You are on page 1of 17

Research in Developmental Disabilities 72 (2018) 79–95

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Research in Developmental Disabilities


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/redevdis

Research Paper

Comparing motor performance, praxis, coordination, and MARK


interpersonal synchrony between children with and without
Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD)

Maninderjit Kaura,1,3, Sudha M. Srinivasana,2,3, Anjana N. Bhata,b,c,
a
Physical Therapy Department & The Biomechanics & Movement Science Program, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19713, USA
b
Physical Therapy Program, Department of Kinesiology, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT 06269, USA
c
Institute for Collaboration on Health, Intervention, and Policy, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT 06269, USA

AR TI CLE I NF O AB S T R A CT

Number of reviews completed are 2 Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) have basic motor impairments in balance, gait,
Keywords: and coordination as well as autism-specific impairments in praxis/motor planning and inter-
Autism personal synchrony. Majority of the current literature focuses on isolated motor behaviors or
Children domains. Additionally, the relationship between cognition, symptom severity, and motor per-
Motor formance in ASD is unclear. We used a comprehensive set of measures to compare gross and fine
Coordination motor, praxis/imitation, motor coordination, and interpersonal synchrony skills across three
Praxis groups of children between 5 and 12 years of age: children with ASD with high IQ (HASD),
Interpersonal synchrony
children with ASD with low IQ (LASD), and typically developing (TD) children. We used the
Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency and the Bilateral Motor Coordination subtest of the
Sensory Integration and Praxis Tests to assess motor performance and praxis skills respectively.
Children were also examined while performing simple and complex rhythmic upper and lower
limb actions on their own (solo context) and with a social partner (social context). Both ASD
groups had lower gross and fine motor scores, greater praxis errors in total and within various
error types, lower movement rates, greater movement variability, and weaker interpersonal
synchrony compared to the TD group. In addition, the LASD group had lower gross motor scores
and greater mirroring errors compared to the HASD group. Overall, a variety of motor impair-
ments are present across the entire spectrum of children with ASD, regardless of their IQ scores.
Both, fine and gross motor performance significantly correlated with IQ but not with autism
severity; however, praxis errors (mainly, total, overflow, and rhythmicity) strongly correlated
with autism severity and not IQ. Our study findings highlight the need for clinicians and
therapists to include motor evaluations and interventions in the standard-of-care of children with
ASD and for the broader autism community to recognize dyspraxia as an integral part of the
definition of ASD.

Abbreviations: ASD, Autism Spectrum Disorder; IQ, Intelligent Quotient; LASD, children with ASD with low IQ; HASD, children with ASD with high IQ; TD, Typically
Developing; BOT-2, Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency-2nd Edition; SIPT-BMC, Bilateral Motor Coordination subtest of Sensory Integration and Praxis
Tests; CV, Coefficient of Variation; IPS, Interpersonal Synchrony

Corresponding Author Anjana Bhat, PT, PhD, Associate Professor in Physical Therapy, University of Delaware, 540 South College Avenue, Newark, DE 19713, USA.
E-mail address: abhat@udel.edu (A. N. Bhat).
1
Maninderjit Kaur is now at the Psychology and Human Development Department, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN 37212, USA.
2
Sudha Srinivasan is now at the IDC School of Design, Indian Institute of Technology Bombay, Powai, Mumbai 400076, Maharashtra, India.
3
Dr. Kaur and Dr. Srinivasan are co-first authors due to their equal contributions to this project.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2017.10.025
Received 5 January 2017; Received in revised form 30 October 2017; Accepted 30 October 2017
0891-4222/ © 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
M. Kaur et al. Research in Developmental Disabilities 72 (2018) 79–95

1. Introduction

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a multisystem neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by primary deficits in social
communication skills and highly repetitive and restricted behaviors and interests (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). In ad-
dition to these diagnostic impairments, around 50–85% children with ASD demonstrate consistent deficits in several aspects of
perceptuo-motor performance (Bhat, Landa, & Galloway, 2011; Chukoskie, Townsend, & Westerfield, 2013; Green et al., 2009; Hilton,
Zhang, Whilte, Klohr, & Constantino, 2012). A systematic examination of motor deficits in ASD is warranted for several reasons. First,
motor symptoms are some of the earliest identifiable impairments noticed in infants and toddlers who may go on to develop a
diagnosis of ASD (Bhat, Galloway, & Landa, 2012; Bedford, Pickles, & Lord, 2016; Flanagan, Landa, Bhat, & Bauman, 2012;
Landa & Garrett-Mayer, 2006). Second, there is substantial evidence suggesting that motor skill development is intimately linked to
social communication and cognitive development in children with ASD (Bhat, Srinivasan, Woxholdt, & Shield, 2016; Dziuk, Larson,
Apostu, Mahone, Denckla, & Mostofsky, 2007; Dowell, Mahone, & Mostofsky, 2009; Leary & Hill, 1996). Lastly, a detailed examina-
tion of the motor system can provide insights into the functioning of the well-understood neural substrates underlying motor per-
formance as well as its neighboring brain regions responsible for social performance, and ultimately the neuropathology of ASD
(Dowell et al., 2009; Gizzonio et al., 2015). Therefore, in the current study, we used a comprehensive set of measures to assess
different axes of motor performance in school-age children with ASD with a wide range of intellectual abilities and autism severity.

1.1. Motor impairments in ASD

Research on motor impairments has suggested that children with ASD have substantial deficits in basic motor control skills as well
as specific impairments in praxis (Dewey, Cantell, & Crawford, 2007; Fournier, Hass, Naik, Lodha, & Cauraugh, 2010; Hallett et al.,
1993; Jansiewicz et al., 2006; Mostofsky et al., 2006; Noterdaeme, Mildenberger, Minow, & Amorosa, 2002; Srinivasan, Lynch,
Bubela, Gifford, & Bhat, 2013). In terms of basic motor skills, children with ASD demonstrate poor postural control (Freitag, Kleser,
Schneider, & von Gontard, 2007; Minshew, Sung, Jones, & Furman, 2004; Teitelbaum, Teitelbaum, Nye, Fryman, & Maurer, 1998),
gait abnormalities (Hallett et al., 1993; Rinehart et al., 2006; Vilensky, Damasio, & Maurer, 1981), as well as impairments in bilateral
coordination (Fournier et al., 2010; Isenhower et al., 2012; Kaur, Gifford, Marsh, & Bhat, 2013; Marsh et al., 2013). Children also
demonstrate poor fine motor control including manual dexterity, handwriting, object control, and visuo-motor integration skills
(Berkeley, Zittel, Pitney, & Nichols, 2001; Fuentes, Mostofsky, & Bastian, 2009; Green et al., 2002; Kushki, Chau, & Anagnostou, 2011;
Mayes & Calhoun, 2003; McPhillips, Finlay, Bejerot, & Hanley, 2014; Miyahara et al., 1997; Provost, Heimerl, & Lopez, 2007; Provost,
Lopez, & Heimerl, 2007; Sacrey, Germani, Bryson, & Zwaigenbaum, 2014). Children with ASD received lower scores on multiple
standardized tests of basic motor performance compared to typically developing (TD) children as well as children with other de-
velopmental diagnoses (Ament et al., 2015; Barbeau, Meilleur, Zeffiro, & Mottron, 2015; Ghaziuddin & Butler, 1998; Green et al.,
2009; Hilton, Graver, & LaVesser, 2007; Jansiewicz et al., 2006; Liu & Breslin, 2013; Manjiviona & Prior, 1995; Miyahara et al., 1997;
Provost, Heimerl et al., 2007; Provost, Lopez et al., 2007; Staples & Reid, 2010; Whyatt & Craig, 2012). In addition to these basic
motor deficits, children with ASD also demonstrate dyspraxia, i.e. impaired performance of skilled motor sequences/gestures during
imitation, on verbal command, and during tool use, that cannot be wholly explained by basic perceptuo-motor deficits (Carmo,
Rumiati, Siugzdaite, & Brambilla, 2013; Dewey et al., 2007; Dowell et al., 2009; Dziuk et al., 2007 ; Ham et al., 2011; Mostofsky et al.,
2006; Srinivasan et al., 2013; Vanvuchelen, Roeyers, & De Weerdt, 2007). While impairments in basic gross and fine motor skills are
also found in children with other developmental disorders including children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and
Developmental Coordination Disorder, impairments in praxis seem to be specific to autism (Dewey et al., 2007; MacNeil and
Mostofsky, 2012). Children with ASD demonstrated both spatial (incorrect body positioning, body-part-for-tool errors) and temporal
(poor movement timing, increased time to initiate movement) errors during imitation/praxis tasks (Dewey et al., 2007; Gizzonio
et al., 2015; Mostofsky et al., 2006; Salowitz et al., 2013; Vanvuchelen et al., 2007). Overall, there is considerable evidence for the
presence of robust impairments in non-specific basic motor skills as well as more specific motor functions such as praxis abilities in
ASD.

1.2. Neural mechanisms underlying motor impairments in ASD

The broad range of motor impairments in gait, posture, coordination, and imitation/praxis in ASD could be attributed to ab-
normal brain connectivity reported in this population (Courchesne, Campbell, & Solso, 2011; Just, Keller, Malave, Kana, & Varma,
2012). There is mounting evidence to support the presence of excessive short-range connectivity within cortical regions (such as the
frontal and parietal cortices) as well as poor long-range connectivity between cortico-cortical structures (such as the frontal and
occipital cortices) or cortico-subcortical structures (including the primary motor, premotor, and supplementary motor cortices with
the cerebellum or basal ganglia) (Mostofsky et al., 2009; Turner, Frost, Linsenbardt, McIlroy, & Müller, 2006). For instance, atypical
long-range functional connectivity between the motor areas and the visual areas of the brain is thought to underlie poor imitation
skills in individuals with ASD (Nebel et al., 2016). Similarly, greater movement variability and poor movement timing may be related
to abnormal cortico-cerebellar and cortico-striatal connections (Mostofsky et al., 2009; Rinehart et al., 2006). In fact, the pattern of
movement slowing in ASD has been likened to bradykinesia seen in patients with Parkinson’s disease (Hallett et al., 1993; Mari,
Castiello, Marks, Marraffa, & Prior, 2003; Vilensky et al., 1981). The more specific praxis impairments in ASD have been attributed to
deficits in the bilaterally distributed brain network thought to underlie praxis comprising areas V1 and V5 of the occipital cortex, the
superior temporal sulcus, inferior parietal region, premotor region, and the primary motor cortex (Mahajan, Dirlikov,

80
M. Kaur et al. Research in Developmental Disabilities 72 (2018) 79–95

Crocetti, & Mostofsky, 2016; Mostofsky & Ewen, 2011; Nebel et al., 2016).

1.3. Linkages between motor and social/cognitive skills in ASD

In addition to understanding the nature of perceptuo-motor impairments, considerable research has been directed towards ex-
amining the impact of motor impairments on social, communication and behavioral skills in individuals with ASD (Hilton et al.,
2007; Freitag et al., 2007; Fuentes et al., 2009; Fitzpatrick et al., 2017; Nebel et al., 2016). Some have proposed a fundamental role of
motor deficits in contributing to the classical symptoms of ASD (Bhat et al., 2011; Fitzpatrick et al., 2017; Koehne, Hatri,
Cacioppo, & Dziobek, 2016; Leary & Hill, 1996; Marsh et al., 2013; Whyatt & Craig, 2013a). For instance, Whyatt and Craig propose a
sensory-motor theory of ASD suggesting that individuals with ASD demonstrate a basic perception-action coupling deficit, which not
only affects spatio-temporal aspects of movement control but also forms the basis of more complex social communication and
cognitive behaviors (Whyatt & Craig, 2013a). Similarly, Fitzpatrick and colleagues argue that social motor coordination in the form of
both imitation and interpersonal synchrony is a critical prerequisite for successful social interactions (Fitzpatrick, Diorio,
Richardson, & Schmidt, 2013, 2016,2017). The authors found that in a pendulum coordination paradigm, high functioning adoles-
cents with ASD synchronized to a lesser degree with their parents both spontaneously and intentionally, compared to TD controls
(Fitzpatrick et al., 2016). Moreover, spontaneous social-motor synchronization was associated with responsive joint attention, co-
operation, and theory of mind skills, whereas intentional coordination was associated with initiating joint attention and theory of
mind skills in ASD (Fitzpatrick et al., 2017).

1.4. Neural mechanisms explaining motor − social/cognitive linkages in ASD

Neuroscientific studies supporting the links between social and motor skills in ASD suggest impairments in the mirror neuron
systems (MNS) (Dapretto et al., 2006; Oberman et al., 2005; Théoret et al., 2005; Williams et al., 2006). The core mirror neuron
network comprises neurons in the inferior frontal gyrus and inferior parietal lobule that are active both when we watch a social
partner perform an action and also when we ourselves perform the same action (Iacoboni et al., 1999; Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004).
This common coding between perception and action enables the observer to encode the goal of the action as well as its motor
characteristics, thereby playing a primary role in motor imitation of a social partner (Rizzolatti, Fogasi, & Gallese, 2001). More
recently, a broader mirror neuron network comprising multiple areas in the frontal, parietal, and temporal cortices has been proposed
to underlie several other social-cognitive and emotional skills including theory of mind (Hamilton & Grafton, 2006; Frith & Frith,
1999; Iacoboni et al., 2005), empathy (Oberman & Ramachandran, 2007; Dapretto et al., 2006), interpersonal synchrony
(Vicaria & Dickens, 2016), language (Gallese, 2008), and intentional understanding (Iacoboni et al., 2005). Dysfunction in the MNS
has therefore been linked directly to dyspraxia as well as broader deficits in social communication and emotional skills in ASD
(Dowell et al., 2009; Hamilton, 2013).

1.5. Relationship between motor measures, intelligence, and autism severity

An area of considerable debate in the ASD literature is the relationship between motor skills, intellectual abilities, and measures of
autism severity. From a neuroscience perspective, significant correlations between cognitive and motor abilities may point towards
impaired brain systems that contribute to both motor and cognitive functioning in individuals with ASD (Green et al., 2009). Al-
ternatively, the presence of motor deficits even after controlling for IQ levels may shed light on motor impairments that are specific to
the ASD diagnosis and are considered true deficits independent of task understanding (Whyatt and Craig, 2013a, 2013b). Around
70–75% of children with ASD demonstrate co-occurring moderate to severe intellectual disability (Charman et al., 2011). Several
studies assessing motor performance in ASD used subjects with intelligence quotient (IQ) scores above 70 (Green et al., 2002; Hilton
et al., 2007; Jansiewicz et al., 2006; Miyahara et al., 1997; Whyatt & Craig, 2012). However, this amounts to selective sampling of a
highly heterogeneous population and does not provide a holistic overview of the range of impairments observed across the autism
spectrum. Out of the studies that systematically assessed the impact of IQ on motor skills in ASD, some studies found a significant
positive correlation between IQ and motor function (Ghaziuddin & Butler, 1998; Green et al., 2009; Manjiviona & Prior, 1995; Mari
et al., 2003; Papadopoulos, McGinley, Tonge, Bradshaw, Saunders, & Rinehart, 2012), whereas other studies found motor impair-
ments in individuals with ASD regardless of their IQ scores (Dziuk et al., 2007; Mostofsky et al., 2006; Vanvuchelen et al., 2007;
Barbeau et al., 2015). Fournier et al. (2010) in their meta-analysis of motor coordination deficits in this population also acknowledged
this lack of consensus pertaining to the association between motor and cognitive skills in ASD. Moreover, an added limitation is that a
majority of the above-mentioned studies assessed the association between motor and cognitive skills in ASD using a single test of
motor performance.
In terms of the relation between motor skills and measures of autism severity, multiple studies have reported a correlation
between praxis performance and autism severity (Dziuk et al., 2007; Ham et al., 2011; Gizzonio et al., 2015). Dziuk et al., 2007 found
that overall praxis performance was significantly correlated with total scores on the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, a
standardized measure for autism severity. Furthermore, praxis errors were also correlated with individual components of the ADOS
including the reciprocal social interaction, communication, and stereotyped/repetitive behaviors subdomains (Dziuk et al., 2007).
However, there is currently limited knowledge on how other aspects of motor functioning (including imitation, praxis, gross and fine
motor function, and interpersonal synchrony) in children with ASD relate to autism severity.

81
M. Kaur et al. Research in Developmental Disabilities 72 (2018) 79–95

Table 1
Participant demographics.

Group Age (years) (Mean ± SE) N, F:M ABIQ SES Ethnicity ADOS-2 Comparison Scores

TD 7.75 ± 0.55 12, 3:9 NA 59.38 ± 2.08 7C, 5A NA


HASD 7.44 ± 0.57 12, 0:12 115.86 ± 17.03 51.17 ± 2.98 10C, 2O 7.91 ± 1.76
LASD 8.74 ± 0.59 12, 2:10 60.07 ± 9.50 47.46 ± 2.97 6C, 5 AA, 1 A 9.27 ± 1.10
p-value (t-test/chi square)
TD vs. HASD ns 0.05 NA 0.04 NA
TD vs. LASD ns ns NA < 0.01 NA
HASD vs. LASD ns ns < 0.01 ns 0.04

N, number of participants; F:M, number of females and males; ABIQ: Abbreviated IQ; SES, Socio Economic Status; NA, Not Assessed; C, Caucasian; AA, African
American; A, Asian; O, Others; ns, not significant with p > 0.05.

1.6. Aims of current study

In the current study, we used a variety of measures for the comprehensive profiling of the motor system in children with ASD. As
discussed in the previous sections, few studies to date have holistically assessed motor function in ASD using both standardized
assessments and experimental tasks (Barbeau et al., 2015; Whyatt and Craig, 2013a, 2013b). We used a battery of standardized tests
and behavioral paradigms to assess gross and fine motor performance, praxis, bilateral motor coordination including solo and social
motor coordination, as well as interpersonal/social synchrony in school-age children with ASD. To understand the relationship
between motor skills and cognitive abilities, we included three groups of children between 5 and 12 years of age − children with ASD
with low IQ scores (IQ ≤ 70) (LASD group), children with ASD with high IQ scores (IQ > 70) (HASD group), and TD children. In
addition, we also correlated motor performance with IQ and ADOS scores in the ASD group. We hypothesized that both the ASD
groups would receive lower scores than the TD group on all the tested motor measures. In line with previous evidence, we hy-
pothesized that in children with ASD, overall gross and fine motor performance may correlate with IQ scores whereas praxis scores
may correlate with ADOS scores.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

The sample reported in the current study was part of a larger randomized controlled trial (RCT) conducted in children with ASD to
examine the effects of novel movement-based interventions in ASD (Srinivasan, Eigsti, Gifford, & Bhat, 2016; Srinivasan, Eigsti,
Neelly, & Bhat, 2016; Srinivasan, Kaur, et al., 2015; Srinivasan, Park, Neelly, & Bhat, 2015). Twenty-four children with ASD between
5 and 12 years of age were included in the current study, along with a convenience sample of 12 age-matched TD children (Table 1).
The participant demographics including age, gender, social economic status (SES), and ethnicity are provided in Table 1. We cal-
culated SES scores using the Hollingshead scale (Hollingshead, 1975). As seen in Table 1, the groups were age-matched but differed in
terms of gender and SES scores. Both ASD groups had a greater proportion of males compared to females, which is not surprising
given the higher prevalence of ASD in males, 4.5 times than females (Christensen et al., 2016). Furthermore, both ASD groups had
lower average SES scores compared to TD families perhaps due to the financial burden imposed on families by the diagnosis
(Montes & Halterman, 2008).
All parents were interviewed using a screening questionnaire to collect information about the child’s birth, development, and
medical history. All TD children in this sample were studying in public or private schools in grade levels appropriate for their age
(i.e., they were not delayed in their classroom placements), had no special needs, and were not receiving any special services at the
time. In addition, the TD group had no significant birth history, family history of ASD, or a history of developmental delays. Further,
none of the participating children had any other genetic/neurological/orthopedic disorders, or any vision and hearing impairments
that could have restricted study participation. Children were recruited through local day care centers, autism service providers such
as clinics and schools, web postings, and by word of mouth. Parents signed the informed consent approved by the University of
Connecticut Institutional Review Board before the children participated in the study and children above 7 years of age also provided
assent to participation.

2.2. Materials and procedures

We videotaped all children during two testing visits each lasting for around 45 minutes to 1 hour. Trained and experienced
doctoral students with a background in physical therapy conducted both sessions with training and supervision from the last author.
During the first visit, children performed the gross and fine motor items of the BOT-2. During the second visit, children completed the
Bilateral Motor Coordination subtest of the SIPT (SIPT-BMC) and an experimental paradigm assessing bilateral coordination and
interpersonal synchrony. For children with ASD only, a research-reliable clinical psychology graduate student conducted a third visit
to confirm the diagnosis of the child and to obtain their IQ scores. We used various strategies throughout the sessions to ensure task
comprehension especially in children with ASD, including visual demonstration of the activity by the tester, use of simple instructions

82
M. Kaur et al. Research in Developmental Disabilities 72 (2018) 79–95

Fig. 1. A typically developing child performing (A) gross motor and fine motor actions from the standardized motor test, BOT-2, (B) action sequences from the
Bilateral Motor Coordination subtest of the SIPT, and (C) simple and complex clap, march, march clap, and drum actions in solo and social contexts.

such as “can you do this?”, use of pictures to illustrate the activity, as well as provision of a practice trial with manual feedback, if
required.

2.2.1. Bruininks-Oseretsky test of motor proficiency- 2nd edition (BOT-2, Fig. 1a)
BOT-2 is a reliable and valid measure of fine and gross motor performance in individuals between 4 and 21 years of age
(Bruininks & Bruininks, 2005). We used five of the BOT-2 subtests to assess gross and fine motor performance in the children. The
balance and bilateral coordination subtests were administered to get the composite score for gross motor control (i.e. body co-
ordination score). The fine motor precision and fine motor integration subtests were administered to get the composite score for fine
motor control (i.e. fine manual score). In addition, we also included the manual dexterity subtest. Finally, we used the short form of
the BOT-2 (BOT-SF) to get a comprehensive estimate of overall motor performance in our sample. We did not include the strength,
running & agility, and upper-limb coordination subtests due to time constraints and as they did not directly contribute to the cal-
culation of the gross and fine motor composites within the scoring system of the BOT-2. We are reporting standard scores for the BOT-
SF, body coordination composite, and fine manual control composite, as well as scaled scores for the manual dexterity subtest.

2.2.2. Bilateral motor coordination subtest of the sensory integration and praxis test (SIPT-BMC, Fig. 1b)
SIPT is a comprehensive assessment tool for evaluating praxis and sensory integration in children between 4 and 8.11 years of age
(Ayres, 1988). We used the bilateral motor coordination subtest of the SIPT (SIPT-BMC), which includes 22 action sequences in-
volving either the upper limbs or the lower limbs. Out of these 22 action sequences, 14 actions involve the upper limbs whereas 8
actions involve the lower limbs. Some exemplar actions include alternate tapping of hands on the thighs, hand tapping followed by
clapping, alternate leg stomping, alternate foot tapping, etc. The tester and the child sat across each other and the child was in-
structed to copy and mirror the tester’s actions i.e., if the adult used the right hand, the child was asked to use the left hand. A practice
trial was completed for the first action so that the children understood the “mirroring” principle. We manually coded children’s
videos in real time for various spatio-temporal errors, including rhythmicity, mirroring, and overflow based on the error classification

Table 2
Praxis error classification for the SIPT-BMC actions.

Error type Definition

Spatial Mirroring Inability to mirror the tester’s actions (ideally, child should use right hand/leg if the tester uses left hand/leg)
Overflow Additional movements towards the end of the action sequence beyond what the tester performed
Total Summation of rhythmicity, mirroring, and overflow errors
Temporal Rhythmicity Inability to imitate rhythm of the action sequence
Time to completion Time in seconds required to complete the action sequence from start to finish

83
M. Kaur et al. Research in Developmental Disabilities 72 (2018) 79–95

proposed by Dewey (1993), Dewey et al. (2007) and Dziuk et al. (2007) (see Table 2). Physical therapy doctoral students trained in
the administration of the praxis subtests coded recorded videos of children from the testing visit for movement errors mentioned
above. Each child received a score of 0 (no error) or 1 (error) based on the absence/presence of an error for each action sequence. We
then summed the child’s scores across all 22 action sequences for each error type to calculate the rhythmicity, mirroring, and
overflow error score. In addition, we summed scores on all error types across all actions to obtain a total error score. Lastly, time to
completion was recorded for each action sequence using a stopwatch. We calculated an average time to completion measure across all
individual action sequences.

2.2.3. Motor coordination experimental paradigm (Fig. 1c)


During the second visit, children performed various simple and complex rhythmic actions requiring coordination of the two sides
of the body in 2 contexts − (1) solo context where children moved on their own while synchronizing to a metronome beat, and (2)
social context where children were asked to synchronize their actions with those of an adult tester while following the metronome
beat. In both contexts, children performed test actions including clapping, marching, marching and clapping, and drumming. We
deliberately chose a combination of dual and multilimb, symmetrical and asymmetrical actions to capture the range of movements
children perform every day. In addition, these actions have been used in similar work conducted in TD children and children with
ASD (Getchell, 2006, 2007; Getchell and Whitall, 2003; Isenhower et al., 2012; Kaur et al., 2013). We ensured that the test actions
included both upper and lower limb actions of varying complexity in terms of bilateral coordination. For instance, although all test
actions were bilateral in nature, drumming, clapping, and marching were dual limb movements whereas the march-clap action was a
multi-limb movement. Similarly, clapping requires symmetrical movements of both upper limbs, whereas the other 3 test actions
involved asymmetrical movements of upper and/or lower limbs. The drumming test involved two actions: simple drumming that
involved an alternate drumming pattern where a hit with one hand was followed by a hit with the other hand, and complex
drumming involving an asymmetrical quarter-eighth or 2:1 pattern where 2 hits were performed by 1 hand followed by 1 hit by the
other hand. For the multi-limb march-clap action, we separately analyzed the clap and march components and termed them complex
clap and complex march (as opposed to the simple clapping and simple marching mentioned above). Overall, for the purpose of
analysis we had 6 different actions in each context − (1) simple clapping, (2) complex clapping, (3) simple marching, (4) complex
marching, (5) simple drumming, and (6) complex drumming. Movement speeds were controlled using a metronome beat of 120
beats/minute.
The order of actions was fixed across all children: clapping, followed by marching, then march-clap, then alternate drumming,
and lastly the quarter-eighth drumming pattern. These actions were first conducted in the solo context followed by the social context.
For each action, the tester first demonstrated the action, and then allowed the child a brief practice bout of 10 seconds, followed by
the test trial for that action. For each activity, we collected a single trial of 50 seconds. A short break was provided after completion of
all solo trials before the social portion of the test.
We recorded video data from children as they performed the coordination test paradigm. We completed second-to-second coding
of the video data using open source Datavyu coding software (www.datavyu.org). Specifically, we coded the time frame for each
event i.e. clap (contact between hands), march (right and left stomp), and drum hit (right and left hand hit) within a trial. The coded
files were run through a custom-developed MATLAB program to calculate inter-event duration in seconds i.e., the time between two
successive claps, stomps or drum hits. Next, we calculated the Coefficient of Variation (CV) of the inter-event duration to obtain a
variability measure for each action, with lower CV indicating greater consistency in movement and hence, better performance. We
calculated Movement Rates as the number of events per second for each action. As mentioned above, our metronome beat was set to
120 beats/min. Therefore, if the child was perfectly synchronizing to the metronome beat then he/she would move at the rate of two
movements per second or 100 movements in each 50-second trial, except for the drumming actions. In case of the drumming actions,
we evaluated the right-hand hits only; hence, perfect synchronization to the metronome beat would result in one hit per second or 50
hits per trial. Lastly, we coded interpersonal synchrony for the social context of the rhythmic actions, i.e. the percentage of time the
child perfectly synchronized his/her actions with the tester (in synchrony) or not (out of synchrony). This was coded by tagging the
start and end time frames of “in-synchrony” periods with remaining seconds in the trial considered “out-of-synchrony”.

2.2.4. Autism diagnosis observation schedule − 2nd edition (ADOS-2)


The ADOS-2 is a gold standard assessment to confirm autism diagnosis of children (Lord et al., 2012). It involves semi-structured
play-based activities for assessing social interactions/affect and repetitive/restricted behaviors (RRB) of children. The scoring of the
ADOS-2 provides a total score and a comparison score ranging from 1–10 to gauge the severity of the disorder, with higher scores
indicating greater severity. Autism diagnosis was confirmed for all 24 children with ASD. Details of ADOS comparison scores in the 2
ASD groups are provided in Table 1.

2.2.5. Stanford binet intelligence scales − 5th edition (SBIQ)


We obtained the Intelligence Quotient (IQ) scores using the SBIQ (Becker, 2003) in the ASD group. The SBIQ is a standardized test
to measure non-verbal and verbal cognitive skills of individuals between 2 and 90 years of age on five different domains − fluid
reasoning, knowledge base, quantitative reasoning, visual-spatial skills, and working memory. For the purpose of this study, we used
the abbreviated IQ score, which is a quick IQ measure that can be calculated using the two routing subtests of the SBIQ (non-verbal
fluid reasoning and verbal knowledge). Based on the abbreviated IQ scores of children with ASD, we divided them into two groups: a)
children with ASD with average to high IQ scores (HASD), i.e. IQ > 70 and b) children with ASD with low IQ scores (LASD), i.e.
IQ ≤ 70 (Table 1).

84
M. Kaur et al. Research in Developmental Disabilities 72 (2018) 79–95

Table 3
Results of the Intraclass Correlation Coefficients (ICC) used for calculating reliability of coding for dependent variables.

Subtest Inter-rater ICC Intra-rater ICC

BOT-2 Gross motor 0.99 0.99


Fine motor 0.98 0.98
SIPT-BMC Rhythmicity 0.96 0.97
Mirroring 0.99 0.97
Overflow 0.80 0.95
Time 0.80 0.99
Variability, Rate Clap 0.99 0.99
March 0.92 0.99
Drum 0.98 0.99
IPS Clap 0.97 0.99
March 0.96 0.93
Drum 0.84 0.99

2.3. Statistical analysis

To establish intra-rater and inter-rater reliability among the coders, we calculated Intra-class correlations coefficients (ICC) using
25% of the dataset for each of the testing measures. The ICC values were > 93% for the intra-rater and > 80% for the inter-rater
comparisons (Table 3). Since our data did not satisfy the assumptions of parametric statistics, we used non-parametric Kruskal-Wallis
tests to compare the dependent variables across the three groups. In case of a significant Kruskal-Wallis test, we further used Mann-
Whitney U tests to identify the specific groups that differed significantly from each other on our dependent variables. We also
conducted pairwise Pearson’s correlations to examine the relationship between IQ, autism severity, and all the motor measures.
Significance level was set at p ≤ 0.05.

3. Results

3.1. BOT-2

The Kruskal-Wallis test revealed a significant group effect for the BOT-SF, body coordination composite, fine manual composite,
and manual dexterity subtest scores (Table 4). The post-hoc Mann-Whitney U tests indicated that both the ASD groups scored lower
than the TD group on all the BOT-2 outcome measures (p values < 0.006, see Fig. 2). In terms of differences between the two ASD
groups, the LASD group received lower scores than the HASD group on the BOT-SF and body coordination subtests(p values ≤ 0.004).
Both groups demonstrated equally poor performance on the fine manual composite and manual dexterity subtest (see Fig. 2).

3.2. SIPT-BMC

The Kruskal Wallis test indicated a significant group effect for the rhythmicity, mirroring, overflow, and total error scores as well
as for the time to completion variable (Table 4). Post-hoc Mann-Whitney-U tests suggested that the LASD group had significantly
greater rhythmicity, mirroring, overflow, and total errors compared to the TD group (p = 0.001 to 0.006), whereas the HASD group
only showed greater overflow errors compared to the TD group (p = 0.01, Fig. 3). Additionally, the two ASD groups differed in terms
of mirroring errors (p = 0.01) and total errors (p = 0.03) with greater errors in the LASD compared to the HASD group (see Fig. 3).
Lastly, both the LASD and the HASD groups took longer to complete the action sequences compared to the TD group (p-values <
0.001, LASD = 5.42 ± 0.33 s; HASD = 4.54 ± 0.24 s; TD = 3.39 ± 0.17 s), with the LASD group also demonstrating a trend for
slower movements compared to the HASD group (p = 0.05).

3.3. Movement variability (CV)

The Kruskal-Wallis tests showed a significant group effect for 9 out of 12 actions (social context of simple clap, solo and social
contexts of complex clap, simple march, complex march, and simple drum), and no differences for the remaining 3 actions (solo
context of simple clap, solo and social contexts of complex drum, see Table 4). The subsequent Mann Whitney U tests indicated that
both the LASD and the HASD groups had greater movement variability compared to the TD group for all the aforementioned actions,
except for the simple drum action in the solo context, for which only the HASD group had greater movement variability compared to
the TD group (p-values = 0.001 − 0.05, Fig. 4). There were no significant differences in movement variability between the two ASD
groups for any of the actions.

3.4. Movement rate

The Kruskal-Wallis tests revealed a significant group effect for 8 actions (solo and social contexts of simple clap, complex clap,
simple march, and complex march), and no differences for 4 actions (solo and social contexts of simple drum and complex drum;

85
M. Kaur et al. Research in Developmental Disabilities 72 (2018) 79–95

Table 4
Results of the Kruskal-Wallis Tests.

Variable χ2 df p-values

BOT-2 BOTSF 21.76 2 < 0.001


Body coordination 23.67 2 < 0.001
Fine manual 19.93 2 < 0.001
Manual Dexterity 21.10 2 < 0.001
SIPT-BMC Rhythmicity 8.13 2 0.017
Mirroring 12.25 2 0.002
Overflow 10.33 2 0.006
Total Error 12.05 2 0.002
Time to Completion 18.17 2 < 0.001
Movement Variability Simple clap- Solo 5.73 2 ns
Complex clap- Solo 9.75 2 0.008
Simple march- Solo 12.52 2 0.002
Complex march- Solo 14.12 2 0.001
Simple drum- Solo 7.65 2 0.02
Complex drum- Solo 0.64 2 ns
Simple clap- Social 16.72 2 < 0.001
Complex clap- Social 11.52 2 0.003
Simple march- Social 13.36 2 0.001
Complex march- Social 14.36 2 0.001
Simple drum- Social 16.80 2 < 0.001
Complex drum- Social 1.99 2 ns
Movement Rate Simple clap- Solo 6.55 2 0.04
Complex clap- Solo 7.22 2 0.03
Simple march- Solo 12.99 2 0.002
Complex march- Solo 5.85 2 0.05
Simple drum- Solo 0.72 2 ns
Complex drum- Solo 3.71 2 ns
Simple clap- Social 10.23 2 0.006
Complex clap- Social 14.58 2 0.001
Simple march- Social 10.97 2 0.004
Complex march- Social 11.23 2 0.004
Simple drum- Social 2.81 2 ns
Complex drum- Social 0.08 2 ns
Interpersonal synchrony Simple clap 11.16 2 0.004
Complex clap 13.77 2 0.001
Simple march 13.43 2 0.001
Complex march 13.35 2 0.001
Simple drum 9.13 2 0.01
Complex drum 9.26 2 0.01

χ2, Chi-square; df, degree of freedom; ns, not significant with p > 0.05

Table 4). The post-hoc Mann-Whitney U tests indicated that the LASD group had slower movement rates compared to the TD group
for all 8 actions listed above. The HASD group had slower movement rates for only 3 actions − solo context of complex clap and
social contexts of simple and complex march actions (p values = 0.001 to 0.05, see Fig. 5). Additionally, the LASD group had a slower
movement rate compared to the HASD group for only 1 action − solo context of the simple clap action (p = 0.02).

3.5. Interpersonal synchrony (IPS)

The Kruskal-Wallis tests revealed a significant group effect for all actions (Table 4). The post-hoc Mann Whitney-U tests indicated
that both the LASD and the HASD groups spent less time in-synchrony with their adult partner compared to the TD group for all
actions (LASD: p values = 0.001 to 0.01; HASD: p values = 0.001 to 0.04) (see Fig. 6). There were no significant differences between
the two ASD groups on this dependent variable.

3.6. Correlating IQ and ADOS with motor measures in ASD

IQ significantly correlated with BOT-2 measures (Table 5, r = 0.45–0.65, p values = 0.03 to 0.0009). Note that IQ did not cor-
relate with any other measures of praxis, movement variability, movement rate, or IPS. ADOS scores significantly correlated with
praxis errors including total errors (Table 5, r = 0.54 to 0.61, p values = 0.002 to 0.009), overflow errors (Table 5, r = 0.59 to 0.69, p
values = 0.004 to 0.0004), and rhythmicity errors (Table 5, r = 0.39–0.49, p values = 0.02 to 0.05). ADOS scores also correlated with
the time taken to complete the action sequences in the SIPT-BMC test (r = 0.46, p = 0.03). Additionally, ADOS scores also correlated
with movement variability during solo march-clap and social simple drum motions (Table 5, r = 0.44–0.51, p values = 0.02 to 0.04).
No other significant correlations were found between motor skills, cognitive skills, and measures of autism severity.

86
M. Kaur et al. Research in Developmental Disabilities 72 (2018) 79–95

Fig. 2. Standard/scaled scores for BOT-2 subtests in the TD, HASD, and LASD groups.
Note: BOT-2 standard scores have a mean of 50 and standard deviation of 10. BOT-2 scaled scores have a mean of 15 and standard deviation of 5.

Fig. 3. Praxis errors for SIPT-BMC in the TD, HASD, and LASD groups.

4. Discussion

Our study suggests that regardless of the cognitive status of children, motor dysfunction is pervasive in children with ASD.
Compared to TD children, children with ASD have universal difficulties in several aspects of motor function including − (1) gross and
fine motor performance, (2) certain aspects of praxis during performance of sequential, imitation-based tasks, (3) simultaneous
coordination of the two sides of the body during rhythmic upper and lower-limb tasks, as well as (4) social-motor coordination and
interpersonal synchrony. Nevertheless, standardized gross and fine motor measures correlated significantly with IQ and not ADOS
scores. In contrast, praxis errors and time taken to complete action sequences strongly correlated with ADOS scores but not IQ.
Certain movement variability measures correlated with the ADOS scores but none of the other motor measures correlated with ADOS
or IQ. In the next few sections, we discuss the possible reasons underlying our findings and their implications with respect to our
current understanding of ASD and potential targets for intervention in this disorder.

87
M. Kaur et al. Research in Developmental Disabilities 72 (2018) 79–95

Fig. 4. Movement variability or Coefficient of Variation for the rhythmic actions in the TD, HASD, and LASD groups.

4.1. Gross and fine motor performance

On the standardized BOT-2 measure, both the HASD and LASD groups had significantly lower scores in terms of their gross and
fine motor skills compared to the TD group. Our findings fit with previous work in this area by other research groups (Ament et al.,
2015; Barbeau et al., 2015; Green et al., 2009; Jansiewicz et al., 2006; Liu and Breslin, 2013; Provost, Heimerl et al., 2007; Provost,
Lopez et al., 2007; Whyatt and Craig, 2012, 2013a, 2013b). In general, the LASD group received lower scores than the HASD group on
the BOT-2. We also observed a strong relationship between IQ and gross/fine motor performance measures. However, group dif-
ferences between the HASD and LASD group were only significant for the body coordination composite and not for the fine manual
composite or the manual dexterity subtests of the BOT-2. While we think these results should be further replicated in future studies,
this difference could be explained by the extensive neuropathology in the LASD group and/or the possible gross motor gains with
development specifically occurring in the HASD group. Nevertheless, we want to emphasize that motor dysfunction existed across the
autism spectrum (i.e., for all motor scores, both HASD & LASD < TD) regardless of children’s cognitive abilities. Our findings align
with those of Smits-Englesman and colleagues who report that although cognitive and motor skills are inter-related possibly through
common underlying neural substrates, cognitive skills only account for 19% of the variance in motor skills in TD children and
children with developmental motor difficulties (Smits-Engelsman & Hill, 2012).

4.2. Praxis and imitation skills

Compared to the TD group, we found greater praxis errors and longer completion times in both HASD and LASD groups. Our
findings replicate the results of other studies that also report praxis impairments in children with ASD (Dewey et al., 2007; Dowell
et al., 2009; Gizzonio et al., 2015). From a neuroscience perspective, as discussed in the introduction, deficits in the bilaterally
distributed praxis network and poor long- range connectivity between the visual and motor areas of the brain have been thought to
underlie the imitation and praxis impairments in ASD (Nebel et al., 2016; Mahajan et al., 2016; Mostofsky & Ewen, 2011; Haswell,
Izawa, Dowell, Mostofsky, & Shadmehr, 2009). We also found a strong correlation between ADOS scores and both praxis errors (total,
overflow, rhythmicity) and time taken to complete actions in the ASD group, although no such relationships existed between praxis
measures and IQ scores. Several other groups have also demonstrated that praxis performance of children with ASD is correlated with
overall autism severity but not with intelligence (Bhat et al., 2016; Dowell et al., 2009; Dziuk et al., 2007; Gizzonio et al., 2015).
Overall, given this consistent evidence for the specificity of praxis impairments in ASD, we recommend that dyspraxia be considered a

88
M. Kaur et al. Research in Developmental Disabilities 72 (2018) 79–95

Fig. 5. Movement rates for the rhythmic actions in the TD, HASD, and LASD groups.

defining feature of ASD.


Similar to past studies, we found that children with ASD universally took longer to complete rhythmic action sequences compared
to the TD group (Green et al., 2009; Biscaldi et al., 2014; Isenhower et al., 2012; Jansiewicz et al., 2006). Children may have
intentionally slowed their actions within the action sequence. This might have been a trade-off used by children to maintain accuracy
in complex praxis tasks that require both speed and accuracy (Whyatt and Criag, 2012; Von Hofsten, 2007). Furthermore, there was
also a trend for slower movement times in the LASD group compared to the HASD group. On a related note, we found that the LASD
group made greater rhythmicity errors compared to the HASD and TD groups. Perhaps the movement slowing helped the HASD group
reduce their rhythmicity errors and improve accuracy of imitation of the movement pattern, but the LASD group continued to show
these errors despite movement slowing. In fact, movement slowing is a common compensatory strategy shown by patients with
incoordination following cerebellar disease (Bastian, Martin, Keating, & Thach, 1996). In individuals with ASD, Mari and colleagues
have similarly reported significant slowing of reach-to-grasp movements in a ‘low-ability’ (IQ: 70–79) group compared to the
‘average-ability’ and ‘high-ability’ groups (IQ: 80–109) (Mari et al., 2003).
Both ASD groups demonstrated significant overflow errors compared to the TD group. Similarly, Dewey and colleagues found that
even after controlling for IQ, children with ASD made greater overflow errors compared to TD children and children with devel-
opmental coordination disorder (DCD) (Dewey et al., 2007). Overflow errors have been attributed to poor inhibitory control of
neurons within the cerebral cortex as well as a delayed functional segregation of upper and lower limb areas within the primary
motor cortex in individuals with ASD (Barber, Wetherby, & Chambers, 2012; Mostofsky, Newschaffer, & Denckla, 2003; Nebel et al.,
2014). Lastly, consistent with the limited literature on this topic, we found that the HASD group did not demonstrate any significant
differences in mirroring compared to the TD group (Carmo et al., 2013; Vanvuchelen et al., 2007; Smith & Bryson, 1994). However,
our findings of significant mirroring errors in the LASD group compared to the TD group need to be replicated and investigated
further.

4.3. Bilateral coordination and social synchrony skills

We found that compared to the TD group, the LASD group had more variable and slower movements for all types of rhythmic
actions (except drumming) performed alone and with a social partner. Although the HASD group in general had more variable
movements (except drumming) compared to the TD group, they had slower movement rates only for few actions (complex clap in the
solo context and the simple and complex march actions in the social context). The lack of any group differences for drumming actions

89
M. Kaur et al. Research in Developmental Disabilities 72 (2018) 79–95

Fig. 6. Interpersonal synchrony during the social context of rhythmic actions in the TD, HASD, and LASD groups.

could be possibly because even TD children found the drumming actions hard; in fact, the complex drumming action was the most
variable across all three groups (see Fig. 4). Past studies have also reported greater movement variability in individuals with ASD
during rhythmic upper- and lower-limb tasks such as drumming, maraca shaking, and walking (Fleury, Kushki, Tanel,
Anagnostou, & Chau, 2013; Isenhower et al., 2012; Kaur et al., 2013; Nayate et al., 2012) as well as during discrete discontinuous
actions (Fleury et al., 2013). Interestingly, Fleury et al. (2013) found that children with ASD demonstrated greater movement
variability during discontinuous/discrete circle drawing but not during continuous circle drawing. In contrast, we found significant
movement impairments during continuous rhythmic actions. These differences in findings may be due to differences in taskdemands
across the two studies. We asked children to perform whole body gross motor movements to a metronome beat, whereas Fleury and
colleagues assessed a fine motor action without additional temporal demands of synchronization with an external beat (Fleury et al.,
2013). In terms of movement rates, in line with our findings, slower movement rates have been reported in children and adolescents
with ASD during motor tasks such as drumming and repetitive hand movements (Biscaldi et al., 2014; Isenhower et al., 2012;
Jansiewicz et al., 2006).
We also found that both HASD and LASD groups were equally impaired in synchronizing with their social partners during
rhythmic actions compared to the TD group. The limited literature on interpersonal synchrony lends overall support to our findings
(Marsh et al., 2013; Fitzpatrick et al., 2016, 2017). For instance, Marsh and colleagues reported weaker “in-phase synchrony”
between pairs of ASD children and their caregivers compared to pairs of TD children and their caregivers during a rocking chair
experiment (Marsh et al., 2013). Overall, the findings of this study advance our current understanding of social synchrony in ASD by
providing systematic evidence for the pervasive presence of such deficits in both high- and low-functioning children with ASD.
From a perception-action perspective, children with ASD demonstrate deficits in perception, action, and perception-action cou-
pling. Specifically, children with ASD have poor social attention skills (Dawson, Bernier, & Ring, 2012; Marsh et al., 2013), poor
fundamental movement control skills (Green et al., 2009; Fournier et al., 2010; Kaur et al., 2013), as well as poor visuomotor
coordination skills (Barbeau et al., 2015; Nebel et al., 2016), all of which could contribute to poor bilateral coordination and social
synchrony in ASD. Atypical functional segregation in the primary motor cortex and reduced corpus callosal size may contribute to
poor bilateral coordination skills in ASD (Casanova et al., 2009; Casanova et al., 2011; Nebel et al., 2014). In terms of social
synchrony, as discussed earlier, poor long-range connectivity between the frontal and occipital lobes could contribute to the vi-
suomotor integration difficulties in ASD (Mostofsky, Burgess, & Gidley-Larson, 2007; Courchesne et al., 2011). Moreover, impaired
activation in the “mirror neuron” system may also contribute to the difficulties faced by individuals in synchronizing their actions
with their social partners (Dapretto et al., 2006; Oberman et al., 2005; Théoret et al., 2005; Williams et al., 2006).

90
M. Kaur et al. Research in Developmental Disabilities 72 (2018) 79–95

Table 5
Results of pairwise correlations between IQ, ADOS, and motor measures.

Motor vs IQ, ADOS Pearson's correlation P-value (s)

BOT-BC (GM) v IQ 0.57 0.005*


BOT-FM v IQ 0.54 0.009*
BOT-SF v IQ 0.66 0.0009*
BOT-MD v IQ 0.45 0.03*
BOT v ADOS Ranges from −0.01 to −0.38 all p-values ns
SIPT Total v ADOS Social Affect 0.56 0.006*
SIPT Total v ADOS RRB 0.54 0.009*
SIPT Total v ADOS Total 0.61 0.002*
SIPT Total v ADOS comparison 0.46 0.06|
SIPT Time v ADOS RRB 0.46 0.03*
SIPT Overflow v ADOS Social Affect 0.59 0.004*
SIPT Overflow v ADOS RRB 0.66 0.0009*
SIPT Overflowv ADOS Total 0.69 0.0004*
SIPT Overflow v ADOS Comparison 0.44 0.03*
SIPT Rhythmicity v ADOS Social Affect 0.48 0.02*
SIPTRhythmicity v ADOS RRB 0.39 0.06|
SIPT Rhythmicity v ADOS Total 0.49 0.02*
SIPT Rhythmicity v ADOS Comparison 0.3 ns
SIPT Mirror v ADOS Social Affect 0.38 0.07|
SIPT Mirror v ADOS RRB 0.35 0.1|
SIPT Mirror v ADOS Total 0.4 0.05|
SIPT Mirror v ADOS Comparison 0.2 ns
SIPT scores v IQ Ranges from −0.2 to −0.3 ns
SIPT Time v IQ −0.4 0.05|
CV Solo March-Clap v ADOS Social Affect 0.44 0.04*
CV Social Simple Drum v ADOS Comparison 0.51 0.02*

* = significant difference < 0.05.


| = statistical trend 0.05–0.1.
ns = not significant p > 0.1.

4.4. Clinical implications

Our study findings reinforce and add to the decades of research evidence on the presence of motor impairments across the autism
spectrum. Both high- and low-functioning children with ASD in our study demonstrated consistent impairments in several aspects of
motor function including gross and fine motor performance, praxis/imitation, bilateral coordination, and interpersonal synchrony
skills compared to age-matched TD peers. Moreover, for almost all variables included in this study, we found that both HASD and
LASD groups were equally impaired, with no significant differences between the two ASD groups. Despite irrefutable evidence to date
documenting movement dysfunction, motor symptoms are largely under-recognized in ASD. Our study calls for the recognition of
motor dysfunction as an integral part of the core difficulties faced by individuals with ASD. Furthermore, certain motor impairments
such as dyspraxia must be considered a defining feature of ASD given the repeated evidence for a strong relationship with autism
severity. Children with ASD demonstrate deficits in different aspects of movement control; therefore, from an assessment point-of-
view, we recommend that clinicians evaluate all aspects of motor function in ASD using a comprehensive battery of standardized tests
and behavioral tasks similar to the ones used in this study. Moreover, given the links between motor, social communication, and
cognitive development (Bhat et al., 2011; Dziuk et al., 2007; Fitzpatrick et al., 2016; Nebel et al., 2016), our study emphasizes the
need for inclusion of movement-based interventions into the standard of care for children with ASD with various motor axes as
treatment targets. In addition to the existing focus on promoting fine motor skills important for academic achievement of children
with ASD, clinicians should also encourage gross motor play, imitation-based activities, and cooperative games within structured and
semi-structured contexts. Recently, few studies have documented positive effects on balance, coordination, and flexibility following
engagement in motor activities in children with ASD (Hilton et al., 2014; MacDonald et al., 2012; Pan et al., 2017). Moreover, to
ensure that children find training activities fun and engaging, clinicians could encourage children to participate in creative, alter-
native therapies such as whole-body yoga or music-based rhythmic activities that effectively promote motor and social commu-
nication skills in children with ASD (Srinivasan, Eigsti, Gifford, & Bhat, 2016; Srinivasan, Eigsti, Neelly, & Bhat, 2016; Srinivasan,
Kaur, et al., 2015; Srinivasan, Park, Neelly, & Bhat, 2015; Kaur, 2016).

4.5. Limitations & future directions

Our study had a relatively small sample size; therefore, we did not have the power to systematically assess the effects of con-
founding variables such as age, gender, and socioeconomic status of children on their motor skills. Another limitation of the current
study was the lack of IQ scores for the TD group. However, we confirmed that the TD children in this sample were at or above their
grade level in schools and that their parents did not report any atypical birth/family history or any developmental delays/diagnoses
during the screening interview. Moreover, we only have abbreviated IQ scores from our ASD sample with no further decomposition of

91
M. Kaur et al. Research in Developmental Disabilities 72 (2018) 79–95

cognitive abilities into verbal and non-verbal IQ scores. In the current study, we did not control for order effects of actions during the
experimental motor coordination paradigm and instead asked all children to perform actions in a fixed order. Moreover, although we
tried to comprehensively evaluate motor performance in ASD using a battery of standardized tests and experimental tasks, we
acknowledge the lack of objective kinematic measures of motor performance in this study. We recommend that future studies use
more robust objective tools such as kinematic analyses and dynamical methods to identify subtle impairments in movement control
and quality in children with ASD. Further, given the limited current knowledge regarding the neural substrates underlying different
movement parameters in children with ASD, future studies could employ neuroimaging tools such as functional Magnetic Resonance
Imaging (fMRI) and functional Near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) to identify impairments in structural, functional, and effective
connectivity in individuals with ASD (Nebel et al., 2014; Bhat et al., 2017).

5. Conclusion

We compared motor performance using a comprehensive battery of tests and behavioral paradigms in three groups of children
between 5 and 12 years of age: HASD, LASD, and TD. Regardless of IQ scores, children with ASD had significant impairments in gross
and fine motor performance, praxis, bilateral coordination, and interpersonal synchrony compared to their TD peers. Moreover, on
the majority of our motor variables both, HASD and LASD groups were equally impaired further ascertaining the pervasiveness of
motor dysfunction across the entire autism spectrum. Overall gross and fine motor performance significantly correlated with in-
telligence but not with autism severity. Praxis errors and time to completion strongly correlated with autism severity but not in-
telligence. Lastly, our findings emphasize the need for motor assessments and interventions to promote both motor and social
communication development in children with ASD and to include dyspraxia among the defining features of ASD.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest regarding the presented work.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all the children and families who participated in the study. We thank Dr. Deborah Fein and her students
Lauren Haisley and Dasal Jashar at the University of Connecticut for completing the ADOS and IQ assessments for children with ASD.
We thank Kerry Marsh at the University of Connecticut for sharing her ideas on the assessment of interpersonal synchrony, and
graduate students, Isabel Park and Prajakta Nair at the University of Connecticut for helping with data collections and analyses.
Lastly, we would like to thank the National Institute of Mental Health for the R21MH089441 and R33MH089441 awards, Autism
Speaks for the pilot treatment award # 8137, and the UConn Foundation for their large grant awarded to the last author. The last
author’s writing efforts were supported by a pilot grant (Project PI: Bhat) from an Institutional Development Award (IDeA) from the
National Institute of General Medical Sciences of the National Institutes of Health under grant number U54-GM104941 (Site PI:
Binder-Macleod).

References

Ament, K., Mejia, A., Buhlman, R., Erklin, S., Caffo, B., Mostofsky, S., & Wodka, E. (2015). Evidence for specificity of motor impairments in catching and balance in
children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 742–751.
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.
Ayres, J. (1988). Sensory integration and praxis tests (SIPT). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
Barbeau, E. B., Meilleur, A. A. S., Zeffiro, T. A., & Mottron, L. (2015). Comparing motor skills in autism spectrum individuals with and without speech delay. Autism
Research, 8(6), 682–693.
Barber, A. B., Wetherby, A. M., & Chambers, N. W. (2012). Brief report: Repetitive behaviors in young children with autism spectrum disorder and developmentally
similar peers: A follow up to Watt et al. (2008). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(9), 2006–2012.
Bastian, A. J., Martin, T. A., Keating, J. G., & Thach, W. T. (1996). Cerebellar ataxia: Abnormal control of interaction torques across multiple joints. Journal of
Neurophysiology, 76(1), 492–509.
Becker, K. A. (2003). History of the Stanford-Binet intelligence scales: Content and psychometrics (5th ed.). Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.
Bedford, R., Pickles, A., & Lord, C. (2016). Early gross motor skills predict the subsequent development of language in children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism
Research, 9(9), 993–1001.
Berkeley, S. L., Zittel, L. L., Pitney, L. V., & Nichols, S. E. (2001). Locomotor and object control skills of children diagnosed with autism. Adapted Physical Activity
Quarterly, 18(4), 405–416.
Bhat, A. N., Landa, R. J., & Galloway, J. C. (2011). Current perspectives on motor functioning in infants, children, and adults with autism spectrum disorders. Physical
Therapy, 91, 1116–1129.
Bhat, A. N., Galloway, J. C., & Landa, R. J. (2012). Relation between early motor delay and later communication delay in infants at risk for autism. Infant Behavior and
Development, 35(4), 838–846.
Bhat, A. N., Srinivasan, S., Woxholdt, C., & Shield, A. (2016). Differences in praxis performance and receptive language during fingerspelling between deaf children
with and without Autism Spectrum Disorder. Autism, 1–12.
Bhat, A. N., Hoffman, M. H., Trost, S. L., Culotta, M. L., Eilbott, J., Tsuzuki, D., & Pelphrey, K. A. (2017). Cortical activation during action observation, action
execution, and interpersonal synchrony in adults: A functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, 431.
Biscaldi, M., Rauh, R., Irion, L., Jung, N. H., Mall, V., Fleischhaker, C., & Klein, C. (2014). Deficits in motor abilities and developmental fractionation of imitation
performance in high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 23(7), 599–610.
Bruininks, R. H., & Bruininks, B. D. (2005). Bruininks-Oseretsky test of motor proficiency (2nd ed.). Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson.
Carmo, J. C., Rumiati, R. I., Siugzdaite, R., & Brambilla, P. (2013). Preserved imitation of known gestures in children with high-functioning autism. ISRN
Neurology, 1–8.

92
M. Kaur et al. Research in Developmental Disabilities 72 (2018) 79–95

Casanova, M. F., El-Baz, A., Mott, M., Mannheim, G., Hassan, H., Fahmi, R., & Farag, A. (2009). Reduced gyral window and corpus callosum size in autism: Possible
macroscopic correlates of a minicolumnopathy. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(5), 751–764.
Casanova, M. F., El-Baz, A., Elnakib, A., Switala, A. E., Williams, E. L., Williams, D. L., & Conturo, T. E. (2011). Quantitative analysis of the shape of the corpus
callosum in patients with autism and comparison individuals. Autism, 15(2), 223–238.
Charman, T., Pickles, A., Simonoff, E., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., & Baird, G. (2011). IQ in children with autism spectrum disorders: Data from the Special Needs and
Autism Project (SNAP). Psychological Medicine, 41(03), 619–627.
Christensen, D. L., Baio, J., van Naarden Braun, K., Bilder, D., Charles, J., Constantino, J. N., & Yeargin-Allsopp, M. (2016). Prevalence and characteristics of autism
spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years — autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2012. Surveillance
Summaries, 65(3), 1–23.
Chukoskie, L., Townsend, J., & Westerfield, M. (2013). Motor skill in autism spectrum disorders: a subcortical view. International Review of Neurobiology, 113(7),
207–249.
Courchesne, E., Campbell, K., & Solso, S. (2011). Brain growth across the life span in autism: Age-specific changes in anatomical pathology. Brain Research, 1380,
138–145.
Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Iacoboni, M. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: Mirror neuron
dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9(1), 28–30.
Dawson, G., Bernier, R., & Ring, R. H. (2012). Social attention: A possible early indicator of efficacy in autism clinical trials. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders,
4(1), 11.
Dewey, D., Cantell, M., & Crawford, S. G. (2007). Motor and gestural performance in children with autism spectrum disorders, developmental coordination disorder,
and/or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13(02), 246–256.
Dewey, D. (1993). Error analysis of limb and orofacial praxis in children with developmental motor deficits. Brain and Cognition, 23(2), 203–221.
Dowell, L. R., Mahone, E. M., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2009). Associations of postural knowledge and basic motor skill with dyspraxia in autism: Implication for ab-
normalities in distributed connectivity and motor learning. Neuropsychology, 23(5), 563.
Dziuk, M. A., Larson, J. C., Apostu, A., Mahone, E. M., Denckla, M. B., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2007). Dyspraxia in autism: association with motor, social, and com-
municative deficits. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 49(10), 734–739.
Fitzpatrick, P., Diorio, R., Richardson, M. J., & Schmidt, R. C. (2013). Dynamical methods for evaluating the time-dependent unfolding of social coordination in
children with autism. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 7.
Fitzpatrick, P., Frazier, J. A., Cochran, D. M., Mitchell, T., Coleman, C., & Schmidt, R. C. (2016). Impairments of social motor synchrony evident in autism spectrum
disorder. Frontiers in Psychology, 7.
Fitzpatrick, P., Romero, V., Amaral, J. L., Duncan, A., Barnard, H., Richardson, M. J., ... Schmidt, R. C. (2017). Social motor synchronization: Insights for understanding
social behavior in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 1–16.
Flanagan, J. E., Landa, R., Bhat, A., & Bauman, M. (2012). Head lag in infants at risk for autism: a preliminary study. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 66(5),
577–585.
Fleury, A., Kushki, A., Tanel, N., Anagnostou, E., & Chau, T. (2013). Statistical persistence and timing characteristics of repetitive circle drawing in children with ASD.
Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 16(4), 245–254.
Fournier, K. A., Hass, C. J., Naik, S. K., Lodha, N., & Cauraugh, J. H. (2010). Motor coordination in autism spectrum disorders: A synthesis and meta-analysis. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(10), 1227–1240.
Freitag, C. M., Kleser, C., Schneider, M., & von Gontard, A. (2007). Quantitative assessment of neuromotor function in adolescents with high functioning autism and
Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(5), 948–959.
Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (1999). Interacting minds–a biological basis. Science, 286(5445), 1692–1695.
Fuentes, C. T., Mostofsky, S. H., & Bastian, A. J. (2009). Children with autism show specific handwriting impairments. Neurology, 73(19), 1532–1537.
Gallese, V. (2008). Mirror neurons and the social nature of language: The neural exploitation hypothesis. Social Neuroscience, 3(3-4), 317–333.
Getchell, N., & Whitall, J. (2003). How do children coordinate simultaneous upper and lower extremity tasks? The development of dual motor task coordination.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 85(2), 120–140.
Getchell, N. (2006). Age and task-related differences in timing stability, consistency, and natural frequency of children's rhythmic, motor coordination. Developmental
Psychobiology, 48(8), 675–685.
Getchell, N. (2007). Developmental aspects of perception-action coupling in multi-limb coordination: rhythmic sensorimotor synchronization. Motor Control, 11(1), 1.
Ghaziuddin, M., & Butler, E. (1998). Clumsiness in autism and Asperger syndrome: A further report. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 42(1), 43–48.
Gizzonio, V., Avanzini, P., Campi, C., Orivoli, S., Piccolo, B., Cantalupo, G., & Fabbri-Destro, M. (2015). Failure in pantomime action execution correlates with the
severity of social behavior deficits in children with autism: A praxis study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(10), 3085–3097.
Green, D., Baird, G., Barnett, A. L., Henderson, L., Huber, J., & Henderson, S. E. (2002). The severity and nature of motor impairment in Asperger's syndrome: A
comparison with specific developmental disorder of motor function. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(5), 655–668.
Green, D., Charman, T., Pickles, A., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., Simonoff, E., & Baird, G. (2009). Impairment in movement skills of children with autistic spectrum
disorders. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 51(4), 311–316.
Hallett, M., Lebiedowska, M. K., Thomas, S. L., Stanhope, S. J., Denckla, M. B., & Rumsey, J. (1993). Locomotion of autistic adults. Archives of Neurology, 50(12),
1304–1308.
Ham, H. S., Bartolo, A., Corley, M., Rajendran, G., Szabo, A., & Swanson, S. (2011). Exploring the relationship between gestural recognition and imitation: Evidence of
dyspraxia in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(1), 1–12.
Hamilton, A. F. D. C. (2013). Reflecting on the mirror neuron system in autism: a systematic review of current theories. Developmental cognitive neuroscience, 3, 91–105.
Hamilton, A. F. D. C., & Grafton, S. T. (2006). Goal representation in human anterior intraparietal sulcus. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(4), 1133–1137.
Haswell, C. C., Izawa, J., Dowell, L. R., Mostofsky, S. H., & Shadmehr, R. (2009). Representation of internal models of action in the autistic brain. Nature Neuroscience,
12(8), 970–972.
Hilton, C., Graver, K., & LaVesser, P. (2007). Relationship between social competence and sensory processing in children with high functioning autism spectrum
disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 1(2), 164–173.
Hilton, C. L., Zhang, Y., Whilte, M. R., Klohr, C. L., & Constantino, J. (2012). Motor impairment in sibling pairs concordant and discordant for autism spectrum
disorders. Autism, 16(4), 430–441.
Hilton, C. L., Cumpata, K., Klohr, C., Gaetke, S., Artner, A., Johnson, H., & Dobbs, S. (2014). Effects of exergaming on executive function and motor skills in children
with autism spectrum disorder: A pilot study. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 68(1), 57–65.
Hollingshead, A. (1975). Four factor index of social status. New Haven, CT: Yale University [Unpublished Manuscript].
Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitations. Science, 286(5449),
2526–2528.
Iacoboni, M., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Gallese, V., Buccino, G., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Grasping the intentions of others with one's own mirror neuron
system. PLoS Biology, 3(3), e79.
Isenhower, R. W., Marsh, K. L., Richardson, M. J., Helt, M., Schmidt, R. C., & Fein, D. (2012). Rhythmic bimanual coordination is impaired in young children with
autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6(1), 25–31.
Jansiewicz, E. M., Goldberg, M. C., Newschaffer, C. J., Denckla, M. B., Landa, R., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2006). Motor signs distinguish children with high functioning
autism and Asperger’s syndrome from controls. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(5), 613–621.
Just, M., Keller, T., Malave, V., Kana, R., & Varma, S. (2012). Autism as a neural systems disorder: A theory of frontal-posterior underconnectivity. Neuroscience and
Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(4), 1292–1313.
Kaur, M., Gifford, T., Marsh, K. L., & Bhat, A. (2013). Effect of robot-child interactions on bilateral coordination skills of typically developing children and a child with
autism spectrum disorder: A preliminary study. Journal of Motor Learning and Development, 1(2), 31–37.

93
M. Kaur et al. Research in Developmental Disabilities 72 (2018) 79–95

Kaur, M. (2016). Creative yoga intervention for children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (Doctoral dissertation). University of Delaware.
Koehne, S., Hatri, A., Cacioppo, J. T., & Dziobek, I. (2016). Perceived interpersonal synchrony increases empathy: Insights from autism spectrum disorder. Cognition,
146, 8–15.
Kushki, A., Chau, T., & Anagnostou, E. (2011). Handwriting difficulties in children with autism spectrum disorders: A scoping review. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 41(12), 1706–1716.
Landa, R., & Garrett-Mayer, E. (2006). Development in infants with autism spectrum disorders: A prospective study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(6),
629–638.
Leary, M. R., & Hill, D. A. (1996). Moving on: Autism and movement disturbance. Mental Retardation, 34(1), 39.
Liu, T., & Breslin, C. M. (2013). Fine and gross motor performance of the MABC-2 by children with autism spectrum disorder and typically developing children.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7(10), 1244–1249.
Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. L. (2012). Autism diagnostic observation schedule, (ADOS-2) manual (part 1): Modules 1–4 (2nd ed.).
Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services.
MacDonald, M., Esposito, P., Hauck, J., Jeong, I., Hornyak, J., Argento, A., & Ulrich, D. A. (2012). Bicycle training for youth with Down syndrome and autism spectrum
disorders. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 27(1), 12–21.
MacNeil, L. K., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2012). Specificity of dyspraxia in children with autism. Neuropsychology, 26(2), 165.
Mahajan, R., Dirlikov, B., Crocetti, D., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2016). Motor circuit anatomy in children with autism spectrum disorder with or without attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder. Autism Research, 9(1), 67–81.
Manjiviona, J., & Prior, M. (1995). Comparison of Asperger syndrome and high-functioning autistic children on a test of motor impairment. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 25(1), 23–39.
Mari, M., Castiello, U., Marks, D., Marraffa, C., & Prior, M. (2003). The reach?to?grasp movement in children with autism spectrum disorder. Philosophical Transactions
of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 358(1430), 393–403.
Marsh, K. L., Isenhower, R. W., Richardson, M. J., Helt, M., Verbalis, A. D., Schmidt, R. C., & Fein, D. (2013). Autism and social disconnection in interpersonal rocking.
Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 7.
Mayes, S. D., & Calhoun, S. L. (2003). Ability profiles in children with autism: Influence of age and IQ. Autism, 7(1), 65–80.
McPhillips, M., Finlay, J., Bejerot, S., & Hanley, M. (2014). Motor deficits in children with autism spectrum disorder: a cross-Syndrome study. Autism Research, 7(6),
664–676.
Minshew, N. J., Sung, K., Jones, B. L., & Furman, J. M. (2004). Underdevelopment of the postural control system in autism. Neurology, 63(11), 2056–2061.
Miyahara, M., Tsujii, M., Hori, M., Nakanishi, K., Kageyama, H., & Sugiyama, T. (1997). Brief report: motor incoordination in children with Asperger syndrome and
learning disabilities. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27(5), 595–603.
Montes, G., & Halterman, J. S. (2008). Association of childhood autism spectrum disorders and loss of family income. Pediatrics, 121(4), 821–826.
Mostofsky, S. H., & Ewen, J. B. (2011). Altered connectivity and action model formation in autism is autism. The Neuroscientist, 17(4), 437–448.
Mostofsky, S. H., Newschaffer, C. J., & Denckla, M. B. (2003). Overflow movements predict impaired response inhibition in children with ADHD. Perceptual and Motor
Skills, 97(3 suppl), 1315–1331.
Mostofsky, S. H., Dubey, P., Jerath, V. K., Jansiewicz, E. M., Goldberg, M. C., & Denckla, M. B. (2006). Developmental dyspraxia is not limited to imitation in children
with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12(03), 314–326.
Mostofsky, S. H., Burgess, M. P., & Gidley Larson, J. C. (2007). Increased motor cortex white matter volume predicts motor impairment in autism. Brain, 130(8),
2117–2122.
Mostofsky, S. H., Powell, S. K., Simmonds, D. J., Goldberg, M. C., Caffo, B., & Pekar, J. J. (2009). Decreased connectivity and cerebellar activity in autism during motor
task performance. Brain, 132(9), 2413–2425.
Nayate, A., Tonge, B. J., Bradshaw, J. L., McGinley, J. L., Iansek, R., & Rinehart, N. J. (2012). Differentiation of high-functioning autism and Asperger’s disorder based
on neuromotor behaviour. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(5), 707–717.
Nebel, M. B., Joel, S. E., Muschelli, J., Barber, A. D., Caffo, B. S., Pekar, J. J., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2014). Disruption of functional organization within the primary motor
cortex in children with autism. Human Brain Mapping, 35(2), 567–580.
Nebel, M. B., Eloyan, A., Nettles, C. A., Sweeney, K. L., Ament, K., Ward, R. E., ... Mostofsky, S. H. (2016). Intrinsic visual-motor synchrony correlates with social
deficits in autism. Biological Psychiatry, 79(8), 633–641.
Noterdaeme, M., Mildenberger, K., Minow, F., & Amorosa, H. (2002). Evaluation of neuromotor deficits in children with autism and children with a specific speech and
language disorder. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 11(5), 219–225.
Oberman, L. M., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2007). The simulating social mind: The role of the mirror neuron system and simulation in the social and communicative
deficits of autism spectrum disorders. Psychological Bulletin, 133(2), 310.
Oberman, L. M., Hubbard, E. M., McCleery, J. P., Altschuler, E. L., Ramachandran, V. S., & Pineda, J. A. (2005). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism
spectrum disorders. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(2), 190–198.
Pan, C. Y., Chu, C. H., Tsai, C. L., Sung, M. C., Huang, C. Y., & Ma, W. Y. (2017). The impacts of physical activity intervention on physical and cognitive outcomes in
children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 21(2), 190–202.
Papadopoulos, N., McGinley, J., Tonge, B. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Saunders, K., & Rinehart, N. J. (2012). An investigation of upper limb motor function in high functioning
autism and Asperger's disorder using a repetitive Fitts’ aiming task. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6(1), 286–292.
Provost, B., Heimerl, S., & Lopez, B. R. (2007). Levels of gross and fine motor development in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Physical & Occupational
Therapy in Pediatrics, 27(3), 21–36.
Provost, B., Lopez, B. R., & Heimerl, S. (2007). A comparison of motor delays in young children: autism spectrum disorder, developmental delay, and developmental
concerns. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(2), 321–328.
Rinehart, N. J., Tonge, B. J., Iansek, R., McGinley, J., Brereton, A. V., Enticott, P. G., & Bradshaw, J. L. (2006). Gait function in newly diagnosed children with autism:
Cerebellar and basal ganglia related motor disorder. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 48(10), 819–824.
Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.
Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience,
2(9), 661–670.
Sacrey, L. A. R., Germani, T., Bryson, S. E., & Zwaigenbaum, L. (2014). Reaching and grasping in autism spectrum disorder: A review of recent literature. Frontiers in
Neurology, 5.
Salowitz, N. M., Eccarius, P., Karst, J., Carson, A., Schohl, K., Stevens, S., & Scheidt, R. A. (2013). Brief report: Visuo-spatial guidance of movement during gesture
imitation and mirror drawing in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(4), 985–995.
Smith, I. M., & Bryson, S. E. (1994). Imitation and action in autism: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 116(2), 259–273.
Smits-Engelsman, B., & Hill, E. L. (2012). The relationship between motor coordination and intelligence across the IQ range. Pediatrics, 130(4), e950–e956.
Srinivasan, S. M., Lynch, K. A., Bubela, D. J., Gifford, T. D., & Bhat, A. N. (2013). Effect of interactions between a child and a robot on the imitation and praxis
performance of typically developing children and a child with autism: A preliminary study. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 116(3), 885–904.
Srinivasan, S. M., Eigsti, I. M., Gifford, T., & Bhat, A. N. (2016). The effects of embodied rhythm and robotic interventions on the spontaneous and responsive verbal
communication skills of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD): A further outcome of a pilot randomized controlled trial. Research in Autism Spectrum
Disorders, 27, 73–87.
Srinivasan, S. M., Eigsti, I. M., Neelly, L., & Bhat, A. N. (2016). The effects of embodied rhythm and robotic interventions on the spontaneous and responsive social
attention patterns of children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD): A pilot randomized controlled trial. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 27, 54–72.
Srinivasan, S. M., Kaur, M., Park, I. K., Gifford, T. D., Marsh, K. L., & Bhat, A. N. (2015). The effects of rhythm and robotic interventions on the imitation/praxis,
interpersonal synchrony, and motor performance of children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD): A pilot randomized controlled trial. Autism Research and
Treatment, 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2015/736516 Article ID 736516.

94
M. Kaur et al. Research in Developmental Disabilities 72 (2018) 79–95

Srinivasan, S. M., Park, I. K., Neelly, L. B., & Bhat, A. N. (2015). A comparison of the effects of rhythm and robotic interventions on repetitive behaviors and affective
states of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 18, 51–63.
Staples, K. L., & Reid, G. (2010). Fundamental movement skills and autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(2), 209–217.
Teitelbaum, P., Teitelbaum, O., Nye, J., Fryman, J., & Maurer, R. G. (1998). Movement analysis in infancy may be useful for early diagnosis of autism. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences, 95(23), 13982–13987.
Théoret, H., Halligan, E., Kobayashi, M., Fregni, F., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2005). Impaired motor facilitation during action observation in in-
dividuals with autism spectrum disorder. Current Biology, 15(3), R84–R85.
Turner, K., Frost, L., Linsenbardt, D., McIlroy, J., & Müller, R. (2006). Atypically diffuse functional connectivity between caudate nuclei and cerebral cortex in autism.
Behavioral and Brain Functions, 2(34), http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-2-34.
Vanvuchelen, M., Roeyers, H., & De Weerdt, W. (2007). Nature of motor imitation problems in school-aged boys with autism A motor or a cognitive problem? Autism,
11(3), 225–240.
Vicaria, I., & Dickens, L. (2016). Meta-analyses of intra and inter-personal coordination. Journal of Non-Verbal Behavior, 40, 335–361.
Vilensky, J. A., Damasio, A. R., & Maurer, R. G. (1981). Gait disturbances in patients with autistic behavior: a preliminary study. Archives of Neurology, 38(10),
646–649.
Von Hofsten, C. (2007). Action in development. Developmental Science, 10(1), 54–60.
Whyatt, C. P., & Craig, C. M. (2012). Motor skills in children aged 7–10 years, diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
42(9), 1799–1809.
Whyatt, C., & Craig, C. (2013a). Sensory-motor problems in autism. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 7.
Whyatt, C., & Craig, C. M. (2013b). Interceptive skills in children aged 9–11 years, diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders,
7(5), 613–623.
Williams, J. H., Waiter, G. D., Gilchrist, A., Perrett, D. I., Murray, A. D., & Whiten, A. (2006). Neural mechanisms of imitation and ‘mirror neuron’functioning in autistic
spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 44(4), 610–621.

95

You might also like