You are on page 1of 11

818167

research-article2018
AUT0010.1177/1362361318818167AutismAu-Yeung et al.

Original Article

Autism

Experience of mental health diagnosis 1­–11


© The Author(s) 2018
Article reuse guidelines:
and perceived misdiagnosis in autistic, sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/1362361318818167
https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361318818167

possibly autistic and non-autistic adults journals.sagepub.com/home/aut

Sheena K Au-Yeung1 , Louise Bradley1, Ashley E Robertson1 ,


Rebecca Shaw1,2, Simon Baron-Cohen3,4 and Sarah Cassidy1,3,5

Abstract
Previous research shows that autistic people have high levels of co-occurring mental health conditions. Yet, a number
of case reports have revealed that mental health conditions are often misdiagnosed in autistic individuals. A total of
420 adults who identified as autistic, possibly autistic or non-autistic completed an online survey consisting of questions
regarding mental health diagnoses they received, whether they agreed with those diagnoses and if not why. Autistic and
possibly autistic participants were more likely to report receiving mental health diagnoses compared to non-autistic
participants, but were less likely to agree with those diagnoses. Thematic analysis revealed the participants’ main reasons
for disagreement were that (1) they felt their autism characteristics were being confused with mental health conditions
by healthcare professionals and (2) they perceived their own mental health difficulties to be resultant of ASC. Participants
attributed these to the clinical barriers they experienced, including healthcare professionals’ lack of autism awareness
and lack of communication, which in turn prevented them from receiving appropriate support. This study highlights
the need for autism awareness training for healthcare professionals and the need to develop tools and interventions to
accurately diagnose and effectively treat mental health conditions in autistic individuals.

Keywords
adults, autism spectrum disorders, diagnosis, mixed methods, prevalence, psychiatric comorbidity, qualitative research

Introduction during their lifetime, with mood and anxiety disorders


being the most common, and at a significantly greater rate
Autism spectrum condition (ASC)1 is classified as a neu- compared to non-autistic adults.
rodevelopmental condition, characterised by persistent Despite increased risk factors for, and incidence of,
difficulties in social communication and interaction, as mental health conditions in autistic individuals, conditions
well as restricted, repetitive patterns of behaviour, interests such as depression may still be under-diagnosed given the
and activities (American Psychiatric Association (APA), lack of validated assessment tools able to accurately detect
2013). These symptoms present during early development the unique presentation of mental health difficulties in this
and cause a range of strengths and difficulties in multiple group (Cassidy et al., 2018a, 2018b). Although ASC is not
domains. Recent studies suggest that autistic difficulties
could increase the likelihood of being exposed to risk fac-
tors for developing mental health conditions. For example, 1Coventry University, UK
higher autistic traits were associated with self-reported 2Coventry and Warwickshire Partnership NHS Trust, UK
3University of Cambridge, UK
loneliness in autistic adults (Hedley et al., 2018) and the 4Cambridge Lifespan Asperger Syndrome Service (CLASS), UK
experience of social disconnectedness, and feeling a bur- 5University of Nottingham, UK
den on others in the general population (Pelton and
Cassidy, 2017); these factors in turn increased the risk of Corresponding author:
Sheena K Au-Yeung, Centre for Innovative Research Across the Life
both depression and suicidality. Indeed, Lever and Geurts Course, Coventry University, Richard Crossman 4th Floor, Jordan
(2016) established that 79% of autistic adults met the cri- Well, Coventry CV1 5FB, UK.
teria for some form of psychiatric co-occurring condition Email: Sheena.Au-Yeung@coventry.ac.uk
2 Autism 00(0)

a mental health condition, the characteristics of ASC can and perceived misdiagnoses were more common in autis-
overlap with many indicators of mental health conditions; tic individuals compared to the general population. We
for example, social withdrawal, appetite and sleep distur- included a group of possibly autistic people who reported
bance are common characteristics in both ASC and depres- suspecting that they may have an undiagnosed ASC. The
sion (Stewart et al., 2006). This means that an established reasons for this are that yet-to-be-diagnosed autistic indi-
diagnosis of ASC could overshadow the presence of men- viduals may be particularly vulnerable to misdiagnosis of
tal health symptoms, potentially leaving co-occurring mental health conditions (Dosseter, 2007; Luciano et al.,
mental health conditions unrecognised (Matson and 2014; Van Schalkwyk et al., 2015) and also that autistic
Williams, 2013). The consequences of failing to accurately individuals often play an important role in the recognition
diagnose co-occurring mental health conditions are poten- of their ASC. Jones et al. (2014) looked at the experience
tially devastating and increase the risk of contemplating, of those who had received an ASC diagnosis and found
planning (Cassidy et al., 2014) and dying by suicide that 44.5% of their respondents reported raising the possi-
(Hirvikoski et al., 2016). bility of ASC themselves, whereas only 4.1% reported that
It is also possible that symptom overlap could lead to a professional raised the possibility. It is also well known
overdiagnosis and misdiagnosis of mental health condi- that certain characteristics affect the likelihood of receiv-
tions in autistic individuals (Lai and Baron-Cohen, 2015). ing an autism diagnosis, for example, females are diag-
In a case series, Dosseter (2007) re-evaluated four young nosed less frequently than males (Brugha et al., 2011).
clients, aged 8–16 years, who had been originally diag- This may be due to differences in presentation, such as
nosed with early-onset psychosis. The individuals reported better nonverbal communication skills and the ability to
unusual experiences, exhibited bizarre behaviours and had camouflage other diagnostic features (Rynkiewicz et al.,
preoccupations with objects and imaginary people or 2016). Seeking a diagnosis in adulthood may be problem-
worlds. However, when the clients’ impairments in social atic due to the development of compensatory strategies
communication and reciprocity were taken into considera- and a possible lack of informant (e.g. parent) for develop-
tion, it was concluded that clients’ presentation could be mental history (Lai and Baron-Cohen, 2015). For these
better explained by ASC, with the preoccupations reasons, it is important for the experiences of this previ-
accounted for under ‘restricted, repetitive patterns of ously under-researched group, namely, possibly autistic
behaviour, interest, and activities’ (APA, 2013). Other case adults, to be heard.
reports (Luciano et al., 2014; Van Schalkwyk et al., 2015)
also indicated that mental health professionals’ failure to
collect information about developmental history, and lack Method
of understanding about ASC presentation, contributed to
mental health misdiagnoses in both individuals with an
Materials
existing autism diagnosis and those who obtained a diag- Survey development. Research priorities for the autism
nosis during the studies. Misdiagnosis of ASC as a mental community can be very different from those of the
health condition could lead to unnecessary treatment (e.g. researchers (Pellicano et al., 2014). Therefore, it is impor-
medication), which could preclude the possibility of hav- tant that autistic people participate in setting priorities to
ing other, more relevant, needs met. ensure that the research conducted is useful to those most
While previous case studies (Dosseter, 2007; Luciano affected by it (Fletcher-Watson et al., 2018). As such, an
et al., 2014; Van Schalkwyk et al., 2015) provide impor- online survey exploring various topics including mental
tant clues to the contributing factors for mental health health diagnoses, self-injury and suicidality was devel-
misdiagnosis in autistic individuals from the perspective oped in partnership with a steering group of eight autistic
of professionals specialising in ASC, there is currently adults (six females, two males) through a series of six
little known about autistic individuals’ subjective experi- focus groups (see Cassidy et al., 2018c, for details of the
ence of mental health diagnosis and perceived misdiagno- full data set). At the start of the study, the steering group
sis. Therefore, this article aims to address the following discussed what their role should be and decided it would
research questions: (1) Are autistic individuals more be to inform the study conception, design, recruitment and
likely to report receiving mental health diagnosis(es) and dissemination of results. The first three focus groups
if so, what kind of diagnoses were they more likely to focused on developing the questions in the online survey
receive? (2) Are autistic individuals more likely to disa- about a number of topics identified from a systematic
gree with these diagnoses? and (3) What were the reasons review of the literature thought to be important contribu-
behind any disagreement? tors to mental health and suicidality in autism. The focus
Three participant groups were included, comprising group fed back on the relevance of these proposed topics,
individuals who were identified as autistic, non-autistic or whether any important topics were missing and their own
possibly autistic. Inclusion of the non-autistic group experiences in relation to the topics identified. The
allowed us to examine whether mental health diagnoses researchers then developed a survey with questions that
Au-Yeung et al. 3

would allow participants’ experiences of these topics to be themselves on 50 statements with the options ‘Definitely
captured. Once the survey had been designed, the steering Agree’, ‘Slightly Agree’, ‘Slightly Disagree’ and ‘Defi-
group provided feedback on three drafts until agreement nitely Disagree’. Woodbury-Smith et al. (2005) recom-
was reached that the questions were comprehensive, rele- mended a cut-off of 26 for screening in clinical practice
vant and clear. The data collected in relation to mental and a cut-off of 32 for the general population, to limit false
health diagnoses were analysed and are reported in this positives. We followed Hoekstra et al.’s (2007) methodol-
article. Data relating to other topics collected from the sur- ogy to manage missing items. If five or fewer items were
vey are reported elsewhere (see Camm-Crosbie et al., left blank, the following equation was used to correct for
2018). the number of missing items: total subscale score + (mean
subscale item score × number of missing items in sub-
Mental health diagnosis questions. Participants answered scale). The corrected subscale scores were summed to get
closed- and open-ended questions on their mental health the total AQ score. Missing items accounted for 0.19% of
diagnoses. First, they were asked ‘Have you ever been the analysed data. If a participant left more than five items
diagnosed with a mental health condition(s) or other blank, their data were discarded. Two participants’ data
condition(s) by a trained clinician?’ to which they were excluded based on this criterion.
responded with either ‘Yes’ or ‘No’. They were subse-
quently asked to select the specific mental health or other
Participants
condition(s) including ‘Depression, Anxiety, Obsessive
Compulsive Disorder (OCD), Bipolar, Personality Disor- Participants were recruited through charities, Cambridge
der, Schizophrenia, Anorexia, Bulimia and Other’. If par- Autism Research Database (CARD), Cambridge
ticipants selected ‘Personality Disorder’ or ‘Other’ from Psychology, online advertisement, MHAutism newsletter
the list, they were given a free text box to specify the sub- and MHAutism webpage (http://mhautism.coventry.
type of Personality Disorder or other mental health condi- ac.uk). Participants were invited to take part in an online
tions they were diagnosed with. Participants were then survey exploring experiences of mental health problems,
asked, ‘Do you agree with your diagnosis/es’, with the fol- self-injury and suicidality and could take part regardless of
lowing response options: ‘Yes’, ‘No’ or ‘Not entirely’. If prior experience of these difficulties. Data from 420 adults
participants selected ‘No’ or ‘Not entirely’, they were sub- (18–67 years) who completed the ‘mental health diagno-
sequently asked, ‘Please tell us what mental health diagno- ses’ section of the online survey were analysed. Data from
sis/es you disagree with, and why’ and indicated their respondents who did not report their age (n = 52), gender
response using a free text box. (n = 3) or diagnostic status (n = 0) were excluded. The
These diagnoses and order were chosen due to a combi- autistic group was significantly younger than the non-
nation of standard questions our group regularly uses in autistic group (p = 0.013), and there was a significantly
previously published research and also feedback from the greater proportion of males and smaller proportion of
steering group regarding clarity. For example, participants females compared to both the possible autistic and non-
noted that it was easier to have staged questions starting autistic groups (ps > 0.05). Welch’s analysis of variance
wide, and then narrowing (Personality Disorder subtype), (ANOVA) revealed that there were significant between-
to keep the survey questions relevant throughout. group differences in AQ scores, F(2, 189) = 281, p < 0.001.
Autistic and possibly autistic participants scored signifi-
Demographic questions.  As part of the survey, participants cantly higher on the AQ than non-autistic participants
were asked to provide information such as age, gender, (ps < 0.001). Autistic participants also scored significantly
autism diagnosis and age of diagnosis. Participants were higher than possibly autistic participants on the AQ
divided into three groups for analysis based on their autism (p = 0.005). Participants’ characteristics are reported in
diagnostic status. Participants in the non-autistic group Table 1.
reported no formal diagnosis of ASC and no suspicion that
they have ASC. Participants in the autistic group reported
Procedure
receiving a formal diagnosis of ASC from a qualified clini-
cian. Participants in the possibly autistic group reported The study was approved by Coventry University
that they thought they may have ASC and either (1) were Psychology Ethics Committee, the autism steering group
awaiting assessment from a qualified clinician or (2) had who fed back on the questionnaire and the scientific
not yet sought an assessment. advisory group at the Autism Research Centre, University
of Cambridge. Participants were invited to complete
Autism Spectrum Quotient.  The Autism Spectrum Quotient the survey on Qualtrics (https://www.qualtrics.com/uk/).
(AQ; Baron-Cohen et al., 2001) is a measure of autistic Participants read the participant information sheet and
traits with scores ranging from 0 to 50. Greater scores indi- indicated informed consent to participate via an online
cate higher levels of autistic traits. Participants rated form. Participants were fully briefed about the nature of
4 Autism 00(0)

Table 1.  Participants’ characteristics.

Autistic (n = 208) Possibly autistic (n = 71) Non-autistic (n = 141)


Mean age (SD), range 38.6 (11.4), 18–67 40.1 (8.9), 20–57 42.1 (10.6), 20–60
Number of males 72 (34.6%) 11 (15.5%) 20 (14.2%)
Number of females 136 (65.4%) 60 (84.5%) 121 (85.8%)
Mean AQ (SD) 38.6 (6.8) 35.8 (5.9) 17.5 (8.7)
Mean age of diagnosis (SD), range 34.5 (12.2), 4–59  

SD: standard deviation.

Table 2.  Number of participants who reported diagnosis of mental health condition or other conditions.

‘Have you ever been diagnosed with a mental health Autistic Possibly autistic Non-autistic
condition(s) or other condition(s) by a trained clinician?’ (n = 208) (n = 71) (n = 141)
Yes 183 (88%) 57 (80.3%) 74 (52.5%)
No 25 (12.0%) 14 (19.7%) 67 (47.5%)

the research, that they could skip sections and/or ques- themes. Thereafter, related themes and subthemes were
tions that they felt uncomfortable with, and were provided consolidated, while those that were deemed to not mean-
with information about relevant support services before ingfully address the research question were discarded.
and after participation.
Results
Analysis
Rate of lifetime mental health diagnoses and
Quantitative analysis. Statistical analyses were conducted
using SPSS version 24. Chi-square tests for homogeneity
other conditions
were computed to test for between-group differences in the There were significant between-group differences in the
proportions of (1) individuals reported receiving mental proportions of participants reporting mental health or other
health and other diagnoses, (2) specific mental health diag- diagnoses, χ2(2) = 57.5, p < 0.001, V = 0.370 (see Table 2).
noses received and (3) agreement with mental health diag- Autistic and possibly autistic participants were more likely
noses. If a chi-square was significant, post-hoc z tests were to report receiving mental health or other diagnoses com-
then computed to further examine pairwise differences in pared to non-autistic participants (p < 0.05). There was no
group proportions with automated adjusted p-values (Bon- significant difference between autistic and possibly autis-
ferroni method). Fisher’s exact tests were used instead if tic participants (p < 0.05).
any of the expected cell count were less than 5. Cramer’s Thereafter, we compared the proportion of individuals
V was calculated for effect size (small effect = 0.1, medium reporting specific mental health diagnosis between groups
effect = 0.3, large effect = 0.5; Cohen, 1988). (see Table 3 for omnibus test statistics and pairwise com-
parisons). Types of diagnoses that participants selected or
Qualitative analysis.  Thematic analysis was conducted for mentioned were grouped for analysis based on Diagnostic
participants’ responses to the question, ‘Please tell us what and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed.;
mental health diagnosis/es you disagree with, and why’. DSM-5 classifications; APA, 2013). There were no signifi-
Responses with no mention of the specific diagnosis(es) cant between-group differences in proportion of individu-
they disagreed with were excluded. Braun and Clarke’s als who reported diagnosis of Schizophrenia and Other
(2006) methodology for thematic analysis was followed. Psychotic Disorders, Bipolar and Related Disorders,
This enabled exploration of the research question in a data- Dissociative Disorders, Gender Dysphoria and Substance
driven manner informed by participants’ perspective and Related and Addictive Disorders. However, there were sig-
experience. S.K.A-Y. read through the responses to gain an nificant between-group differences in the proportion of
overall impression of the data set and noted any emerging individuals reporting diagnoses of Depressive Disorders,
patterns. The responses were then reread and initial codes Anxiety Disorders, Obsessive Compulsive and Related
were generated. Codes describing similar concepts were Disorders, Trauma and Stressor Related Disorder, Feeding
grouped into preliminary themes and subthemes, which and Eating Disorders and Personality Disorders, with con-
were subsequently reviewed by A.E.R., L.B. and S.C. Dis- sistently greater number of participants in the autistic and/
cussion among the authors led to further refinement of the or possibly autistic groups reporting these diagnoses.
Au-Yeung et al. 5

Table 3.  Number of participants who reported mental health diagnoses.

Autistic Possibly autistic Non-autistic Omnibus test Pairwise


(n = 208) (n = 71) (n = 141) comparisons
Depressive Disorders 161 (77.4%) 52 (73.2%) 60 (42.6%) χ2(2) = 47.4, p < 0.001, ASC > NASC
V = 0.336 PASC > NASC
ASC = PASC
Anxiety Disorders 148 (71.2%) 45 (63.4%) 48 (34%) χ2(2) = 48.6 p < 0.001, ASC > NASC
V = 0.340 PASC > NASC
ASC = PASC
Obsessive-Compulsive 40 (19.2%) 8 (11.3%) 3 (2.1%) χ2(2) = 23.1, p < 0.001, ASC > NASC
and Related Disorders V = 0.235 PASC > NASC
ASC = PASC
Personality Disorders 25 (12.0%) 8 (11.3%) 6 (4.3%) χ2(2) = 6.41, p = 0.041, ASC > NASC
V = 0.124 ASC = PASC
PASC = NASC
Feeding and Eating 21 (10.1%) 8 (11.3%) 4 (2.8%) χ2(2) = 7.49, p = 0.024, ASC > NASC
Disorders V = 0.134 PASC > NASC
ASC = PASC
Bipolar and Related 12 (5.8%) 5 (7%) 2 (1.4%) Fisher’s exact test = 5.49,  
Disorders p = 0.051, V = 0.108
Schizophrenia 7 (3.4%) 3 (4.2%) 1 (0.7%) Fisher’s exact test = 3.49,  
Spectrum and Other p = 0.149, V = 0.087
Psychotic Disorders
Trauma and Stressor 6 (2.9%) 6 (8.5%) 1 (0.7%) Fisher’s exact test = 8.19, PASC > NASC
Related Disorders p = 0.012, V = 0.150 ASC = NASC
ASC = PASC
Gender Dysphoria 2 (1%) 0 (0%) 1 (0.7%) Fisher’s exact test = 0.505,  
p = 1.00, V = 0.041
Dissociative Disorders 0 (0%) 1 (1.4%) 0 (0%) Fisher’s exact test = 3.46,  
p = 0.169, V = 0.108
Substance Related and 0 (0%) 1 (1.4%) 0 (0%) Fisher’s exact test = 3.46,  
Addictive Disorders p = 0.169, V = 0.108

ASC: autistic group; NASC = non-autistic group; PASC = possibly autistic group. More detailed analyses were carried out on the subtypes of
personality disorders, but no significant between-group differences were found (see Supplemental Materials A).

Table 4.  Number of participants who show agreement or disagreement with their mental health diagnosis.

‘Do you agree with your Autistic Possibly autistic Non-autistic


mental health diagnosis/es?’ (n = 182) (n = 57) (n = 74)
Yes 106 (58.2%) 26 (45.6%) 64 (86.5%)
No 12 (6.6%) 2 (3.5%) 3 (4.1%)
Not entirely 64 (35.2%) 29 (50.9%) 7 (9.5%)

Disagreement with mental health diagnosis that a higher proportion of autistic and possibly autistic par-
ticipants did not entirely agree with their mental health
There were significant between-group differences in the
diagnosis compared to non-autistic participants, while a
proportion of individuals who reported disagreement with
greater proportion of non-autistic participants agreed with
their mental health diagnoses as denoted in response to an
open-ended question on this topic, Fisher’s exact test = 31.8, their mental health diagnosis compared to the other two
p < 0.001, V = 0.218 (see Table 4). Two of the post-hoc groups. None of the other seven comparisons reached sig-
pairwise comparisons were significant: these were the 2 nificance (ps > 0.05). Proportions of participants who disa-
(non-autistic vs autistic) × 2 (Yes vs Not entirely) compari- greed with specific type of mental health diagnoses were
son and the 2 (non-autistic vs possibly autistic) × 2 (Yes vs compared between groups, but no significant differences
Not entirely) comparison (both ps < 0.001). These showed were found (see Supplemental Materials B).
6 Autism 00(0)

Thematic analysis category of ‘restricted, repetitive patterns of behaviour,


interests, or activities’ (APA, 2013):
This section explored the reasons underlying disagreement
with mental health diagnosis(es). Data for the autistic and … I think that ties in with possibly having an ASC as not
possibly autistic participants were analysed together, coping well with change is a characteristic of that.
because our quantitative analysis showed that both groups (445-PASC)
were significantly more likely to report disagreement with
their mental health diagnoses compared to the non-autistic Similarly, another possibly autistic participant who
group (see Table 4). Therefore, it is likely that participants received a bipolar disorder diagnosis felt their intense
in the possibly autistic group have shared experiences with interest had been mistaken as a sign of mania:
those in the autistic group. Spelling and grammatical errors
were corrected in participants’ extracts; these are denoted … I get mood swings, but I believe that is more due to the
in square brackets in the quotes. Each quote is followed Autism than manic depression. I am subdued when I am
by a participant number and their diagnostic status uncomfortable and what may look manic when I am doing
something I like … (238-PASC)
(ASC = autistic, PASC = possibly autistic). Data from the
non-autistic group were not analysed as very few of these
Some participants spoke about sensory sensitivities that
participants showed disagreement and responded to the
they felt were mistaken as eating disorders or psychotic
open-ended question. Two major themes emerged from the
symptoms:
thematic analysis: ‘Problems with Mental Health
Diagnosis’ and ‘Clinical Barriers’. My issues with food were regarding the texture and the act of
eating. (452-PASC)
Problems with mental health diagnosis.  This theme consists
of two sub-themes: ‘ASC characteristics confused with I stopped going to the school canteen as a teenager and lost a
symptoms of a mental health condition’ and ‘mental health lot of weight because I couldn’t stand the social/sensory
difficulties perceived to be resultant of ASC’. environment. (2-ASC)

ASC characteristics confused with symptoms of a mental I am also not psychotic – I have [enhanced] sensory perception.
health condition. The first most common reason for par- (28-ASC)
ticipants’ disagreement with their mental health diagnoses
In addition to having autism characteristics miscatego-
was that they felt their ASC characteristics were mistaken
rised by healthcare professionals, some participants
as symptoms of a mental health condition. For example, in
thought clues that were indicative of ASC might have been
the following extract, an autistic participant explains how
missed. One autistic participant diagnosed with personal-
their difficulties with emotional regulation and self-inju-
ity disorder reported:
rious behaviours may have been misinterpreted as symp-
toms of a borderline personality disorder (BPD):2 … autism runs so strongly in my family (all four children
have AS/autism, and I think my father probably has Asperger
… [it’s] just aspergers with me, the out coming behaviour
Syndrome). (175-ASC)
might look or even be the same sometimes ([self-harm],
getting [emotional] etc) but the causes are completely
Likewise, a possibly autistic participant who was diag-
different- [I] learnt to hide my emotions and feelings to
survive school and home without being hurt so only [got] nosed with OCD reported,
visibly upset in the last moment when [it became] unbearable-
just because they cannot read my face [doesn’t] mean [I’m] … I suspect that is really ASC, but it was the 80s and I am
not having those emotions before … (153-ASC) female. I have never been able to deal with much of anything
in the morning, and apparently shoe tying before school was
the last straw when little. (741-PASC)
The same participant also reported that their lack of eye
contact was mistaken as evidence of schizophrenia rather
These extracts highlight that the importance of family
than social communication difficulty in ASC:
history and behavioural patterns in early childhood in
… I was given anti-psychotics for my behaviour … they think making differential diagnosis between mental health con-
because [I don’t] look at them and [I] am nervous of talking ditions and ASC.
then [I] am ‘guarded’ or have ‘flat affect’ … (153-ASC)
Mental health difficulties perceived to be resultant of
One possibly autistic participant did not ‘entirely agree’ ASC.  The second most common reason for participants’
with their Trauma and Stressor Related Disorder diagnosis disagreement with their mental health diagnoses was
because they felt their difficulties with resistance to change because they felt that their mental health conditions were
could be better explained by ASC, and specifically, the resultant of the difficulties they experienced as part of
Au-Yeung et al. 7

living with ASC. These feelings were especially preva- … didn’t understand me. (1-ASC)
lent in participants who received depression and anxiety
diagnoses (nine participants). One autistic participant Or lacked an understanding of ASC:
explained that their depression stemmed from difficulties
… [the] health professionals who diagnosed BPD aren’t
connecting with others:
familiar enough with the symptoms of a female with AS …
Now it feels like I’ve got a BPD diagnosis and they refuse to
… because I did not ever really fit in socially and I couldn’t
look at anything else. (196-ASC)
understand what I was doing wrong, why people made friends
but didn’t include me, why I could never work out when to
join in a conversation in a group situation etc, I was depressed Other participants felt some healthcare professionals
because I thought I must be a very bad [unlovable] person. did not communicate with them. The following partici-
Now I know I definitely have Aspergers I understand why I pants reported not having appropriate discussions regard-
don’t often connect with others properly. (120-ASC) ing their diagnoses, and not being listened to, or given
opportunities to raise concerns:
Another autistic participant reported that their depres-
sion and anxiety were a result of difficulties dealing with I never had a proper assessment or discussion for my diagnosis
social demands: of EUPD2 and nobody told me, it just appeared on my medical
records. (586-PASC)
Depression and anxiety were a reaction to stress associated
with having autistic spectrum difficulties. I am not anxious … I was misdiagnosed but no one would listen to me and I
and depressed when I am in a suitable environment [emphasis was told I was lying when I tried to explain. (2-ASC)
added]. (468-ASC)
… After obtaining medical notes, it is clear that my account of
my experiences were being misunderstood and I was not
Daily living can be exhausting for autistic individuals,
given opportunities to clarify as information was withheld
which could manifest as symptoms of depression. This
from me … (8-ASC)
was experienced by the following participant who disa-
greed with her depression diagnosis: The consequence of a lack of autism awareness and
communication was that it hindered participants receiving
… I live in a society that is not a suitable environment in
which I can thrive, as I am autistic. It is hard to access
adequate support and having their needs met. The follow-
accommodations. People are not kind and ask ignorant and ing autistic participants reported dissatisfaction with medi-
hurtful questions, and make harmful assumptions when I cation being the focus of their treatment:
plead with them to be accommodating as I am autistic. This is
quite taxing, and I often feel very tired. (480-ASC) … I feel my GP is focusing purely on the depression and once
he has a ‘result’ through the anti-depressants, then I will be
An autistic participant felt that due to their autism not deemed fully well again. Although I have taken the anti-
depressants for 8 weeks now, I still struggle with dark
being understood, healthcare professionals tended to focus
thoughts, feelings of wanting to end it all and find life poses
on the co-occurring mental health condition – a more useful the same difficulties to me when it comes to functioning.
treatment would be to address their psycho-social needs: (178-ASC)

… My underlying ASC is hard to explain [to] the GP in that I … my GP tries to keep throwing antidepressants at me but [I]
have expressed that I feel my depression is only a result of think [I] just need to understand myself better [and] get other
my, kind of, ‘untreated Autism’ in effect. Meaning, that if I conditions diagnosed. (468-ASC)
can find help through relevant counselling, connecting with
others, to accept and understand the autistic side of me and Similarly, a possibly autistic participant talked about
discover what my real needs are then I have a chance at comparable experience with regard to therapeutic inter-
learning a new vocabulary to express those needs and this will ventions received, with the focus on the treating the men-
greatly reduce the resultant mental health issues that [recur], tal health condition overshadowing their undiagnosed
particularly depression. (178-ASC)
autism:
This kind of support, however, may be difficult to … I’ve had therapy with MIND3 and [counselling] sessions,
access – the second theme explores why this is the case. although they helped at the time, no one got to the bottom of
what was wrong. (24-PASC)
Clinical barriers.  This theme describes the clinical barriers
participants faced in seeking accurate diagnosis and sup- The following participant’s experience is a fitting illus-
port. These barriers were related to the participants’ inter- tration of this theme and the difficulties participants face in
actions and experiences with healthcare professionals, seeking accurate diagnosis and support. This participant
who they felt either: was misdiagnosed with bipolar and prescribed various
8 Autism 00(0)

medications over many years to manage a condition they rigid thinking and rumination) may also affect how stress-
did not have: ful events are processed; additionally, sensory sensitivities
and sudden changes in routines and environment are a
… I had a formal diagnosis of bipolar (II) condition for around source of stress for autistic individuals over and above
17 years. I actually satisfy the DSM criteria for this, but have what is experienced by non-autistic individuals. Although
only [ever] been hypomanic once and that was antidepressant the effect of these factors on autistic people’s mental health
induced. I don’t benefit from mood stabilisers, antipsychotics
is not yet fully understood, it seems clear that these are
or most antidepressants, and things like stress, working long
days and staying up late don’t induce mania. I have tried lots
significant and widespread issues within the autistic
of different medications over the years, but for most of my population.
[years] with a bipolar diagnosis, I worked in developing Our findings showed that only half of the participants in
countries, didn’t take any [psychotropic medications] and both the autistic (58.2%) and possibly autistic (45.5%)
remained relatively mentally well … Hence, once my autism groups agreed with the mental health diagnosis(es) they
had been diagnosed, I was able to get a consultant psychiatrist received, compared to over 86.5% in the non-autistic
to say that she didn’t think I’d ever had bipolar and it has been group. Qualitative responses from autistic and possibly
revoked as a current diagnosis. (339-ASC) autistic participants indicated that similarities in symptom
presentation between ASC and a range of mental health
Discussion conditions could be the source of these perceived misdiag-
noses. Crane et al. (2018) conducted a survey and inter-
This study explored the experience of mental health diag- views exploring the mental health experience of young
nosis comparing non-autistic, autistic and possibly autistic autistic people (16–25 years). They found that young autis-
people and addressed the following research questions: (1) tic people perceive themselves as different from non-autis-
Are autistic individuals more likely to report receiving tic people in many domains (e.g. that they are more likely
mental health diagnosis(es) and if so, what kind of diagno- to feel under strain or unhappy) even when they are not
ses are they more likely to receive? (2) Are autistic indi- experiencing a mental health problem. This has implica-
viduals more likely to disagree with these diagnoses? (3) tions for the development of diagnostic tools, because it
What were the reasons behind any disagreement? We could be the case that measures developed with the general
found that autistic and possibly autistic adults reported population in mind may therefore fail to tease apart
receiving more mental health diagnoses compared to non- whether an autistic person is experiencing psychological
autistic adults. Specifically, significant differences were distress that is over and above their typical behavioural
found for Depressive Disorders, Anxiety Disorders, presentation. One of the current research priorities of our
Obsessive Compulsive and Related Disorders, Trauma and group is to develop validated diagnostic tools to accurately
Stress Related Disorders, Feeding and Eating Disorders assess mental health conditions in the autistic population
and Personality Disorders. Autistic and possibly autistic (Cassidy et al., 2018a, 2018b).
adults were also significantly less likely to agree with their Another contributing factor to the participants’ per-
mental health diagnoses than non-autistic adults. There ceived misdiagnosis was the lack of awareness and under-
were two major reasons for their disagreement: (1) ASC standing of ASC among healthcare professionals. This
characteristics were confused with symptoms of a mental study highlights the importance of autism awareness train-
health condition and (2) mental health difficulties per- ing for health and social care professionals, as outlined by
ceived to be resultant of ASC. Autistic and possibly autis- statutory guidance (Department of Health, March 2015).
tic individuals spoke about the clinical barriers that hinder Such training would arm professionals with the confidence
accurate diagnosis and support, which include healthcare to communicate with, understand and support autistic peo-
professionals’ lack of awareness and understanding of ple. Mental health professionals should be trained to
autism, poor communication between autistic adults and understand the cognitive differences between autistic and
healthcare professionals and treatment not being suited to neurotypical people, which will help them modify thera-
their needs. peutic interventions so they are effective for autistic peo-
Our findings are consistent with previous research doc- ple. In addition, more research is needed to test the
umenting that autistic people (both diagnosed and undiag- effectiveness of modified therapeutic interventions and
nosed) experience greater rates of co-occurring mental medications for various mental health conditions for the
health conditions compared to the general population autistic population.
(Lever and Geurts, 2016). There may be several reasons as An important topic arose from our qualitative analysis
to why autistic people are more vulnerable to developing relating to diagnostic overshadowing. Our participants
mental health conditions. For example, Haruvi-Lamdan reported that they felt healthcare professionals gave prior-
et al. (2017) suggested that autistic people are more likely ity to treating mental health conditions and thus leaving
to experience stressful or traumatic life events such as the underlying autism unaddressed and other psycho-
being bullied or ostracised; cognitive features (e.g. having social needs unmet. Similarly, in the study by Crane et al.
Au-Yeung et al. 9

(2018), interviews with young autistic people highlighted unable to verify whether the self-reported rates of disa-
numerous barriers for support. These included services not greement consistent with actual rate of misdiagnosis due to
being tailored to their needs in relation to their autism, the lack of previous research in this area. We are also una-
diagnostic and mental health services being ‘disjointed’, ble to conclude whether autistic and possibly autistic indi-
and worse, available support becoming even scarcer when viduals were more likely to disagree with specific mental
they transitioned to adulthood. Given that many autistic health diagnoses that they received because of the small
adults received their diagnosis in adulthood, there is a real sample size for the disagreement data; therefore, these
need to develop adequate post-diagnostic support systems. comparisons are likely to lack power and should be inter-
Post-diagnostic support could include: psycho-social edu- preted with caution (see Supplemental Materials B).
cation; provision of opportunities for autistic people to Finally, the self-selecting nature of our sample meant
connect with others through local support networks; advo- that it was not possible for us to control the variability in
cacy services to support autistic people to express their age and gender. How might these confounding variables
needs; and signposting to local resources. Mainstream ser- have influenced our findings? First, we might expect that
vices such as adult mental health services may also benefit participant groups with higher age to have a higher rate of
from the support and consultation of a specialised ASC lifetime mental health diagnosis, however, this was not the
service in the local area (see National Institute for Health case; our autistic group reported significantly more life-
and Care Excellence (NICE), 2014, for the Bristol Model). time mental health diagnoses despite being statistically
younger than the non-autistic group. Second, we might
expect that the participant group with greater proportion of
Strengths and limitations females to have higher rates of lifetime mental health diag-
A key strength and novel aspect of this study was the par- nosis based on findings previously reported for females, in
ticipatory approach, where autistic adults prioritised the particular, for mood and anxiety disorders (Riecher-
study topic and questions utilised in the research. This Rössler, 2016). This is again not the case in our sample;
ensured that the study was relevant, important and clear to our autistic group reported significantly higher rates of
autistic people and thus more likely to be useful to their mental health diagnosis despite having a smaller propor-
daily lives – a feature lacking in much previous autism tion of females. While these confounding variables do not
research (Fletcher-Watson et al., 2018; Pellicano et al., seem to have affected the findings in the predicted manner,
2014). Furthermore, we included possibly autistic people we acknowledge that the confounding variables still pose a
in the study who had not yet been formerly diagnosed, but limitation to the study and may affect the generalisability
strongly identified as autistic, as suggested by our autistic of our findings.
steering group. This inclusion allowed us to explore
whether yet-to-be-diagnosed autistic adults shared similar Acknowledgements
experiences with those with a clinically confirmed diagno- The authors sincerely thank members of the Coventry Autism
sis of ASC, which was important given many autistic Steering Group, who assisted the researchers in designing and
adults remained undiagnosed or received a diagnosis much advertising the study, and Paula Smith, database manager at the
later in life (Lai and Baron-Cohen, 2015). Interestingly, Autism Research Centre, University of Cambridge for her assis-
the results from the confirmed and possibly autistic groups tance with contacting participants registered in the Cambridge
Autism Research Database. They also thank Dr Jacqui Rodgers,
were very similar. The mean AQ score of the possibly
Professor Clare Wood, David Mason, Professor Helen
autistic group (35.8) was well above the recommended McConachie, Professor Rory O’Connor, Professor David Mosse,
cut-off (32+) for clinically significant level of autistic Dr Carrie Allison and Dr Rebecca Kenny for valuable discus-
traits, and similar rates of mental health diagnoses and sions and also thank everyone for taking part in the study. They
level of disagreement indicated shared experiences appreciate that this is a difficult topic to think and talk about and
between these groups. However, we do acknowledge that appreciate their support in increasing understanding and preven-
there is no way of knowing whether participants in the tion of mental health problems, self-injury and suicide.
possibly autistic group truly met the diagnostic criteria of
ASC without a comprehensive clinical assessment. Declaration of conflicting interests
We found lifetime rate of mental health and other diag- The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with
noses to be 88% for the autistic group and 52.5% for the respect to the research, authorship and/or publication of this
non-autistic group. Previous work by Lever and Geurts article.
(2016) reported similar lifetime rates of psychiatric condi-
tions in their autistic sample (79%) and non-autistic group Funding
(48.8%). This shows that that the self-reported rates of The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support
mental health diagnoses in our study are consistent with for the research, authorship and/or publication of this article:
previously reported rates using standardised instruments This work was supported by the Economic and Social Research
for diagnosing mental health disorders. However, we are Council (grant number ES/N000501/2), Autistica (grant number
10 Autism 00(0)

7247) and a research pump prime from Coventry University port and treatment for mental health difficulties, self-injury
(received by S.C.). S.B-C. was supported by the Autism Research and suicidality. Autism. Epub ahead of print 29 November.
Trust, the MRC, the National Institute for Health Research doi: 10.1177/1362361318816053
(NIHR) Collaboration for Leadership in Applied Health Research Cassidy SA, Bradley L, Bowen E, et al. (2018a) Measurement
and Care East of England at Cambridgeshire and Peterborough properties of tools used to assess depression in adults with
NHS Foundation Trust and the Innovative Medicines Initiative and without autism spectrum conditions: a systematic
Joint Undertaking under grant agreement no. 115300, resources review. Autism Research 11(5): 738–754.
of which are composed of financial contribution from the Cassidy SA, Bradley L, Bowen E, et al. (2018b) Measurement
European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007- properties of tools used to assess suicidality in autistic and
2013) and EFPIA companies’ kind contribution. The views general population adults: a systematic review. Clinical
expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the Psychology Review 62: 56–70.
NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health. The funders had no Cassidy SA, Bradley P, Robinson J, et al. (2014) Suicidal idea-
role in the data collection, analysis or interpretation or any aspect tion and suicide plans or attempts in adults with Asperger’s
pertinent to the study or publication. The corresponding author syndrome attending a specialist diagnostic clinic: a clinical
has full access to the data in the study and had final responsibility cohort study. The Lancet Psychiatry 1(2): 142–147.
for the decision to submit for publication. Cassidy SA, Bradley L, Shaw R, et al. (2018c) Risk markers for
suicidality in autistic adults. Molecular Autism 9: 42.
Notes Cohen J (1988) Statistical Power and Analysis for the Behavioral
Sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
1. We use the term condition, rather than disorder, in recogni-
Associates, Inc.
tion of the strengths and difficulties associated with autism
Crane L, Adams F, Harper G, et al. (2018) ‘Something needs to
and in order to be less stigmatising. Recent research has also
change’: mental health experiences of young autistic adults
found that the autistic community uses a range of terms to
in England. Autism. Epub ahead of print 1 February. DOI:
describe autism, but the term ‘autistic person’ is most pre-
10.1177/1362361318757048.
ferred (Kenny et al., 2016). Thus, we use the terms ASC,
Department of Health (2015) Statutory guidance for local author-
autism and autistic synonymously throughout this article in
ities and NHS organisations to support implementation of
recognition of this.
the adult autism strategy. Available at: https://www.gov.
2. The abbreviated terms for Borderline Personality Disorder
uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/
(BPD) and Emotionally Unstable Personality Disorder
file/422338/autism-guidance.pdf
(EUPD) were used interchangeably in participants’ extract;
Dosseter DR (2007) ‘All that glitter is not gold’: misdiagnosis
these are synonyms for the same condition.
of psychosis in pervasive developmental disorders – a case
3. Mind is a registered mental health charity and company that
series. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry 12(4):
provides advice and support to anyone experiencing a men-
537–548.
tal health problem across England and Wales.
Fletcher-Watson S, Adams J, Brook K, et al. (2018) Making the
future together: shaping autism research through meaning-
Supplemental material ful participation. Autism. Epub ahead of print 10 August.
Supplemental material for this article is available online. DOI: 10.1177/1362361318786721.
Haruvi-Lamdan N, Horesh D and Golan O (2017) PTSD and
ORCID iDs autism spectrum disorder: co-morbidity, gaps in research,
and potential shared mechanisms. Psychological Trauma:
Sheena K Au-Yeung https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0516-4755
Theory, Research, Practice and Policy 10: 290–299.
Ashley E Robertson https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9914-3199
Hedley D, Uljarević M, Foley K-R, et al. (2018) Risk and protective
Sarah Cassidy https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1982-3034
factors underlying depression and suicidal ideation in autism
spectrum disorder. Depression and Anxiety 35: 648–657.
References Hirvikoski T, Mittendorfer-Rutz E, Boman M, et al. (2016)
American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and Premature mortality in autism spectrum disorder. British
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Washington, Journal of Psychiatry 208(3): 232–238.
DC: American Psychiatric Association. Hoekstra RA, Bartels M, Verwejj CJ, et al. (2007) Heritability
Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, et al. (2001) The of autistic traits in the general population. Archives of
Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine 161(4): 372–377.
syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, Jones L, Goddard L, Hill EL, et al. (2014) Experiences of receiv-
scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and ing a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder: a survey
Developmental Conditions 31(1): 5–17. of adults in the United Kingdom. Journal of Autism and
Braun V and Clarke V (2006) Using thematic analysis in psy- Developmental Disorders 44(2): 3033–3044.
chology. Qualitative Research in Psychology 3(2): 77–101. Kenny L, Hattersley C, Molins B, et al. (2016) Which terms
Brugha TS, McManus S, Bankart J, et al. (2011) Epidemiology should be used to describe autism? Perspectives from the
of autism spectrum disorders in adults in the community in UK autism community. Autism 20(4): 442–462.
England. Archives of General Psychiatry 68(5): 459–465. Lai M-C and Baron-Cohen S (2015) Identifying the lost genera-
Camm-Crosbie L, Bradley L, Shaw R, et al. (2018) ‘People like tion of adults with autism spectrum conditions. The Lancet
me don’t get support’: autistic adults’ experiences of sup- Psychiatry 2(11): 1013–1027.
Au-Yeung et al. 11

Lever AG and Geurts HM (2016) Psychiatric co-occurring theory of suicide in a non-clinical young adult sample.
symptoms and disorders in young, middle-aged, and older Autism Research 10(11): 1891–1904.
adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Riecher-Rössler A (2016) Sex and gender differences in mental
Developmental Disorders 46(6): 1916–1930. disorders. The Lancet Psychiatry 4(1): 8–9.
Luciano CC, Keller R, Politi P, et al. (2014) Misdiagnosis of high Rynkiewicz A, Schuller B, Marchi E, et al. (2016) An investi-
function autism spectrum disorders in adults: an Italian case gation of the ‘female camouflage effect’ in autism using a
series. Autism 4: 131. computerized ADOS-2 and test of sex/gender differences.
Matson JL and Williams LW (2013) Differential diagnosis and Molecular Autism 7: 10.
comorbidity: distinguishing autism from other mental health Stewart M, Barnard L, Pearson J, et al. (2006) Presentation of
issues. Neuropsychiatry 3(2): 233–243. depression in autism and Asperger syndrome: a review.
National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (2014) Autism 10: 103–116.
Implementation pack: developing a multi-agency local Van Schalkwyk GI, Peluso F, Qayyum Z, et al. (2015) Varieties
autism team. Putting NICE guidance into practice. Available of misdiagnosis in ASD: an illustrative case series.
at: https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/cg170/resources/the- Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 45(4):
bristol-autism-spectrum-service-bass-pdf-436748082 911–918.
Pellicano E, Dinsmore A and Charman T (2014) What should Woodbury-Smith MR, Robinson J, Wheelwright S, et al. (2005)
autism research focus upon? Community views and priori- Screening adults for Asperger syndrome using the AQ: a
ties from the United Kingdom. Autism 18(7): 756–770. preliminary study of its diagnostic validity in clinical prac-
Pelton MK and Cassidy SA (2017) Are autistic traits associated tice. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 35(3):
with suicidality? A test of the interpersonal-psychological 331–335.

You might also like