You are on page 1of 12

Ecography 39: 001–012, 2016

doi: 10.1111/ecog.02191
© 2016 The Authors. Ecography © 2016 Nordic Society Oikos
Subject Editor: Thorsten Wiegand. Editor-in-Chief: Miguel Araújo. Accepted 2 August 2016

Exploring the interaction of avian frugivory and plant spatial


heterogeneity and its effect on seed dispersal kernels using a
simulation model

Andrew P. McKenzie Pegman, George L. W. Perry and Mick N. Clout­


A. P. McK. Pegman (andrewpegman@xtra.co.nz), Laingholm, Auckland, New Zealand. – G. L. W. Perry and APMcKP, School of Environment,
The Univ. of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand. – M. N. Clout (m.clout@auckland.ac.nz), School of Biological Sciences, The Univ. of Auckland,
Auckland, New Zealand.­

Seed dispersal by avian frugivores is one of the key processes influencing plant spatial patterns, but may fail if there
is disruption of plant–frugivore mutualisms, such as decline in abundance of dispersers, fragmentation of habitat, or
isolation of individual trees. We used simulation model experiments to examine the interaction between frugivore density
and behaviour and the spatial arrangement of fruiting plants and its effect on seed dispersal kernels. We focussed on two
New Zealand canopy tree species that produce large fruits and are dispersed predominantly by one avian frugivore
(Hemiphaga novaeseelandiae). Although the mean seed dispersal distance decreased when trees became more aggregated,
there were more frugivore flights between tree clusters, consequently stretching the tails of the dispersal kernels. Conversely,
when trees were less aggregated in the landscape, mean dispersal distances increased because seeds were deposited over larger
areas, but the kernels had shorter tails. While there were no statistically meaningful changes in kernel parameters when
frugivore density changed, decreases in density did cause a proportional reduction in the total number of dispersed seeds.
However, birds were forced to move further when fruit availability and fruit ripening were low. Sensitivity analysis showed
that dispersal kernels were primarily influenced by the model parameters relating to disperser behaviour, especially those
determining attractiveness based on distance to candidate fruiting trees. Our results suggest that the spatial arrangement
of plants plays an important role in seed dispersal processes – although tree aggregation curbed the mean seed dispersal
distance, it was accompanied by occasional long distance events, and tree dispersion caused an increase in mean dispersal
distance, both potentially increasing the probability of seeds finding suitable habitats for germination and growth. Even
though low frugivore densities did not cause dispersal failure, there were negative effects on the quantity of seed dispersal
because fewer seeds were dispersed.

Dispersal of seeds, for example by avian frugivores, in large be conducted using simulation models in order to recon-
part determines the spatial pattern of seedling recruitment struct seed dispersal distributions and derive dispersal
and is essential for colonisation of landscapes via long kernels (including parameter values) directly from informa-
distance events (Wenny 2000, Santamaria et  al. 2007). tion describing the natural history of seed production and
However, seed dispersal may fail if plant-disperser mutual- behaviour of dispersal agents. The seed dispersal kernel is the
isms are disrupted, for example, via decline in abundance of probability of a seed being deposited at a particular distance
keystone seed dispersers or isolation of trees (Cordeiro and from the parent plant (Nathan and Muller-Landau 2000)
Howe 2003, Sekercioglu et  al. 2004, Nathan 2005). One and is described in terms of its essential parameters: 1) the
way of testing this assertion is by comparing empirical seed mean seed dispersal distance, and 2) the rate at which the
distributions, obtained by matching dispersed propagules tail of the distribution decays with distance. In the Weibull
with their parents, across different plant–frugivore scenarios distribution, for example, mean seed dispersal distance and
(Bullock et  al. 2006, Cortes and Uriarte 2013). However, its variance set the spatial ‘scale’ of dispersal and ‘shape’, the
such data are difficult to obtain because long distance seed inverse function of the kurtosis of the kernel, indicates long
dispersal is challenging to quantify (Robledo-Arnuncio distance seed dispersal when it is numerically low (Morales
et al. 2014) and deposition patterns emerge from multiple and Carlo 2006). Seed dispersal effectiveness incorporates
interactions between frugivore behaviour and the distribu- these parameters and is the product of the total number of
tion of fruit, both of which vary spatially and temporally dispersed seeds and the probability that a dispersed seed
(Levey et al. 2008, Cortes and Uriarte 2013). As an alterna- produces a new adult, all of which influence demographic
tive, virtual in silico (Zurell et  al. 2010) experiments can features of plant populations, such as the rate and pattern of

Early View (EV): 1-EV


spread and colonisation of new areas (Schupp 1993, Cousens such as open husks and teeth marks; however, this is again
and Rawlinson 2001, Levin et al. 2003, Spiegel and Nathan infrequent and Pegman (2012) showed that only 2.2  1.3%
2007, Schupp et al. 2010). of dispersed P. ferruginea seeds (n  434 seeds across 6 trees)
To quantify the effect of disruption of plant–frugivore had open husks, and mammal damage to dispersed V. lucens
mutualisms via decline in frugivore density or changes in seeds was not observed.
tree spatial patterns, we developed a spatially explicit indi- We utilised these two tree species because 1) they are
vidual-based simulation model (SEIBM, DeAngelis and the two large-fruited NZ species for which there are data
Mooij 2005) combining disperser foraging and movement to parameterize our model, 2) they differ in their envi-
rules with data describing frugivore gut passage time of ronmental requirements, therefore not growing together,
seeds and individual tree distributions. We varied frugivore 3) they have different reproductive strategies, and 4) they
density across different spatial patterns of trees to evalu- have differing spatial distributions. Mean annual fruit pro-
ate how seed dispersal kernels were affected by changes in duction for P. ferruginea (ca 5000) is around twice that of
frugivore density or by the effects of forest structure on fru- V. lucens, and the proportion of annual fruit production that
givore movements. Based on our results, we discuss what the is actively dispersed by H. novaeseelandiae is ca 46% for both
demographic effects might be for the tree species we con- tree species (Pegman 2012). Field measurements showed
sider. We applied our model to Hemiphaga novaeseelandiae that the distance between P. ferruginea trees (30.2  6.1
(New Zealand pigeon or kereru, Columbidae), which m, n  3 sites) is around twice the respective value for
is endemic to New Zealand (NZ) and has a conserva- V. lucens (Pegman 2012). The two tree species also have
tion dependent but increasing status (Clout et  al. 1991, different shaped seeds (V. lucens is irregular, whereas P.
Miskelly et  al. 2008, Kelly et  al. 2010). This large frugi- ferruginea is smooth), affecting frugivore gut passage time
vore is the only widespread avian disperser capable of (Table 1, Supplementary material Appendix 1, Traveset
dispersing the propagules of large-fruited native trees, 1998). All of these factors may result in different outcomes
such as our exemplars, Vitex lucens (NZ teak or puriri, for each tree species in our simulations.
Verbenaceae) and Prumnopitys ferruginea (brown pine or
miro, Podocarpaceae).
Model design

Methods Our model is concerned with how patterns in seed deposi-


tion emerge from interactions between frugivore behav-
Study system and species iour and the environment, represented by different tree
spatial patterns. The model is semi-mechanistic, spatially
Hemiphaga novaeseelandiae are large (ca 650 g adult) frugivo- explicit, individual-based, stochastic, and event driven; it
rous pigeons. They live in lowland forests throughout NZ encompasses fruit production and avian frugivore forag-
and are highly mobile, inferred from inter-annual changes ing in simulated forests and was ‘built from the ground
in the numbers in northern forests and radio-telemetry up’, having fundamental elements in common with
data, but individuals can also be sedentary (Clout et  al. other models of seed dispersal such as those described
1991, Wotton 2007). Vitex lucens are large angiosperm trees by Morales and Carlo (2006) and Will and Tackenberg
up to 20 m tall, with trunks up to 1.5 m wide, occurring (2008). We implemented our model in NetLogo 5.3
mainly in temperate coastal areas of NZ (Salmon 1980). (Wilensky 1999). A complete description of our model
Hermaphroditic flowers are produced all year, with bright following the ODD (overview, design concepts, details)
red fruits being present mainly between December and June. protocol of Grimm et  al. (2010) and the code are pro-
Prumnopitys ferruginea are tall dioecious gymnosperm trees vided in the Supplementary material Appendix 1 and 2
up to 25 m tall, with trunks up to 1 m wide, occurring in (see Supplementary material Appendix 1, Fig. A1 for
shady environments throughout NZ, from lowland forest to schematic overview).
ca 1000 m elevation (Salmon 1980). Male trees have cones In brief, our model represents the movement of variable
and female trees produce large red diaspores, mainly between numbers of individual birds (in our case, H. novaeseelan-
December and June, consisting of a single seed surrounded diae) through landscapes of fixed abundance of fruiting
by fleshy ovuliferous cone scales (McEwen 1988) – for con- plants (in our case, V. lucens or P. ferruginea) whose spatial
venience, these structures will henceforth also be referred to arrangement can be varied. Individual birds select the tree
as ‘fruit’. The fruit of both tree species are long (V. lucens: they move to based on the multiplicative interaction of
15.4  0.2 mm, n  286 fruit; P. ferruginea: 16.1  0.1 mm, its available fruit and the distance from the bird’s current
n  702 fruit; Pegman 2012) and have single seeds that are location. The rationale for this interaction is that there is
hard and ‘woody’ surrounded by nutritious, edible, and a positive correlation of frugivory with fruit abundance
‘fleshy’ pulp, indicating that they are consumed by fruit- (Saracco et  al. 2005) and that frugivores prefer local,
eating animals (Coates-Estrada and Estrada 1988, Tiffney rather than more distantly located, resources. For example,
2004). Because fruits and birds vary in size within species, plants in denser neighborhoods have greater fruit removal
mid-sized birds such as tui Prosthemadera novaezelandiae or rates than more isolated plants, even when the latter have
blackbird Turdus merula can consume smaller individual desirable fruit (Levey et  al. 1984, Morales et  al. 2012).
fruits of both tree species, but this occurs infrequently (Kelly However, this rationale provides no indication about the
et al. 2010). Mammals may also act as dispersers since seeds mathematical function that most closely describes such
of our study species occasionally showed signs of predation relationships. In our model, there is one feeding bout at

2-EV
Table 1. Baseline simulation model parameters and sources.

Parameter Description Value Source


nK Number of kereru 800 or 2 ha–1 2, 4
v Mean kereru flying speed 7 m s–1 4
e Maximum gut capacity of kereru 30 fruits 4
c Max. consumption rate in hyperbolic functional response 10 fruits visit–1 3, 4
d Half saturation in hyperbolic functional response 2 fruits 3, 4
aPT Scale of gamma distribution of kereru PT 0.67 min 5
bPT Shape of gamma distribution of kereru PT 0.02 5
aGPTP Scale of gamma distribution of miro GPT 13.89 min 1
bGPTP Shape of gamma distribution of miro GPT 0.15 1
aGPTV Scale of gamma distribution of puriri GPT 4.06 min 6
bGPTV Shape of gamma distribution of puriri GPT 0.04 6
aF Factor in tree attractiveness based on number of fruits 0.001 3
bF Exponent in tree attractiveness based on number of fruits 2 3
aD Factor in tree attractiveness based on distance 5  10–5 3
bD Exponent in tree attractiveness based on distance 2 3
pLONG Probability (p) of kereru moving to a random new tree 0.01 4
rtA Roost tree attractiveness at p  0.3 0.6 4
maxFP Maximum number of miro fruits per tree 5000 4
maxFV Maximum number of puriri fruits per tree 2500 4
rF Fruit ripening rate per day 50 3, 4
nT Number of fruiting trees 1200 or 3 ha–1 4
k Number of tree clusters per landscape 120 4
sP Degree of miro tree aggregation 100 m 4
sV Degree of puriri tree aggregation 20 m 4
mF Background fruit loss per day 0.5% 4
Notes: a  scale  mean  mean/variance, b  shape  mean/variance, a  factor, b  exponent, PT  daily perching time, GPT  frugivore
gut passage time of seeds, kereru  H. novaeseelandiae, miro  P. ferruginea, puriri  V. lucens.
Sources: 1Clout and Tilley (1992), 2Greene (2003), 3Morales and Carlo (2006), 4Pegman (2012), 5Wotton (2007), 6Wotton et al. (2008).

a random point during the time spent at each tree dur- physiognomy of V. lucens and P. ferruginea, obtained through
ing a mean of 120 minutes per day of perching time experiments, field observations, and from the relevant
(Wotton 2007). A gut passage time for each feeding event literature (Table 1). We conducted a univariate sensitivity
is drawn from an appropriate probability distribution analysis, following the methods described by Hamby (1994),
(Supplementary material Appendix 1), seeds are deposited by imposing a  25% change on each specified baseline
after that time elapses, and in due course the seed disper- model input parameter and then calculating absolute ratios
sal distribution emerges from this activity (Fig. 3b). There (Sy, x) of change in kernel parameters to assess how these seed
are no interactions among birds in the model with respect dispersal characteristics changed with model parameteriza-
to their seed dispersal service (they do not flock due to tion (Table 2 and Supplementary material Appendix 4). The
current low densities, although they likely did so in the model parameters we tested were chosen because they were
past) and so the final seed deposition pattern emerges from a priori deemed to be the most relevant to the questions
independent superposition of individual dispersal events we were focussing on or were the most uncertain. Because
across all birds. The parameters of the seed dispersal kernel the sensitivity analysis suggested that it was a crucial driver
are then fitted to the simulated seed deposition pattern of the model’s dynamics, we also conducted a structural
(see below). analysis on the mathematical function governing frugivores’
Other elements in our model reflect the biology of the choice of tree based on the distance from its current loca-
systems we considered and include: 1) ‘roost’ trees i.e. tion (Supplementary material Appendix 1, Eq. A2). Since
trees that are attractive to birds but do not provide a food we were assessing whether the degree of tree aggregation
source (Beveridge 1964 and Johnson 2001 describe the use (sigma, s) was a major driver of seed distributions, we used
of Weinmannia racemosa in this way) and 2) the occasional the entire biologically reasonable range (i.e. s  2 to 100 m)
longer distance random movement by individual dispersers in the assessment of its sensitivity.
within the landscape, rather than on the basis of the attrac- We then conducted a series of in silico plant–frugivore
tiveness of trees. We also included background fruit loss experiments to explore the extent to which seed dispersal
from trees due to wind, abscission, predation, mishandling kernels are influenced by interactions between tree spa-
by birds, etc. tial patterns and frugivore density. A factorial design was
employed in which the density of H. novaeseelandiae (nK)
Model parameterisation, sensitivity and structural varied between 0.1 and 10 ha–1, representing the range of
analyses, and in silico experiments plausible values (Pegman 2012). In support of this, Greene
(2003) calculated H. novaeseelandiae densities of 0.1 to 2.6
The model parameterization was informed by data describing ha–1 in Pureora Forest Park, NZ, reflecting seasonal move-
the foraging behavior of H. novaeseelandiae and the ments linked to food availability. The degree of aggregation

3-EV
Table 2. Univariate sensitivity analysis (exemplified here by V. lucens; see Supplementary material Appendix 4 for P. ferruginea) according to
the methods of Hamby (1994). All model runs comprised 30 replicates, each of 14 d. Rank denotes rank of S ratio (the ratio of the relative
change in the state variable y for a given relative change in the value of parameter x) based on mean of the ratios. Cases where Sy, x  1 are
in bold. See Table 1 for description of baseline model parameter abbreviations.

Scale parameter Shape parameter


Baseline model parameter Sy, x (–25%) Sy, x ( 25%) Rank Sy, x (–25%) Sy, x ( 25%) Rank
bD  2 6.473 2.521 1 1.856 1.298 1
aD  5  10–5 0.634 0.426 2 0.260 0.195 2
aPT  0.67 min. 0.400 0.291 3 0.027 0.082 4
bGPT  0.04 0.414 0.243 4 0.021 0.014 9
bPT  0.02 0.313 0.314 5 0.017 0.034 5
aGPT  4.06 min 0.305 0.280 6 0.021 0.014 10
pLONG  0.01 0.105 0.134 7 0.168 0.086 3
bF  2 0.017 0.050 8 0.024 0.021 6
PT  120 min 0.052 0.014 9 0.007 0.010 13
maxF  2500 0.024 0.022 10 0.031 0.010 8
rtA  0.60 0.008 0.035 11 0.014 0.003 12
rF  50 0.021 0.022 12 0.021 0.003 11
aF  0.001 0.015 0.028 13 0.021 0.000 14
e  30 fruit 0.016 0.025 14 0.010 0.000 15
No. of roost trees  100 0.033 0.001 15 0.003 0.003 16
s  20 m 0.002 0.004 16 0.030 0.026 7

of trees (sigma, s) was varied by tightening or relaxing clus- shape (i.e. kurtosis–1; low shape values equate to high kurto-
ters in the Thomas point process, used to simulate patterns sis and ‘fat’ long tails and vice versa).
of individual trees from aggregated (low s) to diffuse (high β x β−1
s), with ca 86% of all trees of a given cluster located within f ( x | α, β ) =   e − ( x / α )β (1)
α  α
2s of the cluster centre (Illian et  al. 2008, Supplementary
material Appendix 1, Fig. A2). We varied s from 10 to 200 where x  distance from tree, a  scale parameter, and
m (representing nearest fruiting tree neighbors at ca 11 to b   shape parameter, all  0.
28 m) and held abundance constant at 1200 fruiting trees in For each analysis, we estimated the scale and shape param-
120 clusters per simulated landscape of 400 ha (i.e. 3 trees eters of the seed dispersal kernels; for these data, we were
ha–1), based on field estimates of the density of both tree most interested in the scenario analyses that used empiri-
species (Pegman 2012). We also conducted experiments to cally derived ‘field’, or ‘observed pattern’, estimates of tree
assess the combined effects of low fruit availability and low aggregations while varying disperser densities, and vice versa.
fruit ripening on the seed dispersal kernels. We also quantified the total number of dispersed seeds, seed
There were 30 simulations for each combination of fac- rain map plots, frugivore foraging ranges (via convex hulls,
tors, with each replicate comprising 120 min of daily frugi- computed in the R Spatstat library, Baddeley and Turner
vore perching time (time from arrival until departure from a 2005) and the flight distances that resulted in them, and
tree, including foraging, Wotton 2007) across 14 d, adjust- performed a spatial analysis of seed rain using a bivariate
ing frugivore gut passage time of seeds and fruit produc- pair correlation function (Illian et al. 2008) based on spatial
tion according to the tree species examined (Table 1). Our association between trees and deposition events.
analyses used seed dispersal events that occurred during the Data available from the Dryad Digital Repository:
last three days of the model runs; this minimised the risk of < http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.15d8v > (Pegman et  al.
‘burn in’ effects, but still had ample sample size for robust 2016).
parameter estimation. Each individual bird started the simu-
lation at a randomly chosen tree, and the number of fruit Results
removed from each tree and the dispersal distance of every
seed were recorded. Sensitivity and structural analyses
Seed dispersal distributions were summed across individ-
ual birds to characterize the population level pattern for each The scale and shape parameters of the seed dispersal kernels
tree species. A Weibull probability density function (Weibull were both strongly influenced by the model parameters
1951, Eq. 1) was fitted to the distributions by maximum relating to frugivore movement and also by gut passage
likelihood estimation using the ‘fitdistr’ command in the time. In particular, seed dispersal kernels were highly sensi-
MASS library in R–3.1.1 (R Development Core Team). We tive to the coefficient determining attractiveness based on
used the Weibull distribution because it is flexible, approxi- distance to candidate fruiting tree (bD, Table 2). Otherwise,
mating a number of other probability distribution functions all other model parameters were robust (absolute sensitivity
that have been used to summarize seed dispersal kernels, and values  1) and outcomes were symmetric (i.e. increases and
it quantifies the properties of dispersal kernels that we are decreases had similar relative effects). As expected, sigma was
interested in i.e. scale (mean seed dispersal distance) and robust when held constant while testing the effect of differ-

4-EV
ent frugivore densities (Table 2), but we found that there was that were less aggregated resulted in longer mean seed dis-
an increase (although less than proportional) of both shape persal distances, which were associated with high value shape
and scale parameters in both tree species when s increased parameters and kernels with short tails (Fig. 1a, b, 2a, b show
across all of its plausible values (Supplementary material results at ‘observed pattern’ bird densities; Supplementary
Appendix 3, Table A7). We assumed that there was a strong material Appendix 3, Table A3 to A6 show seed dispersal ker-
decline in attractiveness with distance by frugivores for fruit- nel parameter values obtained in different landscape scenarios
ing trees, hence we used a hyperbolic function to determine in the factorial experiments). However, the total number of
the effect of distance on tree selection; this function has been dispersed seeds was not significantly affected by the spatial
used in other similar models that have produced plausible distribution of either tree species.
biological outcomes, e. g. Morales and Carlo (2006). There Variation of disperser density did not significantly
is no empirical data on bird movements at a sufficient reso- influence mean seed dispersal kernel parameters when
lution to derive empirical fits for this chosen function, or to fruit availability was high, except in V. lucens forests at
parameterize it. However, our use of a hyperbolic function the highest simulated frugivore density (i.e. 10 ha–1), but
was justified by the fact that our frugivore foraging ranges, these comprised a difference of only a few metres and so are
and movements which resulted in them, were similar to unlikely to be ecologically important (Fig. 1c, d, 2c, d show
those derived from empirical observation; we focused on results in ‘observed pattern’ forests; Supplementary material
movement data and foraging ranges, rather than mean seed Appendix 3, Table A3 to A6 show dispersal kernel param-
dispersal distance, because there is ample empirical data for eter values obtained in different frugivore density scenarios
the first two factors (see below), against which we could vali- in the factorial experiments). The total number of dispersed
date our model, but little for the latter. Nevertheless, because seeds changed directly in proportion to H. novaeseelandiae
of the importance of this functional relationship for model density. However, inter-realisation variance of individual
results and because it utilised bD (Supplementary material kernel shape and scale parameters increased with decreasing
Appendix 1, Eq. A2), which was both a sensitive and uncer- disperser density (Fig. 1d and 2d), suggesting that in these
tain model parameter (Table 2), we conducted a structural scenarios, long distance seed dispersal events will become
analysis to assess the robustness of the model outcomes to increasingly stochastic, reducing the predictability of esti-
this decision. In the structural analysis we assessed three mates relying on it e.g. plant connectivity, spatial distribu-
other distance-attractiveness functions: 1) a linear decline, tions, and dynamics (Nathan 2005). When we concurrently
2) a negative exponential decline (to zero attractiveness at lowered fruit availability and fruit ripening, we found that
250 m), and 3) a null model (where all trees were equally seed dispersal distances increased when frugivore densities
attractive irrespective of distance). The linear and negative increased because birds had to fly further to locate fruit
exponential relationships produced similar foraging range (Supplementary material Appendix 3, Table A2, exemplified
sizes to the hyperbolic (linear  hyperbolic ∼ negative by V. lucens).
exponential), but those from a null model were much larger
(Supplementary material Appendix 3, Table A1, exemplified
by V. lucens). Morales and Carlo (2006) also rejected a null Frugivore foraging ranges
diffusion model because they found that mean seed disper-
sal distances were not linearly related to the degree of plant Under ‘observed pattern’, or baseline, conditions, our simulated
aggregation. The fact that the outcome from the negative frugivore foraging ranges (mean  one SEM  27.3  2.3
exponential is similar to the hyperbolic also suggests that our ha, statistical range  1.2 to 134.2 ha, n  100) are consis-
results do not depend on an arbitrary decision to use a spe- tent, both in area and time frame, with those determined
cific function for distance attractiveness. in other studies (Fig. 3a, b, Pierce and Graham 1995,
Bell 1996, Hill 2003, Schotborgh 2005, Campbell 2006,
Powlesland et al. 2011). For example, Hill (2003) reported
Seed dispersal kernels values from ca 14 to 704 ha, Bell (1996) reported ca 20
to 30 ha, and Schotborgh (2005) reported ca 2 to 22 ha,
Across both tree species, our simulated seed dispersal dis- all similar to our values. Powlesland et al. (2011) reported
tributions ranged between being exponential to gamma in much higher values (ca 619 to 31 732 ha) because their
appearance (Fig. 6), both of which can be described by the data captured infrequent, possibly seasonal, long-distance
Weibull distribution. The shape parameters (b), obtained by movements between distinct habitats (from mainland NZ
fitting the Weibull, corresponded to mainly fat tailed curves to an offshore island). Hemiphaga novaeseelandiae had larger
at high tree aggregation (b  1), exponential at b  1, and foraging ranges in simulated P. ferruginea landscapes than
‘hump’ shaped at low tree aggregation (b  1.1 to 1.4), but in V. lucens landscapes since the former were less aggregated
none corresponded to simple diffusion i.e. b  2. and therefore the birds had to fly further to forage. Our
Variation of the distribution of trees in the landscape trans- model produced mean flight distances between 60 and
lated into clear differences in the scale and shape parameters 100 m, consistent with a mean  one SEM of 77.0  7.0 m in
of the seed dispersal kernels. Because the movement rules are Wotton and Kelly (2012) and ca 11 to 102 m in Powlesland
fixed, these differences were the outcome of emergent inter- et  al. (2011). The short flight distances and rapid flying
actions between seed dispersers and the landscape structure. speed of frugivores (ca 7 ms–1, Pegman 2012) meant that
Increased tree aggregation resulted in shorter mean seed dis- most seeds were deposited at trees rather than during flight
persal distances and kernels with long and ‘fat’ tails, as evi- in the model, consistent with the observed behaviour of
denced by low shape parameters. Conversely, tree populations H. novaeseelandiae (McEwen 1978).

5-EV
Figure 1. Seed dispersal kernels and corresponding scale and shape parameters for V. lucens using the Weibull distribution, n  30 model
runs  14 d; (a) and (b) are across sigma (s, degree of tree aggregation) of 10 m (highly aggregated trees, dark blue) to 200 m (least
aggregated, red) at a disperser density of two H. novaeseelandiae (kereru) ha–1, and (c) and (d) are across kereru densities of 0.1 ha–1 (dark
blue) to 10 ha–1 (red) at s  20 m.

Seed rain map plots and spatial analysis 2008) based on all seed dispersal events, showed that the
distances over which seed deposition occurred with respect
In highly aggregated populations of either tree species, maps to fruiting trees increased when trees became less aggregated
of the seed rain show that deposition mainly occurred around (Fig. 6).
tree clusters, but was anisotropic and spatially non-uniform
across the landscape; the numbers of seeds deposited differed
markedly between adjacent areas, with some receiving none, Discussion
and deposition occurred only occasionally near roost trees
(Fig. 4 and 5). Empirical measurements of seed deposition We aimed to investigate how seed dispersal is influenced by
at the sites of individual fruiting trees of our study species the interaction between frugivore behaviour and plant spa-
showed that it is also anisotropic (Pegman 2012), reflect- tial patterns, using in silico experiments. We varied frugivore
ing biotic factors such as canopy shape, perching behaviour density across different spatial arrangements of large-fruited
of birds, and overlapping seed shadows, plus abiotic effects tree species prevalent in NZ temperate forests and examined
such as micro-topography (Bullock et al. 2006). the resulting changes in the main characteristics of seed dis-
When fruiting trees were less aggregated, seed rain was persal kernels, and how these might influence the dynamics
more uniform across the landscape, reflecting different pat- of plant populations.
terns of disperser movement; anisotropy was still evident, but Tree spatial distribution influenced bird movement
seed rain occurred more frequently near roost trees. In both patterns, resulting in clear differences in the main statisti-
scenarios, seed distribution patterns were independent of bird cal characteristics of the seed dispersal kernels. As forests
densities, but the total number of dispersed seeds changed became more aggregated, the mean seed dispersal distance
directly in proportion to disperser numbers. These outcomes (or scale of the dispersal kernel) decreased because birds
are in agreement with the seed dispersal kernel results pre- were restricted mainly to clusters of high tree density. One
sented above, where it was shown that tree patterns, but not outcome of this limited dispersal distance was reduced seed
bird densities, significantly influenced the main parameters deposition near roost trees. Previous simulation studies have
of seed dispersal kernels when resources were not scarce. A also suggested that clumped plant spatial patterns influ-
spatial association between trees and seed deposition events ence disperser or pollinator behaviour by confining them to
using the bivariate pair correlation function (Illian et  al. monospecific patches (Morales and Carlo 2006, Hanoteaux

6-EV
Figure 2. Seed dispersal kernels and corresponding scale and shape parameters for P. ferruginea using the Weibull distribution, n  30 model
runs  14 d; (a) and (b) are across sigma (s, degree of tree aggregation) of 10 m (highly aggregated trees, dark blue) to 200 m (least
aggregated, red) at two H. novaeseelandiae (kereru) ha–1, and (c) and (d) are across kereru densities of 0.1 ha–1 (dark blue) to 10 ha–1 (red)
at s  100 m.

et  al. 2013). Over the long-term, shortened seed dispersal shown that localized seed dispersal drives the emergence of
distances may amplify any existing local tree aggregation and aggregated plant spatial distributions (Holmes and Wilson
increase neighborhood competition and negative density- 1998, Fedriani et  al. 2010, Rodriguez-Perez et  al. 2012).
dependent effects (Augspurger and Kelly 1984). In sup- In our case, there were also occasional traverses between
port of this expectation, some spatially explicit models have tree clusters in aggregated forest settings, resulting in long

Figure 3. (a) Example of distribution of foraging ranges (ha) of 100 H. novaeseelandiae after 14 d in V. lucens and P. ferruginea forests, and
(b) example of emergence of a foraging range of a single H. novaeseelandiae after 14 d; colored points depict foraging at trees, with colours
representing visitation (blue to red, with unvisited in grey) – in this case the area visited was 22.5 ha.

7-EV
Figure 4. Seed deposition by H. novaeseelandiae across landscapes of 400 ha (2000  2000 m, showing ‘patches’, each 100 m2) with 1200
V. lucens trees (black points) and 100 roost trees (white points) after one fruiting season, at varying disperser density (D, H. novaeseelandiae
ha–1) and tree aggregation i.e. s (m), low values indicate high tree aggregation and vice versa. Legends indicate the total number of seeds
deposited ha–1 (note they vary across sub plots).

distance seed dispersal events, consequently stretching the Pegman 2012). However, fruit depletion in some ecosystems
tail of the dispersal kernel; such events, even if rare, enhance can be very high (Forget 1992) and in such cases, frugivore
seed dispersal efficiency and allow plant population spread movement patterns are different. For example, the plant
(Howe and Miriti 2004). Conversely, when the distribu- taxon represented in the model by Morales and Carlo (2006)
tion of trees was less aggregated, mean dispersal distances was frequently depleted of fruit, resulting in longer frugivore
increased because birds had larger foraging ranges and there- foraging movements and an increase in mean seed dispersal
fore had the opportunity to disperse seeds at longer distances distance and kurtosis of the dispersal kernels. Decreased dis-
and in a more uniform pattern, including under roost trees. perser abundance in our simulations did, however, result in
Variation in frugivore density did not significantly a proportional reduction in the total number of dispersed
influence seed dispersal kernels in our factorial experi- seeds, with the commensurate effect of decreasing the prob-
ments. Increased disperser densities had no direct effect on ability of long distance seed dispersal, thus reducing seed
seed dispersal kernels because they arose from independent dispersal efficiency and potentially diminishing population
superposition of individual seed shadows of all birds, and spread (Spiegel and Nathan 2007, Nathan et al. 2008). In
resources did not become scarce, so birds did not have to support of this finding, Wotton and Kelly (2011) showed
venture out of fruiting patches to obtain more fruit. Both that lack of seed dispersal has resulted in low recruitment
of the tree species we considered have high annual fruit rates in large-fruited NZ tree species. Conversely, increased
production, with up to 83% of V. lucens fruit and 70% of disperser abundance resulted in a proportional increase in
P. ferruginea fruit falling uneaten beneath their respective the total number of dispersed seeds in our simulations. We
canopies, due in part to low disperser numbers in modern also tested a mechanism of frugivore density that indirectly
ecosystems (Clout and Hay 1989, Wotton and Kelly 2011, had the potential to change the shape of the seed rain, by

8-EV
Figure 5. Seed deposition by H. novaeseelandiae across landscapes of 400 ha (2000  2000 m, showing ‘patches’, each 100 m2) with 1200
P. ferruginea trees (black points) and 100 roost trees (white points) after one fruiting season, at varying disperser density (D, H. novaeseelan-
diae ha–1) and tree aggregation i.e. s (m), low values indicate high tree aggregation and vice versa. Legends indicate the total number of
seeds deposited ha–1 (note they vary across sub plots).

concurrently reducing the availability and ripening of fruits Kelly (2012) reported that seed dispersal distances were
on trees in the model. As a result, at high frugivore den- established primarily by frugivore movements, but did not
sities, birds were forced to move further afield to forage, comment on the role of landscape structure in this process.
as evidenced by significantly longer mean seed dispersal Our sensitivity analyses revealed that the model param-
distances. eters having the largest influence on the results were those
Other simulations that investigated how plant–frugivore relating to bird behaviour, while the effect of landscape
mutualisms affect seed dispersal kernels have produced dif- characteristics (sigma) was much smaller (Table 2 and
ferent outcomes, possibly due to the different ways in which Supplementary material Appendix 4). Mean seed dispersal
they represented disperser activity or landscape structure. distances increased disproportionately when the parameter
For example, despite clear changes in bird movement in (i.e. bD) that was used to calculate the hyperbolic decline in
response to landscape configuration, Uriarte et  al. (2011) tree attractiveness with distance varied (shape also increased,
stated that there was negligible effect of landscape struc- but to a lesser degree, Table 2). The sensitivity of this model
ture on seed dispersal distances. However, their model used parameter suggests that frugivore movement plays an impor-
fragmented forest patches that did not take into account tant role in the emergence of the seed dispersal kernel; this
aggregation of individual trees. Will and Tackenberg (2008) finding is consistent with other studies, which have shown
found that seed dispersal kernels, based on epizoochorous that spatial patterns of recruitment are influenced by move-
and endozoochorous dispersal by mammals, are influenced ment patterns of animal dispersers (Wenny 2000, Santamaria
more by changes in the animal disperser than on the varia- et al. 2007). Although we found little empirical information
tion of vegetation, but this was determined using random with which to estimate bD for our study taxa, our hyperbolic
rather than directional disperser movement. Wotton and functional response (which utilised bD, see above) produced

9-EV
Figure 6. The left hand column shows seed dispersal kernels generated by 30 randomly selected H. novaeseelandiae from the total pool (grey
shaded), with the red curved line showing the kernel density estimated across all frugivores, with s, the degree of tree aggregation,
increasing from top to bottom (highly aggregated to diffuse trees). The right hand column shows the spatial association between trees and
seed deposition events using the bivariate pair correlation function (PCF, Illian et al. 2008) for each of 30 replicates (grey lines) for each
value of s. The red straight line is the theoretical expection (g[r] ≡ 1) under a null homogeneous Poisson model, and the distance (x-axis)
at which the grey lines approach this value gives an estimate of the distances over which seed deposition occurred with respect to fruiting
trees.

frugivore foraging ranges, and movements that resulted in important, on the basis of their sensitivity analysis of aD,
them, that were closest to empirical values, providing sup- the factor in tree attractiveness based on distance (Table 1
port for our representation of this process. and Supplementary material Appendix 1, Eq. A2). However,
Frugivore gut passage time of seeds (GPT) and daily fru- Wotton and Kelly (2012) showed in their simulations that
givore perching time (PT) also influenced the scale (but not seed dispersal distances were not determined by GPT since
the shape) of the dispersal kernels. As expected, an increase birds never flew for longer than seed retention times, as they
in GPT caused dispersal distances to increase because did in our scenarios.
seeds passed through the frugivore gut more slowly and Simulation models that link plant spatial patterns and
therefore had the opportunity to travel further. When the the behaviour of organisms are uncommon (Morales and
shape parameter of the probability distribution describing Carlo 2006, Will and Tackenberg 2008), and ours is one
GPT increased, there was a reduction in mean seed disper- of few that include frugivore roosting behaviour. The only
sal distances because the shortened tail of the GPT curve previous in silico model of seed dispersal by H. novaeseelan-
did not allow longer passage times, and so seeds typically diae is a phenomenological model that combined gut pas-
were expelled at shorter distances. An increase in PT caused sage time of seeds with radio-telemetry data (Wotton and
a decrease in the mean seed dispersal distances because Kelly 2012). Incorporating other indigenous large-fruited
the longer frugivores remained at a given perch, the more co-fruiting tree species could enhance our model by intro-
likely they were to disperse seeds under the parent tree or ducing seasonal variation. The simultaneous representa-
in the immediate vicinity of it. When the shape parameter tion of multiple tree species would create dynamics that are
of the distribution of PT increased, there was a concurrent more realistic because there are usually multiple fruit sources
increase in mean seed dispersal distance, consistent with the in forests, and the dietary preferences of frugivores may
shorter perching time resulting from shorter tail of the PT affect where seeds are deposited (Morales and Carlo 2006,
curve. Morales and Carlo (2006) showed in their scenarios Morales et al. 2013). For example, Pegman (2012) showed
that mean seed dispersal distance was primarily influenced that seeds of three other large-fruited NZ indigenous tree
by the parameters of the distributions of GPT and PT, but species (Beilschmiedia tarairi, B. tawa, and Corynocarpus
found that plant attractiveness based on distance was less laevigatus) were frequently deposited under P. ferruginea and

10-EV
V. lucens. The mean seed dispersal distances produced by our southeastern sector of Banks Peninsula, New Zealand. – MSc
simulations may therefore need further refinement because thesis, Lincoln Univ., Canterbury, New Zealand.
the presence of other local attractive tree species in the land- Clout, M. N. and Hay, J. R. 1989. The importance of birds as
scape may act to reduce seed dispersal distances, assuming browsers, pollinators and seed dispersers in New Zealand
forests. – N. Z. J. Ecol. 12 (Suppl.): 27–33.
all trees are equally attractive. Conversely, if tree species that Clout, M. N. and Tilley, J. A. V. 1992. Germination of miro
have low abundance in the landscape are highly favoured, (Prumnopitys ferruginea) seeds after consumption by New
this may act to extend dispersal distances. Based on data Zealand pigeons (Hemiphaga novaeseelandiae). – N. Z. J. Bot.
from study sites that had multiple fruiting species, Wotton 30: 25–28.
and Kelly (2012) calculated, via simulation, a shorter mean Clout, M. N. et  al. 1991. Seasonal movements of New Zealand
seed dispersal distance for V. lucens than ours, using iden- pigeons from a lowland forest reserve. – Notornis 38: 37–47.
tical gut passage times of seeds and perching times for Coates-Estrada, R. and Estrada, A. 1988. Frugivory and seed
H. novaeseelandiae (their value was ca 98 m versus ours of dispersal in Cymbopetalum baillonii (Annonaceae) at Los
Tuxtlas, Mexico. – J. Trop. Ecol. 4: 157–172.
ca 151 m, Supplementary material Appendix 3, Table A3, Cordeiro, N. J. and Howe, H. F. 2003. Forest fragmentation severs
using nK  2, s  20; nevertheless, up to 20% of V. lucens mutualism between seed dispersers and an endemic African
seeds in their simulations were dispersed  100 m). tree. – Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 100: 14052–14056.
Cortes, M. C. and Uriarte, M. 2013. Integrating frugivory and
animal movement: a review of the evidence and implications
Conclusion for scaling seed dispersal. – Biol. Rev. 88: 255–272.
Cousens, R. D. and Rawlinson, A. A. 2001. When will plant
It is challenging to evaluate the link between plant demog- morphology affect the shape of a seed dispersal ‘kernel’? – J.
raphy and behavior of organisms because they interact Theor. Biol. 211: 229–238.
reciprocally. Our experiments show the potential of sim- DeAngelis, D. L. and Mooij, W. M. 2005. Individual-based
ulation-based approaches for exploring plant–organism modeling of ecological and evolutionary processes. – Annu.
interactions and identified that plant spatial heterogeneity is Rev. Ecol. Syst. 36: 147–168.
Fedriani, J. M. et al. 2010. Spatial pattern of adult trees and the
a key determinant of bird behaviour and the seed dispersal
mammal-generated seed rain in the Iberian Pear. – Ecography
kernels that arise from it. Although this outcome is specific 33: 545–555.
to the scenarios we examined, we believe our results apply Forget, P. M. 1992. Seed removal and seed fate in Gustavia superba
to other similar mutualisms. Perhaps the most critical result (Lecythidaceae). – Biotropica 24: 408–414.
from our simulations is that the loss of dispersers resulted Greene, T. 2003. Distance sampling populations of New Zealand
in fewer seeds being transported, decreasing the probabil- birds – estimating kaka and kereru abundance. – < www.doc.
ity of rare long distance seed dispersal events, reducing seed govt.nz/Documents/science–and.../SciencePoster64 >.
dispersal effectiveness and therefore ultimately diminishing Grimm, V. et  al. 2010. The ODD protocol: a review and first
update. – Ecol. Model. 221: 2760–2768.
population spread, genetic connectivity, and survival of tree
Hamby, D. M. 1994. A review of techniques for parameter sensitiv-
species (Nathan 2005). Such understanding is essential in ity analysis of environmental models. – Environ. Monitoring
view of declining animal dispersers and habitats worldwide. Assess. 32: 135–154.
Hanoteaux, S. et  al. 2013. Effects of spatial patterns on the
pollination success of a less attractive species. – Oikos 122:
Acknowledgements – This study was funded by a Univ. of Auckland 867–880.
Doctoral Scholarship (awarded to APMcKP), the School of Envi- Hill, M. T. 2003. Diet, dispersal and distribution of kereru
ronment Graduate Research Fund, and the Auckland Regional (Hemiphaga novaeseelandiae) in a lowland podocarp-hardwood
Council. We thank the editor and reviewers for useful comments forest. – MSc thesis, Massey Univ., Palmerston North,
on the manuscript and the birds for dispersing seeds.­­­­­­­ New Zealand.
Holmes, E. E. and Wilson, H. B. 1998. Running from trouble:
long-distance dispersal and the competitive coexistence of
References inferior species. – Am. Nat. 151: 578–586.
Howe, H. F. and Miriti, M. N. 2004. When seed dispersal matters.
Augspurger, C. K. and Kelly, C. K. 1984. Pathogen mortality of – Bioscience 54: 651–660.
tropical tree seedlings: experimental studies of the effects Illian, J. et  al. 2008. Statistical analysis and modeling of spatial
of dispersal distance, seedling density, and light conditions. point patterns. – John Wiley and Sons.
– Oecologia 61: 211–217. Johnson, S. 2001. Prumnopitys ferruginea (miro) seedling recruit-
Baddeley, A. and Turner, R. 2005. Spatstat: an R package for ment patterns in the Catlins, New Zealand, and dispersal by
analyzing spatial point patterns. – J. Stat. Softw. 12, Hemiphaga novaeseelandiae (New Zealand pigeon). – MSc
< www.jstatsoft.org/ >. thesis in Environmental Science, Univ. of Otago, New Zealand.
Bell, R. 1996. Seed dispersal by kereru (Hemiphaga novaeseelandiae) Kelly, D. et al. 2010. Mutualisms with the wreckage of an avifauna:
at Wenderholm Regional Park. – MSc thesis, Auckland Univ., the status of bird pollination and fruit-dispersal in New Zealand.
New Zealand. – N. Z. J. Ecol. 34: 66–85.
Beveridge, A. E. 1964. Dispersal and destruction of seed in central Levey, D. J. et al. 1984. Fruit choice in neotropical birds: the effect
North Island podocarp forests. – Proc. N. Z. Ecol. Soc. 11: of distance between fruits on choice patterns. – Ecology 65:
48–56. 844 – 850.
Bullock, J. M. et al. 2006. Measuring plant dispersal: an introduc- Levey, D. J. et al. 2008. Modeling long-distance seed dispersal in
tion to field methods and experimental design. – Plant Ecol. heterogeneous landscapes. – J. Ecol. 96: 599–608.
186: 217–234. Levin, S. A. et al. 2003. The ecology and evolution of seed dispersal:
Campbell, K. L. 2006. A study of home ranges, movements, diet a theoretical perspective. – Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 34:
and habitat use of kereru (Hemiphaga novaeseelandiae) in the 575–604.

11-EV
McEwen, W. N. 1978. The food of the New Zealand pigeon Santamaria, L. et  al. 2007. Predicting spatial patterns of plant
(Hemiphaga novaeseelandiae novaeseelandiae). – N. Z. J. Ecol. recruitment using animal-displacement kernels. – PLoS One
1: 99–108. 2: e1008.
McEwen, W. N. 1988. Cone and seed phenology in several Saracco, J. F. et al. 2005. Crop size and fruit neighbourhood effects
New Zealand conifer tree species. – Tuatara 30: 66–76. on visitation to fruiting trees. – Biotropica 37: 80–86.
Miskelly, C. M. et al. 2008. Conservation status of New Zealand Schotborgh, H. M. 2005. An analysis of home ranges, movements,
birds. – Notornis 55: 117–135. foods and breeding of kereru (Hemiphaga novaeseelandiae) in a
Morales, J. M. and Carlo, T. A. 2006. The effects of plant distribu- rural–urban landscape on Banks Peninsula, New Zealand.
tion and frugivore density on the scale and shape of dispersal – MSc thesis, Lincoln Univ., Canterbury, New Zealand.
kernels. – Ecology 87: 1489–1496. Schupp, E. W. 1993. Quantity, quality and the effectiveness of seed
Morales, J. M. et al. 2012. Neighborhood effects on seed dispersal dispersal by animals. – Vegetatio 107: 15–29.
by frugivores: testing theory with a mistletoe–marsupial system Schupp, E.W. et al. 2010. Seed dispersal effectiveness revisited: a
in Patagonia. – Ecology 93: 741–748. conceptual review. – New Phytol. 188: 333–353.
Morales, J. M. et al. 2013. Frugivore behavioural details matter for Sekercioglu, C. H. et  al. 2004. Ecosystem consequences of bird
seed dispersal: a multi-species model for Cantabrian thrushes declines. – Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 101: 18042–18047.
and trees. – PLoS One 8:e65216. Spiegel, O. and Nathan, R. 2007. Incorporating dispersal distance
Nathan, R. 2005. Long-distance dispersal research: building a into the disperser effectiveness framework: frugivorous birds
network of yellow brick roads. – Divers. Distrib. 11: provide complementary dispersal to plants in a patchy environ-
125–130. ment. – Ecol. Lett. 10: 718–728.
Nathan, R. and Muller-Landau, C. 2000. Spatial patterns of seed Tiffney, B. H. et  al. 2004. Vertebrate dispersal of seed plants
dispersal, their determinants and consequences for recruit- through time. – Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 35: 1–29.
ment. – Trends Ecol. Evol. 15: 278–285. Traveset, A. 1998. Effect of seed passage through vertebrate
Nathan, R. et  al. 2008. Mechanisms of long distance dispersal. frugivores’ guts on germination: a review. – Perspect. Plant
– Trends Ecol. Evol. 23: 638–647. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 1: 151–190.
Pegman, A. P. McK. 2012. Reconstruction of seed dispersal via Uriarte, M. et al. 2011. Disentangling the drivers of reduced long-
modeling, seedling recruitment, and dispersal efficiency of distance seed dispersal by birds in an experimentally fragmented
Hemiphaga novaeseelandiae in Vitex lucens and Prumnopitys landscape. – Ecology 92: 924–937.
ferruginea in New Zealand. – PhD thesis, Auckland Univ., Weibull, W. 1951. A statistical distribution function of wide
New Zealand. applicability. – J. Appl. Mech. 18: 293–297.
Pegman, A. P. McK. et al. 2016. Data from: Exploring the interac- Wenny, D. G. 2000. Seed dispersal, seed predation, and seedling
tion of avian frugivory and plant spatial heterogeneity and its recruitment of a neotropical montane tree. – Ecol. Monogr.
effect on seed dispersal kernels using a simulation model. 70: 331–351.
– Dryad Digital Repository, < http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/ Wilensky, U. 1999. NetLogo. – Center for Connected Learning
dryad.15d8v >. and Computer-Based Modeling, Northwestern Univ., Evanston,
Pierce, R. J. and Graham, P. J. 1995. Ecology and breeding biol- IL, USA, < http://ccl.northwestern.edu/netlogo/ >.
ogy of kukupa (Hemiphaga novaeseelandiae) in Northland. Will, H. and Tackenberg, O. 2008. A mechanistic simulation model
– Science and Research Series 91, Dept of Conservation, of seed dispersal by animals. – J. Ecol. 96: 1011–1022.
Wellington. Wotton, D. M. 2007. Consequences of dispersal failure: kereru and
Powlesland, R. G. et  al. 2011. Satellite tracking of kereru large seeds in New Zealand. – PhD thesis, Canterbury Univ.,
(Hemiphaga novaeseelandiae) in Southland, New Zealand: New Zealand.
impacts, movements and home range. – N. Z. J. Ecol. 35: Wotton, D. M. and Kelly, D. 2011. Frugivore loss limits recruitment
229–235. of large-seeded trees. – Proc. R. Soc. B 278: 3345–3354.
Robledo-Arnuncio, J. J. et al. 2014. Space, time and complexity in Wotton, D. M. and Kelly, D. 2012. Do larger frugivores move seeds
plant dispersal ecology. – Movement Ecol. 2: 16. further? Body size, seed dispersal distance, and a case study of
Rodriguez-Perez, J. et  al. 2012. Adult proximity and frugivore’s a large, sedentary pigeon. – J. Biogeogr. 39: 1973–1983.
activity structure the spatial pattern in an endangered plant. Wotton, D. M. et al. 2008. Seed retention times in the New Zealand
– Funct. Ecol. 26: 1221–1229. pigeon, Hemiphaga novaeseelandiae. – N. Z. J. Ecol. 32: 1–6.
Salmon, J. T. 1980. Native trees of New Zealand. – Reed Methuen Zurell, D. et al. 2010. The virtual ecologist approach: simulating
Publishers. data and observers. – Oikos 119: 622–635.

Supplementary material (Appendix ECOG-02191 at < www.


ecography.org/appendix/ecog-02191 >). Appendix 1–4.­

12-EV

You might also like