You are on page 1of 7

Dyslexia

A new model of this reading disorder emphasizes defects


in the language-processing rather than the visual system. It explains
why some very smart people have trouble learning to read

by Sally E. Shaywitz

O ne hundred years ago, in


November 1896, a doctor in
Sussex, England, published
the first description of the learning dis-
order that would come to be known as
But the experience of millions of dyslex-
ics like Percy F. has shown that assump-
tion to be false. In dyslexia, the seem-
ingly invariant relation between intelli-
gence and reading ability breaks down.
nation of why some very intelligent peo-
ple have trouble learning to read and
performing other language-related tasks.
In the course of our work, my col-
leagues and I at the Yale Center for the
developmental dyslexia. Percy F.,... Early explanations of dyslexia, put Study of Learning and Attention have
aged 14,... has always been a bright and forth in the 1920s, held that defects in evaluated hundreds of children and
intelligent boy, wrote W. Pringle Mor- the visual system were to blame for the scores of men and women for reading
gan in the British Medical Journal, reversals of letters and words thought disabilities. Many are students and fac-
quick at games, and in no way inferior to typify dyslexic reading. Eye training ulty at our universitys undergraduate,
to others of his age. His great difficulty was often prescribed to overcome these graduate and professional schools. One
has beenand is nowhis inability to alleged visual defects. Subsequent re- of these, a medical student named Greg-
learn to read. search has shown, however, that chil- ory, came to see us after undergoing a
In that brief introduction, Morgan dren with dyslexia are not unusually series of problems in his first-year cours-
captured the paradox that has intrigued prone to reversing letters or words and es. He was quite discouraged.
and frustrated scientists for a century that the cognitive deficit responsible for Although he had been diagnosed as
since: the profound and persistent diffi- the disorder is related to the language dyslexic in grade school, Gregory had
culties some very bright people face in system. In particular, dyslexia reflects a also been placed in a program for gifted
learning to read. In 1996 as in 1896, deficiency in the processing of the dis- students. His native intelligence, togeth-
reading ability is taken as a proxy for tinctive linguistic units, called phonemes, er with extensive support and tutoring,
intelligence; most people assume that if that make up all spoken and written had allowed him to graduate from high
someone is smart, motivated and words. Current linguistic models of read- school with honors and gain admission
schooled, he or she will learn to read. ing and dyslexia now provide an expla- to an Ivy League college. In college,

The Paradox of Dyslexia

98 Scientific American November 1996 Copyright 1996 Scientific American, Inc. Dyslexia
Gregory had worked extremely hard about brain organization and function. the phonological module of the brain.
and eventually received offers from sev- Investigators from many laboratories, In spoken language, this process oc-
eral top medical schools. Now, howev- including my colleagues and I at the Yale curs automatically, at a preconscious
er, he was beginning to doubt his own Center, have had the opportunity to test level. As Noam Chomsky and, more re-
competence. He had no trouble com- and refine this model through 10 years cently, Steven Pinker of the Massachu-
prehending the intricate relations among of cognitive and, more recently, neuro- setts Institute of Technology have con-
physiological systems or the complex biological studies. vincingly argued, language is instinc-
mechanisms of disease; indeed, he ex- tiveall that is necessary is for humans
celled in those areas requiring reason- The Phonological Model to be exposed to it. A genetically deter-
ing skills. More problematic for him mined phonological module automati-
was the simple act of pronouncing long
words or novel terms (such as labels
used in anatomic descriptions); perhaps
T o understand how the phonological
model works, one has first to con-
sider the way in which language is pro-
cally assembles the phonemes into words
for the speaker and parses the spoken
word back into its underlying phono-
his least well-developed skill was rote cessed in the brain. Researchers concep- logical components for the listener.
memorization. tualize the language system as a hierar- In producing a word, the human
Both Gregory and his professors were chical series of modules or components, speech apparatusthe larynx, palate,
perplexed by the inconsistencies in his each devoted to a particular aspect of tongue and lipsautomatically com-
performance. How could someone who language. At the upper levels of the hi- presses and merges the phonemes. As a
understood difficult concepts so well erarchy are components involved with result, information from several pho-
have trouble with the smaller and sim- semantics (vocabulary or word mean- nemes is folded into a single unit of
pler details? Could Gregorys dyslexia ing), syntax (grammatical structure) sound. Because there is no overt clue to
he was still a slow readeraccount for and discourse (connected sentences). At the underlying segmental nature of
his inability to name body parts and tis- the lowest level of the hierarchy is the speech, spoken language appears to be
sue types in the face of his excellent rea- phonological module, which is dedicat- seamless. Hence, an oscilloscope would
soning skills? ed to processing the distinctive sound register the word cat as a single burst
It could, I explained. Gregorys histo- elements that constitute language. of sound; only the human language sys-
ry fit the clinical picture of dyslexia as it The phoneme, defined as the smallest tem is capable of distinguishing the three
has been traditionally defined: an unex- meaningful segment of language, is the phonemes embedded in the word.
pected difficulty learning to read despite fundamental element of the linguistic Reading reflects spoken language, as
intelligence, motivation and education. system. Different combinations of just my colleague Alvin M. Liberman of
Furthermore, I was able to reassure 44 phonemes produce every word in the Haskins Laboratories in New Haven,
Gregory that scientists now understand English language. The word cat, for Conn., points out, but it is a much hard-
the basic nature of dyslexia. example, consists of three phonemes: er skill to master. Why? Although both
Over the past two decades, a coherent kuh, aah, and tuh. (Linguists in- speaking and reading rely on phonolog-
model of dyslexia has emerged that is dicate these sounds as |k|, || and |t|.) ical processing, there is a significant dif-
based on phonological processing. The Before words can be identified, under- ference: speaking is natural, and reading
phonological model is consistent both stood, stored in memory or retrieved is not. Reading is an invention and must
with the clinical symptoms of dyslexia from it, they must first be broken down, be learned at a conscious level. The task
and with what neuroscientists know or parsed, into their phonetic units by of the reader is to transform the visual

YAN NASCIMBENE

Dyslexia Copyright 1996 Scientific American, Inc. Scientific American November 1996 99
the phonological deficit hypothesis.
According to this hypothesis, a cir-
cumscribed deficit in phonological pro-
cessing impairs decoding, preventing
word identification. This basic deficit in
what is essentially a lower-order lin-
guistic function blocks access to higher-
order linguistic processes and to gain-
ing meaning from text. Thus, although
the language processes involved in com-
prehension and meaning are intact, they
SPEAKING is carried out at an auto- cannot be called into play, because they
matic and unconscious level by a bio- can be accessed only after a word has
logically determined phonological been identified. The impact of the pho-
module in the brain. First, the relevant nological deficit is most obvious in read-
phonemic structures are selected and ing, but it can also affect speech in pre-
assembled. These individual phonemes dictable ways. Gregorys dilemma with
are then coarticulatedthat is, over-
long or novel words, for example, is en-
lapped and mergedby the speech ap-
paratus. Coarticulation permits the tirely consistent with the body of evi-
rapid production of phonetic strings dence that supports a phonological
but obscures the underlying segmental model of dyslexia.
nature of speech. That evidence began accumulating
more than two decades ago. One of the
earliest experiments, carried out by the
late Isabelle Y. Liberman of Haskins
Laboratories, showed that young chil-
dren become aware between four and
six years of age of the phonological
structure of spoken words. In the exper-
iment, children were asked how many
sounds they heard in a series of words.
None of the four-year-olds could cor-
rectly identify the number of phonemes,
but 17 percent of the five-year-olds did,
and by age six, 70 percent of the children
demonstrated phonological awareness.
By age six, most children have also
had at least one full year of schooling,
including instruction in reading. The de-
velopment of phonological awareness,
then, parallels the acquisition of reading
YAN NASCIMBENE

skills. This correspondence suggested


that the two processes are related. These
findings also converge with data from
the Connecticut Longitudinal Study, a
project my colleagues and I began in
READING is not automatic but must be learned. The reader must develop a conscious
awareness that the letters on the page represent the sounds of the spoken word. To 1983 with 445 randomly selected kin-
read the word cat, the reader must parse, or segment, the word into its underlying dergartners; the study continues in 1996
phonological elements. Once the word is in its phonological form, it can be identified when these children are age 19 and out
and understood. In dyslexia, an inefficient phonological module produces representa- of high school. Testing the youngsters
tions that are less clear and hence more difficult to bring to awareness. yearly, we found that dyslexia affects a
full 20 percent of schoolchildrena fig-
ure that agrees roughly with the pro-
percepts of alphabetic script into lin- is precisely what happens when a child portion of Libermans six-year-olds who
guistic onesthat is, to recode graph- learns to read. could not identify the phonological
emes (letters) into their corresponding In contrast, when a child is dyslexic, structure of words. These data further
phonemes. To accomplish this, the be- a deficit within the language system at support a connection between phono-
ginning reader must first come to a con- the level of the phonological module logical awareness and reading.
scious awareness of the internal phono- impairs his or her ability to segment the During the 1980s, researchers began
logical structure of spoken words. Then written word into its underlying pho- to address that connection explicitly.
he or she must realize that the orthog- nological components. This explana- The groundbreaking work of Lynette
raphythe sequence of letters on the tion of dyslexia is referred to as the Bradley and Peter E. Bryant of the Uni-
pagerepresents this phonology. That phonological model, or sometimes as versity of Oxford indicated that a pre-

100 Scientific American November 1996 Copyright 1996 Scientific American, Inc. Dyslexia
GENERAL
INTELLIGENCE

VOCABULARY
DECODING WORD
TEXT DECODING MEANING
YAN NASCIMBENE (cat); JANA BRENNING

DECODING IDENTIFICATION
REASONING
CAT
CONCEPT
FORMATION

IN READING, the word (here, cat) is first decoded into its (small furry mammal that purrs). In people who have dyslex-
phonological form (kuh, aah, tuh) and identified. Once it is ia, a phonological deficit impairs decoding, thus preventing the
identified, higher-level cognitive functions such as intelligence reader from using his or her intelligence and vocabulary to get
and vocabulary are applied to understand the words meaning to the words meaning.

schoolers phonological aptitude predicts ard Katz of Haskins Laboratories, I ex- word sour without the s sound.
future skill at reading. Bradley and Bry- amined 378 children from seven to nine This measure was most related to a
ant also found that training in phono- years old on a battery of tests that as- childs ability to decode single words in
logical awareness significantly improves sessed both linguistic and nonlinguistic standardized tests and was independent
a childs ability to read. In these studies, abilities. Our results as well as those of of his or her intelligence, vocabulary
one group of children received training Keith E. Stanovich and Linda S. Siegel and reasoning skills. When we gave this
in phonological processing, while an- of the Ontario Institute for Studies in and other tests of phonemic awareness
other received language training that Education made it clear that phonolog- to a group of 15-year-olds in our Con-
did not emphasize the sound structure ical deficits are the most significant and necticut Longitudinal Study, the results
of words. For example, the first group consistent cognitive marker of dyslexic were the same: even in high school stu-
might work on categorizing words by children. dents, phonological awareness was the
their sound, and the second group would One test in particular seemed quite best predictor of reading ability.
focus on categorizing words according sensitive to dyslexia: the Auditory Anal- If dyslexia is the result of an insuffi-
to their meaning. These studies, togeth- ysis Test, which asks a child to segment ciently developed phonological special-
er with more recent work by Benita A. words into their underlying phonologi- ization, other consequences of impaired
Blachman of Syracuse University, Joseph cal units and then to delete specific phonological functioning should also
E. Torgesen of Florida State University phonemes from the words. For exam- be apparentand they are. Ten years ago
and Barbara Foorman of the University ple, the child must say the word block the work of Robert B. Katz of Haskins

YAN NASCIMBENE (cat); CAROL DONNER (brain); from Brain, Mind and Behavior, by F. E. Bloom and A. Lazerson, W. H. Freeman, 1988
of Houston, clearly demonstrate that without the buh sound or say the Laboratories documented the problems
phonological training in particu-
larrather than general language
instructionis responsible for the
improvements in reading. PHONOLOGICAL PROCESSING
(KUH-AAH-TUH) WORD MEANING
Such findings set the stage for
our own study, in the early 1990s,
of the cognitive skills of dyslexic
and nondyslexic children. Along
with Jack M. Fletcher of the Uni-
versity of TexasHouston and LETTER IDENTIFICATION
Donald P. Shankweiler and Leon- (C-A-T)

NEURAL ARCHITECTURE for


reading has been suggested by func-
tional magnetic resonance imaging.
Letter identification activates the
extrastriate cortex in the occipital
lobe; phonological processing acti-
vates the inferior frontal gyrus (Bro-
cas area); and accessing meaning ac-
tivates primarily the superior tem-
poral gyrus and parts of the middle
temporal and supramarginal gyri.

Dyslexia Copyright 1996 Scientific American, Inc. Scientific American November 1996 101
poor readers have in naming objects idence in suggesting that whereas the decode and identify the written word,
shown in pictures. Katz showed that phonological component of the language she could not access her fund of knowl-
when dyslexics misname objects, the in- system is impaired in dyslexia, the high- edge about its meaning when she came
correct responses tend to share phono- er-level components remain intact. Lin- across it in reading.
logical characteristics with the correct guistic processes involved in word mean- Of course, many dyslexics, like Greg-
response. Furthermore, the misnaming ing, grammar and discoursewhat, col- ory, do learn to read and even to excel
is not the result of a lack of knowledge. lectively, underlies comprehensionseem in academics despite their disability.
For example, a girl shown a picture of a to be fully operational, but their activi- These so-called compensated dyslexics
volcano calls it a tornado. When given ty is blocked by the deficit in the lower- perform as well as nondyslexics on tests
the opportunity to elaborate, she dem- order function of phonological process- of word accuracythey have learned
onstrates that she knows what the pic- ing. In one of our studies, Jennifer, a how to decode or identify words, there-
tured object isshe can describe the at- very bright young woman with a read- by gaining entry to the higher levels of
tributes and activities of a volcano in ing disability, told us all about the word the language system. But they do so at a
great detail and point to other pictures apocalypse. She knew its meaning, its cost. Timed tests reveal that decoding
related to volcanoes. She simply cannot connotations and its correct usage; she remains very laborious for compensat-
summon the word volcano. could not, however, recognize the word ed dyslexics; they are neither automatic
This finding converges with other ev- on a printed page. Because she could not nor fluent in their ability to identify

Playing Past Learning Disabilities

D yslexia is the most common of the learning disorders, condi- there is nonetheless broad overlap between the two groups. Studies
tions that interfere with a normally intelligent childs ability to have suggested that as many as 8 percent of all children may be lan-
acquire speech, reading or other cognitive skills. Children with learn- guage-impaired; of this group, more than 85 percent also exhibit
ing disabilities have become the basis of a thriving industry since dyslexia.
1968, when federal education officials first earmarked funds to help Tallal, who began studying language impairment in the late 1970s,
them. The number of children identified as having learning disabili- has long suspected that this problem stems from an inability to pro-
ties soared from 780,000 in 1976 to 2.3 million in 1993. An estimated cess auditory information rapidly enough. Whereas most children
$15 billion is spent annually on the diagnosis, treatment and study of can process phonemes lasting less than 40 milliseconds, the lan-
such disorders. guage-impaired may require as much as 500 milliseconds. To them,
The definitions and diagnostic criteria for learning disorders are of- the word bat may be indistinguishable from pat. This hypothesis,
ten subjective or ambiguous; Tallal says, is compatible with
their causes are typically obscure the phonological-deficit model
or controversial. For example, of dyslexia but places more em-
psychologist Gerald Coles of the phasis on the role of timing in
University of Rochester challeng- neural processing.
es the claim that 20 percent of Language impairment, Tallal
children are dyslexic, and not all believes, usually stems from an
researchers and educators ac- organic deficit rather than from
cept a phonological (or even bi- environmental factors. Magnetic
ological) explanation for dyslex- resonance scans and other im-
ia. Treatment is another area that aging studies, she states, have
has been fraught with controver- turned up distinct neural differ-
sy and, often, disappointment. ences between people with nor-
Over the years, educators and mal language skills and the lan-
parents have subscribed to many guage-impaired. But just be-
techniques that promised to help cause something is biologically
children overcome their learning based doesnt mean its irreme-
disabilities, despite the absence diable, Tallal adds.
of independent research to back FIVE-YEAR-OLD KEILLAN LECKY interacts with a language- Two years ago she teamed up
up those claims. Nevertheless, learning program at Rutgers University in Newark, N.J. with Merzenich and several oth-
ongoing research holds out pros- er scientists to develop a com-
pects for some real progress. puter-based therapyan animated video game, essentiallyfor
One of the most lauded treatments for learning disabilities to training language-impaired children. The core of the therapy is a
emerge in recent years has been developed by a group led by Paula speech-processing program that enables the researchers to alter the
Tallal, co-director of the Center for Molecular and Behavioral Neuro- amplitude and duration of recorded sounds.
science at Rutgers University in Newark, N.J., and Michael M. Merze- In one of the programs, which has a circus motif, a clown utters
nich of the Keck Center for Integrative Neuroscience at the Universi- two closely related phonemes, such as pa and da, that have been
NINA BERMAN Sipa Press

ty of California at San Francisco. Their research has not focused on stretched out to a length that the children can easily comprehend.
dyslexics per se but on language-impaired children who have diffi- When the children correctly distinguish between the sounds, the
culty understanding speech. Not all language-impaired children are clown congratulates them; progress is also represented by a bear
dyslexic, Tallal notes, and not all dyslexics are language-impaired, but moving along a tightrope.

102 Scientific American November 1996 Copyright 1996 Scientific American, Inc. Dyslexia
words. Many dyslexics have told us carried out by a specific network of brain
how tiring reading is for them, reflect- cells. Until recently, however, research- The Myths of Dyslexia
ing the enormous resources and energy ers have had no firm indication of how
they must expend on the task. In fact, that scheme maps onto the actual func- Mirror writing is a symptom of dyslexia.
extreme slowness in making phonolog- tional organization of the human brain. In fact, backwards writing and reversals
ically based decisions is typical of the Unlike many other functions, reading of letters and words are common in the
group of compensated dyslexics we have cannot be studied in animals; indeed, early stages of writing development
assembled as part of a new approach to for many years the cerebral localization among dyslexic and nondyslexic chil-
understanding dyslexia: our neuroim- of all higher cognitive processes could dren alike. Dyslexic children have prob-
aging program. be inferred only from the effects of brain lems in naming letters but not in copy-
injuries on the people who survived ing letters.
The Neurobiology of Reading them. Such an approach offered little to
illuminate the phenomena my col- Eye training is a treatment for dyslexia.

T he phonological model incorporates


a modular scheme of cognitive pro-
cessing in which each of the component
leagues and I were interested in. What
we needed was a way to identify the re-
gions of the brain that are engaged when
More than two decades of research
have shown that dyslexia reflects a lin-
guistic deficit. There is no evidence that
processes used in word identification is healthy subjects are reading or trying to eye training alleviates the disorder.
read.
Our group became quite excited, then, More boys than girls are dyslexic.
with the advent in the late 1980s of Boys reading disabilities are indeed
Once the children have mastered phonemes functional magnetic resonance imaging identified more often than girls, but
of a given durationsay, 400 milliseconds (fMRI). Using the same scanning ma- studies indicate that such identification
they can move on to more rapid, realistic pho- chine that has revolutionized clinical is biased. The actual prevalence of the
nemes. The youngsters also listen to stretched imaging, fMRI can measure changes in disorder is nearly identical in the two
recordings of whole words, sentences and sto- the metabolic activity of the brain while sexes.
ries, such as The Cat in the Hat. Tallal and Mer- an individual performs a cognitive task.
zenich reported in Science this past January Hence, it is ideally suited to mapping Dyslexia can be outgrown.
that 11 children trained with these methods the brains response to stimuli such as Yearly monitoring of phonological skills
had acquired two years worth of language reading. Because it is noninvasive and from first through 12th grade shows
skills in only one month. A control group given uses no radioisotopes, fMRI is also ex- that the disability persists into adult-
identical therapy, but without the stretched cellent for work involving children. hood. Even though many dyslexics
speech, progressed only one quarter as much. Since 1994, I have worked with sev- learn to read accurately, they continue
This year Tallal, Merzenich and two col- to read slowly and not automatically.
eral Yale colleagues to use fMRI in study-
leagues founded a company called Scientific ing the neurobiology of reading. Ben-
Learning Principles, based in San Francisco, to Smart people cannot be dyslexic.
nett A. Shaywitz, Kenneth R. Pugh, R.
develop and market an interactive CD-ROM Intelligence is in no way related to pho-
Todd Constable, Robert K. Fulbright,
containing their learning program. They plan nological processing, as scores of bril-
John C. Gore and I have used the tech-
to test prototypes in 25 or more special educa- liant and accomplished dyslexics
nique with more than 200 dyslexic and among them William Butler Yeats, Al-
tion schools and clinics in the U.S. and Canada
nondyslexic children and adults. As a bert Einstein, George Patton, John Irv-
over the next year. As many as 500 children are
result of this program, we can now sug- ing, Charles Schwab and Nicholas Ne-
expected to participate.
gest a tentative neural architecture for groponteattest.
The studies will include not only language-
reading a printed word. In particular, the
impaired children but also those diagnosed
with dyslexia, attention-deficit disorder and
identification of letters activates sites in
other common learning disabilities. We want the extrastriate cortex within the occip-
to determine the generalizability of this tech- ital lobe; phonological processing takes
nique, Tallal notes. If all goes well, she says, the place within the inferior frontal gyrus; brain organization for any cognitive
CD-ROMs will be made available to certified and access to meaning calls on areas function. The fact that womens brains
learning centers beginning next year. within the middle and superior tempo- tend to have bilateral representation for
Since the media first reported on this re- ral gyri of the brain. phonological processing explains sever-
search a year ago, Tallal and her colleagues Our investigation has already revealed al formerly puzzling observations: why,
have been inundated with queries from the a surprising difference between men for example, after a stroke involving the
press and parents. In part to satisfy these de- and women in the locus of phonologi- left side of the brain, women are less
mands, they have created a World Wide Web cal representation for reading. It turns likely than men to have significant decre-
site (http://www.scilearn.com). out that in men phonological process- ments in their language skills, and why
Tallal emphasizes that the questions raised ing engages the left inferior frontal gy- women tend more often than men to
by Coles and other skeptics about the causes rus, whereas in women it activates not compensate for dyslexia.
and frequency of learning disabilities are im- only the left but the right inferior frontal As investigators who have spent our
portant. She nonetheless thinks it is a mistake gyrus as well. These differences in later- entire professional lives trying to under-
to focus on all these differences in definition. alization had been suggested by behav- stand dyslexia, we find the identification
Real progress, she says, will come about only ioral studies, but they had never before of brain sites dedicated to phonological
through empirical research. been demonstrated unequivocally. In- processing in reading very excitingit
John Horgan, staff writer deed, our findings constitute the first means that we now have a possible
concrete proof of gender differences in neurobiological signature for read-

Dyslexia Copyright 1996 Scientific American, Inc. Scientific American November 1996 103
MALE FEMALE
information, needing to retrieve it rap-
idly and present it orally often results in
calling up a related phoneme or incor-
rectly ordering the retrieved phonemes.
Under such circumstances, dyslexics will
pepper their speech with many ums, ahs
and other hesitations. On the other hand,
when not pressured to provide instant
responses, the dyslexic can deliver an

BENNETT A. SHAYWITZ ET AL. Yale NMR Research


excellent oral presentation. Similarly, in
reading, whereas nonimpaired readers
can decode words automatically, indi-
viduals such as Gregory frequently need
to resort to the use of context to help
them identify specific words. This strat-
egy slows them further and is another
reason that the provision of extra time is
necessary if dyslexics are to show what
BRAIN ACTIVATION PATTERNS during reading, as revealed in these functional they actually know. Multiple-choice ex-
magnetic resonance images, differ in men and women. During phonological process- aminations, too, by their lack of suffi-
ing, men show primarily unilateral activation, in the left inferior frontal gyrus. In wom- cient context, as well as by their wording
en, phonological processing activates both the left and the right inferior frontal gyri. and response format, excessively penal-
ize dyslexics.
But our experience at the Yale Center
ing. The isolation of such a signature critical thinking and vocabulary. In- suggests that many compensated dys-
brings with it the future promise of deed, compensated dyslexics such as lexics have a distinct advantage over
more precise diagnosis of dyslexia. It is Gregory may use the big picture of nondyslexics in their ability to reason
possible, for example, that the neural theories, models and ideas to help them and conceptualize and that the phono-
signature for phonological processing remember specific details. It is true that logical deficit masks what are often ex-
may provide the most sensitive measure when details are not unified by associ- cellent comprehension skills. Many
of the disorder. Furthermore, the discov- ated ideas or theoretical frameworks schools and universities now appreciate
ery of a biological signature for reading when, for example, Gregory must com- the circumscribed nature of dyslexia
offers an unprecedented opportunity to mit to memory long lists of unfamiliar and offer to evaluate the achievement
assess the effects of interventions on the namesdyslexics can be at a real disad- of their dyslexic students with essays
neuroanatomic systems serving the read- vantage. Even if Gregory succeeds in and prepared oral presentations rather
ing process itself. memorizing such lists, he has trouble than tests of rote memorization or mul-
producing the names on demand, as he tiple choices. Just as researchers have
Putting It in Context must when he is questioned on rounds begun to understand the neural sub-
by an attending physician. The phono- strate of dyslexia, educators are begin-

T he phonological model crystallizes


exactly what we mean by dyslexia:
an encapsulated deficit often surround-
logical model predicts, and experimen-
tation has shown, that rote memoriza-
tion and rapid word retrieval are par-
ning to recognize the practical implica-
tions of the disorder. A century after W.
Pringle Morgan first described dyslexia
ed by significant strengths in reasoning, ticularly difficult for dyslexics. in Percy F., society may at last under-
problem solving, concept formation, Even when the individual knows the stand the paradox of the disorder. SA

The Author Further Reading


SALLY E. SHAYWITZ is, along with Bennett A. The Alphabetic Principle and Learning to Read. Isabelle Y. Liberman, Don-
Shaywitz, co-director of the Yale Center for the Study ald P. Shankweiler and Alvin M. Liberman in Phonology and Reading Disability.
of Learning and Attention and professor of pediatrics Edited by D. P. Shankweiler and I. Y. Liberman. University of Michigan Press, 1989.
at the Yale University School of Medicine. She re- Learning to Read. Edited by Laurence Rieben and Charles A. Perfetti. Lawrence
ceived her M.D. from the Albert Einstein College of Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, N.J., 1991.
Medicine in Bronx, N.Y., and has spent her entire Evidence That Dyslexia May Represent the Lower Tail of a Normal Dis-
professional career at Yale, where, since 1983, she has tribution of Reading Ability. Sally E. Shaywitz, Michael D. Escobar, Bennett
directed the Connecticut Longitudinal Study. Current- A. Shaywitz, Jack M. Fletcher and Robert Makuch in New England Journal of
ly she is using functional magnetic resonance imaging Medicine, Vol. 326, No. 3, pages 145150; January 16, 1992.
to study the neurobiology of dyslexia in children and Sex Differences in the Functional Organization of the Brain for Lan-
young adults. A pediatrician and neuroscientist, she guage. Bennett A. Shaywitz, Sally E. Shaywitz, Kenneth R. Pugh, R. Todd Con-
received the impetus to study dyslexia from the many stable, Pawel Skudlarski, Robert K. Fulbright, Richard A. Bronen, Jack M.
very bright dyslexics she came to know as patients, Fletcher, Donald P. Shankweiler, Leonard Katz and John C. Gore in Nature, Vol.
students and, often, colleagues. She acknowledges the 373, pages 607609; February 16, 1995.
helpful comments of the Shaywitz tribeAdam, Jon Toward a Definition of Dyslexia. G. Reid Lyon in Annals of Dyslexia, Vol.
and Davidin preparing this article. 45, pages 327; 1995.

104 Scientific American November 1996 Copyright 1996 Scientific American, Inc. Dyslexia

You might also like