You are on page 1of 19

R E V I E W A R T I C L E

Motor Activity in Children


With Autism: A Review of
Current Literature
Rebecca Downey, PT, DPT; Mary Jane K. Rapport, PT, DPT, PhD
Physical Therapy Program, School of Medicine, University of Colorado, Denver, Colorado.
Downloaded from http://journals.lww.com/pedpt by BhDMf5ePHKav1zEoum1tQfN4a+kJLhEZgbsIHo4XMi0hCywCX1AWnYQp/IlQrHD3i3D0OdRyi7TvSFl4Cf3VC1y0abggQZXdtwnfKZBYtws= on 07/07/2021

Physical therapists have expanded their role and visibility in the treatment of children with autism spectrum
disorders (ASD). Limitations in motor activity have not been considered in the assessments of core deficits
of this population; however, physical therapists should be prepared to discuss and address these limitations
in children with ASD. Purpose: The primary purposes of this review were to summarize current evidence for
motor activity limitations in children with ASD and suggest further areas of research in physical therapy and
autism while considering how physical therapy may benefit children with autism. Method: A literature search
was carried out in 2009 and 2010 by using multiple search engines. Results: Forty-nine articles met inclusion
criteria and were included in the review. Conclusion: Findings indicate that limitations in motor activity may
be present in individuals with ASD, and further research is needed to identify specific functional limitations.
(Pediatr Phys Ther 2012;24:2–20) Key words: Asperger syndrome, autism spectrum disorder, child, female,
male, motor activity, pervasive developmental disorder, systematic review

INTRODUCTION by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention4 indi-


Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelop- cates that based on parent report, the incidence of autism
mental disorder that is characterized by limitations in so- is 110 per 10 000, with a higher incidence in males than
cial interactions and communication, restricted interest, in females.5
and stereotyped or repetitive behaviors.1 The term autism The approach to evaluation and treatment of children
spectrum disorder is often used to describe individuals who with ASD is frequently a multidisciplinary team approach.6
have been diagnosed with autism disorder (AD), pervasive Although the term clumsiness has been used in describing
development disorder not otherwise specified (PDD-NOS), individuals with AS, limitations in motor activity are not
or Asperger syndrome (AS) on the basis of medical and de- considered to be core deficits of individuals with ASD.7,8
velopmental history and clinical observations of behavior Historically, physical therapists have not been involved in
(see Table 1 for definitions).2,3 Recent research supported the evaluation and treatment process of individuals with
ASD; the role of the physical therapist is evolving with
regard to both the evaluation and intervention processes.
There is an increasing body of literature related to ASD,
0898-5669/110/2401-0002
Pediatric Physical Therapy but evidence regarding physical therapy (PT) and inter-
Copyright C 2012 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & vention with this population continues to be limited in
Wilkins and Section on Pediatrics of the American Physical Therapy scope. To best understand PT intervention in children
Association
with autism, physical therapists need to assess the effect
Correspondence: Mary Jane K. Rapport, PT, DPT, PhD, Physical Ther-
of motor activity limitations including motor anomalies,
apy Program, School of Medicine, University of Colorado, 13121 E 17th delays, or weaknesses on a child’s ability to fully par-
Avenue, C244, Aurora, CO 80045 (maryjane.rapport@ucdenver.edu). ticipate in daily activities and routines. These deficits in
Grant Support: This study was funded by Leadership Education in Neu- motor activity inform physical therapists’ clinical decision
rodevelopmental and Related Disorders Training (LEND), Health Re- making.
sources and Service Administration (HRSA), and Maternal Child Health The purposes of this review were to summarize cur-
Bureau (MCHB), Award T73 MC11044.
rent evidence for limitations in motor activity in children
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
with ASD and suggest further areas of research related
DOI: 10.1097/PEP.0b013e31823db95f
to PT and autism while considering how PT may benefit

2 Downey and Rapport Pediatric Physical Therapy


Copyright © 2012 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins and the Section on Pediatrics of the American Physical Therapy
Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
TABLE 1
Definitions and Abbreviations of Developmental Disorders Identified in the Literature and Associated With Autism

Classification Definition

Autism spectrum disorder ASD is a neurodevelopmental disorder that is characterized by limitations in social interactions and
(ASD)1 communication, restricted interest, and stereotyped or repetitive behaviors. There is a continuum of
behaviors represented within the ASD diagnosis
Pervasive developmental The PDD diagnosis includes impaired social interaction and communication skills or the presence of
disorder (PDD)a stereotyped behaviors or restricted interests that are not congruent with developmental or cognitive ages.
PDD encompasses several disorders including autistic disorder, Rett’s disorder, childhood disintegrative
disorder, Asperger’s disorder, and PDD not otherwise specified.
Autism disordera The diagnosis of autistic disorder is based on impaired social interaction and communication and the presence
of repetitive or stereotyped behavior. There must also have been a delay in social interaction, social or
communicative language, and play prior to the age of 3 years.
Asperger syndromea,b The diagnosis of Asperger syndrome is based on impaired social interaction and restricted or stereotyped
interests that interfere with daily functioning. There is no delay in language, cognitive development, or
adaptive behaviors and activities of daily living skills.
Pervasive developmental The diagnosis of PDD-NOS is used when there is impairment in social interaction that is associated with
disorder—not otherwise communication skills or is present with stereotyped behavior and restricted interest. These symptoms should
specified (PDD-NOS)a,b not be accounted for by PDD, schizophrenia, schizotypical personality disorder, or avoidant personality
disorder. PDD-NOS includes “atypical autism” (when the criteria have not been met for autism disorder).

a Adapted from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders–Fourth Edition (DSM-IV).1
b Basedon proposed revisions of the DSM, these currently used diagnoses may be incorporated into one diagnosis (ASD) when the fifth edition is
published.3

children with autism. The term motor activity was selected January 1, 2009, and October 31, 2009. Seventeen search
by the authors in an attempt to capture and describe mo- terms were used in an attempt to best capture the broad
tor abnormalities, delays, and general motor function of range of articles addressing children with autism or re-
children with ASD. lated diagnoses, motor activity, and PT (see Figure for
search terms). Care was given to be certain that each search
yielded all possible evidence in the published literature.
METHODS
The search was then updated during May 2010 using the
A literature search was carried out using OVID, same strategies with each of the 17 search terms. In all,
PubMed, and Google Scholar search engines between 90 searches were completed (Figure). Further review of

Fig. Search strategy.

Pediatric Physical Therapy Motor Activity in Autism 3


Copyright © 2012 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins and the Section on Pediatrics of the American Physical Therapy
Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
an article was dependent on the appearance of the search 1 group was classified as low-risk for development of
terms in the abstract of the article as identified by the first autism and the other as high-risk for development of
author (Figure). Articles were then graded according to autism. Children were examined with the Mullen Scales
the level of evidence, on the basis of criteria from Sackett of Early Learning at 6, 14, and 24 months. At the 24-
et al.9,10 Only articles that were found to be at Sackett levels month visit, further testing was administered to classify
1-3B were used for this analysis. Other exclusion criteria children who had typical development, ASD, or language
included systematic reviews that did not include specific delay. No differences on the Mullen Scales of Early Learn-
research methods, articles in which the search terms were ing were found in children with ASD and typical children
present in the abstract but did not include analysis of motor at 6 months; however, by 14 months, the children with
function, and articles that were not accessible in English ASD began to demonstrate a slowing in development com-
(see Figure). pared with the other groups. By 24 months, significant
differences were found between the group with ASD and
the group developing typically in all domains, as well as
RESULTS between the group with ASD and the group with language
The search strategy and inclusion criteria resulted in delay. The children with ASD demonstrated the slowest
a total of 49 articles that were acceptable for this literature rate of increase in developmental skills over time.
review of motor activity in children with ASD. No level In another study, Esposito et al13 retrospectively ex-
1 articles appropriate for analysis were identified during amined videotapes of 3 groups of infants aged 12 to 21
the search. Two level 2 articles and 47 level 3 articles were weeks: those diagnosed with ASD, those with develop-
identified and included in the analysis. Articles classified as mental delays not associated with ASD, and children with
levels 4 and 5 were identified but not used in the literature typical development. These researchers found that the
review. Articles that originally met the inclusion criteria group with ASD had significantly less static and dynamic
but did not address motor activities as part of study criteria symmetry in the supine position than the other groups.
or outcomes also were not included. Review articles that Teitelbaum et al14 also suggested that motor abnormalities,
did not include specific methods and inclusion criteria also including asymmetrical movement patterns, are present at
were excluded from the analysis (see Figure). As is the birth in children with ASD and may aid in the early identi-
case with all searches of the literature, additional articles fication of ASD. On the basis of video analysis, Baraneck15
that would meet the search criteria may be found if a new suggested that symptoms of autism, including sensory-
search following the same strategy was conducted today. motor symptoms, might be present and identifiable be-
The search strategy used here yielded all articles meeting tween 9 and 12 months of age. In another study, Dewrang
inclusion criteria as of May 2010. and Sandberg16 used retrospective parent reports to com-
Studies that inform health care providers about chil- pare individuals with AS with a group of young adults who
dren with ASD and addressed motor activity were ana- were developing typically. They found that during the first
lyzed and reviewed (see Appendix). During the review 2 years of life, individuals with AS demonstrated impaired
process, various themes related to the characteristics and imitation, increased clumsiness, and poor coordination.
concerns related to ASD arose in the literature. The articles These findings suggest that evaluation of motor activity
were grouped and analyzed on the basis of these themes. may play a role in early prediction of ASD.
The themes were not derived through a specific qualita- In contrast, Ozonoff et al17 reported that infants who
tive methodological approach; rather, they were generated are later diagnosed with ASD do not demonstrate an in-
through the work of the first author, and corroborated with creased number of movement abnormalities or a lack of
the second author, during the process of sorting articles protective reactions when compared with a group of chil-
that met the search criteria. Based on recurring similarities dren who are developing typically. They did, however, find
in the content of the available literature as reviewed, the a slower rate of development in reaching mature motor pat-
following thematic categories emerged: early motor find- terns. In addition, they suggested that more comprehen-
ings, gestures and motor imitation, postural control, and sive motor evaluations might be useful in early detection of
dyspraxia. autism. In another study by Loh et al,18 stereotyped behav-
iors and postures found in children with ASD were similar
to those in the comparison group. These authors also sug-
Early Motor Findings gested that more sensitive testing might be required to
Several recent studies sought evidence of motor im- identify motor impairments.
pairments to provide early motor identification markers
and characteristics of ASD. For example, in a study by
Provost et al,11 68% of children with ASD who were tested Gestures and Motor Imitation
on the Bayley Scales of Infant Development II Motor Scale The ability to use gestures and motor imitation relies
and 63% of children who were tested on the Peabody De- on motor activity to communicate with others. Interest-
velopmental Motor Scales, Second Edition, would have ingly, children with ASD have difficulty with communica-
qualified for early intervention services. Landa and Garrett- tion as well as difficulty using motor activity (eg, gestures
Mayer12 also prospectively studied 2 groups of infants: and imitation) as forms of communication to support social

4 Downey and Rapport Pediatric Physical Therapy


Copyright © 2012 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins and the Section on Pediatrics of the American Physical Therapy
Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
interaction. Motor imitation has been identified as a signif- scores. Although this may be related to praxis, these au-
icant impairment in previous literature on individuals with thors suggested that it might also be related to altered
ASD, particularly in relation to social communication.19-21 neural substrates or language deficits.
In a literature review by Williams et al,22 the authors sug- More recently, Ben-Sasson et al30 examined gestural
gested that imitation impairments are present in children representations in 3 groups of children: children with HFA,
with ASD and are more apparent in younger age groups children with language impairment, and children who are
(below the age of 4 years) when compared with other developing typically. The authors concluded that children
children. with HFA have increased difficulty with gestural repre-
Stone et al21 suggested that imitation impairments in sentations, which they suggest may be related to a motor
children with ASD are due to a delay in acquiring imita- planning deficit. There appeared to be decreased quality
tion skills, rather than disordered sequencing. Although of gesture performance as well as a discrepancy between
some improvement may be seen between the ages of 2 and gestures and verbal descriptions, when compared with the
3 years, this delay was apparent in young children, as other 2 groups. The authors hypothesized that perform-
well as in preschool-aged children.22,23 In another study, ing motor actions might be more difficult when the task
Rogers et al20 found that children with ASD have impaired is combined with verbal description. This may be related
imitation skills on sequential imitation tasks when com- to a lack of integration between motor and language. Ben-
pared with a group of children with developmental delays. Sasson et al30 suggest that individuals with ASD may ben-
No support was found for a relationship between imitation efit from further testing to examine motor planning and
impairment and play skills, language skills, or dyspraxia in sequencing.
children with ASD. In a more recent study, Rogers et al24
noted that during a simple task, children with autism fail
more imitation tasks than a group of children with devel- Postural Control
opmental delays and a group of children who were devel- Postural control requires a level of stability neces-
oping typically. These findings were especially meaningful sary prior to executing additional motor skills or activities.
in children with autism who were younger than 14 months. Without this control, motor activity may be limited to more
In the older age group (older than 30 months), children static positions. Individuals with autism tend to have de-
with regressive type autism continued to fail more tasks, creased postural control.31,32 Minshew et al31 found that
especially nonfunctional imitation tasks. individuals with autism have decreased postural stability,
Other researchers have suggested a possible link be- particularly in circumstances where there is sensory con-
tween imitation impairments and the presence of motor ac- flict. Compared to a group of children who were develop-
tivity abnormalities. For example, Van Vuchelen et al25 re- ing typically and adults, development of postural stability
ported the presence of an imitation impairment, especially appeared to be delayed in children with autism. Postural
in nonmeaningful gestures, in children with ASD who stability did not appear to improve in individuals with
have low-functioning autism and high-functioning autism autism until the age of 12 years. At the age of 15 years,
(HFA). They also found increased impairment on motor the group that was developing typically appeared to have
testing. Green et al26 also found an association between a plateau in postural stability; however, this same level of
motor and imitation scores, with lower and more variable control was not achieved in the group with ASD. Based
scores in children with AS than in children with specific on the data from a bimanual lift task by Schmitz et al,32
developmental delays of motor function. Spatiotemporal children with ASD rely on reactive postural control rather
errors were more common in the group with AS. In a than on the typical anticipatory postural control seen in
study by Mostofsky et al,27 children with ASD had more the comparison group when performing lifting tasks.
imitation errors on the Florida Apraxia Screening Test than In another study, Kohen-Raz et al33 examined postu-
children who were developing typically. Spatial errors were ral stability in various standing positions between a group
the most common in both groups. In the groups with ASD, of children with ASD and a group of children who were
errors were increased when gesturing on command and developing typically. Children with ASD demonstrated in-
imitation, rather than tool use. Notably, no significant dif- creased sway, abnormal weight distribution, and the ab-
ference was found in the number of total errors between sence of typical ankle strategies in standing. A “paradoxi-
individuals diagnosed with HFA and those with AS. cal stress response” was noted in individuals with autism,
In another study, Smith and Bryson28 found that chil- indicated by an increase in postural stability in stressful
dren with ASD have increased difficulty performing and conditions (defined as removal of vision). In a follow-up
naming both meaningful and nonmeaningful gestures but study, Molloy et al34 also found that children with ASD
no difficulty understanding or identifying gestures. The had significantly more sway in standing than a compari-
authors suggested that these impairments might be related son group of children who are developing typically. Instead
to dyspraxia.28 Dewey et al29 also found that when com- of a “paradoxical stress response,” they found that chil-
pared with children with both developmental coordination dren with ASD experienced a larger increase in sway when
disorder (DCD) and attention-deficit/hyperactive disorder visual input was removed and somatosensory input modi-
(ADHD), DCD only, or ADHD only, children with ASD fied, indicating that children with ASD rely on visual input
have significantly lower motor and gestural performance for balance. This finding points to impaired processing

Pediatric Physical Therapy Motor Activity in Autism 5


Copyright © 2012 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins and the Section on Pediatrics of the American Physical Therapy
Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
abilities with sensory conflict in individuals with ASD. score was correlated with the Autism Diagnostic Obser-
Molloy et al34 argued that the presence of a paradoxical vation Schedule score, also suggesting that dyspraxia may
response found in the Kohen-Raz et al33 study might have be a core symptom of ASD. In addition, Dowell et al40
been the result of additional visual and auditory input used reported that children with ASD have slower timed move-
in their methods. Further support for impaired postural ments and score significantly lower on postural knowledge
control in children with ASD was provided by Fournier testing.
et al,35 who reported that children with ASD have increased Glazebrook et al41 noted that individuals with ASD
postural sway in quiet stance without manipulation of sen- are able to use advance information; however, more time
sory input as well as altered center of pressure shifts during is required to plan movements. Glazebrook et al42 and Naz-
gait initiation. zarali et al43 noted that individuals with ASD required more
time to plan and execute goal-directed movements. Rine-
hart et al44 also noted a slower preparation time in children
Dyspraxia with HFA and AS when compared with a cohort developing
Quality of movement may be altered in children with typically. This was further supported in another study by
ASD, and dyspraxia has been noted when comparing chil- Rinehart et al,45 where children with HFA demonstrated
dren with ASD with children who are developing typically. increased preparation time compared with a cohort de-
Motor delay and motor variability have also been noted and veloping typically, and children with AS demonstrated a
described in some studies and refuted in others. Although trend toward a motor preparation deficit. On the basis of
motor abnormalities were noted in individuals who were an analysis of goal-directed gait, Vernazza-Martin et al46
first described with AS,8 these limitations have not been suggested that when compared with a typical comparison
consistently identified in individuals with ASD. While no group, children with ASD have impaired motor planning
level 1 research supporting the presence of limitations in and execution. Whereas differences in motor planning are
motor activities was found, several lower-level studies indi- present in some individuals with ASD, the alterations in
cate that impairments in motor activity may be common in patterns are unclear. Hughes47 suggested that individuals
children with ASD. Researchers have sought to distinguish with autism demonstrate difficulty executing simple goal-
differences across individuals with ASD, as well as to com- directed motor tasks that might be related to sequencing,
pare those with ASD with individuals who are developing vision, or consequence prediction.
typically or have other developmental concerns. Staples and Reid48 compared a group of children di-
Manjiviona and Prior36 found limits in motor activ- agnosed with ASD to 3 groups developing typically. The
ity and function in children with HFA and AS. Ghaziuddin 3 typical groups were matched with children in the group
and Butler37 noted that children with autism, AS, and PDD- with ASD by chronological age, cognitive development,
NOS have motor impairments. Statistically significant dif- or movement skill development. They found that children
ferences were found only between those with AS and those with ASD have significantly poorer motor scores than chil-
with AD (children with AD were noted to be more clumsy) dren who are developing typically and who are chronolog-
as measured by the Bruinink-Oseretsky Test of Motor Pro- ically age matched and cognitively age matched. Specifi-
ficiency. Individuals diagnosed with AD had higher levels cally, children with ASD had difficulty with bilateral coor-
of motor activity impairment, while those with AS demon- dination and performed at a similar motor level as children
strated less impairment on gross motor, fine motor, and approximately of half their chronological age. The authors
total battery scores. A strong correlation existed between suggested that by late childhood, motor skills in children
intelligence quotient (IQ) scores and test results. When with ASD are significantly delayed.
adjusted for level of intelligence, no significant difference Jansiewicz et al49 noted that boys with HFA and AS
remained between groups. Green et al38 found a similar have increased difficulty with balance, gait, and dysrhyth-
correlation between IQ and motor scores in individuals mia with timed hand and foot movements. In another
with ASD, indicating that motor impairments might be re- study, Weimer et al50 examined tests of apraxia and ba-
lated to IQ level. Current research has demonstrated that sic motor function in a group of children and young adults
there is no significant difference in level of motor impair- with AS and a comparison group. The authors found that
ments among children with AS, AD, or PDD-NOS; how- deficits were present on tests of apraxia, especially on mea-
ever, there is a trend toward higher cognitive limitations sures where visual input was removed, suggesting reliance
correlated to lower motor scores. on vision with a proprioceptive impairment. In addition,
Several researchers have suggested that when com- Freitag et al51 reported that when compared with a group
pared with a comparison group developing typically, chil- developing typically, individuals with HFA and AS are
dren with ASD have dyspraxia. Mostofsky et al27 suggested “strongly impaired” in dynamic balance and diadochokine-
that based on the Florida Apraxia Screening test, motor im- sis, and integration between sensory and motor input may
itation may be linked to dyspraxia, particularly to a delay be also impaired. They also noted a positive association of
in spatial mapping. Dzuik et al39 proposed that dyspraxia motor scores to the level of social withdrawal.
may be separate from other motor skills in children with In a study by Fuentes et al,52 the authors found that
ASD and may be strongly correlated to the core deficits as- when compared with a group of children who were devel-
sociated with autism.1 According to Dowell et al,40 praxis oping typically, children with ASD had significantly poorer

6 Downey and Rapport Pediatric Physical Therapy


Copyright © 2012 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins and the Section on Pediatrics of the American Physical Therapy
Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
motor and writing scores overall. More specifically, scores lectual delay. Balance in individuals with ASD may be
were decreased on gait, stance, and timed movement ac- decreased; however, continued research is necessary to
tivities. Poor gross motor skills were correlated with poor identify the severity and the pattern of deficit.
handwriting scores. They hypothesized that if therapies Although impaired motor skills and function are not
address overall motor control, handwriting scores would a core deficit of ASD, they are considered a core deficit
improve as a result of increased ability to control and ma- of DCD and ADHD.1 In a study by Dewey et al,29 the au-
nipulate arm movements. In contrast, van Swieten et al53 thors found that when tested on the Bruinink-Oseretsky
sought to differentiate between motor and executive plan- Test of Motor Proficiency (short form) and a gestural per-
ning abilities in 3 groups of children and adults: a group formance test, children with ASD had significantly lower
with ASD, a group with DCD, and a group with typical scores than children with DCD, ADHD, or ADHD and
development. Based on what the authors suggested to be DCD. This supports the presence of difficulty with motor
a pure motor planning test, no difference was found be- activity in children with ASD. This finding was further sup-
tween the group with typical development and the group ported by Pan et al,59 who found that children with ASD
with ASD. performed significantly lower on motor tests than children
Miyahara et al54 found high rates of motor delay in with ADHD and children who are developing typically.
2 groups of children: a group of children with AS and a Specific limitations were noted on tests of locomotion and
group of children with a learning disability. A significant object manipulation. They suggested that poor motor per-
difference was found between groups for manual dexterity formance might be a sign of autism, with poor skills being
scores. The children with AS had a trend toward poorer ball related to a lack of social skills as well as lack of motivation
skills, which, the authors hypothesized, might be related to practice. The authors encouraged clinicians to screen for
to the type of preferred play. motor impairments as poor motor skills were found to be
No difference in motor profile was found between correlated with poor self-esteem, increased anxiety, and
children with ASD who were chronologically age matched decreased social function.
and children with other developmental delays.55 The au- In a rare study with an all-female sample, Kopp et al60
thors did, however, note that scores of children with ASD compared several groups of girls: girls who were develop-
were more variable than those with developmental delays. ing typically, girls with ASD, and girls with ADHD. They
In another study by Provost et al,11 the authors evaluated found that a large percentage of girls with ASD also fit the
3 groups of children—children with ASD, developmen- diagnosis for DCD, especially those in the preschool-aged
tal delays, and developmental concerns—on the basis of group. Predictors of poor motor scores included younger
results from the Bayley Scales of Infant Development. In age, presence and severity of ASD symptoms, and low IQ.
this study, none of the children in the group with ASD Poor motor scores were related to poor activities of daily
tested within normal limits, and at least 68% of these chil- living and physical education participation.
dren would have qualified for early intervention services
based on a delay of 25% or more. Motor scores of children
with ASD did not differ when compared with children DISCUSSION
with developmental delay. These results suggest that there Based on this review of literature, evidence is emerg-
is a limitation in motor function in children with ASD. ing that supports the identification of impaired motor ac-
Further research is needed to identify the specific type of tivity in children who have the diagnosis of autism. Al-
dysfunction. though impaired motor activity is not included in the di-
Matson et al56 also noted gross and fine motor impair- agnosis, impaired motor activity appears to be an observ-
ments in toddlers with AD when compared with toddlers able trend. The ability to understand and address the entire
with atypical development between 18 and 36 months of clinical picture of the child, including all areas of function,
age. No significant differences were found between tod- becomes an essential component of any intervention plan.
dlers with PDD-NOS and children who are developing The majority of current evidence does support the pres-
typically. The authors suggested that motor impairments ence of motor activity abnormalities prior to 2 years of age
are present at a young age and benefit may be obtained in children who are later diagnosed with ASD that per-
from early intervention. In another study, children with sists into early childhood. As children are being diagnosed
ASD were compared with children with specific speech earlier with ASD and receiving early intervention services,
and language disorders and a comparison group of chil- physical therapists should consistently be part of the team
dren who were developing typically. The children with addressing all the needs of the child.
speech disorders had lower scores on all gross and fine mo- Although we still have much to learn about the timing
tor domains except coordination, whereas children with of motor development and the patterns of motor activity in
autism had significantly poorer scores on all fine and gross children with ASD, evidence supports the presence of spe-
motor scores (including balance), except oral motor and cific difficulties related to motor activity in individuals with
coordination.57 Morin and Reid58 noted that although in- ASD. The link between imitation and motor activity is still
dividuals with autism have poor motor performance, they unclear; however, research indicates that there are limita-
can obtain higher balance scores than those with intel- tions in motor imitation in children with ASD. Questions

Pediatric Physical Therapy Motor Activity in Autism 7


Copyright © 2012 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins and the Section on Pediatrics of the American Physical Therapy
Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
still remain as to whether restrictions in social behavior participation restrictions rather than solely by impairment
limit imitation, or whether limitations in motor activity and disability. Physical therapists can and should play a
restrict social participation and adversely affect imitation. unique role in promoting functionally based intervention
The presence of postural instability is also supported in strategies to enhance motor activity and improve function
the literature. Decreased postural stability can significantly in children with ASD.
limit participation in activities since the simplest of move-
ments require complex control61,62 and further research is
needed to examine the severity, cause, and functional out- ACKNOWLEDGMENT
comes related to postural control. The literature also lends
support to difficulty with motor planning in children with The authors thank Stephanie Lyle, PT, DPT, for her
ASD. early contributions to this work.
Limitations in motor activity in children with ASD
might decrease the opportunity for social interactions and REFERENCES
learning opportunities. Although limited research related
to interventions for motor activity impairments in children 1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual
of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American
with ASD was found, Travers et al63 did address motor- Psychiatric Association; 1994.
linked implicit learning in children with ASD. In a study 2. Rapin I, Tuchman R. Autism: definition, neurobiology, screening,
comparing a group of individuals with HFA, ASD, and a diagnosis. Pediatr Clin N Am. 2008;55:1129-1146.
group developing typically, Travers et al63 reported that 3. DSM-5: The future of psychiatric diagnosis. American Psy-
for simple tasks, motor-linked implicit learning might be chiatric Association DSM-5 Development. http://www.dsm5.org/
Pages/Default.aspx. Published 2010. Accessed December 2010.
intact in children with ASD, which might influence thera- 4. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Autism and de-
peutic approach. velopment disabilities monitoring (ADDM) network. http://www
Motor activity delays have been observed in infants .cdc.gov/ncbddd/autism/addm.html. Published March 2011. Ac-
and toddlers with autism and may affect future motor de- cessed June 2011.
velopment. As with all developmental delays, early iden- 5. Kogan MD, Blumberg SJ, Schieve LA, et al. Prevalence of parent-
reported diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder among children in
tification leading to the initiation of early service delivery the US, 2007. Pediatrics. 2009;124:1395-1403.
might have a positive benefit on motor skills and long-term 6. Rogers E. Functional behavioral assessment and children with
disability in individuals diagnosed with ASD.64,65 Since autism: working as a team. Focus Autism Dev Disord. 2001;16:228-
children with ASD are ambulatory, they previously would 231.
not have been thought to benefit from PT. Physical ther- 7. Wing L. Asperger syndrome: a clinical account. Psychol Med.
1981;11:115-129.
apists need to consider how to address these impairments 8. Tantam D. Annotation Asperger syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiat.
in motor activity within the child’s daily routines. 1988;29:245-255.
Although the results of this literature review do in- 9. Sackett DL, Strauss SE, Richardson WS, et al. Evidence-Based
dicate that motor activity impairments may be present in Medicine: How to Practice and Teach EBM. Philadelphia, PA:
children with ASD, there are limitations to this analysis. Churchill-Livingstone; 2000.
10. Glaros S. All evidence is not created equal: a discussion of levels of
An updated literature search may provide increased evi- evidence. PT: Magazine of Phys Ther. 2003;11:42-52.
dence supporting motor activity impairments as well as 11. Provost B, Lopez BR, Heimerl S. A comparison of motor delays in
provide documentation for intervention strategies for chil- young children: autism spectrum disorder, developmental delay, and
dren with ASD. No consistent measure was used in each developmental concerns. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:321-328.
of the reviewed studies, which limits generalization of the 12. Landa R, Garrett-Mayer E. Development in infants with autism
spectrum disorders: a prospective study. J Child Psychol Psychiat.
findings. 2006;47:629-638.
13. Esposito G, Venuti P, Maestro S, Muratori F. An exploration of sym-
metry in early autism spectrum disorders: analysis of lying. Brain
CONCLUSION Dev-JPN. 2009;31:131-138.
14. Teitelbaum P, Teitelbaum O, Nye J, Fryman J, Maurer RG. Movement
Although there may be limitations in motor activ-
analysis in infancy may be useful for early diagnosis of autism. Proc
ity present in children with ASD, much research is still Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998;95:13982-13987.
needed to identify the age at which these limitations in 15. Baraneck GT. Autism during infancy: a retrospective video analysis
motor activity are present and to what extent they differ of sensory-motor and social behaviors at 9-12 months of age. J Autism
from children who are developing typically. As with many Dev Disord. 1999;29:213-224.
16. Dewrang P, Sandberg AD. Parental retrospective assessment of devel-
other diagnoses, lack of evidence has not prohibited the PT
opment and behavior in Asperger syndrome during the first 2 years
profession from forging ahead with new research and on- of life. Res Autism Spectrum Disord. 2010;4:461-473.
going intervention. Future research is necessary to identify 17. Ozonoff S, Young GS, Goldring S, et al. Gross motor development,
to what extent functional activities are limited in children movement abnormalities, and early identification of autism. J Autism
with ASD. Research is also necessary to determine the un- Dev Disord. 2008;38:644-656.
18. Loh A, Soman T, Brian J, et al. Stereotyped motor behaviors associated
derlying causes as well as the most appropriate interven-
with autism in high-risk infants: a pilot videotape analysis of a sibling
tions. As physical therapists move toward consistent use sample. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:25-36.
of the ICF,66 it may be useful to implement interventions 19. Ingersoll B. The social role of imitation in autism implications for the
and research their efficacy based on activity limitation and treatment of imitation deficits. Infant Young Child. 1998;21:107-119.

8 Downey and Rapport Pediatric Physical Therapy


Copyright © 2012 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins and the Section on Pediatrics of the American Physical Therapy
Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
20. Rogers SJ, Hepburn SL, Stackhouse T, Wehner E. Imitation perfor- 42. Glazebrook CM, Elliott D, Lyons J. A kinematic analysis of how
mance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental young adults with and without autism plan and control goal-directed
disorders. J Child Psychol Psychiat. 2003;44:763-781. movements. Motor Control. 2006;10:244-264.
21. Stone WL, Ousley OY, Littleford CD. Motor imitation in young 43. Nazzarali N, Glazebrook CM, Elliott D. Movement planning and
children with autism: what’s the object? J Abnorm Child Psych. reprogramming in individuals with autism. J Autism Dev Disord.
1997;25:475-485. 2009;39:1401-1411.
22. Williams JHG, Whiten A, Singh T. A systematic review of action imi- 44. Rinehart NJ, Bradshaw JL, Bereton AV, Tonge BJ. Movement prepa-
tation in autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2004;34:285- ration in high-functioning autism and Asperger disorder: a serial
299. choice reaction time task involving motor reprogramming. J Autism
23. Stone WL, Lemanek KL, Fishel PT, Fernandez MC, Altemeier WA. Dev Disord. 2001;31:79-88.
Play and imitation skills in the diagnosis of autism in young children. 45. Rinehart NJ, Bellgrove MA, Tonge BJ, Brereton AV, Howells-Rankin
Pediatrics. 1990;86:267-272. D, Bradshaw JL. An examination of movement kinematics in young
24. Rogers SK, Young GS, Cook I, Giolzetti A, Ozonoff S. Imitation ac- people with high-functioning autism and Asperger’s disorder: fur-
tions on objects in early-onset and regressive autism: effects and im- ther evidence for a motor planning deficit. J Autism Dev Disord.
plications of task characteristics on performance. Dev Psychopathol. 2006;36:757-767.
2010;22:71-85. 46. Vernazza-Martin S, Martin N, Vernazza A, et al. Goal directed loco-
25. Van Vuchelen M, Roeyers H, Weerdt WD. Nature of motor imitation motion and balance control in autistic children. J Autism Dev Disord.
problems in school-aged males with autism: how congruent are the 2005;35:91-102.
error types? Dev Med Child Neurol. 2007;49:6-12. 47. Hughes C. Brief report: planning problems in autism at the level of
26. Green D, Baird G, Barnett AL, Henderson L, Huber J, Henderson SE. motor control. J Autism Dev Disord. 1996;26:99-107.
The severity and nature of motor impairment in Asperger syndrome: a 48. Staples KL, Reid G. Fundamental movement skills and autism spec-
comparison with specific developmental disorder of motor function. trum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2010;40:209-217.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2002;43:655-668. 49. Jansiewicz EM, Goldberg MC, Newschaffer CJ, Denckla MB, Landa
27. Mostofsky SH, Dubey P, Jerath VK. Developmental dyspraxia is not R, Mostofsky SH. Motor signs distinguish children with high func-
limited to imitation in children with autism spectrum disorders. J Int tioning autism and Asperger syndrome from controls. J Autism Dev
Neuropsych Soc. 2006;12:314-326. Disord. 2006;36:613-621.
28. Smith IM, Bryson SE. Gesture imitation in autism: II. Symbolic ges- 50. Weimer AK, Schatz AM, Lincoln A, Ballantyne AO, Trauner DA.
tures and pantomimed object use. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2007;24:679- “Motor” impairment in Asperger syndrome: evidence for a deficit in
700. proprioception. Dev Behav Ped. 2001;22:92-101.
29. Dewey D, Cantell M, Crawford SG. Motor and gestural performance 51. Freitag CM, Kleser C, Schnieder M, von Gontard A. Quantitative as-
in children with autism spectrum disorders, developmental coordi- sessment of neuromotor function in adolescents with high function-
nation disorder, and/or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Int ing autism and Asperger syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:948-
Neuropsych Soc. 2007;13:246-256. 959.
30. Ben-Sasson A, Stimmell KE, Cermak SA. Sequence of gestural repre- 52. Fuentes CT, Mostofsky SH, Bastian AJ. Children with autism
sentations in children with high functioning autism. Israeli J Occup show specific handwriting impairments. Neurology. 2009;73:
Ther. 2009;18:E57-E73. 1532-1537.
31. Minshew NJ, Sung MB, Jones BL, Furman JM. Underdevelopment 53. van Swieten LM, van Bergen E, Williams JHG, et al. A test of motor
of the postural control system in autism. Neurology. 2004;63:2056- (not executive) planning in developmental coordination disorder and
2061. autism. J Exp Psychol Human. 2001;36:493-499.
32. Schmitz C, Martineau J, Barthélémy C, Assaiante C. Motor control 54. Miyahara M, Tsujii M, Hori M, Nakanishi K, Kageyama H, Sugiyama
and children with autism: a deficit of anticipatory function? Neurosci T. Brief report: motor incoordination in children with Asperger syn-
Lett. 2003;348:17-20. drome and learning disabilities. J Autism Dev Disord. 1997;27:595-
33. Kohen-Raz R, Volkmar FR, Cohen DJ. Postural control in children 603.
with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 1992;22:419-432. 55. Provost B, Heimerl S, Lopez BR. Levels of gross and fine motor de-
34. Molloy CA, Dietrich KN, Bhattacharya A. Postural stability in velopment in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Phys
children with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. Occup Ther Pediatr. 2007;27(3):21-36.
2003;33:643-652. 56. Matson JL, Mahan S, Fodstad JC, Hess JA, Neal D. Motor skill
35. Fournier KA, Kimberg CI, Radonovich KL, et al. Decreased static and abilities in toddlers with autistic disorder, pervasive developmen-
dynamic postural control in children with autism spectrum disorders. tal disorder—not otherwise specified, and atypical development. Res
Gait Posture. 2010;32: 6-9. Autism Spectrum Dis. 2010;4:444-449.
36. Manjiviona J, Prior M. Comparison of Asperger syndrome and high- 57. Noterdaeme M, Mildenberger K, Minow F, Amorosa H. Evaluation
functioning autistic children on a test of motor impairment. J Autism of neuromotor deficits in children with autism and children with a
Dev Disord. 1995;25:23-39. specific speech and language disorder. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry.
37. Ghaziuddin M, Butler E. Clumsiness in autism and Asperger syn- 2002;11:219-225.
drome: a further report. J Intell Disabil Res. 1998;42:43-48. 58. Morin B, Reid G. A quantitative and qualitative assessment of autis-
38. Green D, Charman T, Pickles A. Impairment in movement skills tic individuals on selected motor tasks. Adapt Phys Act Q. 1985;2:
of children with autistic spectrum disorders. Dev Med Child Neurol. 43-55.
2009;51:311-316. 59. Pan CY, Tsai CL, Chu CH. Fundamental movement skills in chil-
39. Dzuik MA, Larson JC, Apostu A, Mahone EM, Denckla MB, Mostof- dren diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders and attention deficit
sky SH. Dyspraxia in autism: association with motor, social, and hyperactivity disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2009;39:1964-1705.
communicative deficits. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2007;49:734-739. 60. Kopp S, Beckung E, Gillberg C. Developmental coordination disor-
40. Dowell LR, Mahone EM, Mostofsky SH. Associations of postural der and other motor control problems in girls with autism spectrum
knowledge and basic motor skill with dyspraxia in autism: implica- disorder and/or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Res Dev Dis-
tion for abnormalities in distributed connectivity and motor learning. abil. 2010;31:350-361.
Neuropsychology. 2009;23:563-570. 61. Frank JS, Earl M. Coordination of posture and movement. Phys Ther.
41. Glazebrook CM, Elliott D, Szatmari P. How do individuals with 1990;70:855-863.
autism plan their movements? J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38: 62. Wallman HV. The basics of balance and falls. Home Health Care Manag
114-126. Pract. 2009;21:436-439.

Pediatric Physical Therapy Motor Activity in Autism 9


Copyright © 2012 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins and the Section on Pediatrics of the American Physical Therapy
Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
63. Travers BG, Klinger MR, Mussey JL, Klinger LG. Motor-linked im- 65. Berlin LJ, Brooks-Gunn J, McCarton C, McCorminck MC. The effec-
plicit learning in persons with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Res. tiveness of early intervention: examining risk factors and pathways
2010;3:68-77. to enhanced development. Prev Med. 1998;27:238-245.
64. Gomby DS, Larner MB, Stevenson CS, Behrman RE. Long-term out- 66. Jette A. Toward a common language for function, disability, and
comes of early childhood programs: analysis and recommendations. health. Phys Ther. 2006; 86:726-734.
Future Child. 1995;5:6-24.

APPENDIX
Brief Summary of Each Article Revieweda

Study Design/
Author(s) Methodology Study Groups Examination Toolsb Summary of Findings

Provost et al11 Comparison Autism spectrum disorder Bayley Scales of Infant According to scores on the BSID II and the
(ASD) (n = 19) Development II PDMS-2, 63% and 68% (respectively) of
Developmental delay (DD) Motor Scale (BSID II) children with ASD would qualify for early
with concerns for motor Peadbody intervention services on the basis of a 25%
delay chronologically Developmental motor delay. These scores were similar to
aged matched within 3 Motor Scales, 2nd those of a group of children diagnosed
months (n = 19) Edition (PDMS-2) with DD.
Developmental concerns
without motor delay
chronologically aged
matched within 3 months
(NMD) (n = 18)
Landa and Prospective High risk for autism Mullen Scales of Early Participants were initially identified from
Garrett- comparison (n = 60) Leaning (MSEL) 2 groups: infants considered to be at high
Mayer12 Low risk for autism (n = 27) risk of autism because they were siblings
of children with autism and infants
considered at low risk because there was
no family history of autism. Review of test
scores and clinical judgment led to
categorization of these infants as
unaffected, ASD, or language delayed. On
the basis of MSEL scores, children with
ASD had slowed in development in all
domains except visual reception by 14
months. By 24 months, significant
differences were found between the group
with ASD and the group of children
developing typically on all domains of the
MSEL.
Esposito et al13 Retrospective ASD (n = 18) Eschkol-Wachman Based on retrospective video analysis, infants
comparison Typical development (TD) Movement Notation (12-21 weeks) who were later diagnosed
(video (n = 18) static and dynamic with ASD had higher rates of asymmetry
analysis) DD (n = 12) symmetry in supine static and dynamic lying
postures. Symmetry was noted in some
children with ASD; however, children
with early onset ASD were more likely to
demonstrate lower levels of symmetry.
Teitelbaum et al14 Retrospective ASD (n = 17) Eschkol-Wachman Based on retrospective video analysis of
comparison TD (n = 15) Movement Notation infants, most of the children with ASD
(video demonstrated altered movement patterns
analysis) in mouth shape and lying, righting, sitting,
crawling, and walking that could be
identified within the first few months of
life.
Baraneck15 Retrospective Autism disorder (AD) Video analysis and Based on retrospective video analysis of
comparison (n = 11) coding of behavioral infants between 9 and 12 months’
(video DD (n = 10) categories: looking, corrected chronological age, subtle
analysis) TD (n = 11) affect, response to sensory-motor deficits were present in
name, anticipatory infants who were later diagnosed with AD.
postures, Social deficits were also noted.
motor/object
stereotypies, social
touch, sensory
modulation
(continued)

10 Downey and Rapport Pediatric Physical Therapy


Copyright © 2012 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins and the Section on Pediatrics of the American Physical Therapy
Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
APPENDIX
Brief Summary of Each Article Revieweda (Continued)

Study Design/
Author(s) Methodology Study Groups Examination Toolsb Summary of Findings

Dewrang and Retrospective Asperger syndrome (AS) Parent questionnaire- According to results from retrospective
Sandberg16 comparison (n = 23) symptoms of autism parent questionnaire, individuals
(parent TD (n = 12) before the age of with AS demonstrated difficulties in
questionnaire) 2 years (SAB-2) the first
2 years of life with several areas of
development, including motor
skills. Parents did report difficulty
with imitation of motor skills and
coordination.
Ozonoff et al17 Retrospective AD (n = 54) including Infant Motor Maturity On the basis of retrospective video
comparison -Autism: no regression and Atypically analysis, children who were later
(video (At(NR)) (n = 26) Coding Scales diagnosed with AD did not
analysis) -Autism: regression (At(R)) demonstrate higher rates of
(n = 28) movement abnormalities or fewer
DD (n = 25) protective responses before the age
TD (n = 24) of 2. There were slower rates of
motor development noted in the
group with At(NR) in early skills
and in the group with At(R) in
walking.
Loh et al18 Retrospective ASD (from a population of Coding of motor Videos were analyzed of children at 12
comparison children with siblings mannerisms during and 18 months of age. The arm wave
(video diagnosed with ASD) standardized testing posture was more commonly seen in
analysis) (n = 8) children with ASD in both age
Nondiagnosed siblings of groups. At 18 months, the
children with ASD hand-to-ear posture was noted in
(n = 9) both the group with ASD and the
TD (n = 15) nondiagnosed siblings of children
with ASD. Overlap between all
groups was present for stereotyped
behaviors.
Rogers et al20 Comparison AD (n = 24) Imitation battery Children with AD had decreased
DD (mixed etiology) Praxis battery imitation performance when
(n = 20) compared with children with DD or
Fragile × Syndrome (FXS) children developing typically. No
(n = 18, not included in differences were found in motor
group comparison skills between children with AD,
analysis) including: DD, or children developing
-FXS without AD typically, and no correlation was
(n = 13) found between motor skills and
-FXS with AD (n = 5) imitation abilities in children
TD (n = 15) diagnosed with AD.
Stone et al21 Part 1 Part 1: Motor Imitation Part 1: Children with ASD under 31/2
ASD (n = 18) Scale years old have poorer imitation
DD (n = 18) skills than children without ASD but
TD (n = 18) with developmental delays when
matched on mental age,
chronological age, and language
ability. Difficulties were noted
specifically with imitation of body
movements and nonmeaningful
actions. Difficulties were similar
across all groups, suggesting that
motor imitation skills in children
with ASD may be delayed in
acquisition and not disordered.
Part 2 Part 2: Motor Imitation Part 2: Motor imitation improved in
ASD (n = 26) Scale (only total, children with ASD between the age
body, and object of 2 and 3 years.
scores)
(continued)

Pediatric Physical Therapy Motor Activity in Autism 11


Copyright © 2012 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins and the Section on Pediatrics of the American Physical Therapy
Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
APPENDIX
Brief Summary of Each Article Revieweda (Continued)

Study Design/
Author(s) Methodology Study Groups Examination Toolsb Summary of Findings

Williams et al22 Systematic Based on a literature review of 21 studies,


review imitation deficits were present in
children with ASD and more apparent
in younger children (under the age of
4) and with nonmeaningful tasks. More
research is necessary to further
delineate autism and dyspraxia.
Stone et al23 Comparison AD (n = 22) Imitation motor tasks Preschool-aged children with AD had
Intellectually delayed with (12 total) significantly lower motor imitation
an IQ less than 70 scores compared with preschool-aged
(n = 15) children who have an intellectual delay,
Hearing-impaired (n = 15) are hearing or language impaired, and
Language- impaired children developing typically. Motor
(n = 19) imitation scores strongly differentiated
TD (n = 20) children with AD from children with
other developmental delays and may be
a useful screening tool.
Rogers et al24 Comparison Early onset AD (n = 17) Motor imitation in 2 Children younger than 14 months in the
Regressive-onset AD conditions: combined AD group demonstrated
(n = 24) functional and increased errors on imitation tasks
DD (n = 22) nonfunctional when compared with the other groups,
TD (n = 22) to which they were matched on the
basis of nonverbal skill age. All groups
of older aged children demonstrated
similar imitation skills except for the
group with regressive-onset AD, who
demonstrated impaired nonfunctional
imitation skills. No differences were
found between groups on error type or
pattern, nor was there a difference in
emulation of the task between groups.
The results suggest that imitation
differences are not due to a motor
impairment; rather, they may be due to
mirror neuron network impairments or
atypical brain mechanisms associated
with the mirror neuron system.
Van Vuchelen Comparison Cognitive impairment PDMS-2- administered Children with ASD who have been
et al25 (n = 21) to those in the group diagnosed with LFA or HFA, when
with IQ < 80 including with a cognitive matched for age, sex, and
-Low functioning ASD impairment developmental level with a comparison
(LFA) (n = 8) Movement assessment cohort, demonstrated increased errors
-Cognitive impairment battery for children with imitation tasks. Children in the
without ASD (n = 13) (MABC)- group with ASD also demonstrated
No cognitive impairment administered to those significantly poorer motor scores than
with IQ > 80 (n = 34) with high the comparison group. Based on the
including functioning IQ results, the authors suggested that
-High functioning ASD Motor imitation test difficulties with imitation arise from a
(HFA) (n = 17) delayed “action production system.”
-TD (n = 17)
Green et al26 Comparison AS (n = 11) MABC Although not statistically significant,
Specific developmental The Gesture Test children with AS performed more
disorder of motor poorly and variably on the MABC and
function (SDD-MF) the Gesture Test when compared with
(n = 9) children with SDD-MF of similar age.
All children with AS tested below the
15th percentile on the MABC. In the
group with AS, poor scores on the
MABC were correlated with lower
scores on the Gesture Test.
(continued)

12 Downey and Rapport Pediatric Physical Therapy


Copyright © 2012 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins and the Section on Pediatrics of the American Physical Therapy
Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
APPENDIX
Brief Summary of Each Article Revieweda (Continued)

Study Design/
Author(s) Methodology Study Groups Examination Toolsb Summary of Findings

Mostofsky et al27 Comparison ASD (n = 21) including Florida Apraxia While error type was similar between the 2
-HFA (n = 13) Screening Test groups, children with ASD demonstrated
-AS (n = 8) (Revised) significantly higher errors on the Florida
Gender and age matched Apraxia Screening Test than the
TD (n = 24) comparison group. In the group with ASD,
Inclusion for all subjects: children with HFA had significantly more
IQ > 80 errors on the body-part-for-tool than the
group with AS; however, no other
significant differences existed. The authors
suggested that these findings are indicative
of a praxis issue in children with ASD, not
just difficulty with imitation.
Smith and Comparison AD (n = 20) Tests of: Children with AD had no difficulty
Bryson28 Language impairment 1. Memory and understanding or recognizing motor
chronologically and comprehension of gestures. They demonstrated increased
receptive age matched gestures difficulty naming and imitating gestures.
(n = 20) 2. Gesture production The authors suggested that this might be
TD receptive age matched and imitation due to a praxis impairment with difficulty
(n = 20) in mapping movements as well as the
representation of movements.
Dewey et al29 Comparison ASD (n = 49) Bruininks-Oseretsky On testing to assess gesturing, children with
Developmental coordination Test of Motor ASD had significantly higher rates of
disorder (DCD) (n = 46) Proficiency Short errors than children in the other groups.
Attention-deficit/ Form (BOT-SF) Overall, on motor testing, children with
hyperactivity disorder The Gestures Test ASD demonstrated significantly poorer
(ADHD) (n = 27) scores with increased variability in scores
ADHD and DCD (n = 38) than children in any of the other groups
TD (n = 78) (41% of children with ASD did not meet
criteria for motor impairment based on the
BOT-SF). Although this may be related to
praxis, these authors suggested that it
might also be related to altered neural
substrates or language deficits, as errors
were still present when motor deficits
were accounted for.
Ben-Sasson et al30 Comparison HFA (n = 23) with IQ > 70 Demonstration task Children with HFA or language impairment
including portion of the Autism demonstrated significantly lower levels of
-AD (n = 15) Diagnostic gesture representation than children
-Pervasive developmental Observation developing typically on a demonstration
disorder–not otherwise Schedule (ADOS) task. The authors suggested that in the
specified (PDD-NOS) group with HFA, this might be attributed
(n = 11) to motor planning or language
Language impairment impairments. Difficulties may have also
(n = 23) been exacerbated by the requirement to
TD (n = 30) speak and gesture, as well as the lack of
natural environment.
Minshew et al31 Comparison HFA (n = 79) Dynamic Individuals between the ages of 5 and
TD “group matched” posturography 52 years with HFA demonstrated
(n = 61) (EquiTest) decreased postural control when tested on
Inclusion for all subjects: full the EquiTest compared to a sample with
scale and verbal IQ > 70 TD. Increased difficulty was noted during
the conditions of sensory conflict. Postural
control did not improve until the age of 12
and individuals with HFA never achieved
adult levels (plateau seen at approximately
20 years of age in comparison group).
Schmitz et al32 Comparison AD (n = 8) (right-hand Bimanual load lift task During a bimanual load lift task, muscle
dominance) with kinematic and latencies and increased unloading time
TD (n = 16) (right-hand electromyographic found in children with AD suggest a
dominance) analysis decreased use of anticipatory control seen
in a group with TD. Children with AD
demonstrated an increased use of reactive
postural control.
(continued)

Pediatric Physical Therapy Motor Activity in Autism 13


Copyright © 2012 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins and the Section on Pediatrics of the American Physical Therapy
Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
APPENDIX
Brief Summary of Each Article Revieweda (Continued)

Study Design/
Author(s) Methodology Study Groups Examination Toolsb Summary of Findings

Kohen-Raz et al33 Comparison AD (n = 91) Tetra-ataxiametry With posturographic testing, children with AD
TD (n = 166) method for postural had increased variability in performance and
control during abnormal weight distribution with less use of
posturographic the typical anteroposterior sway. Adolescent
testing aged children with AD demonstrated
decreased stability when compared to
preschool-aged children with TD. Notably,
the authors also found a “paradoxical”
response to stressful situations, as children
had increased postural stability in stressful
conditions (removal of vision).
Molloy et al34 Comparison ASD (n = 8) Posturographic testing Children were tested in 4 balance positions. On
TD chronologically age the basis of the results, children with ASD
matched (n = 8) had less postural stability than the children
developing typically with removal of visual
cues and deviation of somatosensory cues.
Children with ASD tended to rely on visual
input, demonstrated by increased sway with
removal of visual cues regardless of
somatosensory input. A “paradoxical stress
response” was not found. Authors reported
that these results in a previous study might
have been due to additional visual stimuli.
Fournier et al35 Comparison ASD (n = 13) Posturographic testing Children with ASD demonstrated significantly
TD chronologically age higher levels of mediolateral and
matched (n = 12) anteroposterior sway, as well as sway area,
than children with TD during quiet stance.
The authors also noted a decreased
displacement of the center of pressure toward
the swing leg during gait in the group with
ASD. This can cause a decrease in shift of the
center of mass to the stance limb, creating an
increased need for postural control. The
authors suggested that children with ASD
demonstrate postural instability.
Manjiviona and Comparison AS (n = 12) Test of Motor On the basis of motor testing, children with AS
Prior36 HFA (n = 9) Impairment- and HFA have variability in motor activities.
Inclusion for all subjects: Henderson Revision Fifty percent of the children with AS and
“normal or near (TOMI-H) 66.7% of the children with HFA
normal IQ” demonstrated motor impairments when
compared with a normative sample data for
the TOMI-H. No significant differences were
noted between the 2 groups. A significant
negative correlation was found between
TOMI-H scores and IQ. The authors
suggested that the lack of difference between
the group scores provides support for AS
being included in ASD diagnoses, rather than
its own diagnostic classification.
Ghaziuddin and Comparison AD (n = 12) Bruininks-Oseretsky Children in all 3 age-matched groups
Butler37 AS (n = 12) Test (BOT) demonstrated motor activity impairments
PDD-NOS (n = 12) when tested on the BOT. Children with AD
scored significantly lower on the BOT than
those with AS, and no other significant
differences were noted between groups. A
strong correlation between IQ scores and
motor scores was found. The authors also
suggested that while the BOT does test for
motor impairments, a pattern of impairment
is not yet clear for individuals with ASD.
(continued)

14 Downey and Rapport Pediatric Physical Therapy


Copyright © 2012 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins and the Section on Pediatrics of the American Physical Therapy
Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
APPENDIX
Brief Summary of Each Article Revieweda (Continued)

Study Design/
Author(s) Methodology Study Groups Examination Toolsb Summary of Findings

Green et al38 Comparison AD (n = 45) MABC Motor impairments, as tested by the


ASD (broad) (n = 56) Developmental MABC (defined by <5th percentile),
Coordination were present in 79.2% of all children
Disorder (9-10 years old) in the study. Group
Questionnaire scores were similar with children in
both groups having definite motor
impairments. When IQ was accounted
for, 97.1% of the children with low IQ
(<70) had definite motor problems, as
compared with 69.7% of children with
typical IQ (>70).
Dzuik et al39 Comparison ASD (n = 47) Florida Apraxia Children with ASD demonstrated
TD (n = 47) Screening Test significantly poorer scores on the
(Revised) Florida Apraxia Screening Test and the
Physical and PANESS, suggesting poorer basic motor
Neurological skills as well as praxis. Scores on praxis
Assessment of Subtle testing significantly predicted scores on
Signs (PANESS) the ADOS, whereas basic motor skill
did not. The authors suggested that
although individuals with ASD may
have impaired basic motor skills,
dyspraxia may actually be independent
of motor skills and may be a core
symptom of ASD.
Dowell et al40 Comparison ASD (n = 37) Florida Apraxia The children in the group diagnosed with
TD (n = 50) Screening Test ASD had significantly poorer scores on
(modified for the PANESS, the postural knowledge
children) test, and the Florida Apraxia Screening
PANESS Test. No significant difference was
Postural knowledge test noted between children with HFA and
(modified for AS on the postural knowledge test,
children) although age predicted praxis and IQ
did not. When age and IQ were
accounted for, postural knowledge and
basic motor score predicted praxis
score, however; when age, IQ, postural
knowledge, and basic motor score were
all accounted for, praxis performance
was significantly related to diagnosis.
The authors found that praxis
performance was significantly
associated with ADOS score, suggesting
that praxis may be a core symptom of
ASD.
Glazebrook Comparison Part 1 Part 1: Calculation of Part 1: Individuals with AD demonstrated
et al41 AD (n = 18) reaction and lower and significantly more variable
Without AD (n = 18) movement times reaction times than those without AD.
during an adapted The group with AD also demonstrated
precue paradigm significantly longer times to execute
movement. Similar to individuals
without AD, those with AD were able
to use advanced visual cues to plan
movements and decrease reaction time.
Part 2 Part 2: Calculation of Part 2: Individuals with AD demonstrated
AD (n = 9) reaction time and lower reaction times and longer times
Without AD (n = 9) movement times to execute movement than those
based on a rapid without AD. Individuals with AD
aiming task adopted appeared to use results from the prior
from earlier research trial to assist in movement planning for
the current trial.
(continued)

Pediatric Physical Therapy Motor Activity in Autism 15


Copyright © 2012 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins and the Section on Pediatrics of the American Physical Therapy
Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
APPENDIX
Brief Summary of Each Article Revieweda (Continued)

Study Design/
Author(s) Methodology Study Groups Examination Toolsb Summary of Findings

Glazebrook Comparison AD (n = 9) Calculation of reaction The group with AD required more


et al42 Age-matched individuals time and movement time to plan movements. They also
without AD (n = 9) times based on a demonstrated significantly increased
rapid aiming task times to perform movements with
adopted from earlier significantly decreased peak
research velocities and peak accelerations
than the group without AD. Verbal
ability was correlated to reaction
and movement times and nonverbal
ability was correlated with reaction
times. Overall, individuals in the
group with AD demonstrated poorer
motor ability than individuals
without AD.
Nazzarali et al43 Comparison Part 1 Part 1: Variation of Part 1: Individuals with AD
AD (n = 12) protocol from demonstrated slower reaction and
Without AD (n = 12) Glazebrook movement times than the group
et al41 developing typically. They were able
to use advance cues to plan
movements and decrease reaction
times.
Part 2 Part 2: Measurement of Part 2: Individuals with AD
AD (n = 12) reaction and demonstrated increased difficulty
Without AD (n = 12) movement times reprogramming an already-planned
during a reaching movement. This was more difficult
task that was hand when the task required a change in
manipulated or hands than a change in directions.
direction The authors suggested that this
manipulated might be due to a slowed visual
responsiveness for spatial attention
or inefficient connections between
hemispheres of the brain.
Rinehart et al44 Comparison HFA with performance and Measurement of Although individuals with HFA and AS
verbal IQ >70 (n = 12) preparation and demonstrated similar errors in a
TD (matched on age, sex, movement time with serial-choice task, both groups
IQ) (n = 12) a serial-choice demonstrated increased preparation
AS (n = 12) button-pressing time when compared with a cohort
TD (matched on age, sex, apparatus with TD. The group with AS
IQ) (n = 12) demonstrated slower preparation
movements, while the group with
HFA demonstrated a lack of
anticipation in preparation.
Rinehart et al45 Comparison HFA with performance and Upper extremity Individuals with HFA demonstrated
verbal IQ > 70 (n = 12) kinematic task to significantly slowed preparation
TD (matched on age, sex, measure movement times when compared with a cohort
IQ) (n = 12) preparation and with TD. Although no significant
AS (n = 12) movement time difference was found between
TD (matched on age, sex, individuals with AD and a cohort
IQ) (n = 12) with TD, the authors did note a
trend toward increased preparation
time in the group with AD. They
suggested that there is a true
planning deficit, rather than a
slowed movement. This deficit is
more predominant in individuals
with ASD. The authors further
suggested that although motor
impairments may be present in both
groups, the underlying cause may be
different and further research is
necessary to examine these causes.
(continued)

16 Downey and Rapport Pediatric Physical Therapy


Copyright © 2012 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins and the Section on Pediatrics of the American Physical Therapy
Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
APPENDIX
Brief Summary of Each Article Revieweda (Continued)

Study Design/
Author(s) Methodology Study Groups Examination Toolsb Summary of Findings

Vernazza-Martin Comparison AD (n = 9) Gait analysis The authors found no significant difference


et al46 TD (n = 6) between the group with AD and the group
with TD on gait parameters (stride duration,
step length, velocity, cadence, and string and
stride length), except for stride length, which
was found to be shorter in children with AD.
On the basis of analysis, children with AD
demonstrated increased oscillations of the
head, shoulder, and trunk causing less stable
and more variable posture. Although children
with AD demonstrated increased oscillations,
they were able to stabilize in the frontal
plane. Gait parameters and stability appeared
similar between groups; however, locomotion
pattern was not maintained in the group with
AD. The group with AD demonstrated
difficulty with gait trajectory based on an
imposed goal, suggesting difficulties with
motor planning.
Hughes47 Comparison AD (n = 36) Reach, grasp, and Based on hand positioning during a
Moderate learning place task (Bar reach-and-grasp task, the authors suggested
disabilities (n = 24) Game) with that individuals with AD have increased
TD (n = 28) examiner report of difficulty with executing even simple
hand positions goal-directed tasks when compared with
children with moderate learning disabilities
or children with TD. The authors suggested
that the pattern seen in older children with
AD is similar to that seen in preschool-aged
children with TD, suggesting that
development is delayed rather than altered.
They also hypothesized that the differences
may be due to sequencing, vision, or
consequence prediction.
Staples and Comparison ASD (n = 25) Test of Gross Motor Children with ASD demonstrated significantly
Reid48 TD, age matched (n = 25) Development lower scores on locomotor and object control
TD, movement skill scores than children who were
performance matched chronologically age matched and mental age
(n = 22) matched. No difference was found between
TD, mental age matched the group with ASD and the developmental
(n = 19) age matched, as they were matched on the
basis of motor scores. In general, children
with ASD had increased difficulty
coordinating both sides of the body for a task,
as arm movements were noted to be awkward.
On the basis of comparison with children
who were developmentally matched, children
with ASD appear to be delayed rather than
disordered, as skills aligned with children
with TD approximately half their age.
Jansiewicz et al49 Comparison ASD (n = 40) PANESS In the group with ASD, no differences were
TD with no neurologic or found on the PANESS between individuals
psychiatric diagnoses diagnosed with HFA or AS. The group with
(n = 55) ASD demonstrated significant differences
from the group with TD on all variables of the
PANESS, except for impersistence and
patterned timed movements (a trend toward
significant was present). Overall, children
with ASD tended to have difficulty with
balance, gait, and timed movement of the
hands and feet.
(continued)

Pediatric Physical Therapy Motor Activity in Autism 17


Copyright © 2012 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins and the Section on Pediatrics of the American Physical Therapy
Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
APPENDIX
Brief Summary of Each Article Revieweda (Continued)

Study Design/
Author(s) Methodology Study Groups Examination Toolsb Summary of Findings

Weimer et al50 Comparison AS (n = 10) Motor testing: Finger Individuals with AS demonstrated poorer
TD age matched (n = 10) Tapping, Grooved scores on tests of apraxia, balance
Pegboard, Trail (tandem and single leg), and
Making, Assessment finger-thumb apposition, but did not
of Apraxia, demonstrate significantly poorer scores
Finger-Thumb on tests of basic motor function than a
Apposition, cohort with TD. The balance scores were
Assessment of Ataxia, poorer with eyes closed. On the basis of
Assessment of these results and the lack of dizziness
Visuomotor usually found with a vestibular
Integration dysfunction, the authors suggested that
individuals with AS may have a reliance
on visual input and a proprioceptive
deficit.
Freitag et al51 Comparison ASD with Full Scale IQ >70 Zurich Neuromotor The group with ASD demonstrated
(n = 16) including AS Assessment increased difficulty with dynamic balance
(n = 4) HFA (n = 12) and diadochokinesis. The authors
TD IQ matched (n = 16) suggested that these impairments might
be a result of poor integration of motor,
sensory, and executive function. An
association between motor scores, the
core symptoms of ASD, and level of
withdrawal was also found.
Fuentes et al52 Comparison ASD (n = 14) Minnesota Handwriting Children developing typically had
TD (n = 14) Assessment Revised significantly higher scores on the
PANESS PANESS when compared with children
with ASD, particularly for the sections
examining gait and timed movements.
Handwriting scores were lower in the
group with ASD; however, children with
ASD did not demonstrate difficulty
aligning or sizing letters. Overall scores
on the PANESS, as well as scores on the
timed movement section, were found to
be predictive of handwriting scores. The
authors suggested that when overall
motor skills are addressed in children
with ASD, handwriting abilities may
improve as a result of increased control
and ease of manipulation.
van Swieten Comparison DCD (n = 27) Grasp and turn task to Children with ASD demonstrated similar
et al53 ASD (n = 20) measure preferred grip selection to age-matched children
TD (n = 70) grip developing typically on a task that the
authors used to test motor planning. The
authors hypothesized that the need for
motor planning was not strong enough
to elicit a difference in the children with
ASD.
Miyahara et al54 Comparison AS (n = 26) MABC Based on motor testing, 85% of the children
Learning disabilities with AS and 88% of the children with
(n = 16) learning disabilities qualified for the
diagnosis of SDD-MF, which the authors
reported as being 42-44 times higher
than the typical population. There was a
significant difference between groups for
manual dexterity skills. The children
with AS trended toward poorer ball
skills. The authors suggested that this
trend may be due to type of preferred
play or decreased interpersonal skills in
children with AS.
(continued)

18 Downey and Rapport Pediatric Physical Therapy


Copyright © 2012 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins and the Section on Pediatrics of the American Physical Therapy
Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
APPENDIX
Brief Summary of Each Article Revieweda (Continued)

Study Design/
Author(s) Methodology Study Groups Examination Toolsb Summary of Findings

Provost et al55 Comparison ASD (n = 19) PDMS-2 When children with ASD were matched with
DD (n = 19) children with DD based on age, gender,
and cognitive age, similar gross and fine
motor profiles were found.
Matson et al56 Comparison AD (n = 117) Battelle Developmental There was no significant difference found
PDD-NOS (n = 112) Inventory, 2nd between children with AD and PDD-NOS
Atypically developing Edition (differences did approach significance), or
without ASD (n = 168) children with PDD-NOS and children
atypically developing on gross and fine
motor scores. Children with AD did have
significantly lower fine and gross motor
scores than children who were developing
atypically. Motor impairments were
present in all groups. The authors
suggested that motor impairments in
children with AD are apparent at an early
age and they may benefit from early
intervention services.
Noterdaeme Comparison AD (n = 11) Standardized Qualitatively, statistically significant
et al57 Expressive language neurological differences were found on motor
disorder age and IQ examination: fine performance between the children with
matched (n = 11) motor, gross motor, TD and the children with expressive and
Receptive language disorder coordination, receptive language disorders in all areas
age and IQ matched balance, and oral except for coordination. Children with AD
(n = 11) motor for global had significantly poorer scores than
TD age and IQ matched neuromotor children with TD for all sections except for
(n = 11) impairment score, oral motor and coordination tasks.
performance times
Morin and Reid58 Comparison AD (n = 8) 5 test items: dynamic On motor testing, there was an overall trend
Intellectually delayed balance, catching, (not significant) toward poorer motor
(n = 8) standing long jump scores in the group with AD than the
(adapted from the group that was intellectually delayed. The
BOT), throwing, group with AD demonstrated significantly
running higher balance scores than the group that
was intellectually delayed, which the
authors suggested may be due to slowed
movements seen in functional play. The
group that was intellectually delayed
demonstrated superior target throwing
skills. The authors suggested that low
motor scores in children with AD might be
related to level of cognitive impairment,
rather than diagnosis alone.
Pan et al59 Comparison ASD (n = 28) Test of Gross Motor Children with ASD demonstrated
ADHD (n = 29) Development– significantly poorer scores on locomotion,
TD (n = 34) Second Edition, object control, and GMDQ than children
calculation of the with ADHD and children with TD.
Gross Motor Children with ADHD also demonstrated
Development significantly poorer scores on measures
Quotient (GMDQ) than children with TD. When children in
the group with ASD who demonstrated
attention deficits were omitted from
analysis, results still indicated that
children with ASD have poorer scores,
suggesting that motor ability is not related
to attention. The authors suggested that
based on GMDQ, differences might be
secondary to limited social skills.
(continued)

Pediatric Physical Therapy Motor Activity in Autism 19


Copyright © 2012 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins and the Section on Pediatrics of the American Physical Therapy
Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
APPENDIX
Brief Summary of Each Article Revieweda (Continued)

Study Design/
Author(s) Methodology Study Groups Examination Toolsb Summary of Findings

Kopp et al60 Comparison ASD (n = 20) EB-test (used for All study participants were female. High
ADHD (n = 34) children older than 6 rates of DCD were found in the group with
TD age and IQ matched years), Cailler-Asuza ASD and ADHD. In school-aged girls, 25%
(n = 57) Scale (children below of those with ASD and 32% of those with
4 years), MABC, ADHD were diagnosed with DCD. In the
motor-neurological- preschool-aged girls, 80% of girls with
perceptual ASD were diagnosed with DCD. On the
assessment EB-test, girls with ASD and ADHD scored
significantly lower than girls with TD, and
girls with ADHD had lower overall scores
than those with ASD. On the basis of
overall clinical picture, the authors
suggested that younger age, low IQ, and
autistic symptoms are predictors for lower
motor scores.
a Articles are listed in the same order in which they appear in the article.
b Tests related to motor and imitation skills were reported in the table. Studies may have used other testing (such as the ADOS), for diagnostic purposes,

and these tests were not included in the descriptions of each study in the table. See specific studies for more details on tests used.

20 Downey and Rapport Pediatric Physical Therapy


Copyright © 2012 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins and the Section on Pediatrics of the American Physical Therapy
Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

You might also like