You are on page 1of 10

Journal of Counseling Psychology Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association

2005, Vol. 52, No. 4, 640 – 649 0022-0167/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-0167.52.4.640

Measuring the Real Relationship in Psychotherapy: Initial Validation of the


Therapist Form

Charles J. Gelso Frances A. Kelley


University of Maryland Louisiana Tech University

Jairo N. Fuertes Cheri Marmarosh


Fordham University Catholic University of America

Stacey E. Holmes Catarina Costa


University of Maryland Fordham University

Gregory R. Hancock
University of Maryland

The development and initial validation of a therapist-rated measure of the real relationship in psycho-
therapy (the Real Relationship Inventory—Therapist Form [RRI–T]) is reported. Using a sample (n ⫽
80) of practicing psychotherapists and on the basis of prior theory, the authors developed a 24-item
measure consisting of 2 subscales (Realism and Genuineness) and a total score. This 24-item version and
other measures used for validation were completed by 79 additional practicing therapists and 51
counseling graduate students (n ⫽ 130). The RRI–T was found to have high reliability and sound initial
validity. As theorized, the RRI–T correlated significantly with measures of working alliance, session
outcome (depth and smoothness), client intellectual and emotional insight, and client negative transfer-
ence. Discriminant validity was supported by a nonsignificant relation to social desirability.

Keywords: real relationship, therapist measure, therapeutic relationship, validation

The relationship that exists between client and therapist may be the personal relationship that exists between therapist and client
seen as containing several elements (Gelso & Carter, 1985, 1994). from the first moment of contact. Although the personal relation-
Elements such as working alliance, transference, and countertrans- ship may be seen as influencing and being influenced by the
ference have been abundantly theorized about for many years (see working alliance, it has been theorized as being more basic than
Gelso & Hayes, 1998, review). These concepts have also been the alliance and as existing apart from the work of therapy (Gelso,
empirically investigated to varying degrees (Barber, Foltz, & 2002; Gelso & Carter, 1985, 1994; Gelso & Hayes, 1998). In other
Weinryb, 1998; Constantino, Castonguay, & Schut, 2002; Gelso & words, whereas the working alliance may be seen as the part of the
Hayes, 2002; Multon, Patton, & Kivlighan, 1996). total relationship that exists solely for the purpose of the work that
Over the years, some theoreticians have also alluded to another is to be done in therapy, the personal relationship is the relatively
component of the relationship, one that goes beyond the work of transference-free part of the total relationship that must exist in all
therapy (i.e., the working alliance) and the unconscious projections relationships. Following the lead of the psychoanalyst Ralph
and/or distortions of each participant toward the other (i.e., Greenson (1967), Charles J. Gelso and his collaborators have
transference– countertransference). This component has to do with
labeled this personal connection or bond the real relationship
(Gelso, 2002; Gelso & Carter, 1985, 1994; Gelso & Hayes, 1998).
The real relationship, although it is interrelated with working
Charles J. Gelso, Department of Psychology, University of Maryland;
Frances A. Kelley, Department of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, alliance and transference, has been theorized to contribute impor-
Louisiana Tech University; Jairo N. Fuertes and Catarina Costa, Graduate tantly and independently to psychotherapeutic process and out-
School of Education, Fordham University at Lincoln Center; Cheri Mar- come across all theoretical approaches (Gelso, 2002, 2004; Gelso
marosh, Counseling Center and Department of Psychology, Catholic Uni- & Hayes, 1998).
versity of America; Stacey E. Holmes, Department of Counseling and The concept of a real relationship has been present in the
Personnel Services, University of Maryland; Gregory R. Hancock, Depart- literature for many years (e.g., A. Freud, 1954; S. Freud, 1937;
ment of Measurement, Statistics and Evaluation, University of Maryland.
Rogers, 1957). However, the idea has always been a controversial
We thank Dawn Szymanski for assistance with aspects of the statistical
design.
one (see Beutler & Sandowicz, 1994; Greenberg, 1994). Some
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Charles observers have been critical of the construct, claiming that because
J. Gelso, Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College everything in therapy is real, the concept of real relationship does
Park, MD 20742. E-mail: gelso@psyc.umd.edu not add anything helpful to theory. Others have challenged the

640
MEASURING THE REAL RELATIONSHIP 641

concept on the grounds that even if there is a reality, we cannot The real relationship may be assessed from various perspec-
ever know it fully. Still others have raised the question of who, in tives—therapist, client, supervisor, outside raters. In the present
the final analysis, is the arbiter of what is real. In responding to study, we seek to develop a measure from the therapist’s perspec-
these criticisms, Gelso (2002) has commented that, first, although tive. In sum, we developed and validated items tapping the con-
in a certain sense everything that happens in any relationship is cepts of genuineness and realism as well as the subconcepts of
real, the construct real relationship captures elements in therapy magnitude and valence under each of the two main defining
that other relationship elements fail to capture. Second, the fact features. Thus, therapists were asked to rate themselves, their
that what is real is not fully knowable in no way negates the clients, and the therapy relationship on items representing genu-
concept of real relationship. Last, the merit of a concept such as ineness and realism (and magnitude and valence within genuine-
real relationship does not depend on one or another member of the ness and realism).
therapy being the total arbiter of what is real. Instead, what is real In seeking to validate our measure, we focus on constructs to
may be seen as constructions of both members of the dyad. In sum, which it theoretically ought to relate. Such constructs reflect rela-
despite the controversy, the present research is based on the tional and process variables as well as session-outcome variables.
argument that real relationship is a theoretically useful and empir- First, we hypothesized that the real relationship would relate
ically measurable construct that can help in understanding the positively to the working alliance between therapist and client. The
process and outcome of treatment. basis for this hypothesis is that the alliance has been theorized as
What is the real relationship? Gelso (2004) has defined it as “the stemming from and resting on the real relationship (Greenson,
personal relationship existing between two or more people as 1967; Safran & Muran, 1998). The alliance represents the human
reflected in the degree to which each is genuine with the other and bond that is part of the work of therapy, whereas the real relation-
perceives the other in ways that befit the other” (p. 6). Thus, real ship reflects the human bond that exists in all relationships and that
relationship is seen as consisting of two key, interrelated elements: underlies a working bond.
genuineness and realism. Genuineness, a key concept in virtually Because the real relationship is theorized to represent, to an
all humanistic– experiential systems, may be defined as “the ability extent, the realistic and nondistorted aspect of the relationship, it
to be who one truly is, to be nonphoney, to be authentic in the here ought to be negatively related to aspects that are distorted by the
client’s defenses, wishes, fears, and so forth. That is, a stronger
and now” (Gelso, 2002, p. 37). Realism, conversely, is seen as “the
real relationship ought to be indicative of less distortion. Because
experiencing or perceiving of the other in ways that befit him or
the latter is what ordinarily is seen as indicative of transference
her, rather than as projections of wished for or feared others (i.e.,
(Gelso & Hayes, 1998), our second hypothesis was that there is a
transference)” (p. 37). Realism is often seen as an important
negative relation between real relationship and transference. It
element in the more psychodynamic systems.
should be noted that the type of transference that has been found
Although genuineness and realism are the two general defining
to be especially predictive of other relational, process, and out-
features of the real relationship, to make sense of this construct two
come variables is negative transference (Gelso, Hill, & Kivlighan,
other aspects are needed: magnitude and valence. Magnitude refers
1991; Gelso, Wine, Kivlighan, Jones, & Friedman, 1997; Wood-
to just how much genuineness and realism exist. Gelso (2002) has
house, Schlosser, Crook, Ligiero, & Gelso, 2003). Positive trans-
theorized that an ebb and flow occurs in the magnitude of the real
ference relates to such variables in the same manner as does
relationship. The magnitude or degree of genuineness should in- negative transference, but the correlations are consistently of lower
crease linearly as the work progresses, whereas the magnitude of magnitude. Thus, we particularly expected a relation between real
realism may be greater at the beginning and end phases of treat- relationship and negative transference. Furthermore, because we
ment. Valence, conversely, refers to how positive versus negative expected client self-insight to allow for the working through of
the participants’ feelings are toward the realistically experienced transference and in this way to foster realistic perceptions, we
other. Such feelings may range from highly positive to highly hypothesized that there would be a positive relation between
negative. Positive feelings may take the form of liking, caring, therapist-rated client self-insight and real relationship. We ex-
respect, or even nonromantic loving. Negative reactions, too, may pected this relation to be most salient for the kind of insight often
occur within the real relationship, and in some instances the real referred to as emotional insight rather than intellectual insight (see
relationship may even be largely negative. A negative real rela- definitions under Measures). This expectation was based on the
tionship, especially early in the therapy, may in fact be responsible finding that emotional insight more powerfully influences the
for many so-called premature terminations (Gelso, 2002, 2003, therapeutic process than does intellectual insight (Gelso & Hayes,
2004). 1998; Gelso et al., 1991, 1997).
Despite the fact that the real relationship has been discussed for A thread running through the literature on real relationship over
many years (see Couch, 1999), next to no empirical research has the decades is that a good or positive real relationship leads to
been done on it. Studies have looked at elements of the real more effective psychotherapy. At the level of the therapy session,
relationship (e.g., genuineness), but we could locate only one study this effect of the real relationship should be manifested in terms of
that directly examined real relationship (Eugster & Wampold, session outcome. That is, the stronger and more positive the real
1996), and that study strongly supported the importance of the relationship is, the more positive should be the outcomes of any
client-rated real relationship in the outcomes of treatment. Gelso given therapy session. Thus, we posited that there would be a
and Hayes (1998) have suggested that the most fundamental rea- positive relation between session outcome and real relationship.
son for the lack of research has been the absence of a reliable, The elements of session outcome that we examined are depth and
valid, and economical measure. The main purpose of the present smoothness (Stiles, 1980; Stiles, Gordon, & Lani, 2002).
study is to develop such a measure and to provide beginning The relations discussed so far have pertained to convergent
reliability and validity data on the measure. validity. Theoretically, they reflect constructs to which a valid
642 GELSO ET AL.

measure of real relationship ought to relate. However, a valid experience (M ⫽ 1.98 years of experience since last degree). The mean age
measure of real relationship also should not relate to certain was 31.60 years (SD ⫽ 9.10). About half (49%) saw their designated client
constructs, thus reflecting discriminant validity. The construct at the counseling center of the university at which these graduate students
chosen to assess discriminant validity in the present study is social were studying, whereas the remaining half saw their client in a range of
other settings (inpatient settings, mental health clinics, etc.). In terms of
desirability. In developing a therapist measure of the real relation-
theoretical orientation, on the same 5-point ratings scale as the profession-
ship, we sought to write items that would not pull for impression
als used (1 ⫽ low and 5 ⫽ high), the following means emerged: cognitive–
management and socially desirable responses. However, in any behavioral ⫽ 3.69 (SD ⫽ 1.03), humanistic– existential ⫽ 3.78 (SD ⫽
value-laden measure, such as the one we created, the relation to 1.09), and psychoanalytic–psychodynamic ⫽ 2.96 (SD ⫽ 1.11). Thus, the
social desirability must be assessed. It was hypothesized that real graduate student sample was also theoretically diverse, although these
relationship would be unrelated to social desirability. students favored a humanistic orientation more than did the professional
The theorized elements of the real relationship, genuineness and sample.
realism, are seen as interrelated. A question may be raised about It should be noted that demographic data were not gathered on the clients
the degree to which they are correlated and whether, in fact, the on whom the therapists based their ratings. However, the therapist and
two may be empirically separable. Thus, we sought to determine setting data as well as the client selection method (see Procedure) strongly
suggest that the large majority of clients were White and that most were
whether the two theorized factors were empirically separable. The
female.
research question was as follows: Does a two-factor solution
including genuineness and realism provide a better fit to the data
than a one-factor model? Item Development
Each member of our research team wrote items believed to be reflective
Method of the real relationship. An effort was made to write items in theoretically
neutral language, and items that seemed slanted toward a particular theory
Participants (e.g., psychoanalysis) were either deleted or reworded during the item
development process. All team members were familiar with the writing of
Two different samples were used in the present study: a professional Gelso and his collaborators on this topic (Gelso & Carter, 1985, 1994;
sample (n ⫽ 159) and a sample of graduate students (n ⫽ 51). The graduate Gelso & Hayes, 1998), and the writing of items followed the conceptual-
sample was included to enhance external validity, as a significant amount ization of these authors. Three of the six team members were doctoral-level
of therapy is conducted during graduate education. The professional sam- counseling psychologists with 6 to 30 years of postdoctoral clinical expe-
ple was randomly selected from the 2000 membership directory of Divi- rience. The remaining three were postmaster’s-level doctoral students in
sions 29 (Psychotherapy) and 42 (Independent Practice) of the American counseling psychology.
Psychological Association (APA). The graduate student sample consisted Items were written to capture realism and genuineness as two funda-
of students from master’s and doctoral programs in counseling and coun- mental elements of the real relationship. Furthermore, both realism and
seling psychology at two universities in the northeastern United States. genuineness were subdivided into valence (degree of positive vs. negative
It should be underscored that the professional sample was divided into feelings and perceptions) and magnitude (how much real relationship
two subsamples. One of these (n ⫽ 80) was used in the item development existed). Finally, each item was written so that it pertained either to the
phase of the study, whereas the other subsample (n ⫽ 79) was used in the therapist, to the client, or to the relationship. Thus, all real relationship
validation phase. The graduate student sample, conversely, was used only items rated by therapists refer to the therapists, their clients, or the rela-
in the validation phase. These phases and subsamples are further clarified tionship between the therapists and their clients.
in the Item Development and Procedure sections. Initially, 130 items were generated by the research team members. Four
Regarding the professional sample, 155 of the 159 participants had a members from the research team then sought to eliminate items that were
PhD, 1 had an MD, and 3 had master’s degrees. Sixty-five (41%) were repetitive, unclear, or not consistent with theory on the real relationship.
female, and 94 (59%) were male. Regarding race/ethnicity, of those who This process reduced the item pool to 99. At this point, these four members
completed this index, 147 (96%) were White/Caucasian, 3 were Asian designated each item as belonging to 1 of 12 categories (genuineness or
American, 3 were Latino, and 3 were of other ethnicities; 3 did not reply. realism ⫻ valence or magnitude ⫻ self, other, or relationship). The 99
The sample was highly experienced, with an average of 22.0 years (SD ⫽ items were submitted to the two remaining team members to classify into
8.9) of practice experience since their last degree. The mean age was 55.0 the categories of realism or genuineness and then valence or magnitude.
years (SD ⫽ 8.8). Items were kept only if the two subgroups agreed on their classification
Most of the therapists in the professional sample worked with the client into categories of realism and genuineness. On the basis of this procedure,
used for the current study (see Procedure) in an independent practice along with further elimination of redundancy, the measure was reduced to
setting (n ⫽ 140; 88%), although a few saw the client in a university 44 items (23 reflecting the realism dimension and 21 reflecting
counseling center (n ⫽ 3) or a mental health center (n ⫽ 6). When genuineness).
therapists were asked to rate their belief in and adherence to three general The 44-item version was then mailed to 200 randomly selected members
theory clusters on a 5-point scale (1 ⫽ low and 5 ⫽ high), the following of either Division 29 or Division 42 of APA. Two mailings and a postcard
mean ratings emerged: cognitive– behavioral ⫽ 3.90 (SD ⫽ 0.98), human- reminder yielded usable returns from 80 participants. (Specifics of the
istic– existential ⫽ 3.20 (SD ⫽ 1.07), and psychoanalytic–psychodynamic return rate are reported in the Procedure section.) Therapists were asked to
⫽ 3.06 (SD ⫽ 1.21). Thus, the professional sample favored cognitive– select the most recent client they had seen during the past week with whom
behavioral therapy over humanistic– existential and dynamic–analytic ap- they had worked for at least five sessions. Our aim in asking therapists to
proaches, although it appears that the sample was highly diverse use the most recent client was to eliminate the potential confound of
theoretically. therapist self-selection, for example, of their most rewarding client. Ther-
Regarding the graduate student sample, 45 of the 51 (94%) had a apists were then asked to “use the following scale to evaluate your
master’s degree, 4 had not yet attained the master’s, and 2 had already relationship with the designated client over the last five sessions.” Items
earned a doctoral degree. Twelve (24%) were male, 38 (75%) were female, were rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to
and 1 did not indicate gender. Regarding ethnicity, 35 (69%) were White/ 5 (strongly agree). Results from the 80 participants, referred to subse-
Caucasian, 8 (16%) were Black, 2 were Asian American, 4 were Latino, quently as our item-development subsample, were used to reduce the Real
and 2 were multiethnic. As expected, this sample had limited clinical Relationship Inventory—Therapist Form (RRI–T) to its final number of 24
MEASURING THE REAL RELATIONSHIP 643

items (12 realism and 12 genuineness). The item reduction procedure is present study, the short form of the WAI (WAI–S) was used. This is a
described below. 12-item version, with 4 items per subscale. Therapists rated items in
In determining the final items for the measure, we paid particular reference to the overall evaluation of the last five sessions with a desig-
attention to (a) item–total correlations within the two subscales, Realism nated client (see Procedure). The WAI–S was originally proposed by
and Genuineness, and (b) the reduction in Cronbach’s alpha when an item Tracey and Kokotovic (1989), who provided initial reliability and validity.
was deleted within the two subscales. We decided a priori to reduce these Since that initial investigation, several studies have provided further sup-
two subscales to 12 items each. This number permitted sound reliability port for the WAI–S. Most recently, Busseri and Tyler (2003) have sup-
while also enhancing ease of usage. The rules we followed in reducing the ported the interchangeability of the short and original forms for both the
scale from 44 to 24 items were (a) no item–total correlation (within the therapist and the client versions for the total and subscale scores. In the
subscale the item was in) would be less than .40 if an item were to be present study, the therapist version (total score) of the WAI–S was used.
maintained, (b) an item would be subject to elimination if the alpha was not For our combined validation sample of doctoral-level professionals who
reduced when the item was eliminated, and (c) items with the highest completed the WAI–S (n ⫽ 41; see Procedure for clarification of different
item–total correlations would be maintained provided that there was at samples sizes for different measures) and graduate student counselors (n ⫽
least one item in each of six cells per subscale. These six cells were 51), the internal consistency estimate (Cronbach’s alpha) was .85.
determined by ratings of magnitude and valence ⫻ self (therapist), other Transference and insight. Therapists rated their designated client’s
(client), and relationship. It should be noted that if items initially among the insight and transference. Transference was rated for the last session and the
top 12 for a subscale had to be eliminated to fill cells, we used redundancy last five sessions, whereas insight was rated for only the last session. For
as the decisive factor in determining what item to eliminate. This occurred transference, three single items were used that asked therapists to rate their
only for the Realism subscale, in which we eliminated two magnitude– client’s positive transference, negative transference, and transference
other items to fill magnitude–self and valence–self cells. The two added amount. For insight, two items were used, one requesting that therapists
items did have item–total correlations that were lower than .40 (rs ⫽ .34 rate intellectual insight, and the other asking them to rate emotional insight.
and .37), but we believed this a worthwhile trade-off. The alpha coeffi- The transference items were initially used by Graff and Luborsky (1977) in
cients for the subscales were as follows: Realism ⫽ .89, Genuineness ⫽ their study of the process of psychoanalysis, and the insight items were
.87. The correlation between the two subscales was .75, suggesting a great developed by Gelso et al. (1991) in their study of session outcome.
deal of overlap between them (in fact, when taken together, the final 24 Therapists were provided a definition of each of these two constructs
items exhibited an overall Cronbach’s alpha of .93). (transference and insight) and asked to use these definitions in their ratings.
At the same time that the 44-item measure was mailed to members of Ratings were on a 5-point scale, where 1 ⫽ none or slight and 5 ⫽ very
APA’s Divisions 29 and 42, a back-translation (Dawis, 1987) of these much. Definitions were as follows.
items was conducted. The raters were five doctoral students in clinical
psychology from a university in Washington, DC. These raters were given Transference: The degree to which the client is dealing with material
clear written and verbal definitions, descriptions, and examples of realism that is overtly or covertly related to the therapist. This material must
and genuineness. They were then asked to place all items into the catego- be a manifestation of or displacement from an early important rela-
ries of either realism or genuineness. Of the 24 items that had been arrived tionship(s). The previous person (or transference source), however,
at through the above psychometric analysis, 19 were correctly categorized need not be mentioned; he or she may be inferred because of, for
by at least four of five raters, and only 1 item was correctly categorized by example, the presence of distortion, strong affect, inappropriate affect,
less than three of the five. Because that item had a very high item–total and so forth. Positive transference may be seen as positive reactions
correlation, made theoretical sense, and was not redundant with other to or perceptions of the therapist that are transference based, whereas
items, we decided to maintain it in the final form. negative transference is reflected in negative reactions to or percep-
The final 24-item version of the RRI–T is presented in the Appendix. As tions of the therapist that are transference based.
can be seen, 6 items reflect therapists’ ratings of their own reactions, 11
reflect their ratings of their clients’ reactions, and 7 reflect their ratings of Insight: The extent to which the client displays accurate understand-
the client–therapist relationship. For the final 24 items, the means tended to ing of material being explored. Understanding may be of the relation-
be high (overall mean for the 24 items ⫽ 3.75), and 3 items had means over ship, client’s functioning outside of counseling, or aspects of the
4.25. Although from a psychometric perspective it would have been client’s dynamics and behavior. Intellectual insight reflects an under-
preferable to develop items or scales yielding lower scores, we maintained standing of cause– effect relationships but lacks depth because it does
the current items and scales because of the view that, on the average, real not connect to affect underlying the client’s thoughts. Emotional
relationship in actual therapy practice should be on the positive or stronger insight connects affect and intellect; the client is thus connected
side. In addition, the 3 items with the highest scores contained content that emotionally to his or her understanding.
was desirable and behaved satisfactorily psychometrically—for example,
satisfactory standard deviations and item–total correlations that were ac- Although single-item measures can be problematic psychometrically,
ceptable. (Copies of a table containing item means, standard deviations, they have the advantage in field research of being highly practical and
and item–total correlations for the final 24 items are available from Charles economical. More important, these particular items on transference and
J. Gelso.) insight have been used in a series of studies, and in each case, they have
behaved in theoretically sensible ways, which thus supports their construct
validity (Gelso et al., 1991, 1997; Graff & Luborsky, 1977; Kivlighan,
Measures
1995; Multon et al., 1996; Woodhouse et al., 2003). In the initial studies,
Working Alliance Inventory (WAI; Horvath & Greenberg, 1986, 1989). Luborsky and his colleagues (Graff & Luborsky, 1977; Luborsky,
The WAI is composed of 36 items and contains three subscales (12 items Crabtree, Curtis, Ruff, & Mintz, 1975; Luborsky, Graff, Pulver, & Curtis,
each) measuring client–therapist agreement on goals, agreement on tasks, 1973) provided evidence for moderate interjudge agreement between ther-
and bond (Bordin, 1979). Parallel forms exist for therapists and clients, and apist ratings of transference and those of outside raters. Subsequent studies
participants rate items on a 7-point scale anchored by 1 (never) and 7 found the transference items to relate (a) interactively with insight in
(always). This measure has been rigorously studied over the years and has affecting session (Gelso et al., 1991) and treatment (Gelso et al., 1997)
been found to be highly reliable (Kivlighan & Shaughnessy, 2000). It has outcome, (b) to a multi-item measure of transference (Multon et al., 1996),
also been soundly validated through the demonstration of its relation to and (c) to clients’ pretherapy schema ratings for their mother and father
treatment outcomes, client characteristics, and therapist technical activity (Patton et al., 1995). Counselor and supervisor ratings of transference on
(see Constantino et al., 2002; Kivlighan & Shaughnessy, 2000). In the these items have also been found to be highly intercorrelated (Kivlighan,
644 GELSO ET AL.

1995). As noted, the insight items, too, have received sound support in Item development subsample. Two hundred of the 400 recipients were
terms of their interactive effects with transference on session and treatment randomly selected to receive the cover letter and only the 44-item RRI–T.
outcome. Those from this pool who completed our measures were used to reduce the
Session outcome. Two of the subscales of the Session Evaluation measure to 24 items.
Questionnaire (SEQ; Stiles & Snow, 1984), Depth and Smoothness, were Validation subsample. The 200 remaining professionals in the pool
used as measures of session outcome. The SEQ consists of 24 bipolar formed the validation subsample, which was used to validate the 24-item
adjective pairs that tap four constructs: depth, smoothness, positivity, and measure. The research plan for the validation study was to ask potential
arousal. However, Depth and Smoothness are the two that are most often participants to complete only a portion of the measures. We took this
used and the subscales that have received the greatest empirical support. approach to ask less of the participants and thus increase return rate and
Therapists rated their last session with a designated client. Each item external validity. One hundred of the remaining 200 professional partici-
requires a rating on a 7-point semantic differential scale, where higher pants were sent the cover letter and the RRI–T as well as the WAI and the
scores reflect greater depth and smoothness. The Depth subscale taps Transference and Insight Questionnaire. A second group of 100 received
respondents’ perceptions of sessions as deep, powerful, valuable, full, and the cover letter, the RRI–T, and the SEQ along with the Transference and
special; the Smoothness subscale taps perceptions of sessions as smooth, Insight Questionnaire.
relaxed, pleasant, easy, and comfortable. Counselors often equate depth Ninety-five respondents of the 400 returned the packet after the first
with good sessions, whereas clients give more equal weight to smoothness mailing. Three weeks after this mailing, a reminder postcard was sent to all
(Stiles et al., 2002). Internal consistency estimates for both subscales are participants who had not yet returned their surveys. This reminder yielded
consistently in the vicinity of .90, and construct validation has been an additional 51 surveys over the next 4-week period. At this point, we also
extensively supported through documented relations of both Depth and received 52 surveys that were undeliverable. A second mailing of the
Smoothness to a wide range of counseling process and outcome measures packets was sent to the remaining 202 recipients, along with a bookmark
(Stiles et al., 2002). In the present study, the values of coefficient alpha for as a gift. The second mailing yielded a return of 44 surveys, for a total of
our validation sample of graduate students (n ⫽ 51) and professionals who 190 completed packets. Thus, the return rate for the study was 55%, or 190
completed the SEQ (n ⫽ 39; see Procedure for clarification of sample out of 348 packets that were delivered. Of the 190 packets, however, 31
sizes) were .82 for Depth and .83 for Smoothness. were not usable. Approximately 85% of these were from recipients who
Social desirability. The Marlowe–Crowne Social Desirability Scale reported not having seen a client during the last week, as specified in the
(SDS; Crowne & Marlowe, 1960; Reynolds, 1982) is a 13-item self-report instructions. Thus, usable returns were obtained from 159 of those who
short version of the 33-item SDS. The short version was used to enhance were surveyed.
therapist cooperation and because it has received sound support in terms of Data were also collected over a 3-week period from master’s and
reliability and validity. The SDS uses a true–false format and aims to doctoral students in counseling and counseling psychology programs at
identify individuals who describe themselves as possessing culturally sanc- two northeastern universities. Students who were enrolled in counseling
tioned characteristics considered rare in the general population to obtain practicum classes were invited to participate in the study. They were
approval from others. This measure has been used extensively to assess informed of the criterion for inclusion (having seen a client during the last
social desirability as a response tendency in studies implementing self- week) and assured that participation was voluntary and confidential. These
report formats. High scores are indicative of more socially desirable students were asked to complete the questionnaires with respect to the last
responding, reflecting both impression management and self-deception client they interviewed during the past week for at least five sessions or the
(Paulhus, 1991). The 13-item version developed by Reynolds (1982) has an latest client with whom they had worked for a minimum of five sessions.
acceptable level of internal consistency, a very strong relation with the Students were given a packet of measures along with a self-addressed,
longer version of the SDS (i.e., greater than .90), and a significant relation stamped envelope in which to return the completed questionnaires. Of the
with the Edwards Social Desirability Scale (O’Grady, 1988). For the 73 students surveyed in class, 51 (70%) agreed to participate and com-
present study, the internal consistency estimate based on the graduate pleted all measures, including the Marlowe–Crowne SDS, which was not
student sample (n ⫽ 51) was .80. completed by the professional sample. Responses from the graduate stu-
dent sample were used only in the validation (not the item reduction) phase
Procedure of the study.

As indicated, the sample consisted of both professionals and graduate Results


students. Below we describe the procedure for the professional sample first
and then for the graduate students. Reliability
Using the 2000 membership directory of the APA, we randomly selected Earlier we reported reliability estimates based on the item de-
200 names from Division 29 (Psychotherapy) and 200 from Division 42 velopment subsample of professionals (n ⫽ 80). For the validation
(Independent Practice). When we encountered individuals who had mem- subsample of professionals (n ⫽ 79) and for the graduate students
bership in both divisions or had international addresses, we selected the
(n ⫽ 51) on whom the following validity analyses were performed,
name immediately following the name that was to have been selected. Each
potential participant was assigned a number for confidentiality purposes.
the coefficient alpha values were as follows: Realism ⫽ .79,
One week prior to the survey mailing, we sent out a postcard to all 400 Genuineness ⫽ .83, total score ⫽ .89. The differences in internal
individuals notifying them that they would be receiving a mailing within a consistencies between the professional and graduate samples were
few days. The survey mailing consisted of a personalized cover letter minuscule, with the exception of the Realism subscale. On that
explaining the purposes of the study and giving directions for completing subscale, the alpha for the professional sample was .81, whereas
and mailing the survey back to us; a self-addressed, stamped envelope; and for the student sample it was .72. It should be noted that because
the surveys, which were also coded on the front page with a number from real relationship has been conceptualized as fluid over the course
1 to 400. In the cover letter, participants were asked to consider their of treatment, stability analysis (e.g., test–retest coefficients) did
relationship with the last client they saw during the past week and with not seem called for or appropriate theoretically.
whom they had worked for a minimum of five sessions. They were asked
to complete the measures with that client in mind. All cover letters were Validity Analysis
personally signed by the three doctoral-level investigators. The pool of
professionals was divided into two subsamples, an item development The bivariate correlation coefficients between the RRI–T and
subsample and a validation subsample, each consisting of 200 individuals. the measures against which the RRI–T was validated are presented
MEASURING THE REAL RELATIONSHIP 645

in Table 1. We maintained the subscale analyses for Realism and Mardia’s (1970) coefficient of multivariate kurtosis to be 1.5822.
Genuineness and present correlations for both these subscales as This estimate was well below the recommended threshold of
well as for the total score. Note also that, because therapist expe- 3.0000, making normal-theory maximum likelihood estimation a
rience level (years of therapy experience), as indicated on the reasonable choice for method of estimation. Sample size recom-
demographic questionnaire, was only negligibly related to the mendations for maximum likelihood CFA are highly varied, in-
main variables in the study, the professional and graduate student cluding 5–10 cases per variable (e.g., Bryant & Yarnold, 1995), 5
samples were combined. cases per model parameter (Bentler & Chou, 1987), and potentially
As can be seen in Table 1, consistent with expectations, the considerably more liberal recommendations on the basis of loading
RRI–T total score and one or both subscale scores correlated strength (Marsh, Hau, Balla, & Grayson, 1998). The CFA models
significantly ( p ⬍ .05) with the WAI, the SEQ Depth and Smooth- tested use six variables, and the oblique model specifically requires
ness subscales, emotional insight, intellectual insight, and negative the estimation of 13 parameters; by any of the above sample size
transference for the last session and the last five sessions. How- guidelines, the current validation sample of 131 is more than
ever, the RRI–T did not correlate with positive transference when adequate.
rated in either the last or the last five sessions. Finally, as pre- Two primary models served as the focus of the CFA analyses,
dicted, the RRI–T did not correlate with the measure of social a two-factor oblique model (i.e., with factors allowed to covary)
desirability. and a one-factor model. Other potential alternatives to CFA mod-
els include so-called higher order models, the most common of
Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) which posit that the covariation between Realism and Genuineness
Our research question focuses on whether a model consisting of is itself the result of another factor that has no measured indicator
two factors, Genuineness and Realism, represents the best fit to the variables itself; however, in the current case, such a model is
data. We used a CFA to test the underlying factor structure of the locally underidentified given that there are only two first-order
RRI–T for the validation subsample of 80 professionals (the 79 factors (i.e., Realism and Genuineness). A second type of higher
noted earlier plus 1 respondent who completed only the RRI–T) order CFA model, a bilevel CFA model, has a general factor
and 51 graduate students. However, rather than using the 24 influencing all measured indicators and then specific factors af-
individual items as indicator variables of the two factors, which fecting the subsets of theoretically related variables (i.e., Realism
can be quite taxing with somewhat smaller samples, we followed variables and Genuineness variables). Such a bilevel model was
the recommendation of Bagozzi and Heatherton (1994) to create actually attempted with the current data but suffered from multiple
aggregated variables or item parcels within each factor. Specifi- irreparable offending estimates (specifically, Heywood cases). As
cally, we randomly parsed the 12 Realism items into three subsets a result, the one-factor and two-factor oblique models remained
of 4 items, each forming a new indicator variable consisting of a our primary focus.
subtotal of the 4 items. We repeated the same process for the All analyses were executed via EQS 6.1 (Bentler, 2004), and
Genuineness items, which thus yielded three indicators of each of data-model fit was evaluated with multiple fit indices, as per the
the two factors of interest. The three resulting Realism composites recommendations of Hu and Bentler (1999). These authors found,
had alpha coefficients of .63, .51. and .53, whereas the three on the basis of their extensive simulation study, the standardized
resulting Genuineness composites had alpha coefficients of .69, root-mean-square residual (SRMR), the comparative fit index
.62. and .53. (CFI), and the root-mean-square error of approximation to be
Prior to conducting the CFA, we used the structural equation particularly useful. These values as well as the model chi-square,
modeling software EQS 6.1 (Bentler, 2004) to assess the distribu- degrees of freedom, and EQS’s version of the Akaike information
tional characteristics of the data. Specifically, using the data for the criterion are reported for both models in Table 2. According to one
validation subsample, we computed the normalized estimate of of Hu and Bentler’s recommended joint criteria guidelines, SRMR

Table 1
Pearson Correlations of the Real Relationship Inventory—Therapist Form (RRI–T) (Realism, Genuineness, and Total) With Validation
Measures

RRI–T scales

Measure n Realism Genuineness Total

Working Alliance Inventory 92 .32** .55** .47**


Session Evaluation Questionnaire—Depth 90 .30** .38** .36**
Session Evaluation Questionnaire—Smoothness 90 .42** .40** .43**
Emotional insight 130 .21* .31** .28**
Intellectual insight 130 .18* .20* .21*
Negative transference—last session 130 ⫺.27** ⫺.26** ⫺.29**
Negative transference—last five sessions 130 ⫺.16 ⫺.21* ⫺.20*
Positive transference—last session 130 .13 .09 .12
Positive transference—last five sessions 130 .09 .05 .08
Social Desirability Scale 51 .06 .10 .08

* p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01.
646 GELSO ET AL.

Table 2
Confirmatory Factor Analysis Results

Model ␹2 df AIC SRMR RMSEA(90% CI) CFI

One-factor model 21.459 9 3.459 .043 .103(.047, .160) .966


Two-factor oblique model 21.324 8 5.324 .043 .113(.056, .172) .964

Note. AIC ⫽ Akaike information criterion; SRMR ⫽ standardized root-mean-square residual; RMSEA ⫽ root-mean-square error of approximation; CI ⫽
confidence interval; CFI ⫽ comparative fit index.

should be .10 or less, and CFI should be at least .96. As seen in emotional and intellectual insight, and social desirability. Because
Table 2, for both models, the fit is deemed acceptable by these predictions were made about these relations and given the large
modern (and quite rigorous) criteria. The standardized loadings for number of analyses conducted, we set the alpha level (error rate)
the one-factor model, all of which were statistically significant conservatively at .01. In Table 3, we present the means and
( p ⬍ .05), were .799, .732, and .664 for the Realism indicators and standard deviations for each of the variables as well as the inter-
.810, .762, and .710 for the Genuineness indicators. The standard- correlations among the variables.
ized loadings for the two-factor oblique model differed only by
thousandths: .805, .734, and .666 for the Realism indicators and Discussion
.814, .763, and .713 for the Genuineness indicators (all with p ⬍
.05). Further, for the two-factor model, the Realism–Genuineness It appears that the RRI–T has sound reliability and beginning
interfactor correlation was a statistically significant .986. validity. It correlates as it should with therapists’ ratings of the
Because a hierarchical relation exists between the two models, working alliance, the depth and smoothness of sessions with the
with the one-factor model nested within the two-factor oblique client who was rated, and therapists’ ratings of both the intellectual
model, their fit may be compared statistically by a likelihood ratio and emotional insight displayed by the client. In addition, the
(chi-square difference) test. The difference in the models’ chi- RRI–T, as theorized, was negatively associated with negative
square values was a minuscule 0.135 with one degree of freedom, transference. Thus, the stronger the real relationship was over the
which is not statistically significant ( p ⫽ .713). The inference, last five sessions in a given therapy, the lower was the amount of
then, is that the additional complexity of distinguishing between negative transference during both the last session and the last five
Realism and Genuineness is not actually necessary, as one might sessions. The RRI–T also displayed beginning evidence of dis-
surmise from their interfactor correlation of .986 in the two-factor criminant validity in that it did not correlate with social
oblique model. Thus, we retained the one-factor model as a highly desirability.
satisfactory and simpler explanation of the relations among the six Each of the relations just noted is theoretically crucial in vali-
indicator variables. dating a real relationship measure. For example, because the
working alliance has been theorized as emerging from the real
relationship (Greenson, 1967), a fairly strong relation should exist
Additional Analyses
between the two. However, these two constructs ought to be
Correlation coefficients were computed among the 12 main empirically distinguishable from one another. The correlation be-
variables, other than the RRI–T total or subscale scores. Included tween real relationship and working alliance that emerged (r ⫽
in this analysis were therapist experience level, theoretical orien- .47) appears to fit this expectation of both overlap and distinguish-
tation, working alliance, SEQ Depth and Smoothness subscales, ability. In addition, given the close theoretical link between real
positive and negative transference scores for the last session, client relationship and working alliance, with the latter seen as emerging

Table 3
Sample Sizes, Means, Standard Deviations, and Interconnections for and Among Major Variables

Measure n M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12

1. Experience 129 14.34 12.99 — .09 ⫺.11 .11 .02 ⫺.25 .18 .15 .17 .15 ⫺.05 .04
2. Psychoanalytic 131 2.95 1.19 — .12 ⫺.38** ⫺.17 .18 .07 .29** .25** .11 ⫺.04 ⫺.09
3. Humanistic 130 3.59 1.06 — ⫺.10 .14 .12 ⫺.00 .00 ⫺.02 ⫺.06 ⫺.10 .00
4. Cognitive–behavioral 131 3.87 1.00 — .07 ⫺.10 .10 ⫺.15 ⫺.03 ⫺.01 .01 ⫺.04
5. Working alliance 92 5.37 1.09 — .14 .16 ⫺.25 ⫺.00 .13 .39** ⫺.02
6. SEQ—Depth 89 5.22 0.76 — ⫺.01 .02 ⫺.07 .30** .26 ⫺.22
7. SEQ—Smoothness 88 4.45 1.25 — ⫺.35** ⫺.00 ⫺.01 .19 .18
8. Negative transference 131 1.82 0.95 — .28** .06 ⫺.11 ⫺.10
9. Positive transference 130 2.82 1.21 — .10 .15 ⫺.01
10. Intellectual insight 131 3.52 0.90 — .41** .00
11. Emotional insight 131 2.94 1.01 — .01
12. Social desirability 51 7.75 3.37 —

Note. SEQ ⫽ Session Evaluation Questionnaire.


** p ⬍ .01.
MEASURING THE REAL RELATIONSHIP 647

from the former for the purposes of the work of therapy, it remains sessions combined that they had with the designated client. We
to be seen whether each of the two possesses incremental validity believed that such a rating provides a more stable estimate of real
in relation to the other in predicting therapy-relevant criteria. For relationship than would ratings of, for example, one session and a
example, does real relationship predict aspects of process and more precise estimate than would assessments of real relationship
outcome beyond the predictive power of working alliance? This without the mention of a session number. It is our expectation that
key question must await future empirical scrutiny. the findings derived under this method will hold up when partic-
With respect to transference, the real relationship theoretically ipants are asked to rate the last session, the therapy in general, or
reflects lack of or at least low levels of transference, especially any other particular cluster of sessions. However, further research
negative transference. Thus, a valid measure of real relationship is required to empirically test this expectation.
must be associated with lower levels of negative transference, A key limitation of the current work resides in the exclusive use
which is precisely what was found in the present study. It should of therapist ratings. Because our aim was to develop the therapist
be noted that the negative correlations between real relationship version of the RRI, naturally we sought therapists’ ratings of real
and negative transference were not high, with each accounting for relationship items. However, in the future it will be important to
between about 4% and 9% of the variance in the other, a small to determine how therapists’ ratings of the real relationship correlate
medium effect size. However, a high correlation between the two with clients’ and outside raters’ evaluations of aspects of the
should not, in theory, be expected. That is because transference relationship—for example, client insight, working alliance, and
and real relationship are seen not as the opposites of each other but session outcome. A second limitation is that a very small percent-
rather as existing on two separate dimensions that are negatively age of the professionals sampled were members of racial/ethnic
related to one another (Gelso, 2004; Gelso & Hayes, 1998). minority groups (6%, or 9 of 156 who indicated race/ethnicity).
The degree of insight exhibited by the client seems to be crucial This figure increased, however, when the graduate student sample
to real relationship in the sense that such insight ought to allow was added, given that 31% of those participants were racial/ethnic
clients to appreciate the real relationship and prevent transference minorities. Thus, 12% (25 of 207 who indicated race/ethnicity) of
distortions from damaging or eroding the real relationship. Also, a our total sample were minorities. Future research might profit from
strong real relationship ought to foster insight because of the sense examining how cultural variables interact with the real relationship
of safety engendered by a good relationship. Finally, stronger real and its role in therapy.
relationships ought to lead to better sessions, and vice versa. Thus, Although additional work is needed to provide further validation
the fact that a substantial relation was found between the RRI–T of the RRI–T, the existence of such a measure should finally open
and session outcome (depth and smoothness of sessions) is seen as the door to research on the real relationship. As Gelso and Hayes
a vital source of validation. (1998) have pointed out, “When a measure is at once reliable,
Despite the high correlation between the two subscales of the valid, and convenient, it stirs the minds and hearts of researchers”
RRI–T (Realism and Genuineness) and the fact that the CFA did (p. 27). We hope that the RRI–T serves this function. Numerous
not support a two-factor solution, we believe it wise to maintain questions await empirical scrutiny. Key among them are the fol-
two subscales (Genuineness and Realism) at this time. The ratio- lowing: How do therapists’ perceptions of the real relationship
nale for this is twofold. First, the theory from which the measures relate to clients’ and others’ perceptions? How does the real
emanated is embedded in this dual notion of real relationship, and relationship unfold over the course of brief and longer term ther-
it seems premature to eliminate subscales after these initial anal- apy; for example, is it stable or fluid? How does it differ according
yses. Second and perhaps more important, we can envision situa- to therapists’ theoretical orientation? How does real relationship at
tions in which the two subscales would behave differently from various points in treatment relate to outcome, and is the connection
one another, that is, correlate differentially with certain constructs. to outcome direct or mediated, for example, by working alliance?
In fact, this phenomenon occurred in the current analyses when the How does it interact with other relational constructs, such as
subscales of the RRI–T were correlated with working alliance, working alliance, transference, countertransference, therapist em-
where post hoc analysis revealed that the correlations of working pathy, and both therapist and client attachment styles, in affecting
alliance with the Realism (r ⫽ .32) and Genuineness (r ⫽ .55) process and outcome? Last but certainly not least, what client,
subscales of the RRI–T were significantly different from one therapist, and treatment factors contribute to the formation and
another, t(89) ⫽ 5.13, p ⬍ .01. We also uncovered differential maintenance of a strong real relationship?
correlations in preliminary analyses of an in-process study of the
relation of real relationship to various therapy process and out-
References
come variables (Marmarosh, Gelso, Markin, Doschek, & Majors,
2004). Bagozzi, R. P., & Heatherton, T. F. (1994). A general approach to repre-
As is often the case with relational constructs, one might ask just senting multifaceted personality constructs: Application to state self-
what aspect of real relationship is being measured and what term esteem. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 1,
best captures that aspect. Is it valence? Or is it amount? Because 35– 67.
the RRI–T deliberately contains items tapping both valence (pos- Barber, J. P., Foltz, C., & Weinryb, R. M. (1998). The Central Relationship
itive vs. negative) and magnitude (how much), both of these Questionnaire: Initial report. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 45,
131–142.
qualities must be incorporated into any term used to describe the
Bentler, P. M. (2004). EQS structural equations program. Encino, CA:
aspect being measured. We see the RRI–T as measuring the Multivariate Software.
strength of the real relationship, where strength is a result of both Bentler, P. M., & Chou, C. (1987). Practical issues in structural modeling.
valence and magnitude. Sociological Methods & Research, 16, 78 –117.
It should be noted that participants in the professional sample Beutler, L. E., & Sandowicz, M. (1994). The counseling relationship: What
were asked to complete the RRI–T with respect to the last five is it? Counseling Psychologist, 22, 98 –103.
648 GELSO ET AL.

Bordin, E. S. (1979). The generalizability of the psychoanalytic concept of the Horvath, A. O., & Greenberg, L. S. (1986). The development of the
working alliance. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, and Practice, 16, 252–260. Working Alliance Inventory. In L. Greenberg & W. M. Pinsoff (Eds.),
Bryant, F. B., & Yarnold, P. R. (1995). Principal-components analysis and The psychotherapeutic process (pp. 529 –556). New York: Guilford
exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis. In L. G. Grimm & P. R. Press.
Yarnold (Eds.), Reading and understanding multivariate statistics (pp. Horvath, A. O., & Greenberg, L. S. (1989). The development and valida-
99 –136). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. tion of the Working Alliance Inventory. Journal of Counseling Psychol-
Busseri, M., & Tyler, J. D. (2003). Interchangeability of the Working ogy, 36, 223–233.
Alliance Inventory and the Working Alliance Inventory, Short Form. Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance
Psychological Assessment, 15, 193–197. structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struc-
Constantino, M. J., Castonguay, L. G., & Schut, A. J. (2002). The working tural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 1–55.
alliance: A flagship for the “scientist-practitioner” model in psychother- Kivlighan, D. M. (1995). Similarities and differences among counselor,
apy. In G. S. Tryon (Ed.), Counseling based on process research: supervisor, and observer ratings of individual counseling process. Un-
Applying what we know (pp. 81–131). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. published manuscript, University of Missouri—Columbia.
Couch, A. S. (1999). Therapeutic functions of the real relationship in Kivlighan, D. M., & Shaughnessy, P. (2000). Patterns of working alliance
psychoanalysis. In A. Solnit, P. Neubauer, S. Abrams, & A. Dowling development: A typology of clients’ working alliance ratings. Journal of
(Eds.), The psychoanalytic study of the child: Vol. 54 (p. 130 –167). New Counseling Psychology, 47, 362–371.
Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Luborsky, L. L., Crabtree, L., Curtis, H., Ruff, G., & Mintz, J. (1975). The
Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability concept of “space” for transference for eight psychoanalysts. British
independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, Journal of Medical Psychology, 48, 65–70.
349 –354. Luborsky, L. L., Graff, H., Pulver, S., & Curtis, H. (1973). A clinical-
Dawis, R. V. (1987). Scale construction. Journal of Counseling Psychol- quantitative examination of consensus on the concept of transference.
ogy, 34, 481– 489. Archives of General Psychiatry, 29, 69 –75.
Eugster, S. L., & Wampold, B. E. (1996). Systematic effects of participant Mardia, K. V. (1970). Measures of multivariate skewness and kurtosis with
role on evaluation of the psychotherapy session. Journal of Consulting applications. Biometrika, 57, 519 –530.
and Clinical Psychology, 64, 1020 –1028. Marmarosh, C., Gelso, C., Markin, R., Doschek, E., & Majors, R. (2004,
Freud, A. (1954). The widening scope of indications for psychoanalytic August). Real relationship and the process and outcome of brief psy-
discussion. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 2, chotherapy. Paper presented at the International Conference of the
607– 620. Society for Psychotherapy Research, Rome.
Freud, S. (1937). Analysis terminable and interminable. In J. Strachey Marsh, H. W., Hau, K.-T., Balla, J. R., & Grayson, D. (1998). Is more ever
(Ed.), Standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund too much? The number of indicators per factor in confirmatory factor
Freud (Vol. 23, pp. 209 –253). London: Hogarth Press. analysis. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 33, 181–220.
Gelso, C. J. (2002). Real relationship: The “something more” of psycho- Multon, K. M., Patton, M. J., & Kivlighan, D. M. (1996). Development of
therapy. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 32, 35– 40. the Missouri Identifying Transference Scale. Journal of Counseling
Gelso, C. J. (2003, August). Measuring the real relationship: Theoretical Psychology, 43, 243–252.
foundation. Paper presented at the 111th Annual Convention of the O’Grady, K. E. (1988). The Marlowe-Crowne and Edwards Social Desir-
American Psychological Association, Toronto, Ontario, Canada. ability Scales: A psychometric perspective. Multivariate Behavioral
Gelso, C. J. (2004, June). A theory of the real relationship in psychother- Research, 23, 87–101.
apy. Paper presented at the International Conference of the Society for Patton, M. J., Kivlighan, D. M., & Multon, K. D. (1995). Missouri
Psychotherapy Research, Rome. psychoanalytic counseling research project: First findings. Unpublished
Gelso, C. J., & Carter, J. A. (1985). The relationship in counseling and manuscript, University of Missouri—Columbia.
psychotherapy: Components, consequences, and theoretical antecedents. Paulhus, D. L. (1991). Measurement and control of response bias. In J.
Counseling Psychologist, 13, 155–244. Robinson, P. Shaver, & L. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of personality
Gelso, C. J., & Carter, J. A. (1994). Components of the psychotherapy and social psychological attitudes (pp. 17–59). San Diego, CA: Aca-
relationships: Their interaction and unfolding during treatment. Journal demic Press.
of Counseling Psychology, 41, 296 –306. Reynolds, W. M. (1982). Development of reliable and valid short forms of
Gelso, C. J., & Hayes, J. A. (1998). The psychotherapy relationship: the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. Journal of Clinical
Theory, research, and practice. New York: Wiley. Psychology, 38, 119 –125.
Gelso, C. J., & Hayes, J. A. (2002). The management of countertransfer- Rogers, C. R. (1957). The necessary and sufficient conditions for therapeu-
ence. In J. Norcross (Ed.), Psychotherapy relationships that work: tic personality change. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 21, 95–103.
Therapist contributions and responsiveness to patients (pp. 267–284). Safran, J. D., & Muran, J. C. (1998). Negotiating the therapeutic alliance:
New York: Oxford University Press. A relational treatment manual. New York: Guilford Press.
Gelso, C. J., Hill, C. E., & Kivlighan, D. M. (1991). Transference, insight, Stiles, W. B. (1980). Measurement of the impact of psychotherapy ses-
and the counselor’s intentions during a counseling hour. Journal of sions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48, 176 –185.
Counseling and Development, 69, 428 – 433. Stiles, W. B., Gordon, L. E., & Lani, J. A. (2002). Session evaluation and
Gelso, C. J., Wine, B., Kivlighan, D. M., Jones, A., & Friedman, S. C. the Session Evaluation Questionnaire. In G. S. Tryon (Ed.), Counseling
(1997). Transference, insight, and the course of time-limited therapy. based on process research (pp. 325–343). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 44, 209 –217. Stiles, W. B., & Snow, J. S. (1984). Counseling session impact as viewed
Graff, H., & Luborsky, L. L. (1977). Long-term trends in transference and by novice counselors and their clients. Journal of Counseling Psychol-
resistance: A report on a quantitative-analytic method applied to four ogy, 31, 3–12.
psychoanalyses. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, Tracey, T. J., & Kokotovic, A. M. (1989). Factor structure of the Working
25, 471– 490. Alliance Inventory. Psychological Assessment, 1, 207–210.
Greenberg, L. (1994). What is real in the real relationship? Journal of Woodhouse, S., Schlosser, L. Z., Crook, R., Ligiero, D., & Gelso, C. J.
Counseling Psychology, 41, 307–309. (2003). Client attachment to therapist: Relations to transference and
Greenson, R. R. (1967). The technique and practice of psychoanalysis. client recollections of parental caregiving. Journal of Counseling Psy-
New York: International Universities Press. chology, 50, 395– 408.
MEASURING THE REAL RELATIONSHIP 649

Appendix

The Real Relationship Inventory—Therapist Form

Please complete the items below in terms of your relationship with your client or patient. Use the following
1–5 scale in rating each item, placing your rating in the space adjacent to the item.

Strongly Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly


Agree Disagree
5 4 3 2 1

1. My client is able to see me as a real person separate from my role as a therapist.


2. My client and I are able to be genuine in our relationship.
3. My client feels liking for the “real me.”
4. My client genuinely expresses his/her positive feelings toward me.
5. I am able to realistically respond to my client.
6. I hold back significant parts of myself.
7. I feel there is a “real” relationship between us aside from the professional relationship.
8. My client and I are honest in our relationship.
9. My client has little caring for who I “truly am.”
10. We feel a deep and genuine caring for one another.
11. My client holds back significant parts on him/herself.
12. My client has respect for me as a person.
13. There is no genuinely positive connection between us.
14. My client’s feelings toward me seem to fit who I am as a person.
15. I do not like my client as a person.
16. I value the honesty of our relationship.
17. The relationship between my client and me is strengthened by our understanding of one another.
18. It is difficult for me to express what I truly feel about my client.
19. My client has unrealistic perceptions of me.
20. My client and I have difficulty accepting each other as we really are.
21. My client distorts the therapy relationship.
22. I have difficulty being honest with my client.
23. My client shares with me the most vulnerable parts of him/herself.
24. My client genuinely expresses a connection to me.

Note. Realism subscale items ⫽ 1, 3, 5, 7, 9, 12, 14, 15, 17, 19, 20, 21; Genuineness subscale items ⫽ 2, 4,
6, 8, 10, 11, 13, 16, 18, 22, 23, 24. Reverse scored items ⫽ 6, 9, 11, 13, 15, 18, 19, 20, 21, and 22.

Received August 31, 2004


Revision received January 19, 2005
Accepted January 20, 2005 䡲

You might also like