a theory of mind context in young children, who were with multiple weak associates are most difcult to output (Snyder
either assigned to card-sorting training or false-belief (perspec- et al., 2010).
tive taking) training. The card-sorting task involved categorizing This high- vs. low-name-agreement asymmetry has been exam-
cards with two distinct features (e.g., two yellow apples, one green ined in non-uent aphasic patients with VLPFC damage the
apple), with the relevant dimension changing (from number to same patients mentioned above who exhibit generally poor con-
color) after each set of cards was fully sorted. The false-belief ict resolution and cognitive control on a variety of non-linguistic
task required children to answer questions about a conicting conict-resolution tasks like Stroop and the recent-no task. This
situation in which one puppet performed an action on another, population demonstrates exaggerated effects of production dif-
but claimed that it, instead, acted on a different puppet. To culty for high-competition conditions that require the recruitment
assess the training-mediated effects of card-sorting and theory of of conict-resolution resources, such that they take signicantly
mind, two novel assessments were implemented: the card-sorting longer or even fail to produce these items altogether relative to
transfer task included incorporating multiple rules for new cards low-competition items (Novick et al., 2009). Patients with this
(sort by number then color) and sorting an entirely different set neuroanatomical prole have difculty with other verbal uency
of cards on novel dimensions. The false-belief-transfer measure tasks, including completing sentences when the options are open-
was a traditional Sally-Ann task using the same puppets from ended (and therefore ambiguous), vs. when the to-be-completed
training. Reciprocal far-transfer was observed for both types of fragments provide a highly constrained context, yielding little

Frontiers in Psychology | Cognition May 2012 | Volume 3 | Article 158 | 8


Hussey and Novick Cognitive training for language use

competition from possible alternative continuations (Robinson extensive training regimens, the result of which could be attenu-
et al., 1998, 2005). Similarly, healthy speakers take longer to pro- ated processing difculty during language use across a range of
duce the names of pictured objects when they are presented in tasks, as indexed through measures of far-transfer. Such hypothe-
semantically homogeneous (e.g., snake, cow, dog, ant) vs. mixed ses are motivated also by the demonstration of positive transfer
contexts (e.g., snake, bus, axe, chair) due to the increase in lexical- effects in non-linguistic cognitive domains following regimens
semantic competition among semantically related competitors targeting other EFs. This work could be particularly applicable
(Belke et al., 2005). In one study, non-uent aphasics with cir- to patients with lesions restricted to left posterior VLPFC, to
cumscribed VLPFC damage generated more errors when naming determine (a) if their conict-resolution performance changes
objects in homogeneous contexts; a companion neuroimaging on linguistic and non-linguistic tasks post-training, and (b) what
experiment further showed that even healthy adults with a greater new compensatory processes or brain systems they engage to
VLPFC response to naming under homogeneous conditions are support any observed performance increases (evaluated through
prone to more naming errors compared to individuals with less pretest/posttest neuroimaging). There are similar implications for
VLPFC activation (Schnur et al., 2009). young children, whose comprehension might fail for similar rea-
sons as the patients (i.e., decits in cognitive control). Generally,
this research program could suggest new inferences about the
Hypothesis
plasticity of the mind and brain, with respect to language pro-
Careful consideration of the literature suggests that language pro-
cessing especially, and the causal effects of language and cognition
duction under conditions of conict appears to be modulated by
interactions.
general EF abilities, like those governing conict resolution on
Given prior evidence for far-transfer from WM training tasks
Stroop-like tasks. Consequently, training tasks tapping these same
to other measures such as task-switching, updating, and general
underlying neural networks may, hypothetically, be drawn on as
uid intelligence, the major goal that we are outlining, based on
tools to boost word selection abilities under elevated conict-
our theory of the role of left VLPFC and cognitive control in lan-
resolution demands. The idea is that better conict-resolution
guage processing, would be to design training studies in search of
skills acquired through training might generalize to an increased
generalized effects to language measures, in hopes of mitigating
ability to resolve among semantically related lexical items that
difculties under certain production and comprehension condi-
compete for selection, carrying important implications for clini-
tions during everyday language use. Except for a study conducted
cal interventions in populations with decits in verbal uency that
by our group on the effects of conict-resolution training on
accompany a more general decit in conict resolution.
syntactic ambiguity resolution, we are unaware of other research
Furthermore, training may also have consequences for select-
investigating whether broader improvements might be observed
ing among competing alternative names during states of elevated
in language processing assessments in adults, both healthy and
anxiety. Indeed, one study reveals that more anxious individu-
impaired, and even in young children. EF interventions might be
als (evaluated by a composite score of anxious apprehension) are
particularly attractive in clinical arenas as a technique to remedi-
impaired relative to less anxious subjects when they must gener-
ate conict-resolution decits broadly construed, including how
ate an associated verb (in response to a given noun) under high
such impairments affect non-uent production and comprehen-
retrieval demands, an effect mediated by VLPFC (Snyder et al.,
sion difculties under high-EF demands. Considering the patterns
2010). This suggests that EF resources are depleted in cases of
we reviewed suggesting a shared role for domain-general conict-
anxiety (Gray et al., 2002), which can negatively affect word selec-
resolution processes across a variety of language processing tasks, a
tion processes under elevated EF demands (e.g., high-competition
common training regimen targeting this EF could, hypothetically,
items). Future research on conict-resolution training, therefore,
be successful in correcting problems observed in each of these
might also address whether the right interventions can be used to
tasks. Future research should test this, perhaps through various
offset such effects of anxiety and other deleterious affective states
ways to evaluate transfer, including behavioral changes, changes
in both production and comprehension (but see Beilock and Carr,
in brain-activation patterns in regions commonly recruited across
2005).
training and transfer tasks, changes in evoked response potentials
(McLaughlin et al., 2004), changes in neural connectivity (Geva
SUMMARY, CAVEATS, AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS et al., 2011), changes in eye-movement patterns and reading-time
Overall, we reviewed a sample of language tasks that depend latencies, or any combination of these measures.
heavily on posterior regions of left VLPFC, which support conict-
resolution abilities in a variety of populations. Among these CAVEATS
measures there is great overlap in the EF processes involved to There are, however, important caveats to consider. Despite sev-
carry them out successfully, whether it means employing conict- eral instances of successful generalization to unpracticed tasks,
resolution to produce the right word, resolve lexical ambiguities, some reports describe research efforts failing to observe trans-
take a speakers perspective to avoid errors in interpretation despite fer. One explanation for the absence of transfer ndings may be
referential ambiguity, or recover from temporary misanalysis dur- that in at least one study, EF training was implemented casually,
ing sentence parsing. We believe that in view of these convergent rather than consistently enough to actually tax trainees EF abili-
ndings, the theory that conict resolution and cognitive-control ties throughout the regimen (Owen et al., 2010). In this report, not
contributes to language use may lead to the hypothesis that these all individuals in the training group received the same exposure
domain-general conict-control processes could be the target of to training, a dosage-dependent factor known to confer varying

www.frontiersin.org May 2012 | Volume 3 | Article 158 | 9


Hussey and Novick Cognitive training for language use

levels of transfer (Jaeggi et al., 2008). Another reason for failure Finally, research examining healthy adults and patients with
to show transfer involves the use of performance-non-adaptive neurological disorders demonstrates that EF hinges on the involve-
training tasks (regimens that maintain a constant level of dif- ment of a widespread network that comprises both cortical (e.g.,
culty, rather than keeping participants on the threshold of their PFC, cingulate, and parietal) and subcortical (e.g., striatal) regions,
best performance), despite strong evidence favoring such designs clearly not just on prefrontal cortex alone (Corbetta and Shul-
to facilitate transfer effects (Klingberg et al., 2005; Brehmer et al., man, 2002; Cools et al., 2007; Burgess et al., 2011; inter alia).
2011). Clearly more research is needed to determine what charac- This pattern is bolstered by training studies documenting the
terizes an appropriate training regimen, as well as how dependent underlying neural signatures accompanying post-intervention dif-
transfer effects are on the amount of training an individual receives ferences, including increased activation of frontoparietal regions
(Jaeggi et al., 2008, 2011). Finally, studies failing to show transfer (Olesen et al., 2004); greater structural integrity evaluated by
might lack appropriate linking hypotheses between the types of increased ber tracts (white matter) connecting areas adjacent
EF required to perform certain tasks; these must be understood in to intraparietal sulcus (Takeuchi et al., 2010); and an increase
order to design effective training regimens, which will ultimately in the density of cortical dopamine receptors, perhaps linked
inform how future intervention studies are implemented. to changes in striatal structures (McNab et al., 2009). Although
Furthermore, there appear to be important individual differ- behavioral and neuroimaging ndings suggest domain-general
ences in training success (Chein and Morrison, 2010; Jaeggi et al., processes in PFC that underlie cognitive-control functions across
2011), such that only certain individuals achieve performance various conditions (Thompson-Schill et al., 2005), an intricate
increases on the training tasks over time, and thus demonstrate balance exists between PFC and subcortical regions that adjusts
transfer to unpracticed measures shown through improved per- performance over different EFs (Cools et al., 2007). Such a cor-
formance at retest (indeed, we observed this in our own train- tical/subcortical tradeoff should be considered when choosing
ing work). It is unclear if responders and non-responders can training and language-transfer tasks to maximize theoretical and
be categorized simply by baseline EF abilities, and these differ- functional-anatomical overlap, thereby increasing the prospect of
ences are unlikely due to motivational factors alone (Jaeggi et al., transfer yield.
2011; Novick et al., submitted for publication). So, future research
should address who is most likely to benet from training, how to FUTURE DIRECTIONS
identify these individuals, and how training protocols should be As we have highlighted, transfer might be expected only if the
modied or tailored to maximize transfer across a range of groups EFs (e.g., conict resolution) underlying certain language tasks are
and populations (see Shipstead et al., 2012). targeted through training so as to affect shared processes that facil-
Another remaining question concerns the lasting effects of itate performance on particular language tasks (i.e., WM training
training. Presumably, like physical tness conditioning, the bene- tasks not involving conict-resolution are not expected to confer
ts of cognitive training do not persist indelibly without continued transfer). Future work might continue to identify these functional-
practice, though some have demonstrated maintained benets anatomical overlaps across different memory and language tasks.
three to six months after training ceased (Holmes et al., 2009; We believe however that there has been sufcient data accumulated
Jaeggi et al., 2011; Klingberg et al., 2005). Future work should to suggest a good candidate regimen targeting VLPFC-mediated
address the long-term effects of cognitive conditioning, including conict-resolution processes, which could affect certain language
the advantages of giving a periodic booster training session to processing skills.
reinstate the benets after a regimen completion. It is important to note that although we chose to focus on
We included young children in our brief review of the role of conict-resolution functions given the extant data, this does not
conict resolution in language use to illustrate a population whose preclude the involvement of other EFs in the abovementioned lan-
poor EF abilities yield certain language-performance failures. guage tasks (Miyake et al., 2000). To this end, the lack of mutual
However, research on training this population might proceed cau- exclusivity of certain general cognitive processes should be con-
tiously, particularly concerning language outcomes. The reason is sidered when interpreting transfer effects within a process-specic
that the protracted development of frontal cortex although asso- framework, as multiple EFs might be confounded within a sin-
ciated with suboptimal performance on cognitive-control (and gle training task; thus, changes in several EFs may be responsible
relevant language) tasks might actually confer certain advantages for resultant improvements in outcome measures, a positive out-
throughout development that overshadow the drawbacks. For come if the goal is to show widespread transfer (e.g., Morrison
instance, delayed PFC development and by extension, delayed and Chein, 2011; Shipstead et al., 2012). Likewise, in the examples
EF abilities may bestow a benet to certain aspects of cognitive cited above, the magnitude of the hypothesized transfer effects
development such as language acquisition (as opposed to language will likely be sensitive to the level of conict-resolution required
performance) and creativity (Thompson-Schill et al., 2009). There for each task. The amount of transfer will hinge on the degree
may be a complex tradeoff between bottom-up (data-driven) and to which underlying EFs are shared between training and assess-
top-down (rule-based) thinking in young children that may pro- ment tasks, and this mechanistic overlap is probably inuenced
mote learning and social development. Therefore, if EF training is by both the relative involvement of a single trained EF and the
aimed at enhancing cognitive-control abilities, such interventions extent to which other EFs are recruited in the training and out-
might have negative consequences, at least temporarily, for this come tasks. For example, re-characterization of representations on
population. Future work should address this concern, in addition the high-conict lure trials of the n-back task likely requires other
to the long-term effects of training. EFs beyond just conict resolution (e.g., monitoring, updating).

Frontiers in Psychology | Cognition May 2012 | Volume 3 | Article 158 | 10


Hussey and Novick Cognitive training for language use

Similarly, syntactic ambiguity resolution will, of course, rely on that the trained EFs do not signicantly contribute to the process-
updating processes in addition to conict resolution. Methodolog- ing of the particular language condition. This type of approach
ically confounding EFs is an issue that plagues training studies, provides a powerful tool for choosing among several explana-
rendering it difcult to extricate distinct mechanisms entirely; tions for the same data set, where the best account of the data
however, by having linking hypotheses, EF overlap across tasks can be gleaned from the results of a well-designed training study
maximizes chances of successful transfer. Careful design of train- that poses process-specic linking hypotheses. For example, some
ing regimens, including tasks performed by comparison groups argue that the difculty experienced while comprehending the
for instance by maintaining minimal task differences between meaning of abstract (compared to concrete) words hinges almost
training and active-control tasks (e.g., a group completing the entirely on domain-general processes (Hoffman et al., 2010), while
n-back task without the lure component) can help elucidate the other accounts posit little to no contribution from EFs (Barsa-
contribution of distinct EFs. lou and Wiemer-Hastings, 2005; Rodrguez-Ferreiro et al., 2011).
Correspondingly, the transfer conditions under which selec- The opportunity exists, then, to investigate whether successful EF
tive improvement is observed within an assessment task may training permits better abstract-meaning selection.
mark those relying most on the trained EF. To maximize trans- Finally, it is important to consider a growing body of research
fer, it is important to pinpoint the measures in the assessments demonstrating that balanced bilinguals enjoy certain cognitive
that capture cognitive processes of interest. For instance, in our advantages relative to their monolingual peers, as this work has
training experiment, we argued that the strongest indices of re- important implications for language education and intervention.
interpretation ability and real-time reanalysis respectively were On tasks requiring cognitive control, some ndings suggest that
accuracy to comprehension questions gaging lingering effects of bilinguals outperform monolinguals selectively on trials inducing
misinterpretation and regression-path reading time in disam- conict across a range of tasks such as the Simon task (Bia-
biguating sentence regions. Likewise, decreased gaze duration to lystok et al., 2004). Other data patterns reveal a broader effect,
privileged items in a common-ground assessment task, for exam- namely that bilinguals are better at conict monitoring: they per-
ple, probably involves information re-characterization, rendering form faster on both conict and non-conict trials under high,
this a candidate measure to observe conict-resolution training- but not low, conict-monitoring conditions, in which subjects
related changes. The ability to make specic predictions for when cannot predict when a conict-related item type (an incon-
and where transfer is selectively expected, as well as the condi- gruent anker trial) might occur because their appearance is
tions under which it is not, will ultimately lend important insight equally probable relative to non-conict trials (Costa et al., 2009).
to the EFs affected during successful intervention when transfer Regardless of the specics, it has become increasingly clear that
effects are observed in studies carried out under proper linking rich linguistic experience (akin to the rich cognitive experi-
assumptions and within a theoretically guided process-specic ence achieved through training) benets conict-resolution and
account. cognitive-control performance widely, perhaps due to bilinguals
Also worth mentioning is the contribution of several consistent switching across the two language systems they know
perhaps even overlapping domain-general resources that and/or their frequent suppression of one lexicon/grammar over
may be recruited during language tasks not discussed here another, thus placing a premium on EFs associated with updat-
(e.g., mnemonic aspects of WM, maintenance, updating, task- ing, conict resolution, and set-shifting (Martin-Rhee and Bia-
switching, etc). This should be carefully considered upon design- lystok, 2008; Costa et al., 2009). In other words, lifelong bilin-
ing outcome language assessments that will be the target of trans- gualism may be a naturalistic form of cognitive-control training.
fer benets. In fact, we strongly believe that verbal WM span Indeed, future work should attempt to disentangle the various
processes, which involve maintenance, processing, and temporary processing demands that are associated with being a bilingual
storage components, must play a role in spoken language com- speaker (e.g., frequent code switches) that might yield the putative
prehension tasks in which the listener cannot review the input cognitive-control advantage they show; such an understanding
(as she can in normal reading) once it is spoken, without using might help extract the various EFs, in addition to conict res-
mnemonic rehearsal strategies. This is likely true regardless of the olution, that are at the heart of bilinguals benet. It will also
presence of ambiguity or conicting representations, and, indeed, be benecial to know how bilinguals cognitive-control advan-
verbal WM by itself has been shown to play a role in reading studies tage concerning conict resolution or conict monitoring inu-
using a moving-window paradigm that does not permit rereading ences this groups linguistic abilities on the conict-related lan-
(Fedorenko et al., 2006). Thus, in future work it will be important guage tasks reviewed in this paper. For instance, does bilinguals
to design training protocols using tasks that maximize a theoret- cognitive-control advantage result in a better ability to recover
ical match between the cognitive (and neural) processes involved the correct interpretation of garden-path sentences, following a
in assessment and training measures, including WM tasks that do misanalysis? The answer to this question could suggest impor-
not necessarily involve the conict-resolution aspect of cognitive tant inferences one could draw about the prospective impact that
control, when appropriate. process-specic conict-resolution training might have on this
Cognitive training may also provide a novel approach to under- group.
standing whether EFs are critical for a multitude of language uses. Recent ndings suggest that bilingualism confers protective
The degree to which training improvement predicts changes in benets against cognitive decline: bilingual patients diagnosed
language processing can reveal the EFs involved in each condition; with Alzheimers disease (AD), who are matched on a range
if no transfer is observed in selective cases, one might conclude of factors (e.g., degree of cognitive impairment, symptomatic

www.frontiersin.org May 2012 | Volume 3 | Article 158 | 11


Hussey and Novick Cognitive training for language use

expression, demographic variables) to monolinguals with the Conversely, if a low-prociency group that rarely switches between
same diagnosis, have signicantly more brain atrophy in areas linguistic systems does not demonstrate a cognitive-control advan-
commonly examined to differentiate AD patients from healthy tage compared to monolinguals, this would suggest opportunity
adults (Schweizer et al., 2011). The implication is that bilinguals for EF training to bestow benets. If neither high- nor low-
may have greater cognitive reserve than would be predicted prociency groups demonstrates a cognitive-control advantage,
given the amount of neuropathology they exhibit; that is, the then perhaps learning a second language in adulthood does not
cognitive symptoms associated with AD may be delayed in this enhance EF abilities similar to how early acquisition of two lin-
population because of their premorbid advantage. What about guistic systems does. EF training could therefore be benecial to
bilingual children and VLPFC patients? Are they inoculated unbalanced groups across a range of prociency levels. Ultimately,
from the cognitive-control decits they are otherwise known for future work in this area will clarify our understanding of the inter-
(in monolinguals) in terms of their non-linguistic and language play between bilingualism, cognitive control, and the effects of
processing abilities under high-conict demands? If so, what training on language and other tasks that share cognitive processes.
behavioral mechanisms and neural systems do they recruit to
compensate?
CLOSING REMARKS
Furthermore, will cognitive-control training over the long-term
EF training holds promise to result in gains in cognition and
yield similar protective benets in monolinguals? Will their perfor-
language use in both production and comprehension domains,
mance begin to approach that of (untrained) bilinguals? Will EF
easing processing difculty when multiple active and equally
training confer comparable protection against normal age-related
compelling representations are at odds (underdetermined repre-
cognitive decline (Richmond et al., 2011), regardless of AD? These
sentational conict), or when dominant biases must be reined-in
are open empirical questions and might be the focus of future
(prepotent conict). Such interventions could potentially mitigate
longitudinal research. Also: to what extent does prociency level
problems in language use under generally high conict demands,
matter in adults who have learned a second language, regarding
not just in special populations (e.g., non-uent aphasics with
the cognitive-control benets they reap and the implications for
conict-resolution decits), but also in healthy individuals, includ-
intervention? Balanced bilinguals, as sketched above, enjoy cer-
ing developing children, who experience occasional difculty in
tain advantages; presumably highly procient (but unbalanced)
reading, listening, or speaking due to heightened demands for cog-
bilinguals and those with lower prociency levels will pattern
nitive control (in some cases perhaps due to resource depletion).
somewhere in between the balanced group and the monolinguals
Such research, provided reliable demonstrations of far-transfer,
regarding cognitive-control performance, depending on the rela-
would add insight to our current understanding of how broad,
tive processing demands associated with their prociency levels.
non-linguistic cognitive abilities contribute to language use.
Where such bilinguals pattern can provide useful insight into the
design of future training studies to bring these groups perfor-
mance ranges closer to approximate the balanced population. How ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
much room is there for balanced bilinguals to gain from EF train- This research was funded by the University of Maryland Center
ing? If a highly procient group shows a similar cognitive-control for Advanced Study of Language and an NSF IGERT Grant on
advantage to that of bilinguals, then it may suggest the prospect Biological and Computational Foundations of Language Diversity
of similar benets (in terms of effect sizes) gained from training. (grant DGE-001465).

REFERENCES Bedny, M., Hulbert, J. C., and (2009). Neural correlates of seman- underlie the relationship between
Altmann, G. T., and Kamide, Y. (1999). Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2007). tic competition during processing of uid intelligence and working mem-
Incremental interpretation at verbs: Understanding words in context: ambiguous words. J. Cogn. Neurosci. ory span. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 140,
restricting the domain of subsequent the role of Brocas area in word 21, 960975. 674692.
reference. Cognition 73, 247264. comprehension. Brain Res. 1146, Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, Chein, J. M., and Morrison, A. B.
Badre, D., and Wagner, A. D. (2007). Left 101114. D. M., Carter, C. S., and Cohen, J. (2010). Expanding the minds work-
ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and Beilock, S. L., and Carr, T. H. (2005). D. (2001). Conict monitoring and space: training and transfer effects
the cognitive control of memory. When high-powered people fail: cognitive control. Psychol. Rev. 108, with a complex working memory
Neuropsychologia 45, 28832901. working memory and Choking 624652. span task. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 17,
Balota, D. A., and Faust, M. E. Under Pressure in math. Psychol. Brehmer, Y., Rieckmann, A., Bel- 193199.
(2001). Attention in dementia of Sci. 16, 101105. lander, M., Westerberg, H., Fis- Christianson, K., Williams, C. C., Zacks,
the Alzheimers type, in Handbook Belke, E., Meyer, A., and Damian, M. cher, H., and Bckman, L. (2011). R. T., and Ferreira, F. (2006).
of Neuropsychology, eds F. Boller, F. (2005). Refractory effects in pic- Neural correlates of training-related Younger and older adults Good-
and S. F. Cappa (New York: Elsevier ture naming as assessed in a seman- working-memory gains in old age. enough interpretations of garden-
Science), 5180. tic blocking paradigm. Q. J. Exp. Neuroimage 58, 11101120. path sentences. Discourse Process. 42,
Barsalou, L. W., and Wiemer-Hastings, Psychol. 58, 667692. Brown-Schmidt, S. (2009). The role 205238.
K. (2005). Situating abstract con- Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., of executive function in perspective Cools, R., Sheridan, M., Jacobs, E. J., and
cepts, in Grounding Cognition: The and Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilin- taking during online language com- DEsposito, M. D. (2007). Impul-
Role of Perception and Action in gualism, aging, and cognitive con- prehension. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 16, sive personality predicts dopamine-
Memory, Language, and Thought, trol: evidence from the Simon task. 893900. dependent changes in fronto-striatal
eds D. Pecher, and R. Zwaan (New Psychol. Aging 19, 290303. Burgess, G. C., Gray, J. R., Conway, A. activity during component processes
York: Cambridge University Press), Bilenko, N. Y., Grindrod, C. M., R., and Braver, T. S. (2011). Neural of working memory. J. Neurosci. 27,
129163. Myers, E. B., and Blumstein, S. E. mechanisms of interference control 55065514.

Frontiers in Psychology | Cognition May 2012 | Volume 3 | Article 158 | 12


Hussey and Novick Cognitive training for language use

Copland, D. A., Sefe, G., Ashley, J., prefrontal cortex plays an executive Klingberg, T., Fernell, E., Olesen, P. work? The promise and challenges
Hudson, C., and Chenery, H. J. regulation role in comprehension J., Johnson, M., Gustafsson, P., of enhancing cognition by training
(2009). Impaired semantic inhibi- of abstract words: convergent neu- Dahlstrm, K., Gillberg, C., Forss- working memory. Psychon. Bull. Rev.
tion during lexical ambiguity repe- ropsychological and rTMS evidence. berg, H., and Westerberg, H. (2005). 18, 4660.
tition in Parkinsons disease. Cortex J. Neurosci. 30, 1545015456. Computerized training of working Nelson, J. K., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A.,
45, 943949. Holmes, J., Gathercole, S. E., and Dun- memory in children with ADHD a Sylvester, C. C., Jonides, J., and
Corbetta, M., and Shulman, G. L. ning, D. L. (2009). Adaptive training randomized, controlled trial. J. Am. Smith, E. E. (2003). Dissocia-
(2002). Control of goal-directed leads to sustained enhancement of Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 44, ble neural mechanisms underly-
and stimulus-driven attention in poor working memory in children. 177186. ing response-based and familiarity-
the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, Dev. Sci. 12, F9F15. Klingberg, T., Forssberg, H., and West- based conict in working memory.
201215. Hussey, E., Teubner-Rhodes, S., erberg, H. (2002). Increased brain Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100,
Costa, A., Hernndez, M., Costa- Dougherty, M., Bunting, M., and activity in frontal and parietal cortex 1117111175
Faidella, J., and Sebastin-Galls, N. Novick, J. (2010). Improving underlies the development of visu- Nilsen, E., and Graham, S. (2009). The
(2009). On the bilingual advantage garden-path recovery in healthy ospatial working memory capacity relations between childrens com-
in conict processing: Now you see adults through cognitive control during childhood. J. Cogn. Neurosci. municative perspective-taking and
it, now you dont. Cognition 113, training. Talk Presented at the 16th 14, 110. executive functioning. Cogn. Psychol.
135149. Annual Conference on Architectures Kloo, D., and Perner, J. (2003). Train- 58, 220249.
Dahlin, E., Neely, A. S., Larsson, A., and Mechanisms for Language ing transfer between card sort- Norman, W., and Shallice, T. (1986).
Bckman, L., and Nyberg, L. (2008). Processing, York, UK. ing and false belief understand- Attention to action: willed and
Transfer of learning after updating Huttenlocher, P. R., and Dabholkar, ing: Helping children apply con- automatic control of behavior, in
training mediated by the striatum. A. S. (1997). Regional differences icting descriptions. Child Dev. 74, Consciousness and Self Regulation:
Science 320, 15101512. in synaptogenesis in human cere- 18231839. Advances in Research and Theory,
DEsposito, M., and Postle, B. R. bral cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 387, Li, S., Schmiedek, F., Huxhold, O., Vol. 4, eds R. J. Davidson, R.
(1999). The dependence of span 167178. Rcke, C., Smith, J., and Linden- Schwartz, and D. Shapiro (New York:
and delayed-response performance Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., berger, U. (2008). Working memory Plenum), 118.
on prefrontal cortex. Neuropsycholo- and Perrig, W. J. (2008). Improving plasticity in old age: Practice gain, Novick, J. M., Kan, I. P., Trueswell, J. C.,
gia 37, 13031315. uid intelligence with training on transfer, and maintenance. Psychol. and Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2009).
Fedorenko, E., Gibson, E., and Rohde, working memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Aging 23, 731742. A case for conict across multi-
D. (2006). The nature of working Sci. U.S.A. 105, 68296833. Martin-Rhee, M. M., and Bialystok, E. ple domains: Memory and language
memory capacity in sentence pro- Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, (2008). The development of two impairments follow damage to ven-
cessing: evidence against domain- J., and Shah, P. (2011). Short and types of inhibitory control in mono- trolateral prefrontal cortex. Cogn.
specic working memory resources. long term benets of cognitive train- lingual and bilingual children. Bil- Neuropsychol. 26, 527567.
J. Mem. Lang. 54, 541553. ing. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, ing. Lang. Cogn. 11, 8193. Novick, J. M., Trueswell, J. C., and
Feuerstein, R. (1980). Cognitive mod- 1008110086. McLaughlin, J., Osterhout, L., and Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2005).
iability in adolescence: cognitive January, D., Trueswell, J. C., and Kim, A. (2004). Neural correlates Cognitive control and parsing:
structure and the effects of interven- Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2009). Co- of second-language word learning: reexamining the role of Brocas
tion. J. Spec. Educ. 15, 269287. localization of Stroop and syntac- minimal instruction produces rapid area in sentence comprehension.
Frazier, L., and Rayner, K. (1982). Mak- tic ambiguity resolution in Brocas change. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 703704. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 5,
ing and correcting errors during Area: Implications for the neural McNab, F.,Varrone, A., Farde, L., Jucaite, 263281.
sentence comprehension: eye move- basis of sentence processing. J. Cogn. A., Bystritsky, P., Forssberg, H., and Novick, J. M., Trueswell, J. C., and
ments in the analysis of structurally Neurosci. 21, 24342444. Klingberg, T. (2009). Changes in cor- Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2010).
ambiguous sentences. Cogn. Psychol. Jonides, J. (2004). How does prac- tical dopamine D1 receptor binding Brocas area and language pro-
14, 178210. tice make perfect? Nat. Neurosci. 7, associated with cognitive training. cessing: evidence for the cognitive
Friedman, N. P., and Miyake, A. (2004). 1011. Science 323, 800803. control connection. Lang. Linguist.
The relations among inhibition and Jonides, J., and Nee, D. E. (2006). Brain Milham, M. P., Banich, M. T., Webb, Compass 4, 906924.
interference cogntiive functions: a mechanisms of proactive interfer- A., Barad, V., Cohen, N. J., Wsza- Olesen, P., Westerberg, H., and Kling-
latent variable analysis. J. Exp. Psy- ence in working memory. Neuro- lek, T., and Kramer, A. F. (2001). berg, T. (2004). Increased prefrontal
chol. Gen. 133, 101135. science 139, 181193. The relative involvement of ante- and parietal brain activity after
Geva, S., Correia, M., and Warburton, E. Kan, I. P., and Thompson-Schill, S. L. rior cingulate and prefrontal cortex training of working memory. Nat.
A. (2011). Diffusion tensor imaging (2004). Effect of name agreement on in attentional control depends on Neurosci. 7, 7579.
in the study of language and aphasia. prefrontal activity during overt and nature of conict. Cogn. Brain Res. Owen, A. M., Hampshire, A., Grahn, J.
Aphasiology 25, 543558. covert picture naming. Cogn. Affect. 12, 467473. A., Stenton, R., Dajani, S., Burns, A.
Gray, J. R., Braver, T. S., and Raichle, Behav. Neurosci. 4, 4357. Miller, E. K., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). An S., Howard, R. J., and Ballard, C. G.
M. E. (2002). Integration of emo- Kane, M. J., and Engle, R. W. (2000). integrative theory of prefrontal cor- (2010). Putting brain training to the
tion and cognition in the lateral pre- Working memory capacity, proac- tex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, test. Nature 465, 775778.
frontal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. tive interference, and divided atten- 167202. Persson, J., Welsh, K. M., Jonides, J., and
U.S.A. 99, 41154120. tion: Limits on long-term memory Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2007). Cogni-
Gray, J. R., Chabris, C. F., and Braver, retrieval. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., tive fatigue of executive processes:
T. S. (2003). Neural mechanisms of Cogn. 26, 333358. and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity interaction between interference res-
general uid intelligence. Nat. Neu- Karbach, J., and Kray, J. (2009). How and diversity of executive functions olution tasks. Neuropsychologia 45,
rosci. 6, 316322. useful is executive control training? and their contributions to complex 15711579.
Hamilton, A. C., and Martin, R. C. Age differences in near and far trans- Frontal Lobetasks: a latent variable Richmond, L., Morrison, A., Chein,
(2005). Dissociations among tasks fer of task-switching training. Dev. analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49100. J., and Olson, I. (2011). Work-
involving inhibition: a single-case Sci. 12, 978990. Monsell, S. (1978). Recency, immediate ing memory training and transfer
study. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. Khanna, M. M., and Boland, J. E. (2010). recognition and reaction time. Cogn. in older adults. Psychol. Aging 26,
5, 113. Childrens use of language context Psychol. 10, 465501. 813822.
Hoffman, P., Jefferies, E., and Lambon in lexical ambiguity resolution. Q. J. Morrison, A. B., and Chein, J. M. (2011). Robinson, G., Blair, J., and Cipolotti,
Ralph, M. A. (2010). Ventrolateral Exp. Psychol. 63, 160193. Does working memory training L. (1998). Dynamic aphasia: An

www.frontiersin.org May 2012 | Volume 3 | Article 158 | 13


Hussey and Novick Cognitive training for language use

inability to select between compet- in the frontal lobes. Science 283, activity. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15, Ostensson, M. L., Bartfai, A., and
ing verbal responses? Brain 121, 16571661. 219224. Klingberg, T. (2007). Computerized
7789. Snyder, H. R., Hutchison, N., Nyhus, E., Thompson-Schill, S. L., Ramscar, M., working memory training after
Robinson, G., Shallice, T., and Cipolotti, Curran, T., Banich, M. T., OReilly, and Chrysikou, E. G. (2009). Cog- stroke a pilot study. Brain Inj. 21,
L. (2005). A failure of high level ver- R. C., and Munakata, Y. (2010). nition without control: when a little 2129.
bal response selection in progressive Neural inhibition enables selec- frontal lobe goes a long way. Curr. Ye, Z., and Zhou, X. (2009). Conict
dynamic aphasia. Cogn. Neuropsy- tion during language processing. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 18, 259263. control during sentence comprehen-
chol. 22, 661694. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, Thompson-Schill, S. L., Swick, D., sion: fMRI evidence. Neuroimage 48,
Rodrguez-Ferreiro, J., Gennari, S. P., 1648316488. Farah, M. J., DEsposito, M., Kan, I. 280290.
Davies, R., and Cuetos, F. (2011). Staub, A., and Rayner, K. (2007). Eye P., and Knight, R. T. (1998). Verb
Neural correlates of abstract verb movements and on-line compre- generation in patients with focal
processing. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, hension processes, in The Oxford frontal regions: a neuropsycholog-
106118. Handbook of Psycholinguistics, ed. G. ical test of neuroimaging ndings. Conict of Interest Statement: The
Schnur, T. T., Schwartz, M. F., Kimberg, Gaskell (Oxford: Oxford University Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95, authors declare that the research was
D. Y., Hirshorn, E., Coslett, H. B., Press), 327342. 1585515860. conducted in the absence of any com-
and Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2009). Takeuchi, H., Sekiguchi, A., Taki, Trueswell, J. C., Sekerina, I., Hill, N. mercial or nancial relationships that
Localizing interference during nam- Y., Yokoyama, S., Yomogida, Y., M., and Logrip, M. L. (1999). The could be construed as a potential con-
ing: convergent neuroimaging and Komuro, N.,Yamanouchi, T., Suzuki, kindergarten-path effect: studying ict of interest.
neuropsychological evidence for the S., and Kawashima, R. (2010). Train- on-line sentence processing in young
function of Brocas area. Proc. Natl. ing of working memory impacts children. Cognition 73, 89134. Received: 01 August 2011; accepted: 02
Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 322327. structural connectivity. J. Neurosci. Van der Linden, D., Frese, M., and Meij- May 2012; published online: 21 May
Schweizer, T. A., Ware, J., Fischer, C. 30, 32973303. man, T. F. (2003). Mental fatigue and 2012.
E., Craik, F. I., and Bialystok, E. Tanenhaus, M. K. (2007). Eye move- the control of cognitive processes: Citation: Hussey EK and Novick JM
(2011). Bilingualism as a contributor ments and spoken language process- effects on preservation and plan- (2012) The benets of executive con-
to cognitive reserve: evidence from ing, in Eye Movements: A Window ning. Acta Psychol. (Amst) 113, trol training and the implications for
brain atrophy in Alzheimers disease. on Mind and Brain, eds R. P. G. van 4565. language processing. Front. Psychology
Cortex 12, 815. Gompel, M. H. Fischer, W. S., Mur- Vuong, L. C., and Martin, R. C. (2011). 3:158. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00158
Shipstead, Z., Redick, T. S., and Engle, ray, and R. L. Hill (Oxford: Elsevier), LIFG-based attentional control and This article was submitted to Frontiers
R. W. (2010). Does working memory 309326. the resolution of lexical ambiguities in Cognition, a specialty of Frontiers in
training generalize? Psychol. Belg. 50, Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. in sentence context. Brain Lang. 116, Psychology.
245276. J., Eberhard, K. M., and Sedivy, J. 2232. Copyright 2012 Hussey and Novick.
Shipstead, Z., Redick, T. S., and Engle, E. (1995). Integration of visual and Weighall,A. R. (2008). The kindergarten This is an open-access article distributed
R. W. (2012). Is working memory linguistic information in spoken lan- path effect revisited: childrens use under the terms of the Creative Commons
training effective? Psychol. Bull. guage comprehension. Science 268, of context in processing structural Attribution Non Commercial License,
PMID: 22409508. [Epub ahead of 16321634. ambiguities. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. which permits non-commercial use, dis-
print]. Thompson-Schill, S. L., Bedny, M., and 99, 7595. tribution, and reproduction in other
Smith, E. E., and Jonides, J. (1999). Goldberg, R. F. (2005). The frontal Westerberg, H., Jacobaeus, H., forums, provided the original authors and
Storage and executive processes lobes and the regulation of mental Hirvikoski, T., Clevberger, P., source are credited.

Frontiers in Psychology | Cognition May 2012 | Volume 3 | Article 158 | 14

You might also like