You are on page 1of 15

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/301336310

Interventions to improve mathematical performance for children with


mathematical learning difficulties (MLD)

Chapter  in  Cohrane Database of Systematic Reviews · April 2016


DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD012130

CITATION READS

1 815

4 authors, including:

Mairead Furlong Sinead McGilloway


National University of Ireland, Maynooth Maynooth University Ireland
45 PUBLICATIONS   639 CITATIONS    161 PUBLICATIONS   1,470 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

David C Geary
University of Missouri
331 PUBLICATIONS   18,148 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Evolution of sex differences in trait- and age-specific vulnerabilities View project

Irish National Evaluation of the Incredible Years programme View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Mairead Furlong on 10 April 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Interventions to improve mathematical outcomes for children
with dyscalculia (Protocol)

Furlong M, McLoughlin F, McGilloway S, Butterworth B

ly
On
w
vie
re
rP

This is a reprint of a Cochrane protocol, prepared and maintained by The Cochrane Collaboration and published in The Cochrane
Fo

Library 2015, Issue 3


http://www.thecochranelibrary.com

Interventions to improve mathematical outcomes for children with dyscalculia (Protocol)


Copyright © 2015 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
TABLE OF CONTENTS
HEADER . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1
ABSTRACT . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1
BACKGROUND . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1
OBJECTIVES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4
METHODS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9

ly
REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9
CONTRIBUTIONS OF AUTHORS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 11
DECLARATIONS OF INTEREST . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 11
SOURCES OF SUPPORT . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 12

On
w
vie
re
rP
Fo

Interventions to improve mathematical outcomes for children with dyscalculia (Protocol) i


Copyright © 2015 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
[Intervention Protocol]

Interventions to improve mathematical outcomes for children


with dyscalculia

Mairead Furlong1 , Fergal McLoughlin2 , Sinead McGilloway1 , Brian Butterworth3

ly
1 Department of Psychology, National University of Ireland Maynooth, Maynooth, Ireland. 2 Health Service Executive (HSE) in

Dublin/Mid Leinster, Mullingar, Ireland. 3 Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, University College London,

On
London, UK

Contact address: Mairead Furlong, Department of Psychology, National University of Ireland Maynooth, North Campus, John Hume
Building, Maynooth, Co Kildare, Ireland. Mairead.M.Furlong@nuim.ie. mmm_furlong@hotmail.com.

Editorial group: Cochrane Developmental, Psychosocial and Learning Problems Group.


Publication status and date: New, published in Issue 3, 2015.

w
Citation: Furlong M, McLoughlin F, McGilloway S, Butterworth B. Interventions to improve mathematical outcomes for children with
dyscalculia. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2015, Issue 3. Art. No.: CDXXXXXX. DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CDXXXXXX.

Copyright © 2015 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
vie
ABSTRACT

This is the protocol for a review and there is no abstract. The objectives are as follows:
re

The objective of this review is to examine the effectiveness of interventions for children with dyscalculia in improving mathematical
performance compared to a control group of no treatment, waiting list, standard schooling, placebo or other treatment.
rP

BACKGROUND However, the specificity clause has recently been removed in order
to better represent the heterogeneity of the disorder and to im-
prove the clinical utility of DSM diagnoses (Kaufmann 2012; APA
2013). Therefore, dyscalculia is currently understood to involve
Description of the condition
three subtypes: (1) a specific and isolated disorder of mathematics;
Arithmetic is of prime importance in everyday life, and is necessary (2) a mathematics impairment in the context of deficiencies in
Fo

for simple but essential tasks such as counting, reading the clock, other non-numerical cognition (e.g. a general learning disability);
and for budgeting time and money resources. Moreover, numer- and (3) dyscalculia with comorbid disorders (e.g. attention-deficit
ical competency is related to longer-term educational and occu- hyperactivity disorder [ADHD] and dyslexia) (Kaufmann 2012).
pational outcomes, as well as to the economic status of countries Dyscalculia (DC) is as common in girls as in boys, and is a brain-
(Fuchs 2009; Gross 2009). However, research indicates that be- based disorder, with the left parieto-temporal sulcus being of par-
tween 3-6.5% of children fail to achieve competency in basic stan- ticular significance (Molko 2003; Butterworth 2010; Kaufmann
dard mathematics, such as within numerical operations (addition, 2011). Mathematical difficulties appear to have an early onset
subtraction, multiplication and division) and/or within arithmetic (Schopman 1996) and, unless specifically treated, will persist into
reasoning (Hein 2000; Butterworth 2011). Mathematical disor- late adolescence and adulthood (Shalev 2005; Butterworth 2011;
ders, also called dyscalculia, have typically been defined as a spe- Gerber 2012). Impairments in working memory for numerical in-
cific and isolated impairment in numeracy despite normal intelli- formation, visuospatial skills and/or auditory-perceptual abilities
gence and scholastic opportunity (APA 2000; WHO 1992/2007).
Interventions to improve mathematical outcomes for children with dyscalculia (Protocol) 1
Copyright © 2015 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
all appear to be implicated in the disorder (Rourke 1997; Rosselli set size; understanding ordinal position (e.g. 5 in 52 means 50
2006). Comorbidity with ADHD and dyslexia occurs in approxi- but 5 in 25); and translating between words and digits for quan-
mately one quarter of cases (Ramaa 2002), although comorbidity tity (Gersten 2009; Butterworth 2010; Cohen 2013). Neverthe-
with dyslexia appears to produce the most profound impairments less, such interventions may differ from each other with regard
when compared to those with DC alone, or those with DC and to a number of factors, including: the use of a computer, reme-
ADHD (Shalev 1997). Moreover, children with dyscalculia often dial teaching or peer learning; the size of the groups; the intensity
suffer severe emotional distress because they perform poorly in and duration of the intervention (e.g. ranging from one session of
school, which can lead to mathematics anxiety and school phobia 30 minutes to regular sessions conducted over a 6- or 12-month

ly
(Krinzinger 2006; Kaufmann 2012). period); the instructional procedures employed;and the age and
Although the prevalence of DC is comparable to the incidence grade of students.
of dyslexia, children with DC are often not diagnosed or treated Emerging evidence suggests that transcranial electrical stimula-

On
properly due to a persistent lack of knowledge about the disor- tion (TES) may enhance the effectiveness of targeted mathemat-
der (Dowker 2004). This may be linked, in part, to the ‘hidden’ ics interventions (Cohen 2013). For instance, a small number of
nature of specific DC as teachers and parents may fail to under- studies have reported improvements in basic numerical skills and
stand that someone with ’normal’ intelligence may suffer from an automaticity using TES, including a transfer effect to new mate-
isolated disorder of arithmetic skills (Kaufmann 2012). Further- rial, and long-term efficiency in brain functions in the stimulated
more, until recently, the study of mathematical difficulties was rel- brain region (Cohen 2010; Snowball 2013). Results, to date, have
atively neglected; for instance, between 1996 and 2005, the ratio indicated that stimulation needs to be paired with a cognitive-
of studies on reading disabilities to mathematic disabilities was 5: training intervention and that the timing of stimulation with re-
1. This was a dramatic improvement over the ratio of 16:1 in the spect to task performance has important effects (Stagg 2011). TES
prior decade (Gersten 2009). However, in recent years, a growing
interest in child numeracy has led to the development of a range w
of different interventions to treat DC, with a particular emphasis
involves the delivery of weak electrical currents (e.g.,1-2 mA) via
electrodes, most frequently at the size of 25-35 cm2 , which are
placed on the scalp above the brain area of interest. When the cur-
vie
on educational interventions that teach specific techniques to im- rent is applied over a short duration (20 min), it passes painlessly
prove mathematical competencies (Shalev 2004). Early interven- through the scalp and skull and alters spontaneous neural activity
tion is critical to prevent a life-course of suffering and secondary (Fritsch 2010).
emotional and behavioural problems. Acquisition of non-curricular skills may also be useful for students
with dyscalculia. Examples include arriving prepared for lessons,
meeting deadlines, appropriate school behaviour, following direc-
Description of the intervention tions and completing homework assignments. Thus, school/class-
re

room-based interventions that focus on strengthening these com-


The treatment of children and adolescents with dyscalculia is com-
petencies may be helpful in improving arithmetic skills for dyscal-
plex due to the heterogeneity of the disorder and the comorbid
culic students (Shalev 2004).
disorders often associated with it (Kaufmann 2012). Ideally, treat-
Psychotherapy and/or medication may sometimes be necessary
ment for dyscalculia should address the multiple facets of the disor-
rP

if DC is accompanied by anxiety, depression or ADHD (Shalev


der while focusing on educational interventions to improve num-
2004;Kaufmann 2012). Little research has examined the effective-
ber perception and arithmetic concepts (Shalev 2004).
ness of interventions that include pharmacological or psychother-
Therefore, eligible interventions will include any intervention
apeutic components in the treatment of DC. Nevertheless, pre-
(whether educational, psychotherapeutic, pharmacological or
liminary evidence indicates that methylphenidate may improve
non-invasive brain stimulation) that screened children for dyscal-
working memory in children with DC comorbid with ADHD,
culia and measured their mathematical competencies. Compara-
although it appears to have little effect on their basic numerical
Fo

tors will include standard schooling in mathematics, wait-list con-


skills (Grizenko 2006; Rubinsten 2008).
trol, placebo-controlled groups, no treatment or other treatment.
Educational interventions that specifically target mathematical
performance typically involve several components including: di-
rect, explicit instruction; use of cognitive heuristics/strategies; stu-
How the intervention might work
dent verbalisations of their mathematical reasoning; using visual This review includes a broad range of intervention strategies in-
representations while solving problems; provision of a range and volving: targeted mathematics interventions (with or without non-
sequence of examples; repeated practice; and corrective feedback invasive brain stimulation); classroom-based approaches; pharma-
(Kroesbergen 2003; Gersten 2009). Examples of cognitive strate- cology; and psychotherapy. Therefore, a large number of putative
gies include: counting on or back from first summand (e.g. 9 + 3 mechanisms are implicated in mediating intervention outcomes.
is solved by counting on 3 from 9); learning complementarity of More detail is provided below.
addition and subtraction (e.g. a + b = c → c - a = b); estimating Mathematics interventions

Interventions to improve mathematical outcomes for children with dyscalculia (Protocol) 2


Copyright © 2015 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
The strategies employed in these interventions (described above) duction of disruptive and aggressive behaviour [Wilson 2007]; fol-
are believed to develop the physical, visual, and verbal representa- lowing directions, staying on task [McGilloway 2012]), which, in
tions of quantity, i.e. students acquire a sense of the mental num- turn, may encourage students to focus their attention on mathe-
ber line (Butterworth 2010; Kaufmann 2012). The mental num- matics. Psychotherapy may enable students with DC to develop
ber line appears to be of fundamental importance for arithmetical effective cognitive and behavioural strategies to address anxieties
reasoning and for mental calculation, and extends the semantic about mathematical performance. For instance, the development
range of the concept of number to a more complex and abstract of self-efficacy beliefs (e.g. adopting a positive belief that they can
level (Kaufmann 2012). In addition, targeted mathematics inter- improve their skills with practice) and requesting help from par-

ly
ventions involve the active engagement of students in mathemati- ents and teacher may reduce mathematics anxiety and improve
cal reasoning, which has been shown to be more effective than pas- numerical competencies (Kaufmann 2012). Pharmacotherapy has
sive communication of the subject (Swanson 2000). Students also been shown to improve attention and working memory in chil-

On
appear to develop mathematical competency through verbalisa- dren with DC (Rubinsten 2008), which might potentially enhance
tion of mathematical reasoning, dedicated practice, and receiving their mathematical abilities.
detailed corrective feedback. Verbalisation anchors students’ self-
regulation and encourages them to utilise a step-by-step solution
strategy rather than a more random, impulsive approach (Gersten Why it is important to do this review
2009). Repeated practice appears to foster the automatic recall of
Numerical competency is vital to thrive in a modern economy
learned content, thereby lessening the demand on working mem-
and yet very little is known about how to support students who
ory. This demand is especially high in multi-digit calculations with
suffer from dyscalculia. Most research in the field has focused on
carrying, where interim results must be retained and manipulated
(e.g. adding 87 and 45) (Cohen 2013). Ongoing corrective feed-
w
back and the collation of detailed knowledge of performance may
also mediate positive outcomes through positive reinforcement of
the development of targeted arithmetic interventions for children
with mathematical difficulties. However, experts in the area also
recommend the provision of medical and psychotherapeutic treat-
ment if required (Kaufmann 2012). Therefore, the current review
vie
accurate responses and through goal setting (Gersten 2009). Com-
will also include interventions which comprise psychotherapeutic
puter-based interventions may be particularly useful in this respect
or medical elements.
as they can be designed to adapt for different performance profiles
Previous reviews in the field have examined the effectiveness of spe-
and provide intensive training in a stimulating environment. In
cific arithmetic interventions to improve mathematics skills, and
addition, research suggests that the reward-based nature of such
have reported inconsistent results, with effect sizes ranging from
instruction may act on the dopaminergic system that is involved
-0.44 to above 3 (Xin 1999; Kroesbergen 2003; Gersten 2009;
in plasticity (Lisman 2011).
re

Slavin 2009; Codding 2011; Fischer 2013). The inconsistent find-


Transcranial electrical stimulation
ings may be due to the heterogeneity of populations, study de-
TES is generally delivered alongside targeted mathematics inter-
signs, and interventions that were included in these reviews. For
ventions, and therefore it may be difficult to disaggregate the
instance, one review included only typically performing students
mediators of change. Nevertheless, research indicates that the ef-
(Slavin 2009), another included only children with specific DC
rP

fects of TES are protein-synthesis dependent and are accompanied


(Kroesbergen 2003), while the remaining reviews pooled results
by several mechanisms, including the modifications of intracellu-
across those with specific DC and those with mathematical diffi-
lar cyclic adenosine monophosphate (cAMP) and calcium levels,
culties in the context of general cognitive disabilities (Xin 1999;
brain- derived neurotrophic factor, and activation of adenosine
Gersten 2009; Codding 2011). Furthermore, all but one of these
A1 receptors (Márquez-Ruiz 2012). Therefore, these mechanisms
reviews (Gersten 2009) combined effect sizes across a wide range
share some features with long-term potentiation (Castillo 2011).
of study designs, including: randomised controlled trials (RCTs);
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (FMRI) experiments have
Fo

single-case designs; pre-post (non-controlled) studies; and non-


found that TES can alter local and remote brain activation (Keeser
matched controlled studies. Therefore, included studies were of
2011). Magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) studies found
varying quality, with single case designs notable for producing
change in the local concentration of gamma-aminobutyric acid
particularly elevated effect sizes (Busse 1995), while quasi-exper-
(GABA) and glutamate, which are critically involved in learning
iments may also fail to reproduce experimental results unless the
and memory (Stagg 2009).
assignment mechanism is completely known (Glazerman 2003;
Classroom-based, psychotherapeutic and pharmacological in-
Cook 2008). The review by Gersten 2009 only included RCTs
terventions
and treatment versus treatment designs but effect sizes were pooled
Very little research has investigated the efficacy of these interven-
together. This latter review was also the only one which assessed
tions in the treatment of DC (Kaufmann 2012). Putative mech-
selection or attrition bias within included studies; however no sen-
anisms of classroom-based approaches may involve positive rein-
sitivity analyses were undertaken in order to test the robustness of
forcement of desired non-curricular classroom activities (e.g. re-
the results across areas of risk of bias.

Interventions to improve mathematical outcomes for children with dyscalculia (Protocol) 3


Copyright © 2015 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
In addition, all but two of the reviews (Kroesbergen 2003; Gersten OBJECTIVES
2009) included only one outcome measure of mathematics skills
The objective of this review is to examine the effectiveness of in-
whereas the research indicates that students with DC may suffer
terventions for children with dyscalculia in improving mathemat-
from difficulties in basic numerical operations or numerical rea-
ical performance compared to a control group of no treatment,
soning or both (Fuchs 2009). Thus, this review will conduct sep-
waiting list, standard schooling, placebo or other treatment.
arate meta-analyses for basic numerical operations, numerical rea-
soning, and more advanced arithmetic (e.g. algebra). The impact
of interventions on precursor skills to mathematical competency

ly
(e.g. counting, number conservation) will also be examined. The METHODS
current review is also unique in that it will further assess secondary
outcomes associated with dyscalculia, such as emotional and be-

On
havioural problems, adverse outcomes and cost effectiveness. Criteria for considering studies for this review
Moreover, this review will explore the moderating impact of pop-
ulation and intervention characteristics on mathematical perfor-
mance. For instance, evidence suggests that children with DC co- Types of studies
morbid with dyslexia, or in combination with more general cog-
Eligible study designs will include randomised, quasi-randomised
nitive disabilities, are more impaired than those with DC alone
and cluster-randomised controlled trials. Cross-over trials will also
or with DC comorbid with ADHD (Shalev 2004; Fuchs 2009;
be included. We will not include single-case designs, pre-post
Butterworth 2010). In addition, younger participants may de-
studies without a control group, non-matched control groups, or
rive more benefit from interventions than older children (Gersten
2009). Furthermore, research indicates that mathematical diffi-
w
culties may be related to low socio-economic status (SES), gender,
and to place of residence (e.g. low income countries) (Royer 2007;
groups matched at post-hoc after results were known. Baseline
equivalence of the analytic sample must be demonstrated, or sta-
tistical controls must be used in the analysis to control for any
between-group differences at baseline. Studies which compare two
vie
Price 2013). Indeed, such inequity variables may reduce access to
similar treatments (e.g. targeted mathematics interventions) with-
appropriate interventions or education. For instance, many less
out a control group will not be included. We will include studies
developed countries lack teachers with mathematical training at
which compare two different treatments although a separate anal-
second level education, and student participation is low, particu-
ysis will be performed when the comparator is another treatment.
larly among females (Anderson 2009). Preliminary evidence also
Costs or cost-effectiveness studies conducted alongside, or sub-
indicates that children of low SES may derive less benefit from a
sequent to RCTs that meet eligibility criteria, will be included
targeted mathematics intervention than children with higher SES
re

(Shemilt 2011).
(Siegler 2008; Wilson 2009).
Intervention characteristics that may potentially affect outcomes
include treatment duration and agent of delivery (e.g. teacher- Types of participants
led, peer-assisted, or computerised programme). Identifying the
We will include students aged 7 to 18 years with all subtypes of
rP

most effective and resource efficient intervention is important, as


DC, i.e. those with isolated DC with normal intelligence, those
such interventions are more likely to be consistently implemented
with DC in the context of wider cognitive disabilities; and those
(Codding 2011). To date, evidence for the relationship between
with DC comorbid with dyslexia, ADHD, or emotional and be-
effect size and training intensity/duration is mixed: two studies
havioural problems. General intelligence will be defined as ‘nor-
found that shorter interventions were more effective (Kroesbergen
mal’ if students score 85 or above on a validated measure of intel-
2003; Gersten 2009), one favoured longer interventions of more
ligence (e.g. the Wechsler scales; Wechsler 2003). Study authors
than 30 sessions (Codding 2011), whereas another reported no
Fo

will be contacted for further information if the level of general


relationship between effect size and treatment duration (Fischer
intelligence is not specified in the article.
2013). Furthermore, it remains unclear to what extent treat-
In addition, we will include students whose mathematical com-
ment agent affects outcomes: a recent review found no relation-
petencies are at a ‘clinical’ or ‘at risk’ level. In accordance with the
ship (Fischer 2013), two studies reported that interventions de-
literature, a ‘clinical’ level will be defined as scoring below the 10
livered by teachers were more effective than computerised pro- th percentile on a standardised mathematics test and being ‘at risk’
grammes or peer-assisted instruction (Kroesbergen 2003; Slavin
will be defined as scoring below the 25th percentile (Moeller 2012).
2009), whereas another review found that interventions involving
An example of a standardised mathematics test is the Weschsler
both computer and staff instruction produced equivalent results
Individual Achievement Test (WIAT-II, Wechsler 2005).
(Xin 1999).
Children must be aged above 7 years as it is normal for some chil-
dren to experience some level of developmental delay in math-
ematical competencies (Fuchs 2009). However, we will include

Interventions to improve mathematical outcomes for children with dyscalculia (Protocol) 4


Copyright © 2015 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
studies if more than 90 per cent of the students therein are between Electronic searches
7 and 18 years.
We will search the following databases:
Cochrane Library, Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials
Types of interventions (CENTRAL)
MEDLINE
We will include any intervention that addresses mathematical per- EMBASE
formance as long as children are screened for dyscalculia and stud- Academic Search Complete

ly
ies include a measure of mathematical skills at pre- and post-inter- ASSIA (Applied Social Sciences Index and Abstracts)
vention. Studies which do not report a measure of the primary out- CINAHL
come of mathematical skill will not be included in the review. In- Dissertations and Theses Abstracts
terventions will include targeted mathematics interventions, class- ERIC

On
based interventions, non-invasive brain stimulation, pharmaco- metaRegister of Controlled Trials (mRCT)
logical and psychotherapeutic interventions. Proquest Dissertations and Theses,
PsycINFO
Science Citation Index Expanded
Types of outcome measures
Social Science Citation Index
Sociological Abstracts

Primary outcomes Economic sources:


Mathematical skill as measured by:

classification, seriation;
w
(i) Precursor arithmetic skills - counting, number conservation,

(ii) Basic numerical operations - addition, subtraction, multipli-


DARE
Econlit
Health Economic Evaluations Database (HEED)
Health Technology Assessments (HTA)
vie
cation and division; NHS Economic Evaluation Database (NHS EED)
(iii) Numerical reasoning - problem-solving and reasoning from Paediatric Economic Evaluation Database (PEED)
word problems; or No date, publication, geographic or language restrictions will be
(iv) More advanced mathematics - e.g. algebra, calculus. applied to the searches
Medline search strategy
1 Dyscalculia/
re

Secondary outcomes 2 Learning Disorders/


Language skills (reading or writing); 3 Developmental Disabilities/
Internalising problems (e.g. anxiety, depression); 4 Intellectual Disability/
Externalising problems (e.g. aggression, defiance); 5 aculculia.ab.
Hyperactivity or attention symptoms; 6 development* dyscalculi*.ab.
rP

User satisfaction; 7 (learn* n3 (disabilit* or difficult* or problem*)).ab.


Costs and cost-effectiveness data; and 8 (math* n3 (learn* disabilit* or learn* difficult*).ab.
Adverse impact - e.g. increased psychological distress from partic- 9 mld.ab.
ipating in the intervention. 10 (math* n3 (disabilit* or difficult* or disorder* or perform*
Timing of outcomes or competenc* or underachiev* or low perform* or achiev* or
We will conduct separate meta-analyses for outcomes measured at outcome* or operation* or reason* or skill* or educat*)).ab.
Fo

several periods of follow-up: within the first six months, between 11 (numer* n3 (disability* or difficult* or disorder* or perform*
6 to 12 months post-intervention, between 12-24 months post- or competenc* or underachiev* or low perform* or achiev* or
intervention (and longer follow-up points, if such data are avail- outcome* or operation* or reason* or skill* or educat*)).ab.
able). 12 (computation* n3 fluen*).ab.
13 (math* n3 fluen*).ab.
14 (numer* n3 fluen*).ab.
15 Low* numer*.ab.
Search methods for identification of studies 16 mental* retard*.ab.
We will identify studies through key and text word searches of 17 (number* n3 concept*).ab.
electronic databases, as well as searching grey literature (conference 18 or/1-17
papers, unpublished PhD theses, reference lists of other relevant 19 Child/
reviews) and personal communication with experts in the field. 20 Adolescent/

Interventions to improve mathematical outcomes for children with dyscalculia (Protocol) 5


Copyright © 2015 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
21 child*.ab. Selection of studies
22 adolescen*.ab. Two authors (MF and FM) will independently assess titles and ab-
23 or/19-22 stracts identified through searches in order to determine their po-
24 (math* n3 (train* or intervention* or teach* or instruct* or tential eligibility for inclusion in the review. Any citation deemed
educat*)).ab. potentially relevant by at least one author will be retrieved in full
25 (arithmetic* n3 (train* or intervention* or teach* or instruct* text and, again, will be independently assessed by MF and FM. If
or educat*)).ab. necessary, we will contact study authors to obtain additional infor-
26 (numer* n3 (train* or intervention* or teach* or instruct* or

ly
mation. We will resolve disagreements by consensus with a third
educat*)).ab. author (SMcG). We will document the specific reasons for exclu-
27 (cognit* n3 (train* or intervention* or teach* or instruct* or sion for each study that might reasonably have been expected to
educat*)).ab. have been included but which did not meet the inclusion criteria.

On
28 (educat* n3 intervention*).ab. Studies in other languages for which we cannot obtain translators
29 addition intervention*.ab. will be referred to the editorial base of the CDPLPG.
30 subtraction intervention*.ab.
31 multiplication intervention*.ab.
32 division intervention*.ab. Data extraction and management
33 element* educat*.ab.
Two authors (MF and FM) will extract data independently us-
34 primary educat*.ab.
ing a piloted data extraction form. The authors will extract in-
35 second* educat*.ab.
formation on study design and implementation, setting, sample
36 (comput* n3 (intervention* or program* or instruct*)).ab.
37 digital* n3 intervention*.ab.
38 (school* n3 (intervention* or program* or instruct* or
train*)).ab.
w characteristics, intervention characteristics and outcomes from all
included studies. With regard to costs or cost-effectiveness stud-
ies, MF and FM will independently extract characteristics, such
as: year of study; details of interventions and comparators; study
vie
39 (classroom*n3 (intervention* or instruct* or train*)).ab.
design; type of economic evaluation; source(s) of resource use;
40 Electric Stimulation/
unit costs; decision making jurisdiction; geographical and organi-
41 transcranial electrical stimulation.ab.
sational setting;analytic perspective; discount rates; and time hori-
42 tes.ab.
zon for both costs and effects. Any differences between review au-
43 non invasive brain stimulation.ab.
thors in data extraction will be resolved by discussion or by arbi-
44 Psychotherapy/
tration with SMcG if agreement cannot be reached.
45 psychotherap*.ab.
re

Data will be entered into Review Manager 5.2.11 (RevMan)


46 Pharmacy/
(Review Manager 2012). Where data are not available in the pub-
47 Pharmacology/
lished trial reports, we will contact the authors and ask them to
48 pharmac*.ab.
supply the missing information.
49 methylphenidate.ab.
rP

50 Clinical Medicine/
51 or/24-50 Assessment of risk of bias in included studies
52 18 and 23 and 51
Review authors (MF and FM) will independently assess the risk of
53 Costs and Cost Analysis/
bias (that is ’high’, ’low’ or ’unclear’) within each included study
54 Econom* or cost* or price* or budget* or resource* or utili?
across the following six domains:
sation.ab.
(1) sequence generation;
55 or/53-54
Fo

(2) allocation concealment;


56 52 and 55
(3) blinding;
(4) incomplete outcome data;
Searching other resources (5) selective outcome reporting; and
We will examine the reference lists of included studies and of (6) other sources of bias, such as comparability of baseline char-
reviews in the field in order to find other eligible studies. We will acteristics between groups and attempts to control for imbalance.
also contact authors of included studies, as well as experts working Where judgments differ, additional input will be sought from
in the area, in order to search for unpublished and ongoing studies. SMcG, BB and NH. Missing data on ’risk of bias’ criteria will
be sought from study authors. Judgments will be entered into the
’Risk of bias’ table in RevMan.
In assessing risk of bias in cross-over trials, we will also judge:
Data collection and analysis
• The appropriateness of using a cross-over design

Interventions to improve mathematical outcomes for children with dyscalculia (Protocol) 6


Copyright © 2015 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
• Whether the trial was biased from carry-over effects (e.g. If cross-over trials are included, effect sizes should be calculated
order or learning effects) from paired analyses using within-participant comparisons. This
evaluates the value of ’measurement on experimental intervention’
If costs or cost effectiveness studies are included in the re-
(E) minus ’measurement on control intervention’ (C) separately
view, we will use the ’Drummond checklist’ to critically appraise
for each participant. The effect estimate (i.e. mean and standard
the methodological quality of included health economic studies
error) may be included in a meta-analysis using the generic inverse-
(Shemilt 2011). MF and FM will assess such quality parameters
variance method (Higgins 2011). If such paired analyses are not
independently, with any disagreements being resolved through dis-
reported or available, and if we judge there is little likelihood of

ly
cussion. Again, study authors will be contacted for missing infor-
carry-over effects, we will take all measurements from intervention
mation.
E periods and all measurements from intervention C periods and
analyse these as if the trial were a parallel group trial of E versus

On
Measures of treatment effect C. This can lead to a unit of analysis error but is generally judged
as less serious than other unit of analyses errors due to under-
Where outcomes are reported as dichotomous data, we will use the
weighting of the study (Higgins 2011).
risk ratio (RR) with a 95% confidence interval (CI) to summarise
Economic evaluation
results within each study.
Studies will initially be classified according to whether they mea-
Continuous outcomes will be reported, where possible, as mean
sure resource costs or whether or not they calculate an incremental
differences. Where different scales measure the same outcome
cost effectiveness ratio (ICER). An ICER point estimate compares
across studies, standardised mean differences (SMDs) will be used
the costs and consequences of running an intervention relative to
to summarise results within each study. Again, confidence inter-
the costs and consequences of a specified alternative (most com-

w
vals of 95% will be used for individual study data and pooled es-
timates throughout. If reported outcomes have insufficient data,
additional information will be requested from the authors. In ad-
dition, it may be possible to calculate effect sizes within contin-
monly chosen to be the status quo). In circumstances where there
is evidence of little variation in resource or cost use between stud-
ies, it may be regarded as legitimate to
vie
present a pooled estimate. Otherwise, we will clearly present the
uous data through reported t-tests, F-tests, exact p-values, and so
distribution of costs (Shemilt 2011). If a decision is made to con-
forth. Due to expected heterogeneity, a random-effects meta-anal-
duct meta-analyses of resource use or
ysis will be employed.
cost data, this will be supported by a thorough critical appraisal of
If several different instruments measure an outcome within in-
the methods used to derive such estimates within the correspond-
cluded studies, we will obtain a mean effect size and standard er-
ing health economics studies, alongside use of 95% confidence
ror for that outcome within the study. This effect estimate will
intervals and statistical methods to investigate and incorporate be-
re

be entered into the generic inverse variance method in RevMan


tween- study heterogeneity (e.g.I² statistic, Chi² test, random-ef-
(Review Manager 2012), using the effect measure of SMD or RR,
fects models). Cost estimates collected from multiple studies will
as appropriate.
be adjusted to a common currency and price year before these data
In multi-arm studies where several experimental (or control)
are pooled. Careful consideration will be given to the jurisdiction,
groups are eligible for inclusion (e.g. three variants of a targeted
rP

analytic perspective and time horizon for both costs and effects.
mathematics intervention), we will combine groups to create a
If meta-analyses of resource use or cost data are conducted, a nar-
single pair-wise comparison. However, in order to conduct the
rative summary will be included in the Results section to comment
relevant subgroup analyses (as specified below; e.g. one experi-
on the direction and magnitude of results and their precision. Sim-
mental group is teacher led and another is peer-assisted), it may
ilarly, if two or more health economics studies are included in a
be necessary to include each pair-wise comparison separately for
review, but a decision is taken not to pool resource use and/or cost
each experimental group within the study, but with the sample size
data from these studies, this will be stated in the Methods section
Fo

of shared intervention groups divided out approximately evenly


(Shemilt 2011).
among the comparisons. This latter approach may incur a unit of
analysis error but it still remains a practical means of performing
approximate investigations of heterogeneity (Higgins 2011).
If studies report multiple measures of the same construct at dif- Unit of analysis issues
ferent points in time, we will conduct separate meta-analyses for If cluster-randomised trials are included, data will be controlled for
outcomes measured at several periods of follow-up: within the first clustering using the procedures outlined by Higgins 2011 in the
six months, between 6 to 12 months post-intervention, between Cochrane Handbook. When outcome measures are dichotomous,
12-24 months post-intervention (and longer follow-up points, if the number of events and number of participants per trial arm
such data are available). If within any of these periods, measures will be divided by the design effect [1 = (1-m) * r], where m is the
are reported more than once, then a single summary effect will be average cluster size and r is the intra-cluster correlation coefficient
obtained within that time period. (ICC). When outcome measures are continuous, the number of

Interventions to improve mathematical outcomes for children with dyscalculia (Protocol) 7


Copyright © 2015 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
participants per trial arm will be divided by the design effect, with pooled. Between-study heterogeneity will be assessed using the
the mean values unchanged. To determine the ICC, authors will Chi² test, the I²statistic and random-effects models.
use estimates in the primary trials on a study-by-study basis. If
these values are not reported, the authors will use external estimates
of the ICC that are appropriate to each trial context and average Assessment of reporting biases
cluster size. Funnel plots will be drawn, where possible, to investigate any re-
As noted above, a unit of analysis error may occur when conducting lationship between effect size and standard error. Such a relation-
subgroup analyses if more than one pair-wise comparison from the ship could be due to publication or related biases, or due to sys-

ly
same study is entered into meta-analysis. Such subgroup analyses tematic differences between small and large studies. Where such a
will be interpreted with caution. relationship is identified, the experimental diversity of the studies
In addition, there may be a unit of analysis error if a paired anal- will be further examined as a possible explanation (Egger 1997).

On
ysis is not reported within cross-over trials. A common approach
to incorporating cross-over trials in a meta-analysis is to take all
measurements from intervention E periods and all measurements Data synthesis
from intervention C periods and analyse these as if the trial were a We will perform meta-analysis where studies have sufficiently sim-
parallel group trial of E versus C. This method may under-weight ilar participants, interventions, comparators and outcome mea-
the study but some view this type of unit-of-analysis error is less sures. We will analyse outcome data separately for the various cat-
serious than other types of unit-of-analysis errors (Higgins 2011). egories of intervention: targeted mathematics intervention, class-
based intervention, non-invasive brain stimulation, pharmacolog-

Dealing with missing data


We will assess missing data and dropouts for each included study
and report the number of participants who were included in the
w ical and psychotherapeutic interventions. A separate meta-analysis
will be conducted where an intervention represents a combination
of these categories. Where a comparator is another intervention,
we will conduct a separate meta-analysis. In addition, separate
vie
final analysis as a proportion of all randomised participants in each meta-analyses will be performed for each type of mathematical
study. We will provide reasons, where known, for missing data in skill noted in the primary outcome (i.e. precursor arithmetic skills,
the ’Risk of bias’ section, as well as details of investigators’ use basic numerical operations, numerical reasoning, advanced math-
of intention-to-treat analysis (where applicable). Authors will be ematics such as algebra and calculus). Furthermore, as noted ear-
contacted to supply missing data. If missing data are not available lier, separate meta-analyses will be conducted for outcomes mea-
in studies, a sensitivity analysis will be conducted to assess potential sured at different time points.
bias in the analysis. In carrying out meta-analysis, the weight given to each study is the
re

inverse of the variance so that more weight will be given to more


precise estimates (from larger studies with more events).
Assessment of heterogeneity
We will assess clinical heterogeneity by comparing the distribution Subgroup analysis and investigation of heterogeneity
rP

of important factors such as participant demographics, type of in-


tervention and control comparators, quality of trials (randomisa- If sufficient studies are found or if information is available, we will
tion, blinding, losses to follow-up) and outcomes measured across examine the following subgroups:
studies. Statistical heterogeneity will be assessed visually and by • Effect of child’s age on outcomes (i.e. compare children
examining the I² statistic, a quantity which describes the approxi- aged between 7-12 years with those between 13-18 years)
mate proportion of variation in point estimates that is due to het- • Effect of subtype of DC on outcomes (i.e. compare isolated
Fo

erogeneity rather than sampling error. With the I² statistic, 30% DC, DC with general cognitive impairments, and DC comorbid
to 60% may be interpreted as moderate heterogeneity; 50% to with ADHD, dyslexia and emotional and behavioural problems)
90% as substantial heterogeneity; and 75% to 100% as consid- • Efffect of treatment agent on outcomes (i.e. compare
erable heterogeneity. This will be supplemented by the Chi² test, teacher led, peer-assisted, computerised programme, and teacher
where a P value < 0.05 indicates heterogeneity of treatment effects. in combination with computer programme)
Sensitivity and subgroup analyses will also be used to investigate
any possible sources of heterogeneity. • Effect of treatment duration on outcomes (i.e. compare
For the economic evaluations, careful attention will be given to interventions comprising 0-10 hours versus those with 10-20
whether the metric in question has equivalent meaning across stud- hours, 20-30 hours or more than 30 hours)
ies before data are pooled (Shemilt 2011). Cost estimates collected • Effect of inequity variables on outcomes (i.e. compare
from multiple studies will be adjusted to a common currency us- outcomes for gender, socioeconomic status, and comparison of
ing purchasing power parity and price year before these data are low-, middle- and high-income countries)

Interventions to improve mathematical outcomes for children with dyscalculia (Protocol) 8


Copyright © 2015 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Sensitivity analysis ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
Sensitivity analyses will be performed to evaluate the robustness of We would like to convey a special note of thanks to Professor
the results across various components of methodological quality, Sandra Jo Wilson (Editor of Campbell’s Education Coordinating
including: Group) for her helpful suggestions and advice in writing this pro-
• Removing quasi-randomised, cluster randomised, and cross tocol. We also thank Sean Grant (former managing editor) and
over trials Carlton Fong (managing editor) for their support. In addition, we
• Removing studies without blind assessment of outcomes would like to thank the editor (Professor Geraldine MacDonald)

ly
• Exclusion of studies with attrition rates larger than 20% or and managing editor (Joanne Wilson) of the Cochrane DPLPG
which did not conduct an intention-to-treat analysis for the co-registration of this review.

On
REFERENCES

Additional references Cohen 2013


Cohen KR, Dowker A, Heine A, Kaufmann L, Kucian K.
Anderson 2009 Interventions for improving numerical abilities: Present and
Developing Countries Strategies Group International future. Trends in Neuroscience and Education 2013;2:85–93.
Mathematical Union. Mathematics in Africa: challenges
Cook 2008
and opportunities. A report to the John Templeton
Cook TD, Shadish WR, Wong VC. Three conditions
Foundation 2009.
APA 2000
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders, Text Revision. 4th Edition.
w under which experiments and observational studies
produce comparable causal estimates: new findings from
within-study comparisons. Journal of Policy Analysis and
Management 2008;27(4):724–50.
vie
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association, 2000.
Dowker 2004
APA 2013 Dowker A. What Works for Children with Mathematical
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Difficulties?. London, UK: Department for Education and
Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th Edition. Arlington, VA: Skills, 2004.
American Psychiatric Publishing, 2013. Egger 1997
Busse 1995 Egger M, Davey-Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias
Busse RT, Kratochwill TR, Elliott SN. Meta-analysis for in Metaanalysis detected by a simple graphical test. British
re

single-case consultation outcomes: applications to research Medical Journal 1997;315:629–34.


and practice. Journal of School Psychology 1995;33(4): Fischer 2013
269–86. Fischer U, Moeller K, Cress U, Nuerk H-C. Interventions
Butterworth 2010 supporting children’s mathematics school success: a meta-
Butterworth B, Laurillard D. Low numeracy and dyscalculia: analytic review. European Psychologist 2013;18(2):89–113.
rP

identification and intervention. ZDM Mathematics Fritsch 2010


Education 2010;42:527-39. Fritsch B, Reis J, Martinowich K, Schambra HM, Ji Y,
Butterworth 2011 Cohen LG, et al. Direct current stimulation promotes
Butterworth B, Varma S, Laurillard D. Dyscalculia: from BDNF-dependent synaptic plasticity: potential implications
brain to education. Science 2011;332(6033):1049–53. for motor learning. Neuron 2010;66(2):198-204.
Castillo 2011 Fuchs 2009
Fo

Castillo PE, Chiu CQ, Carroll RC. Long-term plasticity at Fuchs LS, Powell SR, Seethaler PM, Cirino PT, Fletcher
inhibitory synapses. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2011; JM, Fuchs D, et al. Remediating number combination and
21(2):328–38. word problem deficits among students with mathematics
Codding 2011 difficulties: a randomized control trial. Journal of
Codding RS, Burns MK, Lukito G. Meta-analysis of Educational Psychology 2009;101(3):561–76.
mathematic basic-fact fluency interventions: a component Gerber 2012
analysis. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice 2011;26 Gerber PJ. The impact of learning disabilities on adulthood:
(1):36–47. a review of the evidenced-based literature for research and
Cohen 2010 practice in adult education. Journal of Learning Disabilities
Cohen KR, Soskic S, Iuculano T, Kanai R, Walsh V. 2012;45(1):31–46.
Modulating neuronal activity produces specific and long Gersten 2009
lasting changes in numerical competence. Current Biology Gersten R, Chard DJ, Jayanthi M, Baker SK, Murphy P,
2010;20(22):2016–20. Flojo J. Mathematics instruction for students with learning
Interventions to improve mathematical outcomes for children with dyscalculia (Protocol) 9
Copyright © 2015 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
disabilities: a meta-analysis of instructional components. McGilloway 2012
Review of Educational Research 2009;79(3):1202–42. McGilloway S, Ní Mháille G, Furlong M, Hyland L, Leckey
Glazerman 2003 Y, Kelly P, et al. Long-Term Outcomes of the Incredible
Glazerman S, Levy DM, Myers D. Non-experimental versus Years Parent and Teacher Classroom Management Training
experimental estimates of earnings impacts. Annals of the Programmes (Combined 12-month Report). Dublin, Ireland:
American Academy of Political and Social Science 2003;589 Archways, 2012.
(1):63–93. Moeller 2012
Grizenko 2006 Moeller K, Fischer U, Cress U, Nuerk H-C. Diagnostics

ly
Grizenko N, Bhat M, Schwartz G, Ter-Stepanian M, Joober and intervention in dyscalculia: current issues and novel
R. Efficacy of methylphenidate in children with attention- perspectives. In: Brexnitz Z, Rubinsten O, Molfese V,
deficit hyperactivity disorder and learning disabilities: Molfese DL editor(s). Reading, Writing, Mathematics and
a randomised crossover trial. Journal of Psychiatry and the Developing Brain: Listening to Many Voices. Heidelberg,

On
Neuroscience 2006;31(1):46–51. Germany: Springer, 2012:233–76.
Gross 2009 Molko 2003
Gross J, Hudson C, Price D. The Long Term Costs of Molko N, Cachia A, Riviere D, Mangin J-F, Bruandet M,
Numeracy Difficulties. London, UK: Every Child a Chance Le Bihan D, et al. Functional and structural alterations of
Trust, 2009. the intraparietal sulcus in a developmental dyscalculia of
Hein 2000 genetic origin. Neuron 2003;40(4):847–58.
Hein, J, Bzufka MW, Neumärker K-J. The specific Márquez-Ruiz 2012
disorder of arithmetical skills. Prevalence study in a Márquez-Ruiz J, Leal-Campanario R, Sánchez-Campusano
rural and an urban population sample and their clinico- R, Molaee-Ardekani B, Wendling F, Miranda PC, et al.
neuropsychological validation. European Child & Adolescent
Psychiatry 2000;9(2 Suppl):87–101.
Higgins 2011
Higgins JPT, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Special topics in
w Transcranial direct-current stimulation modulates synaptic
mechanisms involved in associative learning in behaving
rabbits. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
2012;109:6710-5.
vie
statistics. In: Higgins PT, Green S editor(s). Cochrane Price 2013
Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. Chichester: Price GR, Ansari D. Dyscalculia: characteristics, causes, and
John Wiley & Sons, 2011. treatments. Numeracy 2013;6:Article 2.
Kaufmann 2011 Ramaa 2002
Kaufmann L, Wood G, Rubinsten O, Henik A. Meta- Ramaa S, Gowramma IP. A systematic procedure for
analyses of developmental fMRI studies investigating identifying and classifying children with dyscalculia among
typical and atypical trajectories of number processing primary school children in India. Dyslexia 2002;8(2):
re

and calculation. Developmental Neuropsychology 2011;36: 67–85.


763–87. Review Manager 2012
Kaufmann 2012 The Nordic Cochrane Centre, The Cochrane Collaboration.
Kaufmann L, Von Aster M. The diagnosis and management Review Manager (RevMan). 5.2. Copenhagen: The Nordic
of dyscalculia. Deutsches Ärzteblatt International 2012;109
rP

Cochrane Centre, The Cochrane Collaboration, 2012.


(45):767–77. Rosselli 2006
Keeser 2011 Rosselli M, Matute E, Pinto N, Ardila A. Memory abilities
Keeser D, Meindl T, Bor J, Palm U, Pogarell O, Mulert in children with subtypes of dyscalculia. Developmental
C, et al. Prefrontal transcranial direct current stimulation Neuropsychology 2006;30(3):801–8.
changes connectivity of resting-state networks during fMRI. Rourke 1997
Journal of Neuroscience 2011;31(43):15284-93. Rourke BP, Conway JA. Disabilities of arithmetic and
Fo

Krinzinger 2006 mathematical reasoning: perspectives from neurology and


Krinzinger H, Kaufmann L. Mathematics anxiety neuropsychology. Journal of Learning Disabilities 1997;30
and computing power: language, voice and hearing (1):34–46.
[Rechenangst und rechenleistung. Sprache, stimme, gehör]. Royer 2007
Zeitschrift für Kommunikationsstörungen 2006;30:160–4. Royer JM, Walles R. Influences of gender, ethnicity, and
Kroesbergen 2003 motivation on mathematical performance. In: Berch DB,
Kroesbergen EH, Van Luit JEH. Mathematics interventions Mazzocco MMM editor(s). Why is Math so Hard for Some
for children with special educational needs: a meta-analysis. Children? The Nature and Origins of Mathematical Learning
Remedial and Special Education 2003;24(2):97–114. Difficulties and Disabilities. Baltimore, MD: Paul H Brookes
Lisman 2011 Publishing Co, 2007.
Lisman J, Grace AA, Duzel E. A neoHebbian framework Rubinsten 2008
for episodic memory; role of dopamine-dependent late LTP. Rubinsten O, Bedard AC, Tannock R. Methylphenidate
Trends in Neurosciences 2011;34(10):536–47. improves general but not core numerical abilities in ADHD
Interventions to improve mathematical outcomes for children with dyscalculia (Protocol) 10
Copyright © 2015 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
children with comorbid dyscalculia or mathematical cortical neurotransmitters by transcranial stimulation.
difficulties. Journal of Open Psychology 2008;1:11–7. Journal of Neuroscience 2009;29:5202–9.
Schopman 1996 Stagg 2011
Schopman EAM, Van Luit JEH. Learning and transfer of Stagg CJ, Jayaram G, Pastor D, Kincses ZT, Matthews PM,
preparatory arithmetic strategies among children with a Johansen-Berg H. Polarity and timing-dependent effects of
developmental lag. Journal of Cognitive Education 1996;5: transcranial direct current stimulation in explicit motor
117–31. learning. Neuropsychologia 2011;49:800–4.
Shalev 1997 Swanson 2000

ly
Shalev RS, Manor O, Gross-Tsur V. Neuropsychological Swanson HL, Sachse-Lee C. A meta-analysis of single-
aspects of developmental dyscalculia. Math Cognition 1997; subject design intervention research for students with LD.
3(2):105–20. Journal of Learning Disabilities 2000;33:114–36.
Shalev 2004

On
Wechsler 2003
Shalev RS. Developmental dyscalculia. Journal of Child Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-
Neurology 2004;19:765–71. 4th Edition (WISC-IV®). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt
Shalev 2005 Assessment, 2003.
Shalev RS, Manor O, Gross-Tsur V. Developmental Wechsler 2005
dyscalculia: a prospective six-year follow-up. Developmental Wechsler D. Wechsler Individual Achievement Test
Medicine & Child Neurology 2005;47:121-5. 2nd Edition (WIAT II). London: The Psychological
Shemilt 2011 Corporation, 2005.
Shemilt I, Mugford M, Byford S, Drummond M, Eisenstein WHO 1992/2007
E, Knapp M, et al. Incorporating economics evidence. In:
Higgins PT, Green S editor(s). Cochrane Handbook for
Systematic Reviews of Interventions. Chichester: John Wiley
& Sons, 2011.
w World Health Organization. International Statistical
Classification of Disease and Related Health Problems. Tenth
Revision (ICD-10). Geneva, Switzerland: World Health
Organization, 1992/2007.
vie
Siegler 2008
Wilson 2007
Siegler RS, Ramani GB. Playing linear numerical
Wilson SJ, Lipsey M. School-based interventions for
boardgames promotes low-income children’s numerical
aggressive and disruptive behavior: update of a meta-
development. Developmental Science 2008;11:665–61.
analysis. American Journal of Preventive Medicine 2007;33:
Slavin 2009 130–43.
Slavin RE, Lake C, Groff, C. Effective programs in middle Wilson 2009
and high school mathematics: a best-evidence synthesis.
re

Wilson AJ, Stanislas D, Dubois O, Fayol M. Effects of an


Review of Educational Research 2009;79:839–911. adaptive game Intervention on accessing number sense in
Snowball 2013 low-socioeconomic-status kindergarten children. Mind,
Snowball A, Tachtsidis I, Popescu T, Thompson J, Delazer Brain and Education 2009;3:224–34.
M, Zamarian L, et al. Long-term enhancement of brain Xin 1999
rP

function and cognition using cognitive training and brain Xin YP, Jitendra AK. The effects of instruction in solving
stimulation. Current Biology 2013;23:987-92. mathematical word problems for students with learning
Stagg 2009 problems: a meta-analysis. Journal of Special Education
Stagg CJ, Best JG, Stephenson MC, O’Shea J, Wylezinska 1999;32:207–25.
M, Kincses ZT, et al. Polarity-sensitive modulation of ∗
Indicates the major publication for the study
Fo

CONTRIBUTIONS OF AUTHORS
Mairead Furlong (MF) wrote the text of the protocol, with feedback from the rest of the review team (Fergal McLoughlin [FM], Sinead
McGilloway [SMcG], Brian Butterworth [BB] and Noirin Hayes [NH]). All team members were involved in developing the search
strategy, and will liaise with the information retrieval specialist at Campbell in searching for studies. MF and FM will independently
select trials and extract data from them, with SMcG arbitrating in the event of a dispute. MF will conduct the analysis, with all members
of the team being involved in interpretation. MF will draft the text of the final review, with input from all of the team. MF will be
responsible for keeping the review up to date.

Interventions to improve mathematical outcomes for children with dyscalculia (Protocol) 11


Copyright © 2015 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
DECLARATIONS OF INTEREST
Professor Butterworth’s research resides primarily in the investigation of the neural basis of mathematical cognition. However, he has
recently been involved in the development of two digital interventions for dyscalculia. These are called Dots2Track and Dots2Digits and
are designed to help the learner discern the relationship between the numerosity in a dot pattern and its representation as a digit, and
on a number line (Butterworth 2010). The other four authors have no known conflicts of interest to report.

ly
SOURCES OF SUPPORT

Internal sources

On
• Department of Psychology, National University of Ireland Maynooth, Ireland.

External sources
• No sources of support supplied

w
vie
re
rP
Fo

Interventions to improve mathematical outcomes for children with dyscalculia (Protocol) 12


Copyright © 2015 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

View publication stats

You might also like