You are on page 1of 17

Epistemological Beliefs, Mathematical Problem‐Solving Beliefs, and Academic Performance of

Middle School Students


Author(s): Marlene Schommer‐Aikins, Orpha K. Duell and Rosetta Hutter
Source: The Elementary School Journal, Vol. 105, No. 3 (January 2005), pp. 289-304
Published by: University of Chicago Press
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.1086/428745
Accessed: 26-02-2016 02:39 UTC

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/
info/about/policies/terms.jsp

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content
in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship.
For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

University of Chicago Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Elementary School
Journal.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 130.113.111.210 on Fri, 26 Feb 2016 02:39:38 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Epistemological Abstract

Beliefs, Mathematical This study examined the structure of middle


school students’ general epistemological beliefs
Problem-Solving and domain-specific mathematical problem-
solving beliefs by asking whether the 2 belief
Beliefs, and Academic systems are related and whether they predict
students’ academic performance. Over 1,200
seventh- and eighth-grade students completed
Performance of an Epistemological Questionnaire, the Indiana
Mathematical Belief Scale, and the Fennema-
Middle School Sherman Usefulness Scale. Based on regression
analyses, beliefs in quick/fixed learning (i.e.,
Students that learning is fast and instinctual) and studying
aimlessly (i.e., studying without strategy) were
significantly related to beliefs about effortful
math, useful math, understand math concepts,
and math confidence. Furthermore, path analy-
Marlene Schommer-Aikins sis suggested that both general and domain-
specific epistemological beliefs predicted aca-
Orpha K. Duell demic performance as measured by solving
Rosetta Hutter mathematic problems and overall grade point
average.
Wichita State University

Beliefs about the nature of knowledge and


learning, or epistemological beliefs, have
been linked to numerous aspects of aca-
demic learning, particularly among college
and high school students. For example, the
more college students believe that knowl-
edge is simple, the less likely they are to
comprehend academic text, monitor their
comprehension, and use sophisticated study
strategies (Schommer, 1990; Schommer,
Crouse, & Rhodes, 1992; Schraw, Dunkle, &
Bendixen, 1995). The more high school stu-
dents believe in quick learning, the more
likely they are to earn a low grade point av-
erage (Schommer, Calvert, Gariglietti, & Ba-
jaj, 1997). And the more high school students
The Elementary School Journal believe in the innate ability to learn, the more
Volume 105, Number 3
䉷 2005 by the University of Chicago. All rights reserved.
likely they are to devalue education (Schom-
0013-5984/2005/10503-0003$05.00 mer & Walker, 1997). There is little doubt

This content downloaded from 130.113.111.210 on Fri, 26 Feb 2016 02:39:38 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
290 THE ELEMENTARY SCHOOL JOURNAL

that epistemological beliefs are critical fac- that the ability to learn is fixed at birth and
tors to be considered among older students. that the purpose of an academic task is sim-
What are students’ epistemological beliefs ply to document their intelligence. When
before high school? And do epistemological faced with a difficult task, these children
beliefs of younger students relate to other as- tend to display helpless behavior. In con-
pects of students’ cognition? The research re- trast, other children believe that the ability
ported in this article extends inquiry in this to learn is improvable over time and with
area by identifying middle school students’ experience and that the purpose of an aca-
epistemological beliefs and examining the demic task is to improve their intelligence.
relations between these beliefs and beliefs When faced with a difficult task, these chil-
about mathematical problem solving. dren tend to try different strategies and per-
Most studies of epistemological beliefs sist in their efforts to learn.
have involved older students. For example, Schoenfeld (1983, 1985) has also studied
in the mid-1950s, Perry (1968) interviewed epistemological beliefs independently of
and surveyed college undergraduates. He Perry’s work. Through observations and in-
concluded that undergraduates enter col- terviews with high school students as they
lege believing that knowledge is simple, were solving mathematical problems,
certain, and handed down by authority. By Schoenfeld concluded that part of the prob-
the time they reach their senior year, many lem-solving process is influenced by stu-
students believe that knowledge is com- dents’ beliefs about the nature of mathe-
plex, tentative, and derived through reason matical knowledge and learning. For
and empirical evidence. example, he observed that students who
Many of Perry’s followers studied per- did not engage in successful problem solv-
sonal epistemology by focusing on a single ing appeared to believe that mathematics
dimension. For example, Kitchener and problems should be solved in 10 minutes or
King (1981, 1989) studied how students jus- less, that mathematicians are gifted individ-
tify knowledge. Their epistemological uals, and that as students they must rely on
model, called the Reflective Judgment mathematicians to give them basic formulas
Model, describes a seven-stage develop- and proofs.
mental path. Early in development, stu- Schommer (1990) proposed a reconcep-
dents believe that reality is concrete and tualization of epistemological beliefs that
knowledge is absolute. Seeing is believing, synthesized much of the earlier research on
and there is little need for justification. Mid- personal epistemology. She suggested that
way through this developmental path, stu- epistemological beliefs be conceived as a
dents believe there is a different reality for system of more or less independent beliefs.
everyone, and one person’s opinion is as By system she meant that more than one
good as another’s. In the final stages of de- belief composed personal epistemology. By
velopment, students acknowledge a tenta- more or less independent, she meant that
tiveness in knowledge, yet they also realize these beliefs could, but not necessarily
that not all knowledge is of equal credibil- would, develop in synchrony. Originally,
ity. Reason and evidence can help discern Schommer hypothesized five beliefs, in-
the quality of knowledge. cluding beliefs about the structure of
Other researchers have investigated knowledge (ranging from isolated bits to in-
epistemological beliefs independently from tegrated concepts), the stability of knowl-
Perry’s work. For example, Dweck and her edge (ranging from certain to evolving), the
colleagues (Dweck & Bempechat, 1983; source of knowledge (ranging from handed
Dweck & Leggett, 1988) studied young chil- down by authority to derived from reason
dren’s beliefs about intelligence. Dweck’s and evidence), the speed of learning (from
theory suggested that some children believe quick or not at all to gradual), and the abil-

JANUARY 2005

This content downloaded from 130.113.111.210 on Fri, 26 Feb 2016 02:39:38 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
BELIEFS AND PERFORMANCE 291

ity to learn (ranging from fixed at birth to pects of cognition, affect, and ultimately
improvable). academic performance (Schommer-Aikins,
Schommer (1990) constructed a ques- 2002). Hence, to demonstrate the utility of
tionnaire that assesses four of these hypoth- epistemological beliefs among middle
esized beliefs: structure of knowledge, sta- school children, it is important to show the
bility of knowledge, speed of learning, and link between epistemological beliefs and an
ability to learn. This factor structure was outward manifestation of them. The chal-
replicated with other college students and lenge of this inquiry is that epistemological
with high school students (Schommer, 1993; beliefs’ powerful influence is likely hidden
Schommer et al., 1992, 1997). Other research- because many of their effects are indirect
ers have uncovered related multidimen- rather than direct (Schommer, 1994, 1998;
sional structures using different epistemo- Winne, Graham, & Prock, 1993). For exam-
logical belief instruments (Buehl, Alexander, ple, epistemological beliefs (e.g., that knowl-
& Murphy, 2002; Hofer, 2000; Schraw, Ben- edge structure equals isolated facts) may
dixen, & Dunkle, 2002; Wood & Kardash, lead to an internal standard (e.g., learning
2002). completed equals ability to repeat facts),
Since 1990, research has shown that which leads to choice of study strategy (e.g.,
epistemological beliefs predict numerous mindless repetition), which leads to what the
aspects of academic performance among instructor ultimately sees (an ability to re-
high school and college students. For ex- peat knowledge but an inability to under-
ample, beliefs in the structure and certainty stand or apply the knowledge). Support has
of knowledge predict comprehension, me- been found for the idea that general episte-
tacomprehension, and interpretation of in- mological beliefs about the speed and effort
formation among college students (Jehng, of learning relate to strategy use while read-
Johnson, & Anderson, 1993; Kardash & ing text aloud (Kardash & Howell, 2000).
Scholes, 1996; Schommer, 1990; Schommer Kardash and Howell’s hypothesis tested in
et al., 1992; Schraw et al., 1995). Beliefs our study is that general epistemological be-
about the speed of learning and the ability liefs affect students’ domain-specific mathe-
to learn predict comprehension, valuing of matical beliefs, which in turn influence stu-
education, and overall grade point average dents’ mathematical ability.
for college and high school students Recently, De Corte and his colleagues
(Schommer, 1990; Schommer et al., 1997; (De Corte, & Op’t Eynde, 2003; De Corte,
Schommer & Walker, 1997). Op’t Eynde, & Verschaffel, 2002; Op’t Eynde
Because the majority of epistemological & De Corte, 2003) have urged those study-
research has been carried out with older ing students’ mathematical beliefs and per-
students, the research reported in this arti- formance to take a more systemic approach
cle focused on the personal epistemology of to their investigations. They noted that
middle school students. We emphasize the mathematics is part of a more complete sys-
contemporary notion of an epistemological tem of classroom context, beliefs about self,
belief system. As in the studies of older stu- and beliefs about the nature of mathemat-
dents and adults, we sought to identify the ics. Another intriguing idea posed by De
structure of middle school students’ epis- Corte et al. (2002, p. 306) is that “if episte-
temological beliefs. Just as importantly, we mological beliefs, i.e., beliefs about the na-
wanted to determine if students’ epistemo- ture of knowledge and the processes of
logical beliefs could predict other aspects of knowing, are essentially very fundamental
their cognition. and general in nature (Hofer & Pintrich,
The study of epistemological beliefs has 1997), then mathematics-related beliefs may
practical importance based on the premise not be considered as epistemological beliefs
that these beliefs play a role in other as- as such, but rather be perceived as domain-

This content downloaded from 130.113.111.210 on Fri, 26 Feb 2016 02:39:38 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
292 THE ELEMENTARY SCHOOL JOURNAL

specific manifestations of the general epis- high school and college students, they
temological beliefs.” found that scores on the scales were related
This statement is not without contro- to mathematical performance and to suc-
versy. The existence of general and domain- cess in college remedial mathematics
specific epistemological beliefs is open to classes.
question. A more recent stance is that the In the study reported here we tested the
issue of domain specificity/generality is not hypothesis that general epistemological be-
either/or (Buehl et al., 2002; Hofer, 2000; liefs are linked to the mathematical problem-
Schommer, 1994; Schommer-Aikins, 2002). solving beliefs. Furthermore, links between
Rather, the issue is how these different be- these two systems of beliefs and students’
lief systems interact or whether they are reading, mathematical problem solving, and
systems within a system. overall grade point average were examined.
In the present study we examined epis- We conducted analyses in several stages
temological beliefs in relation to other as- to examine the interrelations among these
pects of cognition and academic perfor- variables. First, historically, epistemological
mance in order to provide a systemic beliefs have been considered developmen-
framework of inquiry. Specifically, we ex- tal in nature (Kitchener & King, 1989; Perry,
amined the relation between students’ 1968). That is, students’ sophistication in-
general epistemological beliefs and their creased with age and experience. Whether
domain-specific beliefs about mathematical the structure of students’ beliefs is also de-
problem solving. velopmental is unknown. Therefore, ex-
To understand mathematical problem ploratory factor analysis was conducted on
solving, Kloosterman and Stage (1992) de- both the epistemological and mathematical
veloped an instrument to assess beliefs beliefs measures to explore the number and
about mathematical problem solving, the nature of students’ beliefs. Next, to examine
Indiana Mathematics Scale. The idea behind the relations among epistemological beliefs,
their work is that beliefs about problem mathematical problem-solving beliefs, and
solving are likely to influence willingness academic performance, we calculated a se-
to engage in problem solving as well as the ries of regressions among all variables. Fi-
choice of strategies to use during the prob- nally, to examine a potential causal chain of
lem-solving process. Influenced by earlier events, from epistemological beliefs in gen-
researchers, Kloosterman and Stage iden- eral, to mathematical problem-solving be-
tified six beliefs about mathematical prob- liefs, and ultimately to academic perfor-
lem solving that they believe are critical to mance, we conducted a path analysis.
the learner’s motivation and strategy use.
Five beliefs are assessed by their Indiana Method
Mathematics Scale, which includes beliefs Participants
that mathematical problem solving (a) is A total of 1,269 students from two middle
time-consuming, (b) requires understand- schools in the Midwest participated in the
ing, (c) involves more than step-by-step study. Students were approximately equal in
procedures, (d) involves word problems representation by gender (boys, n ⳱ 587;
(not just calculations), and (e) can be im- girls, n ⳱ 657; not reported, n ⳱ 25) and
proved with effort. They also field tested grade (7th, n ⳱ 619; 8th, n ⳱ 644; not re-
Fennema and Sherman’s (1976) Mathemat- ported, n ⳱ 6). Students were predomi-
ics Is Useful Scale, which assesses the de- nately white (86% European American, 5%
gree to which students believe that mathe- African American, 5% Hispanic American,
matics is useful in their daily lives. In 3% Asian American, 3% Native American)
addition to generating psychometric prop- and middle class (23% receiving free or
erties for these instruments for use with reduced-price lunch).

JANUARY 2005

This content downloaded from 130.113.111.210 on Fri, 26 Feb 2016 02:39:38 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
BELIEFS AND PERFORMANCE 293

Measures lem solving were assessed using the Indi-


Epistemological beliefs. A middle ana Mathematics Belief Scale (Kloosterman
school version of an epistemological belief & Stage, 1992) and the Usefulness of Math-
questionnaire (EB) was constructed using ematics Scale (Fennema & Sherman, 1976),
high school and college versions of an epis- both developed for high school and college
temological belief instrument (Schommer, students. The original Indiana Mathematics
1990, 1993, 1998) as a guide. These original Belief Scale measured five beliefs about
63-item instruments assessed four episte- mathematical problem solving: (a) I can
mological belief factors including beliefs solve time-consuming mathematics prob-
about (a) the structure of knowledge (rang- lems; (b) There are word problems that can-
ing from isolated pieces to integrated con- not be solved with simple, step-by-step
cepts), (b) stability of knowledge (ranging procedures; (c) Understanding concepts is
from certain knowledge to changing knowl- important in mathematics; (d) Word prob-
edge), (c) speed of learning (ranging from lems are important in mathematics; and
quick learning to gradual learning), and (d) (e) Effort can increase mathematical ability.
ability to learn (ranging from fixed at birth The Usefulness of Mathematics Scale as-
to improvable). Cronbachs alphas range sesses the belief that mathematics is useful
from .63 to .85 for the college version and in daily life. Cronbach’s alphas for these
from .51 to .81 for the high school version scales range from .54 to .84. Validity is ev-
(Duell & Schommer-Aikins, 2001). Validity idenced in the instruments’ prediction of
is evidenced in the instruments’ prediction students’ mathematics scores and success
of students’ comprehension, metacompre- in remedial college mathematics (Stage &
hension, interpretation of information, and Kloosterman, 1995).
valuing of education (Kardash & Scholes, The Indiana Mathematics Belief Scale
1996; Schommer, 1990; Schommer et al., contains 30 items, and the Usefulness of
1992; Schommer & Walker, 1997). The EB Mathematics Scale contains six items. Stu-
scale was revised to have fewer items and, dents respond on a Likert scale from 1
if necessary, simpler expression of ideas to (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).
be more appropriate for middle school stu- Statements assess students’ beliefs about
dents. Details of EB scale development can mathematical problem solving (e.g., “Hard
be found in Schommer-Aikins, Mau, Brook- work can increase one’s ability to do
hart, and Hutter (2000). math”). Items were written so that a less
The middle school EB scale contains 30 epistemologically developed individual
items. Students respond on a Likert scale would agree with about half the items and
from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly disagree with the remaining items. Half of
agree). Statements were written to assess the items were reverse scored so that the
students’ beliefs about knowledge and higher the score the more epistemologically
learning (e.g., “If I can’t understand some- developed the respondent. We combined
thing right away, I will keep on trying”). and randomly ordered the items for both
Items were written so that a less epistemo- instruments to form a single instrument that
logically developed individual would agree we refer to as the Mathematics Problem-
with about half the items and disagree with Solving Beliefs Scale (MPSB). Items for the
the remaining items. The order of items was MPSB Scale were not revised because
randomized. About half of the items were Kloosterman and Stage (1992) advised that
reverse scored so that the higher the score, these scales are appropriate for middle
the less epistemologically developed the re- school and high school students. In addi-
spondent. tion, the Effort Can Increase Mathematical
Mathematical problem-solving beliefs. Ability Scale has been tested with seventh
Students’ beliefs about mathematical prob- graders and has a generated reliability in-

This content downloaded from 130.113.111.210 on Fri, 26 Feb 2016 02:39:38 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
294 THE ELEMENTARY SCHOOL JOURNAL

dex that is almost identical to that of college walk, but for the next 3 nights the rain
populations (Kloosterman, 1988). added 4 inches of mud each night; the
next 3 nights the rain added 3 inches of
Academic performance. Three measures mud each night; the next 3 nights the rain
of academic performance were available added 2 inches of mud each night; and
from one of the participating schools: math- then for 3 nights the rain added 1 inch of
ematical problem solving, reading, and mud each night. How many days did it
overall grade point average. Both content- take Harry to clear his sidewalk?
area tests were part of the Kansas State As-
sessment instruments developed by the Once we had gathered the data and ex-
University of Kansas and administered in amined the distribution of scores, we trans-
January of each year. formed the rubric scoring into a four-point
For the problem-solving assessment, scale to allow a more level distribution of
students were given two mathematical scores: (a) 1 ⳱ no response to inadequate
problems and asked to solve them, show response (n ⳱ 61), (b) 2 ⳱ weak to adequate
their work, and explain the rationale behind response (n ⳱ 64), (c) 3 ⳱ good-quality re-
their thinking. Four trained teachers ap- sponse with a few minor errors (n ⳱ 84),
plied a six-point rubric to score each stu- and (d) 4 ⳱ superior response (n ⳱ 144).
dent’s solution: (a) 0 ⳱ no response, (b) 1 ⳱ Students’ Kansas State Assessment
inadequate response (e.g., contains major reading scores were norm referenced. Read-
computation errors, focuses entirely on the ing comprehension was reported with nor-
wrong mathematical idea or procedure, mal equivalent scores (M ⳱ 50, SD ⳱ 21).
shows copied parts of the problem with no Because the problems students solved in-
attempt at a solution), (c) 3 ⳱ adequate re- volved reading, we used reading scores as
sponse (e.g., omits parts or elements of the a control variable in these analyses.
problem, contains computation errors,
shows some deficiencies in understanding Procedure
the problem), and (d) 5 ⳱ superior (e.g., is To obtain maximum standardization in
clear and unambiguous, communicates ef- administration of these instruments, the be-
fectively, shows mathematical understand- liefs assessments became an integral part of
ing of the problem’s ideas and require- the schools’ yearly Quality Performance Ac-
ments). creditation assessment process. Schools in
Teacher training in using this rubric was Kansas are required to develop goals and
conducted by the Kansas State Department show yearly progress toward those goals.
of Education. The training consisted of Mathematics problem solving is a required
workshops to explain what each category goal. Hence, scores from the MPSB Scale
means as well as what each level of re- were used as part of the school district’s re-
sponse would look like. Teachers were port to the state. School personnel were also
given sample responses to score and were willing to assess epistemological beliefs to
required to reach a criterion of 80% or more determine if they affected student academic
agreement. For the state assessment, two outcomes. Teachers administered these in-
teachers scored each student’s solutions. If struments along with other standardized
these two teachers disagreed, they resolved measures for this yearly assessment and en-
differences through discussion. couraged students to take all state assess-
The following is an example of the type ments seriously.
of problem students needed to solve: To avoid student fatigue, we conducted
assessments over 2 weeks. The EB and MPSB
A heavy downpour of rain left a foot of Scales were administered on different weeks,
mud on the ground. Each day Harry and order of scale administration was coun-
shoveled 3 inches of mud off of his side- terbalanced. Teachers were provided both

JANUARY 2005

This content downloaded from 130.113.111.210 on Fri, 26 Feb 2016 02:39:38 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
BELIEFS AND PERFORMANCE 295

written and oral instructions on how to ad- middle school students. Data from the
minister the scales. Students were asked to MPSB Scale were subjected to exploratory
give their true attitudes in responding to factor analysis using all 36 items. We used
these scales and were assured there were no varimax rotation to maximize the indepen-
right or wrong answers. Rather, they were dence of the factor scores. Examination of
told that this was an opportunity to voice factor loadings and the scree plot indicated
their own beliefs. Teachers gave quiet activ- that seven factors were a plausible fit. Be-
ities to students who finished the scales cause the default cutoff of an eigenvalue of
early so that other students could complete 1 was met with the seven-factor solution,
the scales undisturbed. On average, stu- we did not conduct additional exploratory
dents took 15–25 minutes to complete the factor analyses. The seven factors accounted
surveys. for 48.52% of the total variance. The items
and factor loadings are shown in Table 2.
Results Twenty-four items loaded onto this seven-
Factor Structure factor structure. Factor titles and Cronbach’s
Epistemological beliefs. To determine alpha scores were as follows: effortful math
the middle school students’ epistemological (.80), useful math (.80), persistence in math
beliefs, we conducted an exploratory factor (.62), math confidence (.63), understand
analysis on data from the epistemological math concepts (.70), word problems (.62),
questionnaire in a three-stage process. In and nonprescription math (.66). These titles
step one, exploratory factor analysis was are stated from the mature point of view,
applied to the full EB questionnaire of 30 which is consistent with earlier research.
items. We used varimax rotation to maxi- We took a conservative approach to
mize independence of factor scores. Exam- these analyses. To be cautious in interpret-
ination of factor loadings and the scree plot ing data, we retained belief factors that
indicated that four factors were a plausible reached minimal psychometric standards in
fit. In step two, we ran an exploratory factor the following analyses. Therefore, factors
analysis again with all 30 items. This time with Cronbach’s alphas of at least .55 and
we requested SPSS to generate four factors composed of a minimum of three items
only. We then examined items that loaded were used in the remaining analyses. These
highly on the four-factor structure. In step factors included quick/fixed learning,
three, we ran the final exploratory analysis studying aimlessly, effortful math, useful
using only the 21 items that had high load- math, math confidence, and understand
ings in step two. Ultimately, these analyses math concepts. Correlations among these
generated four factors accounting for belief factors are shown in Table 3.
40.35% of the total variance. The items and With these belief factors in mind, we
factor loadings are shown in Table 1. Factor tested several hypotheses in the next stage
titles and Cronbach’s alpha scores, which of analyses. We hypothesized that the less
served as a measure of internal consistency students believed in quick/fixed learning
for items, were as follows: quick/fixed (i.e., that learning should occur quickly and
learning (.77), studying aimlessly (.55), om- is related to ability rather than to effort), the
niscient authority (.55), and certain knowl- more likely they would be to believe that
edge (.36). These titles are stated from the mathematical problem solving requires ef-
less mature point of view, which is consis- fort, confidence, and understanding math.
tent with earlier research. We also hypothesized that the less students
Mathematical problem-solving beliefs. believed in studying aimlessly (i.e., that
Because the MPSB Scale was originally de- studying does not involve strategy or ef-
veloped for high school students, we fort), the more likely they were to believe
needed to determine the factor structure for that mathematical problem solving requires

This content downloaded from 130.113.111.210 on Fri, 26 Feb 2016 02:39:38 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
296 THE ELEMENTARY SCHOOL JOURNAL

Table 1. Factor Loadings of Items in Each Factor for the Epistemological Belief Scale

Factor Loadings

Questionnaire Item Q/F SA OA CK

Quick/fixed learning (Q/F):


An expert is someone who is really born smart in
something. .67 ⳮ.01 .05 .07
If I cannot understand something quickly, it usually
means I will never understand it. .62 .19 .04 ⳮ.07
Working hard on a difficult problem only pays off for
the really smart students. .61 .17 .05 .04
Some people are just born smart, others are born
dumb. .51 ⳮ.02 .14 ⳮ.04
If I am ever going to be able to understand something,
it will make sense to me the first time I hear it. .51 .08 ⳮ.04 .00
Students who are “average” in school will remain
“average” for the rest of their lives. .50 .09 .18 .00
The really smart students don’t have to work hard to
do well in school. .48 .07 .09 ⳮ.05
Successful students understand things quickly. .40 ⳮ.18 ⳮ.06 ⳮ.05
If I can’t understand something right away, I will keep
on trying. (reverse scored) .35 .28 .16 ⳮ.02
You will get mixed-up if you try to combine new ideas
in a textbook with what you already know. .34 ⳮ.05 ⳮ.05 ⳮ.02
Studying aimlessly (SA):
If I find the time to re-read a textbook chapter, I get a
lot more out of it the second time. (reverse scored) .24 .46 .18 ⳮ.17
What students learn from a textbook depends on how
they study it. (reverse scored) .09 .41 .11 .13
You cannot learn anything more from a textbook by
reading it twice. .38 .40 ⳮ.07 ⳮ.15
The knowledge of “how to study” is usually learned
as we grow older. (reverse scored) ⳮ.07 .38 .08 .09
Getting ahead takes a lot of work. (reverse scored) .02 .36 .17 .15
Learning something really well takes a long time.
(reverse scored) ⳮ.21 .34 .04 .07
A class in study skills would probably help slow
learners. (reverse scored) .10 .33 .10 .08
Omniscient authority (OA):
If scientists try hard enough, they can find the truth to
almost everything. ⳮ.02 ⳮ.23 ⴑ.57 ⳮ.05
Scientists can get to the truth if they just keep
searching for it. ⳮ.12 ⳮ.25 ⴑ.54 ⳮ.03
Certain knowledge (CK):
The only thing you can be sure of is that nothing is
sure. (reverse scored) ⳮ.10 .06 .04 .45
Today’s facts may be tomorrow’s fiction. (reverse
scored) .06 .20 .01 .42

Note.—In each column of factor loadings, items that were a part that factor are in bold print.

effort and understanding. Finally, we hy- lated to failure to persist and to the belief
pothesized that the less students believed in that the purpose of studying is simply to
quick/fixed learning and studying aim- document one’s intelligence (rather than to
lessly, the more likely they were to believe learn) (Dweck & Leggett, 1988). The belief
that mathematics may be useful. These hy- that learning occurs quickly or not at all has
potheses are consistent with findings from been related to strategy selection (Kardash
earlier research. For example, researchers & Howell, 2000) and reading comprehen-
have found that belief in fixed ability is re- sion (Schommer, 1990). Belief in fixed abil-

JANUARY 2005

This content downloaded from 130.113.111.210 on Fri, 26 Feb 2016 02:39:38 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Table 2. Factor Loadings of Items in Each Factor for the Mathematical Problem-Solving Beliefs Scale

Factor Loadings
Questionnaire Item EM UM MP MC UMC WP NPM

Effortful math (EM):


Ability in math increases when one studies hard. .63 .10 .09 .14 .07 .09 ⳮ.09
By trying harder, one can become smarter in math. .60 .16 .14 .02 .14 .10 .04
I can get smarter in math if I try hard. .57 .22 .13 .06 .08 .06 ⳮ.10
Working can improve one’s ability in mathematics. .57 .19 .10 .11 .14 .11 ⳮ.13
I can get smarter in math by trying harder. .53 .03 .13 .03 .06 .02 ⳮ.01
Hard work can increase one’s ability to do math. .50 .17 .17 .08 .11 .05 ⳮ.05
I find I can do hard math problems if I just hang in there. .42 .24 .13 .29 .17 .11 ⳮ.11
Useful math (UM):
Mathematics is a worthwhile and necessary subject. .31 .57 .16 .21 .15 .11 ⳮ.01
I study mathematics because I know how useful it is. .24 .49 .07 .30 .03 .14 ⳮ.07
Studying mathematics is a waste of time. (reverse scored) .20 .44 .25 .17 .31 .15 ⳮ.09
Mathematics has no relevance to my life. (reverse scored) .25 .43 .26 .13 .21 .21 ⳮ.01
Mathematics will not be important to me in my life’s work. (reverse scored) .22 .42 .28 .08 .16 .18 ⳮ.09
Knowing mathematics will help me earn a living. .31 .42 .12 .16 .17 .09 ⳮ.10
Math persistence (MP):
If I can’t do a math problem in a few minutes, I can’t do it at all. (reverse scored) .21 .22 .56 .19 .14 .15 ⳮ.02
If I can’t solve a math problem quickly, I quit trying. (reverse scored) .17 .21 .44 .33 .09 .12 ⳮ.06
Math confidence (MC):
I feel I can do math problems that take a long time to complete. .28 .10 .16 .61 .11 .09 ⳮ.05
Math problems that take a long time don’t bother me. .02 .19 ⳮ.01 .59 .04 .08 ⳮ.03
I’m not very good at solving math problems that take a while to figure out. (reverse
scored) .00 .06 .27 .51 .04 .12 .07
Understand math concepts (UMC):
It’s not important to understand why a mathematical procedure works as long as it
gives a correct answer. (reverse scored) .17 .16 .27 .13 .57 .20 ⳮ.07
Getting the right answer in math is more important than understanding why the
answer works. (reverse scored) .22 .16 .33 .04 .47 .19 .02
It doesn’t really matter if you understand a math problem if you can get the right
answer. (reverse scored) .17 .22 .25 .04 .41 .24 ⳮ.01
Word problems (WP):

All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions


Word problems are not a very important part of mathematics. (reverse scored) .19 .18 .15 .11 .13 .60 ⳮ.03
Math classes should not emphasize word problems. (reverse scored) .17 .13 .25 .17 .17 .50 ⳮ.13
Nonprescription math (NPM):
Any word problem can be solved if you know the right steps to follow. (reverse
scored) ⳮ.37 ⳮ.09 ⳮ.10 ⳮ.03 ⳮ.03 ⳮ.07 .64
Any word problem can be solved by using the correct step-by-step procedure.

This content downloaded from 130.113.111.210 on Fri, 26 Feb 2016 02:39:38 UTC
(reverse scored) ⳮ.33 ⳮ.12 ⳮ.04 ⳮ.05 .02 ⳮ.03 .53

Note.—In each column of factor loadings, the items that were a part that factor are in bold print. See Kloosterman and Stage (1992) for the original scales.
298 THE ELEMENTARY SCHOOL JOURNAL

Table 3. Correlations among Belief Factors included in Analyses

Factor Q/F SA EM UM MC UMC

Quick/fixed learning (Q/F) ... .22** ⳮ.39** ⳮ.45** ⳮ.29** ⳮ.50**


Studying aimlessly (SA) ... ⳮ.40** ⳮ.33** ⳮ.12** ⳮ.27**
Effortful math (EM) ... .59** .30** .45**
Useful math (UM) ... .42** .55**
Math confidence (MC) ... .28**
Understand math concepts (UMC) ...

**p ⬍ .01.

ity is related to students’ appreciation of lessly, the more likely they were to believe
the benefits of education to one’s future that mathematical problem solving is ef-
(Schommer & Walker, 1997). fortful and useful. The amount of variance
accounted for was modest, ranging from
Epistemological Beliefs as Predictors of 5% to 9%. Although the relation between
Mathematical Problem-Solving Beliefs studying aimlessly and understanding math
To test these hypotheses, each MPSB was significant, the amount of variance ac-
Scale factor was regressed on two blocks of counted for (3%) was too small to consider
variables. First, because grade level has meaningful.
been associated with epistemological de-
velopment (Schommer et al., 1997) and gen- Predicting Mathematical Problem-
der has been associated with mathematical Solving Performance
performance (Reis & Park, 2001), these two To determine if general and mathemati-
variables were statistically controlled for by cal beliefs predicted mathematical problem
entering the equation in the first step. Sec- solving, we conducted a step-wise regres-
ond, we entered EB Scale factors into the sion. Epistemological beliefs, mathematical
equation using step-wise regression. That problem-solving beliefs, and gender com-
is, variables within blocks competed for en- peted for entry. The variable accounting for
try, so that the variable that accounted for the most variance entered the equation at
the most variance entered at each step. Only each step. Two variables predicted mathe-
variables that were significant at the .05 matical problem solving—belief in quick/
level were allowed to enter the equation. fixed learning and the belief that math is use-
Scores for each factor were sums of items ful. The less students believed in quick/fixed
that composed each factor. Table 4 presents learning, F(1, 316) ⳱ 24.07, p ⬍ .01, R2 ⳱ .07,
the regression analyses. and the more they believed in useful math,
As indicated by the negative b-weights, F(1, 315) ⳱ 5.20, p ⬍ .05, R2 ⳱ .02, the better
the less students believed in quick/fixed they were at problem solving.
learning, the more likely they were to believe
that mathematical problem solving is effort- Relating Belief Systems to Academic
ful and useful. Furthermore, they were more Performance
likely to believe problem solving requires To test the notion that epistemological
understanding and to have confidence in beliefs and mathematical problem-solving
their mathematical problem-solving ability. beliefs relate to academic performance as
The amount of variance accounted for by part of a larger system of cognition, we con-
this belief ranged from 10% to 25%. ducted a path analysis relating academic
Belief in studying aimlessly also related performance to belief system. To avoid is-
to mathematical problem-solving beliefs. sues of multicollinearity, we used the sig-
The less students believed in studying aim- nificant predictors in the previous regres-

JANUARY 2005

This content downloaded from 130.113.111.210 on Fri, 26 Feb 2016 02:39:38 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
BELIEFS AND PERFORMANCE 299

Table 4. Summary of Regression Statistics

Criterion/Predictor Variables R2 Change b weight Total R2 Total F

Effortful math: .25 127.97*


Grade ⬍ .01 ⳮ.62
Studying aimlessly .16 ⳮ.48
Quick/fixed learning .09 ⳮ.22
Useful math: .25 131.33*
Gender ⬍ .01 .63
Quick/fixed learning .20 ⳮ.31
Studying aimlessly .05 ⳮ.28
Math confidence: .12 54.93*
Gender .02 ⳮ.72
Grade .02 ⳮ.42
Quick/fixed learning .12 ⳮ.14
Understand math concepts: .30 169.06*
Gender .03 .89
Quick/fixed learning .25 ⳮ.20
Studying aimlessly .03 ⳮ.11

Note.—Boys were coded as 1, girls as 2.


*p ⬍ .05.

sion equation to represent general and Discussion


domain-specific beliefs. Hence, belief in Exploring the factor structure of middle
quick/fixed learning was the endogenous school students’ epistemological beliefs in-
variable that we hypothesized would di- dicates that a multidimensional model is
rectly affect belief about useful math and all applicable. Although initially we found
three measures of academic performance four domain general beliefs, we approached
(mathematical problem solving, reading, the data conservatively and continued anal-
and overall grade point average). We hy- yses with the two strongest epistemological
pothesized that belief in useful mathematics belief factors, quick/fixed learning and
would have a direct effect on two measures studying aimlessly. Because the multidi-
of academic performance—mathematical
mensional approach to middle school stu-
problem solving (research has indicated
dents’ epistemological beliefs is in its in-
that students who believe mathematics is
fancy, we cannot assume that these two
useful are more willing to take optional
beliefs capture the personal epistemology of
mathematical classes [Reyes, 1984]) and
all middle school students. Refinement of
overall grade point average (because grade
the questionnaire could yield more episte-
point average is derived from a number of
classes involving mathematics, i.e., sciences mological dimensions in the future. This
as well as mathematics). Research has shown limitation notwithstanding, these two epis-
that both general and domain-specific beliefs temological beliefs provide information
have indirect effects on academic perfor- that has not been revealed in previous re-
mance as well. Because reading comprehen- search.
sion is part of understanding the mathemat- In earlier research with older students,
ical problem to be solved, we hypothesized beliefs in quick learning and fixed ability
that comprehension scores would predict were more distinct because these beliefs of-
both mathematical problem solving and ten emerged as separate factors (Schommer,
overall grade point average. Figure 1 shows 1990). In contrast, the beliefs of the middle
the path model. All paths were significant at school students in this study about the na-
a .05 level. Overall fit indices were at ac- ture of learning emerge as a single factor,
ceptable levels: v2 ⳱ 1.51, p ⳱ .22, NFI ⳱ quick/fixed learning. This developmental
1.00; CFI ⳱ 1.00; RMSEA ⳱ .04. trend from undifferentiated to differenti-

This content downloaded from 130.113.111.210 on Fri, 26 Feb 2016 02:39:38 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
300 THE ELEMENTARY SCHOOL JOURNAL

Fig. 1.—Path analysis showing the relations among a general epistemological belief, a mathematical problem-
solving belief, and measures of academic performance. Standardized path coefficients are shown.

ated thinking is consistent with Wellman’s students believe that luck brings success,
(1990) conception of children’s theory of the then they view learning as out of their con-
mind. He suggested that young children trol rather than as strategic and effortful.
have a global theory of the mind. In con- Exploring middle school students’ math-
trast, adults conceptualize the mind as com- ematical problem-solving beliefs again pro-
posed of distinct processes and components vides new insight into the minds of younger
(Montgomery, 1992). students. Although the factors generated
Additionally, our results generate an from the analyses are similar to findings for
epistemological belief that is not evident older students, there are only four psycho-
among older students, namely, studying metrically adequate factors: effortful math,
aimlessly, which suggests that some young useful math, understand math concepts, and
students do not believe that learning is stra- math confidence. Similar mathematical be-
tegic. Rather, they view learning as a chance liefs, such as self-confidence in math, per-
event. Ironically, if students receive an ac- ceived usefulness of math, and importance
ceptable grade after engaging in haphazard of math, have been obtained by interviewing
studying, their belief in studying aimlessly young students (Kloosterman & Cougan,
can be strengthened. The belief in studying 1994; Kloosterman, Raymond, & Emenaker,
aimlessly seems to be consistent with one 1996).
view of attribution theory, the view that The fact that belief in quick/fixed learn-
success is related to luck (Weiner, 2000). If ing is a predictor of all four mathematical

JANUARY 2005

This content downloaded from 130.113.111.210 on Fri, 26 Feb 2016 02:39:38 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
BELIEFS AND PERFORMANCE 301

problem-solving beliefs indicates the poten- thermore, in our data a strong relation exists
tial influence of this belief. This finding is between general and domain-specific be-
consistent with past research showing that liefs. Whether mathematical beliefs are a
belief in quick learning, in particular, influ- manifestation of general epistemological
ences students’ thinking in high school and beliefs, as De Corte et al. (2002) proposed,
college. For example, the more high school remains to be examined in future research.
students believe in quick learning, the What is more important is that researchers
lower the grade point average they earn begin to understand how beliefs fit with
(Schommer, 1993; Schommer et al., 1997) and function among cognitive and affective
and the more likely they are to display poor characteristics of learners.
mathematical problem-solving strategies In summary, our findings have important
(Schoenfeld, 1983). The more college stu- theoretical and practical implications. First,
dents believe in quick learning, the more middle school students’ epistemological be-
poorly they comprehend, the less likely liefs can be conceptualized as multidimen-
they are to monitor their comprehension of sional. The dimensions are somewhat differ-
complex text accurately (Schommer, 1990), ent from those of the beliefs of older students
and the fewer cognitive processes they tend in that beliefs in quick learning and fixed
to exhibit when reading controversial text ability are tightly woven in our sample. Be-
(Kardash & Howell, 2000). lief in strategic studying, or lack thereof, is
The results of our study also suggest critical to middle school students’ beliefs
that both general epistemological beliefs about mathematical problem solving.
and mathematical beliefs may play a role in The fact that epistemological beliefs are
students’ problem-solving performance. multiply linked to mathematical problem-
Path analysis provides deeper examination solving beliefs suggests that general epis-
of the interrelations between belief systems temological beliefs innervate students’
and academic performance. In the model thinking in specific academic domains.
we tested in this study, it appears that stu- Consequently, this research suggests topics
dents’ belief in quick/fixed learning leads to investigate in the future, for example,
to viewing mathematics as of little use. In the influences that reading teachers, sci-
turn, the less students believe mathematics ence teachers, music teachers, or writing
is useful, the less likely they are to solve teachers have on students’ general episte-
problems successfully. Believing in quick/ mology. In turn, how do general episte-
fixed learning also implies a belief that the mological beliefs influence what students
modus operandi of studying is to speed think about the learning or problem-solv-
through the reading and solving of mathe- ing process in various school subjects?
matics problems. All these cognitive and af- This research offers some thought-pro-
fective processes appear to lead to a lower voking ideas for the classroom teacher. For
grade point average. In short, our results example, if middle school students have a
support the hypothesis that belief in quick/ strong belief in quick/fixed learning, they
fixed learning may guide students in their will assume that all assignments should be
choice of problem-solving strategies and the completed in a short amount of time. When
amount of time they spend on solving faced with more challenging assignments,
mathematical problems (Schoenfeld, 1985; some students who focus on the quick belief
Schommer et al., 1992). may have a predetermined amount of time
Both general epistemological beliefs and in which they study. When the time is up,
mathematical beliefs appear to influence they move to different activities. Other stu-
mathematical performance and overall ac- dents who focus on the fixed belief and do
ademic performance. Both systems of be- not experience success immediately may
liefs have direct and indirect effects. Fur- cease to try because they assume that if they

This content downloaded from 130.113.111.210 on Fri, 26 Feb 2016 02:39:38 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
302 THE ELEMENTARY SCHOOL JOURNAL

did have the ability, they would have solved De Corte, E., & Op’t Eynde, P. (2003, April).
a problem quickly, a notion that is consistent “When girls value mathematics as highly as
boys”: An analysis of junior high students’ math-
with Dweck’s work (Dweck & Leggett, ematics-related beliefs. Paper presented at the
1988). What this could mean for the teacher annual meeting of the American Educational
is that some students may need to be fore- Research Association, Chicago.
warned that a task will be challenging and De Corte, E., Op’t Eynde, P., & Verschaffel, L.
time-consuming. Some students may need (2002). “Knowing what to believe”: The rele-
vance of students’ mathematical beliefs for
help and encouragement to put in the time mathematics education. In B. K. Hofer & P.
and effort needed to complete the assign- R. Pintrich (Eds.), Personal epistemology: The
ment with accuracy and understanding. psychology of beliefs about knowledge and know-
If middle school students have a strong ing (pp. 297–320). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
belief that mathematics is not useful in their Duell, O. K., & Schommer-Aikins, M. (2001).
Measures of people’s beliefs about knowl-
lives or future careers, they may resist edge and learning. Educational Psychology Re-
spending time or effort needed to be suc- view, 13, 419–449.
cessful at it. It may be helpful if teachers can Dweck, C. S., & Bempechat, J. (1983). Children’s
make mathematics tasks instrinsically inter- theories of intelligence: Consequences for
esting and situate mathematics problems in learning. In S. G. Paris, G. M. Olson, & H. W.
Stevenson (Eds.), Learning and motivation in
middle school students’ current interests, the classroom (pp. 239–256). Hillsdale, NJ: Erl-
such as sports, music, and popular culture. baum.
In short, teachers can make mathematics Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-
fun and applicable to students’ lives. cognitive approach to motivation and per-
We see this research as only the begin- sonality. Psychological Review, 95, 256–273.
Fennema, E., & Sherman, J. (1976). Fennema-Sher-
ning of understanding young students’ man mathematics attitudes scales: Instrument
epistemological beliefs. The middle school designed to measure attitude toward the learning
epistemological questionnaire, though use- of mathematics by females and males. Madison:
ful at this point, may need to be revised so University of Wisconsin, Center for Educa-
that more epistemological and mathemati- tional Research.
Hofer, B. (2000). Dimensionality and discipli-
cal belief factors are assessed by more items. nary differences in personal epistemology.
Studies are necessary to understand the Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25,
epistemological beliefs of students who are 378–405.
more culturally diverse. And research on Hofer, B., & Pintrich, P. (1997). The development
how epistemological beliefs function as a of epistemological theories: Beliefs about
knowledge and knowing and their relation
part of a much larger system would be in- to learning. Review of Educational Research, 67,
formative. That is, researchers should deter- 88–140.
mine how epistemological beliefs are related Jehng, J. J., Johnson, S. D., & Anderson, R. C.
to other aspects of cognition, such as prob- (1993). Schooling and students’ epistemolog-
ical beliefs about learning. Contemporary Edu-
lem solving, self-concept, motivation, study
cational Psychology, 18, 23–35.
strategies, comprehension monitoring, cul- Kardash, C. M., & Howell, K. L. (2000). Effects
tural identity, and cognitive flexibility. of epistemological beliefs and topic-specific
beliefs on undergraduates’ cognitive and
strategic processing of dual-positional text.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 524–
535.
References Kardash, C. M., & Scholes, R. J. (1996). Effects of
preexisting beliefs, epistemological beliefs,
and need for cognition on interpretation of
Buehl, M. M., Alexander, P. A., & Murphy, P. K. controversial issues. Journal of Educational
(2002). Beliefs about schooled knowledge: Psychology, 88, 260–271.
Domain specific or domain general? Con- Kitchener, K. S., & King, P. M. (1981). Reflective
temporary Educational Psychology, 27, 415– judgement: Concepts of justification and
449. their relationship to age and education. Jour-

JANUARY 2005

This content downloaded from 130.113.111.210 on Fri, 26 Feb 2016 02:39:38 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
BELIEFS AND PERFORMANCE 303

nal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 2, 89– Schommer, M. (1994). An emerging conceptu-


116. alization of epistemological beliefs and their
Kitchener, K. S., & King, P. M. (1989). The reflec- role in learning. In R. Garner & P. A. Alex-
tive judgement model: Ten years of research. ander (Eds.), Beliefs about text and instruction
In M. L. Commons, C. Armon, L. Kohlberg, with text (pp. 25–40). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
F. A. Richards, T. A. Grotzer, & J. D. Sinot Schommer, M. (1998). The role of adults’ beliefs
(Eds.), Adult development 2: Models and meth- about knowledge and learning in school,
ods in the study of adolescent and adult thought work, and everyday life. In M. C. Smith & T.
(pp. 63–78). New York: Praeger. Pourchot (Eds.), Adult learning and develop-
Kloosterman, P. (1988). Self-confidence and mo- ment: Perspectives from educational psychology
tivation in mathematics. Journal of Educational (pp.127–143). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Psychology, 80, 345–351. Schommer, M., Calvert, C., Gariglietti, G., & Ba-
Kloosterman, P., & Cougan, M. C. (1994). Stu- jaj, A. (1997). The development of epistemo-
dents’ beliefs about learning school mathe- logical beliefs among secondary students: A
matics. Elementary School Journal, 94, 375–388. longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psy-
Kloosterman, P., Raymond, A. M., & Emenaker, chology, 89, 37–40.
C. (1996). Students’ beliefs about mathemat- Schommer, M., Crouse, A., & Rhodes, N. (1992).
ics: A three-year study. Elementary School Epistemological beliefs and mathematical
Journal, 97, 3–56. text comprehension: Believing it is simple
Kloosterman, P., & Stage, F. K. (1992). Measuring does not make it so. Journal of Educational
beliefs about mathematical problem solving. Psychology, 84, 435–443.
School Science and Mathematics, 92, 109–115. Schommer, M., & Walker, K. (1997). Epistemo-
Montgomery, D. E. (1992). Young children’s the- logical beliefs and valuing school: Consid-
ory of knowing: The development of a folk erations for college admissions and reten-
epistemology. Developmental Review, 12, 410– tion. Research in Higher Education, 38, 173–
430. 186.
Op’t Eynde, P., & De Corte, E. (2003, April). Stu- Schommer-Aikins, M. (2002). An evolving theo-
dents’ mathematics-related belief systems: Design retical framework for an epistemological be-
and analysis of a questionnaire. Paper presented lief system. In B. K. Hofer & P. R. Pintrich
at the annual meeting of the American Edu- (Eds.), Personal epistemology: The psychology of
cational Research Association, Chicago. beliefs about knowledge and knowing (pp. 103–
Perry, Jr., W. G. (1968). Patterns of development in 118). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
thought and values of students in a liberal arts Schommer-Aikins, M., Mau, W., Brookhart, S., &
college: A validation of a scheme. Cambridge, Hutter, R. (2000). Understanding middle stu-
MA: Harvard University, Bureau of Study dents’ beliefs about knowledge and learning
Counsel. (ERIC Document Reproduction using a multidimensional paradigm. Journal
Service No. ED 024315) of Educational Research, 94, 120–127.
Reis, S. M., & Park, S. (2001). Gender differences Schraw, G., Bendixen, L. D., & Dunkle, M. E.
in high-achieving students in math and sci- (2002). Development and validation of the
ence. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 25, epistemic belief inventory (EBI). In B. K.
52–73.
Hofer & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Personal episte-
Reyes, L. H. (1984). Affective variables and
mology: The psychology of beliefs about knowl-
mathematics education. Elementary School
edge and knowing (pp. 261–275). Mahwah, NJ:
Journal, 84, 558–581.
Schoenfeld, A. H. (1983). Beyond the purely cog- Erlbaum.
nitive: Belief systems, social cognitions, and Schraw, G. S., Dunkle, M. E., & Bendixen, L. D.
metacognitions as driving forces in intellec- (1995). Cognitive processes in well-defined
tual performance. Cognitive Science, 7, 329– and ill-defined problem solving. Applied Cog-
363. nitive Psychology, 9, 523–538.
Schoenfeld, A. H. (1985). Mathematical problem Stage, F. K., & Kloosterman, P. (1995). Gender,
solving. New York: Academic Press. beliefs, and achievement in remedial college-
Schommer, M. (1990). The effects of beliefs level mathematics. Journal of Higher Educa-
about the nature of knowledge on compre- tion, 66, 294–311.
hension. Journal of Educational Psychology, Weiner, B. (2000). Intrapersonal and interper-
82, 498–504. sonal theories of motivation from an attri-
Schommer, M. (1993). Epistemological develop- butional perspective. Educational Psychology
ment and academic performance among sec- Review, 12, 1–14.
ondary students. Journal of Educational Psy- Wellman, H. M. (1990). The child’s theory of mind.
chology, 85, 406–411. Cambridge, MA: Bradford/MIT Press.

This content downloaded from 130.113.111.210 on Fri, 26 Feb 2016 02:39:38 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
304 THE ELEMENTARY SCHOOL JOURNAL

Winne, P. H., Graham, L., & Prock, L. (1993). A in the design and analysis of studies of epis-
model of poor readers’ text-based inferenc- temology. In B. K. Hofer & P. R. Pintrich
ing: Effects of explanatory feedback. Reading (Eds.), Personal epistemology: The psychology of
Research Quarterly, 28, 52–66. beliefs about knowledge (pp. 231–261). Mah-
Wood, P., & Kardash, C. (2002). Critical elements wah, NJ: Erlbaum.

JANUARY 2005

This content downloaded from 130.113.111.210 on Fri, 26 Feb 2016 02:39:38 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like