You are on page 1of 20

Journal of Vestibular Research, Vol. 7, No.4, pp.

283-302,1997
Copyright © 1997 Elsevier Science Inc.
Printed in the USA. All rights reserved
0957-4271/97 $17.00 + .00
ELSEVIER PH S0957 -4271 (97)00027 -X

Review

INTERACTION OF EYE-, HEAD-, AND TRUNK-BOUND INFORMATION


IN SPATIAL PERCEPTION AND CONTROL*

Horst Mittelstaedt
Max-Planck-Institut fOr Verhaltensphysiologi, 0-82319 Seewiesen, Germany
Reprint address: Horst Mittelstaedt, Max-Planck-Institute fOr Verhaltensphysiologie,
0-82319 Seewiesen, Germany. Tel: +49-8157-932371; Fax: +49-8157-932209;
E-mail: mittelstaedt@mpi-seewiesen.mpg.de

D Abstract- This article reviews the author's in- lower body indicate that yet another source of so-
vestigations on the perception and control of spa- matic graviception may exist, for example, one
tial relations if the carriers of the relevant sense that exploits the hydrostatics of blood pressure or
organs are mobile and controlled independently of the inertia of the mass of the abdominal
each other. In the dragonfly, head rotation is con- viscera. © 1997 Elsevier Science Inc.
trolled by the head's inertia, as well as by cervico-
collic, optokinetic, and dorsal light reflexes and, in D Keywords - neck reflex; efference copy;
turn, controls trunk rotation by means of neck somatic graviceptor; lower body negative pressure
reflexes on the wings. In humans, invariance of (LBNP); cervicocollic reflex; renal afference;
head-referenced visual direction under eye-to-head otolith.
rotation is attained by feedforward of an efference
copy. In the pigeon, invariance of responses to
trunk tilt under head-to-trunk rotation is, in From Eye to Head to Trunk
flight, achieved by feedforward of head-to-trunk
information yielded by neck receptors. But in The Head as a Torsion Pendulum
standing or walking, this is accomplished by
means of gravity sense organs in the trunk. Such It all began with a chance observation. Erich
organs are also shown to exist in the human trunk
von Holst, who had discovered the "dorsal light
by means of experiments on a sled centrifuge.
reflex" in fishes, had accidentally seen a drag-
From tests with paraplegic and neuromectomized
subjects, it is concluded that truncal graviception onfly landing upside down on a surface illumi-
1) is not influenced by mechanoreceptors in the nated from below. He supposed that orientation
legs, the skin, and between the vertebrae but 2) is
j
to the luminance centroid of the sky might be
affected by at least two afferent inputs, one origj· substituted for orientation in an animal
nating in the kidneys, another in the tissues that without statoliths, and suggested dragonfly ori-
support the large blood vessels against the gravita- entation as a topic for my doctoral thesis (1).
tionalload. These conclusions are corroborated by Instead of the then usual procedure, namely,
experiments with bilaterally nephrectomized sub- of observing the behavior of the freely moving
jects and by means of positive air pressure to the animal under stationary environmental condi-
legs, respectively. Recent results under application
tions, I restricted the dragonfly movement to
of positive and negative air pressure to the entire
pure roll by means of a harness and a system of
strings, and rotated the visual environment
*This is an autobiographical review based on a lecture at
around it (Figure 1). Thus, the animal hovered
the Third International Symposium on the Head/Neck in the center of a cylinder that could be rotated
System, Vail, Colorado, July 2-6, 1995. about an earth horizontal axis, collinear with the

283
284 H. Mittelstaedt

Lst

z
<

J F

l'----_I _I _ _Sf _ __

Figure 1. Harness consisting of a string and strips of linen, glued to the thorax and the first abdominal segment
of the dragonfly (Anax imperator Leach). By means of braces (Sb) and elastic strings (F) the harnessed animal
is hung in the axis of a rectangular wire frame (R), about which a contoured or plain white cylinder (Z), illumi-
nated by a shielded (P) light bulb, can be rotated.

insect's long axis, either homogeneously white I found, then, that the head acted like a torsion
or half-black and half-white, or with longitudi- pendulum (Figure 2d), and its angular deviation
nal stripes. Notably, this method has become from the trunk elicited compensatory ret1exes
quite popular since, and much refined through on the wings (Figure 2, panels a, b, and c) as
automatic control of the environment's move- well as counter-rolling cervicocollic ret1exes on
ment by feedback of the animal's movement. the head. Subsequently, I discovered a complex
But in 1947, I had to be the controller myself. system of hair plates in the neck (Figures 3 and
The dragont1y turned its back into the cen- 4) and showed by means of nerve sections how
troid of the luminance indeed, irrespective of its it released these two types of reflexes. I con-
direction to gravity. It also followed the stripes cluded that the position of the head, controlled
with very small retinal slip by head and trunk by its own inertia and the cervicocollic ret1exes,
movements (Figure 2a). Thus, if the dragont1y as well as by the optokinetic and the dorsal light
was tilted in a structured environment, the head ret1exes, elicited neck ret1exes on the wings that
remained virtually upright, while the wings forced the trunk to follow suit.
showed prompt and effective compensatory re-
t1exes. Yet, to my surprise, this also happened
in a perfectly homogeneous environment or in The Principle of Reafference
darkness (Figure 2b). Even in an anesthetized
animal, the head remained briet1y in place when Thus, from the very start, I found myself in-
the trunk was quickly tipped. volved in head-neck problems. In my thesis (1,
Spatial Perception and Control 285

a. 6Y-=-=

b.

:~:I~
---. " oS.. ~ ~ l\ ~

c.
Figure 2. The head of Anax as a torsion pendulum. (a,b,c) Experiments in the set-up of Figure 1: (a) If the
striped cylinder is rotated to the right, the head is rolled to the right and the wings twisted so as to produce a
rightward roll of the body; (b) its momentum of inertia lets the head lag behind movements of the body, eliciting
compensatory reflexes on the wings; (c) the body follows suit if the head, with a tiny iron filing glued to the
clypeus, is rotated by means of a magnet. (d) model of the head's mechanics, showing the laterocervical scler-
ites that carry' the head by means of minute twin joint (KG), as well the 6 head muscles, 4 recti (HM, 8M), and 2
obliqui (RM).

printed in 2) I discussed the question whether seen during eye cleaning!) As expected, rota-
dragonflies are able to make voluntary head tion of the striped drum about a vertical axis
movements just to look around, that is, without caused the fly with inversed eyes to rotate in the
eliciting neck reflexes on the trunk. I realized opposite sense of the drum. In a homogeneously
that this problem appears not only in the: case of white cylinder, illuminated from below, such a
head movement, but also in the case of volun- fly moves around quite normally. If the opto-
tary movements in general: How are voluntary motor reflex were inhibited during voluntary
movements in spite of the opposing static and movements, the same normal behavior should
dynamic reflexes at all possible? At that time, be seen when the homogeneous cylinder is re-
the generally accepted solution for this problem placed by a stationary striped drum. Yet now
posited that during a voluntary movement all the fly rotated or oscillated about its dorsoven-
opposing reflexes had to be shut off by inhibi- tral axis as soon as it started to move. I con-
tion. But I found it hard to believe that the ani- cluded (3) that within the fly, a central nervous
mal should be deprived of all of its head refer- mechanism existed that coupled a voluntary
enced information when it was most needed. motor command with the optokinetic afference
In order to put the inhibition hypothesis to caused by it. This idea was further elaborated in
test, I rolled the head of a drone fly (Eristalis my thesis (1,2), but I did not specify the mecha-
tenax) by 180 0 and glued it to the prothorax in nism by which this coupling of the command
that attitude (3). (Such large torsions are often with its inevitable afferent echo might be ef-
286 H. Mittelstaedt

Figure 3. Lateral view of the dragonfly's head and prothorax. Only the head is bisected (somewhat to the left of
the median plane). 1: head jOint at the rostral tip of the laterocervical sclerite (22) of the prothorax which carries
the head; 32: tentorium, the central head skeleton to which the head joint is attached; 23: triangular pad at the
rear end of 22, which can be pressed into a fitting groove (40) at the head to fixate it, or to set it free by contrac-
tion of the 4 recti (41) or the 2 transverse muscles (42), respectively; 2: hair plate at the joint, stimulated by a
sclerite (3) during left roll; 11: its afferent nerve to the subesophageal ganglion (6); 4,43,45: ventral and dorsal
hair plates, innervated by the prothoracic (13) ganglion (for example, 4 by 15). Note that the largest masses
within the head, the compound eyes (39) and the mouth parts (57-61), are concentrated at the periphery, maxi-
mizing the momentum of inertia.

fected-quite wisely so, as we shall see pres- "Das Reafferenzprinzip" (5), we explained this
ently. phenomenon and the behavior of the fly by as-
But later in that propitious year, von Holst suming that a "copy" of the motor command to
ran into me one morning with a reprint of Korn- the eye, named "Efferenzkopie," is added to the
muller's report (4) on the effect of eye paralysis visual afference caused by the command,
on his vision, and cried, "Here is what you al- named "Reafferenz." The two signals are cali-
ways say should then happen: The entire visual brated so as to annul each other if (and only if)
world moved to the right when he tried to look no externally caused visual afference, named
to the right!" In our common article entitled "Exafferenz ," is present. If the calibration is
Spatial Perception and Control 287

Figure 4. Ventral view of head and prothorax. The mouth parts are removed and the left side of the prothorax is
opened. For notation, see Figure 3 legend.

perfect, only the ex afferent part of the total af- lieved that the permanent rotation could only be
ferent inflow can pass the summation, and provided by an internal positive feedback loop,
hence that part which contains information is and we used, or rather misused, an efference
separated from that which does not. copy to that end in our model (see Abb. 4 and 7
In the following years, I tried to formulate in reference 5), just as did Roger Sperry (7) in
this hypothesis in terms of control theory. In the the case of a newt with reversed eyes. However,
case of the invariance of headcentric visual di- although not necessary and rather unlikely, the
rection under eye rotation, the analysis showed existence of such a positive internal loop has
that, given the experimental evidence, ar: effer- beer~ ex::1Lldec1
ence copy is indeed necessary to provide a co- neither in the fly nor in the newt.
ordinate transformation of visual direction from Still later, in 1958, I found that, in order to an-
eye to head. HoweveL the oscillation and rota- nul the reafference, the efference copy must em-
tion of the fly with reversed vision does not nec- body the same transfer function as the product of
essarily imply an efference copy. Rather, it may the transfer functions of all systems in the reaf-
be due to the reversal of the sign of the optomo- ferent pathway from the efference it copies to the
tor feedback loop alone. Thus, in the case of the reafference it annuls: An efference copy must act
normal fly, addition of the command itself to like an internal model of the systems it bridges.
the optomotor feedback loop would suffice to This then led to insights into the principles of the
make voluntary movements of the fly possible information processing that yields invariance of
(see pages 155 to 157 and Abb. 7 in ref. 6). We spatial perception and control under eye and head
(5) did not realize this in 1950 because we be- movements (8, reviewed in 9 and 10).
288 H. Mittelstaedt

1=-300 I = -30'~

r = _30 0 r = -;-.30" r= 0
0

(a)
v (b) (c)

Figure 5. Wing responses of the pigeon to static roll tilt. I: angular deviation of the head from the vertical, h: an-
gular deviation of the head from the trunk, r: angular deviation of the trunk from the vertical. In (a) and (b), equal
but opposite wing reflexes are seen; in (c), no reaction.

The Head-to- Trunk Coordinate at any trunk posture and even under an in-
Transfo rmation creased G-load (12). Hence, the gravity infor-
mation could not be yielded by the utricles
When a sitting pigeon is passively roll tilted alone. Rather, I suggested a cross multiplication
with its long axis in an eru.ih horizontal position mechanism of the utricular and the saccular sig-
under exclusion of visual cues, it makes prompt naIs \vith representations of the respective sine
balancing movements with its wings and legs and c9sine components of the angle between
that would rotate it back if it were free to move. neck and trunk (Figure 6, lower diagram), in
The response is the same whether the head is fact, a complete 3-dimensional vectorial coordi-
tilted with the trunk or kept horizontal during nate transformation from the head-bound infor-
trunk tilt. However, when the head is tilted while mation to the trunk-bound motor control.
the trunk stays horizontal, nothing happens (Fig- In order to test this hypothesis, I examined
ure 5). In 1906, Trendelenburg (11) had con- three labyrinthectomized pigeons. These pi-
cluded that these responses are released by grav- geons never flew again in the three years that
ity sense organs in the trunk. In my thesis (1,2; we observed them. However, some months after
p. 439) I showed that they could be explained in the operation, they were indistinguishable from
yet another way, namely, by subtraction of a the normals in the above-named tilt tests of the
neck position signal from the otolithic signal wing reflexes. It turns out, then, that both Tren-
about head posture. When the entire animal is delenburg's and my own hypothesis are con-
tilted, the response is then released by the firmed by these results: Pigeons need the vestib-
otoliths; when only the trunk is tilted, then the ular system (and, of course, the eyes) when they
response is released by the neck receptors; when are t1ying, and hence must then perform a com-
only the head is tilted, then both cancel exactly, plete coordinate transformation from the head
provided the responses in the first two cases are to the trunk. Yet they also possess a second
the same for the same tilt angle (Figure 6, upper gravity sense organ in the trunk which is used in
diagram; throughout this article "position" sitting and walking.
means the angular relation of a part of the body Thus, for gravity orientation in t1ight the
to another, whereas "posture" means its relation otoliths are necessary and, possibly, also suffi-
to the direction of gravity, and "tilt" means a cient, whereas for gravity orientation in sitting
deviation from its upright posture). and walking, the truncal graviceptors are suffi-
Later on, I found that this invariance of the cient and, possibly, also necessary.
trunk response under head rotation is preserved From then on I wondered whether a similar
Spatial Perception and Control 289

f$ '* d = '*-h*

~
h w

h h*

h d = sin I . cos h-cos I . sin h

cos I . si n h

Figure 6. Models of the basic information processing structure of the wing reflexes of Figure 5. Upper diagram:
Subtraction of central representations I. and h. of the angles I and h, derived from the otolithic and the neck af-
ferences, respectively. Lower diagram: Cross-multiplication of utricular and saccular afferences with the re-
spective sine and cosine components of the neck afference and subsequent subtraction of the products. Note
that the resulting invariance of the wing response under head tilts is then preserved even under varying
G-Ioads.

situation might exist in other vertebrates as well a centrifuge that had been designed in the six-
and, possibly, even in man. And this led to the ties by Howard C. Howland. He had provided it
topic, taken up in 1988 (after investigations on with a horizontal platform" and this gave us] the
bicomponent control, idiothetic orientation, path opportunity to fit it with a sled that can be
integration, and the visual vertical) that will shifted radially by remote control (Figure 7).
now be treated in some detail. In the traditional centrifuge paradigms, the
resulting force acts normal or parallel to the
subject's z-axis, that is, on the otoliths and the
Somatic Graviceptors putative somatic graviceptors in the same direc-

Existence Proof
I "We" in the following includes my longtime coworkers
Paradigms and methods. To prove the exist- Evi Fricke, Willi Jensen, Reinhard Strobele, and Karl
ence of graviceptors outside the labyrinth I used Fischer.
290 H. Mittelstaedt

wards the feet, and hence they indicate a head-


up deviation from the horizontal.
But we first need to know whether the sub-
jects are, in fact, able to estimate their subjec-
tive horizontal posture (SHP) accurately. To
that end we tested our subjects on a tiltable
board (Figure 8). The experimenter sets the ini-
tial tilt in total darkness to arbitrary, yet strictly
up-down alternating angles and then asks the
subject to rotate the board until she or he feels
horizontal. The angle E between the subject's
z-axis and the horizontal plane is measured by
means of a potentiometer.
The subject has been instructed not to make
cognitive inferences, but is told, "Just act as you
feel-as you are used to doing when sitting or
standing upright in the dark". At the start of a
session, the experimenter tilts the board (with
constant velocity of 0.5°/s) and signals the sub-
ject to take control of the board. When the sub-
ject has signaled that he or she feels horizontal,
the experimenter records the setting and tilts the
board by an arbitrary amount in the opposite
Figure 7. Subjective horizontal position (SHP) on a direction; and so forth. As a rule a session is ter-
rotating sled centrifuge. The subject is pi aced right minated after 8 pairs of settings, but only the
ear down on a sled that can be moved radially along
the subject's spinal (z-) axis via remote control by the
last 6 pairs are used to compute mean and vari-
subject (as well as by the experimenter). Due to the ance of E as the "subjective horizontal position"
additional acceleration, the subject feels tilted de- (SHP). On average, subjects are able to assume
pending on the distance between the gravity sense
organ(s) and the centrifuge axis, and moves the sled
their personal SHP with an SD of ± 1 degree.
until she feels horizontal. In this subjectively horizon- In a subsequent session on the centrifuge, the
tal position (SHP), the distance d of the centrifuge same procedure is followed with the subject
axis from the binaural axis is then recorded (negative,
if the former is caudal of the latter).
placed in the same way on the sled (Figure 7),
which can, by remote control, be moved radi-
ally over its horizontal surface (sled velocity:
2.2 crnls). In all experiments, the centrifuge is
tion and by the same or nearly the same amount, run with w = 2 'IT times 0.6 rotations per second
respectively. Hence, the resulting phenomena and hence produces 0.0145 G per cm radius.
do not tell us whether they are caused by the Sled position is measured as the distance d of
otoliths or the somatic graviceptors or both. the centrifuge axis from the binaural axis, posi-
But when subjects are oriented earth-hori- tive if the centrifuge axis is cranial of the binau-
zontally and (as in Figure 7) so that the centrifu- ral axis (see Figure 7).
gal force acts along their z-axis, extravestibular
graviceptors must reveal themselves if they Basic results. On the tiltable board, most sub-
have any effect on the perception of posture. jects show an idiosyncratic deviation (E) of their
If the centrifuge axis is collinear with the z-axis from the horizontal plane (upwards posi-
binaural axis (see Figure 7, middle panel), the tive). These deviations are small, but fairly con-
otoliths are not affected by the centrifugal force; stant even over years. In our 32 normal subjects
hence they indicate a horizontal posture. Yet (leg position as in Figure 8) they are normally
graviceptors in the body are then, in addition to distributed about a mean of zero with a standard
gravity, subjected to a force that is directed to- deviation of E = ±3.2°.
Spatial Perception and Control 291

Figure 8. The subjective horizontal position (SHP) on a tiltable board. The subject, head fixed by means of a
bite board, lies on a padded board that may be rotated by remote control. In total darkness, the subject, starting
from alternating head-up or head-down positions, rotates the board until he or she feels horizontal. The SHP is
measured as the elevation (€) of the subject's z-axis from the horizontal plane, head-up being positive.

Obviously, these deviations will also bias the others set the sled more caudally than expected
centrifuge data, hence, they must be accounted under exclusively otolithic control. In normals,
for in their evaluation: Consider a subject who the line of least square distance (LSD) crosses
feels horizontal at a positive elevation E on the the ordinate (e = E = 0) at d = -25 em, whereas
board. If the otoliths alone would determine the in bilaterally neuromectomized subjects, the
perception of posture, this subject would feel intercept of the LSD is at d = -45 cm, that is , in
horizontal on the centrifuge at a positive dis- the area of the last ribs (note that the LSD is to
tance d (as in the upper picture of Figure 7). apply here, instead of the regression line, because
How do we determine that expected distance? e and d are both dependent variables).
Our centrifuge produces 1G at 69 cm distance The result proves the existence of somatic
from the centrifuge axis. Hence, at the site of graviceptors, and it indicates that their mass
the otoliths, the vector resulting on the centrifuge centroid lies near the last ribs. Moreover, the
has the SaIne tilt angle as on the board at a distance slopes of the LSD's in Figure 9 are near unity,
e of 69 em times tan E. Now we have good rea- and, hence, become virtually zero if the e-val-
son to suppose that the subjects. when asked to ues are subtracted from the d-values. and the
orient themselves horizontally, use only the z-axis difference ~d - e) i~ plotted over l
component of the vector and hence fee] hori- Hence. this difference meaSlU'es an effect of the
zontal when e is equal to 69 cm times sin E. Be- somatic graviceptors that is independent of the in-
cause the sine and the tangent are virtually iden- dividual deviation of the subjective from the ob-
tical at the small elevations found in our subjects, jective horizontal: It measures the gain indepen-
our conclusions are independent of whether the dently of the bias. The relative weight W of the
former or the latter is actually realized. somatic graviceptors in the perception of the
In Figure 9, the distance d measured on the horizontal posture may thus be determined by
centrifuge is plotted over the distance e ex- dividing the difference d - e by the distance de
pected under exclusively otolithic control. of their mass centroid from the meatus. Thus W
Hence, if that would be the case, all points is defined as ·(d - e)/de, with de assumed to be
should lie along the dotted line (d = e). Yet the on average at d = - 50 cm. It is a nondimen-
settings of only two subjects come near it. All sional number expected to range from zero (ex-
292 H. Mittelstaedt

d [em]
.
20

10
."
.. -.. '
.' . .' . .+
.
.. ' .. '
~ o

- 0 - - S 113
Normal
Neuromect.

' . " " - " ' . x=y

-10

-20

-30

-40

-50

-60~----~----~----------~-------~----r-----r-----~--~

-10 -5 o 5 10 15 20 25 30
o
e [em]
o o o o
-8.3 o 8.3 16.5 25.8 E [deg]

Figure 9. SHP of 32 normal and 5 labyrinthine-defective subjects, right ear down, legs flexed, on a tiltable board
and on a sied centrifuge, which produces about 1.5 G per meter radius in the direction of the subject's spinal (z-)
axis. Ordinate: distance d of the centrifuge axis from the subject's binaural axis where he or she feels horizontal
(see Figure 7). Abscissa: distance e computed from the subjectively horizontal tilt angle E on the board; e 69 =
cm*sin(e), that is, the distance where the subjects would set their binaural axis if the otoliths alone would con·
trol the SHP. Brackets represent SO. Lines of least square distance (LSD) are fitted to the settings of the two
=
groups. Note that one subject (8113) sets herself almost at d e (in two sessions 4 years apart), 3 subjects be-
have like subjects without otoliths, and the rest show compromise positions.

clusive control of the SHP by otoliths) to unity the inevitable support forces. But if they are act-
(exclusive control by somatic graviceptors). W ing exclusively on the soles, or exclusively on
does not measure the efficacy of the gravicep- the pelvis, thus stressing or straining the legs, no
tors in general however: On the subjective vi- change of the distance d on the centrifuge is ob-
sual vertical (SVV), the somatic graviceptors served (13). When the legs are flexed, however,
have no (or even a slight inverting) effect (13). that is, hips and knees bent by about 90 degrees,
All 32 normal subjects, that is, also the three subjects show a head-up increase of the elevation
who behave like the neuromectomized ones in E on the board and a decrease of the absolute dis-
Figure 10, have a normal SVV, that is, well- tance between centrifuge axis and binaural axis
functioning otoliths. on the centrifuge (that is, an increase of the d-val-
ues and the e-values; compare Figure 11). Again,
this is not due to the support forces to buttocks or
Localization of the Somatic Graviceptors soles, because the d-values are unaffected by the
addition of lead cuffs (13).
Since the area of the last ribs is not too far This result opens the possibility of manipu-
from the mass centroid of the entire body, the ef- lating the effect of the somatic graviceptors. By
fect of centrifugation might be caused by mecha- testing normals and selected patients with legs
noreceptors in the skin or in the legs, affected by extended and flexed on board and centrifuge,
Spatial Perception and Control 293

20 o Normal
d-e[cm] • Neuromect
- 0 - - S 113
10

o ------.----~---I·:--------------~-~-·----·--+-----------------.-­

~1 0
~ :f m~
~20

~30
:a~ t
-40

-50

-60-+----~-----+----~----~----~----~------~--~

-10 -5 o 5 10 15 20 25 30 e [em]
o o o o
~8.3 8.3 16.5 25.8 £ [deg]

Figure 10. The same tests and subjects as in Figure 9, but with the difference d - e plotted over e. The slopes
of all lines of least square distance (LSD) are now virtually zero, indicating that the force-dependent gain on the
centrifuge is largely independent of the force-independent bias on the board. Note that subjects without otoliths
set d - e between -45 and -55 cm, hence, at the site of the centroid of the mass(es) governing the somatic
graviceptors.

the location of the somatic graviceptors can be the vertebrae are excluded as a source because
narrowed down. otherwise, due to the strictly segmental innerva-
Paraplegic patients with total bilateral le- tion of both, deficits should increase and the rel-
sions from the 5th lumbar to the 6th cervical ative weight W should decrease with the height
segment (inclusive) divide into two groups of the lesion.
which are clearly different in their settings from There are (at least) two separate graviceptive
each other but homogeneous in themselves inputs:
(Figure 12). Subjects with lesion between the
5th lumbar and the 12th thoracic segment (in- 1. The first input enters the cord at the 1 ] th tho-
clusive) behave like normals in all four tests. racic dorsaJ root. Since at this point the renal
Subjects of the remaining group, with the le- nerve enters the cord, bilmeraliy nephrecto-
sion between C6 and Thll, called TC-paraple- mized subjects were recruited, and indeed
gics, do not show an effect of leg flexion on the found to behave, in all four tests, like TC-
tiltable board (compare Figure 12), yet they still paraplegics (Figure 14).
exhibit a relatively large one on the centrifuge
(Figure 13). The mean relative weight W is 2. The second input enters the brain cranial of
smaller than in normals, but far from zero. In the 6th cervical segment. Candidates for de-
both groups there is no conelation between the livering information from the trunk to the
individual values of Wand the height of the brain at a point higher than that are the N.
lesion. vagus and the N. phrenicus. So far no sub-
These results afford the following conclu- ject with lesions of these nerves has been
sions: Mechanoreceptors in the skin or between available.
294 H. Mittelstaedt

• board ......... y=1.52x+3.74


o centrifuge ------- y=1.20x+13.57

.. .'

: .
I

o ~'--------------~(~~J'----------~'--,r,~-----------------
'-' 0' :'.
0' (J.:'

-15

normal

-30

-45 ,,
,
, ,, <"0

If
e,d [em]
1ong
-60 ~.-~---'----"------'-----+----r-------"

-60 -45 -30 -1 5 o 15 30


Figure 11. SHP of normal subjects on the tiltable board (e) and on the sled centrifuge (d) with legs flexed (~ong,
d tong ) on the abcissa and with legs flexed (eShorh dshort) on the ordinate. Note that the d-values are far from the
e-values, the "short" values generally larger than the respective "long" values, and the slopes of the lines of
least square distance (LSD) larger than zero.

Hypotheses somatic graviceptors, the weight W would be-


come zero in nephrectomized and TC-paraple-
The source of the input at the j 1th thoracic gic subjects. If not, it should at least be reduced,
segment. The results on the nephrectomized and this is indeed the case. However, because
subjects prove that the kidneys contribute to this reduction is significant only in TC-paraple-
gravity perception. It is an open question, how- gics with flexed legs, the hypothesis needs fur-
ever, whether they function like statoliths or af- ther corroboration.
fect postural perception in another way, for ex-
ample, by changing the parameters of the The source of the input cranial of the 6th cervi-
gravitational control systems. Yet, the prerequi- cal segment. The existence of a second input
sites for a measurement of inertial force are in- follows from the residual weight (W > > 0) in
deed given: A density difference between the TC-paraplegics. A hypothesis about its origin is
kidney and the surrounding adipose capsule and suggested by the large significant effect of leg
a circumferential array of mechanoreceptors flexion found on the centrifuge in these sub-
that measure pressure towards the surface of the jects. Since it is still present when all afference
kidney (14,15). If these receptors were the sole from below the 5th cervical segment is lost,
Spatial Perception and Control 295

Paraplegic Patients
C4
C5
C6
C7
C8
TH1
TH2
TH3
TH4
TH5
TH6
TH7
THB
TH9
TH10
TH11
"
~t.O-
TH12 "
~
L1 ,I

,:
L2 "
~
L3 "
~
L4 "

L5
~
I:
"

51
,:
"

Normal

-1 0 - 5 o 5 10 1 5 20
Figure 12. Dependence of the effect of leg flexion on the height of the lesion in paraplegics, compared to that in
normals (lowest line). Ordinate: Segments of the spinal cord; C: cervical, Th: thoracic, L: lumbar segments.
Points indicate site of lesion (hence an L 1 paraplegia is denoted by a point between Th12 and L 1). Abscissa:
eshort- el ong • Brackets represent SO. Note that this difference is virtually zero and is independent of the height of
lesion from C6 to Th11, yet is indistinguishable from normal, and is highly significantly different from zero in
subjects with lesion from Th12 to L5. An intermediate difference is shown by a paraplegic with loss at Th10 on
one side and Th12 on the other side, indicated by hemicircles.
296 H. Mittelstaedt

• board y=1,02x+O.34
o centrifuge --- .. -- y=O.75x+2.98
20

.'.,'
e,d short[cm]

~
10


0

-10
Te-paraplegics

o
-20 o
o
-30

-40

-50 V
-50 -40 -30 -20 -10 o
e,d 1o n 9 [cm]

10 20
Figure 13. SHP of subjects with complete paraplegia at Th11 to C6 (TC-paraplegics). All conditions and sym-
bols as in Figure 11. Note that 1) the differences eshort - elong are virtually zero, but 2) the d-values are still far
from the e-values, the dshort-values are larger than the dlong-values, and the slope of their LSD is less than that of
the normals. The first indicates the loss of a somatic graviceptive input entering the cord at Th11, the second
the existence of a second input entering the cord or the brain cranial of CG.

only a t1uid distributed throughout the body ceptors of the required type m the ligaments
could possibly produce it. I presumed that this (16,17).
t1uid is the blood in the large vessels, and that
its mass exerts inertial forces on the ligaments
that support the vessels against the gravitational Crucial Tests of Vascular Graviception
load. When the legs are t1exed, the mass cen-
troid of the large vessels, necessarily, is shifted Positive pressure to the legs (PPL). The pre-
craniad, and so is the centrifuge axis in TC- sumed vascular graviception may be crucially
paraplegics. But on the board, the force exerted tested by shifting blood from the legs into the
by the mass of the blood is independent of the trunk by means of an antishock suit that applies
location of the centroid. Hence, leg flexion positive air pressure of 45 mm Hg exclusively
should have no effect. As in the case of the kid- to the legs (the procedure is named PPL). On
neys, the prerequisites for graviception are the centrifuge, the resulting shift of the mass
given: a density difference between the blood centroid should cause a shift of the centrifuge
and the surrounding medium, and mechanore- axis towards the binaural axis. And, indeed, this
Spatial Perception and Control 297

• board ......... y=1 .01 x-0.07


o centrifuge ------- y=0.82x-1.43
20
e,ds ho rt [cm]

~
10
nephrectomized subjects

0
o
-10

o
-20

-30

-40

-50

-60~----~----~----~--~----~-----+----~--~

-60 - 5 0 -40 - 30 -20 -10 o 10 20


Figure 14. SHP of bilaterally nephrectomized subjects. Conditions and symbols as in Figure 11. Note that, as in
TC-paraplegics, leg flexion has no effect on the tiltable board, and the slopes of the LSD's are similar to those of
Figure 13, yet distinctly different from those of the normal subjects (compare Figure 11).

happens (Figure 15). But on the board, PPL, ternative explanation. At any rate, the results
just as leg flexion, should have no effect. This is are well in agreement with the hypothesis that
actually found in subjects who orient them- the gravitational load on the large blood vessels
selves accurately under normal pressure. How- is used in graviception.
ever, as shown in Figure 15, whereas the inter- V\1 e should expect then, that negative pres-
cept of the LSD leads virtually througb the sure to the 0;' to the entire lower
origin, its slope is larger than unity, that is, de- body (LBNP) elicits sensations of tilt in most
viations from e = 0 present at norma] pressure subjects. Astoundingly, in the numerous inves-
are an1plified about 1.4 times by PPL. Rather tigations under application of LBNP and NPL,
than due to an amplification of the bias, this such experiences appear to have been noticed
may be due to a reduction of the relative weight and published only twice. In an experimental
of the vascular graviceptors, possibly caused by study from 1971 (18), they are qualitatively de-
a reduction of the force exerted by the blood's scribed, but not measured. A comprehensive re-
mass, for PPL leads to outward filtration, and view (19; see page 578) relates, without citing
this, in turn, reduces the relevant blood volume. the source, that sensations of tilt have been re-
But a central nervous attenuation of the relative ported by some subjects under LBNP in a su-
weight is a possible (inclusive or exclusive) al- pine posture. Therefore, it appears necessary as
298 H. Mittelstaedt

• board ......... y=1.35x-O.70

o centrifuge ------- y=O.82x+1.67


20
e,d pressu re[cm]
,. "
10

t subjects under leg pressure

o +-------------------------------~~..-----------
x=y

-10

o
:
-20

-40

e,dcontrol[cm]
-50~----~----~----~------~----+-----'-----~

-50 -40 - 30 -20 -10 o 10 20


Figure 15.. SHP of normal subjects, legs extended, under positive air pressure to the legs (PPL) of 45 mm Hg.
The control values under normal pressure (e,dcontrol) are plotted on the abscissa, the respective values with
pressure (e,dpressure) on the ordinate. Note that the result resembles that of the TC paraplegics (compare Figure
13), except that the slope of the LSD of the e-values is larger than unity.

well as promising to test the SHP under nega- than unity, namely, 0.85, hence virtually identi-
tive and positive pressure to the entire lower cal to that under PPL seen above; also, under
body (LBNP and LBPP, respectively). LBNP, it is not significantly different from that
seen under positive pressure. On the tiltable
Pressure to the lower body (LBPPINP). This board, the slope of the LSD is larger than unity
project2 is not yet finished, but the effect of con- under LBPP, just as under PPL, and likewise,
stant LBPP and LBNP of 30 mm Hg on the SHP albeit still somewhat larger, under LBNP. Thus,
is strong and significant, although, especially on not only positive but also negative pressure ap-
the tiltable board, the correlations are consider- pears to reduce the relative weight of these
ably smaller and the variances greater than un- graviceptors. So far, the result not only con-
der PPL (Figures 16 and 17): On the centrifuge firms the above hypothesis, but also constrains
the slope of the LSD under LBPP is smaller its specification.
However, to our surprise, the LSD does not
pass through the origin, but is shifted by a large
2This project has been planned and executed with the
positive or negative amount, respectively, that
collaboration of Dieter Vaitl and Friedhelm Baisch and is, the e-values show a pressure-dependent bias,
supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft. even in subjects who oriented themselves hori-
:$patial Perception and Control 299

o o o o o
-5 0
o 5 10 15 20
3 0 -,---L...----+_ _ _--L-_ _ _ L-.._~---L---..L..,

,, Co
c:
w'"
c..
, 20 0 e;J
I

20
I

o
10

o
5

0
-r-------.~--+_~~------~--~----------~----------+_ 0

0
-5
n=21 II

-1 0 •
0
e pp } normal
e np
Ss

ep p =1.46*e zp 1 +6.37
enp =1.67*ezp2 -10,91

e z p 1 ,e z p 2

- 1 0 - 5 o 5 10 1 5 2 0 2 5

e pp}
en p
Nephrec. Ss •o :p p}
np
Lab.def.S

Figure 16. SHP on the tiltable board of normal, nephrectomized and labyrinthine-defective subjects under con-
stant positive (filled symbols) or negative (open symbols) pressure to the lower body (±30 mm Hg). The e-val-
ues under experimental pressure (epp , enp , respectively) are plotted over the e-values under normal pressure
(ezp 1, ezp 2, respectively) measured in a control series preceding each experimental series. Dotted and dash-dot-
ted lines represent LSD.

zontally under normal pressure (Figure 16). A mean total effect of the pressure difference on
similar bias appears also on the centrifuge (Fig- the board (e pp - e np ) is quantitatively similar to
ure 17). If both have a common cause they that on the centrifuge (dpp - dnp ), its individual
should be strongly correlated. Yet, although the value is not correlated to the latter's in our 21
300 H. Mittelstaedt

Co
2 0 c
"0
C-
o.
"0

1 0

o ~----------------------------~r---r-=-~----*---~-------------
/

- 1 0

- 2 0

/
- 3 0

d zp1 ,d ZP2
-5 O~~ro-r-r~~-r'-~-.-r.-.,-r~.-~-r.-~-r~.-~-r~~

- 5 0 - 4 0 - 3 0 - 2 0 - 1 0 o 1 0 2 0

dpp =O.8S*d zp1 +S.91


dn p =O.97*d zP2 -S.83

Figure 17. SHP on the centrifuge of the normal subjects of Figure 16 under the same protocol as there, exe-
cuted after a respite period on the same day, and plotted with the same outlay as there.

subjects. And this means, presumably, that still have also shown that its dynamic properties are
other graviceptive inputs exist that are affected apt to entail interesting consequences for the
by pressure to the abdomen and that act differ- generation of motion sickness.
ently on board and centrifuge. However that may turn out to be, the results
Two possible sources corne to mind: the hy- under altered air pressure show a strong influ-
drostatics of the blood and the mass of the entire ence of the inertia, the distribution, and possibly
abdominal viscera. The latter has recently been also the hydrostatics of the blood on somatic
suggested by von Gierke and Parker (20), who graviception. Thus, the well-known fact that
Spatial Perception and Control 301

posture affects the vascular system is comple- The Role of Proprioceptors


mented by its reverse, namely, that the vascular
system, via perception and control, affects pos- Proprioceptors in the legs and the skin nota-
ture too. bly playa major role in motor control. Although
the central nervous variables that govern the
perception of posture and those that govern its
control need not be identical, this is often im-
Counterintuitive Insights and plicitly or explicitly assumed. However, whereas
Open Questions the control of posture is shown to be critically
dependent on proprioceptive input from the legs
The Visual and the Postural Vertical (compare, for example, reference 24), the per-
ception of posture turns out to be totally unaf-
In man the vertical is the quintessential ex-
fected by it (compare reference 13 and above).
trinsic reference for perception and action. Even Thus, leg proprioceptors may affect control but
if its measurement, computation, and represen-
not perception of posture, or may affect it only
tation is based on many different sensory cues
indirectly via their effect on leg position; or, al-
and central nervous processes, all measure-
ternatively, they may affect dynamic but not
ments should be averaged to yield a single end static behavior, vertical but not horizontal pos-
result for all modalities of perception, and this
ture, or perception of motion but not of tilt.
is implicitly or explicitly assumed in the tradi-
tion. It turns out, however, that the visual and
the postural vertical are based on separate com- Survival Value
putations leading to separate end results. Both
are sub served by the otoliths, but the visual ver- Lastly, what may be the survival value of so-
tical is also largely affected by a tendency to matic graviceptors?
align with the z-axis (due to the idiotropic vec-
1. They may obviate the head-to-trunk coordi-
tor; compare reference 21), but not (or only
nate transformation.
marginally and even inversely) by the somatic
graviceptors (compare reference 13); whereas 2. They may shorten the latency of reactions to
the postural vertical is also largely influenced trunk tilts, possibly even allowing monosyn-
by the somatic graviceptors (compare refer- aptic reflexes.
ences 22 and 23 and above), but not by the idio-
3. They may have dynamic properties that are
tropic vector (compare reference 21). Except in
complementary to those of the otoliths.
special cases, for example, at or near the upright
posture, the resulting directions differ, often by Thus, many interesting and important ques-
large amounts. tions remain to be answered.

REFERENCES
1. Mittelstaedt H. Physiologie des Gleichgewichtssinnes 6. Mittelstaedt H. Zur Analyse physiologischer Rege'-
bei fliegenden Libellen. Dissertation. Heidelberg, 1949. lungssysteme. Verhandlungen der Deutschen Zoologis-
2. Mittelstaedt H. Physiologie des Gleichgewichtssinnes chen Gesellschaft in Wilhelmshaven 1951. ZooJogischer
bei fliegenden Libellen. Z vergleichende PhysioJ Anzeiger 16. Supplementband J952. p 150-7.
1950;32:422-63. 7. Sperry RW. Neural basis of the spontaneous optoki-
3. Mittelstaedt H. Telotaxis und Optomotorik von Erista- netic response produced by visual inversion. J Comp
lis bei Augeninversion. N aturewissenschaften 1949; PhysioJ PsychoJ 1950;43 :482-9.
36:90-3. 8. Mittelstaedt H. The analysis of behaviour in terms of
4. Kornmtiller AE. Eine experimentelle Anastesie der control systems. In: Schaffner B, ed. Group Processes
auBeren Augenmuskeln eines Menschen und ihre (Transactions of the Fifth Conference, October 1958).
Auswirkungen. Psychol Neurol 1931 ;41 :354-66. New York: The Josiah Macy, Jr. Foundation; 1960:
5. von Holst E, Mittelstaedt H. Das Reafferenzprinzip 45-84.
(Wechselwirkung zwischen Zentralnervensystem und 9. Mittelstaedt H. Reafferenzprinzip-Apologie und Kri-
Peripherie). Naturwissenschaften 1950;37(20):464-76. tik. In: Keidel WD, Plattig KH, eds. Vortrage der Erlanger
302 H. Mittelstaedt

Physiologentagung 1970. Heidelberg: Springer Verlag; 17. Kostreva DR, Pontus SP. Hepatic vein, hepatic paren-
1971 : 161-71. chymal, and inferior vena caval mechanoreceptors with
10. Mittelstaedt H. Basic solutions to the problem of head- phrenic afferents. Am J Physiol 1993b; 265 (Gas-
centric visual localization. In: Warren R, Wertheim AH, trointestinal and Liver Physiology 1993b; 28):G 15-G20.
eds. Perception and control of self-motion. Hillsdale 18. Musgrave FS, Zechman FW, Mains RC. Comparison of
NJ: Erlbaum; 1990:267-87. the effects of 70° tilt and several levels of lower body
11. Trendelenburg W. Ober die Bewegung der Vogel nach negative pressure on heart rate and blood pressure in
Durchschneidung hinterer Ruckenmarkswurzeln: ein man. Aerospace Med 1971;42:1065-9.
Beitrag zur Physiologie des Zentralnervensystems der 19. Wo1thuis RG, Bergman SA, Nicogossian AE. Physio-
Vogel (nach Untersuchungen an Columba domestica). logical effects of locally applied reduced pressure in
Archiv der Anatomie und Physiologie, Leipzig (Physi- man. Physiol Rev 1974;54:566-95.
ologie) 1906:1-126. 20. von Gierke HE, Parker DE. Differences in otolith and
12. Mittelstaedt H. Basic control patterns of orientational abdominal viscera graviceptor dynamics: implications
homeostasis. Symp Soc Exp Bioi [964; 18:365-85. for motion :;ickness and perceived body position. Aviat
13. Mittelstaedt H, Fricke E. The relative effect of saccular Space Environ Med 1994;65:747-51.
and somatosensory information on spatial perception 21. Mittelstaedt H. A new solution to the problem ofthe sub-
and control. Adv Oto Rhino Laryngol 1988;42:24--30. jective vertical. Naturwissenschaften 1983 ;70:272-8l.
14. Gilmore JP, Tomomatsu E. Renal mechanoreceptors in 22. Mittelstaedt H. Somatic versus vestibular gravity recep-
nonhuman primates. Am J Physiol 1985;248: (Regula- tion in man. Ann NY Acad Sci 1992;656: 124--39.
tory Integrative Comparative Physiology, 17) R202- 23. Mittelstaedt H. Evidence for somatic graviception from
R207. new and classical investigations. Acta Otolaryngol
15. Niijima A. Observation on the localization of mechan- (Stockh) 1995;Supplement [in press].
oreceptors in the kidney and afferent nerve fibers in the 24. Hlavatska F, Krizkova M, Horak FB. Modification of
renal nerves in the rabbit. J PhysioI1975;245:81-90. human postural response to leg muscle vibration by
16. Kostreva DR, Pontus SP. Pericardial mechanoreceptors electrical vestibular stimulation. Neurosci Lett 1995;
with phrenic afferents. Am J Physiol1993a; 264 (Heart 189:9-12.
and Circulation Physiology 1993a;33):H1836-46.

You might also like