You are on page 1of 365

Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior :

Cross-Validation, Construct Validation and Application


of a Psychological Taxonomy of Consumer Motives

Nele Geeroms

2007

Dissertation submitted to the Faculty of Economics


and Business Administration, Ghent University, in
fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of
Doctor in Applied Economic Sciences

Promotor : Prof. Dr. P. Van Kenhove


Doctoral Jury :

Prof. Dr. Patrick Van Kenhove


(Ghent University)

Prof. Dr. Maggie Geuens


(Ghent University)

Prof. Dr. Iris Vermeir


(Ghent University & Hogeschool Ghent)

Prof. Dr. Wim Verbeke


(Ghent University)

Prof. Dr. Klaus Grunert


(The Aarhus School of Business, Denmark)

Prof. Dr. Dominika Maison


(Warsaw University, Poland)

Decaan Prof. Dr. Roland Paemeleire


(Ghent University)

Prof. Dr. Eddy Omey


(Ghent University)
Acknowledgments

This dissertation marks the end of a valuable and eventful journey of four years, which began

in October 2002. Looking back upon these years, I would like to seize the opportunity to

thank a number of people, without whom it would have been impossible to have completed

this work successfully.

First of all, I would like to thank my supervisor Patrick Van Kenhove and the late Hendrik

Hendrickx for giving me the chance to start this Ph.D. challenge and to work extensively on

the scientific validation of the motivational framework of Jan Callebaut and colleagues.

Patrick, thanks for encouraging and supporting me during the past four years and for the so

many inspiring discussions at your office. Your expertise as a researcher has been of great

value to me and my work benefited tremendously from your critical suggestions and feedback.

I would like to express my gratitude to Maggie Geuens and Iris Vermeir, members of my

advisory committee, for their helpful comments and constructive suggestions on earlier drafts

of the papers which are included in the present dissertation. I’m also grateful to Klaus

Grunert, Dominika Maison and Wim Verbeke for their willingness to participate in my

examination committee.

Further, I am greatly indebted to the ‘Bijzonder Onderzoeksfonds’ of Ghent University for

providing me a scholarship and all the financial support that was needed to work on this

research project.

V
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

A special word of thanks deserve all the colleagues and former colleagues of the Marketing

department. Leen, Sarah, Tine, Iris, Katrien, Delphine, Anneleen, Geert, Bart, Isabel,

Jonathan, Bernd, Wouter, Anita, Marie, Kristof, Griet, Koen, Dries, Elke, Dirk, Karin, Carole

and Anneke: I really enjoyed the time we spent together! I wish the best for all of you!

Then, there are the friends ‘at home’ whom I didn’t spend enough time with over the last

couple of months. Thank you guys for your friendship and the unforgettable moments over

the years! I’m particularly grateful to Heidi, my ‘alter-ego’, for being my MSN-buddy ;-) and

for keeping me going in times when my mind was troubled.

Finally, my whole family deserves my warmest gratitude for their unconditional love and

support and their never ending beliefs in my capacities. ‘Meter’, ‘grootva’, ‘moe’ and ‘va’:

thank you for being the best grandparents and parents of the world…for so many reasons!

This dissertation is dedicated to the four of you!

Last but definitely not least, I want to say a sincere thank you to Mario for his loving care and

affection day after day. ‘Mar’, none other than you has supported me more during the

completion of this project. Thanks for sharing life’s joys and sorrows with me! Dikke kus!

Nele Geeroms

November, 2006

VI
Table of Contents

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS ....................................................................................................................................V
TABLE OF CONTENTS.......................................................................................................................................I
LIST OF FIGURES ............................................................................................................................................ VI
LIST OF TABLES ............................................................................................................................................ VII
SAMENVATTING.............................................................................................................................................. IX
SUMMARY ......................................................................................................................................................XIII

CHAPTER I : INTRODUCTION ....................................................................................................................... 1

1. SCIENTIFIC ORIENTATION .................................................................................................................. 3


2. RESEARCH OBJECTIVES....................................................................................................................... 5
3. OVERVIEW OF THE STUDIES............................................................................................................... 6
3.1. A TAXONOMY OF CONSUMER MOTIVES: CROSS-VALIDITY .................................................................. 7
3.2. A TAXONOMY OF CONSUMER MOTIVES: CONSTRUCT VALIDITY ......................................................... 8
3.2.1. Consumer Motives & Personal Values............................................................................................ 8
3.2.2. Consumer Motives & Personality Traits......................................................................................... 9
3.3. A TAXONOMY OF CONSUMER MOTIVES: PRACTICAL APPLICATIONS ................................................... 9
3.3.1. Consumer Motives & Health Advertising...................................................................................... 10
3.3.2. Consumer Motives & Ready Meal Consumption Behavior........................................................... 10
REFERENCES.................................................................................................................................................... 12

CHAPTER II : CONSUMER MOTIVES – LITERATURE REVIEW ......................................................... 15

1. INTRODUCTION ..................................................................................................................................... 17
2. CONSUMER MOTIVES .......................................................................................................................... 18
2.1. CONCEPT DEFINITION.......................................................................................................................... 18
2.2. RELATIONSHIP WITH OTHER CONCEPTS............................................................................................... 19
2.2.1. Consumer Motives & Goals .......................................................................................................... 19
2.2.2. Consumer Motives & Values......................................................................................................... 20
2.2.3. Consumer Motives & Personality traits ........................................................................................ 21
3. CONSUMER MOTIVE STRUCTURES ................................................................................................ 22
4. MOTIVATIONAL THEORIES............................................................................................................... 24
4.1. CLASSIC THEORIES OF MOTIVATION .................................................................................................... 24
4.1.1. Freud’s concept of drives.............................................................................................................. 24
4.1.2. Jung’s concept of archetypes ........................................................................................................ 26
4.1.3. Maslow’s hierarchy of needs......................................................................................................... 27
4.1.4. Murray’s list of human needs........................................................................................................ 29

i
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

4.2. RECENT TAXONOMIES ......................................................................................................................... 31


4.2.1. A trio of needs ............................................................................................................................... 31
4.2.2. Wicker et al.’s motive classification.............................................................................................. 32
4.2.3. Ford and Nichols’ goal taxonomy................................................................................................. 33
4.2.4. Chulef et al.’s taxonomy of human goals ...................................................................................... 36
5. ASSESSMENT OF CONSUMER MOTIVES ........................................................................................ 37
5.1. DIRECT SELF-REPORT MEASURES ........................................................................................................ 38
5.2. REYNOLDS AND GUTMAN’S LADDERING TECHNIQUE.......................................................................... 39
5.3. MURRAY’S THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST (TAT) ........................................................................... 40
6. CONCLUSION .......................................................................................................................................... 42
REFERENCES.................................................................................................................................................... 44

A Taxonomy of Conumer Motives : Cross-Validity

CHAPTER III : A TAXONOMY OF CONSUMER MOTIVES THROUGH PREFERRED BRAND


PERSONALITY : EMPIRICAL FINDINGS FOR 11 COUNTRIES ............................................................ 57

1. ABSTRACT ............................................................................................................................................... 59
2. INTRODUCTION ..................................................................................................................................... 60
3. CONCEPTUAL BACKGROUND ........................................................................................................... 61
3.1. RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN MOTIVES AND HUMAN PERSONALITY......................................................... 61
3.2. RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN HUMAN PERSONALITY AND BRAND PERSONALITY ..................................... 63
3.3. RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN MOTIVES AND BRAND PERSONALITY ......................................................... 64
4. EMPIRICAL STUDIES............................................................................................................................ 66
4.1. PERSONALITY TRAIT GENERATION ...................................................................................................... 66
4.2. STIMULI SELECTION ............................................................................................................................ 67
4.3. METHOD ............................................................................................................................................. 67
4.3.1. Subjects ......................................................................................................................................... 67
4.3.2. Procedure...................................................................................................................................... 68
4.3.3. Multidimensional scaling (INDSCAL) .......................................................................................... 69
4.4. RESULTS ............................................................................................................................................. 70
4.4.1. Group Stimulus Space ................................................................................................................... 70
4.4.2. Subject Weight Space .................................................................................................................... 71
5. MOTIVE-BASED INTERPRETATION OF THE PREFERRED BRAND PERSONALITY
STRUCTURE...................................................................................................................................................... 73
6. GENERAL DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS ................................................................................ 79
7. LIMITATIONS AND FUTURE RESEARCH........................................................................................ 82
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS .................................................................................................................................. 83
REFERENCES.................................................................................................................................................... 84
APPENDIX A: LIST OF SCALE ITEMS ........................................................................................................ 93
APPENDIX B: MOTIVATIONAL THEORIES.............................................................................................. 94

ii
Table of Contents

A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives : Construct Validity

CHAPTER IV : RECONSIDERING THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN CONSUMER MOTIVES AND


PERSONAL VALUES........................................................................................................................................ 95

1. ABSTRACT ............................................................................................................................................... 97
2. INTRODUCTION ..................................................................................................................................... 98
2.1. MOTIVE DISPOSITIONS........................................................................................................................ 98
2.2. PERSONAL VALUES ........................................................................................................................... 101
2.3. RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN MOTIVES AND VALUES ............................................................................. 105
3. PRESENT STUDY .................................................................................................................................. 107
3.1. MOTIVE ASSESSMENT....................................................................................................................... 107
3.2. HYPOTHESES .................................................................................................................................... 109
3.3. METHOD ........................................................................................................................................... 111
3.4. RESULTS ........................................................................................................................................... 112
4. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS................................................................................................... 116
5. LIMITATIONS AND FUTURE RESEARCH...................................................................................... 119
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS ................................................................................................................................ 122
REFERENCES.................................................................................................................................................. 123

CHAPTER V : HOW CONSUMER MOTIVES INFLUENCE TRAIT-BEHAVIOR RELATIONSHIPS :


THE CASE OF CONSUMER INNOVATIVENESS AND SELF-CONTROL ........................................... 129

1. ABSTRACT ............................................................................................................................................. 131


2. INTRODUCTION ................................................................................................................................... 132
2.1. A GENERAL TAXONOMY OF CONSUMER MOTIVES ............................................................................. 134
3. STUDY 1: CONSUMER INNOVATIVENESS .................................................................................... 137
3.1. THEORETICAL BACKGROUND AND HYPOTHESES ............................................................................... 137
3.2. METHOD ........................................................................................................................................... 143
3.2.1. Participants ................................................................................................................................. 143
3.2.2. Measures ..................................................................................................................................... 144
3.3. RESULTS ........................................................................................................................................... 148
3.4. DISCUSSION ...................................................................................................................................... 152
4. STUDY 2: SELF-CONTROL................................................................................................................ 153
4.1. THEORETICAL BACKGROUND AND HYPOTHESES ............................................................................... 153
4.2. METHOD ........................................................................................................................................... 159
4.2.1. Participants ................................................................................................................................. 159
4.2.2. Measures ..................................................................................................................................... 160
4.3. RESULTS ........................................................................................................................................... 162
4.4. DISCUSSION ...................................................................................................................................... 165
5. GENERAL CONCLUSIONS AND DISCUSSION .............................................................................. 166
6. LIMITATIONS AND FUTURE RESEARCH...................................................................................... 169
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS ................................................................................................................................ 172
REFERENCES.................................................................................................................................................. 173
APPENDIX A: LIST OF SCALE ITEMS ...................................................................................................... 186

iii
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives : Practical Applications

CHAPTER VI : HEALTH ADVERTISING TO PROMOTE FRUIT AND VEGETABLE INTAKE :


APPLICATION OF MOTIVE-RELATED HEALTH AUDIENCE SEGMENTATION ......................... 189

1. ABSTRACT ............................................................................................................................................. 191


2. INTRODUCTION ................................................................................................................................... 192
3. THEORETICAL BACKGROUND AND HYPOTHESES.................................................................. 194
3.1. A GENERAL TAXONOMY OF CONSUMER MOTIVES ............................................................................. 194
3.2. HEALTH-RELATED MOTIVE ORIENTATIONS ....................................................................................... 197
3.3. HEALTH-RELATED MOTIVE ORIENTATIONS AND FRUIT AND VEGETABLE CONSUMPTION................... 198
3.4. HEALTH-RELATED MOTIVE ORIENTATIONS AND REACTIONS TOWARD HEALTH ADVERTISING .......... 199
4. METHOD................................................................................................................................................. 202
4.1. STIMULI ............................................................................................................................................ 202
4.2. PARTICIPANTS .................................................................................................................................. 203
4.3. MEASURES ........................................................................................................................................ 205
4.3.1. Attitude toward the ad (Aad) and behavioral intention (BI) ....................................................... 205
4.3.2. Health-related motive orientations ............................................................................................. 205
4.3.3. Fruit and vegetable consumption ................................................................................................ 208
5. RESULTS................................................................................................................................................. 209
5.1. MANIPULATION CHECKS .................................................................................................................. 209
5.2. HEALTH AUDIENCE SEGMENTS (H1) ................................................................................................ 211
5.3. FRUIT AND VEGETABLE CONSUMPTION (H2) ................................................................................... 214
5.4. REACTIONS TOWARD HEALTH ADVERTISEMENTS (H3)................................................................... 215
6. DISCUSSION AND RECOMMENDATIONS ..................................................................................... 217
7. LIMITATIONS AND FUTURE RESEARCH...................................................................................... 222
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS ................................................................................................................................ 224
REFERENCES.................................................................................................................................................. 225
APPENDIX A: STIMULI ADVERTISEMENTS .......................................................................................... 234
APPENDIX B: LIST OF SCALE ITEMS ...................................................................................................... 239

CHAPTER VII : UNDERSTANDING READY MEAL CONSUMPTION BEHAVIOR : THE ROLE OF


CONSUMER HEALTH-RELATED MOTIVE ORIENTATIONS ........................................................... 241

1. ABSTRACT ............................................................................................................................................. 243


2. INTRODUCTION ................................................................................................................................... 244
2.1. HEALTH-RELATED MOTIVE ORIENTATIONS ....................................................................................... 245
2.2. RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN HEALTH-RELATED MOTIVE ORIENTATIONS AND GENERAL FOOD BEHAVIOR
248
2.3. RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN HEALTH-RELATED MOTIVE ORIENTATIONS AND READY MEAL CONSUMPTION
BEHAVIOR ....................................................................................................................................................... 251

3. METHOD................................................................................................................................................. 254
3.1. PARTICIPANTS .................................................................................................................................. 254
3.2. MEASURES ........................................................................................................................................ 256
3.2.1. Health-related motive orientations ............................................................................................. 256
3.2.2. General food behavior ................................................................................................................ 258

iv
Table of Contents

3.2.3. Ready meal consumption behavior.............................................................................................. 260


4. RESULTS................................................................................................................................................. 263
4.1. HEALTH AUDIENCE SEGMENTS ........................................................................................................ 263
4.2. GENERAL FOOD BEHAVIOR .............................................................................................................. 267
4.3. READY MEAL CONSUMPTION BEHAVIOR ......................................................................................... 268
5. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS................................................................................................... 279
6. LIMITATIONS AND FUTURE RESEARCH...................................................................................... 283
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS ................................................................................................................................ 284
REFERENCES.................................................................................................................................................. 285
APPENDIX A: LIST OF SCALE ITEMS ...................................................................................................... 291

CHAPTER VIII : SUMMARY & DISCUSSION .......................................................................................... 293

1. RECAPITULATION .............................................................................................................................. 295


1.1. A TAXONOMY OF CONSUMER MOTIVES: CROSS-VALIDITY .............................................................. 296
1.2. A TAXONOMY OF CONSUMER MOTIVES: CONSTRUCT VALIDITY ..................................................... 297
1.3. A TAXONOMY OF CONSUMER MOTIVES: PRACTICAL APPLICATIONS ............................................... 299
2. THEORETICAL IMPLICATIONS ...................................................................................................... 302
2.1. A TAXONOMY OF CONSUMER MOTIVES: CROSS-VALIDITY .............................................................. 302
2.2. A TAXONOMY OF CONSUMER MOTIVES: CONSTRUCT VALIDITY ..................................................... 306
2.3. A TAXONOMY OF CONSUMER MOTIVES: PRACTICAL APPLICATIONS ............................................... 310
3. METHODOLOGICAL IMPLICATIONS ............................................................................................ 312
3.1. A TAXONOMY OF CONSUMER MOTIVES: CROSS-VALIDITY .............................................................. 312
3.2. A TAXONOMY OF CONSUMER MOTIVES: CONSTRUCT VALIDITY ..................................................... 314
3.3. A TAXONOMY OF CONSUMER MOTIVES: PRACTICAL APPLICATIONS ............................................... 316
4. MANAGERIAL IMPLICATIONS........................................................................................................ 317
4.1. A TAXONOMY OF CONSUMER MOTIVES: CROSS-VALIDITY .............................................................. 317
4.2. A TAXONOMY OF CONSUMER MOTIVES: CONSTRUCT VALIDITY ..................................................... 318
4.3. A TAXONOMY OF CONSUMER MOTIVES: PRACTICAL APPLICATIONS ............................................... 319
5. LIMITATIONS AND FUTURE RESEARCH...................................................................................... 322
5.1. A TAXONOMY OF CONSUMER MOTIVES: CROSS-VALIDITY .............................................................. 322
5.2. A TAXONOMY OF CONSUMER MOTIVES: CONSTRUCT VALIDITY ..................................................... 324
5.3. A TAXONOMY OF CONSUMER MOTIVES: PRACTICAL APPLICATIONS ............................................... 327
6. CONCLUDING REMARKS .................................................................................................................. 329
6.1. LINK WITH PREVIOUS TAXONOMIES OF HUMAN MOTIVES.................................................................. 329
6.2. ADDITIONAL CRITICAL REFLECTIONS ................................................................................................ 331
REFERENCES.................................................................................................................................................. 333

v
List of Figures

Figure 2.1 : Maslow’s Hierarchy of Human Needs ................................................................. 28

Figure 3.1 : INDSCAL analysis – Group Stimulus Space ....................................................... 70


Figure 3.2 : INDSCAL analysis – Dimension Weights Plot.................................................... 72
Figure 3.3 : Hypothesized Preferred Brand Personality Dimensions....................................... 73
Figure 3.4 : Callebaut et al.’s (1999) Taxonomy of Consumer Motives ................................. 77

Figure 4.1 : Callebaut et al.’s (1999) Taxonomy of Consumer Motives ............................... 101
Figure 4.2 : Schwartz’ (1992) Value Structure ...................................................................... 105

Figure 5.1 : Callebaut et al.’s (1999) Taxonomy of Consumer Motives ............................... 136
Figure 5.2 : Study 1 – MANOVA results : Consumer Innovativeness .................................. 149
Figure 5.3 : Study 1 – MANOVA results : Adoption of Hedonic Attributes ........................ 150
Figure 5.4 : Study 1 – MANOVA results : Adoption of Utilitarian Attributes...................... 151
Figure 5.5 : Study 2 – MANOVA results : Dispositional Self-Control ................................. 163
Figure 5.6 : Study 2 – MANOVA results : Disciplined Driving Behavior............................ 164

Figure 6.1 : Callebaut et al.’s (1999) Taxonomy of Consumer Motives ............................... 196
Figure 6.2 : Health-related Motive Structure ......................................................................... 212

Figure 7.1 : Geeroms et al.’s Health-related Motive Structure .............................................. 246


Figure 7.2 : Cluster-Health Item Associations (results of correspondence analysis)............. 263

vi
List of Tables

Table 2.1 : Murray’s List of Human Needs ............................................................................. 30


Table 2.2 : Ford and Nichols’ Goal Taxonomy ....................................................................... 35

Table 3.1 : Characteristics of samples...................................................................................... 68


Table 3.2 : INDSCAL analysis – Dimension weights / NRS measures................................... 71
Table 3.3 : Results of Confirmatory Factor Analysis : Measurement Model .......................... 74

Table 4.1 : Motive Definitions and Scale Items..................................................................... 100


Table 4.2 : Value Definitions and Scale Items....................................................................... 103
Table 4.3 : Sample Characteristics (% of respondents, N = 178)........................................... 112
Table 4.4 : Reliability Estimates ............................................................................................ 113
Table 4.5 : Item Correlations.................................................................................................. 114
Table 4.6 : Regression Results ............................................................................................... 115

Table 5.1 : Motive Definitions and Measurement Items........................................................ 135


Table 5.2 : Sample Characteristics Study 1 (% of respondents, N = 210) ............................. 144
Table 5.3 : Four Motive Constructs (Study 1) ....................................................................... 146
Table 5.4 : Factor Analysis – Innovative Mobile Phone Attributes....................................... 147
Table 5.5 : Sample Characteristics Study 2 (% of respondents, N = 173) ............................. 160
Table 5.6 : Four Motive Constructs (Study 2) ....................................................................... 161

Table 6.1 : Motive Definitions ............................................................................................... 195


Table 6.2 : Sample Characteristics (% of respondents, N = 615)........................................... 204
Table 6.3 : Results of Confirmatory Factor Analysis : Measurement Model ........................ 207
Table 6.4 : Means and Standard Deviations for a Five-cluster Solution................................ 210
Table 6.5 : Paired Samples T-test (general ad vs. motive-related ad).................................... 217

Table 7.1 : Five Health Audience Segments .......................................................................... 247


Table 7.2 : Proposed Differences in General Food Behavior Patterns (P2.1-P2.28) ............. 250
Table 7.3 : Proposed Differences in Ready Meal Consumption Patterns (P3.1-P3.44)......... 253
Table 7.4 : Sample Characteristics (% of respondents, N = 1934)......................................... 255
Table 7.5 : Eight Health Constructs ....................................................................................... 257
Table 7.6 : General Food Behavior ........................................................................................ 259
Table 7.7 : Ready Meal Consumption Behavior .................................................................... 261
Table 7.8 : Means and Standard Deviations for a Five-cluster Solution................................ 262
Table 7.9 : ANOVA results General Food Behavior ............................................................. 266
Table 7.10 : Attitude toward Ready Meals and Consumption Frequency ............................. 272
Table 7.11 : ANOVA results Beliefs Ready Meals ............................................................... 273
Table 7.12 : Factor Analysis – Ready Meal Buying Criteria ................................................. 274
Table 7.13 : ANOVA results Buying Criteria........................................................................ 275
Table 7.14 : ANOVA results different Ways of Handling Ready Meals............................... 276
Table 7.15 : Factor Analysis – Attributes Ideal Ready Meal ................................................. 277
Table 7.16 : ANOVA results Ideal Ready Meal ..............................................................278

vii
Samenvatting

Reeds vele jaren staat het beslissingsproces van de consument centraal in het onderzoek naar

consumentengedrag. Een centraal aspect hierbij betreft de vraag op welke manier

consumenten overgaan tot het nemen van aankoopbeslissingen, aangezien dit meestal gepaard

gaat met onzekerheid en het maken van moeilijke afwegingen. Eén manier om

beslissingsprocessen te bestuderen is het beschouwen van de consument als een volkomen

rationeel wezen dat op elk moment in staat is om coherente en weloverwogen beslissingen te

nemen. Hoewel deze cognitieve benadering haar nut reeds heeft bewezen bij het voorspellen

van het keuzegedrag van de consument, toch slaagt ze er niet in om consumentengedrag

volledig te begrijpen gezien de uitsluitende focus op rationele aspecten alleen. Echter, het

gedrag van consumenten is niet volkomen rationeel. Naast observeerbare attitudes en emoties

die gebaseerd zijn op rationeel denken, wordt menselijk gedrag ook geleid door onbewuste

drijfveren en motivaties die niet direct observeerbaar zijn, net zoals het deel van een ‘ijsberg’

dat zich onder het wateroppervlak bevindt. Om een volledig beeld te krijgen van de manier

waarop consumenten zich gedragen en beslissingen nemen is het dus ook belangrijk om de rol

van deze onbewuste drijfveren en motivaties in beschouwing te nemen.

In deze context van het bestuderen van dieperliggende consumentenmotivaties werd in 1999

een tweedimensionaal model ontworpen door Jan Callebaut en collega’s, bestaande uit acht

motivaties die betrekking hebben op de gewenste einddoelen die mensen trachten te bereiken

door middel van het consumeren van goederen en diensten. Heel wat multinationale

bedrijven maken vandaag de dag gebruik van deze taxonomie als middel voor effectieve

ix
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

marketing (bv. Coca-Cola, Johnson and Johnson, Heineken, Nokia en Chefaro). Echter,

niettegenstaande deze praktische relevantie, werd de taxonomie van consumentenmotivaties

van Callebaut en collega’s nog nooit wetenschappelijk gevalideerd of getest. Vandaar de

hoofddoelstelling van dit proefschrift om bij te dragen tot de validatie en kritische

doorlichting van dit model in een marketingcontext.

Drie onderzoeksthema’s werden bestudeerd in dit proefschrift om bovenstaande doelstelling

te bereiken. (1) een Taxonomie van Consumentenmotivaties: Cross-Validiteit. Dit eerste

thema werd behandeld in een eerste paper waarin werd gefocust op het meten van

onderliggende consumentenmotivaties aan de hand van een beperkte set van stabiele (i.e.

betrouwbare) impliciete items. In tweede instantie werd de standvastigheid van de

motivationele structuur van Callebaut et al. (1999) vastgesteld doorheen verschillende

culturen en verschillende productcategorieën. (2) een Taxonomie van Consumentenmotivaties:

Construct Validiteit. Om construct validiteit na te gaan werden in dit onderzoeksthema

consumentenmotivaties gecorreleerd met andere aanverwante concepten in de literatuur. In

een tweede paper werden betekenisvolle relaties vastgesteld tussen consumentenmotivaties en

persoonlijke waarden. Een derde paper op zijn beurt nam de relaties tussen

consumentenmotivaties en twee persoonlijkheidskenmerken onder de loep, namelijk

innovativiteit en zelfcontrole. (3) een Taxonomie van Consumentenmotivaties: Praktische

Toepassingen. In het laatste onderzoeksthema hebben we ons gefocust op de praktische

toepassing van de taxonomie van consumentenmotivaties van Callebaut et al. (1999) als een

nieuw segmentatieparadigma in de context van gezondheidsmarketing. In een vierde paper

werd de bruikbaarheid van dit nieuw paradigma aangetoond bij het ontwikkelen van

effectieve reclameboodschappen voor groenten en fruit, terwijl in een vijfde en laatste paper

x
Samenvatting

deze nieuwe benadering getest werd in de context van de consumptie van kant-en-klare

maaltijden.

Samengevat kunnen we stellen dat dit proefschrift een bijdrage levert tot het beter begrijpen

van motivationeel consumentengedrag door uitgebreid onderzoek te doen naar de (1) cross-

validiteit , (2) construct validiteit en (3) de praktische toepassing van de taxonomie van

consumentenmotivaties van Callebaut et al. (1999) die – zoals we zullen aantonen – een

aantal voordelen heeft ten opzichte van wetenschappelijke taxonomieën die reeds vroeger

ontwikkeld werden.

xi
Summary

For many years, human decision making has been the nexus of the study of consumer

behavior. The major question concerning consumer choice involves how consumers deal

with the decisions they have to make, some of which imply difficult trade-offs and

uncertainties. One approach to studying consumer decisions seem to capture the essence of a

well-educated and rational consumer who seeks information upon which to base consumption

decisions. Though such a cognitive approach has contributed greatly to the prediction of

consumer decisions, it suffers from the fact that it provides an incomplete understanding of

consumers’ actual decision making, given its focus on consumer rationality alone. However,

consumer behavior is not truly rational. Besides observable attitudes and emotions that are

based on logical reason, the human mind also comprises unconscious feelings and emotions,

motivations, urges and needs that are hidden below the observable level, just like the

submerged part of an iceberg. Hence, to arrive at a more complete understanding and

comprehensive description of the behavior of consumers in the marketplace, it is

advantageous to also take into account the role of these unconscious drives and motivations in

consumer decision making.

In the realm of the study of underlying consumer motives, in 1999 a two-dimensional

motivational framework was developed by Jan Callebaut and colleagues, consisting of eight

consumer motives, i.e. desirable end-states that people seek to attain through consumption.

Recently, this framework has been used worldwide by a dozen of prominent multinational

companies to position, reposition or communicate new and existing brands (e.g. Coca-Cola,

xiii
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Johnson and Johnson, Heineken, Nokia and Chefaro). However, notwithstanding this

practical relevance, the consumer motive taxonomy of Callebaut and colleagues has never

been scientifically validated or tested. Hence, the main purpose of the present dissertation

was to contribute to the validation and critical assessment of this motivational framework in a

marketing context.

To meet this purpose, three broad research themes were focused upon in the current work.

(1) a Taxonomy of Consumer Motives: Cross-Validity. In the first paper we focused on the

measurement of underlying consumer motives by a set of stable (i.e. reliable) unidimensional

implicit items and, in addition, we confirmed the robustness of the motive structure of

Callebaut et al. (1999) across different cultures and different product categories. (2) a

Taxonomy of Consumer Motives: Construct Validity. To test for construct validity, in this

research theme we correlated consumer motives with other related concepts in literature. In a

second paper meaningful relationships were detected between consumer motives and personal

values. A third paper focused on the relationships between consumer motives and two

distinct personality traits, i.e. consumer innovativeness and self-control. (3) a Taxonomy of

Consumer Motives: Practical Applications. In the last research theme we focused on the

practical application of the consumer motive taxonomy of Callebaut et al. (1999) as a new

segmentation paradigm in the context of health marketing. In a fourth paper the usefulness of

this new segmentation approach was demonstrated in the process of developing appropriate

fruit and vegetable health advertisements, whereas in a fifth and last paper, clear implications

were shown in the context of ready meal consumption behavior.

In sum, we may conclude that the present dissertation aimed to contribute to a better

understanding of motivational consumer behavior by extensively investigating (1) cross-

xiv
Summary

validity, (2) construct validity, and (3) the practical application of Callebaut et al.’s (1999)

comprehensive framework of consumer motives that – as will be shown – has a number of

advantages over previous attempts at organizing the motivational domain.

xv
CHAPTER I

INTRODUCTION

1
Chapter I

Introduction

1. Scientific orientation

Choices of consumers concerning the selection, consumption and disposal of products and

services can often be difficult and are of importance to the consumer himself, but also to

marketers and policy makers. As a result, human decision making has been the nexus of the

study of consumer behavior yet for many years (e.g. Howard and Sheth, 1969; Engel et al.,

1973; Nicosia, 1966; Bettman et al., 1998). Consideration of current marketplace trends

indicates that this topic will continue to be critically important in the years to come. For

instance, rapid technological change has led to multitudes of new products and decreased

product lifetimes, whereas new communications media such as the World Wide Web have

made enormous amounts of information on options potentially available (e.g. Alba et al.,

1997).

The major question concerning consumer choice involves how consumers deal with the

decisions they have to make, some of which imply difficult trade-offs and uncertainties. One

approach to studying consumer decisions can be attributed to theoretical economics and is

called ‘rational choice theory’ (e.g. Hansen, 1972). Within this approach the consumer is

portrayed as a homo economicus – that is, one who is fully informed and makes perfect

rational decisions based on an economic evaluation of costs and benefits (e.g. Schiffman and

Kanuk, 1983). In contrast to this rational choice model, an alternative ‘information-

3
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

processing model’ more realistically portrays the consumer as unlikely to be fully informed

about all available choice alternatives. Instead, the consumer is assumed to possess a limited

capacity for processing information, with information-seeking efforts being likely to cease

when, what is perceived to be a sufficient amount of information, is obtained concerning

some of the alternatives, i.e. enough information to enable a satisfactory decision to be made

(e.g. Bettman, 1979).

Both the above models of rational choice and information-processing seem to capture the

essence of a well-educated and involved consumer who seeks information upon which to base

consumption decisions. Though such a cognitive approach has contributed greatly to the

prediction of consumer decisions, it suffers from the fact that it provides an incomplete

understanding of consumers’ actual decision making, given its focus on consumer rationality

alone. However, as mentioned by several authors, consumer behavior is not truly rational (e.g.

Etzioni, 1986; Katona, 1975; Morgan, 1980; Pribam, 1983; Cherian and Harris, 1989).

Besides observable attitudes and emotions that are based on logical reason, the human mind

also comprises unconscious feelings and emotions, motivations, urges and needs that are

hidden below the observable level, just like the submerged part of an iceberg (Wilson, 2002;

Zaltman, 2003; Van Raaij and Ye, 2005). Hence, to arrive at a more complete understanding

and comprehensive description of the behavior of consumers in the marketplace, it is

advantageous to also take into account the role of these unconscious drives and motivations in

consumer decision making (e.g. Pham, 1998; Pham et al., 2001; Shiv and Fedorikhin, 1999;

Shapiro, 1999; Loewenstein, 2001; Bargh, 2002; Shiv et al., 2005).

The key to determining the correct questions to ask in the context of motivational consumer

behavior is motivational market research. Though in early years, the term motivational

4
Chapter I : Introduction

research was generally used to refer to qualitative research designed to uncover the

consumer’s underlying or hidden motivations (e.g. Dichter, 1960), in the context of the

present dissertation, all types of research – also quantitative – should be included devoted to

the study of human motives. Operating on the premise that consumers are not always fully

aware of the basic reasons for their actions, motivational research attempts to discover these

unconscious motives for consumer behavior. Rather than solely being interested in the what

of consumer purchase decisions, motivational researchers are more concerned about the why

of product, service or brand usage (e.g. Foxall and Goldsmith, 1994). It is within the latter

area of studying product-specific consumer motives that the present dissertation aims to

deliver a valuable contribution.

2. Research objectives

Subject of research within the present dissertation is a psychological taxonomy of consumer

motives, initially developed by Callebaut et al. (1999). Consumer motives can be defined as

desirable end-states that people seek to attain through consumption. Based on the insights of

depth psychology, Callebaut et al. (1999) propose a framework of eight consumer motives, i.e.

Vitality, Pleasure, Conviviality, Belonging, Security, Control, Recognition and Power. These

eight motive orientations can be organized into a circumplex structure, determined by two

basic, bipolar dimensions, which respectively represent an intrapersonal and interpersonal

motive axis. All these motivational states can be considered as stable dispositions that are

present within every individual and become aroused by a particular product environment.

Given its domain-specific nature, Callebaut et al.’s (1999) taxonomy can be positioned as a

useful instrument to understand how consumer motives work in a brand category context.

5
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Once aroused, the particular consumer motives that are manifest in a given product context

are likely to elicit some kind of motive-directed behavior.

Recently, Callebaut et al.’s (1999) taxonomy has been used worldwide by a dozen of

prominent multinational companies to position, reposition or communicate new and existing

brands. For instance, on the basis of this taxonomy, Coca-Cola launched an exotic energy

drink called Nalu in 2003; Johnson and Johnson repositioned the shampoo brand Nizoral; and

Heineken brewery pre-tested new beer commercials. Also Nokia used the taxonomy to

evaluate the possibility of building a global brand platform based on the universally

underlying consumer motives in mobile phone consumption and the pharmaceutical company

Chefaro to launch their vitamin product DAVIDAMON. However, notwithstanding this

practical relevance, the consumer motive taxonomy of Callebaut et al. (1999) has never been

scientifically validated or tested. Hence, the main purpose of the present dissertation is to

contribute to the validation and critical assessment of the latter motivational framework in a

marketing context. More specifically, we try to meet this purpose by focusing on three broad

research objectives, i.e. (1) cross-validation, (2) construct validation, and (3) the practical

application of the taxonomy of consumer motives of Callebaut et al. (1999). Accordingly, the

studies in this dissertation are organized among these three themes: (1) a Taxonomy of

Consumer Motives: Cross-Validity, (2) a Taxonomy of Consumer Motives: Construct

Validity, and (3) a Taxonomy of Consumer Motives: Practical Applications (cf. infra).

3. Overview of the studies

The present dissertation comprises three major units. In the first unit (Chapter II) we briefly

review some existing literature about consumer motives, including the key conceptual

6
Chapter I : Introduction

definitions and an overview of the most influential motivational theories and frameworks.

The second unit is a bundling of five research papers, organized among three themes: (1) a

Taxonomy of Consumer Motives: Cross-Validity (Chapter III), (2) a Taxonomy of Consumer

Motives: Construct Validity (Chapter IV & Chapter V), and (3) a Taxonomy of Consumer

Motives: Practical Applications (Chapter VI & Chapter VII). We must remark that in this

second unit some overlap may exist, also with the first unit (Chapter II), as each chapter is the

full account of an independent paper containing its own limited literature review which

situates the particular research within its specific context. Finally, in the third unit of this

dissertation (Chapter VIII), per research theme, the findings of our studies are recapitulated

and discussed, theoretical, methodological and managerial implications are provided and

limitations and directions for future research are outlined.

3.1. A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives: Cross-Validity

In Chapter III we aim at empirically validating the circumplex consumer motive taxonomy of

Callebaut et al. (1999), based on respondents’ valence ratings of a list of preferred brand

personality traits. As personality traits associated with a preferred brand can be considered as

expressions of desired human personality dimensions (e.g. Keller, 1993) and, in addition,

desired human personality traits are likely to be function of underlying motives (e.g. Winter et

al., 1998), preferred brand personality traits are prone to channel the behavioral expression of

these underlying motives. Hence, in our research, consumer motives are measured in a

standardized way by a set of 34 preferred brand personality items. More specifically, using

the technique of INDSCAL, the objective in Chapter III is to investigate if a consistent and

replicable structure exists underlying the concept of preferred brand personality, and whether

this structure can be reformulated in terms of the taxonomy of consumer motives of Callebaut

7
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

et al. (1999). General research conclusions are based on the results of empirical studies

conducted in 11 different countries and among brands from four different product categories.

3.2. A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives: Construct Validity

As the issue of construct validity is generally acknowledged as a vital part of the process of

theory testing nowadays (e.g. Smith, 2005), in the second research part of the present

dissertation, we empirically test for construct validity, i.e. convergent and discriminant

validity by correlating consumer motives with other related concepts in literature, i.e.

respectively personal values (Chapter IV) and human personality traits (Chapter V). In the

two sections that follow we provide an overview of the research papers that were written

regarding these topics.

3.2.1. Consumer Motives & Personal Values

In Chapter IV we reconsider the relationship between consumer motives and personal values,

based on both an alternative implicit method of motive assessment and a non-student subject

sample. As methodological problems are posited as one of the most obvious reasons for the

lack of empirical correlations between motives and values (e.g. Brunstein and Schmitt, 2004),

we suggest that motive-value correlations might become more substantial by increasing

method congruence between implicit motive assessments and direct value measures and by

allowing more middle-aged subjects to participate in the study instead of young adults.

Specific hypotheses are developed regarding the proposed relationships between consumer

motives and values. To empirically test these hypotheses simple Pearson correlations and a

8
Chapter I : Introduction

multiple regression analysis are applied. Conclusions are based on information from a sample

of 178 respondents collected through a random walk method.

3.2.2. Consumer Motives & Personality Traits

In the realm of the application of contingency models in personality research (e.g. Dickson,

1982), in Chapter V we investigate the impact of underlying consumer motives on the

relationship between personality traits and several aspects of consumer behavior. In a first

study the trait consumer innovativeness is considered and its link with new-product adoption

behavior, whereas in a second study the focus is set on the relationship between dispositional

self-control and disciplined driving behavior. In addition to detecting meaningful

relationships between consumer motives and the traits innovativeness and self-control

respectively, the main objective in Chapter V is to provide basic support for the proposition

that actual behavioral outcomes of human personality traits are dependent on the alignment

with the particular consumer motives that are active within a given product context. By doing

so, we aim at extending previous work on the role of intervening variables in personality

research, by also demonstrating the impact of person-related variables, i.e. one’s underlying

motive dispositions on trait-behavior relationships, beyond the impact of situational factors as

is considered by most previous authors.

3.3. A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives: Practical Applications

In the final research part of the present dissertation we focus on the practical application of

the consumer motive taxonomy of Callebaut et al. (1999) as a new segmentation paradigm in

the context of health marketing. In Chapter VI the usefulness of this new segmentation

9
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

approach is demonstrated in the process of developing appropriate fruit and vegetable health

advertisements, whereas in Chapter VII clear implications are shown in the context of ready

meal consumption behavior.

3.3.1. Consumer Motives & Health Advertising

Based on the interpretation of Callebaut et al.’s (1999) taxonomy of consumer motives in the

context of health motivation, in Chapter VI, a heterogeneous health audience (N= 615) is

segmented according to the audience members’ health-related motive orientations, i.e.

relatively enduring beliefs that people hold about the fundamental meaning of health. After

identifying consumer segments with different health-related motive orientations and assessing

differences in fruit and vegetable consumption patterns among these segments, our major aim

is to explore the effectiveness of different health advertisements to motivate each of the

segments to behave healthier, i.e. increasing fruit and vegetable intake. In general, we expect

a segment’s reactions toward appropriate motive-related health advertising to be fairly more

positive than its reactions toward health advertising that is not responsive to that segment’s

underlying motives. Based on the findings in Chapter VI some practical suggestions and

recommendations are offered for health communicators to use when developing motive-

related health advertisements to audiences with different health-related motive orientations.

3.3.2. Consumer Motives & Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

In Chapter VII we extend our previous work on motive-related audience segmentation, by

investigating how different health-related motive orientations influence people’s ways of

dealing with convenience-related food solutions, i.e. ready meals. Again, distinct consumer

10
Chapter I : Introduction

segments are identified with different health-related motive orientations. Using (M)ANOVA,

a series of behavioral propositions are tested regarding the differences among the segments in

both general food behavior patterns and ready meal consumption patterns. Based on the

insights stemming from this analysis guidelines are provided for improving consumer-

oriented product development, positioning and marketing of products belonging to the

category of ready meals. Comprehensive positioning strategies are suggested, that go beyond

convenience-related aspects and also take into account psychological health-related motive

orientations of the target audience.

11
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

References

Alba, J.W., Lynch, J.G., Weitz, B., Janiszewski, C., Lutz, R.J., Sawyer, A.G., & Wood, S.L.

(1997). Interactive home shopping: consumer, retailer and manufacturer incentives to

participate in electronic market places. Journal of Marketing, 61(july), 38-53.

Bargh, J.A. (2002). Losing consciousness: automatic influences on consumer judgment,

behavior and motivation. Journal of Consumer research, 29, 280-285.

Bettman, J.R. (1979). An information processing theory of consumer choice. Reading, MA:

Addison-Wesley.

Bettman, J.R., Luce, M.F., & Payne, J.W. (1998). Constructive consumer choice processes.

Journal of Consumer Research, 25(3), 187-217.

Brunstein, J.C., & Schmitt, C.H. (2004). Assessing individual differences in achievement

motivation with the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Research in Personality, 38,

536-555.

Callebaut, J., Janssens, M., Op de Beeck, D., Lorré, D., & Hendrickx, H. (1999).

Motivational marketing research revisited. Leuven-Apeldoorn: Garant.

Cherian, J., & Harris, B. (1989). Capricious consumption and the social brain theory: why

consumers seem purposive even in the absence of purpose. Advances in Consumer

Research, 17, 745-749.

Dichter, E. (1960). A strategy of desire. Garden City, N.Y.: Doubleday.

Dickson, P.R. (1982). Person-situation: segmentation’s missing link. Journal of Marketing,

46, 56-64.

Engel, J.F., Kollat, D.T., & Blackwell, R.D. (1973). Consumer Behavior (2nd ed.). New

York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, Inc.

Etzioni, A. (1986). Rationality is anti-entropic. Journal of Economic Psychology, 7, 17-36.

12
Chapter I : Introduction

Foxall, G.R., & Goldsmith, R.E. (1994). Consumer psychology for marketing. London-New

York: Routledge.

Hansen, F. (1972). Consumer choice behavior: a cognitive theory. New York: Free Press.

Howard, J.A., & Sheth, J.N. (1969). The theory of buyer behavior. New York: John Wiley

& Sons, Inc.

Katona, G. (1975). Psychological Economics. New York: Elsevier.

Keller, K.L. (1993). Conceptualizing, measuring and managing customer-based brand equity.

Journal of marketing, 57 (jan), 1-22.

Loewenstein, G. (2001). The creative destruction of decision research. Journal of Consumer

Research, 28(3), 499-505.

Morgan, J.N. (1980). A realistic economics of the consumer requires some psychology. In

G. Katona (Ed.), Essays on behavioral economics. Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for

Social research.

Nicosia, F.M. (1966). Consumer decision processes: marketing and advertising implications.

Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Pham, M.T. (1998). Representativeness, relevance and the use of feelings in decision

making. Journal of Consumer research, 25, 144-160.

Pham, M.T., Cohen, J.B., Prajecus, J.W., & Hughes, D.G. (2001). Affect monitoring and the

primacy of feelings in judgment. Journal of Consumer Research, 28, 167-188.

Pribam, K. (1983). A history of economic reasoning. Baltimore, MD: The John Hopkins

University Press.

Schiffman, L.G., & Kanuk, L.L. (1983). Consumer behavior (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs,

NJ: Prentice-Hall.

13
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Shapiro, S. (1999). When an ad’s influence is beyond our conscious control: perceptual and

conceptual fluency effects caused by incidental ad exposure. Journal of Consumer

Research, 26, 16-36.

Shiv, B., & Fedorikhin, A. (1999). Heart and mind in conflict: the interplay of affect and

cognition in consumer decision making. Journal of Consumer Research, 26, 278-292.

Shiv, B., Fedorikhin, A., & Nowlis, S.M. (2005). Interplay of the heart and the mind in

decision-making. In S. Ratneshwar, & D.G. Mick (Eds.), Inside Consumption:

consumer motives, goals and desires (p. 166-183). London/ New York: Routledge.

Smith, G.T. (2005). On construct validity: issues of method and measurement. Psychological

Assessment, 17(4), 396-408.

Van Raaij, W.F., & Ye, G. (2005). Conscious and unconscious processing in consumer

motives, goals and desires. In S. Ratneshwar, & D.G. Mick (Eds.), Inside

Consumption: consumer motives, goals and desires (p. 330-339). London/ New York:

Routledge.

Wilson, T.D. (2002). Strangers to ourselves: discovering the adaptive unconscious.

Cambridge, MA: The Belknap Press.

Winter, D.G., John, O.P., Stewart, A.J., Klohnen, E.C., & Duncan, L.E. (1998). Traits and

motives: toward an integration of two traditions in personality research.

Psychological Review, 105(2), 230-250.

Zaltman, G. (2003). How customers think: essential insights into the mind of the market.

Boston, MA: Harvard Business School Press.

14
CHAPTER II

CONSUMER MOTIVES

LITERATURE REVIEW

15
Chapter II

Consumer Motives – Literature Review

1. Introduction

When we notice the world around us, it is obvious that people are different. They seek

different pleasures, have different tastes and preferences, and spend their money in different

ways. However, this apparent diversity in human behavior often causes us to overlook the

fact that people are really very much alike. There are underlying similarities that tend to

operate across many types of people and which serve to explain and to clarify their

consumption behavior (e.g. Sheth et al., 1999). Psychologists and consumer researchers agree

that basically most people experience the same kinds of needs and motives; they simply

express these underlying motives in different ways (e.g. Schiffman and Kanuk, 1983;

Callebaut et al., 2002). Hence, a thorough understanding of consumer motives is very

important to marketers as it gives them the opportunity to understand, and even anticipate,

human behavior in the marketplace.

In this chapter we briefly discuss some of the existing literature concerning consumer motives,

with no intention to give an exhaustive overview. Rather, our aim is to provide the reader of

the present dissertation with a basic theoretical framework, which enables him to understand

and put in perspective the next chapters. Hence, we start this literature review with a

definition of the concept of consumer motives and shed some light on its relatedness with

other concepts in literature such as goals, values and human personality traits. Next, we focus

17
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

on the hierarchical organization of consumer motive structures and we provide an overview of

some important motivational theories and frameworks. In addition, attention is paid to some

traditional techniques to assess consumer motives and to the limitations of these techniques.

Finally, we end up with some general conclusions based on the theoretical insights that were

acquired.

2. Consumer motives

2.1. Concept definition

As in the next chapters consumer motives will be referred to as desirable end-states that

people seek to attain through consumption, an appropriate formal definition of the concept

can be retained from the work of McClelland (1985; 1987). According to this author,

consumer motives can be considered as recurrent concerns for the attainment of affectively

charged incentives, such as having impact on others (i.e. the power motive; Winter, 1973),

establishing and maintaining meaningful relationships with others (i.e. the affiliation and

intimacy motives; Atkinson et al., 1958; McAdams and Powers, 1981), and doing something

better (i.e. the achievement motive; McClelland et al., 1953). Motives are based on emotional

learning and they develop early in life – primarily by nonverbal means – although they may

change with regard to their strength and the maturity of their behavioral expression during a

person’s life course (e.g. McAdams, 1988; McClelland, 1958; 1987; McClelland and Pilon,

1983). They are triggered automatically by environmental cues that signal the availability of

a rewarding affective state. This gives rise to an anticipatory motivational state that energizes,

selects, and orients a person’s behavior toward the attainment of the desired end-state (e.g.

Weinberger and McClelland, 1990; Mowen and Minor, 1997). Because a person’s motives

18
Chapter II : Consumer Motives – Literature Review

are supposed to operate largely unconsciously, it may be difficult to measure them through

direct observations. Rather, it is recommended by McClelland (1980) to assess the strength of

consumer motives indirectly by analyzing the contents of fantasy stories individuals produce

in response to picture cues similar to Murray’s (1943) Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) (cf.

infra).

From the definition of consumer motives we know that the concept pertains to the human

disposition to be concerned with and to strive for certain classes of desired end-states.

However, to fully understand the nature of consumer motives, an attempt should be made to

clarify the links with other related concepts such as goals, values and human personality traits.

In the next section we will briefly discuss the prevailing relationships between consumer

motives and these three concepts.

2.2. Relationship with other concepts

2.2.1. Consumer Motives & Goals

Although consumer motives may energize, select, and organize human behavior over

extended periods of time, they do not determine the specific ways in which a person satisfies

his or her underlying motives, nor do they directly refer to the development and use of

strategies and plans necessary to achieve these desired outcomes (e.g. Cantor and Blanton,

1996). Rather, as suggested by several motivational theorists (e.g. Emmons, 1986; Little,

1983; Cantor and Kihlstrom, 1987), individuals actively construe and choose a number of

personally meaningful goals in order to meet their motives and to link them to the execution

of goal-directed activities. Accordingly, personal goals could be considered as individualized

19
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

and cognitively elaborated representations of an individual’s implicit motives (e.g. Emmons

and King, 1992; Nuttin, 1984; Wurf and Markus, 1991). Notably, however, findings obtained

from several studies indicate that there is only slight (e.g. Emmons and McAdams, 1991) or

no empirical correspondence between people’s motives and their goals (e.g. Brunstein et al.,

1995; King, 1995). Based on these insights, some authors have posited that motives and goals

may constitute two distinctive modes of motivational functioning (e.g. Winter, 1996; Cantor

and Blanton, 1996; McClelland et al., 1989). According to these authors, individuals high in

a certain type of motive can, but need not, be involved to the same extent in the pursuit of

motive-congruent goals. Rather, in addition to pursuing goals in line with their motive

dispositions, individuals may also be frequently involved in the pursuit of goals that do not fit

their underlying motives (e.g. Brunstein et al., 1998).

2.2.2. Consumer Motives & Values

Similar to personal goals, an individual’s value priorities could also be considered as

cognitive instantiations of his or her underlying motives (e.g. Bilsky and Schwartz, 1994;

Schwartz and Bilsky, 1987). However, whereas personal goals are closely related to the

behavioral implementation of motive-related plans and activities, human values in their turn

refer to more abstract beliefs about preferred ways of acting or being, which serve as guiding

principles in the life of a person or other social entity (e.g. Costa and McCrae, 2001).

Another important distinction between goals and values as cognitive representations of

underlying motives is that the former correspond to the objectives of the individual himself,

whereas the latter are much more influenced by cultural norms and standards (e.g. Blackwell

et al., 2001). With regard to the empirical relationships between consumer motives and

values, few significant correlations could be detected between TAT-type measures of motives

20
Chapter II : Consumer Motives – Literature Review

and direct self-report measures of values (e.g. Biernat, 1989; Veroff and Smith, 1985). Hence,

according to McClelland et al. (1989), a distinction should be made between two independent

motivational systems, i.e. an emotion-driven implicit system that is constituted by non-

conscious motives versus a cognition based explicit system constituted by more conscious

goals and values.

2.2.3. Consumer Motives & Personality traits

According to different authors, different links exist between traits (i.e. who people are) and

motives (i.e. what people strive for in their lives) (e.g. Pervin, 1994). In one approach,

consumer motives are assumed to be predicted by personality traits (e.g. Cantor, 1990;

Stagner, 1994; Olver and Mooradian, 2003; Roberts et al., 2004). However, in literature,

stronger evidence is provided for the existence of a reverse causal relationship, in which

motives are assumed to underlie traits (e.g. Borkenau, 1990; Fleeson et al., 1995; Austin and

Vancouver, 1996; Snyder and Omoto, 1992). In this second approach, human personality

traits are conceptualized as motive-based concepts (e.g. Barsalou, 1985; Read et al., 1990),

which direct the ways in which these underlying motives are expressed (e.g. Winter et al.,

1998). Hence, based on the latter conceptualization, personality traits could be considered as

a function of intrinsic motives (e.g. Read and Lalwani, 2000). Basically, this reasoning

appears to be quite the opposite of the first mentioned approach which suggests that motives

modify the expression of underlying traits (e.g. Stagner, 1994).

21
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

3. Consumer motive structures

As inherent in our previous discussion of the relationship between goals and motives, the

consumer motive domain can be thought of at many different levels, ranging from fairly

abstract motives to more specific personal goals and corresponding behavioral plans that

individuals choose to meet these motives. The most common way to represent the structural

interrelations between these motives, goals and plans is to organize them into a hierarchical

structure (e.g. Bettman, 1979; Pieters et al., 1995; Austin and Vancouver, 1996; Huffman et

al., 2000), quite similar to the notion of means-end chain structures of consumers’ product

knowledge (e.g. Gutman, 1982; Olson and Reynolds, 1983). As mentioned, at the most

abstract level of the hierarchy a relatively small set of basic needs or motives can be placed

that regulate behavior. From this level, consumer motive structures can be broken down into

a series of increasingly more concrete goals and sub-goals, plans and sub-plans which have to

be attained in order to reach the goals and motives at higher, i.e. more abstract hierarchical

levels (e.g. Bandura, 1989; Beach, 1990; Carver and Scheier, 1981; Emmons, 1989; Vallacher

and Wegner, 1985). Hence, goals and plans at lower levels in the hierarchy serve as means to

achieve higher-level goals and motives as ends, and thus, consumer motive structures can be

thought of as a means-end structure of sequences of lower and higher-level goals (e.g. Newell

and Simon, 1972; King et al., 1998).

Though most researchers have adopted the view of a hierarchical organization of goals and

motives, a number of different terminologies have been used to specify the different levels in

the hierarchy. For instance, Carver and Scheier (1981) distinguish between the program, the

principle, and the system level, in increasing order of abstractness, whereas Gutman (1997)

and Heckenhausen and Kuhl (1985) propose a hierarchical structure comprised of action,

22
Chapter II : Consumer Motives – Literature Review

outcome and consequence goals. In their turn, Harackiewicz and Elliot (1998) use the terms

target and purpose goals to refer to lower and higher-level goals respectively. Despite these

different names for the various levels, most authors agree that consumer motive structures can

be best represented by means of a three-tiered hierarchy (e.g. Bagozzi and Dholakia, 1999;

Pieters, 1993; Carver and Scheier, 1996; Vallacher and Wegner, 1985). At the center of this

hierarchy the focal goal can be located, which answers the question: “What is it for which I

strive?”. Sub-ordinate goals constitute the means of achieving the focal goal and answer the

question: “How can I achieve that for which I strive?”. At the top of the hierarchy are super-

ordinate goals, i.e. consumer motives, which deal with the question: “Why do I want to

achieve that for which I strive?”

Regarding the empirical assessment of these three-tiered consumer motive structures,

Reynolds and Gutman’s (1988) ‘laddering’ technique (cf. infra) has been proposed as the

most appropriate method to elicit and analyze sequences of linked sub-ordinate and super-

ordinate goals (e.g. Pieters et al., 1995). Starting the laddering procedure at the focal goal

level, i.e. a particular behavior identified by the consumer (e.g. losing weight), consumer

motive structures at the super-ordinate level can be derived by repeatedly asking the question:

“Why is this behavior important to you?”. In a similar vein, it can be proposed that repeated

questions of the form: “How are you planning to accomplish this behavior?” will be helpful in

uncovering sequences of increasingly more sub-ordinate goals below the basic level.

23
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

4. Motivational theories

Though general agreement exists that the consumer motive domain is organized hierarchically,

fewer consensuses have been reached concerning the nature and number of human motives.

Because of the messiness and diversity of consumer motives and desires, scholars have been

reluctant to conceptualize them in a comprehensive way (e.g. Richins, 2005). Nonetheless,

several fair attempts have been made to classify consumer motives into a limited number of

content domains. While these developed taxonomies are not in complete agreement with each

other and none of them has proved entirely satisfactory to the field, they offer useful starting

points and can serve as valuable resources for consumer behavior researchers interested in

understanding the what and why of consumer motivation. In what follows, we briefly review

several theories of motivation, including the most important classic motivational frameworks

as well as some more recently developed taxonomies.

4.1. Classic theories of motivation

4.1.1. Freud’s concept of drives

According to Freud (1933; 1990), human behavior is motivated primarily by internal,

biological instincts or drives originating in the Id, i.e. the oldest and unconscious part of the

human psyche. Emotional and illogical, the Id functions on the Pleasure Principle by which it

seeks to minimize pain and to maximize immediate gratification. Basically, two broad

categories of instincts are incorporated in the Id: life instincts which are embodied in the

human sexual drive and death instincts embodied in the human drive for aggression (e.g.

Reeve, 1992; Graftdijk, 1986; Walters and Bergiel, 1989). In an unrestricted way, these

24
Chapter II : Consumer Motives – Literature Review

biological instincts would inspire people to perform wanton and utterly self-serving behaviors.

However, the operation of the Id is constrained and moderated by two additional forces

incorporated in the human mind, called the Ego and the Superego. The Ego represents the

conscious mind and it is composed of individual perceptions, thoughts, memories and feelings.

Driven by the Reality Principle, the Ego mainly encourages realistic and reasonable thoughts

and actions and, as a result, it channels the urges of the Id into socially acceptable patterns of

behavior. The Superego comprises the third component of the mind and it represents the

traditional ideas and values of the society. As it is guided by the Morality Principle, the

Superego can be considered as a sort of socially defined conscience, which in particular

attempts to compel the ego to pursue goals that match the morality dictated by society and

culture (e.g. Gellner, 1985).

Based on this tripartite framework of human personality, Freud basically assumed three

wellsprings of motivation: biological forces (represented by the Id), societal forces

(represented by the Superego), and human consciousness (represented by the Ego).

Especially the interplay between these three forces can be considered as the foundation for

explaining human motivations and needs, given its direct influence on overt (consumer)

behavior (e.g. Arnould et al., 2004). In the 1950s Freud’s set of doctrines was introduced to

marketing practice under the name of Motivation Research (e.g. Dichter, 1960). The key

feature of his theories that influenced this school of thought was the role of unconscious

wishes and desires in shaping behavior. Much of human behavior was seen as the result of

unconscious efforts to control inner drives for sex, hunger and aggression. Hence, products

and brands were interpreted in appropriate symbolic terms and related to these unconscious

desires of consumers (e.g. Solomon, 1992; Harrell, 1986).

25
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Despite the importance of Freud’s major personality theory to the study of consumer behavior,

not all psychologists are sympathetic toward Freud. For instance, some opponents have

criticized the original, clinical psychoanalytic basis of his work (e.g. Popper, 1986; Colby,

1960), while others argued that Freud’s theory is lacking empirical validation and evidence

(e.g. Grünbaum, 1986; Eysenck, 1986). However, notwithstanding these criticisms, Freud’s

primary thoughts and insights have inspired many motivational researchers after him to

develop their own motivational theories and frameworks. In the next sections, an overview is

provided of the theories of three influential followers of Sigmund Freud, i.e. Carl Jung,

Abraham Maslow and Henry Murray respectively.

4.1.2. Jung’s concept of archetypes

Similar to Freud, Carl Gustav Jung focused on the role of the unconscious as a fundamental

driver of human motivation. However, Jung’s understanding of the unconscious was much

subtler than Freud’s, as the former author subdivided the concept into the personal

unconscious and the collective unconscious (e.g. Jung et al., 1964). The personal

unconscious holds previously conscious experiences that have been repressed, forgotten,

suppressed or ignored. Although stored in the unconscious, these experiences can become

conscious in the form of intuitions or similar inexplicable urges. The concept of the collective

unconscious comprises the most important part of Jung’s ideas about human motivation.

According to Jung et al. (1964) the collective unconscious is a storehouse of latent memory

traces inherited from the human ancestral past. Jung refers to this inherited material as

archetypes, i.e. universal prototypes for ideas that are acquired by humanity’s ancestry and

may be used to interpret observations. In particular, three universal symbols seem to be

26
Chapter II : Consumer Motives – Literature Review

important in Jung’s theory, i.e. the self1, the great mother2, and the hero3. In their most basic

forms, these archetypes are likely to motivate humans to react to the world in a selective

fashion (e.g. Meyers, 1998).

Jung’s approach is important for marketing practice because it provides marketers a way to

explore myths, images, and symbols. In turn, these myths and symbols can be the building

blocks for creating effective marketing phenomena, such as advertisements and promotions.

Following Jung’s analysis, advertising can operate on the unconscious mind and create strong

impressions by appealing to universal archetypes, stored in the collective unconscious of

consumer memory (e.g. Arnould et al., 2004).

4.1.3. Maslow’s hierarchy of needs

The most popular and well-known approach to human motivation is based on the research of

the psychologist Abraham Maslow (1943; 1970). After some twenty years of clinical practice,

Maslow presented the idea that there is a hierarchy of needs in man (see Figure 2.1), ranging

from the lower-order physiological drives (e.g. thirst, hunger and rest), through safety needs

(e.g. shelter, protection and security) and affective needs (e.g. affection, friendship, love and

acceptance), to the higher-order needs for self-esteem (e.g. prestige, success and

accomplishment) and self-actualization (e.g. self-fulfillment and enriching experiences). The

lower-level needs are considered to dominate the higher-level needs. That is, consumers must

satisfy lower-level needs first, before they begin to pursue higher-order needs. According to

Maslow (1943; 1970) the highest level of need is related to self-actualization, i.e. the desire

to live up one’s full potential and to maximize the use of skills and abilities. However, this

1
The ‘self’ refers to the idea of the coherent whole, the unified consciousness and unconscious of a person.
2
The ‘great mother’ refers to the idea of a nature goddess, comprising the themes of nature and motherly care.
3
The ‘hero’ refers to what is good and noble in society, the one who sacrifices the self for the greater good.

27
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

need for self-actualization only becomes activated if all four of the lower-level needs have

already been satisfied.

Figure 2.1 : Maslow’s Hierarchy of Human Needs

Self- Actualization
e.g. self-fulfillment,
enriching experiences
Self-esteem
e.g. prestige, success,
accomplishment
Affective needs
e.g. affection, friendship, love and
acceptance
Safety needs
e.g. shelter, protection and security
Physiological drives
e.g. thirst, hunger and rest
Source : Adapted from Abraham H. Maslow, “A Theory of Human Motivation, “ Psychological
Review, 50 (1943), 370-396

There are many criticisms of Maslow’s theory. At one level, critics question the hierarchical

organization of the basic need taxonomy, as many examples exists in which it seems as if

higher-level needs won out over basic needs (e.g. Ger, 1997). In addition, Maslow’s concepts

are considered as too general. To say that hunger and self-esteem are similar, in that both are

needs, is to neglect the urgent, involuntary nature of the former and the largely conscious,

voluntary nature of the latter. However, the major problem with Maslow’s theory is that there

is little empirical and theoretical evidence to support its basic assumptions (e.g. Drenth et al.,

1984). Finally, the need hierarchy also appears to be culture and time bound as it is very

closely related to the basic needs of contemporary American people (e.g. Nevis, 1983).

28
Chapter II : Consumer Motives – Literature Review

Despite these criticisms, Maslow’s need hierarchy has received wide acceptance in many

social disciplines as it provides a useful tool for understanding human motivation. In addition,

this basic taxonomy can also be useful for marketing practitioners who are interested in

understanding the basic needs of their customers. More specifically, Maslow’s list of needs

can serve as key input for product design and appropriate ad campaigns can be developed that

appeal to one or more of the needs represented in Maslow’s hierarchy (e.g. Arnould et al.,

2004; Foxall and Goldsmith, 1994).

4.1.4. Murray’s list of human needs

In 1938 psychologist Henry Murray prepared a detailed list of twenty-eight psychogenic

needs, which have served as the basic constructs for a number of widely used personality tests,

such as the Thematic Apperception Technique and Edwards Personal Preference Schedule

(e.g. Murray, 1938). Murray believed that everyone has the same basic set of needs, but that

individuals differ in their priority ranking of these needs. The basic needs of Murray include

many motives that are assumed to play an important role in consumer behavior, such as

acquisition, achievement, recognition and exhibition. In Table 2.1 an overview is provided of

Murray’s (1938; 1955) whole list of human needs.

A major criticism of the work of Murray is that it is just a lengthy inventory of needs, and

therefore, this list is difficult and impractical for marketers to use. As was the case for the

aforementioned need hierarchy of Abraham Maslow, Murray’s need inventory lacks both

empirical and theoretical evidence and, in addition, the theory does not necessarily apply

outside the American society where it was developed (e.g. Suarez-Orozco, 1989).

Nevertheless, as Murray’s list of human motives is so comprehensive, it has been considered

29
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

as an important instrument for use when thinking about human motivation from a cross-

cultural perspective (e.g. Markus and Kitayama, 1991).

Table 2.1 : Murray’s List of Human Needs

Needs Associated with Inanimate Objects


Acquisition
Conservance
Order
Retention
Construction
Needs That Reflect Ambition, Power, Accomplishment, and Prestige
Superiority
Achievement
Recognition
Exhibition
Inviolacy (inviolate attitude)
Infavoidance (to avoid shame, failure, humiliation, ridicule)
Defendance (defensive attitude)
Counteraction (counteractive attitude)
Needs Concerned with Human Power
Dominance
Deference
Similance (suggestible attitude)
Autonomy
Contrarience (to act differently from others)
Sado-Masochistic Needs
Aggression
Abasement
Needs Concerned with Inhibition
Blamavoidance (to avoid blame)
Needs Concerned with Affection between People
Affiliation
Rejection
Nurturance (to nourish, aid, or protect the helpless)
Succorance (to seek aid, protection, or sympathy)
Play
Needs Concerned with Social Intercourse (the Needs to Ask and Tell)
Cognizance (inquiring attitude)
Exposition (expositive attitude)
Source : Adapted from Henry A. Murray, “Types of Human Needs,” in David C. McClelland, Studies in
Motivation (New York : Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1955)

30
Chapter II : Consumer Motives – Literature Review

4.2. Recent taxonomies

4.2.1. A trio of needs

Several psychologists have written extensively on the existence of a trio of basic needs: the

needs for power (nPow), for affiliation (nAff), and for achievement (nAch) (e.g. McClelland,

1961; 1985; Tauber, 1972; Dweck, 1986; Ames, 1992; Urdan and Maehr, 1995; Ryan and

Deci, 2000; Thompson et al., 2001). These needs can each be subsumed in prior motivational

classifications, such as the basic need inventories of Maslow and Murray (cf. supra); however,

considered individually, they each have a unique relevance to consumer motivation.

The need for power refers to an individual’s desire to control his or her environment. It

includes the need to impact other people, to feel superior or to be in a position of influence.

This need appears to be closely related to the ego need, in that many individuals experience

increased self-enhancement when they are able to exercise power over objects or people (e.g.

Schmidt and Frieze, 1997; Winter, 1973).

The affiliation motive is defined as the desire to be with people. This need suggests that

individuals’ behavior is highly influenced by the desire for friendship, for acceptance, and for

belonging. People with high affiliation needs tend to have a strong social dependence on

others. They tend to conform to a high extent to the norms and regulations of the in-group to

which they belong (e.g. McAdams and Powers, 1981).

Finally, one of the most studied and talked about motivations is the need for achievement.

The achievement motive can be conceptualized as the desire to experience emotion in

31
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

connection with evaluated performance. Individuals with a strong need for achievement

regard personal accomplishment as an end in itself. The achievement need is closely related

to the egoistic need, in that satisfaction with a job well done serves to enhance the

individual’s self-esteem (e.g. McClelland, 1961; Veroff, 1982).

Together, these three fundamental needs have been considered as some of the most important

determinants of human motivation (e.g. McClelland, 1985). Accordingly, this trio of needs

has been focused upon by marketing researchers too. For instance, consumers with specific

power, affiliation or achievement-related needs tend to be receptive to advertising appeals

directed to those needs. In addition, they also tend to be receptive to certain kinds of products

or stores (e.g. Landon, 1972; Gardner, 1972; Schewe, 1973). Hence, these needs might be

applied by marketers as additional segmentation bases on which to divide their markets into

smaller, homogeneous subgroups (e.g. Schiffman and Kanuk, 1983).

4.2.2. Wicker et al.’s motive classification

Typical of recent attempts to categorize human motives are Wicker et al.’s (1984) analyses of

goal ratings. These authors generated a list of 46 goals (10 goals were added to that list in a

second study, for a total of 56 goals) and asked subjects to rate the valence of each goal

(‘How much do you want it ?’). The ratings were then subjected to factor analysis and

hierarchical cluster analysis. Factors over all subjects were labeled Interpersonal Concern,

Competitive Ambition, Exploration-Play, Balanced Success, Economic status, and Intellectual

Orientation. Supplementary cluster analyses produced corresponding clusters, as well as

Security, Personal Growth, Transpersonal Orientation, and Tranquility Seeking, arranged in

hierarchies of class inclusion, culminating in a broad goal dichotomy described as Individual

32
Chapter II : Consumer Motives – Literature Review

Striving versus Harmony Seeking. According to Austin and Vancouver (1996) these broad

classes of goals describe two sides of an optimization problem, i.e. seeking to meet all the

criteria and seeking to balance the criteria. Kuhl (1994) distinguished these processes as self-

control, in which one goal dominates, and self-regulation, in which goals seek consensus.

As mentioned by Chulef et al. (2001), a major problem with the work of Wicker et al. (1984)

may have been created by the latter authors’ use of importance ratings as the basis for their

taxonomy of human goals. Actually, there is no particular reason to think that similarity in

importance necessarily maps onto conceptual or semantic similarity, or taps into ways in

which goals may be organized in common motivational structures. Although one might think

that items that are similar conceptually tend to be similar in importance, the reverse reasoning

is not necessarily true. Hence, not surprisingly, conceptually heterogeneous clusters could be

detected in the overall motive taxonomy of Wicker et al. (1984).

4.2.3. Ford and Nichols’ goal taxonomy

Another major effort to classify goals into a number of content categories is based on the self-

constructing systems work of the developmental psychologists Ford and Nichols (1987). On

the basis of discussions and interviews with colleagues, students, and counseling clients, the

latter authors produced a comprehensive set of goals and then organized them into categories

based on an extensive review of the literature on achievement motivation, personality, and

social motivation. Their taxonomy – which is fully depicted in Table 2.2 – divides human

goals into two major groups: goals that refer to desired consequences within the person, i.e.

within-person goals and goals that refer to desired consequences of the relationship between

the person and the social environment, i.e. person-environment goals. Within-person goals

33
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

are further subdivided into three more levels of goals, i.e. affective goals, cognitive goals, and

subjective organization goals (e.g. Ford and Nichols, 1987). Affective goals refer to different

kinds of feelings or emotions that people may desire, such as being happy, having fun, feeling

relaxed, or feeling satiated. Cognitive goals refer to different kinds of internal representations

that people may want to construct or maintain, such as interesting thoughts, accurate or

meaningful thoughts, novel thoughts, or positive thoughts about the self. Subjective

organization goals represent special states or experiences that people may seek involving the

integrated functioning of the person or person-environment system. Person-environment

goals, in their turn, comprise self-assertive social relationship goals, integrative social

relationship goals, and task goals. In social relationships with other people individuals try to

accomplish two general kinds of goals: maintenance or promotion of the self, i.e. self-

assertive relationship goals, and maintenance or promotion of other people or social groups,

i.e. integrative relationship goals. Finally, task goals represent the desired relationships

between the individual and various objects in the environment and they include the specific

goals of mastery, task creativity, management, material gain, and safety.

Many of Ford and Nichols’ (1987) specific goal types (see Table 2.2) have been the focus of

long and productive research in the motive domain (e.g. Deci and Ryan, 1985; Cropanzano,

1993; Koestner and McClelland, 1990). A key contribution of the work of Ford and Nichols

to these various research programs is their integration of human goal content into a single

two-part hierarchy that can subsume the major life domains that are of interest to

psychologists (e.g. Austin and Vancouver, 1996).

34
Chapter II : Consumer Motives – Literature Review

Table 2.2 : Ford and Nichols’ Goal Taxonomy

WITHIN-PERSON GOALS
Affective goals
Entertainment Having fun, Avoiding boredom, seeking Heightened arousal
Tranquility Peace of mind, Serenity, Avoiding stress
Happiness Feelings of joy, satisfaction, or well being
Bodily Sensations Experiencing particular bodily sensations, Experiencing physical
movement
Physiological Well Being Experiencing desired physiological states, Feeling healthy
Cognitive Goals
Exploration Curiosity, Intellectual stimulation, Learning
Understanding Ordering, Categorizing, Explaining, Making sense
Intellectual Creativity Inventing new ideas, Expanding one’s limits
Positive or Confirmatory Self-efficacy, Self-esteem, Self-acceptance, Self-worth
Self-Evaluations
Subjective Organisation Goals
Unity Seeking coherence, harmony, or oneness
Transcendence Rising above ordinary experience, Pursuing an idealized state, Spirituality
PERSON-ENVIRONMENT GOALS
Social Relationship Goals
Self-Assertion Maintaining or promoting the self
Individuality Uniqueness, Separateness, Individual identity
Self-Determination Personal control, Freedom, Autonomy
Superiority Social status or importance, Dominance, Winning, Comparing favorably
with others
Resource Acquisition Obtaining support, assistance, advice, and validation from others

Integration Maintaining or promoting other people or social groups


Belongingness Attachments, Intimacy, Friendship, Community, Social identity

Social Responsibility Fulfilling social roles, Keeping interpersonal commitments, Accepting


legitimate social control
Equity Fairness, Justice, Reciprocation, Comparing equally with others

Resource Provision Providing support, assistance, advice, and validation for others
Task Goals
Mastery Improving one’s performance, Meeting a standard of achievement

Task Creativity Inventing new processes or products


Management Handling routine tasks, Organizing people or things, Being productive

Material Gain Obtaining or accumulating money or tangible goods


Safety Avoiding threatening or depriving circumstances
Source: Adapted from M.E. Ford and C.W. Nichols, “A taxonomy of human goals and some possible
applications, “ in M.E. Ford and D.H. Ford, Humans as Self-constructing Systems.(Hillsdale, NJ : Erlbaum,
1987)

35
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

4.2.4. Chulef et al.’s taxonomy of human goals

Similar to many other attempts at organizing the motivational domain (e.g. Maslow, 1943;

Murray, 1938; McClelland, 1961), the aforementioned goal taxonomy of Ford and Nichols

(1987) is primarily based on the theoretical preconceptions of its developers, as major goal

categories are derived from the authors’ a priori knowledge of the field. However, Chulef et

al. (2001) reject these previous theoretically based conceptualizations of goal content, and as a

result, they propose a taxonomy of human goals that is empirically generated, based on the

semantic-similarity judgments of naïve subjects (laypeople) who sorted a set of 135 high-level

goals into conceptually homogeneous categories. A hierarchical cluster analysis performed

on these similarity judgments revealed three major categories: (1) Family, Marriage, Sex, and

Romance; (2) Interpersonal goals related to interacting with people in general; and (3)

Intrapersonal goals. The general Interpersonal goals then subdivided into a number of other

sub-clusters, such as (a) physical goals including Health and Appearance, goals related to (b)

Friendship, (c) Belonging, Social Recognition and Social Approval, and several clusters of

goals related to (d) Receiving Support from Others, (e) Avoiding Rejection, (f) general

Positive Social Qualities (such as being honest and trustworthy), (g) Teaching and Helping

Others, and (h) Leadership. The Intrapersonal goals also subdivided into a number of sub-

clusters, such as a set consisting of various clusters related to (a) Freedom, Ethics, Social

Awareness and Religion; another set containing clusters related to (b) Aesthetics, Creativity,

Entertainment, Excitement and Openness to Experience; a third set related to (c)

Psychological Well-Being, Safety and Stability, Personal Growth, Achievement and Self-

determination; and a final set with clusters concerning (d) Finances, Career, Education and

Intellect. Basically, these numerous lower and higher-level clusters of goals showed high

levels of replicability among very different groups of individuals (see Chulef et al., 2001),

36
Chapter II : Consumer Motives – Literature Review

demonstrating that this particular organization of human goals does a good job of capturing

the conceptual organization of goals as perceived in the minds of a wide range of naïve

subjects.

An important dissimilarity between the respective goal taxonomies of Ford and Nichols (1987)

and Chulef et al. (2001) is that the former authors, on the basis of their knowledge of prior

theories of motivation, placed both social goals (i.e. goals that pertain to the individual in his

or her interaction with other people) and task goals (i.e. goals related to achievement,

improving one’s performance, and keeping one’s safety) within the higher-order category of

person-environment goals, whereas participants in the study of Chulef et al. (2001) split task-

related goals from Interpersonal goals, placing these goals within the category of

Intrapersonal goals. Hence, evidence is provided that goal clusters formed on a bottom-up,

commonsense basis (e.g. Chulef et al., 2001) may be different from clusters generated on the

basis of a top-down, theoretically based conceptualization (e.g. Ford and Nichols, 1987;

Maslow, 1943; Murray, 1938; McClelland, 1961). In particular, these differences between

researchers’ and subjects’ perceptions show that previous theoretically based

conceptualizations of goals have failed to capture important aspects of laypeople’s naïve

conceptions of the structure of human motivation.

5. Assessment of consumer motives

Besides wondering about the nature, structure, and content of human motivation, yet another

important aspect to consider is how consumer motives are identified or can be measured quite

appropriately. However, the question concerning the empirical assessment of underlying

motives is a difficult one to answer because motives are hypothetical constructs, and as a

37
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

result, they cannot be seen or touched, or otherwise tangibly observed. Hence, a variety of

techniques has been proposed in literature to infer the presence and/or the strength of various

consumer motives, ranging from direct self-report measures to rather indirect projective

techniques. In the final sections of the present chapter an explanation is provided of three

frequently used motive assessment techniques, i.e. self-reports, laddering, and the Thematic

Apperception Test (TAT), and particular attention is paid to some important limitations of

these techniques.

5.1. Direct self-report measures

Some researchers claim that the best way to find out about the needs and motives of

individuals is simply to ask them (e.g. Brody, 1980; Klinger, 1995). Hence, a number of

pencil-and-paper tests have been developed in motivation research, which inquire directly

about consumer needs and motives. For instance, several scales have been given birth to

focusing on specific motivations such as need for power (e.g. Schmidt and Frieze, 1997), need

for achievement (e.g. Mehrabian, 1968; 1969), et cetera, as well as more general inventories

such as the Edwards Personal Preference Schedule (EPPS, Edwards, 1957). Most frequently,

these direct self-report measures comprise a series of motive-related statements which have to

be rated by respondents, according to a five or seven-point Likert scale ranging from ‘strongly

disagree’ to ‘strongly agree’. The information so obtained from these ratings is then

quantified (i.e. assigned a numerical score) and an average measure is computed of the

strength of a specific need or motive.

Though direct questionnaires have been largely preferred to other measurement techniques

because of their ease of administration and their reliability (e.g. Brody, 1980), there might be

38
Chapter II : Consumer Motives – Literature Review

some potential problems with self-reports of this nature. First, individuals may not

themselves be aware of the actual reasons or motives underlying their behavior, and as a

result, they may unconsciously rationalize their actions; that is, they may assign reasons or

motives that are acceptable to their personalities but are not, in fact, accurate (e.g. Kruglanski,

1989). Aside from these unconscious rationalizations, direct self-report measures may also

suffer from what is called ‘social desirability bias’, as respondents may deliberately falsify

self-report inventories to impress the researcher, to please the researcher, or to avoid personal

embarrassment (e.g. Fisher, 1993; Holtgraves et al., 1997). Finally, McClelland (1980; 1985b)

suggests that self-report inventories measure not underlying consumer motives as such, but

their conscious counterparts, i.e. consumer goals or values.

For the above reasons of unconscious rationalization, social desirability bias, and not

measuring consumer motives as such, psychologists and other motivational researchers have

advised against the use of direct self-report measures, in favor of some other more appropriate

motive assessment techniques such as ‘laddering’ or projective TAT-type measures. In what

follows, we briefly discuss these two additional methods of measuring underlying consumer

motives.

5.2. Reynolds and Gutman’s laddering technique

As mentioned earlier, the technique of laddering (e.g. Reynolds and Gutman, 1988) has been

suggested within means-end theory as a clear method for discovering a consumer’s pattern of

motivations (e.g. Grunert and Grunert, 1995; Claeys et al., 1995). In a laddering interview,

subjects are first asked to identify salient factors or attributes that distinguish different choice

alternatives in a product class. At the second step of measurement, respondents are asked to

39
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

say why a particular attribute is important to them. This is the start of the laddering process.

The interviewer keeps asking why until the respondent answers with a value (e.g. affiliation,

achievement) or until the respondent becomes fatigued and can no longer answer. This

succession of why-questions is referred to as the laddering process. Presumably, the majority

of consumer means-end chains (i.e. sequences of attributes, consequences, and values which

are referred to as ladders) will culminate with a basic motive or value. Afterwards, these

individual ladders are aggregated and summarized in a hierarchical value map or consumer

decision map (e.g. Reynolds et al., 1994).

As an important limitation of the laddering technique we have to note that the latter method

requires an in-depth interview that is potentially time-consuming and possibly frustrating for

the respondent (e.g. being continually confronted with why questions). Hence, the laddering

technique is usually applied to small samples. It is not particularly feasible for large scale

applications (e.g. Arnould et al., 2004).

5.3. Murray’s Thematic Apperception Test (TAT)

As consumer motives are supposed to operate on a person’s behavior usually outside of his or

her conscious awareness, and thus, people may not be able to accurately report the strength of

their motives (e.g. McClelland, 1980; Biernat, 1989), most authors have recommended to

assess consumer motives indirectly, through the use of appropriate projective techniques (e.g.

Weinberger and McClelland, 1990). The principal advantages of the latter techniques relative

to direct self-report measures comprise their capacity to bypass the conscious defenses of

respondents and to allow for privileged access to important psychological information of

which respondents are not consciously aware (e.g. Dosajh, 1996). Historically, the Thematic

40
Chapter II : Consumer Motives – Literature Review

Apperception Test or TAT (e.g. Murray, 1943; Morgan and Murray, 1935) has been amongst

the most widely used and accepted projective techniques to reveal consumers’ underlying

motive dispositions. In a typical TAT administration situation, subjects are presented with

four to six ambiguous pictures out of a standard series of 31 images (e.g. Smith, 1992;

Morgan, 2002). After looking at the pictures for some time, the subjects are instructed to

write down a story about each picture, with the specific guidelines to include (1) a description

of the event in the picture, (2) the developments that led up to the event, (3) the thoughts and

feelings of the people in the picture, and (4) the outcome of the story. Afterwards, the

resulting TAT protocols are content coded for the presence of a particular consumer motive

by scoring the amount of motive-related imagery that is expressed within the stories (e.g.

Atkinson, 1981; Winter, 1991).

Obviously, the identification and measurement of human motives by means of projective

techniques is still a very inexact process. Many psychologists point out that most TAT-type

measurement techniques do not meet the crucial test criteria of validity and reliability,

especially when considering the low internal consistency among stories from which a

person’s motive scores are obtained and, in addition, the absence of a normative scoring

system to evaluate the motive-related imagery that is elicited by the pictures (e.g. Entwisle,

1972; Hofer and Chasiotis, 2004; Lilienfeld et al., 2000; Tuerlinckx et al., 2002). However,

notwithstanding these psychometric problems inherent in the TAT, the latter technique

remains widely used as a tool for assessing underlying consumer motives and it has been able

to maintain its dominant position, despite the emergence of other, more recently developed,

implicit methods for measuring individual differences in motivational preferences, such as the

Implicit Association Test (IAT) among others (e.g. Greenwald et al., 1998; Maison et al.,

2004).

41
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

6. Conclusion

Without claiming to be exhaustive, based on the present literature review, we are allowed to

draw some important general conclusions. Consumer motives pertain to the human

disposition to be concerned with and to strive for certain classes of desired end-states, and in

this way, the construct is related to other concepts in literature such as goals, values, and

human personality traits. Regarding the structure of consumer motives, general agreement

exists among theorists that the motivational domain is organized hierarchically; that is,

consumer motive structures can be thought of in terms of a means-end structure of sequences

of sub-ordinate and super-ordinate goals. However, less consensuses have been reached

concerning the content and measurement of human motives. With respect to the former

aspect, several researchers have attempted to develop a comprehensive taxonomy of human

needs or motives (e.g. Murray, 1938; Maslow, 1943; McClelland, 1961; Wicker et al., 1984;

Ford and Nichols, 1987; Chulef et al., 2001). As helpful as these goal/motive enumerations

may be, however, none of them has proved entirely satisfactory to the field. One deficiency

that can be noticed in general is the absence of the consumer’s voice in these previous

analyses of motivations. For the most part (see Chulef et al., 2001 for an exception), the goals

or motives described in the former taxonomies are derived theoretically or by statistical

analysis of the perceptions of expert judges, and as a result, we have heard far too little from

consumers themselves about the goals and motives that shape their consumption desires. As a

person’s motives are presumed to operate largely unconsciously, projective TAT-type

measures are preferred over paper-and-pencil tests or laddering techniques as an appropriate

method to assess these underlying motives. However, clearly a need exists for the

development of improved implicit procedures for measuring human motives, as traditional

projective techniques (e.g. TAT) suffer from psychometric problems such as a lack of

42
Chapter II : Consumer Motives – Literature Review

reliability or even measurement validity. In the next chapters we outline the way we

attempted to contribute significantly to the existing consumer motive literature by extensively

investigating (1) cross-validity (Chapter III), (2) construct validity (Chapter IV & Chapter V),

and (3) the practical application (Chapter VI & Chapter VII) of the comprehensive framework

of consumer motives of Callebaut et al. (1999) that has advantages over previous attempts at

organizing the motivational domain. Moreover, we propose an alternative implicit method of

motive assessment in an attempt to resolve some of the psychometric problems inherent in

more traditional TAT-type measures of motives (see Chapter IV & Chapter V for more

details).

43
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

References

Ames, C. (1992). Classroom goals, structures, and student motivation. Journal of Educational

Psychology, 84(3), 261-271.

Arnould, E., Price, L., & Zinkhan, G. (2004). Consumers. New York: McGraw-Hill Book

Co.

Atkinson, J.W. (1981). Studying personality in the context of an advanced motivational

psychology. American Psychologist, 36, 117-128.

Atkinson, J.W., Heyns, R.W., & Veroff, J. (1958). The effect of experimental arousal of the

affiliation motive on thematic apperception. In J.W. Atkinson (Ed.), Motives in

fantasy, action, and society: a method of assessment and study (p. 95-104). Priceton,

NJ: Van Nostrand.

Austin, J.T., & Vancouver, J.B. (1996). Goal constructs in psychology: structure, process

and content. Psychological Bulletin, 120(3), 338-375.

Bagozzi, R.P., & Dholakia, U. (1999). Goal setting and goal striving in consumer behavior.

Journal of Marketing, 63, 19-32.

Bandura, A. (1989). Self-regulation of motivation and action through internal standards and

goal systems. In L.A. Pervin (Ed.), Goal concepts in personality and social

psychology (p. 19-56). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Barsalou, L.W. (1985). Ideals, central tendency and frequency of instantiation as

determinants of graded structure in categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology,

11(4), 629-654.

Beach, L.R. (1990). Image theory: decision making in personal and organizational contexts.

Chichester: Wiley.

44
Chapter II : Consumer Motives – Literature Review

Bettman, J.R. (1979). An information processing theory of consumer choice. Reading, MA:

Addison-Wesley.

Biernat, M. (1989). Motives and values to achieve: different constructs with different effects.

Journal of Personality, 57(1), 69-95.

Bilsky, W., & Schwartz, S.H. (1994). Values and personality. European Journal of

Personality, 8, 163-181.

Blackwell, R.D., Miniard, P.W., & Engel, J.F. (2001). Consumer Behavior (9th edition). Fort

Worth: Harcourt College Publishers.

Borkenau, P. (1990). Traits as ideal-based and goal-derived social categories. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 381-396.

Brody, N. (1980). Social motivation. Annual Review of Psychology, 31, 143-168.

Brunstein, J.C., Lautenschlager, U., Nawroth, B., Pöhlmann, K., & Schultheiss, O. (1995).

Personal goals, social motives, and emotional well-being. Zeitschrift für Differentielle

und Diagnostische Psychologie, 16, 1-10.

Brunstein, J.C., Schultheiss, O.C., & Grässmann, R. (1998). Personal goals and emotional

well-being: the moderating role of motive dispositions. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 75(2), 494-508.

Callebaut, J., Janssens, M., Op de Beeck, D., Lorré, D., & Hendrickx, H. (1999).

Motivational marketing research revisited. Leuven-Apeldoorn: Garant.

Callebaut, J., Hendrickx, H., & Janssens, M. (2002). The naked consumer today. Leuven-

Apeldoorn: Garant.

Cantor, N. (1990). From thought to behavior: ‘Having’ and ‘Doing’ in the study of

personality and cognition. American Psychologist, 45, 735-750.

45
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Cantor, N., & Blanton, H. (1996). Effortful pursuit of personal goals in daily life. In P.M.

Gollwitzer, & J.A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: linking cognition and

motivation to behavior (p. 338-359). New York: Guilford Press.

Cantor, N., & Kihlstrom, J.F. (1987). Personality and social intelligence. Englewood Cliffs,

NJ: Prentice Hall.

Carver, C.S, & Scheier, M.F. (1981). Attention and self-regulation: a control theory

approach to human behavior. New York: Springer.

Carver, C.S., & Scheier, M.F. (1996). On the self-regulation of behavior. New York:

Cambridge University Press.

Chulef, A.S., Read, S.J., & Walsh, D.A. (2001). A hierarchical taxonomy of human goals.

Motivation and Emotion, 25(3), 191-232.

Claeys, C., Swinnen, A., & Vanden Abeele, P. (1995). Consumers’ means-end chains for

think and feel products. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 12, 193-208.

Colby, K. M. (1960). An introduction to psychoanalytic research. New York: Basic.

Costa, P.T., & McCrae, R.R. (2001). A theoretical context for adult temperament. In T.D.

Wachs, & G.A. Kohnstamm (Eds.), Temperament in context (p. 1-21). Mahwah, NJ:

Lawrence Erlbaum.

Cropanzano, R. (1993). Justice in the workplace. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Deci, E.L., & Ryan, R.M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human

behavior. New York: Plenum.

Dichter, E. (1960). A strategy of desire. Garden City, N.Y.: Doubleday.

Dosajh, N.L. (1996). Projective techniques with particular reference to ink-blot tests.

Journal of Projective Psychology and Mental Health, 3, 59-68.

Drenth, P.J., Thierry, H., Willems, P.J., & De Wolf, C.J. (1984). Handbook of work and

organizational psychology. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

46
Chapter II : Consumer Motives – Literature Review

Dweck, C. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. American Psychologist,

41(10), 1040-1048.

Edwards, A.L. (1957). The social desirability variable in personality assessment and

research. New York: Dryden Press.

Emmons, R.A. (1986). Personal strivings: an approach to personality and subjective well-

being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1058-1068.

Emmons, R.A., & King, L.A. (1992). Thematic analysis, experience sampling and personal

goals. In C.P. Smith, & J.W. Atkinson (Eds.), Motivation and personality: handbook

of thematic content analysis (p. 13-86). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Emmons, R.A., & McAdams, D.P. (1991). Personal strivings and motive dispositions:

exploring the links. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 648-654.

Emmons, R.A.. (1989). The personal striving approach to personality. In L.A. Pervin (Ed.),

Goal concepts in personality and social psychology (p. 87-125). Hillsdale, NJ:

Lawrence Erlbaum.

Entwisle, D.E. (1972). To dispel fantasies about fantasy-based measures of achievement

motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 77, 377-391.

Eysenck, H. J. (1986). Failure of treatment--failure of theory? Behavioral and Brain Sciences,

9, 236.

Fisher, R.J. (1993). Social desirability bias and the validity of indirect questioning. Journal

of Consumer Research, 20, 303-314.

Fleeson, W., Zirkel, S., & Smith, E.E. (1995). Mental representations of trait categories and

their influences on person perception. Social Cognition, 13, 365-397.

Ford, M.E., & Nichols, C.W. (1987). A taxonomy of human goals and some possible

applications. In M.E. Ford, & D.H. Ford (Eds.), Humans as Self-constructing

Systems: Putting the Framework to work. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

47
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Foxall, G.R., & Goldsmith, R.E. (1994). Consumer psychology for marketing. London-New

York: Routledge.

Freud, S. (1933). New introductory lectures on psycho-analysis. New York: W.W. Norton.

Freud, S. (1990). Beyond the pleasure principle. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.

Gardner, D.M. (1972). An exploratory investigation of achievement motivation effects on

consumer behavior. Proceedings of the Third ACR Annual Conference, 20-33.

Gellner, E. (1985). The Psychoanalytic movement. London: Paladin.

Ger, G. (1997). Human development and humane consumption: well-being beyond the good

life. Journal of Public Policy and Marketing, 16, 110-125.

Graftdijk, T. (1986). Psychoanalytische theorie 2. Boom: Meppel.

Greenwald, A.G., McGhee, D.E., & Schwartz, J.L.K. (1998). Measuring individual

differences in implicit cognition: the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 74, 1464-1480.

Grünbaum, A. (1986). Précis of The foundations of psychoanalysis: A philosophical critique.

Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 9, 217-284.

Grunert, K.G., & Grunert, S.C. (1995). Measuring subjective meaning structures by the

laddering method: theoretical considerations and methodological problems.

International Journal of Research in Marketing, 12, 209-225.

Gutman, J. (1982). A means–end chain model based on consumer categorization processes.

Journal of Marketing, 46(Spring), 60–72.

Gutman, J. (1997). Means-end chains as goal hierarchies. Psychology and Marketing, 14(6),

545-560.

Harackiewicz, J.M., & Elliot, A.J. (1998). The joint effects of target and purpose goals on

intrinsic motivation: a mediational analysis. Personality and Social Psychology

Bulletin, 24(7), 675-689.

48
Chapter II : Consumer Motives – Literature Review

Harrell, G.D. (1986). Consumer behavior. Michigan: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

Heckenhausen, H., & Kuhl, J. (1985). From wishes to action: the dead ends and short cuts on

the long way to action. In R.R. Vallacher, & D.M. Wegner, D.M. (Eds.), A theory of

action identification. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Hofer, J., & Chasiotis, A. (2004). Methodological considerations of applying a TAT-type

picture-story test in cross-cultural research. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,

35(2), 224-241.

Holtgraves, T., Eck, J., & Lasky, B. (1997). Face management, question wording, and social

desirability. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 27, 1650-1672.

Huffman, C., Ratneshwar, S., & Mick, D.G. (2000). Consumer goal structures and goal-

determination processes: an integrative framework. In S. Ratneshwar, D.G. Mick, &

C. Huffman (Eds.). The why of consumption: contemporary perspectives on consumer

motives, goals and desires (pp. 9-35). London/New York: Routledge.

Jung, C.G., von Franz, M.L., Henderson, J.L., Jacobi, J., & Jaffe, A. (1964). Man and his

symbols. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.

King, L.A. (1995). Wishes, motives, goals, and personal memories: relations of measures of

human motivation. Journal of Personality, 63, 985-1007.

King, L.A., Richards, J.H., & Stemmerich, E. (1998). Daily goals, life goals, and worst fears,

means, ends, and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality, 66(5), 713-744.

Klinger, E. (1995). Effects of motivation and emotion on thought flow and cognition:

assessment and findings. In P.E. Shrout, & S.T. Fiske (Eds.), Personality research,

methods, and theory (p. 257-270). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Koestner, R., & McClelland, D.C. (1990). Perspectives on competence motivation. In L.A.

Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: theory and research (p. 527-548). New York:

Guilford Press.

49
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Kruglanski, A. (1989). Lay epistemics and human knowledge: cognitive and motivational

bases. New York: Plenum.

Kuhl, J. (1994). Motivation and volition. In G. d’Ydewalle, P. Eelen, & P. Bertelson (Eds.),

International perspectives on psychological science (Vol. 2, p. 311-340). Howe,

England: Erlbaum.

Landon, E.L. (1972). A sex role explanation of purchase intention differences of consumers

who are high and low in need for achievement. Proceedings of the Third ACR Annual

Conference, 1-8.

Lilienfeld, S.O., Wood, J.M., & Garb, H.N. (2000). The scientific status of projective

techniques. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 1(2), 27-66.

Little, B.R. (1983). Personal projects: a rationale and method for investigation. Environment

and behavior, 15, 273-309.

Maison, D., Greenwald, A.G., & Bruin, R.H. (2004). Predictive validity of the Implicit

Association Test in studies of brands, consumer attitudes and behavior. Journal of

Consumer Psychology, 14, 405-416.

Markus, H.R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: implications for cognition,

emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224-253.

Maslow, A. (1943). A theory of human motivation. Psychological Review, 50, 370-396.

Maslow, A.H. (1970). Motivation and personality (2nd ed.). New York: harper and Row.

McAdams, D.P. (1988). Power, intimacy, and the life story: personological inquiries into

identity. New York: Guilford Press.

McAdams, D.P., & Powers, J. (1981). Themes of intimacy in behavior and thought. Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 573-587.

50
Chapter II : Consumer Motives – Literature Review

McClelland, D.C. (1958). The importance of early learning in the formation of motives. In

J.W. Atkinson (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society: a method of assessment

and study (p. 437-452). Priceton, NJ: Van Nostrand.

McClelland, D.C. (1961). The achieving society. Princeton, New Jersey: Van Nostrand.

McClelland, D.C. (1980). Motive dispositions: the merits of operant and respondent

measures. In L. Wheeler (Ed.), Review of Personality and Social Psychology (Vol. 1,

p. 10-41). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

McClelland, D.C. (1985). Human Motivation. Glenview, IL: Scott Foresman and Company.

McClelland, D.C. (1985b). How motives, skills and values determine what people do.

American Psychologist, 40, 812-823.

McClelland, D.C. (1987). Human Motivation. Cambridge UK: Cambridge University Press.

McClelland, D.C., & Pilon, D.A. (1983). Sources of adult motives in patterns of parent

behavior in early childhood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 564-

574.

McClelland, D.C., Atkinson, J.W., Clark, R.A., & Lowell, E.L. (1953). The achievement

motive. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

McClelland, D.C., Koestner, R., & Weinberger, J. (1989). How do self attributed and implicit

motives differ? In F. Halisch, & J.H.L. Van Den Bercken (Eds.), International

perspectives on achievement and task motivation (p. 259-289). Amsterdam: Swets &

Zeitlinger

Mehrabian, A. (1968). Male and female scales of the tendency to achieve. Educational and

Psychological Measurement, 28, 493-502.

Mehrabian, A. (1969). Measures of achieving tendency. Educational and Psychological

Measurement, 29, 443-451.

Meyers, D.G. (1998). Psychology. New York: Worth Publishers.

51
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Morgan, C.D., & Murray, H.A. (1935). A method for investigating fantasies. The Archives of

Neurology and Psychiatry, 34, 389-406.

Morgan, W.G. (2002). Origin and history of the earliest Thematic Apperception Test

Pictures. Journal of Personality Assessment, 79(3), 422-445.

Mowen, J.C. & Minor, M. (1997). Consumer behavior. Fifth Edition, Prentice Hall.

Murray, H.A. (1938). Explorations in Personality. New York: Oxford University Press.

Murray, H.A. (1943). The Thematic Apperception Test: Mannual. Cambridge, M.A.:

Harvard University Press.

Murray, H.A. (1955). Types of human needs. In D.C. McClelland (Ed.), Studies in

motivation (p. 63-66). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Nevis, E.C. (1983). Cultural assumptions and productivity: the United States and China.

Sloan Management Review, 24(3), 17-29.

Newell, A., & Simon, H.A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice

Hall.

Nuttin, J. (1984). Motivation, planning, and action: a relational theory of behavior dynamics.

Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Olson, J.C., & Reynolds, T.J. (1983). Understanding consumers’ cognitive structures:

implications for marketing strategy. In L. Percy, & A.G. Woodside (Eds.).

Advertising and consumer psychology (pp. 77-90). Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.

Olver, J.M., & Mooradian, T.A. (2003). Personality traits and personal values: a conceptual

and empirical integration. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 109-125.

Pervin, L.A. (1994). A critical analysis of current trait theory. Psychological Inquiry, 5(2),

103-113.

Pieters, R. (1993). A control view on the behavior of consumers: turning the triangle.

European Journal of Marketing, 27(8), 17-27.

52
Chapter II : Consumer Motives – Literature Review

Pieters, R., Baumgartner, H., & Allen, D. (1995). A means-end chain approach to consumer

goal structures. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 12, 227-244.

Popper, K. (1986). Predicting overt behavior versus predicting hidden states. Behavioral and

Brain Sciences, 9, 254-255.

Read, S.J., & Lalwani, N. (2000). Testing a narrative model of trait inferences: the role of

goals. Unpublished manuscript, University of Southern California.

Read, S.J., Jones, D.K., & Miller, L.C. (1990). Traits as goal based categories, the

importance of goals in the coherence of dispositional categories. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 58(6), 1048-1061.

Reeve, J. (1992). Understanding motivation and emotion. Fort Worth: Holt, Rinehart and

Winston, Inc.

Reynolds, T., & Gutman, J. (1988). Laddering theory: method, analysis, and interpretation.

Journal of Advertising Research, 28(1), 11-31.

Reynolds, T.J., Westberg, S.J., & Olson, J.C. (1994). A strategic framework for developing

and assessing political, social issue, and corporate image advertising. In L. Kahle

(Ed.), Advertising and consumer psychology. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.

Richins, M.L. (2005). What consumers desire: goals and motives in the consumption

environment. In S. Ratneshwar and D.G. Mick (Eds.), Inside consumption (p. 340-

348). New York: Routledge.

Roberts, B.W., O’Donnell, M., & Robins, R.W. (2004). Goal and personality trait

development in emerging adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

87(4), 541-550.

Ryan, R., & Deci, E. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic

motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55(1), 68-

78.

53
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Schewe, C.D. (1973). Selected social psychological models for analyzing buyers. Journal of

Marketing, 37, 31-39.

Schiffman, L.G., & Kanuk, L.L. (1983). Consumer behavior (2nd ed.). London/New Jersey:

Prentice Hall.

Schmidt, L.C., & Frieze, I.M. (1997). A mediational model of power, affiliation and

achievement motives and product involvement. Journal of Business and Psychology,

4, 425-446.

Schwartz, S.H., & Bilsky, W. (1987). Toward a universal psychological structure of human

values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 550-562.

Sheth, J.N., Mittal, B., & Newman, B.I. (1999). Customer behavior: consumer behavior and

beyond. Orlando: The Dryden Press.

Smith, C.P. (1992). Motivation and personality: handbook of thematic content analysis. New

York: Cambridge University Press.

Snyder, M., & Omoto, A.M. (1992). Who helps and why? The psychology of AIDS

volunteerism. In S. Spacapan, & S. Oskamp (Eds.), Helping and being helped:

naturalistic studies (p. 213-219). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Solomon, M.R. (1992). Consumer behavior. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Stagner, R. (1994). Traits and Theoreticians. Psychological Inquiry, 5(2), 166-169.

Suarez-Orozco, M.M. (1989). Psychological aspects of achievement motivation among

recent hispanic immigrants. In H. Trueba, G. Spindler, & L. Spindler (Eds.),

Anthropological perspectives on dropping out (p. 99-116). London: Falmer Press.

Tauber, E.M. (1972). Why do people shop? Journal of Marketing, 36, 46-59.

Thompson, M., Grace, C., & Cohen, L. (2001). Best friends, worst enemies: Understanding

the social lives of children. New York: Ballantine Books.

54
Chapter II : Consumer Motives – Literature Review

Tuerlinckx, F., De Boeck, P., & Lens, W. (2002). Measuring needs with the Thematic

Apperception Test: a psychometric study. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 82(3), 448-461.

Urdan, T., & Maehr, M. (1995). Beyond a two-goal theory of motivation and achievement: A

case for social goals. Review of Educational Research, 65(3), 213-243.

Vallacher, R.R., & Wegner, D.M. (1985). A theory of action identification. Hillsdale, NJ:

Lawrence Erlbaum.

Veroff, J. (1982). Assertive motivations: Achievement versus Power. In D.G. Winter, & A.J.

Stewart (Eds.), Motivation and society. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Veroff, J., & Smith, D.A. (1985). Motives and values over the adult years. In D. Kleiber, &

M. Maehr (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievements: motivation and

adulthood (p. 1-53). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Walters, C.G., & Bergiel, B.J. (1989). Consumer behavior: a decision-making approach.

Ohio: South Western Publ.

Weinberger, J., & McClelland, D.C. (1990). Cognitive versus traditional motivational

models: irreconcilable or complementary? In E.T. Higgins, & R.M. Sorrentino (Eds.),

Handbook of motivation and cognition: foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, p. 562-

597). New York: Guilford Press.

Wicker, F.W., Lambert, F.B., Richardson, F.C., & Kahler, J. (1984). Categorical goal

hierarchies and classification of human motives. Journal of Personality, 52(3), 285-

305.

Winter, D.G. (1973). The power motive. New York: Free Press.

Winter, D.G. (1991). Manual for scoring motive imagery in running text (3rd ed.).

Unpublished Manuscript, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann

Arbor.

55
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Winter, D.G. (1996). Personality: analysis and interpretation of lives. New York: McGraw-

Hill.

Winter, D.G., John, O.P., Stewart, A.J., Klohnen, E.C., & Duncan, L.E. (1998). Traits and

motives: toward an integration of two traditions in personality research.

Psychological Review, 105(2), 230-250.

Wurf, E., & Markus, H. (1991). Possible selves and the psychology of personal growth. In

R. Hogan, D. Ozer, J.M. Healy, & A.J. Stewart (Eds.), Perspectives in personality:

Vol. 3A. Self and emotion (p. 39-62). London: Jessica Kingsley.

56
A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives : Cross-Validity

CHAPTER III

A TAXONOMY OF CONSUMER MOTIVES THROUGH PREFERRED

BRAND PERSONALITY : EMPIRICAL FINDINGS FOR 11

COUNTRIES

This chapter has been presented at the Winter Conference 2005 of the Society for Consumer

Psychology (SCP) (held in St.Pete Beach Florida)

57
Chapter III

A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives through Preferred Brand Personality :

Empirical Findings for 11 countries

1. Abstract

This paper focuses on the cross-cultural validation of Callebaut et al.’s (1999) consumer

motive taxonomy, based on respondents’ valence ratings of a set of 34 preferred brand

personality traits. Results of INDSCAL analysis reveal a consistent, replicable preferred

brand personality structure across 11 different countries and four different product categories.

As preferred brand personality dimensions are considered as behavioral expressions of

underlying motives, the found structure was reformulated in terms of Callebaut et al.’s (1999)

taxonomy of eight consumer motives i.e. Vitality, Pleasure, Conviviality, Belonging, Security,

Control, Recognition and Power, which are determined by two basic, bipolar dimensions, i.e.

intrapersonal versus interpersonal axis. Callebaut et al.’s (1999) taxonomy appears to overlap

considerably with prior classifications of human motives and, in addition, has a number of

advantages over these previous taxonomies. Opportunities in the context of consumer

research are discussed, as well as some directions for future research.²

59
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

2. Introduction

Like people, brands often are personified by consumers as having personalities (e.g. Fournier,

1998; Caprara et al., 2001). Hence, brand personality can be defined formally as “the set of

human characteristics associated with a brand” (Aaker, 1997, p. 347). As previous research

on brand personality took a descriptive approach – with as a main objective to reduce the

brand personality domain to a limited number of distinct factors or dimensions – a clear

picture is provided of how the concept is perceived in consumer minds. For instance, the

most influential theoretical structure of brand personality was developed by Aaker (1997),

who organized the field among five broad dimensions, termed Sincerity, Excitement,

Competence, Sophistication and Ruggedness. However, despite these structural insights, little

is known about how brand personality relates to more fundamental consumer motives, i.e.

which brand personality traits do consumers prefer in the light of underlying motive

satisfaction. When talking about people’s preferred brand personality traits in a given

situation, we move to the concept of preferred brand personality, which can be defined as the

personality traits associated with a preferred brand in a given product category. The main

objective of the present study is to explore if a generalizable structure of preferred brand

personality dimensions exists, that could be considered as expressive of Callebaut et al.’s

(1999) taxonomy of consumer motives.

By integrating conceptual insights about both the relationship between motives and human

personality (e.g. Read et al., 1990; Winter et al., 1998) and the self-expressive use of brands

by consumers (e.g. Sirgy, 1982), we posit that preferred brand personality traits are associated

with particular consumer motives that have to be satisfied. By choosing a brand with a

personality that is congruent with some desired human personality in a certain situation (e.g.

60
Chapter III : A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives

Aaker, 1999) – with this desired personality being function of some manifest motives in that

situation – people may try to execute these underlying motives. In this manner, preferred

brands may achieve some strategic potentiality in the light of occasion-based implicit motive-

states.

By taking such a motive-based approach to the concept of brand personality, this study

provides a more explanatory point of view and gives further insight in why people have a

preference for some brands with specific personalities above others (e.g. Biel, 1993). In what

follows, an outline is given of the conceptual background underlying the proposed

relationship between motives and preferred brand personality, and an explanation is provided

of the empirical method and the results. Then, a motive-based interpretation is suggested of

the found structure of preferred brand personalities, followed by a general discussion and

some directions for future research.

3. Conceptual background

3.1. Relationship between Motives and Human Personality

Over the past few decades, two distinct lines of research – one focused on who people are

(personality traits or dispositions) and the other on what people strive for in their lives

(motives) – have emerged within the field of personality psychology (e.g. Roberts and

Robins, 2000). Dispositional researchers or trait theorists (e.g. Allport, 1937; McCrae, 1994)

have searched for a generalizable taxonomy of personality trait terms in an attempt to assess

the basic structure of human personality. Currently, the most popular view among

dispositional researchers is that five robust factors or dimensions exist that underlie the

61
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

personality construct, commonly labelled the ‘Big Five’ dimensions of personality (e.g.

McCrae and John, 1992; Goldberg, 1993; Wiggins and Trapnell, 1997). These five

personality dimensions are named Extraversion, Neuroticism, Openness to Experience

/Intellect, Agreeableness and Conscientiousness (e.g. Tupes and Christal, 1961; Goldberg,

1990; McCrae and Costa, 1996). Whereas there is a striking convergence on the ‘Big Five’ as

the basic structure within the personality domain, no taxonomy with the same impact has been

developed yet in the motive domain. Though several fair attempts have been made to classify

motive units (i.e. goals, needs, motives) into a limited number of content domains (e.g.

Murray, 1938; McClelland, 1961; Maslow, 1970; Wicker et al., 1984; Ford and Nichols,

1987), none of the existing taxonomies has proved entirely satisfactory to the field (cf. supra:

Chapter II). As a result, many motive researchers changed the focus to the hierarchical

structure of the motive domain, ranging from fairly concrete sub-ordinate goals that guide

specific acts to rather abstract super-ordinate goals in the form of basic motives that regulate

behavior (e.g. Pieters et al., 1995; Bagozzi and Dholakia, 1999; Huffman et al., 2000).

Despite the fact that the above-explained concepts of traits and motives have usually led

separate lives – each confined to its own theoretical school or camp, with little opportunity for

interaction or exchange – the question of the relationship between them is of particular

concern (e.g. Pervin, 1994a). Though general agreement exists that human personality traits

and motives are mutually related, fewer consensuses are reached concerning the nature and

direction of this relationship (e.g. Pervin, 1994b). Whereas some authors consider motives to

be predicted by underlying dispositions (e.g. Cantor, 1990; Stagner, 1994; Olver and

Mooradian, 2003; Roberts et al., 2004), more often in literature a reverse causal relationship

has been established, in which motives are likely to underlie traits (e.g. Borkenau, 1990;

Fleeson et al., 1995). From this second point of view, human personality traits are considered

62
Chapter III : A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives

as motive-based concepts (e.g. Barsalou, 1985; Read et al., 1990), which furnish the resources

to express these underlying motives (e.g. Winter et al., 1998). Hence, dispositions can be

seen as a function of intrinsic motives (e.g. Read and Lalwani, 2000).

As stronger theoretical (e.g. Austin and Vancouver, 1996) and empirical (e.g. Omoto and

Snyder, 1990, 1993; Snyder and Omoto, 1992) evidence is provided for the latter standpoint

about trait-motive relationships compared to the former, the authors of the present study adopt

the latter conceptualization in which personality traits are considered to flow from underlying

motives and as a result direct the ways in which these motives are expressed.

3.2. Relationship between Human Personality and Brand Personality

Besides the ability of brand personality to differentiate brands in a product category (e.g.

Halliday, 1996), one important aspect of brand personality refers to its symbolic or self-

expressive value for consumers (e.g. Keller, 1993; Kleine et al., 1993). By choosing a brand

with a personality that is congruent with either their actual (e.g. Belk, 1988) or ideal (e.g.

Malhotra, 1988) personality, consumers are able to express their sense of self to themselves

and to others (e.g. Batra et al., 1993; Kim et al., 2001; Helgeson and Supphellen, 2004).

Hence, a relationship can be established between consumer’s personality and the personality

of a preferred brand (e.g. Dolich, 1969; Sirgy, 1982; Zinkhan and Hong, 1991). The larger

the congruency between the personality traits that describe an individual’s (actual or ideal)

self and those that describe a brand, the more favorable brand evaluations should be and as a

result, the more pronounced the preference for the brand (e.g. Biel, 1993; Hem and Iversen,

2002).

63
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Although conceptually relevant, empirical support for the self-expressive use of brands has

remained elusive (e.g. Kassarjian, 1971). A primary reason might be the conceptualization by

many authors of the self as a unitary construct that remains invariant across situations (e.g.

Bellenger et al., 1976; Grubb and Stern, 1971). A prevailing assumption has been that an

individual has a stable set of personality traits and therefore should behave similarly across

contexts. However, in the last few years, considerable amount of research has shown that the

self is a malleable construct, consisting of both chronically accessible and situation specific

personality traits (e.g. Linville and Carlston, 1994; Aaker, 1999). As a result, consumers are

likely to act differently across situations and – in addition to self-congruent brands, i.e.

brands that are congruent with people’s chronically accessible personality traits – also have

preferences for brands with personalities that are congruent with personality traits that are

elicited by a certain situation. The premise is that different situational cues invoke different

needs for certain types of personalities to be present. To express the desired personality type

in a given situation, people choose a brand with a congruent personality, a process referred to

here as ‘situation congruity’ (e.g. Kalar, 2003). Both ‘situation congruity’ and the concept of

‘self congruity’ as explained above are essential elements in understanding how brands are

used by consumers for self-expressive purposes.

3.3. Relationship between Motives and Brand Personality

As far as we know, no studies exist that explicitly examine the relationship between preferred

brand personality, i.e. the personality of a preferred brand in a given product category, and

consumer motives. However, an integration of the previously established relationships –

respectively between motives and human personality and human personality and brand

64
Chapter III : A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives

personality – enables us to discover how these two concepts can be conceptually related to

each other.

As preferred brand personality traits seem to be expressions of human personality dimensions

(see earlier: Sirgy, 1982; Keller, 1993; Helgeson and Supphellen, 2004) and, in addition,

human personality traits are likely to be function of underlying motives (see earlier: Read et

al., 1990; Winter et al., 1998; Read and Lalwani, 2000), preferred brand personality is prone

to indirectly channel the ways in which consumer motives are expressed. Actually we

suggest that different consumer situations cause different motives to become manifest.

Consequently, a desire is invoked for different types of human personalities to be present. To

express the human personality type that is manifest in a given situation, people prefer a brand

with a congruent personality (e.g. Aaker, 1999; Kalar, 2003). Hence, the preferred brand –

through its link with the desired human personality – derives some satisfaction potentiality

with regard to the manifest motives. Following Means-End reasoning (e.g. Gutman, 1982;

Olson and Reynolds, 1983), we could say preferred brand personality traits at a lower level

are instrumental to express some desired human personality, which in its turn directs the

behavioral expression of higher-level motives as ends. Depending on which types of human

personalities are desired in a certain situation – which is function of the manifest motives in

that situation – consumers may prefer brands with different personalities.

As mentioned in the introduction, the objective of this study is to discover a generalizable

structure of preferred brand personality dimensions that could be considered as the behavioral

expression of Callebaut et al.’s (1999) taxonomy of consumer motives. In order to meet this

purpose a series of empirical studies were conducted in 11 different countries and among

65
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

brands from four different product categories. In the next section, an overview is given of this

empirical research.

4. Empirical studies

4.1. Personality trait generation

In order to develop a manageable set of preferred brand personality attributes, a three-step

process was followed, similar to the one used in the brand personality literature (e.g. Aaker et

al., 2001). First, to ensure familiarity and relevance of the attributes, a free-association task

was conducted in which participants recruited from the 11 research countries (N=55) were

asked to write down the personality traits that first come to mind when thinking about their

preferred brand in a range of product categories, a process yielding a list of 99 attributes.

Second, to maximize content representation, we added 42 items from the brand personality

scale of Aaker (1997) as well as the 60-item NEO-five factor inventory, developed by Costa

and McCrae (1992) to measure the Big Five human personality dimensions. Finally, from the

total set of 201 personality attributes, those attributes were eliminated that were redundant

(N=81), ambiguous (N=46) or judged to be less relevant to preferred brands in general

(N=40)4. Hence, this resulted in a final set of 34 personality items that were used in the

empirical studies (see Appendix A).

4
The exclusion of both redundant (e.g. ‘reliable’ arose from the free-association task as well as from Aaker (1997)) and ambiguous (e.g.
‘slight’, ‘rigid’,’ unfocused’) items was based on an independent decision of the researcher. To identify the relatively irrelevant items, a
sample of 25 subjects (N female = 13; mean age = 31,7) rated how descriptive 74 attributes (201 minus the redundant and ambiguous items)
were of preferred brands in general (1= not at all descriptive; 7= extremely descriptive). To isolate the most relevant traits, the cut off for the
final list of personality traits was an average scale rating of 6 (very descriptive), thereby leaving a final set of 34 items (cf. Aaker, 1997;
2001).

66
Chapter III : A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives

4.2. Stimuli selection

In order to test the generalizability of the findings, a range of product categories was drawn

upon: two fast-mover goods (shampoo and beer), one durable good (mobile phones) and one

more social theme (political preference). Within each category, a set of salient, well-known

brands was chosen, each time adapted to the local market of a particular research country5.

These brands had to represent a broad spectrum of personality types, in order to enhance the

scope of the scale. To refresh memory of the brands, subjects were given a color picture of

each brand in a particular category6.

4.3. Method

4.3.1. Subjects

The external validity and generalizability of the findings also depended on the subjects chosen

to participate in the study. Hence, a set of samples maximally diverse in such factors as

culture, language, and geographical region was selected. The samples, which are briefly

characterized in Table 3.1, were drawn from 11 countries, representing every continent and 11

different languages. In all countries, a non-student (general) quota-sample was used, one that

represented the research population with respect to several quotas of interest (age, gender,

occupation, and product use). In one country (Belgium), two additional homogeneous student

samples were chosen in order to detect potential differences with the heterogeneous non-

student samples.

5
In case of the social theme ‘political preference’ in Belgium, the seven major political parties were chosen as ‘brands’.
6
In the case of the political parties, a picture was offered of each of the emblems of the parties

67
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Table 3.1 : Characteristics of samples

Country N Language Product category % Age


Female Mean
Belgium
students 1 182 Dutch shampoo 48.3 21
students 2 158 Dutch shampoo 55.1 22
general 178 Dutch political preference 57.9 39
Australia
general 586 English beer 22.8 40
Canada
general 3212 English beer 0 42
41 French 0 42
Brazil
general 200 Portuguese mobile phones 52.5 36
China
general 189 Chinese mobile phones 49.7 32
India
general 191 Hindi/English mobile phones 49.7 34
Mexico
general 197 Mexican mobile phones 51.3 36
Russia
general 200 Russian mobile phones 50.0 35
Indonesia
general 200 Indonesian mobile phones 54.0 34
Turkey
general 194 Turkish mobile phones 49.5 28
Egypt
general 203 Arabic mobile phones 39.4 32

4.3.2. Procedure

Subjects were asked to select their preferred brand out of all brands in a given product

category, for which they had to fill out the brand personality scale of 34 items. Using a five-

point Likert Scale (1= not descriptive at all, 5= very descriptive), subjects rated the extent to

which each personality item describes the personality of their preferred brand. The items

were presented in a list. Prior to rating the items, respondents read the whole list, chose a

limited number of items most descriptive of their preferred brand, which they gave the score 5,

68
Chapter III : A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives

and then chose the items least descriptive of their brand, which they gave the score 1. Next,

they rated the remaining items. Assumed was that the pattern of words judged to be (very)

descriptive of the preferred brand would be a good reflection of the concept of preferred brand

personality which we intended to measure.

4.3.3. Multidimensional scaling (INDSCAL)

The data obtained by the previous procedure were analyzed by means of INDSCAL (Carroll

and Chang, 1970)7. This is one of a variety of multidimensional scaling (MDS) techniques,

which place or ‘map’ stimuli (in this case personality items) onto a set of dimensions. The

distances between the points represent the empirical relations among the stimuli. The more

conceptually similar two personality items are supposed to be, the more related they should be

empirically, and hence, the closer to each other their positions should be in the visual map.

INDSCAL first generates a ‘group stimulus space’, which is a composite MDS space for all

samples in the analysis. Next, it provides a ‘subject weight space’, allowing for comparison

of each individual sample with the average stimulus space. As mentioned before, the

objective is to explore whether it is possible to discover a stable (across countries and product

categories) structure underlying the concept of preferred brand personality. The results of the

INDSCAL analysis are reported in the next section.

7
INDSCAL (Individual Differences Scaling) provides analysis of three-way, two-mode (dis)similarity data matrices by means of an scalar-
products/ weighted Euclidean distance model, using a linear (metric) transformation of the data. The solution consists of a Group Space
(whose dimensions are fixed), and each of the elements in the third way (e.g. individuals, occasions, times etc.) is thought of as applying a
set of (non-negative) weights to the dimensions, to produce a “private” space. The pattern of individual dimensional weights is represented
in a Subject Space (e.g. Carroll and Chang, 1970).

69
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

4.4. Results

4.4.1. Group Stimulus Space

Figure 3.1 shows the composite MDS space for all 13 studies in the analysis (11 quota-

samples + 2 student samples). With regard to the dimensionality of the solution, a good fit of

the INDSCAL model to the data was found in two dimensions. The proportion of variance

explained by the two-dimensional model (Dispersion Accounted For (DAF)) was 90%, with

an acceptable Normalized Raw Stress (NRS) measure of .09. No substantial improvement, in

terms of variance accounted for and stress, was found in three dimensions, so the two-

dimensional solution was used in further analysis.

Figure 3.1 : INDSCAL analysis – Group Stimulus Space

70
Chapter III : A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives

4.4.2. Subject Weight Space

Table 3.2 gives an overview of the individual dimension weights and NRS measures for each

sample in the analysis. Examination of these weights suggests that there is a good

correspondence of the individual perceptual spaces and the aggregate perceptual map (see Fig.

3.1). The weights show few substantial differences on both dimensions and also the

corresponding individual NRS measures are acceptable (ranging between .04 and .13), which

means that all samples employ similar combinations of the dimensions from the common

group space.

Table 3.2 : INDSCAL analysis – Dimension weights / NRS measures

Dimension Weights Normalized


Raw Stress
AXIS 1 AXIS 2

Study 1 (Belgium—political preferences) .47 .45 .12


Study 2 (Belgium—shampoo 1) .56 .39 .04
Study 3 (Belgium—shampoo 2) .53 .40 .09
Study 4 (Australia—beer) .45 .48 .12
Study 5 (Canada—beer) .48 .44 .11
Study 6 (Brazil—mobile phones) .43 .48 .13
Study 7 (China—mobile phones) .46 .46 .12
Study 8 (India—mobile phones) .45 .47 .13
Study 9 (Mexico—mobile phones) .45 .47 .12
Study 10(Russia—mobile phones) .43 .50 .09
Study 11(Indonesia—mobile phones) .48 .44 .11
Study 12 (Turkey—mobile phones) .54 .38 .10
Study 13 (Egypt—mobile phones) .50 .42 .11

71
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

In addition, Figure 3.2 shows a graphical portrayal of the individual samples based on their

weights. Here also, a good level of fit with the solution can be detected as no distinctive

clusters of individual samples emerge (in Fig. 3.2 all vectors are pointing in the same

direction) and all weights have relatively long distances from the origin. Finally, a very high

Tucker’s coefficient of congruence of .95 provides a third source of evidence that all

individual samples are well represented by the aggregate perceptual map, confirming the

robustness of the found structure in Fig. 3.1 across the different countries and different

product categories.

Figure 3.2 : INDSCAL analysis – Dimension Weights Plot

Study 1-13
1
0,6 2
3
0,5
4
0,4 5
Axis2

0,3 6
7
0,2 8
0,1 9
10
0
11
0 0,1 0,2 0,3 0,4 0,5 0,6
12
Axis1 13

72
Chapter III : A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives

5. Motive-based interpretation of the preferred brand personality

structure

As shown in Figure 3.3, based on a visual examination of the MDS distances between the 34

personality items, we hypothesize eight distinct preferred brand personality dimensions to

exist, as groupings of items respectively dominating the four orthogonal positions and the four

quadrants of the perceptual mapping seem empirically closest to each other. Conceptually,

each of the preferred brand personality dimensions is supposed to be congruent with some

desired human personality, with the latter being function of some manifest consumer motive.

Hence, indirectly – through their congruence with the desired human personalities – the eight

preferred brand personalities are prone to be expressive of the manifest motive dimensions (cf.

supra: section 3.3.).

Figure 3.3 : Hypothesized Preferred Brand Personality Dimensions

73
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

To evaluate unidimensionality and convergent validity of the hypothesized preferred brand

personality dimensions, a maximum likelihood confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was

undertaken (Jöreskog and Sörbom, 1993). After several iterations a satisfactory 8-factor

model was obtained, confirming the hypothesized structure (RMSEA=.042; Tucker Lewis

Index NNFI=.92; SRMR=.048; GFI=.86). Reliabilities of the constructs were measured by

the ‘composite reliability’ indicator of Bagozzi (1980). All factors exceeded the minimal

value of .60 (Bagozzi and Yi, 1988) (see Table 3.3). To test the discriminant validity each of

the 28 off-diagonal elements of Φ was fixed to 1.0, in turn, and the model was re-estimated.

Changes in the χ2 goodness-of-fit were statistically significant for all 28 comparisons (∆χ2

ranging from 170.23 up to 285.65, df=1, p<.01), indicating discriminant validity (Steenkamp

and Van Trijp, 1991).

Table 3.3 : Results of Confirmatory Factor Analysis : Measurement Model

Preferred Final Item labels Composite


brand number Reliability
personality of items
dimension

1 4 cheerful, bon-vivant, unrestrained, .73


impulsive
2 4 optimistic, great to get on with, .70
approachable, friendly
3 5 reliable, cooperative, familial, .67
sincere, sympathetic
4 5 simple, calm, careful, traditional, .72
introvert
5 4 self-disciplined, mature, organized, .73
punctual
6 4 intellectual, stylish, special, .78
sophisticated
7 4 powerful, leader, competitive, .81
successful
8 4 adventurous, creative, innovative, .73
contemporary

RMSEA=.042; Tucker Lewis Index NNFI=.92; SRMR=.048; GFI=.86

74
Chapter III : A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives

With respect to the nature of the found preferred brand personality structure, groupings of

items dominating the vertical axis of Figure 3.3 all suggest an intrapersonal orientation.

Upper-side traits (i.e. bon-vivant, unrestrained, impulsive and cheerful) seem to be congruent

with a rather unrestrained human personality type, focusing on enjoying life and being

cheerful whereas traits at the bottom-side of the vertical axis (i.e. self-disciplined, mature,

punctual and organized) represent a more disciplined human personality type, focusing on

punctuality and maturation. Based on the conceptual foundations of some robust motivational

theories8, the names determined to represent best the types of motive constructs expressed in

each of these two personality dimensions are respectively Pleasure, i.e. referring to the human

desire to follow impulses and emotions, to maximise pleasure without inhibition or self-

control (e.g. Enjoyment theory: Williams, 1968) versus Control, i.e. referring to the human

desire to suppress inner feelings and emotions and to behave in a way that is morally

acceptable (e.g. Categorization theory: McGuire, 1974). Furthermore, preferred brand

personality traits making up the horizontal axis seem to reflect an interpersonal orientation as

the focus is set on the relation between an individual and the social environment. Left-side

traits (i.e. successful, leader, powerful and competitive) show congruence with an

individualistic human personality characterized by superiority and standing out, whereas

right-side traits (i.e. familial, sympathetic, reliable, sincere, and cooperative) are reflective of

a rather cooperative human personality, focusing on altruism and caring for others. Hence,

the underlying motive dimensions corresponding to the nature of these two conflicting

personality types are respectively labelled Power, i.e. referring to the human desire to be

better than the rest, to feel superior, to be a leader (e.g. Self-assertion theory: McClelland,

1961; Schmidt and Frieze, 1997) versus Belonging, i.e. referring to the human desire to merge

8
A detailed explanation of the motivational theories that are referred to in this section is provided in Appendix B

75
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

into the group and to be accepted and supported by loved ones (e.g. Intimacy/Affiliation

theory: McAdams and Powers, 1981).

Concerning the four quadrantal positions of the preferred brand personality structure, traits in

the bottom-right quadrant of Fig. 3.3 (i.e. simple, traditional, calm, careful and introvert) are

congruent with a tranquillity seeking human personality type, focusing on safety, security and

emotional stability whereas traits in the upper-left quadrant (i.e. creative, innovative,

adventurous and contemporary) represent an opposite human personality type focusing on

activity, novelty and exploration. In their turn, these human personality types are prone to be

expressive of some underlying motive dimensions, termed respectively Security, i.e. referring

to the human desire to hide away, to feel safe, relaxed and be protected (e.g. Regression

theory: Freud, 1933; Lennox and Wolfe, 1984) versus Vitality, i.e. referring to the human

desire to experience adventure, to test one’s boundaries and discover new things (e.g.

Stimulation theory: McGuire, 1974; Huizinga, 1970). Finally, traits in the upper-right

quadrant (i.e. friendly, approachable, optimistic and great to get on with) seem to reflect a

human personality focusing on social openness and connectedness with others, whereas traits

in the bottom-left quadrant (i.e. sophisticated, intellectual, stylish and special) seem to be

expressive of a human personality type focusing on social recognition and intellectualism.

When looking for some conceptual congruence with existing motivational theories,

respectively manifest motives called Conviviality, i.e. referring to the human desire to be with

people, to open up socially, to share experiences and emotionally connect with others (e.g.

Affiliation theory: McAdams and Powers, 1981) versus Recognition, i.e. referring to the

human desire to be recognized by others for one’s skills, knowledge and appearance, to be

respected by others (e.g. Achievement theory: McClelland, 1961) are likely to underlie these

two desired human personality types.

76
Chapter III : A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives

Taking the whole picture together, a circumplex taxonomy could be proposed of eight

consumer motives, i.e. Vitality, Pleasure, Conviviality, Belonging, Security, Control,

Recognition and Power, determined by two basic, bipolar dimensions, which is reflective of

the motivational framework of Callebaut et al. (1999) (see Fig 3.4). All these motivational

states could be considered as stable dispositions that are present within every individual and

become aroused by a particular product environment (e.g. McClelland, 1980). To satisfy the

consumer motives that are manifest in a given product context, people engage in motive-

directed behavior (e.g. Mowen and Minor, 1997).

Figure 3.4 : Callebaut et al.’s (1999) Taxonomy of Consumer Motives

Source: Adapted from J. Callebaut et al., “Motivational marketing research revisited” (Leuven-Apeldoorn:
Garant, 1999)

77
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Conceptually, Callebaut et al.’s (1999) motivational domains appear to overlap considerably

with those reported by previous investigators of motivation. The major distinctions made

between intrapersonal and interpersonal dimensions of motivation are reminiscent of

classifications from a wide variety of sources. For example, Ford and Nichols (1987) broadly

differentiated between within-person goals – consisting of both affective (i.e. Pleasure) and

cognitive (i.e. Control) sub-domains – and social relationship goals, reflecting a need for self-

assertion (i.e. Power) versus affiliation (i.e. Belonging). In their hierarchical human goal

taxonomy, Chulef et al. (2001) also considered intrapersonal (individual) goals to exist as

opposed to interpersonal (social) goals. Moreover, this distinction is also central in the value

literature, especially with regard to the difference between individualistic and collectivistic

values (e.g. Markus and Kitiyama, 1991; Triandis, 1995), and also seems congruent with the

major distinction in personal orientation between agency and communion (Bakan, 1966;

Spence et al., 1979).

Besides the two basic dimensions, also the eight distinct positions in Callebaut et al.’s (1999)

taxonomy appear compatible with prior motivational classifications. E.g., Wicker et al.’s

(1984) Competitive Ambition cluster seems very similar to the Power motive, their

Interpersonal Concern to the Belonging motive, their Intellectual Orientation to the motive of

Recognition and their Economic Status to the Control domain. Also their Exploration-Play

and their Tranquillity Seeking/Security factors might overlap with the motivational domains

respectively called Vitality/Pleasure and Security. All five factors in Youniss et al.’s (1979)

Orientation and Motivation Inventory can be related to the motivational framework of

Callebaut et al. (1999): their Status-seeking factor to the Power motive, their Seeking and

Helping people to the Belonging domain, their Theoretical Interest to the Recognition

position and their factor of New Experience to the Vitality motive. In addition, their Control

78
Chapter III : A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives

factor corresponds closely to Callebaut et al.’s (1999) basic motive of Control. More

recently, also Chulef et al.’s (2001) goal taxonomy reveals some interesting parallels with

some of the motive domains in the taxonomy of Callebaut et al. (1999). For example, their

Excitement and Entertainment goals are similar to the Vitality and Pleasure domains

respectively, their Friendship and Belongingness goals are captured by the domains of

Conviviality and Belonging respectively, their Achievement and Self-determination goals tap

into the Power domain (and perhaps Recognition) and their Stability and Safety goals

resemble Callebaut et al.’s (1999) domain of Security. Finally, some additional conceptual

similarities between the motivational framework of Callebaut et al. (1999) and some of the

oldest, basic motivational taxonomies that have appeared in psychological literature (e.g.

McDougall, 1933; Young, 1936; Murray, 1938; Cattell, 1957; Maslow, 1970) may further

enhance the overall veridicality of the former consumer motive taxonomy.

6. General discussion and conclusions

The present study serves as a preliminary exploration of the relationship between preferred

brand personality and consumer motives. Empirical studies conducted in 11 different

countries and among brands from four different product categories reveal a consistent,

replicable structure that underlies the concept of preferred brand personality. As preferred

brand personality dimensions are prone to channel the behavioral expression of underlying

motives, we reformulated the found structure in terms of Callebaut et al.’s (1999) taxonomy

of eight consumer motives, i.e. Vitality, Pleasure, Conviviality, Belonging, Security, Control,

Recognition and Power, determined by two basic, bipolar dimensions, i.e. an intrapersonal

and interpersonal motive axis. All these motivational states are present within every

individual but, depending on the specific product context, one or more motives may become

79
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

predominant. The manifest motives invoke a desire for certain types of human personalities

to be present, which are expressed by choosing brands with congruent personalities.

When examining the correspondence between the preferred brand personality dimensions

uncovered in this study and the earlier brand personality classification of Aaker (1997), some

interesting conceptual parallels can be discovered. Aaker’s (1997) Excitement dimension

seems to have considerable overlap with the Vitality domain whereas her brand personality

factor of Sophistication shows congruence with the Recognition position. Moreover, the

results of this study could be considered as a refinement of the Aaker (1997) dimensions, as

some of the subcomponents that make up one broad factor in her classification, in the here

found taxonomy, seem to spread out on two or more adjacent positions. For example, brand

personality traits defining Aaker’s (1997) dimension of Sincerity (e.g. family-oriented,

sincere, cheerful, and friendly) seem to appear in the domains of Belonging as well as

Conviviality and Pleasure. In the same way, traits defining Aaker’s (1997) Competence

dimension (e.g. hard working, intelligent, technical, successful, leader, and confident) share a

common domain with the Power position as well as with the Recognition and Control

positions. In contrast to the other dimensions, Aaker’s fifth dimension of Ruggedness seems

to fit less in the taxonomy discovered in this study, whereas in their turn none of the Aaker

(1997) dimensions seem to fully capture the position of Security.

Besides parallels with the work of Aaker (1997), at a more basic level the results of this study

seem compatible with prior classifications of human motives (cf. supra: section 5). However,

Callebaut et al.’s (1999) taxonomy – as derived and validated in the present study – has a

number of advantages over previous attempts at motive taxonomies. First, it is based on a

much broader (multi-country) sample of subjects than previous attempts, which enhances the

80
Chapter III : A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives

external validity and generalizability of the findings. Second, the taxonomy is empirically

generated, rather than being based on theoretical preconceptions of researchers, as were most

previous attempts (see Chulef et al., 2001 for an exception). Hence, the results obtained in

this study may be less biased by the authors’ knowledge of the field as they represent

subjects’ naïve thoughts and perceptions. Third, the use of INDSCAL analysis instead of

more common hierarchical clustering techniques or factor analysis provides us with additional

insights that are lacking in other attempts at organizing the motivational domain. Whereas

most previous attempts are mainly descriptive in nature and organize the field of human

motives into a limited number of discrete categories, the results of this study reveal a

circumplex structure of continuous motive dimensions in which relations of conflict and

compatibility are represented (i.e. Schwartz, 1992: relations of conflict and compatibility

among values). Adjacent positions are postulated to be most compatible (e.g. Pleasure and

Conviviality). Increasing distance around the circular order indicates decreasing

compatibility and greater conflict. Motive dimensions that emerge in opposing directions

from the origin are postulated to be in greatest conflict (e.g. Pleasure and Control). Finally,

Callebaut et al.’s (1999) motive taxonomy as validated in the present study is based on the

valence ratings of specific brand personality traits instead of more general motive units as

were most previous taxonomies. As a result, a bridge is built between products/brands and

consumer motives. Based on the application of Callebaut et al.’s (1999) motivational

framework, one gets insight into the way people’s motive orientations are at work in a

particular brand category context, rather than being provided with a taxonomy of overall

human motives. Moreover, the established relationship between products/brands and

consumer motives provides opportunities in the context of marketing segmentation and

targeting. A heterogeneous consumer audience could be segmented into distinct clusters,

according to which personality traits people are looking for in a particular brand in the light of

81
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

underlying motive satisfaction. The resulting information could be used by brand managers

and communication specialists to develop appropriate advertising campaigns and brand

strategies.

7. Limitations and future research

Besides previous advantages, the most obvious limitation of the present study is that

behavioral measures were not included. Future research should concentrate on the predictive

potential of Callebaut et al.’s (1999) motive taxonomy and preferences should be checked

against consumer’s memory of recent purchase behavior. Further, to assess construct validity,

Callebaut et al.’s (1999) eight consumer motives should be correlated with other related

concepts in literature, e.g. personal values or other relevant individual difference variables

(e.g. consumer innovativeness, need for uniqueness, impulsiveness, need for affiliation, self-

control….) (cf. infra: chapters IV & V). In particular, it would be interesting to explore how

relationships between these individual difference variables and particular dependent variables

could be influenced by manifest consumer motives.

82
Chapter III : A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the anonymous reviewers of the Winter Conference 2005 of the Society for

Consumer Psychology (SCP) (held in St. Pete Beach, Florida) for their helpful comments on

earlier drafts of this paper, as well as the audience who attended the presentation and

delivered some interesting remarks. The authors also wish to thank the members of the

worldwide division of the market research company Synovate/Censydiam for their help with

the extensive data collection in the 11 research countries.

83
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

References

Aaker, J.L. (1997). Dimensions of brand personality. Journal of Marketing Research, 34

(aug), 347- 356.

Aaker, J.L. (1999). The malleable self: the role of self-expression in persuasion. Journal of

Marketing Research, 36 (feb), 45-57.

Aaker, J.L., Benet-Martinez, V., & Garolera, J. (2001). Consumption symbols as carriers of

culture: a study of Japanese and Spanish brand personality constructs. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 81(3), 492-508.

Allport, G.W. (1937). Personality: a psychological interpretation. New York: Holt,

Rinehart, & Winston.

Austin, J.T., & Vancouver, J.B. (1996). Goal constructs in psychology: structure, process

and content. Psychological Bulletin, 120(3), 338-375.

Bagozzi, R.P. (1980). Causal models in marketing. New York: J. Wiley.

Bagozzi, R.P., & Dholakia, U. (1999). Goal setting and goal striving in consumer behavior.

Journal of Marketing, 63, 19-32.

Bagozzi, R.P., & Yi, Y. (1988). On the evaluation of structural equation models. Journal of

Academy of Marketing Science, 16(1), 74-94.

Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence. Chicago: Rand McNally.

Barsalou, L.W. (1985). Ideals, central tendency and frequency of instantiation as

determinants of graded structure in categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology,

11(4), 629-654.

Batra, R., Lehmann, D.R., & Singh, D. (1993). The brand personality component of brand

goodwill: some antecedents and consequences. In D.A. Aaker, & A. Biel (Eds.),

84
Chapter III : A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives

Brand Equity and Advertising (pp. 83-96). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum

Associates.

Belk, R.W. (1988). Possessions and the extended self. Journal of Consumer Research,

2(sept), 139-168.

Bellenger, D.N., Steinberg, E., & Stanton, W.W. (1976). The congruence of store image and

self image. Journal of Retailing, 52(spring), 17-32.

Biel, A. (1993). Converting image into equity. In D.A. Aaker, & A. Biel (Eds.), Brand

Equity and Advertising (pp. 67-82). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Borkenau, P. (1990). Traits as ideal-based and goal-derived social categories. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 381-396.

Callebaut, J., Janssens, M., Op de Beeck, D., Lorré, D., & Hendrickx, H. (1999).

Motivational marketing research revisited. Leuven-Apeldoorn: Garant.

Cantor, N. (1990). From thought to behavior: ‘Having’ and ‘Doing’ in the study of

personality and cognition. American Psychologist, 45, 735-750.

Caprara, G.V., Barbaranelli, C., & Guido, G. (2001). Brand personality: how to make the

metaphor fit? Journal of Economic Psychology, 22, 377- 395.

Carroll, J.D., & Chang, J.J. (1970). Analysis of individual differences in multidimensional

scaling. Psychometrika, 35, 238–319.

Cattell, R.B. (1957). Personality and motivation structure and measurement. Yonkerson-

Hudson: World Book Company.

Chulef, A.S., Read, S.J., & Walsh, D.A. (2001). A hierarchical taxonomy of human goals.

Motivation and Emotion, 25(3), 191-232.

Costa, P.T., & McCrae, R.R. (1992). Revised NEOPI-R and NEO-FFI professional manual.

Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

85
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Dolich, I.R. (1969). Congruence relationship between self-image and product brands.

Journal of Marketing Research, 6, 80-84.

Fleeson, W., Zirkel, S., & Smith, E.E. (1995). Mental representations of trait categories and

their influences on person perception. Social Cognition, 13, 365-397.

Ford, M.E., & Nichols, C.W. (1987). A taxonomy of human goals and some possible

applications. In M.E. Ford, & D.H. Ford (Eds.), Humans as Self-constructing

Systems: Putting the Framework to work. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Fournier, S. (1998). Consumers and their brands: developing relationship theory in consumer

research. Journal of Consumer Research, 24 (march), 343-373.

Freud, S. (1933). New introductory lectures on psycho-analysis. New York: W.W. Norton.

Goldberg, L.R. (1990). An alternative description of personality: the Big Five structure.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1216–1229.

Goldberg, L.R. (1993). The structure of phenotypic personality traits. American

Psychologist, 48, 26-34.

Grubb, E.L., & Stern, B.L. (1971). Self-concept and significant others. Journal of Marketing

Research, 8(aug), 382-385.

Gutman, J. (1982). A means–end chain model based on consumer categorization processes.

Journal of Marketing, 46(Spring), 60–72.

Halliday, J. (1996). Chrysler brings out brand personalities with ’97 ads. Advertising Age,

30 (sept), 3.

Helgeson, J.G., & Supphellen, M. (2004). A conceptual and measurement comparison of

self-congruity and brand personality. International Journal of Market Research,

46(2), 205-233.

Hem, L.E., & Iversen, N.M. (2002). Decomposed similarity measures in brand extensions.

Advances in Consumer Research, 29, 199-206.

86
Chapter III : A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives

Huffman, C., Ratneshwar, S., & Mick, D.G. (2000). Consumer goal structures and goal-

determination processes: an integrative framework. In S. Ratneshwar, D.G. Mick, &

C. Huffman (Eds.). The why of consumption: contemporary perspectives on consumer

motives, goals and desires (pp. 9-35). London/New York: Routledge.

Huizinga, J. (1970). Homo Ludens: a study of the play element in culture. New York:

Harper and Row.

Jöreskog, K., & Sörbom, D. (1993). Lisrel 8: Structural Equation Modeling with the

SIMPLIS command language. Chicago: SSI Scientific Software International, Inc.

Kalar, S. (2003). Brands as complements to the self. Advances in Consumer Research, 31,

269.

Kassarjian, H.H. (1971). Personality and consumer behavior: a review. Journal of Marketing

Research, 8(Nov), 409-418.

Keller, K.L. (1993). Conceptualizing, measuring and managing customer-based brand equity.

Journal of marketing, 57 (jan), 1-22.

Kim, C.K., Han, D., & Park, S. (2001). The effect of brand personality and brand

identification on brand loyalty: applying the theory of social identification. Japanese

Psychological Research, 43(4), 195-206.

Kleine, R.E., Kleine, S.S., & Kernan, J.B. (1993). Mundane consumption and the self: a

social identity perspective. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 2(3), 209-235.

Lennox, R., & Wolfe, R. (1984). Revision of the self-monitoring scale. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 1349-1364.

Linville, P.W., & Carlston, D.E. (1994). Social cognition of the self. In P.G. Devine, D.L.

Hamilton, & T.M. Ostrom (Eds.), Social cognition: its impact on social psychology

(pp. 396-403). New York: Academic Press.

87
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Malhotra, N.K. (1988). Self-concept and product choice: an integrated perspective. Journal

of Economic Psychology, 9, 1-28.

Markus, H.R., & Kitiyama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: implications for cognition,

emotion and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224-253.

Maslow, A.H. (1970). Motivation and personality (2nd ed.). New York: harper and Row.

McAdams, D.P., & Powers, J. (1981). Themes of intimacy in behavior and thought. Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 573-587.

McClelland, D.C. (1961). The achieving society. Princeton, New Jersey: Van Nostrand.

McClelland, D.C. (1980). Motive dispositions: the merits of operant and respondent

measures. In L. Wheeler (Ed.), Review of Personality and Social Psychology (Vol. 1,

p. 10-41). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

McCrae, R.R. (1994). New goals for trait psychology. Psychological Inquiry, 5, 148-153.

McCrae, R.R., & Costa, P.T. (1996). Toward a new generation of personality theories:

theoretical contexts for the five-factor model. In J.S. Wiggins (Ed.), The five-factor

model of personality: theoretical perspectives (p. 51-87). New York: Guilford.

McCrae, R.R., & John, O.J. (1992). An introduction to the Five-Factor Model and its

applications. Journal of Personality, 60, 175-215.

McDougall, W. (1933). The energies of men. New York: Scribner’s.

McGuire, W. (1974). Psychological motives and communication gratification. In J.F.

Blumer & Katz (Eds.). The uses of mass communication: current perspectives on

gratification research (pp.110-121). Beverly Hills: Sage.

Mowen, J.C. & Minor, M. (1997). Consumer behavior. Fifth Edition, Prentice Hall.

Murray, H.A. (1938). Explorations in Personality. New York: Oxford University Press.

88
Chapter III : A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives

Olson, J.C., & Reynolds, T.J. (1983). Understanding consumers’ cognitive structures:

implications for marketing strategy. In L. Percy, & A.G. Woodside (Eds.).

Advertising and consumer psychology (pp. 77-90). Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.

Olver, J.M., & Mooradian, T.A. (2003). Personality traits and personal values: a conceptual

and empirical integration. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 109-125.

Omoto, A.M., & Snyder, M. (1990). Basic research in action: volunteerism and society’s

response to AIDS. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 152-166.

Omoto, A.M., & Snyder, M. (1993). Sustained helping without obligation: Motivation,

longevity of service and attitude change among AIDS volunteers. Unpublished

manuscript, University of Kansas, Lawrence, and University of Minnesota,

Minneapolis.

Pervin, L.A. (1994a). A critical analysis of current trait theory. Psychological Inquiry, 5(2),

103-113.

Pervin, L.A. (1994b). Further reflections on current trait theory. Psychological Inquiry,

5(2), 169-178.

Pieters, R., Baumgartner, H., & Allen, D. (1995). A means-end chain approach to consumer

goal structures. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 12, 227-244.

Read, S.J., & Lalwani, N. (2000). Testing a narrative model of trait inferences: the role of

goals. Unpublished manuscript, University of Southern California.

Read, S.J., Jones, D.K., & Miller, L.C. (1990). Traits as goal based categories, the

importance of goals in the coherence of dispositional categories. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 58(6), 1048-1061.

Roberts, B.W., & Robins, R.W. (2000). Broad dispositions, broad aspirations: the

intersection of personality traits and major life goals. Personality and Social

Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1284-1296.

89
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Roberts, B.W., O’Donnell, M., & Robins, R.W. (2004). Goal and personality trait

development in emerging adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

87(4), 541-550.

Schmidt, L.C., & Frieze, I.M. (1997). A mediational model of power, affiliation and

achievement motives and product involvement. Journal of Business and Psychology,

4, 425-446.

Schwartz, S.H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: theoretical advances

and empirical tests in 20 countries. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 25,

1–65.

Sirgy, M.J. (1982). Self-concept in consumer behavior: a critical review. Journal of

Consumer Research, 9, 287-300.

Snyder, M., & Omoto, A.M. (1992). Who helps and why? The psychology of AIDS

volunteerism. In S. Spacapan, & S. Oskamp (Eds.), Helping and being helped:

naturalistic studies (p. 213-219). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Spence, J.T., Helmreich, R.L., & Holohan, C.K. (1979). Negative and positive components

of psychological masculinity and femininity and their relationship to self-reports of

neurotic and acting out behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37,

1673-1682.

Stagner, R. (1994). Traits and Theoreticians. Psychological Inquiry, 5(2), 166-169.

Steenkamp, J.B., & Van Trijp, H. (1991). The use of Lisrel in validating marketing

constructs. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 8, 283-299.

Triandis, H.C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

Tupes, E.C., & Christal, R.E. (1961). Recurrent personality factors based on trait ratings.

Technical Report, USAF, Lackland Air Force Base, Texas.

90
Chapter III : A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives

Wicker, F.W., Lambert, F.B., Richardson, F.C., & Kahler, J. (1984). Categorical goal

hierarchies and classification of human motives. Journal of Personality, 52(3), 285-

305.

Wiggins, J.S., & Trapnell, P.D. (1997). Personality structure: The return of the Big Five. In

R. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of Personality Psychology (pp.

737-765). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Williams, R.M. (1968). Values. In E. Sills (Ed.). International encyclopedia of the social

sciences (pp. 89-103). New York: Macmillan.

Winter, D.G., John, O.P., Stewart, A.J., Klohnen, E.C., & Duncan, L.E. (1998). Traits and

motives: toward an integration of two traditions in personality research.

Psychological Review, 105(2), 230-250.

Young, P.T. (1936). Motivation and behavior. New York: Wiley.

Youniss, R.P., Lorr, M., & Stefic, E.C. (1979). Assessment of motives and orientations.

Psychological Reports, 45, 555-561.

Zinkhan, G.M., & Hong, J.W. (1991). Self-concept and advertising effectiveness: a

conceptual model of congruency, conspicuousness, and response mode. Advances in

Consumer Research, 18, 348-354.

91
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Appendix A :

• List of scale items

Appendix B :

• Motivational theories

92
Chapter III : A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives

Appendix A: List of scale items

Final set of 34 preferred brand personality items

1. Cheerful
2. Bon-vivant
3. Unrestrained
4. Impulsive
5. Optimistic
6. Great to get on with
7. Approachable
8. Friendly
9. reliable
10. Cooperative
11. Familial
12. Sincere
13. Sympathetic
14. Simple
15. Calm
16. Traditional
17. Careful
18. Introvert
19. Self-disciplined
20. Mature
21. Organized
22. Punctual
23. Intellectual
24. Stylish
25. Sophisticated
26. Special
27. Leader
28. Competitive
29. Powerful
30. Successful
31. Contemporary
32. Innovative
33. Creative
34. Adventurous

93
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Appendix B: Motivational theories

Enjoyment theory (Williams, 1968)


This theory stresses the human need for pleasure or sensuous gratification for oneself.

Categorization theory (McGuire, 1974)


This theory attempts to explain the human need for structure, order, and rational knowledge as well as the human
need for consistency and objectivity.

Self-assertion theory / Achievement theory (McClelland, 1961)


These theories collectively view people as competitive achievers always seeking success and admiration, and
striving to develop their potentials in order to enhance their self-esteem.

Intimacy/Affiliation theory (McAdams & Powers, 1981)


This theory focuses on people being altruistic, cohesive, and seeking acceptance and affection in relationships
with (close) others.

Stimulation theory (McGuire, 1974)


This theory stresses the need in human organisms for stimulation and self-expression through play and creativity.

Regression theory (Freud, 1933)


This theory focuses on the reversion to an earlier or less mature pattern of feeling or behavior. Actually it posits
that sometimes people need to withdraw, to return emotionally to early childhood experiences and worry-free
moments.

94
A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives : Construct Validity

CHAPTER IV

RECONSIDERING THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN CONSUMER

MOTIVES AND PERSONAL VALUES

This chapter has been presented at the Winter Conference 2006 of the Society for Consumer

Psychology (SCP) (held in Miami Florida)

95
Chapter IV

Reconsidering the Relationship between Consumer Motives

and Personal Values

1. Abstract

The present study re-examines the relationship between consumer motives and personal

values, based on a new method of motive assessment and a non-student subject sample.

Suggested is that motive-value correlations may become more substantial by increasing

method congruence between implicit motive assessments and direct value measures and by

allowing more middle-aged subjects to participate in the study instead of young adults.

Specific hypotheses are developed regarding the proposed relationships between consumer

motives and values. Results indicate that – although rather moderate in magnitude – several

significant correlations exist between measures of motives and values. In general, the

findings are consistent with the hypothesis that people lean toward those values that play to

their inherent motive dispositions, with the latter seeming to color the influence of

environmental factors in predictable ways.

97
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

2. Introduction

Consumer motives and personal values have been extensively researched across a wide range

of disciplines. Although conceptually related, empirical findings suggest that few significant

correlations exist between TAT-type assessments of motives and direct self-report measures

of values. The aim of the present study is to reconsider the empirical relationship between

consumer motives and personal values, based on both an alternative implicit method of

motive assessment and a non-student subject sample.

In what follows we first discuss some conceptual background regarding consumer motives

and values and shed some light on previous research about the relationship between the two

constructs. We then suggest an alternative method to measure people’s underlying motives

and develop testable hypotheses regarding motive-value relations. We present an empirical

test of these hypotheses, followed by a general discussion of the major findings. Finally, an

explanation is provided of the limitations of the study and future research opportunities are

discussed.

2.1. Motive Dispositions

Spearheading the research on human motives has been McClelland (1985), who suggested

that the end states people seek to attain reflect a limited number of motives. Motives have

been defined as relatively enduring preferences for the attainment of certain classes of desired

goal states (e.g. McClelland, 1987). Sometimes, they also are referred to as something

created by needs (e.g. Brehm and Self, 1989). As motives grow out of universal natural

incentives and early childhood experiences, they are considered to be stable dispositions that

become aroused by environmental cues (e.g. McAdams, 1988; McClelland, 1992). Motives

98
Chapter IV : Consumer Motives and Personal Values

are supposed to operate on a person’s behavior usually outside of his/her conscious awareness

(e.g. McClelland, 1980) and therefore are assessed through indirect projective techniques,

akin to Murray’s (1943) Thematic Apperception Test (TAT). Regarding behavioral

influences, motives are assumed to predict operant behaviors, i.e. qualitative responses which

are not tied to or caused by detectable external stimuli, but, in contrast, are seemingly

spontaneous in nature (e.g. deCharms et al., 1955; Skinner, 1938).

9
With respect to the classification of human motives, Geeroms et al. (2005) validated the

circumplex consumer motive taxonomy of Callebaut et al. (1999), based on respondents’

valence ratings of 34 preferred brand personality traits (cf. infra: section 3.1.). The results of

an INDSCAL analysis, conducted across 11 different countries and four different product

categories, revealed a consistent and replicable structure underlying the concept of preferred

brand personality. As preferred brand personality dimensions are prone to channel the

behavioral expression of underlying motive orientations, the found preferred brand

personality structure was reformulated in terms of Callebaut et al.’s (1999) framework of

eight consumer motives, i.e. Vitality, Pleasure, Conviviality, Belonging, Security, Control,

Recognition and Power (described in Table 4.1).

9
This paragraph comprises a brief recapitulation of the major findings of chapter III of this dissertation, in order to refresh the reader’s mind
regarding these issues. For a full explanation of the (cross-cultural) validation of Callebaut et al.’s (1999) consumer motive taxonomy we
thus refer to this corresponding chapter.

99
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Table 4.1 : Motive Definitions and Scale Items

Vitality : desire to experience adventure, test one’s boundaries and discover new things.
Items: Adventurous, Creative, Innovative, Contemporary

Pleasure : desire to follow impulses and emotions, to maximise pleasure without inhibition
or self-control.
Items: Cheerful, Bon-vivant, Unrestrained, Impulsive

Conviviality : desire to be with people, to open up socially, to share experiences and


emotionally connect with others.
Items: Optimistic, Great to get on with, Approachable, Friendly

Belonging : desire to merge into the group and to be accepted and supported by loved
ones.
Items: reliable, Cooperative, Familial, Sincere, Sympathetic

Security : desire to hide away, to feel safe, relaxed, calm and be protected together with
an obedience to norms and regulations of the group.
Items: Simple, Calm, Traditional, Careful, Introvert

Control : desire to suppress inner feelings and emotions to behave in a way that is
morally acceptable.
Items: Self-disciplined, Mature, Organized, Punctual

Recognition : desire to be recognized by others for one’s skills, knowledge and


appearance, to be respected by others.
Items: Intellectual, Stylish, Sophisticated, Special

Power : desire to be better than the rest, to feel superior, to be a leader. Higher priority is
given to individualistic goals and standards as opposed to group norms.
Items: Successful, Leader, Competitive, Powerful

Source : Geeroms et al. (2005)

The relationships among these eight motives can be summarized in terms of a circumplex

structure, determined by two basic, bipolar dimensions, respectively representing an

intrapersonal and interpersonal motive axis (see Fig. 4.1). All these motivational states are

present within every individual, with one or more of them becoming predominant in a given

product context. To satisfy the predominant motives, people engage in motive-directed

behavior (Mowen and Minor, 1997). As Callebaut et al.’s (1999) motive taxonomy has been

100
Chapter IV : Consumer Motives and Personal Values

proven to be valid cross-culturally, seems compatible with prior classifications of human

motives (e.g. Ford and Nichols, 1987) and, in addition, has a number of advantages over these

previous motive taxonomies, it will be focused upon as a basic theoretical framework in the

present study.

Figure 4.1 : Callebaut et al.’s (1999) Taxonomy of Consumer Motives

Source: Adapted from J. Callebaut et al., “Motivational marketing research revisited” (Leuven-Apeldoorn:
Garant, 1999)

2.2. Personal Values

In contrast to rather innate human motives, personal values are learned beliefs about preferred

ways of acting or being, which serve as guiding principles in the life of a person or other

social entity (e.g. Costa and McCrae, 2001). More specifically, values (1) are concepts or

101
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

beliefs, (2) pertain to desirable end states or behaviors, (3) transcend specific situations, (4)

guide selection or evaluation of behavior and events, and (5) are ordered by relative

importance (Schwartz and Bilsky, 1987). As values are considered as conscious, cognitive

and evaluative entities, they can be assessed through direct self-report questionnaires (e.g.

Rokeach, 1973). Whereas human motives are likely to predict operant behaviors, personal

values are assumed to exert their influence on respondent behaviors, i.e. behaviors that are

related to specific eliciting stimuli (e.g. deCharms et al., 1955; Skinner, 1938)

Based on a theoretical analysis of the universal requirements of the human condition,

Schwartz (1992) generated a comprehensive typology of the different types of value content.

Empirical studies in over 60 countries (e.g. Schwartz and Boehnke, 2004) supported the

existence of ten distinct types of values, i.e. Power, Achievement, Hedonism, Stimulation,

Self-direction, Universalism, Benevolence, Tradition, Conformity and Security (described in

Table 4.2).

102
Chapter IV : Consumer Motives and Personal Values

Table 4.2 : Value Definitions and Scale Items

Achievement : personal success through demonstrating competence according to social


standards
Items: Ambitious, Capable, Influential, Intelligent, Self-respect, Successful

Benevolence : preservation and enhancement of the welfare of people with whom one is
in frequent personal contact.
Items: A spiritual life, Forgiving, Helpful, Honest, Loyal, Mature love, Meaning in life,
Responsible, True friendship

Conformity : restraint of actions, inclinations, and impulses likely to upset or harm others
and violate social expectations or norms
Items: Honoring parents and elders, Obedient, Politeness, Self-discipline

Hedonism: pleasure and sensuous gratification for oneself


Items: Enjoying life, pleasure

Power : social status and prestige, control or dominance over people and resources
Items: Authority, Preserving my public image, Social power, Social recognition, Wealth

Security: safety, harmony, and stability of society, of relationships, and of self.


Items: Clean, Family security, Healthy, National security, Reciprocation of favors, Sense
of belonging, Social order.

Self-direction: independent thought and action-choosing, creating, exploring


Items: Choosing own goals, Creativity, Curious, Freedom, Independent

Stimulation: excitement, novelty, and challenge in life.


Items: An exciting life, A varied life, Daring

Tradition : respect, commitment, and acceptance of the customs and ideas that traditional
culture or religion provide
Items: Accepting my portion in life, Detachment, Devout, Humble, Moderate, Respect for
tradition

Universalism: understanding, appreciation, tolerance, and protection of the welfare of all


people and for nature.
Items: A world of beauty, Broad-minded, Protecting the environment, Social justice, Unity
with nature, Wisdom

Source : Adapted from S.H. Schwartz, ‘ Universals in the Content and Structure of Values,’ in M.P. Zanna,
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Orlando : Academic Press, 1992), 1-65

103
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

These values are presumed to encompass the range of motivationally distinct values

recognized across cultures (e.g. Schwartz and Bardi, 2001). The total value structure can be

viewed as composed of four higher-order value types that form two conceptual dimensions,

i.e. Self-enhancement versus Self-transcendence and Openness to change versus Conservation

(e.g. Schwartz, 1992, 1994). The Self-enhancement versus Self-transcendence dimension

arrays values in terms of the extent to which they motivate people to enhance their own

personal interests versus to transcend selfish concerns and promote the welfare of others,

close and distant, and of nature. The second bipolar dimension, Openness to change versus

Conservation, arrays values in terms of the extent to which they motivate people to follow

their own emotional and intellectual interests in unpredictable and uncertain directions versus

to preserve the status quo and the certainty it provides in relationship with close others,

institutions and traditions. Similar to Callebaut et al.’s (1999) motive taxonomy, the pattern

of value relations among these two dimensions can be organized into a circumplex structure,

with conflicting values appearing in opposing directions from the center and congruent values

as adjacent to one another in the circle (see Fig. 4.2). The relative importance attributed to

each of the ten value types constitutes the individual’s system of value priorities.

104
Chapter IV : Consumer Motives and Personal Values

Figure 4.2 : Schwartz’ (1992) Value Structure

Source : Adapted from S.H. Schwartz, ‘ Universals in the Content and Structure of Values,’ in M.P. Zanna,
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Orlando : Academic Press, 1992), 1-65

2.3. Relationship between Motives and Values

On a theoretical basis, motives and values have frequently been considered as representing

two interconnected constructs. For instance, Bilsky and Schwartz (1994) assumed that the

primary content aspect of a value is the type of motivational concern that it expresses. Values

are considered as cognitive representations of important human motives about which people

must communicate in order to coordinate their behavior.

105
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

However, despite this conceptual relatedness, empirical findings obtained from several studies

indicate that among college students, TAT-type assessments of motives show only few or no

significant correlations with self-report measures of values (e.g. Veroff and Smith, 1985;

Biernat, 1989). These results are in accordance with McClelland et al.’s (1989)

conceptualization of two independent motivational systems (cf. supra: Chapter II), whereas

they lend less support to the notion that values represent instantiations of an individual’s

implicit motives (e.g. Schwartz and Bilsky, 1987).

One possible cause for the lack of correlation between motives and values is indicated by

Schultheiss and Brunstein (1999). They posit that – as society and culture are powerful

determinants of the values individuals pursue throughout the life circle – value priorities may

be predisposed to reflect the demands of a person’s social environment at least as much as

they reflect his or her own motives. Hence, people may pursue values that do not fit their

motives with the same vigor as they would pursue motive-congruent values

Second, from a psychometrical perspective, the low convergence could also be due to a

reliability problem inherent in the TAT when one considers the low internal consistency

among stories from which a person’s motive scores are obtained (e.g. Tuerlinckx et al., 2002).

Moreover, self-report tests and the TAT involve different stimuli, i.e. structured questions

versus ambiguous pictures and use different response formats, i.e. selected versus constructed

responses to assess the strength of respectively values and motives. Thus, method variance

might be another, more obvious reason why the two measures fail to agree (e.g. Brunstein and

Schmitt, 2004).

106
Chapter IV : Consumer Motives and Personal Values

Finally, Schultheiss and Brunstein (2001) argue that low motive-value correlations also might

be due to the predominant use of student samples by researchers. They suggest that

correlations may become more substantial when using middle-aged subjects instead of young

adults, as people’s motive/value structures are supposed to become more integrated in midlife

(e.g. Maehr and Kleiber, 1981).

3. Present Study

As methodological problems, i.e. method variance and the predominant use of student

samples are posited as one of the most obvious reasons for the lack of correlation between

measures of motives and values (e.g. Brunstein and Schmitt, 2004; Schultheiss and Brunstein,

2001), the present study aims to reconsider the relationship between consumer motives and

personal values, based on both an alternative method of motive assessment and a non-student

subject sample. In particular, we suggest that motive-value correlations may become more

substantial by increasing method congruence between implicit motive assessments and direct

value measures and by allowing more middle-aged subjects to participate in the study instead

of young adults. In the sections below, an explanation is provided of the proposed method of

motive assessment and specific hypotheses are formulated regarding motive-value relations,

followed by a brief discussion of the research method and the results.

3.1. Motive Assessment10

Consumer motives were assessed by the 34-item Consumer Motive Scale developed by

Geeroms et al. (2005). Based on this scale, all eight positions in the overall motive taxonomy

10
This section comprises a brief recapitulation of the motive assessment procedure described by Geeroms et al. in their 2005 paper. For a
more profound explanation of the rationale behind this particular method of measuring consumer motives we refer to chapter III of this
dissertation.

107
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

of Callebaut et al. (1999) (i.e. Fig. 4.1) are represented by a limited number of motive-

expressive personality traits (see Table 4.1). Subjects were asked to select their preferred

brand out of a set of pictures of several brands in a given product category and rate the extent

to which each of the 34 traits describes the personality of the chosen brand on a five-point

Likert scale ranging from 1= not at all descriptive to 5= very descriptive. The rationale for

considering personality traits associated with a preferred brand as reflective of underlying

consumer motives is based on the conceptual integration of both the previously established

relationships in literature between brand personality and human personality and human

personality and underlying motives (cf. supra: chapter III) As preferred brand personality

traits can be considered as expressions of desired human personality dimensions (e.g. Keller,

1993) and, in addition, desired human personality traits are likely to be function of underlying

motives (Winter et al., 1998), preferred brand personality traits are prone to channel the

behavioral expression of these underlying motives (e.g. Geeroms et al., 2005). Hence, based

on this self-expressive function of a preferred brand personality, the latter concept can be

considered as an appropriate measure to capture consumer motives in the context of the

present study.

As no direct measures were used of human characteristics as expressions of underlying

motives, but rather, people were referring to the personality of a preferred brand as a third-

person, the above technique of assessing consumer motives is mainly implicit in nature.

However, the proposed method is fairly more structured than most traditional projective

techniques as subjects were not supposed to write TAT-type stories about the brand but were

indicated to rate the extent to which the 34 personality items describe their preferred brand on

a Likert-type response scale. By using the same structured response formats (i.e. Likert scales)

in both cases, more method congruence occurred between implicit motive assessments and

108
Chapter IV : Consumer Motives and Personal Values

direct value measures, hereby resolving some of the psychometric problems (i.e. reliability,

validity) inherent in more traditional TAT-type measures.

3.2. Hypotheses

Conceptually – as values are considered as cognitive instantiations of underlying motives (e.g.

Schwartz and Bilsky, 1987) – meaningful relationships could be expected between Callebaut

et al.’s (1999) motive taxonomy (Fig. 4.1) and Schwartz’ (1992) value structure (Fig. 4.2).

Specific hypotheses regarding these relationships are developed below.

The Pleasure motive is characterized by a desire to follow impulses and emotions, to

maximise pleasure without inhibition or self-control. Vitality represents a desire to

experience adventure, test one’s boundaries and discover new things. Accordingly, these

motive dimensions should influence personal value priorities related to Openness to Change,

i.e. Self-direction, Stimulation and Hedonism11 versus Conservation, i.e. Security, Tradition

and Conformity.

H1a: Motives of Pleasure and Vitality will be positively related to Self-direction,

Stimulation, and Hedonism value priorities.

H1b: Motives of Pleasure and Vitality will be negatively related to Security, Tradition, and

Conformity value priorities.

11
The position of Hedonism in Schwartz’ (1992) value structure is not clear. Schwartz (1992) argues that it is related to both Openness to
change and Self-enhancement. Past research (e.g. Feather, 1995; Steenkamp et al., 1999) included Hedonism within the Self-enhancement
domain. However, recently Schwartz and Boehnke (2004) concluded that, although the Hedonism values are correlated significantly with
both Openness to change and Self-enhancement, it is clearly closer to Openness to change. Therefore, we considered Hedonism as belonging
to the Openness to change value domain.

109
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

The Conviviality motive involves a desire to be with people, to open up socially, to share

experiences and emotionally connect with others. Belonging is characterized by the desire to

merge into the group and to be accepted and supported by loved ones. Thus, these motives

will operate on value priorities related to Self-Transcendence, i.e. Universalism and

Benevolence versus Self-Enhancement, i.e. Achievement and Power.

H2a: Motives of Conviviality and Belonging will be positively related to Universalism and

Benevolence value priorities.

H2b: Motives of Conviviality and Belonging will be negatively related to Achievement and

Power value priorities.

The motive of Security includes a desire to hide away, to feel safe, relaxed, calm and be

protected together with an obedience to norms and regulations of the group. Control

represents a desire to suppress inner feelings and emotions to behave in a way that is morally

acceptable. Hence, these motives dimensions should support Conservation, i.e. Security,

Tradition and Conformity versus Openness to change value priorities, i.e. Self-direction,

Stimulation and Hedonism.

H3a: Motives of Security and Control will be positively related to Conformity, Tradition,

and Security value priorities.

H3b: Motives of Security and Control will be negatively related to Self-direction,

Stimulation, and Hedonism value priorities.

Finally, Recognition is characterized by a desire to be recognized by others for one’s skills,

knowledge and appearance, to be respected by others. Also Power includes a need to be

better than the rest, to feel superior, to be a leader. In this case, higher priority is given to

110
Chapter IV : Consumer Motives and Personal Values

individualistic goals and standards as opposed to group norms. Accordingly, these motive

dimensions should influence personal value priorities related to Self-Enhancement, i.e.

Achievement and Power versus Self-Transcendence, i.e. Universalism and Benevolence.

H4a: Motives of Recognition and Power will be positively related to Achievement and

Power value priorities.

H4b: Motives of Recognition and Power will be negatively related to Universalism and

Benevolence value priorities.

3.3. Method

Subjects’ underlying motives were assessed by means of the 34-item Consumer Motive Scale

of Geeroms et al. (2005) and the implicit procedure described earlier (cf. supra: section 3.1).

As personal values and their motivational counterparts guide, control and form the foundation

of especially social behaviors such as political ideology (e.g. Burgess, 1992; Rokeach, 1973),

political preference was chosen as the particular context to be investigated, instead of a

specific product category. The seven major political parties in Belgium were chosen as

‘brands’. A color picture was provided to the subjects of each party’s emblem. Values were

assessed using the 56-item Schwartz Value Survey (Schwartz, 1992), which has been

extensively validated cross-culturally. Questionnaires were administered via a random walk

method to a sample of 250 subjects. Subjects were rewarded for their participation with a

writing pen. In total, 178 subjects provided valid responses on all measures of interest. The

mean age of the respondents was 38.6 years with a standard deviation of 15.3. In comparison

to the Belgian population statistics (NIS, 2005), our sample was slightly biased toward higher

education. Table 4.3 provides an overview of the sample characteristics.

111
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Table 4.3 : Sample Characteristics (% of respondents, N = 178)

Sample Population*
Gender
Male 46.1 48.9
Female 53.9 51.1
Age
<25 22.2 18.4
25-40 33.5 32.8
40-50 19.9 22.6
>50 24.4 26.2

Education
Lower education (age <=18) 39.1 67.4
Higher education (age >18) 60.9 32.6

* Source: NIS (2005). Private Households Statistics. Brussels: NIS, National


Institute for Statistics

3.4. Results

Three motive items – ‘impulsive’, ‘punctual’, and ‘special’ – were dropped because of poor

inter-item correlations with other items in the motive dimensions they were expected to mark

(Pleasure, Control, and Recognition respectively). The remaining motive items were used to

create average sum scores for each motive type. Average sum scores for the Schwartz value

types were computed by using all 56 value items.

Reliability estimates are presented in Table 4.4. Though rather moderate 12 , reliability

indicators for both the eight motive dimensions and the Schwartz value types are generally

acceptable (Cronbach’s α of .60 – .79).

12
The moderate Cronbach’s α measures are not surprising as these indicators describe a continuum within a two-dimensional circumplex
rather than discrete motive or value categories (e.g. Schwartz, 1992).

112
Chapter IV : Consumer Motives and Personal Values

Table 4.4 : Reliability Estimates

Measure No. Of items Cronbach’s α


Callebaut et al.’s Motive Dimensions
Vitality 4 .71
Pleasure 3 .70
Conviviality 4 .64
Belonging 5 .76
Security 5 .66
Control 3 .62
Recognition 3 .77
Power 4 .79

Schwartz’ Value Types


Achievement 4 .76
Benevolence 9 .72
Conformity 4 .73
Hedonism 2 .77
Power 6 .78
Security 7 .74
Self-direction 6 .65
Stimulation 3 .79
Tradition 6 .60
Universalism 9 .74

Table 4.5 gives an overview of the item correlations. Several modest (‫׀‬r‫ ׀‬of .15 – .38) –

though significant – correlations can be detected between the motive measures and the

Schwartz value measures.

113
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Table 4.5 : Item Correlations

VI PL CO BE SE CTRL RE PO
Achievement .13 .10 .02 -.08 -.13 .11 .18* .38**
Benevolence -.01 .11 .18* .17* .12 .02 -.15* -.15*
Conformity -.08 -.03 .05 .06 .18* .19* .11 .10
Hedonism .17* .19** .03 -.10 -.15* -.15* .03 .13
Power -.03 .04 -.18* -.15* -.06 .11 .15* .27**
Security -.17* -.02 .02 .04 .23** .20** .09 .12
Self-direction .16* .15* .12 .06 -.01 -.12 .08 .14
Stimulation .23** .23** .05 -.08 -.15* -.05 .02 .13
Tradition -.06 -.06 -.09 -.01 .19* .24** -.01 -.08
Universalism -.02 .13 .19* .15* .09 -.09 -.16* -.17*

Vitality (VI) 1.0


Pleasure (PL) .26** 1.0
Conviviality (CO) .30** .47** 1.0
Belonging (BE) -.07 .11 .40** 1.0
Security (SE) -.39** -.09 .12 .35** 1.0
Control (CTRL) .10 -.07 .11 .17* .04 1.0
Recognition (RE) .33** .14 -.43** .10 .13 .20** 1.0
Power (PO) .38** .14 -.13 -.06 -.23** .20** .32** 1.0
* p<.05
**p<.01

Results of a multiple regression analysis with consumer motives as independent variables and

Schwartz value types as dependent variables are presented in Table 4.613. As can be seen,

consumer motives account for a significant proportion of unique variance in value priorities in

all equations (R² ranging between .09 and .16).

13
As some of the motive dimensions show significant intercorrelations (see Table 4.5), multi-collinearity tests were performed for each
regression equation. Based on these tests, no big problems could be detected.

114
Chapter IV : Consumer Motives and Personal Values

Table 4.6 : Regression Results

Standardized Beta Coefficients

VI PL CO BE SE CTRL RE PO R²

Achievement .00 .07 .01 -.09 -.02 .10 .18* .31** .16**
Benevolence .07 .03 .21* .17* .09 .08 -.16* -.16* .10*
Conformity -.06 -.01 .08 -.02 .23* .09 .06 .07 .11*
Hedonism .19* .22* .01 -.12 -.18* -.19* -.09 .06 .10*
Power -.08 .11 -.20* -.18* .06 .00 .20* .32** .11*
Security -.10 .02 .04 -.01 .23* .17* -.06 .10 .10*
Self-direction .25* .19* .12 -.05 -.04 -.05 -.15 .14 .10*
Stimulation .18* .19* -.02 -.05 -.10 .01 -.09 .10 .09*
Tradition -.00 -.06 -.06 -.03 .21* .17* .08 .05 .09*
Universalism -.05 .07 .25* .16* .00 .05 -.21* -.28** .12*
VI= Vitality; PL= Pleasure; CO= Conviviality; BE= Belonging; SE= Security; CTRL= Control; RE=
Recognition; PO= Power.
* p<.05
** p<.01

Based on the beta coefficients, H1a is fully supported, whereas H1b receives no support.

Both Pleasure and Vitality motives are positively related to value priorities of Hedonism,

Self-direction, and Stimulation (p<.05), whereas the negative relationship between these two

motive dimensions and respectively Conformity, Security, and Tradition values is not

significant. However, item correlations in Table 4.5 do suggest a significant negative

relationship between Vitality and Schwartz’ value type of Security, hereby providing some

mixed support of H1b.

H2a is fully supported, whereas H2b only partially. The standardized coefficients for both

Conviviality and Belonging are positive and significant for the values of Benevolence and

Universalism (p<.05). Both Conviviality and Belonging have a significant negative

115
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

relationship to the Power value type (p<.05), but not to Achievement. Correlation results in

Table 4.5 are congruent with these findings of the regression analysis.

H3a receives full support for the motive of Security, but only mixed support for the Control

motive. H3b is partially supported for both. Security coefficients are positively related to

Conformity, Security, and Tradition values (p<.05), whereas the motive of Control shows no

significant positive relationship to Conformity. Both Security and Control motives have the

expected negative relationship to Hedonism values (p<.05), but not to Self-direction and

Stimulation values. However, based on the item correlations in Table 4.5, H3a is fully

supported for both motives and regarding H3b, the motive of Security shows a significant

negative relationship to the value type of Stimulation in addition to its negative relationship to

Hedonism (p<.05).

Finally, both H4a and H4b are fully supported. Motives of Recognition and Power are

positively related to Achievement and Power value priorities (p<.01), and negatively related

to Benevolence and Universalism (p<.05).

4. Discussion and conclusions

In this study, we bring together two streams of research: consumer motives, as explicated in

the motive taxonomy of Callebaut et al. (1999), and personal values, particularly as developed

through the pioneering research of Schwartz and colleagues (e.g. Schwartz, 1992, 1994;

Schwartz and Bardi, 2001; Schwartz and Boehnke, 2004). Although personal values

theoretically could be considered as cognitive instantiations of underlying motives (e.g.

Schwartz and Bilsky, 1987; Bilsky and Schwartz, 1994), empirical findings suggest that few

116
Chapter IV : Consumer Motives and Personal Values

significant correlations exist between implicit measures of motives and more direct self-report

measures of values (e.g. Biernat, 1989; Veroff and Smith, 1985). As methodological

problems are likely to contribute to the low convergence between the two constructs (e.g.

Brunstein and Schmitt, 2004; Schultheiss and Brunstein, 2001), the aim of the present

research was to reconsider the relationship between consumer motives and values, by making

use of both an alternative third-person technique to assess people’s motives and a non-student

subject sample. We proposed that motive-value correlations might become more substantial

by increasing method congruence between implicit motive assessments and direct value

measures, i.e. using the same structured response formats (Likert scales) in both cases and by

allowing more middle-aged subjects to participate in the study instead of young adults, i.e.

conducting a random walk method as a means of data collection.

Specific hypotheses were formulated regarding the proposed relationships between Callebaut

et al.’s (1999) motive taxonomy and Schwartz’ (1992) value structure. Results indicated that

– although rather moderate in magnitude – several significant correlations exist between

motive measures and Schwartz’ value measures. In the present study, 38% of the motive-

value correlations appeared to be significant at the alpha .05 level of probability.

In general, the findings are consistent with the hypothesis that people lean toward those

values that play to their inherent motive dispositions. Specifically, motives of Pleasure and

Vitality contribute to the priority that individuals place on pro-Openness to Change values;

Conviviality and Belonging contribute to pro-Self-Transcendence values; motives of Security

and Control support pro-Conservation value priorities; and Recognition and Power motives

contribute to pro-Self-Enhancement values versus Self-Transcendence. Thus, while

environmental factors might have a strong formative effect on an individual’s value priorities

117
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

(e.g. Schultheiss and Brunstein, 1999), the results of this study clearly demonstrate the

influence of internal psychological constructs such as one’s underlying motive dispositions

too. However, given the moderate strength of the found relationships between motives and

values, we may not overestimate the degree of interconnectedness between these two

constructs and must agree that to some extent, McClelland et al.’s (1989) conceptualization of

motives and values as somewhat different dimensions of the personality structure remains

stable in the context of the present study.

Summarizing, the present research significantly contributes to the study of consumer

psychology at least in two ways. First, empirically, the results of this study may indicate that

methodological issues play a role in the previously detected lack of correlation between

measures of motives and values in literature (e.g. Biernat, 1989). However, caution is

warranted when drawing conclusions about the impact of choice of measurement method and

sample on the strength of motive-value relationships as no direct comparisons were assessed

between both student and non-student samples and TAT-type measures of motives and

Geeroms et al.’s (2005) motive assessment procedure (cf. infra: section 5). Moreover, the

issue of method influence should be nuanced in the context of the present research as TAT-

type measures are prone to deal with overall human motives that are stable across situations,

whereas an application of Geeroms et al.’s (2005) motive assessment procedure provides

insight into how consumer motives are at work in a particular brand category context. Hence,

these two distinct methods of motive assessment can not be considered as measuring exactly

the same.

Second, on a theoretical level, as consumer motives and values can not be considered as

completely independent constructs, the results of this study provide new insights into the way

118
Chapter IV : Consumer Motives and Personal Values

internal psychological aspects, i.e. one’s underlying motive dispositions influence people’s

value priorities, in addition to already existing insights about the influence of external factors

on personal values such as culture and social environment (e.g. Schwartz and Sagiv, 1995;

Blackwell et al., 2001; Knafo and Schwartz, 2001; Rohan and Zanna, 1996; Schoenpflug,

2001).

5. Limitations and future research

An intractable limitation of this research is the measurement issue. As noted earlier,

consumer motives could be considered as endogenous dispositions which cannot be directly

observed but must be inferred indirectly, usually as by means of TAT-type measures (e.g.

Smith, 1992). However, in the present study, structured response formats (i.e. Likert scales)

were used to measure the strength of consumer motives, as part of our implicit method of

motive assessment As a result, there is necessarily common method variance in our measures

of motives and values. To account for this problem to some extent, some unrelated filler tasks

were included in the questionnaire that had to be completed between our independent and

dependent measures respectively.

A second limitation is related to the application of brand personality in a political context.

Whereas measures of brand personality have been extensively validated in the context of

product and service brands, retailers (e.g. Aaker, 1997), as well as not-for profit organizations

(e.g. Venable et al., 2005), they have not been yet in the context of political parties. The fact

that some preferred brand personality traits did not perform well in capturing their underlying

motive dimensions and had to be dropped in the present study may be reflective of this

119
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

problem. Hence, future research attempts are encouraged to further validate the use of the

concept of brand personality in the context of political marketing.

Third, some remarks have to be made with regard to the results of the multiple regression

analysis. In general, the findings of this analysis provided support for the majority of our

propositions. However, quite a few predictions were not supported by the data. Whereas the

signs of the beta coefficients truly went in the hypothesized directions, some of the proposed

relationships between motives and values appeared to be non-significant. To some extent,

multi-collinearity between the motivational predictor variables may be proposed as a possible

contributor to this problem. As already mentioned, multi-collinearity tests performed for each

regression equation did not indicate major difficulties. However, some of the motive

dimensions did show significant intercorrelations (‫׀‬r‫ ׀‬ranging between .17 and .47), which

may result in misleading P values in some of the cases.

In addition, some critics may argue that our attempt to trace the effect of underlying motives

on people’s value priorities is plagued by low predictive power (R² ≤ .16). However, as is the

case for previous investigations of individual value differences (e.g. Schiffmann et al., 2003;

Allen and Ng, 1999, 2003), the primary contribution of the present study is to show a

particular pattern of relationships, more so than any absolute strength based on the predictive

power.

To conclude, some avenues for further research may be identified. As internal (e.g.. motive

dispositions) and external factors (e.g. culture, social environment) have mainly been used

independently as predictors of individual differences in value priorities, future research

models may be developed that incorporate both internal and external variables. In particular,

120
Chapter IV : Consumer Motives and Personal Values

it would be interesting to explore how relationships between motives and values could be

moderated by environmental factors (e.g. culture).

However, the most challenging future research opportunity implies a direct comparison of

both a student vs. non-student sample and TAT-type measures of motives vs. Geeroms et al.’s

(2005) motive assessment procedure, for instance, by applying a 2 x 2 between subject design.

Only by performing such kind of analysis reliable conclusions can be drawn regarding the

impact of choice of measurement method and sample on motive-value correlations and

individual effects could be assessed of each of these two variables of interest.

121
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the anonymous reviewers of the Winter Conference 2006 of the Society for

Consumer Psychology (SCP) (held in Miami, Florida) for their helpful comments on earlier

drafts of this paper, as well as the audience who attended the presentation and delivered some

interesting remarks. The authors also wish to thank Véronique Cheyns for collecting the data

for this study.

122
Chapter IV : Consumer Motives and Personal Values

References

Aaker, J.L. (1997). Dimensions of brand personality. Journal of Marketing Research, 34

(aug), 347- 356.

Allen, N.W., & Ng, S.H. (1999). The direct and indirect influences of human values on

product ownership. Journal of Economic Psychology, 20(1), 5-39.

Allen, N.W., & Ng, S.H. (2003). Human Values, Utilitarian Benefits and Identification: the

case of meat. European Journal of Social Psychology, 33(1), 37-56.

Biernat, M. (1989). Motives and values to achieve: different constructs with different effects.

Journal of Personality, 57(1), 69-95.

Bilsky, W., & Schwartz, S.H. (1994). Values and personality. European Journal of

Personality, 8, 163-181.

Blackwell, R.D., Miniard, P.W., & Engel, J.F. (2001). Consumer Behavior (9th edition). Fort

Worth: Harcourt College Publishers.

Brehm, J.W., & Self, E.A. (1989). The intensity of motivations. Annual Review of

Psychology, 40, 109-132.

Brunstein, J.C., & Schmitt, C.H. (2004). Assessing individual differences in achievement

motivation with the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Research in Personality, 38,

536-555.

Burgess, S.M. (1992). Personal values and consumer research: an historical perspective.

Research in Marketing, 11, 35-79.

Callebaut, J., Janssens, M., Op de Beeck, D., Lorré, D., & Hendrickx, H. (1999).

Motivational marketing research revisited. Leuven-Apeldoorn: Garant.

123
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Costa, P.T., & McCrae, R.R. (2001). A theoretical context for adult temperament. In T.D.

Wachs, & G.A. Kohnstamm (Eds.), Temperament in context (p. 1-21). Mahwah, NJ:

Lawrence Erlbaum.

deCharms, R., Morrison, H.W., Reitman, W.R., & McClelland, D.C. (1955). Behavioral

correlates of directly and indirectly measured achievement motivation. In D.C.

McClelland (Ed.), Studies in motivation (p. 414-423). New York: Appleton-Century-

Crofts

Feather, N.T. (1995). Values, valences and choice: the influence of values on the perceived

attractiveness and choice of alternatives. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 68(6), 1135-1151.

Ford, M.E., & Nichols, C.W. (1987). A taxonomy of human goals and some possible

applications. In M.E. Ford, & D.H. Ford (Eds.), Humans as Self-constructing

Systems: Putting the Framework to work. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Geeroms, N., Vermeir, I., Van Kenhove, P., & Hendrickx, H. (2005). A Taxonomy of

Consumer Motives through Preferred Brand Personality: Empirical Findings for 11

countries. Working Paper, Ghent University, Department of Marketing.

Keller, K.L. (1993). Conceptualizing, measuring and managing customer-based brand equity.

Journal of marketing, 57 (jan), 1-22.

Knafo, A., & Schwartz, S.H. (2001). Value socialization in families of Israeli-born and

Soviet-born adolescents in Israel. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32, 213-228.

Maehr, M.L., & Kleiber, D.A. (1981). The graying of achievement motivation. American

Psychologist, 36, 787-793.

McAdams, D.P. (1988). Biography, narrative, and lives: an introduction. In D.P. McAdams,

& R.L. Ochberg (Eds.), Psychobiography and life narratives (p. 1-18). Durham, NC:

Duke University Press.

124
Chapter IV : Consumer Motives and Personal Values

McClelland, D.C. (1980). Motive dispositions: the merits of operant and respondent

measures. In L. Wheeler (Ed.), Review of Personality and Social Psychology (Vol. 1,

p. 10-41). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

McClelland, D.C. (1985). Human Motivation. Glenview, IL: Scott Foresman and Company.

McClelland, D.C. (1987). Human Motivation. Cambridge UK: Cambridge University Press.

McClelland, D.C. (1992). Motivational configurations. In C.P. Smith (Ed.), Motivation and

Personality: Handbook of thematic content analysis (p. 87-99). New York:

Cambridge University Press.

McClelland, D.C., Koestner, R., & Weinberger, J. (1989). How do self attributed and implicit

motives differ? In F. Halisch, & J.H.L. Van Den Bercken (Eds.), International

perspectives on achievement and task motivation (p. 259-289). Amsterdam: Swets &

Zeitlinger.

Mowen, J.C. & Minor, M. (1997). Consumer behavior. Fifth Edition, Prentice Hall.

Murray, H.A. (1943). The Thematic Apperception Test: Mannual. Cambridge, M.A.:

Harvard University Press.

Rohan, M.J., & Zanna, M.P. (1996). Value transmission in families. In C. Seligman, J.M.

Olson, & M.P. Zanna (Eds.), The Psychology of Values: the Ontario Symposium (Vol.

8) (p. 253-276). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values. New York: The Free Press.

Schiffman, L.G., Sherman, E., & Long, M.M. (2003). Toward a better understanding of the

interplay of personal values and the internet. Psychology and Marketing, 20(2), 169-

186.

Schoenpflug, U. (2001). Perspectives on cultural transmission (special issue). Journal of

Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32(2).

125
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Schultheiss, O.C., & Brunstein, J.C. (1999). Goal imagery: bridging the gap between implicit

motives and explicit goals. Journal of Personality, 67(1), 1-38.

Schultheiss, O.C., & Brunstein, J.C. (2001). Assessment of implicit motives with a research

version of the TAT: picture profiles, gender differences, and relations to other

personality measures. Journal of Personality Assessment, 77(1), 71-86.

Schwartz, S.H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: theoretical advances

and empirical tests in 20 countries. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 25,

1–65.

Schwartz, S.H. (1994). Are there universal aspects in the content and structure of values?

Journal of Social Issues, 50(4), 19-45.

Schwartz, S.H., & Bardi, A. (2001). Value hierarchies across cultures: taking a similarities

perspective. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32(3), 268-290.

Schwartz, S.H., & Bilsky, W. (1987). Toward a universal psychological structure of human

values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 550-562.

Schwartz, S.H., & Boehnke, K. (2004). Evaluating the structure of human values with

confirmatory factor analysis. Journal of Research in Personality, 38, 230-255.

Schwartz, S.H., & Sagiv, L. (1995). Identifying culture specifics in the content and structure

of values. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 26, 92-116.

Skinner, B.F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Smith, C.P. (1992). Motivation and Personality: Handbook of thematic content analysis.

New York: Cambridge University Press.

Steenkamp, J.B., ter Hofstede, F., & Wedel, M. (1999). A cross-national investigation into

the individual and cultural antecedents of consumer innovativeness. Journal of

Marketing, 63(2), 55-69.

126
Chapter IV : Consumer Motives and Personal Values

Tuerlinckx, F., De Boeck, P., & Lens, W. (2002). Measuring needs with the Thematic

Apperception Test: a psychometric study. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 82(3), 448-461.

Venable, B.T., Rose, G.M., Bush, V.D., & Gilbert, F.W. (2005). The role of brand

personality in charitable giving: an assessment and validtion. Journal of the Academy

of Marketing Science, 33(3), 295-312.

Veroff, J., & Smith, D.A. (1985). Motives and values over the adult years. In D. Kleiber, &

M. Maehr (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievements: motivation and

adulthood (p. 1-53). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Winter, D.G., John, O.P., Stewart, A.J., Klohnen, E.C., & Duncan, L.E. (1998). Traits and

motives: toward an integration of two traditions in personality research.

Psychological Review, 105(2), 230-250.

127
A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives : Construct Validity

CHAPTER V

HOW CONSUMER MOTIVES INFLUENCE TRAIT-BEHAVIOR

RELATIONSHIPS : THE CASE OF CONSUMER INNOVATIVENESS

AND SELF-CONTROL

129
Chapter V

How Consumer Motives influence Trait-Behavior Relationships :

The case of Consumer Innovativeness and Self-control

1. Abstract

In the realm of the application of contingency models in personality research, present research

investigates the impact of underlying consumer motives on the relationship between

personality traits and several aspects of (consumer) behavior. In the first study the trait

consumer innovativeness is considered and its link with new-product adoption behavior,

whereas study 2 focuses on the relationship between dispositional self-control and disciplined

driving behavior. The results of both studies provide support for the basic proposition that

actual behavioral outcomes of human personality characteristics might be function of the

alignment with the particular consumer motives that are active within a given product context.

In line with the principle of motive compatibility, a dispositional personality trait gives rise to

a particular behavior, especially when that behavior is compatible with the manifest consumer

motives. However, when the behavior can less be aligned with one’s underlying motive

dispositions, high levels of the personality trait are less likely to result into the behavioral

outcome. Based on these results some general conclusions and implications are formulated

concerning the study of trait-behavior relationships in personality research and directions for

future research are proposed.

131
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

2. Introduction

Though over the years a number of different theories of personality have emerged in literature,

most frequently, personality research has been regarded as synonymous with the study of

traits (e.g. Arnauld et al., 2004). Human personality traits, as defined by several trait theorists

(e.g. Guilford, 1959; Cattell, 1950), can be considered as temporally and situationally

invariant personal characteristics (i.e. dispositions) that distinguish different individuals and

lead to consistencies in behavior across situations and over time (e.g. Baumgartner, 2002).

The rich literature on trait-behavior relationships in psychology and other behavioral sciences

has enticed marketing researchers to theorize that personality characteristics should be able to

predict consumer behavior too. From automobile purchase to cigarette smoking, numerous

attempts have been made to study the influence of personality on the behavior of consumers

in the marketplace. However, despite these interests, most empirical findings fail to confirm

meaningful relationships between measures of personality and behavioral measures as, on

average, only 5 percent to10 percent of the variance could be accounted for (e.g. Jacoby, 1971;

Pizzam, 1972; Wells and Beard, 1973; Haugtvedt et al., 1992; Bagozzi, 1994).

One major reason for these weak results might be the conceptual irrelevance generally

existing between traits and behavior. As mentioned by many authors, global personality traits

at a high level of generality should be poor predictors of specific behaviors, given the impact

of many intervening variables also influencing these behaviors in several ways (e.g.

Kassarjian, 1971; Lastovicka and Joachimsthaler, 1988; Kassarjian and Sheffet, 1991).

Hence, human personality could be considered as only one influence among many and, as a

result, contingency models in personality research have been proposed that take into account

132
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

these specific interactions between personality variables and other personal and situational

factors (e.g. Belk, 1975; Dickson, 1982; Miller and Ginter, 1979; Van Kenhove et al., 1999).

The purpose of the present research is to investigate the influence of underlying consumer

motives on the relationship between personality traits and several aspects of (consumer)

behavior. As consumer motives can be referred to as desirable end-states that people seek to

attain through consumption (e.g. McClelland, 1985), these higher level constructs could be

supposed to determine the content of lower level behavioral goals, which then become the

means to realize the higher level motives (e.g. Park and Smith, 1989). In addition, also

general personality traits may predispose consumers to behave in a certain manner (e.g.

Baumgartner, 2002). When considering the combined influence of consumer motives and

personality, actual behavioral outcomes of human personality traits may be assumed to be

function of the alignment with the particular consumer motives that are active within a

specific product environment. Based on the general principle of motive compatibility

developed in literature (e.g. Higgins, 2002), a dispositional personality trait is expected to

give rise to a particular consumer behavior, especially when that behavior is compatible with

(or instrumental to) the manifest consumer motives. However, when the behavior in question

could less be aligned with the underlying motive orientations, high levels of the personality

trait are supposed to be less likely to result into the behavioral outcome. Hence, based on

these insights, significant behavioral differences could be expected among people scoring

equally high on a particular personality trait according to different consumer motives that are

manifest within the given product context.

To test these propositions two studies were conducted within two different research contexts.

In the first study the role of consumer motives is investigated as intervening factors in the

133
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

relationship between innovativeness as a personality trait and consumer adoption behavior for

mobile phones. Though high-innovative consumers might be predisposed to adopt innovative

mobile phone product attributes to a greater extent than low-innovative consumers, according

to the manifest consumer motives, significant differences are expected among the former

consumers regarding the nature of this innovative behavior, i.e. either adopting utilitarian or

hedonic innovative attributes. Study 2, in its turn, further explores the intervening role of

consumer motives in the context of the relationship between dispositional self-control and

disciplined driving behavior. Here again, high levels of human self-control are assumed to

result into more disciplined driving behavior. However, significant differences are expected

among people with high dispositional self-control according to the active consumer motives.

Before going through a detailed explanation of these two empirical studies, the next section

reviews some conceptual background regarding Callebaut et al.’s (1999) taxonomy of

consumer motives, which provides the theoretical motive structure used in this research.

2.1. A general taxonomy of consumer motives14

As stated above, consumer motives pertain to desirable end-states of (consumer) behavior that

people seek to attain (e.g. McClelland, 1985). By conducting a series of empirical studies

across 11 different countries and four different product categories, Geeroms et al. (2005)

validated the circumplex consumer motive taxonomy of Callebaut et al. (1999), based on

respondents’ valence ratings of 34 preferred brand personality traits. The results of a

multidimensional scaling analysis revealed a consistent and replicable structure underlying

the concept of preferred brand personality. As conceptually preferred brand personality

dimensions could be considered as behavioral expressions of underlying motive orientations,

14
The conceptual background reviewed in this section comprises a brief recapitulation of the major findings and developments of chapter III
of this dissertation, in order to refresh the reader’s mind regarding these issues. For a full explanation of the origins of the general consumer
motive taxonomy and the basic statements made in this section we thus refer to this corresponding chapter.

134
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

Geeroms et al. (2005) reformulated the found preferred brand personality structure in terms of

Callebaut et al.’s (1999) framework of eight consumer motives, i.e. Vitality, Pleasure,

Conviviality, Belonging, Security, Control, Recognition and Power. Table 5.1 provides an

overview of the motive definitions as well as the set of preferred brand personality items used

to represent each of the eight motive domains.

Table 5.1 : Motive Definitions and Measurement Items

Vitality : desire to experience adventure, test one’s boundaries and discover new things.
Items: Adventurous, Creative, Innovative, Contemporary

Pleasure : desire to follow impulses and emotions, to maximise pleasure without inhibition
or self-control.
Items: Cheerful, Bon-vivant, Unrestrained, Impulsive

Conviviality : desire to be with people, to open up socially, to share experiences and


emotionally connect with others.
Items: Optimistic, Great to get on with, Approachable, Friendly

Belonging : desire to merge into the group and to be accepted and supported by loved
ones.
Items: reliable, Cooperative, Familial, Sincere, Sympathetic

Security : desire to hide away, to feel safe, relaxed, calm and be protected together with
an obedience to norms and regulations of the group.
Items: Simple, Calm, Traditional, Careful, Introvert

Control : desire to suppress inner feelings and emotions to behave in a way that is
morally acceptable.
Items: Self-disciplined, Mature, Organized, Punctual

Recognition : desire to be recognized by others for one’s skills, knowledge and


appearance, to be respected by others.
Items: Intellectual, Stylish, Sophisticated, Special

Power : desire to be better than the rest, to feel superior, to be a leader. Higher priority is
given to individualistic goals and standards as opposed to group norms.
Items: Successful, Leader, Competitive, Powerful

Source : Geeroms et al (2005)

135
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

As already mentioned, these eight motive orientations could be organized into a circumplex

structure, determined by two basic, bipolar dimensions, which respectively represent an

intrapersonal and interpersonal motive axis (see Fig. 5.1). All these motivational states could

be considered as stable dispositions that are present within every individual and become

aroused by a particular product environment (e.g. McClelland, 1980). To satisfy the

consumer motives that are manifest in a given product context, people engage in motive-

directed behavior (e.g. Mowen and Minor, 1997). As Callebaut et al.’s (1999) motive

taxonomy has been proven to be valid cross-culturally, seems compatible with prior

classifications of human motives (e.g. Ford and Nichols, 1987) and, in addition, has a number

of advantages over these previous motive taxonomies, this structure will serve as the basic

theoretical framework in the present research.

Figure 5.1 : Callebaut et al.’s (1999) Taxonomy of Consumer Motives

Source: Adapted from J. Callebaut et al., “Motivational marketing research revisited” (Leuven-Apeldoorn:
Garant, 1999)

136
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

3. Study 1: Consumer Innovativeness

3.1. Theoretical background and hypotheses

Consumer innovativeness has been discussed extensively in the marketing literature (e.g.

Steenkamp et al., 1999; Grewal et al., 2000; Wood and Swait, 2002; Roehrich, 2004;

Goldsmith et al., 2005). Broadly taken, this construct has undergone two major

conceptualizations. Rogers and Shoemaker (1971, p. 27) define innovativeness as “the degree

to which an individual is relatively quicker in adopting an innovation than other members of

his social system.” Rogers (1983) provides a definition in the same line, as innovativeness is

referred to as the degree to which an individual or adoption unit adopts new ideas, goods or

practices as compared to the average member of the system. Essentially, these are operational

definitions, couched directly in terms of measurement of innovativeness on the behavioral or

observable level. The construct is known as the actual acquisition of new products or services

and therefore called ‘actualized innovativeness’. However, a second research stream argues

that consumer innovativeness should be considered as a hypothetical construct, situated at a

higher level of abstraction than ‘actualized innovativeness’ and not linked with a specific

innovation. Hence, more recent conceptualizations of innovativeness are based on what is

called ‘innate innovativeness’, a generalized unobservable predisposition toward innovations

that can be applied across product classes (e.g. Midgley and Dowling, 1978, 1993; Hirschman,

1980; Foxall, 1988, 1995; Limayem et al., 2000). Though few consensus exists on the exact

definition of ‘innate innovativeness’ (Mudd, 1990), the idea of innovativeness as a

predisposition has been widely accepted in psychology and consumer research (e.g. Kirton,

1976; Gatignon and Robertson, 1985; Price and Ridgway, 1983). Goldsmith and colleagues

(Goldsmith and Hofacker, 1991; Goldsmith et al., 1995) consider this generalized personality

137
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

trait as global innovativeness and distinguished it from domain-specific innovativeness that

can be applied to a specific product category.

In relating innate and actualized innovativeness, there has been debate in the literature on

whether such an innovative predisposition determines new-product adoption behavior (e.g.

Manning et al., 1995; Im et al., 2003). Though few studies indicate no significant results at

all (Foxall and Goldsmith, 1988; Citrin et al., 2000), in general, only weak positive

relationships between consumer innovativeness and innovative adoption behavior have been

empirically detected. At best, only about 10 percent of the behavioral variance could be

accounted for (e.g. Cotte and Wood, 2004; Foxall and Haskins, 2001; Park and Dyer, 1995).

As mentioned in the introduction, these findings are consistent with general personality

research indicating that the relationship between personality variables and consumer behavior

is weak due to the general conceptual irrelevance between traits and behavior (e.g. Kassarjian,

1971; Kassarjian and Sheffet, 1991; Lastovicka and Joachimsthaler, 1988). Hence, in the

diffusion literature, contingency models of innovativeness have been proposed relating new-

product adoption behavior to a combination of both an innovative predisposition and several

other intervening variables (Midgley and Dowling, 1978, 1993; Venkatraman, 1991).

In the latter context, the present study investigates the role of underlying consumer motives in

influencing the relationship between innovativeness and consumer adoption behavior for

mobile phones. Since innate innovativeness is assumed to predispose consumers to adopt

new-product innovations (Craig and Ginter, 1975; Joseph and Vyas, 1984), people scoring

high on this personality trait are expected to be more likely to adopt innovative mobile phone

product attributes. However, the nature of this innovative behavior, i.e. either adopting

138
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

utilitarian or hedonic attributes 15 is supposed to be dependent on the manifest consumer

motivations.

To test these propositions specific hypotheses are developed regarding the relationships

between innovativeness, consumer motives and mobile phone adoption behavior. In general,

based on the weak, though significant, positive relationships that exist between consumer

innovativeness and the adoption of new-product innovations, we can propose:

H1: People scoring high on consumer innovativeness will be more likely to adopt

innovative mobile phone product attributes compared to low-innovative consumers

More specifically, Venkatraman and colleagues in a series of studies (e.g. Venkatraman, 1991;

Venkatraman and MacInnis, 1985) have shown that these relationships between

innovativeness and new-product adoption behavior hold for both hedonic and utilitarian

innovations. Hence, we divided H1 in the following two sub-hypotheses:

H1a: People scoring high on consumer innovativeness will be more likely to adopt hedonic

innovative mobile phone attributes compared to low-innovative consumers

H1b: People scoring high on consumer innovativeness will be more likely to adopt

utilitarian innovative mobile phone attributes compared to low-innovative consumers

Previous studies have linked the construct of consumer innovativeness to a variety of

personality traits. Innovativeness was found to be correlated positively with optimum

15
In the context of present research we made a distinction between ‘hedonic’ attributes of mobile phones that are fun and enjoyable (e.g.
polyphonic ring-tones) and ‘utilitarian’ attributes that serve mainly instrumental functions (e.g. GPRS). This distinction is congruent with
previous literature on hedonic versus utilitarian consumer choice (e.g. Hirschman and Holbrook, 1982; Babin et al., 1994; Dhar and
Wertenbroch, 2000) and is assumed to provide conceptual relevance in the context of our overarching framework of consumer motives (e.g.
Callebaut et al., 1999).

139
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

stimulation level, need for change, need for cognition, independence, extraversion,

impulsivity, risk taking, tolerance of ambiguity, inner-directedness, capacity for status,

flexibility, creativity, venturesomeness and need for uniqueness (e.g. Steenkamp and

Baumgartner, 1992; Cotte and Wood, 2004; Zuckerman, 1979; Venkatraman and Price, 1990;

Bearden et al., 1986; Etzel et al., 1976; Midgley, 1977; Carlson and Grossbart, 1984; Wahlers

et al., 1986; Hirschman and Stern, 1998; Burns and Krampf, 1991). In addition, innovators

have been described as more individualistic, intelligent and higher in achievement motivation

(Rogers, 1983). In contrast, innovativeness correlated negatively with dogmatism,

conservatism, need for structure, need for clarity, other-directedness and uncertainty

avoidance (e.g. Foxall and Bhate, 1993).

Although personality traits are distinct from consumer motives, they are conceptually related

to the consumer motive taxonomy of Callebaut et al. (1999) (cf. supra: chapter III). Traits

found to be correlated positively with consumer innovativeness seem congruent with left-side

motive domains, i.e. Vitality (aligned with need for stimulation, need for change,

independence, extraversion, impulsivity, risk-taking, inner-directedness, creativity and

venturesomeness) and Recognition (aligned with capacity for status, need for cognition, need

for uniqueness, intelligence, need for achievement and individualism). Furthermore,

personality traits considered to affect consumer innovativeness in a negative way are

conceptually closest to the right-side motive domains of Security (aligned with dogmatism,

conservatism and uncertainty avoidance) and Conviviality (aligned with other-directedness).

Hence, based on these insights consumer innovativeness might be expected to be related

positively to consumer motives of Vitality and Recognition, whereas it is assumed to be

related negatively to Security and Conviviality. A second hypothesis concerning these

expectations is formulated in the following way:

140
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

H2: Consumers driven by underlying motives of Vitality or Recognition will exhibit higher

levels of consumer innovativeness compared to consumers driven by motives of Security or

Conviviality, with no significant differences existing between Vitality and Recognition-driven

and Security and Conviviality-driven consumers respectively

As higher levels of consumer innovativeness are predicted to result into increases in adoption

of innovative mobile phone attributes (i.e. H1) and innovativeness is supposed to be related

positively to left-side motive domains of Vitality and Recognition (i.e. H2), it can reasonably

be assumed that consumers driven by the latter motives relative to Security or Conviviality-

driven consumers will be more likely to adopt those innovative attributes. However,

significant differences are expected between Vitality and Recognition-driven consumers

regarding the adoption of either hedonic or utilitarian attributes. To underpin this proposition

we build on the ‘matching or compatibility’ principle, previously established in the motive-

literature (e.g. Higgins, 2002; Pham and Higgins, 2005). Assuming that top-down processes

of motive alignment, by which higher level motives determine the content of lower level

goals (i.e. benefits and objective features searched for in particular products), are most likely

to occur (e.g. Park and Smith, 1989; Huffman et al., 2000), this principle basically implies

that certain attributes in a product category will receive greater importance when they are

compatible with someone’s underlying motive orientation than when they are incompatible

with the active motives. As a result, choice alternatives attractive on the motive-consistent

criteria will be evaluated more favorably compared to motive-incongruent alternatives (e.g.

Aaker and Lee, 2001; Chernev, 2004).

In the context of the present research, hedonic innovative attributes are assumed to be more

compatible with a Vitality orientation, whereas innovative attributes that are utilitarian in

141
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

nature tend to provide a better fit with an underlying motive of Recognition. The rationale for

this prediction follows from the notion that – given their respective positions along the

intrapersonal (i.e. vertical) axis of Callebaut et al.’s (1999) motive taxonomy (see Fig. 5.1) –

Vitality-driven consumers tend to focus on achieving pleasure and stimulation, whereas

Recognition-driven consumers tend to focus on functionality and control. Hence, given the

greater importance attached to the motive-congruent criteria, adoption of hedonic innovative

mobile phone attributes is supposed to be more prominent among Vitality-driven consumers

than among Recognition-driven consumers, while the reverse is true for the adoption of

utilitarian attributes. As Security and Conviviality-driven consumers – given their low scores

on consumer innovativeness – are less likely to be predisposed to the adoption of innovative

mobile phone attributes of any kind, the position along the intrapersonal motive axis is

assumed to be of no influence for the latter people. Hence, no significant differences in

adoption levels for both hedonic and utilitarian innovative attributes are expected between

Security and Conviviality-driven consumers. More formally, the above predictions can be

stated as follows:

H3: Consumers driven by underlying motives of Vitality or Recognition will be more

likely to adopt hedonic innovative mobile phone attributes compared to consumers driven by

motives of Security or Conviviality, with Vitality-driven consumers exhibiting higher

adoption levels than Recognition-driven consumers. No significant differences in adoption

levels will exist between Security and Conviviality-driven consumers.

H4: Consumers driven by underlying motives of Vitality or Recognition will be more

likely to adopt utilitarian innovative mobile phone attributes compared to consumers driven

by motives of Security or Conviviality, with Recognition-driven consumers exhibiting higher

142
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

adoption levels than Vitality-driven consumers. No significant differences in adoption levels

will exist between Security and Conviviality-driven consumers.

Summarizing, when integrating the above hypotheses, it can be assumed that – though

consumer innovativeness might be equally related to motive domains of Vitality and

Recognition – specific behavioral effects are likely to be function of which of these two

motive states is manifest. Based on the principle of compatibility in motive-literature,

innovative behavior with an hedonic and utilitarian character is expected to show up when

respectively consumer motives of Vitality and Recognition become manifest. In what follows,

an overview is given of the empirical research that was conducted to test these propositions.

3.2. Method

3.2.1. Participants

To collect data 270 questionnaires were distributed among Flemish users of mobile phones

via a random walk method. Subjects were rewarded for their participation with a writing pen.

Of the total number of distributed surveys 241 were returned, for an initial response rate of

89.3%. However, of the 241 returned, we could not use 31 surveys due to large amounts of

missing data on key variables. Thus, the final number of usable surveys was 210, accounting

for a final response rate of 77.8%. The sample was representative in terms of gender but

slightly biased toward younger age and higher education, probably due to the fact that mobile

phones were chosen as the topic of investigation. An overview of the sample characteristics

is provided in Table 5.2.

143
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Table 5.2 : Sample Characteristics Study 1 (% of respondents, N = 210)

Sample Population*
Gender
Male 48.6 48.9
Female 51.4 51.1
Age
<25 28.1 18.4
25-40 47.9 32.8
40-50 14.9 22.6
>50 9.1 26.2

Education
Lower education (age <=18) 41.5 67.4
Higher education (age >18) 58.5 32.6
Profession
Labourer 17.6
Employee 33.3
Executive 4.8
Self-employed 7.1
* Functionary 3.8
Keeping house 6.7
Retired 6.7
Unemployed 3.3
Student 16.7

Source: NIS (2005). Private Households Statistics. Brussels: NIS, National


Institute for Statistics

3.2.2. Measures

Consumer motives were assessed by the 34-item Consumer Motive Scale developed by

Geeroms et al. (2005). Based on this scale, all eight positions in Callebaut et al.’s (1999)

motive taxonomy are represented by a limited number of motive-expressive personality traits

(see Table 5.1). Subjects were asked to select their preferred mobile phone16 out of a set of

16
Personality traits associated with a preferred product/brand are assumed to serve a self-expressive function (e.g. Keller, 1993), hereby
revealing what actually drives consumers to purchase or like the product/brand (e.g. Winter et al., 1998). Hence, preferred product/brand
personality traits could be considered to channel the behavioral expression of underlying consumer motives (cf. supra: chapter III).

144
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

several types of mobile phones and rate the extent to which each of the 34 traits describes the

personality of the chosen phone on a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1= not at all

descriptive to 5= very descriptive. As unsatisfactory Cronbach’s α coefficients of below .60

restrained us from computing composite construct measures for all eight motive domains, we

decided to create reliable motive constructs based on combinations of items of adjacent

positions with the highest intercorrelations. Legitimated by the fact that the relationships

among the eight motive domains describe a continuum within a two-dimensional circumplex

rather than discrete motive categories, the positions of Pleasure and Vitality (r=.78), Power

and Recognition (r=.85), Control and Security (r=.72) and Belonging and Conviviality (r=.74)

were respectively combined into single constructs. After dropping some items with poor

inter-item correlations (N=6), four reliable motive constructs were computed based on

average sum scores of the combined items and broadly corresponding to the four quadrants of

Callebaut et al.’s (1999) motive taxonomy 17 . In Table 5.3 some relevant information is

shown pertaining to each of the four constructs, including the number of items, item labels

and reliability coefficients.

17
For reasons of convenience, in the remaining sections, these four motive constructs will be referred to with the names of the
corresponding quadrants, i.e. Vitality, Recognition, Security and Conviviality respectively

145
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Table 5.3 : Four Motive Constructs (Study 1)

Motive Construct Final Item Labels Cronbach’s α


Number of
Items
Pleasure/Vitality 6 adventurous, creative, .72
(i.e. upper-left quadrant) innovative, cheerful,
impulsive, unrestrained
Power/Recognition 8 successful, leader, .77
(i.e. bottom-left quadrant) powerful, competitive,
intellectual, stylish, special,
sophisticated
Control/Security 7 mature, organized, simple, .72
(i.e. bottom-right quadrant) traditional, calm, careful,
introvert
Belonging/Conviviality 7 friendly, approachable, .75
(i.e. upper-right quadrant) optimistic, great to get on
with, familial, sincere,
cooperative

To measure innate consumer innovativeness the Consumer Innovativeness Scale of Manning

et al. (1995) was used. Respondents were asked to indicate their level of (dis)agreement with

the 14 items, using a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1= completely disagree to 5=

completely agree. Initially, this scale comprises two dimensions, i.e. consumer independent

judgment making (CIJM, 6 items, alpha=.84) and consumer novelty seeking (CNS, 8 items,

alpha=.88). However, for the purpose of this research, we abandoned the scores on the

separate subscales in favor of an average sum score on the total scale. The Cronbach’s α

coefficient for the 14-item consumer innovativeness construct was .72. For an overview of

the scale items, we refer to Appendix A.

Adoption of innovative mobile phone product attributes was investigated by asking

respondents about their intentions to buy a mobile phone with each of the following options:

146
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

GPRS, color screen, MP3 player, photo camera, video system, WAP, bluetooth and

polyphonic ring-tones. All of these attributes were considered innovative at the time of

survey in the surveyed region. Purchase intentions were measured on a five-point Likert scale

ranging from 1= definitely will not buy to 5= definitely will buy. A factor analysis performed

on these intention scores revealed two broad factors, simultaneously accounting for about

69.7% of the variance (see Table 5.4). The first factor contained the attributes polyphonic

ring-tones, color screen, MP3 player, photo camera and video system, which could be

considered as hedonic in nature, as they make the use of mobile phones more pleasurable and

entertaining. However, attributes loading on the second factor, i.e. GPRS, WAP and

bluetooth could be labelled utilitarian attributes, as they serve mainly instrumental functions

of data transmission and information exchange between devices. By summing and averaging

intention scores of the items belonging to each of these two factors, two composite purchase

intention constructs were computed – respectively referring to the adoption of hedonic

(alpha= .85) and utilitarian (alpha=.86) innovative mobile phone attributes – that were used as

dependent variables in further analyses.

Table 5.4 : Factor Analysis – Innovative Mobile Phone Attributes

Attributes Factor Exp. Var.


loading (%)
1 Polyphonic ring-tones .87 51.4
Color screen .86
MP3 player .78
Photo camera .77
Video system .70
2 GPRS .94 18.3
WAP .88
Bluetooth .84

147
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

3.3. Results

Individuals were categorized as high or low in consumer innovativeness via a median split

(median= 3.2; M low consumer innovativeness= 2.6, M high consumer innovativeness= 3.8).

Results of an Independent Samples T-test indicated that purchase intention levels for hedonic

innovative mobile phone attributes were significantly higher among high-innovative

consumers (M= 2.5) than among low-innovative consumers (M= 2.0) (t= -4.69; p< .01),

suggesting that high-innovative consumers are more likely to adopt those innovative attributes.

In addition, high-innovative consumers showed significantly higher measures of purchase

intention for utilitarian innovative mobile phone attributes (M= 2.5) compared to low-

innovative consumers (M= 2.0) (t= -3.83; p< .01). Hence, H1a and H1b were fully supported.

In view of the remaining hypotheses, co-ordinates on both the interpersonal (i.e. horizontal)

and intrapersonal (i.e. vertical) motive axes were computed per respondent18 and via a median

split performed on these variables individuals were respectively assigned a left or right-side

and a bottom or upper-side position in Callebaut et al.’s (1999) motive taxonomy (median=

0.7 for the score on the horizontal axis; M left-side position= -0.2, M right-side position= 1.3;

median= 0.4 for the score on the vertical axis; M bottom-side position= -0.4, M upper-side

position= 1.1). Afterwards, a 2 (position horizontal axis: left/right) X 2 (position vertical axis:

bottom/upper) MANOVA analysis 19 was conducted with consumer innovativeness (H2),

adoption of hedonic innovative mobile phone attributes (H3) and adoption of utilitarian

innovative mobile phone attributes (H4) as dependent variables.

18
Relying on the procedure proposed by Feather (1995), co-ordinates on the interpersonal (intrapersonal) motive axes were computed by
subtracting the summed scores for Vitality and Recognition (Recognition and Security) from the summed scores for Conviviality and
Security (Vitality and Conviviality), with higher co-ordinates indicating a more right-side (upper-side) position.
19
MANOVA analysis was conducted instead of moderated regression, due to the problem of multicollinearity between the predictor
variables (i.e. consumer motives and personality traits) and, in addition, the latter analysis would imply a needless complication of the results
and their interpretations.

148
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

In general, the results showed a significant main effect of both position on the horizontal axis

(F(3, 204)= 21.09, p< .01) and position on the vertical axis (F(3, 204)= 42.92, p< .01), as well

as a significant interaction effect (F(3, 204)= 21.72, p< .01).

When looking at the dependent variables separately, for consumer innovativeness, only a

significant main effect of position on the horizontal axis could be detected (F(1, 206)= 43.92,

p< .01). Left-side consumers, i.e. consumers driven by underlying motives of Vitality or

Recognition scored significantly higher on consumer innovativeness (M= 3.5) compared to

right-side consumers, i.e. consumers driven by underlying motives of Conviviality or Security

(M= 2.9). As indicated by the non-significant interaction effect (F(1, 206)= 0.09, p> .05) (see

Fig. 5.2), simple main effects tests revealed no significant differences in consumer

innovativeness scores between Vitality (i.e. upper-left position; M= 3.5) and Recognition-

driven consumers (i.e. bottom-left position; M= 3.6) (F(1, 106)= 0.60, p> .05), as well as

between Conviviality (i.e. upper-right position; M= 2.8) and Security-driven consumers (i.e.

bottom-right position; M= 2.9) (F(1, 100)= 0.13, p> .05). These findings are in full support of

H2.

Figure 5.2 : Study 1 – MANOVA results : Consumer Innovativeness

5,0
Consumer Innovativeness

4,5 Position vertical


4,0 motive axis
3,5 3,6
3,5
Upper
3,0 2,9
2,8 Bottom
2,5
2,0
1,5
1,0
Left Right
Position horizontal motive axis

149
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

With regard to adoption of hedonic innovative mobile phone attributes both position on the

horizontal axis (F(1, 206)= 38.19, p< .01) and position on the vertical axis (F(1, 206)= 22.43,

p< .01) mattered. Left-side consumers, i.e. consumers driven by underlying motives of

Vitality or Recognition showed significantly higher measures of purchase intention for

hedonic innovative mobile phone attributes (M= 2.6) compared to right-side consumers, i.e.

consumers driven by underlying motives of Conviviality or Security (M= 1.9), and so did

upper-side consumers, i.e. consumers driven by underlying motives of Vitality or Conviviality

(M= 2.5) as compared to bottom-side consumers, i.e. consumers driven by underlying motives

of Recognition or Security (M= 2.0). Especially important however, is the significant

interaction effect that could be detected (F(1, 206)= 10.16, p< .01) (see Fig. 5.3). In full

support of H3, simple main effects tests revealed that Vitality-driven consumers (i.e. upper-

left position; M= 2.9) exhibited significantly higher purchase intention levels for hedonic

innovative attributes than Recognition-driven consumers (i.e. bottom-left position; M= 2.2)

(F(1, 106)= 33.13, p< .01), but that purchase intention levels between Conviviality (i.e. upper-

right position; M= 2.0) and Security-driven consumers (i.e. bottom-right position; M= 1.9) did

not differ (F(1, 100)= 1.14, p> .05).

Figure 5.3 : Study 1 – MANOVA results : Adoption of Hedonic Attributes

5,0
Position vertical
innovative attributes

4,5
adoption hedonic

motive axis
4,0
3,5 Upper
3,0 2,9 Bottom
2,5
2,2
2,0 2,0
1,9
1,5
1,0
Left Right
Position horizontal motive axis

150
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

Finally, for adoption of utilitarian innovative mobile phone attributes, the main effects of

both position on the horizontal axis (F(1, 206)= 27,86 p< .01) and position on the vertical axis

(F(1, 206)= 27.34, p< .01) were significant, as was the two-way interaction effect (F(1, 206)=

15.27 p< .01) (see Fig. 5.4). Purchase intention levels for utilitarian innovative mobile phone

attributes were higher among left-side consumers, i.e. consumers driven by underlying

motives of Vitality or Recognition (M= 2.7) than among right-side consumers, i.e. consumers

driven by underlying motives of Conviviality or Security (M= 1.9), as well as among bottom-

side consumers, i.e. consumers driven by underlying motives of Recognition or Security (M=

2.5) as compared to upper-side consumers, i.e. consumers driven by underlying motives of

Vitality or Conviviality (M= 2.0). Simple main effects tests revealed that Recognition-driven

consumers (i.e. bottom-left position; M= 3.1) exhibited significantly higher purchase intention

levels for utilitarian innovative attributes than Vitality-driven consumers (i.e. upper-left

position; M= 2.2) (F(1, 106)= 44.20, p< .01), but that purchase intention levels between

Conviviality (i.e. upper-right position; M= 1.8) and Security-driven consumers (i.e. bottom-

right position; M= 1.9) did not differ (F(1, 100)= 0.82, p> .05). Hence, H4 was also

supported.

Figure 5.4 : Study 1 – MANOVA results : Adoption of Utilitarian Attributes

5,0
Position vertical
innovative attributes

4,5
adoption utilitarian

motive axis
4,0
3,5 Upper
3,0 3,1 Bottom
2,5
2,2
2,0 1,9
1,8
1,5
1,0
Left Right
Position horizontal motive axis

151
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

3.4. Discussion

In the context of the application of contingency models in personality research, this first study

investigated the influence of consumer motives on the relationship between innovativeness as

a personality trait and consumer innovative adoption behavior for mobile phones. Specific

hypotheses were developed regarding the links between these three constructs. The results of

Independent Samples T-test and MANOVA analyses provided support for our propositions.

In line with previous studies, we found that a significant positive relationship exists between

consumer innovativeness and the adoption of innovative mobile phone product attributes

(H1a/H1b). However – though consumer innovativeness was found to be equally related to

consumer motives of Vitality and Recognition (H2) – the nature of the innovative adoption

behavior appeared to be dependent on which of these two motive states was manifest within

the given context. In accordance with the compatibility principle in motive-literature,

innovative consumers driven by the underlying motive of Vitality exhibited higher adoption

levels (in terms of purchase intention) for hedonic innovative mobile phone attributes (H3),

whereas adoption of utilitarian innovative mobile phone attributes was more prominent

among innovative consumers driven by the underlying motive of Recognition (H4). Hence,

based on these findings, consumer motives could be considered as intervening factors in the

relationship between the trait innovativeness and new-product adoption behavior, as

consumers scoring equally high on consumer innovativeness may exhibit different innovative

adoption behaviors according to the manifest consumer motives in the given product context.

To further explore this intervening role of consumer motives a second study was performed

with human self-control as additional personality trait and disciplined driving behavior as the

dependent variable of interest.

152
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

4. Study 2: Self-control

4.1. Theoretical background and hypotheses

One of the most basic, important and distinctively human personality traits is the capacity of

human beings to exert control over the self (e.g. Muraven and Baumeister, 2000). Without

effective self-control human behavior would be driven solely by external stimuli and

automatic responses and the self’s executive component (i.e. the aspect of the self that makes

decisions, initiates and interrupts behavior, and otherwise exerts control) would not be able to

function properly (e.g. Baumeister, 1998). Whereas some theories offer an account of human

self-control as a subordinate aspect of the more inclusive construct of self-regulation (e.g.

Metcalfe and Mischel, 1999; Karlsson, 2003), more often, these two terms are used

interchangeably as both refer to the self’s ability to alter its own states and responses (e.g.

Baumeister, 2002; Baumeister et al., 1998; Baumeister and Heatherton, 1996; Muraven et al.,

1999). Inherent in most definitions of self-control and self-regulation is the notion of time-

inconsistency, broadly conceptualized as the gap between long-run intentions and short-run

actions (e.g. Angeletos et al., 2001; Hoch and Loewenstein, 1991; O’Donoghue and Rabin,

2000; Thaler and Shefrin, 1981; Kim, 2006). Sometimes people have preferences in the long-

run that tend to conflict with short-run preferences for immediate gratification. Self-control,

as a means to overcome such time-inconsistent preferences, has been typically defined as the

ability to resist immediate impulses and to strive toward distant goals, not apparent in the

immediate environment (e.g. Slessareva and Muraven, 2004; Tice et al., 2001; Wertenbroch,

1998). The capacity to maintain self-control and successfully implement the long-run

decisions depends on the relative strength of the opposing forces of desire and willpower.

Most frequently in literature, willpower strength has been conceptualized as a limited

153
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

resource, akin to energy, especially insofar as it becomes depleted over time and needs

replenishment by rest, sleep and positive emotions (e.g. Baumeister, 2003; Muraven and

Shmueli, 2006; Baumeister et al., 2000; Bruyneel et al., 2006).

As mentioned by several authors, individual differences in self-control capacity effectively

predict positive outcomes across a variety of life domains. Empirical evidence indicates that

people with high dispositional self-control show better achievement and task performance (e.g.

Feldman et al., 1995; Wolfe and Johnson, 1995), fewer impulse control problems (e.g. Peluso

et al., 1999; Cook et al., 1998), better psychological adjustment (e.g. Tangney et al., 2004),

higher self-esteem and self-acceptance (e.g. Tangney and Baumeister, 2001), have better

interpersonal relationships (e.g. Kochanska et al., 2000; Krueger et al., 1996) and live more

optimal emotional lives (Leith and Baumeister, 1998). In contrast, a generalized lack of self-

control has been repeatedly related to a number of undisciplined behaviors such as excessive

cigarette smoking and food intake (e.g. Baumeister et al., 1994), alcohol and/or drugs abuse

(e.g. Gottfredson and Hirschi, 1990), impulsive buying behavior (e.g. Youn and Faber, 2000)

and illegal acts such as cheating, shoplifting and driving under the influence of alcohol (e.g.

O’Gorman and Baxter, 2002). Though trait-behavior links across these various contexts all

appeared to be significant, in line with general personality research, the found relationships

were rather modest as self-control accounted for perhaps 10 percent or less of the behavioral

variance in all cases (e.g. Youn and Faber, 2000; O’Gorman and Baxter, 2002). Hence, once

again, the question arises if relevant intervening variables could be detected that influence the

relationship between people’s capacity for self-control and behavior in predictable ways.

To address this question, the second study focuses on the influence of consumer motives on

the relationship between self-control and disciplined behavior in the context of driving one’s

154
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

car. Though people scoring high on the trait self-control may be predisposed to behave more

disciplined compared to people with low self-control levels, here again, actual behavioral

outcomes are assumed to be function of the manifest consumer motivations. In the context of

the present study, disciplined driving behavior (i.e. driving one’s car in accordance to public

norms and prescriptions) is supposed to become manifest among people with high

dispositional self-control especially when an underlying need for obedience to norms and

regulations is compatible with the specific consumer motives these people are driven by.

However, when normative behavior can less be aligned with one’s underlying motives, high

levels of self-control are predicted to be less likely to result into disciplined driving behavior

in this case.

As in the first study, specific hypotheses are developed to test the above propositions. Given

the prominent evidence in literature that high levels of human self-control are closely linked

to volitional or virtuous behavior (e.g. Baumeister and Exline, 2000; Baumeister, 2002;

Tangney et al., 2004), whereas self-control failure should be related to less disciplined and

more imprudent behavior (e.g. Gottfredson and Hirschi, 1990; O’Gorman and Baxter, 2002),

a first hypothesis concerning the relationship between the capacity for self-control and (self-

reported) disciplined driving behavior can be stated as follows:

H1: People with high dispositional self-control will report more disciplined driving

behavior compared to people with low dispositional self-control

Thus far, few studies have investigated the overlap between the construct of self-control and

other human personality traits. However, recently, positive relationships were detected

between measures of self-control and the Big Five dimensions of Conscientiousness (with its

155
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

facets order, competence, dutifulness, achievement striving, self-discipline and deliberation),

Agreeableness (with its facets trust, straightforwardness, altruism, compliance, modesty and

tender-mindedness) and Emotional stability (e.g. Tangney et al., 2004; O’Gorman and Baxter,

2002; Olson, 2005; Jensen-Campbell and Graziano, 2005). Additionally, self-control was

found to be correlated positively with delay of gratification, frugality, perfectionism, risk

aversion, parsimony, social responsibility, need to belong, need for mastery, need for

competence, desire for control, need for cognition and need for achievement (e.g. Baumeister

and Exline, 1999; Kacen and Lee, 2002; Skinner, 1996; Stammerjohan and Webster, 2002;

Rook, 1987). Significant negative relationships were established with reward responsiveness,

drive, fun seeking, need for enjoyment, need for stimulation, impulsiveness, variety seeking

and sensation seeking (e.g. O’Gorman and Baxter, 2002; Stammerjohan and Webster, 2002;

Rook, 1987).

In relationship with the general framework of consumer motives used in these studies,

personality traits showing a positive correlation with human self-control seem conceptually

related to the bottom-side motive domains of Recognition (aligned with competence,

achievement striving, self-discipline, perfectionism, need for mastery and need for cognition)

and Security (aligned with order, dutifulness, desire for control, deliberation, trust,

straightforwardness, altruism, compliance, modesty, tender-mindedness, frugality, social

responsibility, risk aversion and parsimony). In addition, personality traits found to be

correlated negatively with self-control are congruent with the upper-side motive domains of

Vitality (aligned with need for stimulation, sensation seeking, variety seeking and

impulsiveness) and Conviviality (aligned with fun seeking, need for enjoyment and reward

responsiveness). Hence, positive relationships are assumed to show up between the construct

of self-control and consumer motives of Recognition and Security, whereas negative

156
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

relationships are expected with motives of Vitality and Conviviality. More formally, we state

the following hypothesis:

H2: People driven by underlying motives of Recognition or Security will exhibit higher

levels of dispositional self-control compared to people driven by motives of Vitality or

Conviviality, with no significant differences existing between Recognition and Security-

driven and Vitality and Conviviality-driven people respectively

By integrating H1 and H2 as stated above, people driven by underlying motives of

Recognition or Security are predicted to report more disciplined driving behavior relative to

Vitality or Conviviality-driven people. However, significant differences are expected

between Recognition and Security-driven people based on the compatibility principle

established in motive-literature (e.g. Higgins, 2002; Pham and Higgins, 2005). In the context

of the present study, disciplined driving behavior formally implies driving one’s car in

accordance to public norms and prescriptions (e.g. not driving under the influence of alcohol,

not exceeding speed limits, obeying the traffic rules, etc). Given their respective positions

along the interpersonal (i.e. horizontal) axis of the general motive taxonomy (see Fig. 5.1), an

underlying need for obedience to norms and regulations of the group is inherent in the basic

motive orientation of Security-driven people, whereas Recognition-driven people tend to be

more individualistic and give higher priority to their personal goals and standards as opposed

to group norms. In contrast to Security-driven people, who tend to focus on social harmony,

people driven by an underlying motive of Recognition sometimes ignore the potential

negative consequences of their behavior for the group in favor of their own individual

interests. Hence, based on these motive-related characteristics, disciplined driving behavior

as a kind of normative behavior appears to be more compatible with a Security orientation,

157
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

and thus, is expected to be more prominent among Security-driven people than among

Recognition-driven people, with the latter seeming more likely to be predisposed to

disciplined behavior with a more individualistic character. No influence of position on the

interpersonal motive axis, and thus, no significant differences in normative driving behavior

are expected between Vitality and Conviviality-driven people as these people – given their

low levels of dispositional self-control – are less likely to be predisposed to disciplined

behavior in any way. The above predictions are incorporated in our third hypothesis:

H3: People driven by underlying motives of Recognition or Security will report more

disciplined driving behavior compared to people driven by motives of Vitality or Conviviality,

with Security-driven people scoring significantly higher than Recognition-driven people. No

significant differences in disciplined driving behavior scores will exist between Vitality and

Conviviality-driven people.

To summarize, in this second study, the role of consumer motives as intervening variables

will be further explored within a different context and for a different subject sample. Specific

behavioral effects of the trait self-control are assumed to be dependent on the particular

consumer motives that are manifest. Though human self-control is expected to be related to

the same extent to motives of Recognition and Security, disciplined driving behavior as a

resulting outcome is hypothesized to become more prominent among Security-driven people

than among Recognition-driven people based on the principle of compatibility in motive-

literature. An extensive empirical investigation of the above hypotheses is provided in the

following sections.

158
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

4.2. Method

4.2.1. Participants

Similar to study 1, data were collected by distributing 250 questionnaires among Flemish car

drivers via a random walk method. Again, subjects received a writing pen as a reward for

their participation. Of the 213 surveys that were returned, 40 exemplars were not usable as

they contained large amounts of missing data. Hence, a final number of 173 usable surveys

remained, accounting for a final response rate of 69.2%. Compared to the total population

(NIS, 2005), males were slightly overrepresented in the sample as were people with a higher

education. Table 5.5 provides a detailed overview of the characteristics of the sample.

159
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Table 5.5 : Sample Characteristics Study 2 (% of respondents, N = 173)

Sample Population*
Gender
Male 56.5 48.9
Female 43.5 51.1
Age
<25 18.5 18.4
25-40 38.6 32.8
40-50 24.3 22.6
>50 18.6 26.2

Education
Lower education (age <=18) 42.4 67.4
Higher education (age >18) 57.6 32.6
Profession
Labourer 13.6
Employee 27.1
Executive 4.0
Self-employed 9.6
Functionary 12.4
Keeping house 6.4
Retired 16.9
Unemployed 3.7
Student 6.3
* Source: NIS (2005). Private Households Statistics. Brussels: NIS, National
Institute for Statistics

4.2.2. Measures

To assess consumer motives we relied on the same measurement procedure as described in

study 1. Subjects were asked to fill out the 34-item Consumer Motive Scale of Geeroms et al.

(2005) for their preferred car brand, using a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1= not at all

descriptive to 5= very descriptive. Again, unsatisfactory reliability estimates showed up for

the eight motive domains, and thus, items of adjacent positions with high intercorrelations

were combined to create reliable motive constructs. After dropping seven items that did not

perform well, four composite construct measures were computed by respectively summing

160
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

and averaging the scores of the combined items. As in the previous study, these motive

constructs were referred to with the names of the corresponding quadrants in the general

motive taxonomy. Reliability coefficients and other relevant information pertaining to each

of the four constructs are provided in Table 5.6.

Table 5.6 : Four Motive Constructs (Study 2)

Motive Construct Final Item Labels Cronbach’s α


Number of
Items
Pleasure/Vitality 6 adventurous, creative, .69
(i.e. upper-left quadrant) innovative,
contemporary, impulsive,
unrestrained
Power/Recognition 7 successful, leader, .73
(i.e. bottom-left quadrant) powerful, competitive,
intellectual, stylish,
sophisticated
Control/Security 8 mature, organized, .71
(i.e. bottom-right quadrant) punctual, simple,
traditional, calm, careful,
introvert
Belonging/Conviviality 6 friendly, approachable, .74
(i.e. upper-right quadrant) optimistic, great to get on
with, sympathetic,
reliable

Dispositional self-control was measured by the Trait Self-Control (TSC) Scale of Tangney et

al. (2004). This measure consisted of 36 items. Participants were asked to answer all items as

they apply to them on a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1= not at all like me to 5= very

much like me. The Cronbach’s α coefficient for the 36-item scale was .89. Items

conceptually the reverse of the definition of self-control were reverse coded, such that higher

scores suggested higher self-control. A TSC mean score was finally computed for each

respondent based on the 36 items. In Appendix A an overview is given of the entire scale.

161
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

As a behavioral measure five situations were presented each outlining one act of non-

normative driving behavior, i.e. driving while under the influence of alcohol or drugs,

exceeding the speed limits, failing to give priority, ignoring stopping/traffic signals and

driving without wearing a seatbelt. Subjects were instructed to indicate the frequency of their

acting in that way on a five-point Likert scale anchored from 1= always to 5= never, with

lower frequency levels, and thus higher scale scores, suggesting a more disciplined driving

behavior. Scores of the five items were summed and averaged to represent the corresponding

construct. The coefficient alpha for the composite measure of disciplined driving behavior

was .83.

4.3. Results

Via a median split respondents were categorized as high or low in dispositional self-control

(median= 3.6; M low dispositional self-control= 3.1, M high dispositional self-control= 4.0).

Results of an Independent Samples T-test indicated that people with high dispositional self-

control reported significantly higher levels of disciplined driving behavior (M= 4.2) compared

to people scoring low on dispositional self-control (M= 3.5) (t= -8.65; p< .01). Hence, H1

received full support.

To test H2 and H3, individuals were assigned a left or right-side and a bottom or upper-side

motive position by performing a median split on the variables representing the respondents’

co-ordinates on respectively the interpersonal and intrapersonal axis of the general motive

taxonomy20 (median= 0.7 for the score on the horizontal axis; M left-side position= -0.5, M

right-side position= 1.9; median= -0.7 for the score on the vertical axis; M bottom-side

20
As in the previous study, co-ordinates on the interpersonal (intrapersonal) motive axes were computed by subtracting the summed scores
for Vitality and Recognition (Recognition and Security) from the summed scores for Conviviality and Security (Vitality and Conviviality),
with higher co-ordinates indicating a more right-side (upper-side) position (e.g. Feather, 1995).

162
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

position= -2.2, M upper-side position= 1.4). Generally, the results of a 2 (position horizontal

axis: left/right) X 2 (position vertical axis: bottom/upper) MANOVA analysis with

dispositional self-control (H2) and disciplined driving behavior (H3) as dependent variables

revealed a significant main effect of both position on the horizontal axis (F(2, 168)= 12.90,

p< .01) and position on the vertical axis (F(2, 168)= 29.34, p< .01), as well as a significant

interaction effect (F(2, 168)= 3.29, p< .05).

In support of H2, for dispositional self-control only the main effect of position on the vertical

axis appeared to be significant (F(1, 169)= 52.01, p< .01), with bottom-side respondents, i.e.

people driven by underlying motives of Recognition or Security scoring significantly higher

on dispositional self-control (M= 3.9) compared to upper-side respondents, i.e. people driven

by underlying motives of Vitality or Conviviality (M= 3.3). In line with the non-significant

interaction effect (F(1, 169)= 0.51, p> .05) (see Fig. 5.5), simple main effects tests indicated

no significant differences in dispositional self-control between Recognition (i.e. bottom-left

position; M= 3.8) and Security-driven people (i.e. bottom-right position; M= 3.9) (F(1, 88)=

0.34, p> .05), as well as between Vitality (i.e. upper-left position; M= 3.3) and Conviviality-

driven people (i.e. upper-right position; M= 3.2) (F(1, 81)= 0.19, p> .05).

Figure 5.5 : Study 2 – MANOVA results : Dispositional Self-Control

5,0 Position
4,5 horizontal
motive axis
Dispositional

4,0 3,9
Self-control

3,8
3,5 Left
3,3
3,2
3,0 Right
2,5
2,0
1,5
1,0
Bottom Upper
Position vertical motive axis

163
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

With regard to disciplined driving behavior both position on the vertical axis (F(1, 169)=

36.13, p< .01) and position on the horizontal axis (F(1, 169)= 19.22, p< .01) accounted for

significant main effects. Bottom-side respondents, i.e. people driven by underlying motives

of Recognition or Security reported significantly higher levels of disciplined driving behavior

(M= 4.1) compared to upper-side respondents, i.e. people driven by underlying motives of

Vitality or Conviviality (M= 3.5), and so did right-side respondents, i.e. people driven by

underlying motives of Conviviality or Security (M= 4.1) as compared to left-side respondents,

i.e. people driven by underlying motives of Vitality or Recognition (M= 3.6). More important

however, also a significant two-way interaction effect could be detected (F(1, 169)= 6.17,

p< .05) (see Fig. 5.6). Simple main effects tests revealed that Security-driven people (i.e.

bottom-right position; M= 4.4) scored significantly higher on disciplined driving behavior

than Recognition-driven people (i.e. bottom-left position; M= 3.7) (F(1, 88)= 23.79, p< .01),

but that disciplined driving behavior between Vitality (i.e. upper-left position; M= 3.4) and

Conviviality-driven people (i.e. upper-right position; M= 3.5) did not differ (F(1, 81)= 1.81,

p> .05). Hence, H3 was also fully supported.

Figure 5.6 : Study 2 – MANOVA results : Disciplined Driving Behavior

5,0 Position
Disciplined driving

4,5 4,4 horizontal


4,0 motive axis
3,7
behavior

3,5 3,5
3,4 Left
3,0 Right
2,5
2,0
1,5
1,0
Bottom Upper
Position vertical motive axis

164
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

4.4. Discussion

In this second study, the intervening role of consumer motives in trait-behavior relationships

in personality research was investigated within a different research context. Whereas study 1

was undertaken in the specific consumer context of adopting new-product innovations, study

2 comprised a more general situation of driving one’s car in accordance to public norms and

prescriptions. Despite these contextual differences, the results of the second study basically

confirmed and supported our previous findings. In general, a significant positive relationship

could be detected between the trait self-control and normative car driving, as people high in

dispositional self-control reported more disciplined driving behavior compared to people low

in self-control (H1). However, as expected, significant differences were found among people

with high dispositional self-control according to the manifest consumer motives. Though

dispositional self-control appeared to be related equally high to underlying motives of

Recognition and Security (H2), disciplined driving behavior as a resulting outcome became

more prominent among Security-driven people (H3), as this kind of normative behavior was

more compatible with the manifest motive orientation of these people. Hence, based on these

findings of study 2, additional evidence is provided for the role of consumer motives as

intervening variables in the relationship between personality traits and several aspects of

(consumer) behavior. In line with the compatibility principle established in motive-literature,

actual behavioral outcomes of specific individual difference variables are shown to be

dependent on the alignment with the particular consumer motivations that are active within a

given product context.

165
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

In the remaining parts of this paper some general conclusions and implications are formulated

concerning the findings of the previous studies and an outline is provided of the limitations of

the studies and the directions for further research.

5. General conclusions and discussion

Previous studies have addressed the advantage of the use of contingency models in

personality research, taking into account the combined influence of personality traits and

other personal and situational factors on (consumer) behavior (e.g. Dickson, 1982; Van

Kenhove et al., 1999). Against this background, present research investigated the impact of

underlying consumer motives on trait-behavior relationships within different research

contexts. In a first study the intervening role of one’s underlying motive orientations was

analyzed in the context of the relationship between innate innovativeness and consumer

adoption behavior for innovative mobile phone product attributes. Given its obvious link with

new-product adoption behavior (e.g. Midgley and Dowling, 1993), consumer innovativeness

as a personality trait could be considered as having direct relevance to the study of consumer

behavior. Mobile phones were chosen as the topic of investigation due to the rather high-

involvement nature of this particular product class21 and the number of different innovations it

was subjected to during the time of research. Yet another personality characteristic seemingly

having much to offer researchers interested in consumer behavior is the trait of human self-

control, as processes that undermine the capacity for self-control should lead to less

disciplined and more impulsive behavior (e.g. Baumeister, 2002). Hence, a second study was

performed investigating the role of consumer motives as intervening variables in the

relationship between dispositional self-control and people’s disciplined driving behavior.

21
Significant relationships between innate innovativeness and new-product adoption behavior were found to be more prominent in product
categories were involvement is high (e.g. software, electronics) than in product categories were involvement is low (e.g. food) (e.g. Foxall,
1995).

166
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

Though the particular research context in which study 2 was conducted (i.e. driving one’s car

in accordance to public norms and prescriptions) might be less related to consumers’ behavior

in the marketplace in a strict sense, the found results may contribute to the study of trait-

behavior relationships in a social marketing context (e.g. promoting traffic safety).

In general, the results of the two studies above supported our basic proposition that actual

behavioral outcomes of human personality traits might be dependent on the alignment with

the particular consumer motives that are active within a given product context. In study 1, we

found a significant positive relationship existing between innate innovativeness and consumer

adoption behavior for innovative mobile phone product attributes (i.e. H1a/H1b). However,

significant differences were detected among high-innovative consumers, i.e. consumers

driven by underlying motives of Vitality or Recognition (i.e. H2), in either adopting utilitarian

or hedonic innovative attributes. In line with the general principle of compatibility in motive-

literature (e.g. Higgins, 2002), Vitality-driven consumers exhibited higher adoption levels for

hedonic innovative mobile phone attributes (i.e. H3), whereas adoption of utilitarian

innovative mobile phone attributes was more prominent among Recognition-driven

consumers (i.e. H4). In the same way, the results of study 2 suggested a significant positive

relationship between dispositional self-control and disciplined driving behavior, as people

high in dispositional self-control reported more disciplined driving behavior compared to

people low in self-control (i.e. H1). Here again, significant differences were found among

people with high dispositional self-control, i.e. people driven by underlying motives of

Recognition or Security (i.e. H2), according to the manifest consumer motives. In particular,

disciplined driving behavior as an outcome of human self-control appeared to be more

prominent among Security-driven people (i.e. H3), as this kind of normative behavior was

more compatible with the manifest motive orientation of these people. Taking into account

167
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

the motivational make-up of Recognition-driven people, the latter subjects seem to be likely

to be predisposed to disciplined behavior with a more individualistic character (e.g. job

performance). However, this proposition was not explicitly tested in the present research and,

for instance, should be incorporated in future research about this issue. In sum, the findings

of study 1 and study 2 provided preliminary evidence for the intervening impact of consumer

motives on the relationship between human personality and (consumer) behavior. A

dispositional personality trait was found to give rise to a specific behavior, especially when

that behavior aligned with particular manifest consumer motives. In contrast, when less

compatibility could be detected between the behavior in question and underlying motive

orientations, high levels of the personality trait were less likely to result into the behavioral

outcome.

With these findings the present research significantly contributes to an improved

understanding of trait-behavior relationships in personality research. Whereas in prior

research situational factors (e.g. time constraints, interpersonal interactions, task definition,

etc.) were mainly considered as intervening variables in the relationship between personality

traits and several aspects of (consumer) behavior (e.g. Belk, 1975), the research reported here

offers additional insights on how more person-related variables, i.e. underlying motive

dispositions impact trait-behavior relationships in predictable ways. Though personality traits

and consumer motives separately might be of only modest value when one is attempting to

predict (consumer) behavior, taking into consideration the combined influence of both these

variables is likely to give rise to more promising results.

Based on the conclusions drawn from the present research, one important implication could

be considered in the context of consumer-oriented market segmentation. As suggested in

168
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

several studies, the past focus on personality traits alone to effectively segment consumer

markets is truly outmoded. Hence, many theorists no longer study personality factors in

complete isolation but rather focus on the person within specific situations (e.g. Mischel, 1977;

McDonald and Goldman, 1980). The thrust of present research is that also underlying

consumer motives could be considered as useful bases for segmentation in addition to human

personality traits. Especially, the use of a segmentation framework that incorporates both

personality variables and people’s motive orientations in combination will produce a much

richer description and understanding of target markets to our opinion.

6. Limitations and future research

Several limitations concerning this research are appropriate to notice. First, only two specific

research contexts were considered to investigate the impact of underlying consumer motives

on trait-behavior relationships in personality research. Though our basic propositions were

consistently supported across these two studies, the generalizability of the findings could be

enhanced by performing additional studies, validating the results for different personality

traits and different behavioral outcomes.

Second, in the present research consumer motives were assessed by means of the 34-item

Consumer Motive Scale developed by Geeroms et al. (2005). As this measurement

instrument performed rather well in earlier motivational studies (cf. supra: chapter IV), in the

present research, composite construct measures could not be computed for all eight motive

domains due to unsatisfactory Cronbach’s α coefficients. Hence, reliable motive constructs

had to be created based on combinations of items of adjacent positions. Given this lack of

169
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

measurement invariance across different studies, future research should be conducted that

attempts to improve the general psychometric stability of the Consumer Motive Scale.

A third limitation is related to the measurement of new-product adoption behavior in study 1,

by asking respondents about their purchase intentions with regard to one single product (i.e. a

mobile phone with specific innovative attributes). Though measuring purchase intentions

could be considered as one of the main techniques to assess actualized innovativeness (e.g.

Midgley and Dowling, 1978), more often in literature cross-sectional methods are proposed

for this purpose, i.e. measuring the number of new products owned in a particular product

category (e.g. electronics). The main benefits of cross-sectional measures might be that actual

actions are considered instead of intentions, which may not reflect true behavior and that the

results for a whole product category are less situation specific than for a single product (e.g.

Im et al., 2003).

In addition, in study 1, a distinction between hedonic and utilitarian innovative mobile phone

product attributes was made based on a factor analysis performed on a list of eight innovative

attributes. As indicated, the results of this analysis revealed two broad factors that could

easily be interpreted as respectively hedonic and utilitarian in nature. However, future

research could also determine the utilitarian-hedonic nature of products or attributes a priori,

by assessing respondents’ actual perceptions of these products/attributes (e.g. Venkatraman

and Price, 1990). By doing so, stronger evidence is provided that the desired representation

of utilitarian and hedonic products/attributes was actually obtained than by only relying on the

researcher’s subjective interpretation of empirically detected factors.

170
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

Finally, as the main purpose of present research was to provide basic support for the

proposition that behavioral outcomes of human personality traits might be function of the

alignment with the manifest consumer motives within a given product context, the predictive

utility of contingency models of personality that take into account this intervening role of

one’s underlying motive dispositions, was not explicitly assessed. However, as noticed

before, such models should have the potential to account for considerable more variance in

(consumer) behavior than the 10 percent explained by trait-behavioral models as such. Hence,

the authors propose that future research studies should be established that examine this

predictive ability more closely.

171
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to Isabel Verniers and Deborah Voets for their help with the data

collection of study 1 and study 2 respectively.

172
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

References

Aaker, J.L., & Lee, A.Y. (2001). I seek pleasures and we avoid pains: the role of self-

regulatory goals in information processing and persuasion. Journal of Consumer

Research, 28(1), 33-49.

Angeletos, G-M., Laibson, D., Repetto, A., Tobacman, J., & Weinberg, S. (2001). The

hyperbolic consumption model: calibration, simulation and empirical evaluation.

Journal of Economic Perspectives, 15(3), 47-68.

Arnould, E., Price, L., & Zinkhan, G. (2004). Consumers. New York: McGraw-Hill Book

Co.

Babin, B.J., Darden, W.R., & Griffin, M. (1994). Work and/or fun: measuring hedonic and

utilitarian shopping value. Journal of Consumer Research, 20, 644-656.

Bagozzi, R.P. (1994). ACR fellow speech. Advances in Consumer Research, 21(1), 8-11.

Baumeister, R.F. (1998). The self. In D.T. Gilbert, S.T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.).

Handbook of social psychology (4th ed.; pp. 680-740). New York: McGraw-Hill.

Baumeister, R.F. (2002). Yielding to temptation: self-control failure, impulsive purchasing,

and consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 28(march), 670-676.

Baumeister, R.F. (2003). Ego-depletion and self-regulation failure: a resource model of self-

control. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 27(2), 281-284.

Baumeister, R.F., & Exline, J.J. (1999). Virtue, personality and social relations: self-control

as the moral muscle. Journal of Personality, 67(6), 1165-1194.

Baumeister, R.F., & Exline, J.J. (2000). Self-control, morality, and human strength. Journal

of Social and Clinical Psychology, 19(1), 29-42.

Baumeister, R.F., & Heatherton, T.F. (1996). Self-regulation failure: an overview.

Psychological Inquiry, 7(1), 1-15.

173
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Baumeister, R.F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D.M. (1998). Ego depletion: is the

active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5),

1252-1265.

Baumeister, R.F., Heatherton, T.F., & Tice, D.M. (1994). Losing control: How and why

people fail at self-regulation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Baumeister, R.F., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. (2000). Ego depletion: a resource model of

volition, self-regulation and controlled processing. Social Cognition, 18(2), 130-150.

Baumgartner, H. (2002). Toward a personology of the consumer. Journal of Consumer

Research, 29, 286-292.

Bearden, W.O., Calcich, S.E., Netemeyer, R., & Teel, J.E. (1986). An exploratory

investigation of consumer innovativeness and interpersonal influences. Advances in

Consumer Research, 13, 77-82.

Belk, R.W. (1975). Situational variables and consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer

research, 2, 157-164.

Bruyneel, S., Dewitte, S., Vohs, K.D., & Warlop, L. (2006). Repeated choosing increases

susceptibility to affective product features. International Journal of Research in

Marketing, 23(2), 215-225.

Burns, D.J., & Krampf, R.F. (1991). A semiotic perspective on innovative behavior.

Developments in marketing science. 15th Annual Conference, Academy of Marketing

Science, 14, 32-35.

Callebaut, J., Janssens, M., Op de Beeck, D., Lorré, D., & Hendrickx, H. (1999).

Motivational marketing research revisited. Leuven-Apeldoorn: Garant.

174
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

Carlson, L., & Grossbart, S.L. (1984). Toward a better understanding of inherent

innovativeness. In W.B. Russel and A.P. Robert (Eds.), Proceedings of the A.M.A.

educator’s conference (p. 88-91). Chicago: American Marketing Association.

Cattell, R.B. (1950). Personality: a systematic, theoretical and factual study. New York:

McGraw-Hill Book Co.

Chernev, A. (2004). Goal-attribute compatibility in consumer choice. Journal of Consumer

Psychology, 14(1/2), 141-150.

Citrin, A.V., Sprott, D.E., Silverman, S.N., & Stem, D.E. (2000). Adoption of internet

shopping: the role of consumer innovativeness. Industrial Management and Data

Systems, 100(July), 294-300.

Cook, M., Young, A., Taylor, D., & Bedford, A.P. (1998). Personality correlates of alcohol

consumption. Personality and Individual Differences, 4, 641-647.

Cotte, J., & Wood, S.L. (2004). Families and innovative consumer behavior: a triadic

analysis of sibling and parental influence. Journal of Consumer Research, 31(June),

78-86.

Craig, S.C., & Ginter, J.L. (1975). An empirical test of a scale for innovativeness. Advances

in Consumer Research, 2, 555-562.

Dhar, R., & Wertenbroch, K. (2000). Consumer choice between hedonic and utilitarian

goods. Journal of Marketing Research, 37, 60-71.

Dickson, P.R. (1982). Person-situation: segmentation’s missing link. Journal of Marketing,

46, 56-64.

Etzel, M.J., Donnelly, J.H., & Ivancevich, J.M. (1976). Social character and consumer

innovativeness. Journal of Social Psychology, 100, 153-154.

175
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Feldman, S.C., Martinez-Pons, M., & Shaham, D. (1995). The relationship of self-efficacy,

self-regulation and collaborative verbal behavior with grades: preliminary findings.

Psychological Reports, 77, 971-978.

Feather, N.T. (1995). Values, valences and choice: the influence of values on the perceived

attractiveness and choice of alternatives. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 68(6), 1135-1151.

Ford, M.E., & Nichols, C.W. (1987). A taxonomy of human goals and some possible

applications. In M.E. Ford, & D.H. Ford (Eds.), Humans as Self-constructing

Systems: Putting the Framework to work. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Foxall, G., & Haskins, C.G. (2001). Cognitive style and consumer innovativeness: an

empirical test of Kirton’s adaption-innovation theory in the context of food

purchasing. European Journal of Marketing, 20(3/4), 63-80.

Foxall, G.R. (1988). Consumer innovativeness: novelty-seeking, creativity and cognitive

style. Research in Consumer Behavior, 3, 79-113.

Foxall, G.R. (1995). Cognitive styles of consumer initiators. Technovation, 15(5), 269-288.

Foxall, G.R., & Bhate, S. (1993). Cognitive styles and personal involvement of market

initiators for healthy food brands: implications for adoption theory. Journal of

Economic Psychology, 14, 33-56.

Foxall, G.R., & Goldsmith, R.E. (1988). Personality and consumer research: another look.

Journal of the Market Research Society, 30(2), 111-125.

Gatignon, H., & Robertson, T.S. (1985). A propositional inventory for new diffusion

research. Journal of Consumer Research, 11(4), 849-867.

Geeroms, N., Vermeir, I., Van Kenhove, P., & Hendrickx, H. (2005). A Taxonomy of

Consumer Motives through Preferred Brand Personality: Empirical Findings for 11

countries. Working Paper, Ghent University, Department of Marketing.

176
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

Goldsmith, R.E., & Hofacker, C.F. (1991). Measuring consumer innovativeness. Journal of

the Academy of Marketing Science, 19(Summer), 1004-1016.

Goldsmith, R.E., Freiden, J.B., & Eastman, J.K. (1995). The generality/specificity issue in

consumer innovativeness research. Technovation, 15(10), 601-611.

Goldsmith, R.E., Kim, D., Flynn, L.R., & Kim, W.M. (2005). Price sensivity and

innovativeness for fashion among Korean consumers. Journal of Social Psychology,

145(5), 501-508.

Gottfredson, M.R., & Hirschi, T. (1990). A general theory of crime. Stanford, CA: Stanford

University Press.

Grewal, R., Mehta, R., & Kardes, F.R. (2000). The role of the social-identity function of

attitudes in consumer innovativeness and opinion leadership. Journal of Economic

Psychology, 21, 233-252.

Guilford, J.P. (1959). Personality. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Co.

Haugtvedt, C.P., Petty, R.E., & Cacioppo, J.T. (1992). Need for cognition and advertising:

understanding the role of personality variables in consumer behavior. Journal of

Consumer psychology, 3(1), 239-260.

Higgins, E.T. (2002). How self-regulation creates distinct values: the case of promotion and

prevention decision-making. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 12(3), 177-191.

Hirschman, E.C. (1980). Innovativeness, novelty seeking and consumer creativity. Journal

of Consumer Research, 7(dec), 283-295.

Hirschman, E.C., & Holbrook, M.B. (1982). Hedonic consumption: emerging concepts,

methods and propositions. Journal of Marketing, 46(3), 92-101.

Hirschman, E.C., & Stern, B.B. (1998). Consumer behavior and the wayward mind: the

influence of mania and depression on consumption. Advances in Consumer Research,

25, 421-427.

177
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Hoch, S.J., & Loewenstein, G.F. (1991). Time-inconsistent preferences and consumer self-

control. Journal of Consumer Research, 17(march), 492-507.

Huffman, C., Ratneshwar, S., & Mick, D.G. (2000). Consumer goal structures and goal-

determination processes. In S. Ratneshwar and D.G. Mick (Eds.), The why of

consumption (p. 9-36). New York: Routledge.

Im, S., Bayus, B.L., & Mason, C.H. (2003). An empirical study of innate consumer

innovativeness, personal characteristics and new-product adoption behavior. Journal

of the Academy of Marketing Science, 31(1), 61-73.

Jacoby, J. (1971). Multiple-indicant approaches for studying new product adopters. Journal

of Applied Psychology, 55, 384-388.

Jensen-Campbell, L.A., & Graziano, W.G. (2005). The two faces of temptation: differing

motives for self-control. Merrill-palmer Quarterly, 51(3), 287-314.

Joseph, B., & Vyas, S.J. (1984). Concurrent validity of a measure of innovative cognitive

style. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 12(2), 159-175.

Kacen, J.J., & Lee, J.A. (2002). The influence of culture on consumer impulsive buying

behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 12(2), 163-176.

Karlsson, N. (2003). Consumer self-control strategies: an empirical study of their structure

and determinants. Journal of Consumer Policy, 26(1), 23-41.

Kassarjian, H.H. (1971). Personality and consumer behavior: a review. Journal of Marketing

Research, 8(Nov), 409-418.

Kassarjian, H.H., & Sheffet, M.J. (1991). Personality and consumer behavior: an update. In

H.H. Kassarjian and T.S. Robertson (Eds.), Perspectives in consumer behavior (p.

281-303). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Keller, K.L. (1993). Conceptualizing, measuring and managing customer-based brand equity.

Journal of marketing, 57, 1-22.

178
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

Kim, J-Y. (2006). Hyperbolic discounting and the repeated self-control problem. Journal of

Economic Psychology, 27, 344-359.

Kirton, M.J. (1976). Adaptors and innovators: a description and measure. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 61(5), 622-629.

Kochanska, G., Murray, K.T., & Harlan, E.T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood:

continuity and change, antecedents and implications for social development.

Developmental Psychology, 36, 220-232.

Krueger, R.F., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T.E., White, J., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1996). Delay of

gratification, psychopathology and personality: is low self-control specific to

externalizing problems? Journal of Personality, 64, 107-129.

Lastovicka, J.L., & Joachimsthaler, E.A. (1988). Improving the detection of personality-

behavior relationships in consumer research. Journal of Consumer Research,

14(March), 583-587.

Leith, K.P., & Baumeister, R.F. (1998). Empathy, shame, guilt and narratives of

interpersonal conflicts: guilt-prone people are better at perspective taking. Journal of

Personality, 66, 1-37.

Limayem, M., Khalifa, M., & Frini, A. (2000). What makes consumers buy from internet? A

longitudinal study of online shopping. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man and

Cybernetics - Part A: Systems and Humans, 30(4), 421-432.

Manning, K.C., Bearden, W.O., & Madden, T.J. (1995). Consumer innovativeness and the

adoption process. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 4(4), 329-345.

McClelland, D.C. (1980). Motive dispositions: the merits of operant and respondent

measures. In L. Wheeler (Ed.), Review of Personality and Social Psychology (Vol. 1,

p. 10-41). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

McClelland, D.C. (1985). Human motivation. Glenview, IL: Scott Foresman and Company.

179
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

McDonald, S.S., & Goldman, A.E. (1980). Strategies of segmentation research. In G.B.

Hafer (Ed.), A look back, a look ahead (p. 30-42). Chicago: American Marketing

Association.

Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool system analysis of delay of gratification:

dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106, 3-19.

Midgley, D. (1977). Innovation and new product marketing. Londres: Croom Helm.

Midgley, D.F., & Dowling, G.R. (1978). Innovativeness: the concept and its measurement.

Journal of Consumer Research, 4(March), 229-242.

Midgley, D.F., & Dowling, G.R. (1993). A longitudinal study of product form innovation:

the interaction between predispositions and social messages. Journal of Consumer

Research, 19(4), 611-625.

Miller, K.E., & Ginter, J.L. (1979). An investigation of situational variation in brand choice

and attitude. Journal of Marketing Research, 16, 111-123.

Mischel, W. (1977). On the future of personality measurement. American Psychologist, 32,

246-254.

Mowen, J.C. & Minor, M. (1997). Consumer behavior. Fifth Edition, Prentice Hall.

Mudd, S. (1990). The place of innovativeness in models of the adoption process: an

integrative review. Technovation, 10(2), 119-134.

Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R.F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources:

does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126(2), 247-259.

Muraven, M., & Shmueli, D. (2006). The self-control costs of fighting the temptation to

drink. Psychology of Addictive behaviors, 20(2), 154-160.

Muraven, M., Baumeister, R.F., & Tice, D.M. (1999). Longitudinal improvement of self-

regulation through practice: building self-control strength through repeated exercise.

Journal of Social Psychology, 139(4), 446-457.

180
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

NIS (2005). Private Households Statistics. Brussels: NIS, National Institute for Statistics.

O’Donoghue, T., & Rabin, M. (2000). The economics of immediate gratification. Journal of

Behavioral Decision Making, 13, 233-250.

O’Gorman, J.G., & Baxter, E. (2002). Self-control as a personality measure. Personality and

Individual Differences, 32, 533-539.

Olson, K.R. (2005). Engagement and self-control: superordinate dimensions of Big Five

traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 1689-1700.

Park, C.W., & Smith, D.C. (1989). Product-level choice: a top-down or bottom-up process?

Journal of Consumer Research, 16(3), 289-299.

Park, K., & Dyer, C.L. (1995). Consumer use innovative behavior: an approach toward its

causes. Advances in Consumer Research, 22, 566-572.

Peluso, T., Ricciardelli, L.A., & Williams, R.J. (1999). Self-control in relation to problem

drinking and symptoms of disordered eating. Addictive Behaviors, 24, 439-442.

Pham, M., & Higgins, E.T. (2005). Promotion and prevention in consumer decision-making.

In S. Ratneshwar and D.G. Mick (Eds.), Inside consumption (p. 8-43). New York:

Routledge.

Pizzam, A. (1972). Psychological characteristics of innovators. European Journal of

Marketing, 6(3), 203-210.

Price, L.L., & Ridgway, N.M. (1983). Development of a scale to measure use innovativeness.

Advances in Consumer Research, 10, 679-684.

Roehrich, G. (2004). Consumer innovativeness: concepts and measurements. Journal of

Business Research, 57, 671-677.

Rogers, E.M. (1983). Diffusion of innovations. New York (NY): The Free Press.

Rogers, E.M., & Shoemaker, F.F. (1971). Communication of innovations. New York (NY):

The Free Press.

181
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Rook, D.W. (1987). The buying impulse. Journal of Consumer Research, 14(2), 189-199.

Skinner, E.A. (1996). A guide to constructs of control. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 71(3), 549-570.

Slessareva, E., & Muraven, M. (2004). Sensitivity to punishment and self-control: the

mediating role of emotion. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 307-319.

Stammerjohan, C., & Webster, C. (2002). Trait and situational antecedents to non-

consumption. Advances in Consumer Research, 29, 126-132.

Steenkamp, J.B., & Baumgartner, H. (1992). The role of optimum stimulation level in

exploratory consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 19(3), 434-448.

Tangney, J.P., & Baumeister, R.F. (2001). High self-control predicts good adjustment, less

pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Unpublished manuscript,

Department of Psychology, George Mason University, Fairfax, VA 22030.

Tangney, J.P., Baumeister, R.F., & Boone, A.L. (2004). High self-control predicts good

adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of

Personality, 72(2), 271-324.

Thaler, R.H., & Shefrin, H.M. (1981). An economic theory of self-control. Journal of

Political Economy, 89(2), 392-406.

Tice, D.M., Bratslavsky, E., & Baumeister, R.F. (2001). Emotional distress regulation takes

precedence over impulse control: if you feel bad, do it! Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 80(1), 53-67.

Van Kenhove, P., De Wulf, K., & Van Waterschoot, W. (1999). The impact of task-

definition on store-attribute saliences and store choice. Journal of Retailing, 75(1),

125-137.

Venkatraman, M.P. (1991). The impact of innovativeness and innovation type on adoption.

Journal of Retailing, 67(1), 51-67.

182
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

Venkatraman, M.P., & MacInnis, D.J. (1985). The epistemic and sensory exploratory

behaviors of hedonic and cognitive consumers. Advances in Consumer Research, 12 ,

102-107.

Venkatraman, M.P., & Price, L.L. (1990). Differentiating between cognitive and sensory

innovativeness: concepts, measurement and implications. Journal of Business

Research, 20, 293-315.

Wahlers, R.G., Dunn, M.G., & Etzel, M.J. (1986). The convergence of alternative OSL

measures with consumer exploratory behavior tendencies. Advances in Consumer

Research, 13, 398-402.

Wells, W.D., & Beard, A.D. (1973). Personality and consumer behavior. In S. Ward and

T.S. Robertson (Eds.), Consumer behavior: theoretical sources (p. 141-199). New

Jersey: Prentice Hall, Inc.

Wertenbroch, K. (1998). Consumption self-control by rationing purchase quantities of virtue

and vice. Marketing Science, 17(4), 317-337.

Winter, D.G., John, O.P., Stewart, A.J., Klohnen, E.C., & Duncan, L.E. (1998). Traits and

motives: toward an integration of two traditions in personality research.

Psychological Review, 105(2), 230-250.

Wolfe, R.N., & Johnson, S.D. (1995). Personality as a predictor of college performance.

Educational and Psychological Measurement, 55, 177-185.

Wood, S.L., & Swait, J. (2002). Psychological indicators of innovation adoption: cross-

classification based on need for cognition and need for change. Journal of Consumer

Psychology, 12(1), 1-13.

Youn, S., & Faber, R.J. (2000). Impulse buying: its relation to personality traits and cues.

Advances in Consumer Research, 27, 179-185.

183
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Zuckerman, M.D. (1979). Sensation seeking: beyond the optimal level of arousal. Hillsdale,

NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates

184
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

Appendix A

• List of scale items

185
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Appendix A: List of scale items

Consumer Innovativeness Scale (Manning et al., 1995)

CIJM items
1. Prior to purchasing a new brand, I prefer to consult a friend that has experience with the new brand.
2. When it comes to deciding whether to purchase a new service, I do not rely on experienced friends or
family members for advice.
3. I seldom ask a friend about his or her experiences with a new product before I buy the new product.
4. I decide to buy new products or services without relying on the opinions of friends who have already
tried them.
5. When I am interested in purchasing a new service, I do not rely on my friends or close acquaintances
that have already used the new service to give me information as to whether I should try it.
6. I do not rely on experienced friends for information about new products prior to making up my mind
about whether or not to purchase.
CNS items
1. I often seek out information about new products and brands
2. I like to go to places where I will be exposed to information about new products and brands.
3. I like magazines that introduce new brands.
4. I frequently look for new products and services.
5. I seek out situations in which I will be exposed to new and different sources of product information.
6. I am continually seeking new product experiences.
7. When I go shopping, I find myself spending very little time checking out new products and brands.
8. I take advantage of the first available opportunity to find out about new and different products.

Trait Self-Control (TSC) Scale (Tangney et al., 2004)

1. I have a hard time breaking bad habits.


2. I am lazy.
3. I say inappropriate things.
4. I never allow myself to lose control.
5. I do certain things that are bad for me, if they are fun.
6. People can count on me to keep on schedule.
7. Getting up in the morning is hard for me.
8. I have trouble saying no.
9. I change my mind fairly often.
10. I blurt out whatever is on my mind.
11. People would describe me as impulsive.
12. I refuse things that are bad for me.
13. I spend too much money.
14. I keep everything neat.
15. I am self-indulgent at times.
16. I wish I had more self-discipline.
17. I am good at resisting temptation.
18. I get carried away by my feelings.
19. I do many things at the spur of the moment.
20. I don’t keep secrets very well.
21. People would say that I have iron self-discipline.
22. I have worked or studied all night at the last minute.
23. I’m not easily discouraged.
24. I’d be better off if I stopped to think before acting.
25. I engage in healthy practices.
26. I eat healthy foods.
27. Pleasure and fun sometimes keep me from getting work done.
28. I have trouble concentrating.
29. I am able to work effectively toward long-term goals.
30. Sometimes I can’t stop myself from doing something, even if I know it’s wrong.

186
Chapter V : Consumer Motives and Personality Traits

31. I often act without thinking through all the alternatives.


32. I lose my temper too easily.
33. I often interrupt people.
34. I sometimes drink or use drugs to excess.
35. I am always on time.
36. I am reliable.

187
A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives : Practical Applications

CHAPTER VI

HEALTH ADVERTISING TO PROMOTE FRUIT AND VEGETABLE

INTAKE : APPLICATION OF MOTIVE-RELATED HEALTH

AUDIENCE SEGMENTATION

189
Chapter VI

Health Advertising to promote Fruit and Vegetable Intake:

Application of motive-related Health Audience Segmentation

1. Abstract

Given the importance of meaningful audience segmentation in creating effective health

advertisements, this study proposes a new theory-driven segmentation approach based on

Callebaut et al.’s (1999) taxonomy of eight consumer motives. The authors demonstrate the

usefulness of this new segmentation paradigm in the process of developing appropriate health

advertisements in the context of fruit and vegetable consumption. Results of a two-step

cluster analysis reveal five different health segments to exist with different health-realted

motive orientations. Significant differences exist between these segments both with regard to

(category-specific) fruit and vegetable consumption and reactions toward fruit and vegetable

health advertisements. In general, a segment’s reactions toward appropriate motive-related

health advertising were significantly more positive than its reactions toward health

advertising that has a general character (i.e. not responsive to a segment’s underlying

motives). Based on these results some practical suggestions and recommendations are

offered for health communicators to use when developing motive-related health

advertisements to audiences with specific health-related motive orientations.

191
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

2. Introduction

Consumption of diets rich in fruits and vegetables is important for attaining and maintaining a

good health (e.g. Cox et al., 1996; Steinmetz and Potter, 1996; Van Duyn and Pivonka, 2000;

Hu et al., 2000; Li et al., 2000; Lambert, 2001). Therefore, the World Health Organization

(WHO, 1990) recommends a daily intake of at least 400g of vegetables and fruits, which is

equivalent to about five 80g portions of fruits and vegetables per day (Williams, 1995).

Nevertheless, statistical data show that the consumption levels of fresh fruits and vegetables

remain substantially below this recommended level in many countries (e.g. Thompson et al.,

1999; Naska et al., 2000; Ling and Horwath, 2001; Rurik and Antal, 2003; Vereecken et al.,

2004).

To promote compliance with the “Five-a-day” recommendation, several nation-wide public

health campaigns have been established (French et al., 2001), with varying results. Whereas

some of the interventions actually did achieve the meant objectives (e.g. Dixon et al., 1998;

French and Stables, 2003), others were less successful in modifying eating behavior (e.g.

Donohew, 1990; Weaver et al., 1999; Ringold, 2002; Glanz and Yaroch, 2004; Safley et al.,

2004).

As mentioned by several authors, this inconsistent success of previous public health

campaigns might be due to the fact that some of these interventions disregarded the possibility

that the total population consists of a number of smaller sub-groups, with distinct need or

motive patterns that should be taken into account (e.g. Albrecht and Bryant, 1996; Pollard et

al., 2002; Verbeke, 2005). As posited by these authors, marketing health information that is

insufficiently tailored or personally relevant and does not target specific needs or motives

192
Chapter VI : Consumer Motives and Health Advertising

risks to be ineffective, as it is not appropriate for or does not appeal to particular target

audiences (e.g. Forthofer and Bryant, 2000; Fennis, 2003; Rapley and Coulson, 2005). To be

effective, persuasive messages should be developed that are responsive to consumer needs or

motives (e.g. Brehony et al., 1984; Lefebvre and Flora, 1988; Hastings and Haywood, 1994).

Hence, also in social marketing areas, the real challenge lies in identifying and effectively

reaching particular audience segments (Atkin and Freimuth, 1989; Slater and Flora, 1994;

Freimuth et al., 2001; Grier and Bryant, 2005; Boslaugh et al., 2005).

As demographic or geo-demographic homogeneity alone provides precious little help in

constructing tailored health advertisements (e.g. Slater 1995), this study takes a more

sophisticated approach by outlining a new segmentation paradigm based on audience

members’ health-related motive orientations, i.e. relatively enduring beliefs that people hold

about the fundamental meaning of health. As health beliefs have been identified as important

determinants of overall food behavior patterns (e.g. Steptoe et al., 1995), it can reasonably be

assumed that they will also influence consumer behavior for fruits and vegetables. Hence,

health-related motive orientations could be considered as relevant segmentation variables in

the context of fruit and vegetable consumption. As a theoretical framework to assess people’s

health-related motive orientations Callebaut et al.’s (1999) taxonomy of consumer motives

was applied in the present study (cf. infra: section 3.1).

After identifying consumer segments with different health-related motive orientations and

assessing differences in fruit and vegetable consumption patterns among these segments, the

authors of this study will explore the effectiveness of different persuasive messages to

motivate each of the segments to behave healthier (in this case increasing fruit and vegetable

193
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

intake 22 ). It is implicitly assumed that different communication strategies should be

employed for consumers with different health-related motive orientations.

In the context of the present study, four motive-related printed health advertisements are

tested (and compared to a more general ad), each with a different message tonality

(informational vs. transformational) and directionality (self-directed arguments vs. other-

directed arguments). The objective is to determine which message tonality and directionality

fit best a particular segment or audience group’s health-related motives and thus, could be

recommended to use in persuasive messages designed to these segments.

In what follows, we first outline the theoretical background on which our hypotheses are built

and give an overview of the research method. Then, the empirical findings are presented,

followed by a general discussion and recommendations to health practitioners and

communicators. Finally, attention is paid to the limitations of the study and pathways for

future research are proposed.

3. Theoretical background and hypotheses

3.1. A general taxonomy of consumer motives23

In order to extend the heuristic value of theory in the applied context of audience

segmentation (see Silk et al. [2005] for a plea for theoretical strategies for audience

segmentation), we postulate Callebaut et al.’s (1999) taxonomy of consumer motives as the


22
Fruit and vegetable intake is assumed to be a relevant case to be investigated in the context of motive-related audience segmentation, as
generic cognitive approaches are less likely to be effective because of low consumer involvement with regard to fruit and vegetable
consumption (Verbeke & Pieniak., 2006). Moreover, a rich assortment of fruit and vegetable types is available to consumers – each with
different features and/or health-related benefits – that can be positioned in the context of different health-related motive orientations.
23
This section comprises a brief recapitulation of the major findings of chapter III of this dissertation, in order to refresh the reader’s mind.
For a full explanation of the origins of the general consumer motive taxonomy we thus refer to this corresponding chapter.

194
Chapter VI : Consumer Motives and Health Advertising

basis for our new segmentation approach. Consumer motives can be referred to as desirable

end-states of consumer behavior that people seek to attain (e.g. McClelland, 1985). Based on

empirical findings in 11 countries, Geeroms et al. (2005) validated the consumer motive

taxonomy of Callebaut et al. (1999) consisting of eight consumer motives, i.e. Vitality,

Pleasure, Conviviality, Belonging, Security, Control, Recognition and Power, that are at work

within every individual. In Table 6.1 an overview can be found of the basic definitions of all

of these motive domains.

Table 6.1 : Motive Definitions

Vitality : desire to experience adventure, test one’s boundaries and discover new things.

Pleasure : desire to follow impulses and emotions, to maximise pleasure without inhibition
or self-control.

Conviviality : desire to be with people, to open up socially, to share experiences and


emotionally connect with others.

Belonging : desire to merge into the group and to be accepted and supported by loved
ones.

Security : desire to hide away, to feel safe, relaxed, calm and be protected together with
an obedience to norms and regulations of the group.

Control : desire to suppress inner feelings and emotions to behave in a way that is
morally acceptable.

Recognition : desire to be recognized by others for one’s skills, knowledge and


appearance, to be respected by others.

Power : desire to be better than the rest, to feel superior, to be a leader. Higher priority is
given to individualistic goals and standards as opposed to group norms.

Source : Geeroms et al. (2005)

The relationships among the eight motive orientations can be summarized in terms of a

circumplex structure, determined by two basic, bipolar, conceptual dimensions, respectively

195
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

representing an intrapersonal and interpersonal motive axis (see Fig. 6.1). All these

motivational states could be considered as stable dispositions that become aroused by a

particular product environment (e.g. McClelland, 1980). Once aroused, the consumer motives

that are manifest in a given product context are likely to elicit some kind of motive-directed

behavior (e.g. Mowen and Minor, 1997). The motive taxonomy of Callebaut et al. (1999) has

been proven to be valid cross-culturally, seems compatible with prior classifications of human

motives (e.g. Ford and Nichols, 1987) and, in addition, has a number of advantages over these

previous motive taxonomies. Hence, this structure can be used as the basic theoretical

framework in the present study.

Figure 6.1 : Callebaut et al.’s (1999) Taxonomy of Consumer Motives

Source: Adapted from J. Callebaut et al., “Motivational marketing research revisited” (Leuven-Apeldoorn:
Garant, 1999)

196
Chapter VI : Consumer Motives and Health Advertising

3.2. Health-related motive orientations

As mentioned in the introduction, health-related beliefs or concerns are assumed to be

important when considering food behavior such as fruit and vegetable consumption (e.g.

Steptoe et al., 1995). In the majority of studies investigating food-health relationships to date,

a rather restricted conceptualization of health is applied, focusing on physical well-being only

(e.g. Rappoport et al., 1993). However, when applying Callebaut et al.’s (1999) taxonomy of

consumer motives in a health context, a broader conceptualization of health could be

proposed, also comprising psychological dimensions of health concern such as emotional

well-being, enjoying life or social responsibility. In alignment with the definitions of the

respective motive positions in the general taxonomy (i.e. Table 6.1), from an intrapersonal

point of view, health could be expected to be dealt with in an unrestricted, emotional way (i.e.

Pleasure motive) as opposed to a more controlled, functional way of dealing with health (i.e.

Control motive). When interpersonal motives are manifest, we expect an individualistic

interpretation of health to exist (i.e. Power motive) in contrast to an altruistic interpretation

(i.e. Belonging motive). Out of the dynamic interaction of the previous intrapersonal and

interpersonal ways of dealing with health, four different health audience segments could be

assumed to emerge with different health-related motive orientations, corresponding to the four

quadrants of the general motive taxonomy (see Fig. 6.1). Based on the respective motive

definitions of the four quadrantal positions, we derive the following specific hypotheses

regarding the fundamental meaning of health for each of these segments:

H1a: A Vitality segment will exist combining elements of Pleasure and Power-related

motive orientations, for which health is mainly about being active (e.g. practicing sports) and

having energy.

197
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

H1b: A Conviviality segment will exist combining elements of Pleasure and Belonging-

related motive orientations, for which health is mainly about emotional well-being, enjoying

life and feeling good socially.

H1c: A Security segment will exist combining elements of Control and Belonging-related

motive orientations, for which health is mainly about physical well-being, avoiding illness

and taking care of the health of others.

H1d: A Recognition segment will exist combining elements of Control and Power-related

motive orientations, for which health is mainly about managing outward appearance in the

best possible way (e.g. looking good, being slim).

3.3. Health-related motive orientations and fruit and vegetable consumption

As (consumer) motives guide a person’s behavior in a given situation (e.g. Carver and

Scheier, 1981; Pervin, 1989), differences in behavioral patterns could be expected as a result

of particular motive differences. In consumer motive structures, more specific behavioral sub-

goals at lower levels in the hierarchy are instrumental to reach higher level motives as ends

(e.g. Pieters, 1993; 1995). When applying this to the context of health, we expect different

health-related behaviors to exist among people with different health-related motive

orientations, as different behavioral strategies will be appropriate to satisfy the different

motives that are manifest. With regard to fruit and vegetable consumption, this may translate

into significant differences in consumption patterns among members of different health

audience segments. Based on the theoretical arguments posited in this study and their

operationalization in the context of health (i.e. H1a – H1d), we derive the following

hypotheses:

198
Chapter VI : Consumer Motives and Health Advertising

H2a: Significant differences in fruit and vegetable consumption frequency will exist among

the different health audience segments: as members of the Conviviality segment are expected

to enjoy the pleasures of life (e.g. tasty food even if rich in calories), they are supposed to

consume significantly less fruits and vegetables compared to the other segments. However,

as Recognition segment members are assumed to prefer food which has a benefit with respect

to physical appearance (i.e. low-calorie food) and reject what is bad for appearance (i.e. high-

calorie food), their fruit and vegetable intake is expected to be significantly higher. Vitality

and Security segment members are expected to have average fruit and vegetable consumption

levels, as their focus is set on rather balanced meals composed of all necessary food

components (i.e. not only fruits and vegetables).

H2b: Significant differences in category-specific fruit and vegetable consumption will exist

among the different health audience segments: to get power for their body, members of the

Vitality segment are expected to consume significantly more high-energy types of fruits and

vegetables (e.g. bananas) compared to the other segments, whereas Recognition segment

members are assumed to consume significantly more low-calorie types (e.g. raw vegetable

salads) to keep their body slim. More classical or traditional fruit and vegetable types with

high nutritional value (e.g. potatoes and cooked vegetables) are expected to be consumed

significantly more by members of the Security and Conviviality segments, since these people

are assumed to feel strongly about traditional family values and norms.

3.4. Health-related motive orientations and reactions toward health advertising

Besides impact on health-related behavior, we expect health-related motive orientations also

to influence consumers’ reactions toward health advertisements (i.e. attitude toward the ad

(Aad) and behavioral intention (BI)). It is assumed that for health advertising to be effective,

199
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

messages should not be delivered in homogeneous ways (e.g. Witte et al., 1996). Instead,

health practitioners must assess, prior to delivering a message, the manifest health-related

motive orientations of the target audience. Messages are recommended to be developed with

characteristics that are responsive to these manifest motives (e.g. Slater, 1996). From this

association with higher-order motives, motive-related advertisements evoke more personal

relevance (e.g. Rothchild, 1987; Eagle et al., 2004), revealing a tendency of a more positive

Aad and BI (Darley and Lim, 1991).

In this study, two types of message characteristics are manipulated in response to people’s

health-related motive orientations, i.e. message tonality and directionality. Based on the

principle of motive-compatibility previously established in literature (e.g. Higgins, 2002;

Pham and Higgins, 2005) (cf. supra: chapter V), it is hypothesized that when pleasure-related

motives are manifest, a transformational message tonality will be most appropriate, stressing

hedonic consumer experiences, as these message characteristics appear to be most compatible

with the former motive orientations. In contrast, when control-related motive orientations are

manifest, informational message characteristics are assumed to provide the best fit, focusing

on rationality and functionality, as in this case any confrontation with consumer emotions

should be avoided. In a similar vein, we assume that when power-related motives are

manifest, the arguments used in health advertisements should be self-directed, as an

individualistic tendency is inherent in these motive orientations. However, when belonging-

related motive orientations are manifest, arguments should be other-directed, focusing on

social connectedness, as these characteristics are congruent with the altruistic nature of

consumers driven by belonging-related motives. When combining the previous dimensions of

message tonality and directionality, four motive-related health messages could be developed,

each of which we expect to be personally relevant to one of the four hypothesized health

200
Chapter VI : Consumer Motives and Health Advertising

audience segments and thus should evoke more positive reactions within the corresponding

segment. Specifically, we hypothesize:

H3a: Compared to the other segments, the Vitality health audience segment will respond

more positively (Aad + BI) toward a transformational/self-directed health advertisement.

H3b: Compared to the other segments, the Conviviality health audience segment will

respond more positively (Aad + BI) toward a transformational/other-directed health

advertisement

H3c: Compared to the other segments, the Security health audience segment will respond

more positively (Aad + BI) toward an informational/other-directed health advertisement.

H3d: Compared to the other segments, the Recognition health audience segment will

respond more positively (Aad + BI) toward an informational/self-directed health

advertisement.

When comparing each segment’s most appropriate motive-related health advertisement to a

general advertisement (i.e. not responsive to consumer motives), we expect the following:

H3e: A segment’s reactions (Aad + BI) toward the most appropriate motive-related

advertisement will be significantly more positive than its reactions toward a general

advertisement, as the latter advertisement is less personally relevant.

201
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

4. Method

4.1. Stimuli

Stimuli were five printed health advertisements promoting a daily intake of at least five

portions of fresh fruits and vegetables (cf. WHO, 1990). Four motive-related advertisements

were created by the experimenter by using a 2 tonality (informational versus transformational

ad) by 2 directionality (self-directed versus other-directed ad) within-subjects design.

The tonality dimension was manipulated by using an informational versus transformational

advertising strategy (e.g. Rossiter and Percy, 1997). The transformational ads used vivid

pictures (either self-directed or other-directed depending upon the particular condition), paid

attention to hedonic aspects of fruit and vegetable consumption (e.g. enjoying life,

experiencing freedom) and put sentences in relatively personal first-person wording (either

singular (I-sentences) or plural (we-sentences) depending upon the condition). The

informational ad versions used mainly verbal arguments instead of pictures, paid attention to

the functional aspects of eating fruits and vegetables (e.g. avoiding illness or avoiding getting

obese) and wrote sentences in rather removed third-person wording. Directionality was

manipulated by using either self-directed arguments emphasizing the themes of self-identity,

individuality, unique lifestyle and private issue or other-directed arguments focusing on the

theme of we-ness, relationship, family-commitment and joint decision (e.g. Wang and

Mowen, 1997).

202
Chapter VI : Consumer Motives and Health Advertising

For comparison reasons, a fifth, general advertisement was presented to the subjects in

addition to the four experimental conditions24. This additional advertisement had both, a

more neutral tonality (neither informational nor transformational) and directionality (neither

self-directed nor other-directed) and was part of an existing Belgian public health campaign

concerning fruit and vegetable consumption, running at the time of research. All five

advertisements are shown in Appendix A.

4.2. Participants

Participants were recruited via the World Wide Web by using an on-line survey method. A

link to the questionnaire was placed on the website of a widespread national newspaper.

Ensuring that repeated participation was impossible, a sample of 692 subjects was collected.

77 respondents were eliminated from the sample because of incomplete responses on key

variables, yielding a final sample of N=615.

The respondents ranged in age from 17 to 77 years, with a mean age of 40.7. Compared with

the available statistics of the Belgian population (NIS, 2005), the sample was representative in

terms of age, relationship status and children in the family. With regard to gender, women

were slightly overrepresented (59.0%) as were people with a higher education (59.4%). A

detailed overview of the sample characteristics can be found in Table 6.2.

24
Though both carry-over and order effects may occur as a consequence of the use of a within-subject design, these problems were
respectively dealt with by collecting our measures of the dependent variable (i.e. reactions toward the five health advertisements) first, before
assessing people’s health-related motive orientations and by successively presenting all five advertisements in a randomized way.

203
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Table 6.2 : Sample Characteristics (% of respondents, N = 615)

Sample Population*
Gender
Male 41.0 48.9
Female 59.0 51.1
Age
<25 11.4 18.4
25-40 39.6 32.8
40-50 23.7 22.6
>50 25.3 26.2
MEAN 40.7
Education
Lower education (< age of 18) 40.6 67.4
Higher education (> age of 18) 59.4 32.6
Profession
Labourer 7.0
Employee 41.2
Executive 6.6
Self-employed 4.8
Functionary 11.9
Keeping house 6.1
Retired 9.4
Unemployed 2.9
Student 6.4
Other… 3.8
Relationship status
Single 22.6 27.1
Relationship/married 77.4 72.1
Children in family
No (=no children) 37.3 35.1
Yes (=children) 62.7 64.9
Employment Status
Part time employed 14.4
Full time employed 58.9
Not applicable 26.7

* Source: NIS (2005). Private Households Statistics. Brussels: NIS, National


Institute for Statistics

204
Chapter VI : Consumer Motives and Health Advertising

4.3. Measures

4.3.1. Attitude toward the ad (Aad) and behavioral intention (BI)

People’s reactions toward the five health advertisements were measured by a five-item

Attitude Toward the Ad Scale (Aad) and a single-item Behavioral Intention Scale25.

With regard to Aad, three items were based on Zinkhan, Locander and Leigh (1986) and the

other two items were taken from the Attitude Toward the Ad scale of Burke and Edell (1989).

All items ranged from 1= totally disagree to 8= totally agree. For all five advertisements, the

five-item scale reliabilities were very good. Cronbach’s Alpha’s ranged from .94 to .96, all

exceeding the lower critical value level of .60. So, in each case, the scores of the five items

were summed and averaged to form a composite measure of Aad.

Effects on behavioral intention were also measured for all five advertisements. Concretely,

subjects were asked to indicate the likelihood of eating more fresh fruits and vegetables on an

8-point likelihood scale ranging from 1= not at all likely to 8= very likely.

4.3.2. Health-related motive orientations

In Appendix B an overview is given of the scale that was used to measure people’s health-

related motive orientations. This scale consisted of 45 items. 15 items talked about health in

a rather explicit way (fundamental meaning of health) whereas the other 30 items dealt with

25
The rationale for using a single-item behavioral intention scale is based on Rossiter (2002). This author proposes the use of single-item
measures for assessing such concrete constructs as behavioral intention, as multi-item measures often involve the addition of unnecessary
and conceptually inappropriate items in order to obtain a high internal reliability (Cronbach’s alpha).

205
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

health more implicitly by focusing on perceived consequences of a bad health. Subjects rated

these items on a 7-point Likert scale going from 1=totally disagree to 7= totally agree.

All items were constructed based on desk research and qualitative research (18 in-depth

interviews and three group discussions) in which subjects – by means of the Laddering

Method (Reynolds and Gutman, 1988) – were encouraged to talk about their ways of dealing

with health and health-related issues. Based on this procedure all eight positions in Callebaut

et al.’s (1999) general taxonomy of consumer motives (i.e. Fig. 6.1) were translated in a series

of health statements that made up the items in the Health-related Motive Scale. As a

result, initially, eight health constructs were supposed to underlie this scale.

A maximum likelihood confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), using Lisrel 8.50 was performed

(Jöreskog and Sörbom, 1993) to determine whether the eight-factor solution was tenable. The

initial solution did not fit the data well, because of low loadings and poor unidimensionality

of some items. These items that did not perform well were dropped (N=11). With regard to

discriminant validity, the intercorrelation between the constructs ‘pleasure’ and ‘conviviality’

was very high (r=.85), as was the case with the correlation between ‘security’ and ‘control’

(r=.74). Hence, legitimated by the continuous nature of the general consumer motive

taxonomy 26 , we decided to respectively combine these adjacent positions into single

constructs. An additional confirmatory factor analysis was performed with six health

constructs, corresponding to the positions vitality, pleasure/conviviality (combined),

belonging, security/control (combined), power and recognition. A satisfactory six-factor

model emerged out of this analysis (see Table 6.3).

26
The relationships among the eight motives describe a continuum within a two-dimensional circumplex rather than discrete motive
categories. Hence, in CFA, some positions of the consumer motive taxonomy may emerge in combination with adjacent positions, rather
than showing up as separate constructs.

206
Chapter VI : Consumer Motives and Health Advertising

Table 6.3 : Results of Confirmatory Factor Analysis : Measurement Model

Measurement Model (Measures)

Original Final
Health Construct Number of Number of Item Labels* Composite Reliability
Items Items
1. Health = Energy 7 5 Exp1, Exp2, Exp6 .77
(i.e. Vitality-motive) Imp17, Imp29

2. Health = Emotional well-being 11 7 Exp3, Exp13, Exp14 .70


(i.e. Pleasure/Conviviality-motive) Imp4, Imp15, Imp21, Imp23

3. Health = Social responsibility 5 5 Exp4, Exp7 .62


(i.e. Belonging-motive) Imp1, Imp12, Imp25

4. Health = Physical well-being 10 7 Exp5, Exp8, Exp11 .61


(i.e. Security/Control-motive) Imp2, Imp8, Imp11, Imp22

5. Health = Management 4 4 Imp6, Imp20, Imp27, Imp28 .70


(i.e. Power-motive)

6. Health = Outward appearance 8 6 Exp10, Exp12 .82


(i.e. Recognition-motive) Imp7, Imp14, Imp16, Imp30

Measurement Model (Fit Indices)

NNFI .90
RMR .069
RMSEA .062
GFI .87
*For the full text item labels see Appendix B

207
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

To test discriminant validity, each of the 15 off-diagonal elements of Φ was fixed to 1.0, in

turn, and the model was re-estimated. Changes in the χ2 goodness-of-fit were statistically

significant for all 15 comparisons (∆χ2 ranging from 211 up to 252, df=1, p<.05) (Steenkamp

and Van Trijp, 1991). Construct reliabilities were evaluated by calculating the composite

reliabilities. Reliabilities of the six multiple-item scales were satisfactory as they ranged

from .61 to .82 (see Table 6.3). Scores of each item were summed and averaged to represent

the corresponding construct. These composite construct measures were used in all further

analyses.

4.3.3. Fruit and vegetable consumption

To estimate people’s intake of fresh fruits and vegetables, a short food frequency

questionnaire (FFQ) was used, developed and validated by Bogers et al. (2004). This

instrument had a reference period of 1 month and included those types or categories of fruits

and vegetables that were consumed most frequently in Belgium. Participants were asked to

indicate for each category, both their average consumption frequency and portion size.

Consumption frequency was measured on a 9-point scale going from 1= never or less than 1

day a month to 9= 7 days a week. To indicate portion size, a 5-point scale was used going

from 1= 1 serving27 to 5= 5 or more servings. Total intake, expressed in number of servings

per day, was calculated by multiplying consumption frequency with portion size.

Consumption of the various fruit and vegetable categories was summed to obtain total fruit

consumption, total vegetable consumption, and total fruit and vegetable intake

27
Depending on the category of fruits and vegetables, serving size was operationalized as either one 50-60g serving spoon (e.g. cooked
vegetables), one piece (e.g. bananas) or one glass (e.g. fruit juices).

208
Chapter VI : Consumer Motives and Health Advertising

5. Results

5.1. Manipulation Checks

Additionally, in participants’ questionnaire, two manipulation checks were included. These

measures asked about how people perceived a particular advertisement28 respectively in terms

of tonality (informational vs. transformational) and directionality (self-directed vs. other-

directed). Informational and transformational ad content was measured with three semantic

differential items that were based on the standard scale of Puto and Wells (1984): objective

information vs. experience-oriented; formal vs. informal; rational vs. emotional (α = .85).

Self-directedness and other-directedness were also measured with three semantic differentials

derived from Wang and Mowen (1997): individualistic vs. altruistic; self-identity vs. family

commitment; ego-ness vs. we-ness (α = .89). In both cases, each semantic differential used a

5-place scale response format. Tonality and directionality scores were derived by averaging

over the corresponding items, such that higher scores reflect respectively higher

transformationality and other-directedness.

ANOVA results indicated that both our tonality and directionality manipulations had been

successful. Transformational advertisements were perceived as significantly more

‘transformational’ compared to the informational advertisements (M=3.9 vs. M=2.6; F(1,

611)= 295.21, p<0.05). Also, other-directed advertisements were perceived as significantly

more ‘other-directed’ compared to the self-directed advertisements (M= 3.9 vs. M= 2.5; F(1,

611)= 429.57, p<0.05).

28
In this case, respondents were randomly assigned to only one of the four experimental conditions.

209
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Table 6.4 : Means and Standard Deviations for a Five-cluster Solution

Health Constructs (*) Total Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 Cluster 4 Cluster 5


sample (Energetic (Harmonious (Normative (Conscious (Rationalists)
Experimenters) Enjoyers) Carers) Experts)

Health = Energy 4.5 (1.59) 6.0 (.76) 4.1 (1.12) 2.7 (1.14) 4.4 (1.06) 3.0 (.96)
(i.e. Vitality-motive)

Health = Emotional well-being 5.2 (.88) 5.1 (.83) 5.8 (.62) 5.4 (.70) 4.8 (.95) 4.9 (.94)
(i.e. Pleasure/Conviviality-
motive)

Health = Social responsibility 5.2 (.94) 4.8 (1.00) 5.3 (1.06) 5.8 (.68) 5.0 (.87) 5.3 (.71)
(i.e. Belonging-motive)

Health = Physical well-being 5.3 (.81) 4.8 (.78) 5.2 (.77) 5.7 (.57) 5.0 (.80) 5.7 (.61)
(i.e. Security/Control-motive)

Health = Management 3.7 (1.22) 3.5 (1.20) 2.9 (1.05) 3.0 (1.01) 5.4 (.89) 3.7 (1.19)
(i.e. Power-motive)

Health = Outward appearance 3.5 (1.25) 3.3 (1.09) 2.2 (.68) 2.5 (.79) 5.6 (.62) 3.8 (1.00)
(i.e. Recognition-motive)

N= 615 N=207 N=101 N=104 N=109 N=94


(*) For the items of the scales for the health constructs:
- 1 corresponds to ‘totally disagree’
- 7 corresponds to ‘totally agree’

210
Chapter VI : Consumer Motives and Health Advertising

5.2. Health Audience Segments (H1)

To address the first hypothesis, a two-step cluster analysis (hierarchical clustering using

Ward’s method, followed by a K-means clustering) was performed on the six health

constructs that represent people’s health-related motive orientations. Although a four-cluster

solution was hypothesized, a five-cluster solution was deemed to identify the most distinct

segments of the sample population (see Table 6.4).

A correspondence analysis of the scores of the five clusters on the health statements revealed

a health-related motive structure (see Fig. 6.2), reflective of Callebaut et al.’s (1999)

consumer motive taxonomy. The vertical axis of Fig. 6.2 reflects the intrapersonal motive

axis as it refers to the way in which the individual translates ‘health’ for himself. As

expected, on the one hand health gets meaning throughout its emotional aspects of enjoyment

and freedom (i.e. Pleasure motive), whereas on the other hand, health is dealt with in a very

physical manner with a focus on functionality and control (i.e.. Control motive). The

horizontal axis of Fig 6.2 refers to the interpersonal motive axis as it shows how the

fundamental meaning of health is related to the social environment. As was expected in this

case, an altruistic, normative way of dealing with health could be discovered with a focus on

social well-being and responsibility (i.e.. Belonging motive), in contrast to a more

individualistic interpretation of health, focusing on feelings of independence of others and

activity (i.e.. Power motive). Out of the dynamic interaction of both dimensions, five health

audience segments emerge (i.e. results of cluster analysis), with different health-related

motive orientations.

211
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Figure 6.2 : Health-related Motive Structure

* % respondents in the corresponding cluster that associates health with the respective health statement

Variable mean scores in Table 6.4 and cluster-item associations in Fig. 6.2 are suggestive of

the nature of the health-related motive orientations for each cluster. In support of H1a, a

Vitality segment could be detected (i.e. Cluster 1) which we called ‘Energetic

Experimenters’. Members of this segment perceive health mainly in terms of vitality and

energy (M=6.0). 50.7% of them associates health with ‘living an active life’; 49.3% with

‘keeping the body in a good condition’ and 42.0% would perceive it as (extremely) bad not to

be able anymore to practice sports because of health problems. ‘Energetic Experimenters’

have an average age of 37.7, i.e. the youngest segment, and compared to the total sample

(41.0%), males are slightly overrepresented in this segment (52.7%). About 43.0% of Cluster

1 is working as an employee, 49.5% has no children (which is more than the other segments)

and 65.2% has a fulltime employment status. ‘Energetic Experimenters’ are the largest

segment and represent 34% of the total sample.

212
Chapter VI : Consumer Motives and Health Advertising

Cluster 2 subjects, called ‘Harmonious Enjoyers’, are representative for the Conviviality

segment hypothesized in H1b. These people are mainly involved with emotional health

(M=5.8). ‘Emotional well-being’ (75.2%); ‘enjoying life’ (67.3%) and ‘keeping up good

social contacts’ (32.7%) are very important to them. Compared to the other segments,

‘Harmonious Enjoyers’ are slightly older (average age 43.9) with more retired people

(19.0%). Most of them have children (67.3%) and here also, males are a bit overrepresented

compared to the total sample (48.5%). 16% of our sample belongs to this segment.

Also H1c was supported as a Security segment could be identified (i.e. Cluster 3), called

‘Normative Carers’. In contrast to cluster 2, ‘Normative Carers’ are heavily concerned with

the physical aspects of health (M=5.7). Being healthy means in particular ‘avoiding illness’

and ‘having no physical health problems’ (50.6%). In addition, members of Cluster 3

perceive health as a social responsibility (M=5.8). ‘Taking care of other family members’

health’ (29.9%), is considered as very important. Most of the people in Cluster 3 are women

(59.7%) with an average age of 43.6. More than the other segments Normative Carers work

at home (14.3%). This segment accounts for 17% of the sample.

Respondents in Cluster 4 (18% of the sample) are called ‘Conscious Experts’ and could be

considered as the Recognition segment posited in H1d. For them, health is mainly about

management (M=5.4) and physical appearance (M=5.6). They deal with health in a very self-

conscious way with the focus on the health of their own body. As opposed to the other

segments, ‘staying slim’ (24.8%); ‘looking good’ (10.1%); ‘being stylish’ are very important

aspects of feeling healthy for members of Cluster 4. They know their own body the best and

as a result, they want to manage their health by themselves, without needing advice from

213
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

others. Among them, the majority are women (74.1%), fulltime employed (63.3%) and

41.3% works as an employee. The average age of this second youngest segment is 40.2.

Finally, a fifth health audience segment emerged, that was not explicitly hypothesized but

seemed to be congruent with the Control position of the general consumer motive taxonomy.

As Normative Carers, members of Cluster 5 also are engaged with the physical/functional

aspects of health (M= 5.7), but with as main objective ‘having the competence to do their job’

(27.7%) and ‘being able to organize life and to work functionally’ (41.5%), rather than

‘taking care of the health of others’. These ‘Rationalists’ represent 15% of the total sample.

Here also, women are highly overrepresented (75.3%) with a mean age of 41.9. Most of them

are employees (52.1%) with a fulltime employment status (54.3%).

5.3. Fruit and Vegetable Consumption (H2)

Hypothesis 2 concerning the impact of health-related motive orientations on health-related

consumer behavior was tested via one-way ANOVA. No significant differences were found

between the segments with regard to the total intake of fruits and vegetables (F(4, 552)= .85,

p>.05), as all cluster means fluctuated tightly around the sample mean of 5.4 servings a day.

Hence, H2a was not supported.

Significant differences were found between the five health audience segments with regard to

the types of fruits and vegetables that were consumed. In line with H2b, Energetic

Experimenters (i.e. Vitality segment) (M=0.7) consumed significantly more bananas than did

Harmonious Enjoyers (M= 0.3), Normative Carers (M= 0.3), Conscious Experts (M=0.2) and

Rationalists (M=0.3) (F(4, 522)= 3.23, p<.05). Also as expected, Harmonious Enjoyers (i.e.

214
Chapter VI : Consumer Motives and Health Advertising

Conviviality segment) (M=1.9) and Normative Carers (i.e. Security segment) (M=2.1) had a

significantly higher consumption of potatoes than did Energetic Experimenters (M=1.1),

Conscious Experts (M=1.0) and Rationalists (M=1.3) (F(4, 522)= 6.81, p<.01). The same

pattern could be found for the category of cooked vegetables: Harmonious Enjoyers (M=2.6)

and Normative Carers (M=2.8) ate significantly more cooked vegetables than did Energetic

Experimenters (M=2.1), Conscious Experts (M=2.1) and Rationalists (M=2.2) (F(4, 552)=

4.17, p<.01). However, in contrast with H2b, no significant differences were found between

the health segments with regard to the consumption of raw vegetable salads (F(4, 552)= 1.54,

p>.05).

5.4. Reactions Toward Health Advertisements (H3)

The third hypothesis concerning the reactions toward motive-related health advertising was

also tested via one-way ANOVA. Results indicate that each of the five health segments –

when compared to the other segments – responds more positively (Aad + BI) toward the

advertisement that was hypothesized to be most responsive to its health-related motive

orientations. In support of H3a, Energetic Experimenters (i.e. Vitality segment) had

significantly higher Aad (M=5.7) and BI (M= 5.1) scores for the transformational/self-

directed health advertisement (i.e. Ad 2) compared to the other segments (F(4, 611)= 6.93,

p<.01 for Aad and F(4, 611)= 8.64, p<.01 for BI). Also H3b was supported as Harmonious

Enjoyers (i.e. Conviviality segment) had significantly higher measures of both Aad (M=5.9)

and BI (M=5.7) for the transformational/other-directed advertisement (i.e. Ad 1) compared to

the four other segments (F(4, 611)= 5.91, p<.01 for Aad and F(4, 611)= 3.84, p<.01 for BI).

For the informational/other-directed health advertisement (i.e. Ad 4), Normative Carers (i.e.

Security segment) showed significantly higher measures of Aad (M= 6.8) and BI (M= 6.7),

215
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

compared to the other segments (F(4, 611)= 21.89, p<.01 for Aad and F(4, 611)= 15.41,

p<.01 for BI), which supported H3c. Finally, also H3d was supported as Conscious Experts

(i.e. Recognition segment) scored significantly higher on Aad (M= 4.7) and BI (M= 4.5) for

the informational/self-directed advertisement (i.e. Ad 3) (F(4, 611)= 4.57, p<.01 for Aad and

F(4, 611)= 7.86, p<.01 for BI). In addition to the Conscious Experts, also Rationalists (i.e.

Control segment) scored significantly higher on Aad (M= 4.6) and BI (M= 4.5) with regard to

Ad3, which is plausible given this segment’s specific position in the health-related motive

structure (see Fig. 6.2).

To test H3e, for each segment, a Paired Samples T-test was conducted between the score on

Aad and BI for a general advertisement and the most appropriate motive-related

advertisement (see Table 6.5). For BI, in all five cases, the most appropriate motive-related

advertisement elicited a significant higher score compared to the general advertisement (Ad5).

With regard to Aad, significant differences between the appropriate motive-related

advertisement and the general advertisement were only found for three segments. For

Conscious Experts and Rationalists, the motive-related advertisement did not elicit more

positive Aad scores than Ad5. Hence, H3e was fully (Aad + BI) supported in three of the five

cases and partially (only BI) supported in all five cases.

216
Chapter VI : Consumer Motives and Health Advertising

Table 6.5 : Paired Samples T-test (general ad vs. motive-related ad)

Health Segment Reactions motive- General P-value


related Ad (T-test)
Ad

Energetic Experimenters Aad 5.7 4.7 .001**


BI 5.1 4.3 .000**

Harmonious Enjoyers Aad 5.9 4.9 .002**


BI 5.7 4.3 .000**

Normative Carers Aad 6.8 4.3 .000**


BI 6.7 4.3 .000**

Conscious Experts Aad 4.7 4.5 n.s.


BI 4.5 3.9 .010*

Rationalists Aad 4.6 4.6 n.s.


BI 4.5 3.9 .030*
* p<.05
** p<.01
n.s. (not significant)

6. Discussion and recommendations

This paper contributes to the study of health communication by proposing a new theory-

driven health audience segmentation approach based on Callebaut et al.’s (1999) taxonomy of

eight consumer motives and by demonstrating the usefulness of this method in the process of

creating effective health advertisements.

As health beliefs or concerns play an important role in food behavior such as fruit and

vegetable intake, a heterogeneous health audience was segmented according to the audience

members’ health-related motive orientations. Analysis of Hypothesis 1 shows that five

different health audience segments can be identified, instead of the four-cluster solution that

was hypothesized on a theoretical basis. In practice – besides the four hypothesized segments

– one additional health segment seems to be important for which health has another distinct

217
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

meaning. The five health segments can be organized along two dimensions; yielding a

health-related motive structure that is a reflection of Callebaut et al.’s (1999) general

consumer motive taxonomy.

Although theoretically expected, no significant differences in total fruit and vegetable intake

were found among the different health audience segments. These results may indicate that

people’s health-related motive orientations have rather low (or no) influence on daily fruit and

vegetable consumption in general. As a result, ‘low’ fruit and vegetable consumers are not

predominant in one particular segment but instead are equally dispersed over all five

segments. However, as expected, consumer’s health-related motive orientations do have a

significant influence on category-specific fruit and vegetable consumption (i.e. bananas,

potatoes, cooked vegetables). Following Means-End reasoning, this may be due to the fact

that different fruit and vegetable types with different features or (health-related) benefits are

instrumental to achieve different health-related motive orientations. For example, the

perception of bananas as a source of energy and power (e.g. Vitality motive) may account for

the higher consumption of this type of fruit among Energetic Experimenters, whereas potatoes

and cooked vegetables may be more preferred by Normative Carers and Harmonious Enjoyers

because motive-related associations are made between these types of vegetables and for

instance tradition (common habit), family health and tastefulness.

Tests of Hypothesis 3 suggest that significant differences exist among the health audience

segments with regard to their reactions (Aad + BI) toward motive-related health advertising.

When compared to the other segments, all health audience segments responded more

positively (Aad + BI) toward the advertisement that was hypothesized to be most responsive

to their health-related motive orientations. Moreover, when comparing the most appropriate

218
Chapter VI : Consumer Motives and Health Advertising

motive-related advertisement to a general advertisement, in almost all cases a segment’s

reactions (Aad + BI) toward the motive-related advertisement were significantly more

positive than its reactions toward the general advertisement. The failure to observe significant

differences in Aad between the targeted motive-related advertisement and the general

advertisement for Conscious Experts and Rationalists may probably be due to the fact that

these subjects are less influenced by advertising. For none of the advertisements, Conscious

Experts and Rationalists had an Aad en BI score significantly exceeding the value of 4.5

(neutral value) with some scores even falling below the value of 4.0. These results may

indicate that these respondents will stick to their own ideas about health-related issues, rather

than giving advertising the opportunity to make up or change their minds.

This analysis of Hypothesis 3 demonstrates the need to carefully diagnose the manifest

health-related motive orientations of the target audience, prior to delivering health

advertisements. Also the results indicate that different advertising strategies might be

appropriate for people with different health-related motive orientations. In what follows, we

develop specific message recommendations for developing health advertisements targeted

toward different audiences. First, with regard to message tonality we suggest that:

1. When pleasure-related motive orientations are manifest among members of the target

audience, then the tonality of the advertisements developed should be

transformational, focusing on emotional/sensory cues and hedonic experiences, as

people in such situation tend to follow their emotions and impulses.

2. Conversely, when control-related motive orientations are manifest, people tend to

avoid confrontations with their emotions and passions. In this case, health

219
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

practitioners should rather make people think by using an informational advertising

strategy, focusing on verbal arguments and functional aspects of consumption.

With regard to the directionality of the arguments used in health advertisements we propose

the following:

1. When power-related motive orientations are manifest, then the arguments used should

be self-directed, focusing on individuality and private issue, as people in this case tend

to take decisions independently and strive for personal success.

2. Conversely, when belonging-related motive orientations are manifest, then the

arguments used in health advertisements should be other-directed, stressing the

themes of we-ness and family-commitment, as people here want to feel part of a group

and be accepted by their loved ones.

Based on these more general suggestions, we also formulate some specific recommendations

on how to develop appropriate advertisements for the five health audience segments obtained

in this study:

1. Energetic Experimenters: for people with a Vitality-motive with regard to health,

health advertisements should use a transformational/self-directed advertising strategy,

as this segment is positioned within the upper-left quadrant of the health-related

motive structure (combination of pleasure and power). Besides using vivid images

that express movement and vitality, health practitioners should focus on individual

consumer experiences such as feeling free, getting energy and power for the body,

feeling very much alive and kicking. To increase the perception of being addressed

220
Chapter VI : Consumer Motives and Health Advertising

personally, sentences might be put in first-person wording (I-sentences), rather than

using removed third-person wording.

2. Harmonious Enjoyers: as these people deal with health mainly in terms of emotional

well-being (e.g. Pleasure/Conviviality), advertisements designed to them should avoid

focusing too much on functional aspects of consumption. Rather, health

advertisements should express how the recommended behavior (e.g. eating more fruits

and vegetables) might be instrumental to achieve a complete and happy life together

with family and friends. As in the previous case, a transformational advertising

strategy can be strongly recommended (e.g. using vivid pictures, informal

language…). However, in this case, the arguments used in the advertisement should

not be self-directed. Rather, other-directed arguments should be employed that focus

on connectedness and harmony between people.

3. Normative Carers: for this segment positioned within the bottom-right quadrant of

the health-related motive structure (combination between Control and Belonging), we

recommend an informational/other-directed advertising strategy. As Normative

Carers are mainly concerned with the physical aspects of health, recommendations in

the advertisements should focus in particular on functional benefits such as avoiding

illness and health problems. Instead of using vivid images, members of this segment

should be convinced by strong verbal arguments, providing important (health-related)

information and focusing on the advice and norms of expert-others (doctor,

dietician…). In addition, advertisements designed to these people should emphasize

the social responsibility aspect that is associated with health. Communicating how the

recommended responses may contribute to the preservation of the health of close

others (family, children…), might be an appropriate strategy to deal with this issue.

221
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

4. Conscious Experts/Rationalists: in order to counteract these people’s disinterest

toward advertising, advertisements designed to them might emphasize one’s own

independent capacities to deal with health. Make members of these segments feel like

natural experts – being able to keep control over their health by themselves – and your

health advertisement might be more successful. As health-related motive orientations

of these segments are a combination of Control- and Power-related motives, an

informational/self-directed advertising strategy is suggested, with mainly verbal

arguments focusing on outward appearance (looking good, looking thin) and showing

off competence.

7. Limitations and future research

This study faces some limitations. First, because standard measurement instruments appear

not to exist to assess people’s health-related motive orientations, it became necessary to

develop our own scale. Given the conceptual purpose of this paper, we feel confident this

‘self-made’ scale performed well to generate six reliable health constructs that served as a

solid basis for segmentation. However, from a methodological point of view, additional tests

should be done to further improve this scale’s psychometric properties.

Second, with regard to differences in health-related consumer behavior as a result of

differences in health-related motive orientations, only two aspects of fruit and vegetable

consumption were considered in this study, i.e. consumption frequency and types of fruits and

vegetables that were consumed. Future studies may concentrate on assessing the relationship

between health-related motive orientations and other aspects of fruit and vegetable

222
Chapter VI : Consumer Motives and Health Advertising

consumption (e.g. buying process or decision factors) or consumption of other food products

with a predominant health image (e.g. fish or functional foods).

In the same way some future research opportunities may exist with regard to the relationship

between health-related motive orientations and people’s reactions toward health advertising.

In this study only two broad dimensions of advertising strategy were tested, i.e. message

tonality and directionality. Future studies may investigate some other relevant aspects of

campaign development and message creation (e.g. different appeals or different types of

endorsers).

Also, the usefulness of the proposed motive-related health audience segmentation method

could be demonstrated in the context of health prevention instead of health promotion. For

example, when attempting to develop increasingly successful antismoking or fat reduction

advertisements, health communicators may consider the manifest health-related motive

orientations of the target audience.

Finally, in this study, only perceived effects of motive-related health advertising were

assessed, as a better performance was only demonstrated with regard to consumer’s self-

reported reactions toward health advertisements. In general, far more positive Aad and BI

scores were reported for motive-related advertisements compared to advertising that is not

motive-related. Future experimental intervention studies may also investigate actual

behavioral effects, by assessing the potential effectiveness of motive-related health

advertising in increasing real fruit and vegetable intake.

223
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Acknowledgments

The help of the newspaper Het Nieuwsblad is very much acknowledged for giving us the

opportunity to collect the data for this study through its website. The assistance and support

of Patrick Salien and Kurt Minnen are particularly appreciated.

224
Chapter VI : Consumer Motives and Health Advertising

References

Albrecht, T.L., & Bryant, C. (1996). Advances in segmentation modeling for health

communication and social marketing campaigns. Journal of Health Communication,

1, 65-80.

Atkin, C.K., & Freimuth, V. (1989). Formative evaluation research in campaign design. In

R.E. Rice, & C.K. Atkin (Eds.). Public communication campaigns (2nd ed.) (pp. 131-

150). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Bogers, R.P., Van Assema, P., Kester, A.D.M., Westerterp, K.R., & Dagnelie, P.C. (2004).

Reproducibility, validity and responsiveness to change of a short questionnaire for

measuring fruit and vegetable intake. American Journal of Epidemiology, 159, 900-

909.

Boslaugh, S.E., Kreuter, M.W., Nicholson, R.A., & Naleid, K. (2005). Comparing

demographic, health status and psychosocial strategies of audience segmentation to

promote physical activity. Health Education Research, 20(4), 430-438.

Brehony, K.A., Frederiksen, L.W., & Solomon, L.J. (1984). Marketing principles and

behavioral medecine. In L. Frederiksen, L.J. Solomon, & K.A. Brehony (Eds.).

Marketing health behavior: principles, techniques and applications (pp. 3-23). New

York: Plenum Press.

Burke, M.C., & Edell, J.A. (1989). The impact of feelings on ad-based affect and cognitions.

Journal of Marketing Research, 26 (feb), 69-83.

Callebaut, J., Janssens, M., Op de Beeck, D., Lorré, D., & Hendrickx, H. (1999).

Motivational marketing research revisited. Leuven-Apeldoorn: Garant.

225
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Carver, C.S., & Scheier, M.F. (1981). Attention and self-regulation: a control theory

approach to human behavior. New York: Springer-Verlag.

Cox, D., Anderson, A., McKellar, S., Reynolds, J., Lean, M., & Mela, D.J. (1996).

Vegetables and Fruits: barriers and opportunities for greater consumption. Nutrition

& Food Science, 96 (5), 44-47.

Darley, W.K., & Lim, J.S. (1991). Personal relevance as moderator of the effect of public

service advertising on behavior. Advances in Consumer Research, 18, 303-309.

Dixon, H., Borland, R., Segan, C., Stafford, H., & Sindall, C. (1998). Public reactions to

Victoria’s “ 2 Fruit ‘n’ 5 Veg Every Day” campaign and reported cosumption of fruit

and vegetables. Preventive Medicine, 27 (4), 572-582.

Donohew, L. (1990). Public health campaigns: individual message strategies and a model. In

E.B. Ray & L. Donohew (Eds.). Communication and Health: Systems and

Applications (pp.136). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.

Eagle, L., Bulmer, S., Kitchen, P.J. & Hawkins, J. (2004). Complex and controversial causes

for the obesity epidemic: the role of marketing communications. International

Journal of Medical marketing, 3,4, 271-287.

Fennis, B.M. (2003). Advertising, consumer behaviour and health: exploring possibilities for

health promotion. International Journal of Medical Marketing, 3,4, 316-326.

Ford, M.E., & Nichols, C.W. (1987). A taxonomy of human goals and some possible

applications. In D.H. Ford (Ed.). Humans as self-constructing living systems: getting

the framework to work (pp. 289-311). University Park (Pa): Pennsylvania State

University, college of human development.

Forthofer, M.S., & Bryant, C.A. (2000). Using audience-segmentation techniques to tailor

health behavior change strategies. American Journal of Health behavior, 24(1), 36-

43.

226
Chapter VI : Consumer Motives and Health Advertising

Freimuth, V., Cole, G., & Kirby, S. (2001). Issues in evaluating mass media health

communication campaigns. In I. Rootman, M. Goodstadt, B. Hyndman, D.V.

McQueen, L. Potvin, J. Springett, & E. Ziglio (Eds.). Evaluation in health promotion:

Principles and perspectives (pp. 475-492). Copenhagen: WHO Regional Office for

Europe.

French, S.A., & Stables, G. (2003). Environmental interventions to promote vegetable and

fruit consumption among youth in school settings. Preventive Medicine, 37, 593-610.

French, S.A., Story, M., & Jeffery, R.W. (2001). Environmental influences on eating and

physical activity. Annual Review of Public Health, 22, 309-335.

Geeroms, N., Vermeir, I., Van Kenhove, P., & Hendrickx, H. (2005). A Taxonomy of

Consumer Motives through Preferred Brand Personality: Empirical Findings for 11

countries. Working Paper, Ghent University, Department of Marketing.

Glanz, K., & Yaroch, A.L. (2004). Strategies for increasing fruit and vegetable intake in

grocery stores and communities: policy, pricing and environmental change.

Preventive Medicine, 39, S75-S80 Suppl. 2.

Grier, S., & Bryant, C.A. (2005). Social marketing in public health. Annual Review of Public

Health, 26, 319-339.

Hastings, G.B., & Haywood, A.J. (1994). Social marketing: a critical response. Health

Promotion International, 9(1), 59-63.

Higgins, E.T. (2002). How self-regulation creates distinct values: the case of promotion and

prevention decision-making. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 12(3), 177-191.

Hu, F.B., Rimm, E.B., Stampfer, M.J., Ascherio, A., Spiegelman, D., & Willett, W.C. (2000).

Prospective study of major dietary patterns and risk of coronary heart disease in men.

American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 72, 912-921.

227
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Jöreskog, K., & Sörbom, D. (1993). Lisrel 8: Structural Equation Modeling with the

SIMPLIS command language. Chicago: SSI Scientific Software International, Inc.

Lambert, N. (2001). Food choice, phytochemicals and cancer prevention. In L. Frewer, E.

Risvik, & H. Schifferstein (Eds.). Food, people and society (pp 131-154). Berlin:

Springer.

Lefebvre, R.C., & Flora, J.A. (1988). Social marketing and public health intervention. Health

Education Quarterly, 15, 299-315.

Li, R., Serdula, M., Bland, S., Mokdad, A., Bowman, B. & Nelson, D. (2000). Trends in fruit

and vegetable consumption among adults in 16 US states: behavioural risk factor

surveillance system, 1990-1996. American Journal of Public Health, 90, 777-781.

Ling, A.M.C., & Horwath, C. (2001). Perceived benefits and barriers of increased fruit and

vegetable consumption: validation of a decisional balance scale. Journal of Nutrition

Education, 33, 257-265.

McClelland, D.C. (1980). Motive dispositions: the merits of operant and respondent

measures. In L. Wheeler (Ed.), Review of Personality and Social Psychology (Vol. 1,

p. 10-41). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

McClelland, D.C. (1985). Human motivation. Glenview, IL: Scott Foresman and Company.

Mowen, J.C., & Minor, M. (1997). Consumer Behavior (5th Ed.). Prentice Hall.

Naska, A., Vasdekis, V.G.S., Trichopoulou, A., Friel, S., Leonhauser, I.U., Moreiras, O.,

Nelson, M., Remaut, A.M., Schmitt, A., Sekula, W., Trygg, K.U., & Zajkas, G.

(2000). Fruit and vegetable availability among ten European countries: how does it

compare with the ‘five-a-day’ recommendation? British Journal of Nutrition, 84, 549-

556.

NIS (2005). Private Households Statistics. Brussels: NIS, National Institute for Statistics.

228
Chapter VI : Consumer Motives and Health Advertising

Pervin, L.A. (1989). Goal concepts: themes, issues and questions. In L.A. Pervin (Ed.).

Goal concepts in personality and social psychology (pp. 473-479). Hillsdale, NJ:

Erlbaum.

Pham, M., & Higgins, E.T. (2005). Promotion and prevention in consumer decision-making.

In S. Ratneshwar and D.G. Mick (Eds.), Inside consumption (p. 8-43). New York:

Routledge.

Pieters, R. (1993). A control view on the behaviour of consumers: turning the triangle. In

G.J. Bamossy, & W.F. Van Raaij (Eds.). European Advances in Consumer Research,

1, 507-512.

Pieters, R., Baumgartner, H., & Allen, D. (1995). A means-end chain approach to consumer

goal structures. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 12, 227-244.

Pollard, J., Kirk, S.F.L., & Cade, J.E. (2002). Factors affecting food choice in relation to fruit

and vegetable intake: a review. Nutrition Research Reviews, 15, 373-387.

Puto, C.P., & Wells, W.D. (1984). Informational and transformational advertising: the

differential effects of time. In T.C. Kinnear (Ed.). Advances in Consumer Research

(Vol 11, pp. 638-643). Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research.

Rapley, J., & Coulson, N.S. (2005). Stages of change and consumption of fruit and

vegetables among adolescent females: associations with decisional balance and self-

efficacy. British Food Journal, 107(9), 663-669.

Rappoport, L., Peters, G.R., Downey, R., McCann, T., & Huff-Corzine, L. (1993). Gender

and age differences in food cognition. Appetite, 20, 33-52.

Reynolds, T.J., & Gutman, J. (1988). Laddering theory: method, analysis and interpretation.

Journal of Advertising Research, 28 (feb/March), 11-31.

229
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Ringold, D.J. (2002). Boomerang effects in response to public health interventions: some

unintended consequences in the alcohol beverage market. Journal of Consumer

Policy, 25 (1), 27-63.

Rossiter, J.R. (2002). The C-OAR-SE procedure for scale development in marketing.

International Journal of Research in Marketing, 19, 305-335.

Rossiter, J.R., & Percy, L. (1997). Advertising and promotion management. New York:

McGraw-Hill.

Rothchild, M. (1987). Advertising. Lexington, MA: D.C. Heath.

Rurik, I., & Antal, M. (2003). Nutritional habits and lifestyle practice of elderly people in

Hungary. Acta Aliment. Hung, 32, 77-88.

Safley, C.D., Poling, E.B., Wohlgenant, M.K., Sydorovych, O., & Williams, R.F. (2004).

Producing and marketing strawberries for direct market operations. Horttechnology,

14 (1), 124-135.

Silk, K.J., Weiner, J., & Parrott, R.L. (2005). Gene Cuisine or Frankenfood? The theory of

reasoned action as an audience segmentation strategy for messages about genetically

modified foods. Journal of Health Communication, 10, 751-767.

Slater, M., & Flora, J. (1994). Is health behavior consumer behavior? Health behavior

determinants, audience segmentation, and designing media health campaigns. In E.M.

Clark, T.C. Brock, & D.W. Stewart (Eds.). Attention, Attitude and Affect in Response

to Advertising (pp.273-285). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

Slater, M.D. (1995). Choosing audience segmentation strategies and methods for health

communication. In E. Maibach, & R.L. Parrott (Eds.). Designing health messages:

Approaches from communication theory and public health practice (pp.186-198).

Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

230
Chapter VI : Consumer Motives and Health Advertising

Slater, M.D. (1996). Theory and method in health audience segmentation. Journal of Health

Communication, 1, 267-283.

Steenkamp, J.B., & Van Trijp, J.C.M. (1991). The use of Lisrel in validating marketing

constructs. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 8, 283-299.

Steinmetz, K.A., & Potter, J.D. (1996). Vegetables, fruits and cancer prevention. Journal of

the American Dietetic Association, 96, 1027-1039.

Steptoe, A., Pollard, T.M., & Wardle, J. (1995). Development of a measure of the motives

underlying the selection of food: the Food Choice Questionnaire. Appetite, 25, 267-

284.

Thompson, B., Demark, W., Taylor, G., McClelland, J.W., Stables, G., Havas, S., Feng, Z.,

Topor, M., Heimendinger, J., Reynolds, K.D., & Cohen, N. (1999). Baseline fruit and

vegetable intake among adults in seven 5-a-day centres located in diverse geographic

areas. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 99, 1241-1248.

Van Duyn, M.A., & Pivonka, E. (2000). Overview of the health benefits of fruit and

vegetable consumption for the dietetics professional: selected literature. Journal of

the American Dietetic Association, 100 (12), 1511-1522.

Verbeke, W. (2005). Agriculture and the food industry in the information age. European

Review of Agricultural Economics, 32(3), 347-368.

Verbeke, W., & Pieniak, Z. (2006). Benefit beliefs, attitudes and behaviour towards fresh

vegetable consumption in Poland and Belgium. Acta Alimentaria, 35(1), 5-16.

Vereecken, C.A., Keukelier, E., & Maes, L. (2004). Influence of mother’s educational level

on food parenting practices and food habits of young children. Appetite, 43, 93-103.

Wang, C.L., & Mowen, J.C. (1997). The separateness-connectedness self-schema: scale

development and application to message construction. Psychology & Marketing, 14

(March), 185-207.

231
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Weaver, M., Poehlitz, M., & Hutchison, S. (1999). 5 a day for low-income families:

evaluation of an advertising campaign and cooking events. Journal of Nutrition

Education, 31 (3), 161-169.

WHO. (1990). The world health organization report, diet, nutrition and the prevention of

chronic diseases. WHO Technical Report Series 797.

Williams, C. (1995). Healthy eating: clarifying advice about fruit and vegetables. British

Medical Journal, 310, 1453-1455.

Witte, K., Cameron, K.A., McKeon, J.K., & Berkowitz, J.M. (1996). Predicting risk

behaviors: development and validation of a diagnostic scale. Journal of Health

Communication, 1, 317-341.

Zinkhan, G.M., Locander, W.B., & Leigh, J.H. (1986). Dimensional relationships of aided

recall and recognition. Journal of Advertising, 15 (1), 38-46

232
Chapter VI : Consumer Motives and Health Advertising

Appendix A

• Stimuli advertisements

Appendix B

• List of scale items

233
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Appendix A: Stimuli Advertisements

Ad 1 (transformational/other-directed)

234
Chapter VI : Consumer Motives and Health Advertising

Ad 2 (transformational/self-directed)

235
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Ad 3 (informational/self-directed)

236
Chapter VI : Consumer Motives and Health Advertising

Ad 4 (informational/other-directed)

237
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Ad 5 (general ad)

238
Chapter VI : Consumer Motives and Health Advertising

Appendix B: List of scale items

Health-related Motive Scale

Explicit items (Exp)


For me, health is mainly about…

1. keeping the body in a good condition, i.e. fitness, jogging, aerobics…


2. having the energy to do the things I want to do.
3. taking time to relax and to enjoy life.
4. living in harmony with my family.
5. having no physical health problems.
6. living an active life, i.e. practicing sports…
7. taking care of other family members’ health.
8. following the advice of expert-others, i.e. doctor, dietician,…
9. developing a healthy lifestyle of my own.
10. looking good.
11. reducing physical health risks with regard to heart, lungs, liver,….
12. staying slim.
13. emotional well-being, feeling good mentally.
14. keeping up good social contacts.
15. keeping nutritional intake strictly under control (feeling in control over my body).

Implicit items (Imp)


Because of health problems, it would be (extremely) bad not to be able anymore to…
1. help others.
2. feel secure in life.
3. work functionally.
4. live an active social life.
5. behave independently.
6. be successful.
7. be classy.
8. feel protected.
9. be playful.
10. work creatively.
11. get stability in life.
12. share time with family.
13. enjoy life.
14. stay slim.
15. perceive warmth and conviviality.
16. stay beautiful.
17. practice sports.
18. organize and control life.
19. be cheerful.
20. be powerful.
21. have fun with others.
22. get rest in life.
23. have close friends.
24. be spontaneous.
25. care for my family.
26. think rationally.
27. be ambitious.
28. be competent.
29. live an adventurous life.
30. be stylish.

239
A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives : Practical Applications

CHAPTER VII

UNDERSTANDING READY MEAL CONSUMPTION BEHAVIOR :

THE ROLE OF CONSUMER HEALTH-RELATED MOTIVE

ORIENTATIONS

241
Chapter VII

Understanding ready meal consumption behavior:

The role of consumer health-related motive orientations

1. Abstract

Applying a conceptualization of health beyond the physical level, the present study

investigates the impact of psychological dimensions of health concern on consumer behavior

for ready meals. A heterogeneous audience (N= 1934) was segmented according to the

audience members’ health-related motive orientations. Five distinct health segments emerged

out of a two-step cluster analysis. When comparing these segments, several significant and

meaningful relationships could be detected between health-related motive orientations and

both general food behavior patterns and ready meal consumption patterns. Based on these

insights guidelines are provided for improving consumer-oriented product development,

positioning and marketing of products belonging to the category of ready meals.

Comprehensive positioning strategies are suggested, that go beyond convenience-related

aspects and also take into account psychological health-related motive orientations of the

target audience.

243
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

2. Introduction

Several socio-economic changes such as increased female participation in the workforce,

increasing time pressure brought about by job- and leisure-related activities (e.g. Bowers,

2000), a growing number of single-person and small households (e.g. Byrne, 1998), and lack

of abilities and experience with preparing home meals (e.g. Gofton, 1995) have boosted the

demand for convenience in meal preparation (e.g. Costa et al., 2005). Today’s food suppliers

have readily reacted to this convenience trend by considerably expanding their offer of pre-

prepared meal solutions, i.e. ready meals. Ready meals can be defined as complete meals that

require few or no extra ingredients, prepared by external procedures and designed to fully and

speedily replace – at home – the main course of a home-made main meal (e.g. Mahon et al.,

2006; Costa et al., 2001). However, ready meals should be distinguished from ready-to-eat

take away foods, as the former type of home meal replacement still requires cooking or re-

heating prior to serving whereas with take away foods no cooking or heating is needed

anymore (e.g. Verlegh and Candel, 1999).

Given the growing economic importance, i.e. increasing sales and export values of the

prepared consumer foods sector (e.g. Datamonitor, 2003; Distributie Vandaag, 1999), a

thorough understanding is required of the factors that determine a consumer’s ready meal

consumption behavior. Besides a number of other factors, health concern has been proposed

as a potential determinant of consumer behavior for convenience foods (e.g. Jago, 2000). The

perceived healthiness of convenience foods, i.e. perceived consequences for physical health

from consuming convenience foods was found to have a negative effect on the purchase of

these products (e.g. de Boer et al., 2004).

244
Chapter VII : Consumer Motives and Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

Extending previous work by Geeroms et al. (2005b) on motive-related audience segmentation

(cf. supra: chapter VI), the present study investigates how psychological aspects of health

concern, i.e. different health-related motive orientations influence people’s ways of dealing

with convenience-related food solutions. Through focusing on ready meals, the focus will be

on convenience-related specifically to the preparation stage in the food consumption process.

Attention will be directed to three broad research questions. First, we seek to validate the

health audience segmentation approach proposed by Geeroms et al. (2005b) for another

subject sample. Second, we aim to compare the found health segments in terms of their

general food behavior patterns and third, in terms of their ready meal consumption patterns in

particular. In the sections that follow, a more profound rationale is provided for the three

research issues and specific hypotheses are developed regarding each topic.

2.1. Health-related motive orientations29

Health-related motive orientations could be considered as relatively enduring beliefs that

people hold about the fundamental meaning of health. Whereas some people are mainly

concerned about physical health, i.e. avoiding illness, having no physical health problems,

other people are more involved with psychological health-aspects such as emotional well-

being, enjoying life or social responsibility. In their recent study on health audience

segmentation, Geeroms et al. (2005b) apply a conceptualization of health that goes beyond

the physical level and also takes into account these psychological dimensions of health

concern. From a general taxonomy of consumer motives (Callebaut et al., 1999), they derive

29
This section comprises a brief recapitulation of the major findings and developments of chapter VI of this dissertation, in order to refresh
the reader’s mind regarding these issues. For a full explanation of the basic statements made in this section we thus refer to this
corresponding chapter.

245
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

a health-related motive structure, determined by two basic bipolar dimensions, i.e. an

intrapersonal versus interpersonal axis (see Fig. 7.1).

Figure 7.1 : Geeroms et al.’s Health-related Motive Structure

From an intrapersonal point of view, health is supposed to be dealt with in an unrestricted,

emotional way with a focus on enjoyment and freedom as opposed to a more controlled,

functional way of dealing with health focusing on physical well-being only. In addition, from

an interpersonal perspective, an altruistic, normative way of dealing with health could be

discovered with a focus on social responsibility, in contrast to a more ego-centered

interpretation of health, focusing on feelings of independence of others and activity. Out of

the dynamic interaction of both dimensions, five distinct health audience segments were

246
Chapter VII : Consumer Motives and Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

obtained with different health-related motive orientations, named ‘Energetic Experimenters’,

‘Harmonious Enjoyers’, ‘Normative Carers’, ‘Rationalists’ and ‘Conscious Experts. A brief

explanation of the fundamental meaning of health for each of the five segments is provided in

Table 7.1.

Table 7.1 : Five Health Audience Segments

Energetic Experimenters: for these people, health is mainly about physical activity (i.e.
practicing sports), exploring life and having energy to do the things they want to do.

Harmonious Enjoyers : for these people, health is mainly about emotional well-being,
enjoying life and feeling good socially

Normative Carers : for these people, health is mainly about physical well-being (i.e.
avoiding illness) and taking care of the health of others (i.e. social responsibility)

Rationalists : for these people, health is mainly about physical well-being and functional
competence (i.e. being able to work functionally, having job competence)

Conscious Experts : for these people, health is mainly about managing their body and
their appearance in the best possible way (i.e. looking good, staying slim), showing off by
outward appearance
Source: Geeroms et al. (2005b)

In the study of Geeroms et al. (2005b) significant differences in consumption patterns were

found between the different health segments as well as differences in reactions toward

motive-related health advertisements. As mentioned above, an objective of present study is to

test the relevance of this new segmentation paradigm in the context of ready meal

consumption behavior. A first hypothesis can be formulated as follows:

H1: Five consumer segments will exist with different health-related motive orientations,

corresponding to the health audience segments revealed by Geeroms et al. (2005b), i.e.

247
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Energetic Experimenters, Harmonious Enjoyers, Normative Carers, Rationalists and

Conscious Experts.

2.2. Relationship between health-related motive orientations and general food

behavior

In literature an obvious relationship has been established between food habits and consumer

health (e.g. Gilbert, 2000; GfK, 2002; IFIC, 2000; WHO, 1990). As dietary patterns are

considered as important contributors to the possible development of chronic and lifestyle

diseases, such as cancers, cardiovascular diseases, obesity or osteoporosis (e.g. Guinot et al.,

2001; Story et al., 2002), concerns about physical health – together with price, convenience

and sensory appeal (e.g. taste) – are believed to be one of the most important determinants of

food choice (e.g. Mothersbaugh et al., 1993; Steptoe et al., 1995; Olsen, 2003). When

applying a conceptualization of health beyond the physical level, also psychological aspects

of health concern may be assumed to have an impact on consumer food behavior in general.

As different behavioral strategies will be appropriate in the context of different health-related

motives that are manifest (e.g. Geeroms et al., 2005b; Bagozzi and Dholakia, 1999),

differences in health-related motive orientations could be expected to cause differences in

general food patterns and behaviors. In general, we formulate our second hypothesis in the

following way:

H2: Significant differences in general food behavior patterns will exist among consumer

segments with different health-related motive orientations.

248
Chapter VII : Consumer Motives and Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

Though the existing literature provides little insight into any truly configurable behavioral

differences among different health audience segments, some plausible and meaningful

relationships could be proposed between health-related motive orientations and general food

patterns and behaviors, based on a deduction from knowledge about the fundamental meaning

of health for each of the five hypothesized segments (i.e. Table 7.1). For example, a

predominant concern about emotional well-being, enjoying life or exploration may be

positively related to food-related lifestyle dimensions such as novelty, taste, looking for new

ways and social relationships (e.g. Brunsø and Grunert, 1995) and the perception of food as a

source of enjoyment and creativity (e.g. Synovate/Censydiam, 2002) . In contrast, a more

restricted interpretation of health may translate into a more controlled way of dealing with

food, with a focus on security and tradition and the perception of food as a source of

functionality. Hence, based on a general understanding of the health-related motive

orientations of the five health segments, in Table 7.2, some exploratory propositions are

generated regarding the differences among these segments with respect to the general food

behavior measures that are included in this study (cf. infra: section 3.2.2.).

249
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Table 7.2 : Proposed Differences in General Food Behavior Patterns (P2.1-P2.28)

Energetic Harmonious Normative Conscious Rationalists


Experimenters Enjoyers Carers Experts
Food-related lifestyle
dimensions
Shopping
P2.1. Enjoyment from shopping X X
P2.2. Speciality shops X
P2.3. Price criteria X X
Quality aspects
P2.4. Naturalness X X
P2.5. Price/quality relation X X
P2.6. Novelty X X
P2.7. Organic products X X
P2.8. Taste X X
P2.9. Freshness X X
Cooking
P2.10. Interest in cooking X X
P2.11. Looking for new ways X X
P2.12. Whole family X
P2.13. Planning X X X
Consumption situations
P2.14. Social event X
Purchase motives
P2.15. Self-fulfilment in food X X
P2.16. Security X X X
P2.17. Social relationships X
Food perception
dimensions
P2.18. Creativity X X
P2.19. Enjoyment X X
P2.20. Efficiency/showing off X
P2.21.. Functionality X X X
P2.22. Familiarity and tradition X X X
P2.23. Care-taking role X
General dietary patterns
P2.24. Trendy X
P2.25. Sweet X
P2.26. Healthy X
P2.27. Traditional X X
P2.28. Hedonistic X
X = Compared to the other segments, these health segments are assumed to score significantly higher on a
particular food behavior dimension as meaningful relationships could be expected between these segments’
health-related motive orientations and the behavioral dimension in question.

250
Chapter VII : Consumer Motives and Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

2.3. Relationship between health-related motive orientations and ready meal

consumption behavior

At least two different streams of research exist with the objective of explaining consumer

convenience food behavior. First, an econometric tradition tries to explain variation in ready

meal consumption behavior based on socio-economic or demographic determinants such as

employment status, household size, income level, perceived time pressure and role-overload

(e.g. Verlegh and Candel, 1999; Bonke, 1996; Darian and Tucci, 1992). Though the majority

of studies dealing with convenience in meal preparation can be situated within this

econometric tradition, these studies were unable to convincingly demonstrate the existence of

meaningful relationships between economic determinants and the choice of ready meal

solutions (e.g. Reilly, 1982; Candel, 2001). A second stream of research focuses on the

impact of internal psychological constructs on ready meal consumption behavior such as

knowledge, beliefs, attitudes, social norms and other higher-level motivations (e.g. Mahon et

al., 2006; Costa et al., 2005; Milburn, 1995). Despite a few exceptions (e.g. Olsen, 2003;

Scholderer and Grunert, 2005), this psychological tradition excludes the role of external or

demographical variables for both methodological and theoretical reasons (e.g. Ajzen and

Fishbein, 1980).

Within the context of the latter tradition, the present study explores the role of underlying

health-related motive orientations in influencing ready meal consumption behavior. It is

assumed that different ready meal product features and/or benefits will be preferred in the

context of different health-related motives (cf. MEC-theory, Olson & Reynolds, 2001) and

hence that differences in consumption patterns will exist among members of different health

audience segments. Based on the above reasoning, we formulate our third hypothesis:

251
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

H3: Significant differences in ready meal consumption patterns will exist among consumer

segments with different health-related motive orientations.

From our basic knowledge about the fundamental meaning of health for the five hypothesized

audience segments (i.e. Table 7.1), we could deduce some meaningful relationships between

health-related motive orientations and ready meal consumption patterns. For example, people

who deal with health in an individualistic way may perceive ready meals as more positively

and their consumption as more desirable, whereas people holding an altruistic interpretation

of health may experience more guilt feelings regarding ready meal consumption, as in the

latter case spending less time and energy to cooking daily meals might be seen as neglecting

one’s duty toward relevant others (e.g. Costa et al., 2005). Again, as shown in Table 7.3,

some exploratory propositions are derived from our health-related understanding of the five

segments, regarding the differences among these segments with respect to the ready meal

consumption behavior measures under question (cf. infra: section 3.2.3.):

252
Chapter VII : Consumer Motives and Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

Table 7.3 : Proposed Differences in Ready Meal Consumption Patterns (P3.1-P3.44)

Energetic Harmonious Normative Conscious Rationalists


Experimenters Enjoyers Carers Experts
Ready meal consumption
behavior
Attitudes, beliefs and behavior
P3.1. Attitude toward ready meals X X
P3.2. Beliefs about ready meals X X
P3.3. Consumption frequency X X

Buying criteria importance


P3.4. Nutritional value X X
P3.5. Taste X X
P3.6. Exclusiveness X
P3.7. Price X X
P3.8. Healthiness X X
P3.9. Appearance X
P3.10. Brand X
P3.11. Quantity X X
P3.12. Shelf life date X X
P3.13. Freshness X X
P3.14. Package X
P3.15. Ease of using X X
P3.16. Environmental effects X X
P3.17. Ingredients X X
Ways of dealing with ready meals
P3.18. Variation X X
P3.19. Enjoyment X X
P3.20. Feelings of guilt X X
P3.21. Relaxation X X
P3.22. Sophistication/Low calorie X
P3.23. Time saving X X
P3.24. Functionality X X X
Attributes ideal ready meal
P3.25. Healthiness X X
P3.26. Modern Oriental X X X
P3.27. Traditional Oriental X X
P3.28. Sophisticated X
P3.29. Traditional X X
P3.30. Quick to prepare X X
P3.31. Low calorie amount X
P3.32. Modern Mediterranean X X X
P3.33. Traditional Mediterranean X X
P3.34. Freshness X X
P3.35. Trendy X X
P3.36. Substantial X X
P3.37. Easy to prepare/clean up X X
P3.38. Steamed X
P3.39. Vegetarian X
P3.40. Relieve hunger X X
P3.41. Well-known recipe X X
P3.42. Stylish package X
P3.43. Variety X
P3.44. Quality X X
X = Compared to the other segments, these health segments are hypothesized to score significantly higher on a
particular ready meal consumption behavior dimension, as meaningful relationships could be expected between
these segments’ health-related motive orientations and the behavioral dimension in question.

253
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

By answering the above research issues the present study aims to contribute to the study of

consumer behavior at least in two ways. First, by also taking into account several

psychological dimensions of health concern when considering consumer food patterns and

behaviors (e.g. emotional well-being, social responsibility, etc.), we want to assist in a better

understanding of food-health relationships beyond the level of physical health only. Second,

we want to provide an insight into the important health-related aspects that influence people’s

ready meal consumption patterns. These insights can contribute to improving consumer-

oriented product development, positioning and marketing of products belonging to this

particular food category.

3. Method

3.1. Participants

Data were collected by means of an on-line survey method. A prerequisite to participate in

the study was being involved in meal preparation and food shopping, at least from time to

time. By responding to a link to our questionnaire on the website of a widespread national

newspaper, a sample of 1934 subjects provided valid responses for this research (see Table

7.4 for an overview of the sample characteristics).

254
Chapter VII : Consumer Motives and Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

Table 7.4 : Sample Characteristics (% of respondents, N = 1934)

Sample Population*
Gender
Male 46.3 48.9
Female 53.7 51.1
Age
<25 23.0 18.4
25-40 47.8 32.8
40-50 20.3 22.6
>50 8.9 26.2
Mean 36.8
Education
Lower education (age <=18) 30.5 67.4
Higher education (age >18) 69.5 32.6
Profession
Labourer 8.0
Employee 35.9
Executive 5.4
Self-employed 5.6
Functionary 11.9
Keeping house 5.4
Retired 5.5
Unemployed 2.5
Student 16.1
Other… 3.7
Relationship status
Single 27.3 27.1
Relationship/married 72.7 72.1
Children in family
No (=no children) 39.7 35.1
Yes (=children) 60.3 64.9
Employment Status
Part time employed 12.9
Full time employed 58.0
Not applicable 29.1

*Source:NIS(2005). Private Households Statistics. Brussels: NIS, National


Institute for Statistics

The mean age of the respondents was 36.8 years with a standard deviation of 12.7. In

comparison to the Belgian population statistics (NIS, 2005), our sample was slightly biased

255
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

toward younger age, which could be explained by the fact that the internet was used as a

recruitment platform and/or that the chance of winning an iPod nano by participation had

higher attractive value for younger, compared to older people. Maybe due to the same

reasons of recruitment, people with higher education were overrepresented in the sample.

Compared to a population estimate of 32.6%, in our sample as much as 69.5% of persons had

schooling beyond the age of 18. With respect to gender, 46.3% males and 53.7% females

participated in the study, which is rather representative for the population distribution. In

addition, our sample was representative in terms of other relevant variables such as

relationship status and presence of children in the family. In terms of profession and

employment status we can conclude that a wide range of socio-economic classes of the

population took part in our survey, including labourers, employees, students, retired people,

executives, part time/full time employers, etc.

3.2. Measures

3.2.1. Health-related motive orientations

People’s health-related motive orientations were assessed by means of the 45-item Health-

related Motive Scale developed by Geeroms et al. (2005b). This scale consists of 15 explicit

and 30 implicit items about the fundamental meaning of health, which were rated on a seven-

point Likert scale ranging from 1= completely disagree to 7= completely agree. As scale

development was based on the application of Callebaut et al.’s (1999) consumer motive

taxonomy in a health context30, initially eight health dimensions were supposed to underlie

the scale, corresponding to the eight motive positions in the taxonomy. However, previous

30
Based on desk research and exhaustive qualitative research, all eight positions in the general taxonomy of consumer motives were
translated in a limited number of health statements that made up the items in the health-related motive scale (see Geeroms et al., 2005b).

256
Chapter VII : Consumer Motives and Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

analyses suggest that only six reliable constructs can be discovered, comprising only 34 of the

health-related motive items (cf. supra: chapter VI). In order to obtain satisfying reliability

estimates for all eight health dimensions, items that were problematic in previous analyses

(N=11) were revised in the context of present study (see Appendix A for the revised version

of the scale). As shown in Table 7.5 the revisions were successful as in present analysis all

Cronbach’s α measures exceeded the critical value of .60. Hence, scores of each item were

summed and averaged to represent the corresponding construct. These eight composite

construct measures served as the basis for all further analyses.

Table 7.5 : Eight Health Constructs

Health Construct Number Item Labels* Cronbach’s


of Items α
Health = Energy 7 Exp1, Exp2, Exp6 .67
(i.e. vitality-motive) Imp5, Imp10, Imp17, Imp29

Health = Enjoying life 4 Imp9, Imp13, Imp19, Imp24 .68


(i.e. pleasure-motive)

Health = Emotional well-being 7 Exp3, Exp13, Exp14 .71


(i.e. conviviality-motive) Imp4, Imp15, Imp21, Imp23

Health = Social Responsibility 5 Exp4, Exp7, .67


(i.e. belonging-motive) Imp1, Imp12, Imp25

Health = Physical well-being 7 Exp5, Exp8, Exp11 .64


(i.e. security-motive) Imp2, Imp8, Imp11, Imp22

Health = Functionality 4 Imp3, Imp18, Imp26, Imp28 .62


(i.e. control-motive)

Health = Outward appearance 8 Exp9, Exp10, Exp12,Exp15, .82


(i.e. recognition-motive) Imp7, Imp14, Imp16, Imp30

Health = Management 3 Imp6, Imp20, Imp27 .73


(i.e. power-motive)

*For the full text item labels see Appendix A

257
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

3.2.2. General food behavior

In our questionnaire three scales were used to measure people’s general food patterns and

behaviors. First, respondents were asked to answer 51 items of the FRL instrument (Brunsø

and Grunert, 1995), corresponding to 17 food-related lifestyle dimensions in five major life

domains, i.e. shopping, cooking, food quality aspects, consumption situations and purchase

motives 31 . Each dimension consisted of three individual items (statements) that were

translated to Dutch by means of back-translation and had to be answered on seven-point

scales ranging from 1= completely disagree to 7= completely agree. Scores on a dimension

consisted of the average sum score of the three items belonging to that dimension. Reliability

tests using Cronbach’s α were conducted on all dimensions and are shown in Table 7.6.

Second, six food perception dimensions were included in the questionnaire, indicating the

psychological meaning of food for consumers (Synovate/Censydiam, 2002). Again, each

dimension was measured by three individual items, scored on seven-point Likert scales. After

assessing reliability estimates (see also Table 7.6), composite construct measures were

computed for the six dimensions that were used in further analyses.

Finally, five items were derived from Newby and Tucker (2004) to measure the general

dietary profile of the respondents. On seven-point Likert type scales subjects indicated the

extent to which their general dietary pattern could be considered as trendy, sweet, healthy,

traditional or hedonistic. For a brief description of each of these dietary patterns see Table 7.6.

31
The original instrument of Brunsø and Grunert (1995) consisted of 69 items, comprising 23 FRL-dimensions but in order to reduce
questionnaire length, six dimensions that were considered as less relevant in the context of present study were not included in the survey.

258
Chapter VII : Consumer Motives and Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

Table 7.6 : General Food Behavior

Brief description of the dimensions Cronbach’s α


Food-related lifestyle dimensions
Shopping
1. Enjoyment from shopping The enjoyment one gets from shopping .86
2. Speciality shops The desire to shop in speciality shops, get advice .68
3. Price criteria Price sensitivity of an individual .62

Quality aspects
4. Naturalness The importance of natural, healthy food .85
5. Price/quality relation The importance of getting value for money .73
6. Novelty The importance of the aspect of novelty .79
7. Organic products The importance of organic food .87
8. Taste The importance of a good taste .64
9. Freshness The importance of fresh food .75

Cooking
10. Interest in cooking How much is one interested in cooking? .86
11. Looking for new ways Does one experiment in the kitchen? .87
12. Whole family Involvement of the family in meal preparation .80
13. Planning Whether the dinner is planned or last minute .72

Consumption situations
14. Social event The importance of social events like dinner parties, .76
restaurant visits with family or friends

Purchase motives
15. Self-fulfilment in food Getting self-esteem from cooking .70
16. Security Familiarity with foods gives a sense of security .74
17. Social relationships Socialising over a dinner is important .65

Food perception dimensions


1. Creativity Perception of food as a source of creativity, .88
exploration and improvisation
2. Enjoyment Perception of food as a source of enjoyment, fun .91
3. Efficiency/showing off Perception of food in terms of efficiency and .71
showing off
4. Functionality Considering food only for functional reasons, i.e. .79
because the body needs nutrition
5. familiarity and tradition Keeping traditions in relation to food and getting .84
security from preparing familiar foods
6. Care-taking role Perception of food as a source of taking care of the .78
family

General dietary patterns


1. Trendy Eating often trendy food, i.e. cornflakes, soft drinks,
delicacies, sandwich rolls…
2. Sweet Eating often sweet things, i.e. jam, honey, cake,
cookies, chocolate…
3. Healthy Eating often healthy food, i.e. fruits, vegetables, fish,
thee…
4. Traditional Eating often traditional food, i.e. 3-component
meals, bread, soup….
5. Hedonistic Eating often hedonistic food, i.e. red meat, cream
sauces, desserts, alcoholic beverages, coffee…

259
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

3.2.3. Ready meal consumption behavior

The last part of our questionnaire comprised six questions measuring ready meal consumption

behavior. First, four items measured attitudes toward ready meals (e.g. “in my opinion eating

ready meals is good”). Each item was measured on a seven-point Likert scale, ranging from

1= completely disagree to 7= completely agree. The Cronbach’s α coefficient for the multi-

item attitude construct was .92.

Actual consumption behavior was measured by six items, which were measured on a seven-

point frequency scale where 1= every day or almost everyday, 2= 3-4 times a week, 3= 1-2

times a week, 4= 1-3 times a month, 5= 1-5 times every six months, 6= less frequently and 7=

never (Mahon et al., 2006). The questions related to frequency of consumption of different

categories of ready meals, i.e. frozen, hydrated, canned, bottled, fresh from supermarket and

fresh from delicatessen. Total ready meal consumption frequency was obtained by summing

consumption frequency scores of the various categories and therefore ranged from 6 to 42,

with lower scores indicating a higher consumption of ready meals.

Third, we used a list of 11 items to measure respondents’ beliefs about ready meals. This list

contained statements like “ready meals are nutritious” or “ready meals are inexpensive” and

was based on preliminary research on convenience foods, and on earlier studies into the role

of convenience in food consumption (e.g. Brinberg and Durand, 1983; Miller and Ginter,

1979; Verlegh and Candel, 1999). Respondents were asked to indicate their level of

agreement with each of these 11 statements on seven-point Likert scales, ranging from 1=

completely disagree to 7= completely agree.

260
Chapter VII : Consumer Motives and Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

In addition, respondents had to indicate the importance of different criteria when buying ready

meals on a five-point Likert scale, ranging from 1= not at all important to 5= very important.

These criteria were elicited based on previous work of Candel (2001) and additional insights

from literature.

Fifth, seven dimensions were included measuring a number of different ways of handling

ready meals (Synovate/Censydiam, 2001). Each dimension consisted of three statements,

rated again on seven-point Likert scales. Before computing average sum scores reliability

estimates were assessed for all dimensions and are provided in Table 7.7, as well as a brief

explanation of each dimension.

Table 7.7 : Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

Brief description of the dimensions Cronbach’s α


Ways of dealing with ready meals
1. Variation Ready meals are dealt with as a source of variation .61
and change
2. Enjoyment Ready meals are dealt with as a source of enjoyment .69
3. Feelings of guilt Feelings of guilt are associated with the consumption .79
of ready meals
4. Relaxation Ready meals are dealt with as a source of .80
relaxation/withdrawal after a busy day
5. Sophistication/Low calorie Looking for sophisticated recipes with a low amount .60
of calories
6. Time saving Ready meals are dealt with as a time saving element .67
within modern life
7. Functionality Ready meals are consumed only for .58
practical/functional reasons ( no dishes anymore)

Finally, a last question dealt with consumer perception of the ideal ready meal. A list of

product attributes was derived from literature and previous qualitative research

(Synovate/Censydiam, 2003). Respondents were asked to indicate the degree to which each

attribute is applicable to their ideal ready meal. The ratings were provided on a seven-point

scale, ranging from 1= not at all likely to 7= very likely.

261
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Table 7.8 : Means and Standard Deviations for a Five-cluster Solution

Health Constructs (*) Total Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 Cluster 4 Cluster 5


sample (Energetic (Harmonious (Normative (Conscious (Rationalists)
Experimenters) Enjoyers) Carers) Experts)

Health = Energy 4.9 (.87) 5.9 (.59) 5.1 (.78) 4.2 (.71) 5.2 (.67) 4.3 (.68)
(i.e. vitality-motive)

Health = Enjoying life 5.2 (.92) 5.5 (.71) 5.9 (.74) 5.0 (.85) 4.8 (.86) 4.7 (.84)
(i.e. pleasure-motive)

Health = Emotional well-being 5.1 (.81) 5.5 (.70) 5.8 (.60) 5.1 (.65) 4.7 (.80) 4.6 (.75)
(i.e. conviviality-motive)

Health = Social responsibility 5.1 (.91) 4.8 (.94) 5.3 (.83) 5.6 (.74) 4.8 (.94) 5.2 (.82)
(i.e. belonging-motive)

Health = Physical well-being 5.2 (.72) 5.0 (.71) 5.1 (.70) 5.6 (.57) 5.2 (.74) 5.3 (.59)
(i.e. security-motive)

Health = Functionality 5.3 (.94) 5.3 (.87) 4.8 (.91) 5.3 (.81) 5.3 (.88) 5.9 (.70)
(i.e. control-motive)

Health = Outward appearance 3.8 (1.21) 4.1 (.84) 2.5 (.78) 2.9 (.86) 5.8 (.83) 3.9 (..94)
(i.e. recognition-motive)

Health = Management 3.6 (1.41) 4.3 (1.09) 2.4 (1.01) 2.3 (.86) 5.7 (.99) 3.3 (1.04)
(i.e. power-motive)

N= 1934 N=368 N=537 N=364 N=409 N=256


(*) For the items of the scales for the health constructs:
- 1 corresponds to ‘completely disagree’
- 7 corresponds to ‘completely agree’

262
Chapter VII : Consumer Motives and Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

4. Results

4.1. Health Audience Segments

In response to our first hypothesis, a two-step cluster analysis (hierarchical clustering using

Ward’s method, followed by a K-means clustering) was performed on the eight health

dimensions computed before. As expected, the results of this analysis revealed an appropriate

five-cluster solution comparable to the five health segments discovered by Geeroms et al.

(2005b). Hence, H1 was fully supported.

As shown in Table 7.8, Cluster 1 represents the ‘Energetic Experimenters’ as variable mean

scores for this cluster indicate an interpretation of health mainly in terms of vitality and

energy (M= 5.9). Statements such as ‘keeping the body in a good condition’, ‘having energy’

and ‘living an active life’ are among the most important health associations for members of

this cluster (see Fig. 7.2 for an overview of cluster-item associations).

Figure 7.2 : Cluster-Health Item Associations (results of correspondence analysis)

263
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

‘Energetic Experimenters’ represent the youngest segment (Mean Age= 29.8) with an

overrepresentation of males (55.7%) and singles (43.1%). 72.8% of them have no children in

the family. This first segment accounts for about 19.0% of our total sample.

Cluster 2 subjects (27.8% of the sample) represent the ‘Harmonious Enjoyers’ as they mainly

interpret health in terms of enjoying life (M=5.9) and emotional well-being (M= 5.8).

Important health-related motive orientations for these people are reflected in individual

statements such as ‘being cheerful’, ‘having fun with others’ and ‘keeping up good social

contacts’. ‘Harmonious Enjoyers’ have a mean age of 35.8 with a standard deviation of 12.04.

Compared to the total sample, this segment is rather representative in terms of gender,

education, profession, relationship/employment status and presence of children in the family.

Members of Cluster 3 perceive health as a social responsibility (M= 5.6) and they are mainly

concerned about physical well-being (M= 5.6). Their health-related motive orientations

comprise statements such as ‘protecting the body against harmful influences’, ‘respecting

public health norms and prescriptions’ and ‘taking care of other family members’ health’.

This segment accounts for 18.8% of the sample and obviously represents the ‘Normative

Carers’. Clearly, women are overrepresented (64.8%) as well as people keeping the house, i.e.

housewives/househusbands (10.2%). ‘Normative Carers’ are slightly older with a mean age

of 43.3 and 76.4% of them have a family with children.

The fourth segment represents the ‘Conscious Experts’ as mean values are highest for the

health dimensions of Management (M= 5.7) and Outward appearance (M= 5.8). Health-

related motive orientations are characterized by statements such as ‘looking good’, ‘staying

slim’, ‘being successful’, ‘being ambitious’ ‘being powerful’ and ‘being stylish’. Members of

264
Chapter VII : Consumer Motives and Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

this segment could be characterized as young professionals (Mean Age= 34.4), the majority of

them having no children in the family (60.0%). ‘Conscious Experts’ represent 21.1% of the

total sample.

Finally, ‘Rationalists’ (i.e. Cluster 5) take a position between ‘Conscious Experts’ and

‘Normative Carers’. Similar to Cluster 3 subjects, ‘Rationalists’ are slightly older (Mean

Age= 43.5) and most of them have a family to care for (72.3%). However, less

housewives/househusbands could be detected within Cluster 5 (4.3%) as the majority of its

members have a full time job outside home (61.5%). Hence, health is perceived mainly in

terms of functionality (M= 5.9), with a focus on organizing life between professional work

and family. Individual statements such as ‘working functionally’, ‘performing my job my

own way’ and ‘getting things under control’ are reflecting important health-related motive

orientations for this particular segment. About 13.2% of our sample belongs to Cluster 5.

265
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Table 7.9 : ANOVA results General Food Behavior

Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 Cluster 4 Cluster 5 P-value


(EE) (HE) (NC) (CE) (R) (F-test)
Food-related lifestyle
dimensions (*)
Shopping
1. Enjoyment from shopping 4.6 4.8 4.7 4.6 4.5 n.s.
2. Speciality shops 2.9 2.9 2.9 3.0 2.9 n.s.
3. Price criteria 4.0a 4.3b 4.8c 4.1ab 4.7c 0.000**

Quality aspects
4. Naturalness 3.6a 3.7a 4.2b 3.8a 4.1b 0.000**
5. Price/quality relation 5.1ab 5.3b 5.6c 5.0a 5.6c 0.000**
6. Novelty 4.9b 4.8b 4.3a 4.3a 4.1a 0.000**
7. Organic products 3.2 3.3 3.2 3.3 3.2 n.s.
8. Taste 5.9b 6.0b 6.1b 5.5a 5.9b 0.000**
9. Freshness 5.5a 5.9b 5.8b 5.3a 5.8b 0.000**

Cooking
10. Interest in cooking 4.5b 4.7b 4.5b 4.0a 4.0a 0.000**
11. Looking for new ways 4.5c 4.6c 4.0ab 4.1b 3.8a 0.000**
12. Whole family 4.4 4.5 4.3 4.3 4.3 n.s.
13. Planning 3.4a 3.4a 3.9b 3.8b 4.0b 0.000**

Consumption situations
14. Social event 4.9b 4.9b 4.4a 4.4a 4.2a 0.000**

Purchase motives
15. Self-fulfilment in food 4.4 4.4 4.3 4.3 4.2 n.s.
16. Security 3.5a 3.4a 4.1b 4.0b 4.2b 0.000**
17. Social relationships 5.1b 5.5c 5.1b 4.8a 4.7a 0.000**

Food perception
Dimensions (*)
1. Creativity 4.8b 4.7b 4.2a 4.2a 4.1a 0.000**
2. Enjoyment 4.8ab 5.0b 4.7ab 4.5a 4.5a 0.000**
3. Efficiency/showing off 4.0b 3.7a 4.0b 4.1b 4.0b 0.000**
4. Functionality 2.9b 2.6a 2.8ab 3.5c 3.3c 0.000**
5. Familiarity and tradition 3.9a 3.9a 4.5b 4.3b 4.5b 0.000**
6. Care-taking role 4.3a 4.8b 5.2c 4.5a 4.9b 0.000**

General dietary patterns (*)


1. Trendy 4.4d 3.5b 3.1a 4.0c 3.2a 0.000**
2. Sweet 4.0 4.0 3.8 3.9 3.8 n.s.
3. Healthy 4.9 5.0 5.1 5.0 5.1 n.s.
4. Traditional 4.8a 5.2b 5.6c 5.2b 5.5c 0.000**
5. Hedonistic 3.8b 3.9b 3.4a 3.6ab 3.4a 0.000**

Note: The superscripts indicate significantly different means (p<.05) where the alphabetical order of the
superscripts represents the ascending order of the means. Tukey’s honestly significant difference test has been
applied to assess significance between the segments.
** p<.01
(*) For the items of the scales measuring these dimensions:
- 1 corresponds to ‘completely disagree’
- 7 corresponds to ‘completely agree’

266
Chapter VII : Consumer Motives and Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

4.2. General Food Behavior

Concerning hypothesis 2, overall MANOVA results revealed a significant effect of health-

related motive orientations on people’s general food patterns and behaviors (i.e. F(17, 1916)=

18.83, p< .01 for the FRL dimensions; F(6, 1927)= 37.61, p< .01 for the food perception

dimensions and F(5, 1928)= 41.48, p< .01 for the general dietary patterns). When

considering the univariate ANOVA tests (see Table 7.9), 12 food-related lifestyle dimensions,

all food perception dimensions and three dietary profiles appeared significantly different

among the five health segments. Whereas some minor deviations could be detected from

what was proposed a priori (cf. supra: Table 7.2), broadly taken, the found relationships

between health-related motive orientations and general food behavior patterns all went in the

hypothesized directions.

As expected, ‘Energetic Experimenters’ (Cluster 1) and ‘Harmonious Enjoyers’ (Cluster 2)

are more concerned with novelty, get more enjoyment from trying and preparing new recipes

and see food more as a social event. In addition, for ‘Harmonious Enjoyers’ socialising over

a dinner is very important and they have a stronger preference for fresh products. In contrast,

consumers from Cluster 3 (‘Normative Carers’), Cluster 4 (‘Conscious Experts’) and Cluster

5 (‘Rationalists’) are seeking greater security from foods and they plan more in advance what

they are going to eat. In particular, ‘Normative Carers’ and ‘Rationalists’ are more concerned

about price criteria and the relationship between quality and price and they consider freshness

and naturalness as very important food quality dimensions. Compared to other segments,

‘Conscious Experts’ and ‘Rationalists’ are less interested in cooking and do not like to spend

much time in the kitchen. Additionally, ‘Conscious Experts’ seem to attach lower importance

to taste as a quality dimension.

267
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

The pattern of mean differences for the food perception dimensions was very similar to the

pattern of mean differences for the food-related lifestyles. Whereas members of Cluster1

(‘Energetic Experimenters’) and Cluster 2 (‘Harmonious Enjoyers’) mainly perceive food as a

source of creativity, improvisation, exploration and enjoyment, ‘Normative Carers’,

‘Conscious Experts’ and ‘Rationalists’ express more the desire to keep traditions in relation to

cooking habits and get security from preparing familiar foods. More than the other segments,

‘Conscious Experts’ and ‘Rationalists’ perceive food preparation as a source of functionality

and routine, whereas taking care of others through the serving of good food is at the centre of

food perception for ‘Normative Carers’.

With regard to general dietary patterns, a trendy food pattern is most common among

‘Energetic Experimenters’, whereas traditional dietary profiles could be found among

‘Normative Carers’ and ‘Rationalists’, who are also less prone on hedonistic dietary patterns.

4.3. Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

Concerning the impact of health-related motive orientations on ready meal consumption

behavior (i.e. Hypothesis 3), several significant differences were found between the five

health segments with regard to the six behavioral questions under study. Again, the found

relationships all broadly corresponded to our basic propositions made a priori (cf. supra:

Table 7.3).

First, differences in attitude toward ready meals and ready meal consumption frequency were

investigated via one-way ANOVA. A sample mean score of 3.5 on the attitude construct

reflects a rather negative attitude toward ready meals in general. However, when considering

268
Chapter VII : Consumer Motives and Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

the five segments separately, ‘Energetic Experimenters’ and ‘Conscious Experts’ show

significantly higher attitude measures compared to the other segments, with mean scores of

respectively 3.9 and 3.8. In addition, these two segments report significantly higher

frequency levels for both category-specific and total ready meal consumption, as expected

(see Table 7.10).

With regard to the remaining questions, overall MANOVA results indicated a significant

effect of health-related motive orientations on respectively beliefs about ready meals (F(11,

1922)= 11.96, p< .01), importance of ready meal buying criteria (F(14, 1919)= 10.46, p< .01),

different ways of handling ready meals (F(7, 1927)= 13.29, p< .01) and the perception of the

ideal ready meal (F(20, 1913)= 11.47, p< .01).

Table 7.11 highlights univariate differences in beliefs concerning ready meals between the

five cluster groups. No significant differences could be detected between the segments with

regard to convenience-related beliefs. To the same extent, subjects hold the opinion that

ready meals are easy to prepare (M= 5.9), cause few dishes (M= 5.6), can be bought

everywhere (M= 5.2) and can be stored for a long time (M= 4.4). However, significant

differences were found concerning beliefs about other attributes such as price, taste and

presence of additives. Though general beliefs about these attributes are rather weak,

‘Energetic Experimenters’ and ‘Conscious Experts’ believe to a greater extent as compared to

the other segments that ready meals are not expensive, contain few additives, are something

special, have a good taste and are not harmful for one’s figure.

In order to identify underlying dimensions among ready meal buying criteria, factor analysis

(principle component analysis) was performed based on the importance scores. The results of

269
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

this analysis are shown in Table 7.12. The factor explaining most variance (28.9%) contains

several credence criteria such as nutritional value, healthiness, shelf life date, environmental

effects and ingredients. Sensory criteria such as taste, appearance and freshness load

consistently on a second factor, followed by two other factors including mainly search criteria

either relating to showing off (i.e. exclusiveness, brand name and package style) or

functionality (i.e. price, quantity and ease of using). Table 7.13 shows univariate ANOVA

tests for differences in importance scores between members of the five health segments

regarding these four categories of buying criteria. As expected, ‘Normative Carers’ and

‘Rationalists’ perceive credence criteria (i.e. healthiness, nutritional value and shelf life date)

as more important compared to other segments and they are more concerned about price as a

functional criterion. Within the category of sensory criteria, these two segments consider

freshness as very important, as is the case for members of Cluster 2 (i.e. ‘Harmonious

Enjoyers’). In addition, ‘Harmonious Enjoyers’ together with ‘Energetic Experimenters’

attach greater importance to the taste of ready meals. Finally, as proposed a priori, showing

off criteria (i.e. exclusiveness and brand name) are perceived as more important by members

of Cluster 4, i.e. ‘Conscious Experts’.

With regard to different ways of handling ready meals, six dimensions were found to be

significantly different between our five segments (see Table 7.14). Whereas members of

Cluster 1 (‘Energetic Experimenters’) and Cluster 4 (‘Conscious Experts’) apply a more

unrestricted way of dealing with ready meals, considering ready meals more as a time saving

element and as a source of variation, enjoyment and relaxation, ‘Normative Carers’ and

‘Rationalists’ experience more guilt feelings about the consumption of ready meals. Together

with ‘Harmonious Enjoyers’, the latter segments consume ready meals only for

270
Chapter VII : Consumer Motives and Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

practical/functional reasons and see this type of home meal replacement only as a stopgap

solution when having no time for cooking themselves.

Finally, to get an insight into the dimensions underlying consumer perception of the ideal

ready meal, we performed a second factor analysis comprising the list of product attributes

that were included in the study. Results of this analysis revealed seven broad factors,

simultaneously accounting for about 40.0% of the variance (see Table 7.15). Again, a

credence factor could be detected containing attributes such as healthiness, variety and quality.

In contrast to our earlier findings, freshness loads on the same factor as the credence attributes,

denoting a slightly different interpretation of this quality aspect when perceiving the ideal

ready meal. Ethnic recipes of ready meals are grouped into two factors (factor 2 and factor 7),

respectively distinguishing Oriental dishes from Mediterranean dishes. Moreover, four

additional factors emerged out of the analysis including attributes respectively relating to

aspects of sophistication, tradition, functionality and low calorie. When comparing ideal

ready meal perceptions among the five health segments, Table 7.16 indicates that ‘Energetic

Experimenters’ and ‘Harmonious Enjoyers’ have greater preference for ethnic dishes and new

recipes as was expected, whereas the ideal ready meal of ‘Normative Carers’ and

‘Rationalists’ comprises more traditional dishes, scoring high on credence attributes such as

freshness and quality. Compared to the other segments, functionality aspects (i.e. quick to

prepare, easy to clean up) are rated higher by ‘Rationalists’ and ‘Conscious Experts’. In

addition, ‘Conscious Experts’ are guided more by the aspect of sophistication (i.e.

sophisticated dish, trendy dish, stylish package) and are more concerned about the amount of

calories within ready meals. For these people, their ideal ready meal comprises more

vegetarian and steamed dishes, as well as modern ethnic dishes such as sushi, dim-sum or

ravioli pesto.

271
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Table 7.10 : Attitude toward Ready Meals and Consumption Frequency

Total Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 Cluster 4 Cluster 5 P-value


sample (EE) (HE) (NC) (CE) (R) (F-test)
Attitude toward ready meals (*) 3.5 3.9b 3.4a 3.3a 3.8b 3.3a 0.000**

Category-specific ready meal consumption (**)


Frozen ready meals 4.9 4.6a 5.0b 5.2b 4.7a 5.2b 0.000**
Hydrated ready meals 5.7 5.5a 5.8b 5.8b 5.5a 5.9b 0.000**
Canned ready meals 5.9 5.8a 6.1b 6.2b 5.7a 6.1b 0.000**
Bottled ready meals 5.8 5.5a 5.9b 6.0b 5.5a 5.9b 0.000**
Ready meals fresh from supermarket 5.3 5.1a 5.4b 5.5b 5.1a 5.5b 0.000**
Ready meals fresh from delicatessen 5.7 5.6ab 5.8bc 5.9c 5.5a 6.0c 0.000**

Total ready meal consumption (***) 33.3 32.1a 34.0b 34.6b 32.0a 34.6b 0.000**

Note: The superscripts indicate significantly different means (p<.05) where the alphabetical order of the superscripts represents the ascending order of
the means. Tukey’s honestly significant difference test has been applied to assess significance between the segments.
** p<.01
(*)For the items of the scales measuring attitude toward ready meals:
- 1 corresponds to ‘completely disagree’
- 7 corresponds to ‘completely agree’
(**)For the items of the scales measuring consumption frequency:
- 1 corresponds to ‘every day or almost everyday’
- 7 corresponds to ‘never’
(***)Lower sum scores indicate a higher consumption frequency

272
Chapter VII : Consumer Motives and Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

Table 7.11 : ANOVA results Beliefs Ready Meals

Total Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 Cluster 4 Cluster 5 P-value


sample (EE) (HE) (NC) (CE) (R) (F-test)
Ready meals are easy to prepare (*) 5.9 5.8 6.0 6.0 5.8 5.8 n.s.
Ready meals contain no additives 2.2 3.2b 2.1a 2.0a 3.4b 2.1a 0.000**
Ready meals are not expensive 2.9 3.2c 2.8b 2.5a 3.1c 2.5a 0.000**
Ready meals are something special 2.6 2.7b 2.3a 2.3a 2.8b 2.4a 0.000**
Ready meals cause few dishes 5.6 5.5 5.6 5.7 5.5 5.6 n.s.
Ready meals are nutritious 3.1 3.2 3.0 3.0 3.2 3.1 n.s.
Ready meals can be stored for a long time 4.4 4.4 4.5 4.4 4.3 4.3 n.s.
Ready meals taste good 3.7 3.9b 3.5a 3.5a 3.8b 3.6a 0.000**
Ready meals are good for your figure 2.2 2.4b 2.1a 2.0a 2.5b 2.1a 0.000**
Ready meals can be bought everywhere 5.2 5.1 5.3 5.2 5.1 5.0 n.s.
Ready meals are sold in sufficiently large portions 4.1 4.1 4.2 4.0 4.0 4.0 n.s.

Note: The superscripts indicate significantly different means (p<.05) where the alphabetical order of the superscripts represents the ascending order of
the means. Tukey’s honestly significant difference test has been applied to assess significance between the segments.
** p<.01
(*) For the items of the scales measuring beliefs about ready meals:
- 1 corresponds to ‘completely disagree’
- 7 corresponds to ‘completely agree’

273
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Table 7.12 : Factor Analysis – Ready Meal Buying Criteria

Buying criteria Factor Exp. Var.


loading (%)
1 Nutritional value .82 28.9
Healthiness .80
Shelf life date .74
Effects upon the environment .70
Ingredients .62

2 Taste .77 10.3


Appearance .72
Freshness .65

3 Exclusiveness .75 9.9


Brand .71
Package .70

4 Price .74 6.8


Quantity .68
Ease of using .62

274
Chapter VII : Consumer Motives and Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

Table 7.13 : ANOVA results Buying Criteria

Total Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 Cluster 4 Cluster 5 P-value


sample (EE) (HE) (NC) (CE) (R) (F-test)
Credence criteria (*)
Nutritional value 3.6 3.3a 3.5a 3.9b 3.5a 3.8b 0.000**
Healthiness 3.7 3.5a 3.6a 4.0b 3.6a 3.9b 0.000**
Shelf life date 4.3 4.2a 4.2a 4.6b 4.1a 4.5b 0.000**
Effects upon the environment 3.0 2.9a 2.9a 3.3b 2.9a 3.0a 0.000**
Ingredients 3.9 3.8a 3.9a 4.2b 3.8a 3.9a 0.000**

Sensory criteria
Taste 4.3 4.5b 4.6b 4.2a 4.2a 4.2a 0.000**
Appearance 4.0 4.0 4.0 4.1 3.9 3.9 n.s.
Freshness 4.4 4.2a 4.6b 4.6b 4.1a 4.5b 0.000**

Showing off criteria


Exclusiveness 2.5 2.5a 2.3a 2.3a 2.9b 2.4a 0.000**
Brand 2.8 2.9b 2.6a 2.7a 3.2c 2.8ab 0.000**
Package 2.8 2.8 2.7 2.8 2.9 2.9 n.s.

Functional criteria
Price 3.8 3.6a 3.7a 4.0b 3.7a 4.0b 0.000***
Quantity 3.5 3.6 3.5 3.6 3.5 3.5 n.s.
Ease of using 3.7 3.7 3.7 3.8 3.7 3.7 n.s.

Note: The superscripts indicate significantly different means (p<.05) where the alphabetical order of the superscripts represents the ascending order of the means.
Tukey’s honestly significant difference test has been applied to assess significance between the segments.
** p<.01
(*) For the items of the scales measuring the importance of buying criteria:
- 1 corresponds to ‘not at all important’
- 5 corresponds to ‘very important’

275
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Table 7.14 : ANOVA results different Ways of Handling Ready Meals

Total Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 Cluster 4 Cluster 5 P-value


sample (EE) (HE) (NC) (CE) (R) (F-test)

Variation (*) 2.9 3.1b 2.7a 2.6a 3.1b 2.8a 0.000**


Enjoyment 2.6 2.8b 2.4a 2.4a 2.9b 2.5a 0.000**
Feelings of guilt 4.3 4.0a 4.1a 4.7b 4.2a 4.6b 0.000**
Relaxation/Withdrawal 3.0 3.2b 2.8a 2.8a 3.3b 2.9a 0.000**
Sophistication/Low calorie 3.3 3.4 3.2 3.2 3.4 3.3 n.s.
Time saving 3.0 3.2b 2.8a 2.8a 3.4b 2.9a 0.000**
Functionality 4.5 4.1a 4.6b 4.7b 4.3a 4.6b 0.000**

Note: The superscripts indicate significantly different means (p<.05) where the alphabetical order of the superscripts represents the ascending order of
the means. Tukey’s honestly significant difference test has been applied to assess significance between the segments.
** p<.01
(*) For the items of the scales measuring different ways of handling ready meals:
- 1 corresponds to ‘completely disagree’
- 7 corresponds to ‘completely agree’

276
Chapter VII : Consumer Motives and Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

Table 7.15 : Factor Analysis – Attributes Ideal Ready Meal

Attributes Factor Exp. Var.


loading (%)
1 Healthiness .79 26.0
Freshness .78
Variety .76
Quality .75

2 Modern Oriental dish (i.e. sushi, dim-sum…) .81 8.4


Traditional Oriental dish (i.e. nasi or bami goreng…) .71

3 Sophisticated dish .77 7.2


Trendy dish .73
Stylish package .73

4 Traditional dish .70 5.8


Substantial .68
Well-known recipe .66

5 Quick to prepare .83 5.2


Easy to prepare/clean up .80
Relieve a hungry feeling .65

6 Low amount of calories .65 3.6


Steamed dish .63
Vegetarian dish .61

7 Modern Mediterranean dish (i.e. penne a l’arabiata, ravioli pesto, .86 3.5
paella, Moussaka…) .62
Traditional Mediterranean dish (i.e. lasagne or spaghetti…)

277
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Table 7.16 : ANOVA results Ideal Ready Meal

Note: The superscripts indicate significantly different means (p<.05) where the alphabetical order of the
superscripts represents the ascending order of the means. Tukey’s honestly significant difference test has been
applied to assess significance between the segments.
** p<.01
(*) For the items of the scales measuring ideal meal attributes:
- 1 corresponds to ‘not at all likely’
- 7 corresponds to ‘very likely’

278
Chapter VII : Consumer Motives and Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

5. Discussion and conclusions

The primary purpose of this study was to better understand ready meal consumption behavior

by investigating its health-related determinants. Whereas physical health concern previously

has been identified as having a negative effect on the purchase of convenience foods (e.g. de

Boer et al., 2004), the present study explored the impact of more psychological aspects of

health concern, i.e. health-related motive orientations on consumer behavior for ready meals.

A first hypothesis was directed towards validating the motive-related segmentation approach

proposed by Geeroms et al. (2005b) in their earlier study on health audience segmentation.

The original 45-item Health-related Motive Scale was revised in the context of present study

in order to enhance its psychometric properties. Based on the made revisions, eight reliable

health dimensions could be derived, reflecting eight fundamental consumer motives

(Callebaut et al., 1999). As explicitly hypothesized, a two-step cluster analysis performed on

these eight health constructs revealed five distinct health segments, with different health-

related motive orientations. The found segments were generally comparable to the five health

audience segments discovered by Geeroms et al. (2005b), and thus, were called respectively,

‘Energetic Experimenters’, ‘Harmonious Enjoyers’, ‘Normative Carers’, ‘Conscious Experts’

and ‘Rationalists’.

By dealing with hypothesis 2, new insights were provided into the way psychological health

aspects influence people’s general food behavior. In general, several plausible and

meaningful relationships could be detected between health-related motive orientations and

general food patterns and behaviors. For example, the perception of food as a source of

enjoyment, creativity and improvisation, as well as a greater interest in novelty and social

279
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

relationships among ‘Energetic Experimenters’ and ‘Harmonious Enjoyers’ are not surprising

given these segments’ major involvement with health-related aspects such as emotional well-

being, enjoying life and excitement/variation in life. In contrast, a more restricted health

interpretation among ‘Normative Carers’, ‘Conscious Experts’ and ‘Rationalists’ is translated

into a more controlled way of dealing with food, with a focus on familiarity and tradition.

The predominant concern among ‘Normative Carers’ and ‘Rationalists’ about planning,

pricing issues and credence criteria such as freshness and naturalness is quite obvious, as

these food-related dimensions express aspects of security and safety in themselves (e.g.

Brunsø et al., 2004). In addition, Normative Carers’ involvement with social responsibility in

relation to health is expressed through aspects such as taking care of others through the

serving of good food. Finally, a lower interest in cooking, as well as the perception of food as

a source of functionality and routine could be expected among ‘Conscious Experts’, as this

segment’s health-related motive orientations express aspects of social status and prestige (e.g.

being ambitious, being powerful, being successful), which are usually connected with striving

for a professional career and having a busy life, hence, implying less time for shopping and

cooking-related activities (e.g. Brunsø et al., 2004; Ahlgren et al., 2005).

Taking into account previous insights into general food behavior, in hypothesis 3, we

investigated the impact of health-related motive orientations on people’s ready meal

consumption behavior. Whereas with respect to general food behavior, mainly the

intrapersonal axis of our health-related motive structure (see Fig. 7.1) accounted for

significant differences between the five health segments – with rather similar food patterns

among ‘Energetic Experimenters’ and ‘Harmonious Enjoyers’, as opposed to ‘Normative

Carers’, ‘Conscious Experts’ and ‘Rationalists’ – in the case of ready meal consumption,

significant differences in consumption patterns were mainly found along the interpersonal

280
Chapter VII : Consumer Motives and Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

axis. As expected, people dealing with health in an individualistic way, i.e. ‘Energetic

Experimenters’ and ‘Conscious Experts’ showed significantly higher measures of attitudes,

beliefs and behaviors toward ready meals compared to people holding an altruistic

interpretation of health, i.e. ‘Harmonious Enjoyers’, ‘Normative Carers’ and ‘Rationalists’.

Moreover, ‘Energetic Experimenters’ and ‘Conscious Experts’ applied a more unrestricted

way of handling ready meals (i.e. greater perception of ready meals as a source of variation,

enjoyment and relaxation), whereas the other segments experienced more guilt feelings about

ready meal consumption (i.e. neglecting one’s duty toward relevant others), considering this

type of home meal replacement only for practical or functional reasons. The latter findings

are in line with previous insights in literature about the role of individuality versus altruism in

consumer behavior for convenience foods (e.g. Verlegh and Candel, 1999; de Boer et al.,

2004; Costa et al., 2005).

Given the fact that beliefs, attitudes and behaviors toward ready meals are particularly low

among ‘Harmonious Enjoyers’, ‘Normative Carers’ and ‘Rationalists’, the latter segments

need primary attention in future marketing campaigns aiming at increasing ready meal

consumption frequency. In particular, ‘Normative Carers’ and ‘Rationalists’ suffer from

feelings of guilt associated with the consumption of ready meals, as these food products are

considered as less healthy, less nutritious and more artificial compared to traditional home-

made meals. Hence, to attract these segments, marketers should position ready meals as a

valid alternative to self-prepared family meals, scoring equally high on credence attributes

such as healthiness, nutritional value and naturalness. In addition, promotional strategies (e.g.

coupons, price reductions) should be considered for these segments, as price is valued as an

important criterion when buying ready meals. In their turn, ‘Harmonious Enjoyers’ could be

appealed by focusing on hedonic criteria such as freshness and taste and – given the

281
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

importance of social relationships for these subjects – ready meals should be positioned as

‘something special’ that can be enjoyed together with family and friends. By consuming a

ready meal, time saved from cooking during visits can be more valuably spent with the guests

instead of in the kitchen.

Though ‘Energetic Experimenters’ and ‘Conscious Experts’ generally respond more

positively toward ready meals, yet some suggestions and recommendations could be offered

to increase the likelihood of purchase among these segments. For example, ‘Energetic

Experimenters’ could be approached by marketing campaigns focusing on variety and

diversity, with particular attention to ethnic dishes and new recipes. As health-related motive

orientations of ‘Conscious Experts’ imply less time for shopping and cooking-related

activities, marketing campaigns directed to them should focus on convenience-related aspects

such as quick to prepare and easy to clean up. In addition, aspects of sophistication (e.g.

stylish package, trendy dish, exclusive brand) and low calorie (e.g. vegetarian or steamed

dishes) should be stressed to attract these people, as for ‘Conscious Experts’ ready meal

consumption is associated with modernity and showing off, which may compensate for a

rather insecure/traditional mentality when preparing food by themselves (see before).

All in all, what can be concluded from the above findings is that consumer-oriented product

development and positioning might be improved by also taking into account the impact of

health-related motive orientations on ready meal consumer behavior. Though convenience-

related product positioning remains quite obvious in the context of ready meals, the authors of

this study suggest more comprehensive positioning strategies, also comprising more

psychological motives of the target audience, i.e. emotional well-being, enjoyment, social

responsibility, social recognition and activation.

282
Chapter VII : Consumer Motives and Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

6. Limitations and future research

As previous literature regarding the influence of health-related motive orientations on

people’s general food behavior and ready meal consumption behavior is almost non-existing,

the present study is mainly exploratory in nature. Hence, propositions in this study were

inferred from the authors’ personal a priori knowledge about the fundamental meaning of

health for each of the five audience segments. Though the found results provided support for

the majority of our propositions, especially in the case of general food behavior, some minor

deviations emerged from what was proposed in advance. In particular, some of the a priori

proposed relationships appeared to be non-significant, whereas others were slightly different

from our expectations. For example, no significant differences in ‘healthy’ dietary patterns

could be detected between the five health segments (i.e. H2.26), although health-related

motive orientations among these segments are quite distinct. However, the importance of

‘healthiness’ as a ready meal buying criterion truly differed among the five segments (i.e.

H3.8). Future research is advised to further explore these contradictory results or to

additionally validate this study’s major findings in a less exploratory way. For example, other

types of convenience-related meal solutions can be considered such as take away foods,

restaurant meals or pub meals. In addition, future experimental studies may be designed,

investigating the actual behavioral impact of the motive-related positioning strategies

suggested in this study.

283
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Steven Boute for his help with the data collection. Also the help of the

newspaper Het Nieuwsblad is very much acknowledged for giving the opportunity to collect

the data through its website. The assistance and support of Patrick Salien and Kurt Minnen

are particularly appreciated.

284
Chapter VII : Consumer Motives and Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

References

Ahlgren, M.K., Gustafsson, I.B., & Hall, G. (2005). The impact of the meal situation on the

consumption of ready meals. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 29(6), 485-

492.

Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior.

Eaglewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Bagozzi, R.P., & Dholakia, U. (1999). Goal setting and goal striving in consumer behavior.

Journal of Marketing, 63, 19-32.

Bonke, J. (1996). Economic influences on food choice – non-convenience versus

convenience food consumption. In H.L. Meiselmann, & H.J.H. MacFie (Eds.). Food

choice, acceptance and consumption (pp. 293-318). London: Chapman & Hall.

Bowers, D.E. (2000). Cooking trends echo changing roles of women. Food Review, 23, 23-

29.

Brinberg, D., & Durand, J.H. (1983). Eating at fast-food restaurants: an analysis using two

behavioral intention models. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 13(6), 459-472.

Brunsø, K., & Grunert, K.G. (1995). Development and testing of a cross-culturally valid

instrument: food-related lifestyle. Advances in Consumer Research, 22, 475-480.

Brunsø, K., Scholderer, J., & Grunert, K.G. (2004). Testing relationships between values and

food-related lifestyle: results from two european countries. Appetite, 43, 195-205

Byrne, D. (1998). The state we’re in. Marketing, Feb., 14-15.

Callebaut, J., Janssens, M., Op de Beeck, D., Lorré, D., & Hendrickx, H. (1999).

Motivational marketing research revisited. Leuven-Apeldoorn: Garant.

285
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Candel, M.J.J.M. (2001). Consumers’ convenience orientation towards meal preparation:

conceptualization and measurement. Appetite, 35, 15-28.

Costa, A.I.A., Dekker, M., Beumer, R.R., Rombauts, F.M., & Jongen, W.M.F. (2001). A

consumer-oriented classification system for home meal replacements. Food Quality

and Preference, 12, 229-242.

Costa, A.I.A., Schoolmeester, D., Dekker, M., & Jongen, W.M.F. (2005). To cook or not to

cook: a means-end study of motives for choice of meal solutions. Food Quality and

Preference, xxx, xxx-xxx (available online 4 oct 2005 at www.sciencedirect.com).

Darian, J.C., & Tucci, L. (1992). Convenience-oriented food expenditures of working-wife

families: implications for convenience food manufacturers. Journal of Food Products

Marketing, 1, 25-36.

Datamonitor. (2003). Consumer trends in prepared meals: market impact and future

direction (New Consumer Insight Series Report). London: Datamonitor.

de Boer, M., McCarthy, M., Cowan, C., & Ryan, I. (2004). The influence of lifestyle

characteristics and beliefs about convenience food on the demand for convenience

foods in the Irish market. Food Quality and Preference, 15, 155-165.

Distributie Vandaag. (1999). Dossier bereide schotels. België: Koninklijk Belgisch Comité

voor de Distributie.

Geeroms, N., Van Kenhove, P., & Verbeke, W. (2005b). Health advertising to promote fruit

and vegetable intake: application of motive-related health audience segmentation.

Working paper, Ghent University, Department of Marketing..

GfK. (2002). To bio or not to bio: de paradox tussen prijsontwikkeling en de vraag naar

kwaliteit. GfK jaargids 2002: kerncijfers voor marketing- en beleidsplannen. GfK

Belgium: Brussels.

286
Chapter VII : Consumer Motives and Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

Gilbert, L.C. (2000). The functional food trend: What’s next and what American think about

eggs. Journal of the American College of Nutrition, 19(5), 507S-512S.

Gofton, L. (1995). Convenience and the moral status of consumer practices. In D.W. Marshall

(Ed.). Food choice and the consumer (pp. 152-181). London: Blackie Academic &

Professional.

Guinot, C., Latreille, J., Malvy, D., Preziosi, P., Galan, P., Hercberg, S., & Tenenhaus, M.

(2001). Use of multiple correspondence analysis and cluster analysis to study dietary

behavior: food consumption questionnaire in the SU.VI.MAX. cohort. European

Journal of Epidemiology, 17(6), 505-516.

IFIC. (2000). International food information council functional foods attitudinal research.

http://www.ific.org.

Jago, D. (2000). A healthy outlook. Prepared Foods, 169(4), 29.

Mahon, D., Cowan, C., & McCarthy, M. (2006). The role of attitudes, subjective norm,

perceived control and habit in the consumption of ready meals and takeaways in Great

britain. Food Quality and Preference, 17(6), 474-481.

Milburn, K. (1995). Never mind the quantity, investigate the depth. British Food Journal,

97, 36-38.

Miller, K.E., & Grinter, J.L. (1979). An investigation of situational variation in brand choice

behavior and attitude. Journal of Marketing Research, 16, 111-123.

Mothersbaugh, D.L., Herrmann, R.O., & Warland, R.H. (1993). Perceived time pressure and

recommended dietary practices: the moderating effect of knowledge of nutrition.

Journal of Consumer Affairs, 27(1), 106-126.

Newby, P.K., & Tucker, K.L. (2004). Empirically derived eating patterns using factor or

cluster analysis: a review. Nutrition Reviews, 62(5), 177-203.

NIS (2005). Private Households Statistics. Brussels: NIS, National Institute for Statistics.

287
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Olsen, S.O. (2003). Understanding the relationship between age and seafood consumption:

the mediating role of attitude, health involvement and convenience. Food Quality and

Preference, 14, 199-209.

Olson, J.C., & Reynolds, T.J. (2001). The means-end approach to understanding consumer

decision making. In J.C. Olson, & T.J. Reynolds (Eds.). Understanding consumer

decision making: the means-end approach to marketing and advertising strategy (pp.

3-20). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Reilly, M.D. (1982). Working wifes and convenience consumption. Journal of Consumer

Research, 8, 407-418.

Scholderer, J., & Grunert, K.G. (2005). Consumers, food and convenience: the long way

from resource constraints to actual consumption patterns. Journal of Economic

Psychology, 26, 105-128.

Steptoe, A., Pollard, T.M., & Wardle, J. (1995). Development of a measure of the motives

underlying the selection of food: the food choice questionnaire. Appetite, 25, 267-284.

Story, M., Neumark-Sztainer, D., & French, S. (2002). Individual and environmental

influences on adolescent eating behaviors. Journal of the American Dietetic

Association, 102, S40-S51.

Synovate/Censydiam. (2001). Kwalitatief onderzoek inzake koelverse kant-en-klaar

maaltijden. Unpublished research report. Synovate/Censydiam Institute: Antwerp.

Synovate/Censydiam. (2002). Motivationele segmentatiestudie van versbereide maaltijden.

Unpublished research report. Synovate/Censydiam Institute: Antwerp.

Synovate/Censydiam. (2003). Kwalitatief onderzoek come a casa opportuniteiten.

Unpublished research report. Synovate/Censydiam Institute: Antwerp.

Verlegh, P.W.J., & Candel, M.J.J.M. (1999). The consumption of convenience foods:

reference groups and eating situations. Food Quality and Preference, 10, 457-464.

288
Chapter VII : Consumer Motives and Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

WHO. (1990). The world health organization report, diet, nutrition and the prevention of

chronic diseases. WHO Technical Report Series 797.

289
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Appendix A :

• List of scale items

290
Chapter VII : Consumer Motives and Ready Meal Consumption Behavior

Appendix A: List of scale items

Health-related Motive Scale (Geeroms et al., 2005b)

(revised version; original formulations are given in brackets after the revised formulations)

Explicit items (Exp)


For me, health is mainly about…

1. keeping the body in a good condition, i.e. fitness, jogging, aerobics…


2. having the energy to do the things I want to do.
3. taking time to relax and to enjoy life.
4. living in harmony with my family.
5. having no physical health problems.
6. living an active life, i.e. practicing sports…
7. taking care of other family members’ health.
8. respecting public health norms and prescriptions (following the advice of expert-others, i.e. doctor,
dietician,…)
9. managing my body and appearance in the best possible way (developing a healthy lifestyle of my
own).
10. looking good.
11. protecting my body against harmful influences (reducing physical health risks with regard to heart,
lungs, liver,…).
12. staying slim.
13. emotional well-being, feeling good mentally.
14. keeping up good social contacts.
15. keeping nutritional intake strictly under control (feeling in control over my body).

Implicit items (Imp)


Because of health problems, it would be (extremely) bad not to be able anymore to…
1. help others.
2. feel secure in life.
3. work functionally.
4. live an active social life.
5. live very intensely (behave independently).
6. be successful.
7. be classy.
8. feel protected.
9. be playful.
10. explore life and discover new things (work creatively).
11. get stability in life.
12. share time with family.
13. enjoy life.
14. stay slim.
15. perceive warmth and conviviality.
16. stay beautiful.
17. be free to do what I want (practice sports).
18. organize and control life.
19. be cheerful and light-hearted (be cheerful).
20. be powerful.
21. have fun with others.
22. get rest in life.
23. have close friends.
24. be spontaneous.
25. care for my family.
26. get things under control (think rationally).
27. be ambitious.
28. perform my job my own way (be competent).

291
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

29. experience adventure in life (live an adventurous life).


30. be stylish.

292
CHAPTER VIII

SUMMARY & DISCUSSION

293
Chapter VIII

Summary & Discussion

1. Recapitulation

Subject of research within the present dissertation has been the two-dimensional motivational

framework of Callebaut et al. (1999), consisting of eight consumer motives, i.e. desirable end-

states that people seek to attain through consumption. Recently, this framework has been

used worldwide by a dozen of prominent multinational companies to position, reposition or

communicate new and existing brands (e.g. Coca-Cola, Johnson and Johnson, Heineken,

Nokia and Chefaro). However, notwithstanding this practical relevance, the consumer motive

taxonomy of Callebaut et al. (1999) has never been scientifically validated or tested. Hence,

the main purpose of this dissertation was to contribute to the validation and critical

assessment of this motivational framework in a marketing context. More specifically, we

tried to meet this purpose by focusing on three broad research objectives, i.e. (1) cross-

validation, (2) construct validation, and (3) the practical application of the taxonomy of

consumer motives of Callebaut et al. (1999). Accordingly, the studies in this dissertation

were organized among these three themes: (1) in a first paper we focused on the measurement

of underlying consumer motives by a set of stable (i.e. reliable) unidimensional implicit items

and, in addition, we confirmed the robustness of the motive structure of Callebaut et al. (1999)

across different cultures and different product categories (Chapter III); (2) in the second

theme we correlated consumer motives with other related concepts in literature, i.e. personal

values (Chapter IV) and human personality traits (Chapter V); and (3) in the last two studies

295
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

we concentrated on the practical application of Callebaut et al.’s (1999) consumer motive

taxonomy as a new segmentation paradigm in the context of health marketing (Chapter VI &

Chapter VII).

1.1. A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives: Cross-Validity

In a first research paper (Chapter III), an empirical validation of Callebaut et al.’s (1999)

taxonomy of consumer motives was provided based on respondents’ valence ratings of a list

of preferred brand personality traits. As personality traits associated with a preferred brand

seem to be expressions of human personality dimensions (e.g. Sirgy, 1982; Keller, 1993;

Helgeson and Supphellen, 2004) and, in addition, human personality traits are likely to be

function of underlying motives (e.g. Read et al., 1990; Winter et al., 1998; Read and Lalwani,

2000), preferred brand personality traits are prone to channel the ways in which these

underlying motives are expressed. Hence, based on this self-expressive function of a

preferred brand personality, consumer motives were operationalized by a set of 34 preferred

brand personality items. The results of an INDSCAL analysis, conducted across 11 different

countries and four product categories, revealed a consistent and replicable structure

underlying the concept of preferred brand personality. Based on the conceptualization of

preferred brand personality dimensions as expressions of underlying motives, the found

structure was reformulated in terms of Callebaut et al.’s (1999) taxonomy of eight consumer

motives i.e. Vitality, Pleasure, Conviviality, Belonging, Security, Control, Recognition and

Power. The relationships among these eight motives could be summarized in terms of a

circumplex structure, determined by two basic, bipolar dimensions, respectively representing

an intrapersonal and interpersonal motive axis. We considered these motivational states to be

stable dispositions that are present within every individual and become aroused by a particular

296
Chapter VIII : Summary and Discussion

product environment (e.g. McClelland, 1980). Once aroused, the manifest motives invoke a

desire for certain types of human personalities to be present, which are expressed by choosing

brands with congruent personalities.

1.2. A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives: Construct Validity

To test for construct validity, in the second research part of the present dissertation (Chapter

IV & Chapter V) Callebaut et al.’s (1999) eight consumer motives were correlated with other

related concepts in literature. In Chapter IV the objective was to reconsider the relationship

between consumer motives and personal values, based on both an alternative implicit method

of motive assessment and a non-student subject sample. As methodological problems, i.e.

method variance between motive and value measures and the predominant use of student

samples by researchers are posited as one of the most obvious reasons for the lack of

empirical correlations between motives and values (e.g. Brunstein and Schmitt, 2004;

Schultheiss and Brunstein, 2001), we suggested that motive-value correlations might become

more substantial by increasing method congruence between implicit motive assessments and

direct value measures, i.e. using the same structured response formats (Likert scales) in both

cases and by allowing more mature subjects to participate in the study instead of young adults,

i.e. conducting a random walk method as a means of data collection. Specific hypotheses

were formulated regarding the proposed relationships between Callebaut et al.’s (1999) eight

consumer motives and Schwartz’ (1992) ten value types. Based on information from a

sample of 178 respondents, the results of our analysis clearly showed that – although rather

moderate in magnitude – several significant correlations exist between measures of motives

and values. In particular, 38% of the motive-value correlations appeared to be significant at

the alpha .05 level of probability. In general, our findings were consistent with the hypothesis

297
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

that people lean toward those values that play to their inherent motive dispositions.

Specifically, motives of Pleasure and Vitality contributed to the priority that individuals

placed on pro-Openness to Change values (i.e. Self-direction, Stimulation and Hedonism);

Conviviality and Belonging contributed to pro-Self-Transcendence values (i.e. Universalism

and Benevolence); motives of Security and Control supported pro-Conservation value

priorities (i.e. Security, Tradition and Conformity); and Recognition and Power motives

contributed to pro-Self-Enhancement values (i.e. Achievement and Power).

In the realm of the application of contingency models in personality research, in Chapter V

we investigated the impact of underlying consumer motives on the relationship between

personality traits and several aspects of (consumer) behavior. In the first study the trait

consumer innovativeness was considered and its link with new-product adoption behavior,

whereas study 2 concentrated on the relationship between dispositional self-control and

disciplined driving behavior. The results of both studies provided support for the basic

proposition that actual behavioral outcomes of human personality characteristics are

dependent on the alignment with the particular consumer motives that are active within a

given product context. In study 1, we found a significant positive relationship existing

between innate innovativeness and consumer adoption behavior for innovative mobile phone

product attributes. However, significant differences were detected among high-innovative

consumers, i.e. consumers driven by underlying motives of Vitality or Recognition, in either

adopting utilitarian or hedonic innovative attributes. In line with the general principle of

compatibility in the motive-literature (e.g. Higgins, 2002), Vitality-driven consumers

exhibited higher adoption levels for hedonic innovative mobile phone attributes, whereas

adoption of utilitarian innovative mobile phone attributes was more prominent among

Recognition-driven consumers. In the same way, the results of study 2 suggested a significant

298
Chapter VIII : Summary and Discussion

positive relationship between dispositional self-control and disciplined driving behavior, as

people high in dispositional self-control reported more disciplined driving behavior compared

to people low in self-control. Here again, significant differences were found among people

with high dispositional self-control, i.e. people driven by underlying motives of Recognition

or Security, according to the manifest consumer motives. In particular, disciplined driving

behavior as an outcome of human self-control appeared to be more prominent among

Security-driven people, as this kind of normative behavior was more compatible with the

basic motive orientation of these people. In sum, the findings of study 1 and study 2 provided

evidence for the intervening impact of underlying consumer motives on the relationship

between human personality and (consumer) behavior. A dispositional personality trait was

found to give rise to a specific behavior, especially when that behavior aligned with particular

manifest consumer motives. In contrast, when less compatibility could be detected between

the behavior in question and the underlying motive orientations, high levels of the personality

trait were less likely to result into the behavioral outcome.

1.3. A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives: Practical Applications

The last research part of this dissertation focused on the practical application of Callebaut et

al.’s (1999) taxonomy of consumer motives as a new segmentation paradigm in a health

marketing context (Chapter VI & Chapter VII). Given the importance of meaningful audience

segmentation in creating effective health advertisements, in Chapter VI we proposed a new

theory-driven segmentation approach based on Callebaut et al.’s (1999) taxonomy of eight

consumer motives. We demonstrated the usefulness of this new segmentation paradigm in the

process of developing appropriate health advertisements in the context of fruit and vegetable

consumption. As health beliefs or concerns play an important role in food behavior such as

299
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

fruit and vegetable intake (e.g. Steptoe et al., 1995), a heterogeneous health audience (N=

615) was segmented according to the audience members’ health-related motive orientations,

i.e. relatively enduring beliefs that people hold about the fundamental meaning of health. The

results of a two-step cluster analysis revealed five different health segments to exist for which

health has another distinct meaning, named Energetic Experimenters, Harmonious Enjoyers,

Normative Carers, Rationalists and Conscious Experts. These five health segments could be

organized along two dimensions; yielding a health-related motive structure that is a reflection

of Callebaut et al.’s (1999) consumer motive taxonomy. Significant differences were found

between the five health segments with regard to category-specific fruit and vegetable

consumption. Energetic Experimenters consumed significantly more bananas than did

members of the other segments, whereas the consumption of potatoes and cooked vegetables

was significantly higher among Harmonious Enjoyers and Normative Carers. In addition,

significant differences existed between the health audience segments with regard to their

reactions toward motive-related health advertising. When compared to the other segments, all

health audience segments responded more positively toward the advertisement that was

hypothesized to be most responsive to their health-related motive orientations. Moreover,

when comparing the most appropriate motive-related advertisement to a general

advertisement, in almost all cases a segment’s reactions toward the motive-related

advertisement were significantly more positive than its reactions toward the advertisement

that has a general character (i.e. not responsive to a segment’s underlying motives).

Finally, the objective in Chapter VII was to extend our previous work on motive-related

audience segmentation, by investigating how different health-related motive orientations

influence people’s ways of dealing with convenience-related food solutions, i.e. ready meals.

Again, five distinct health segments emerged out of a two-step cluster analysis with different

300
Chapter VIII : Summary and Discussion

health-related motive orientations, which validated our findings in Chapter VI. When

comparing these segments, several significant and meaningful relationships could be detected

between health-related motive orientations and both general food behavior patterns and ready

meal consumption patterns. For instance, Energetic Experimenters and Harmonious Enjoyers

perceived food as a source of enjoyment, creativity and improvisation and had a greater

interest in novelty and social relationships, whereas a more controlled way of dealing with

food could be found among Normative Carers, Conscious Experts and Rationalists, with a

focus on security, tradition, functionality and routine. With respect to ready meal

consumption behavior, people dealing with health in an individualistic way, i.e. Energetic

Experimenters and Conscious Experts showed significantly higher measures of attitudes,

beliefs and behaviors toward ready meals compared to people holding an altruistic

interpretation of health, i.e. Harmonious Enjoyers, Normative Carers and Rationalists.

Moreover, Energetic Experimenters and Conscious Experts applied a more unrestricted way

of handling ready meals, i.e. greater perception of ready meals as a source of variation,

enjoyment and relaxation, whereas the other segments experienced more guilt feelings about

ready meal consumption, i.e. neglecting one’s duty toward relevant others, considering this

type of home meal replacement only for practical or functional reasons. In general, the

findings regarding ready meal consumption behavior are in line with previous insights in

literature about the role of individuality versus altruism in consumer behavior for convenience

foods (e.g. Verlegh and Candel, 1999; de Boer et al., 2004; Costa et al., 2005).

301
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

2. Theoretical implications

2.1. A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives: Cross-Validity

In our first research paper (Chapter III) we validated the circumplex consumer motive

taxonomy of Callebaut et al. (1999), based on valence ratings of a set of 34 preferred brand

personality traits. By integrating conceptual insights about both the relationship between

motives and human personality (e.g. Barsalou, 1985; Read et al., 1990; Winter et al., 1998)

and the self-expressive use of brands by consumers (e.g. Sirgy, 1982), we posited that

preferred brand personality traits are associated with particular motives that have to be

satisfied. Actually we suggested that different consumer situations cause different motives to

become manifest. Consequently, a desire is invoked for different types of human

personalities to be present. To express the human personality type that is manifest in a given

situation, people prefer a brand with a congruent personality (e.g. Aaker, 1999; Kalar, 2003).

Hence, the preferred brand – through its link with the desired human personality – derives

some satisfaction potentiality with regard to the manifest consumer motives.

By taking such a motive-based approach to the concept of brand personality, and thus, by

conceptually relating these two constructs to each other, our research significantly contributes

to both the study of brand personality and the study of consumer motives. For the former

stream of research, our research goes beyond previous insights of Aaker and colleagues (e.g.

Aaker, 1997, 1999; Aaker et al., 2001; Ferrandi et al., 1999, 2001; Koebel and Ladwein, 1999)

by providing a more explanatory point of view and giving additional insights in why people

have a preference for some brand personalities above others (e.g. Biel, 1993). As mentioned

before (cf. supra: Chapter III), most previous works on brand personality deal with the

302
Chapter VIII : Summary and Discussion

concept in a rather perceptual manner. Points of departure are random products/brands by

themselves and how consumers perceive these brands in terms of their personality (e.g. Aaker,

1997:”which personality traits come to mind when you think of brand X”). Brand personality

is looked at from what the concept really ‘is’, namely: ‘the set of human personality traits that

are associated with a particular brand’, as the definition of Aaker points out. This kind of

descriptive approach has as a main advantage that a clear picture is provided of how the

concept of brand personality is perceived in consumer minds, as the main objective is to

reduce the brand personality domain to a limited number of distinct factors or dimensions.

For instance, the most influential theoretical structure of brand personality was developed by

Aaker (1997), who organized the field among five broad dimensions, termed Sincerity,

Excitement, Competence, Sophistication and Ruggedness. However, despite these structural

insights, previous brand personality research falls short in just being descriptive in nature and

not relating brand personality to more fundamental concepts such as consumer motives. To

overcome these shortcomings of previous studies, the research in the present dissertation

employed a more in-depth approach by looking for some kind of interpretative motive

structure underlying the concept of preferred brand personality. In our research, the point of

departure was no longer how consumers perceive brands but rather what they prefer in brands

in terms of their underlying motives. Hence, in this dissertation, we moved from the concept

of ‘perceived’ brand personality, i.e. personality traits associated with a random brand in a

particular product category to what is called ‘preferred’ brand personality, i.e. personality

traits that people are looking for in a preferred brand in the light of underlying motive

satisfaction. Whereas perceived brand personality is clearly related to the ‘actual’ personality

of a brand as triggered by a variety of external marketing variables such as user imagery,

advertising and/or packaging (e.g. Plummer, 1985; Levy, 1959; Batra et al., 1993), the

concept of preferred brand personality has more to do with some ‘desired’ personality

303
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

triggered by the inner world of the individual, i.e. underlying consumer motives. Such a

motive-based approach to brand personality as was employed in this dissertation stands closer

to the current definitions of marketing, with their central focus on consumers and what they

want (e.g. Kotler, 1997).

When examining the correspondence between the eight preferred brand personality

dimensions uncovered in this dissertation and the earlier five perceived brand personality

dimensions developed by Aaker (1997), some interesting conceptual parallels could be

discovered. Aaker’s (1997) Excitement dimension seems to have considerable overlap with

the Vitality domain whereas her brand personality factor of Sophistication shows congruence

with the Recognition position. Moreover, our results could be considered as a refinement of

the Aaker (1997) dimensions, as some of the subcomponents that make up one broad factor in

her classification, in our structure, seem to spread out on two or more adjacent positions. For

example, brand personality traits defining Aaker’s (1997) dimension of Sincerity (e.g. family-

oriented, sincere, cheerful, and friendly) appear in the domains of Belonging as well as

Conviviality and Pleasure. In the same way, traits defining Aaker’s (1997) Competence

dimension (e.g. hard working, intelligent, technical, successful, leader, and confident) share a

common domain with the Power position as well as with the Recognition and Control

positions. Hence, these patterns suggest a slightly asymmetric relationship in the structure of

perceived versus preferred brand personality, and thus, both concepts might operate in slightly

different ways on consumer behavior. In general, one could argue that consumers’

interpretation of the brand personality domain is more nuanced when thinking about brands in

relationship with underlying motives than when solely considering brands in a perceptual

manner. In contrast to the other dimensions, Aaker’s fifth dimension of Ruggedness seems to

304
Chapter VIII : Summary and Discussion

fit less in our preferred brand personality structure, whereas in their turn none of the Aaker

(1997) dimensions seem to fully capture the position of Security.

The contribution of the present dissertation to the study of consumer motives lies in the

scientific validation of Callebaut et al.’s (1999) comprehensive taxonomy to capture people’s

underlying motives, which has been proven to be valid cross-culturally. Based on the

conceptual congruence between some robust motivational theories (e.g. McClelland, 1961;

Williams, 1968; McGuire, 1974; McAdams and Powers, 1981; Freud, 1933) and the overall

nature of the preferred brand personality dimensions uncovered in this research, Callebaut et

al.’s (1999) framework of eight consumer motives was derived, i.e. Vitality, Pleasure,

Conviviality, Belonging, Security, Control, Recognition and Power, determined by two basic,

bipolar dimensions, i.e. an intrapersonal and interpersonal motive axis. In its most basic form,

the taxonomy of Callebaut et al. (1999) has shown to be compatible with prior classifications

of human motives (e.g. Ford and Nichols, 1987; Chulef et al., 2001; Wicker et al., 1984;

Youniss et al., 1979; Murray, 1938; Maslow, 1970). However, this taxonomy – as derived

and validated in the present dissertation – has a number of advantages over previous attempts

at human motive taxonomies. First, by using a much broader, multi-country sample of

subjects than previous attempts, we enhanced the external validity and the generalizability of

the findings. Second, we generated Callebaut et al.’s (1999) taxonomy on an empirical basis,

rather than by relying on theoretical preconceptions as did most previous researchers (see

Chulef et al., 2001 for an exception). Hence, the obtained results are assumed to be less

biased by the authors’ a priori knowledge of the field as they represent subjects’ naïve

thoughts and perceptions. Third, whereas most previous attempts are mainly descriptive in

nature and organize the field of human motives into some discrete categories, the present

dissertation validated a circumplex structure of continuous motive dimensions in which

305
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

relations of conflict and compatibility are represented. Adjacent positions are postulated to be

most compatible (e.g. Pleasure and Conviviality), whereas increasing distance around the

circumplex indicates decreasing compatibility and greater conflict. Motive dimensions that

emerge in opposing directions from the origin are postulated to be in greatest conflict (e.g.

Pleasure and Control). Finally, Callebaut et al.’s (1999) motive taxonomy as validated in this

dissertation is based on respondents’ ratings of specific brand personality traits instead of

more general motive units as were most previous taxonomies. By doing so, we established a

link between products/brands and consumer motives. Based on the application of Callebaut et

al.’s (1999) motivational framework, one gets insight into the way people’s motive

orientations are at work in a particular brand category context, rather than being provided with

a taxonomy of overall human motives.

2.2. A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives: Construct Validity

The issue of construct validity has first been acknowledged by Cronbach and Meehl (1955)

and, since these authors’ pioneering work, has been considered as a vital part of the process of

theory testing across a wide range of disciplines (e.g. Landy, 1986; Messick, 1980). Basically,

the concept refers to the extent to which a measure of some hypothetical construct relates to

measures of other constructs in theoretically predictable ways (e.g. Smith, 2005). In the

second research part of the present dissertation (Chapter IV & Chapter V) we empirically

tested for construct validity by correlating Callebaut et al.’s (1999) eight consumer motives

with other related concepts in literature. More specifically, in Chapter IV several meaningful

relationships were detected between consumer motives and Schwartz’ (1992) ten value types.

In general, our findings indicated that people lean toward those values that play to their

inherent motive dispositions, which provided support for the theoretical proposition that

306
Chapter VIII : Summary and Discussion

personal values can be considered as cognitive instantiations of these underlying motives (e.g.

Schwartz and Bilsky, 1987; Bilsky and Schwartz, 1994). However, taking into account the

moderate strength of the found relationships, the intent of our research was not to

overestimate the degree of interconnectedness between motives and values and thus fully

reject McClelland et al.’s (1989) conceptualization of these two constructs as somewhat

different dimensions of the personality structure. Rather our intent was to demonstrate a

particular pattern of relationships between measures of motives and values, which draws

attention to the influence of internal psychological aspects, i.e. one’s underlying motive

dispositions on people’s value priorities, in addition to already existing insights about the

impact of external factors such as culture and social environment (e.g. Schwartz and Sagiv,

1995; Blackwell et al., 2001; Knafo and Schwartz, 2001; Rohan and Zanna, 1996;

Schoenpflug, 2001). Previously, the way internal psychological constructs influence personal

values has been investigated by relatively few studies (e.g. Luk and Bond, 1993; Yik and

Tang, 1996; Dollinger et al., 1996; Herringer, 1998). In the context of these studies, Olver

and Mooradian (2003) recently provided an explicit test of the relationship between the Big

Five factor model of personality (e.g. McCrae and Costa, 1996) and the Schwartz value types

in a Western sample. Hence, the latter research offers convergent support for our findings

when human personality traits and consumer motives are related conceptually to each other.

The Big Five factor of Openness to Experience (i.e. conceptually related to motives of

Vitality and Pleasure) was positively related to pro-Openness to Change values.

Agreeableness (i.e. conceptually related to motives of Conviviality and Belonging) was

related to greater Self-Transcendence values and finally, Conscientiousness (i.e. conceptually

related to motives of Security and Control and to some extent to Recognition and Power) was

positively associated with pro-Conservation and Self-Enhancement values.

307
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

In early years, human personality variables alone were commonly used to investigate and

predict the behavior of consumers in the market place (e.g. Evans, 1959; Alpert, 1972; Kernan,

1968; Brody and Cunningham, 1968; Robertson and Meyers, 1969). However, most

empirical findings showed that attempting to trace the effect of general personality variables

on specific consumer behaviors is plagued by low predictive power. For instance, various

studies claimed that personality traits have failed to explain more than 10% of the variance in

behavior (e.g. Jacoby, 1971; Lastovicka and Joachimsthaler, 1988; Kassarjian and Sheffet,

1991). Hence, more recently, the use of contingency models has been proposed in personality

research, taking into account the combined influence of general personality traits and several

other intervening variables (e.g. Dickson, 1982; Miller and Ginter, 1979; Van Kenhove et al.,

1999). Against this background, the research conducted in Chapter V of this dissertation

significantly contributed to an improved understanding of trait-behavior relationships in

personality research, by investigating the impact of underlying consumer motives on the

relationship between personality traits and several aspects of consumer behavior. In addition

to the detection of meaningful relationships between consumer motives and human

personality traits – i.e. consumer innovativeness was found to be related positively to

underlying motive orientations of Vitality and Recognition and negatively to Security and

Conviviality, whereas dispositional self-control correlated positively with motives of

Recognition and Security and negatively with Vitality and Conviviality – the results of our

studies provided support for the basic proposition that actual behavioral outcomes of human

personality characteristics are dependent on the alignment with the particular consumer

motives that are active within a given product context. Based on these findings, we extended

the work of Miller and Ginter (1979) and Dickson (1982) who were among the first to

consider the role of intervening variables in personality research. Whereas in these previous

studies the intervening impact of situational factors (e.g. time constraints, interpersonal

308
Chapter VIII : Summary and Discussion

interactions, task definition, etc.) was mainly investigated on the relationship between

personality traits and consumer behavior (e.g. Belk, 1975), our research offered additional

insights on how more person-related variables, i.e. underlying motive dispositions impact

trait-behavior relationships in predictable ways. Though personality traits and consumer

motives separately might be of only modest value when one is attempting to predict consumer

behavior, the results of our research clearly demonstrated that taking into consideration the

combined influence of both these variables is likely to give rise to more promising results.

To summarize, the results of the empirical studies conducted in the second research part of

this dissertation basically contributed to the assessment of the construct validity, i.e.

convergent and discriminant validity of Callebaut et al.’s (1999) consumer motive taxonomy,

as several significant correlations (positive and negative) could be detected between the eight

consumer motives and the related concepts of personal values (Chapter IV) and human

personality traits (Chapter V). Additionally, regarding the study of individual value

differences, the findings of Chapter IV contributed to a better understanding of the role of

internal psychological constructs, i.e. one’s underlying motive dispositions in influencing

people’s value priorities, in addition to the role of external factors such as culture and social

environment. In a similar vein, the results of Chapter V provided new insights into the way

consumers’ motive dispositions as person-related variables impact trait-behavior relationships

in personality research, beyond the impact of situational factors as was considered by most

previous studies.

309
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

2.3. A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives: Practical Applications

Besides its usefulness in commercial marketing areas (e.g. Croft, 1994; Berrigan and

Finkbeiner, 1992), the significance of meaningful audience segmentation is generally

acknowledged in social marketing as well, especially for designing persuasive messages that

are appropriate for and appeal to a particular target audience (e.g. Fennis, 2003; Atkin and

Freimuth, 1989; Slater and Flora, 1994; Freimuth et al., 2001; Grier and Bryant, 2005;

Boslaugh et al., 2005). Regarding marketing health information, people won’t change or

adopt a specific health-related consumption behavior (e.g. eating more fruits and vegetables,

using condoms) solely because some expert health educator told them to. Rather health

message design should be an audience-centered process, which means that health messages

should be designed primarily to respond to the motives and situation of the target audience,

rather than to the motives and situation of the message designers or sponsoring organizations

(e.g. Maibach and Parrott, 1995). The studies conducted in the final research part of the

present dissertation (Chapter VI & Chapter VII) contributed to the existing literature about

motive-related audience segmentation by outlining a new segmentation approach based on the

audience members’ health-related motive orientations, i.e. relatively enduring beliefs that

people hold about the fundamental meaning of health . Whereas most previous health studies

to date apply a rather restricted conceptualization of health focusing on physical well-being

only (e.g. Rappoport et al., 1993), within this new segmentation paradigm a broader health

conceptualization was employed, also comprising psychological dimensions of health concern

such as emotional well-being, enjoying life or social responsibility. Hence, the latter

segmentation paradigm could be considered as relevant for health communication, as

appropriate health messages could be designed that appeal to these psychological dimensions

of health concern, in addition to concerns about physical health. Moreover, as Callebaut et

310
Chapter VIII : Summary and Discussion

al.’s (1999) taxonomy of consumer motives was applied as a theoretical framework to assess

people’s health-related motive orientations, our current work on motive-related audience

segmentation could be considered as an extension of the work of Silk et al. (2005), who

advocated for the use of theory-based segmentation approaches in the context of health

marketing. As mentioned by the latter authors, audience segmentation strategies traditionally

have been based on demographics, psychographics, or geography, with relatively few

attention to how theory might contribute to identifying target audiences. However, some

theories can provide relevant constructs that, when related to the behavior of interest, can

serve as relevant segmentation variables across a variety of contexts. In the present

dissertation, Callebaut et al.’s (1999) conceptual framework of consumer motives provided

the relevant constructs (i.e. health-related motive orientations) based on its application in a

health context. We assumed that including these health-related motive orientations within

message content to directly address different audiences may significantly improve the

effectiveness of these messages. In particular, our proposed segmentation approach could be

described as a kind of benefit-segmentation, based on which advantages people are looking

for in the context of their health-related motive orientations. However, the main advantage

over traditional benefit-segmentation approaches (e.g. Haley, 1968) – as mentioned above – is

that our segmentation paradigm is theory-based, whereas traditional approaches have no real

theoretical basis.

Regarding the application of our new segmentation paradigm in a health marketing context, in

Chapter VI we demonstrated its usefulness in the process of developing appropriate fruit and

vegetable health advertisements. Significant differences were found between health segments

with different health-related motive orientations both with regard to category-specific fruit

and vegetable consumption and reactions toward health advertising. Basically, our findings

311
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

provided an additional test of the proposition that motive-related health advertising is likely to

be more effective, as, in general, a segment’s reactions toward an appropriate motive-related

advertisement were significantly more positive than its reactions toward an advertisement that

was not responsive to that segment’s underlying motives.

By investigating how different health-related motive orientations influence both people’s

general food behavior patterns and ready meal consumption patterns, in Chapter VII, we

contributed to the study of consumer behavior at least in two ways. First, by also taking into

account several psychological dimensions of health concern when considering consumer food

patterns and behaviors (e.g. emotional well-being, social responsibility, etc.), we assisted in a

better understanding of food-health relationships beyond the level of physical health only.

Second, our research provided an insight into the important health-related determinants of

people’s ready meal consumption behavior. Based on these insights consumer-oriented

product development, positioning and marketing could be improved of food products

belonging to the category of ready meals (cf. infra: section 4.3.)

3. Methodological implications

3.1. A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives: Cross-Validity

Based on the research findings in Chapter III of this dissertation, some broad methodological

implications can be raised for both brand personality research and the study of consumer

motives. First, an important reflection has to be made regarding the predictive utility of the

concept of preferred brand personality. Whereas previously the impact of brand personality

on consumer brand evaluations and preferences has mainly been investigated based on self-

312
Chapter VIII : Summary and Discussion

congruity theory, and thus, in terms of the similarity between an individual’s self-image and a

brand’s personality traits (e.g. Sirgy, 1982; Aaker, 1999; Kim et al., 2001), based on the

conceptualizations employed in the present dissertation, we suggest that future research

methods may benefit from also assessing the level of congruity between a brand’s ‘actual’

personality (i.e. the concept of perceived brand personality) and which brand personality is

‘preferred’ in function of one’s underlying motives (i.e. the concept of preferred brand

personality). It can reasonably be assumed that when congruity is high between measures of

both concepts, no significant differences in predictive ability will exist between perceived

brand personality and preferred brand personality, and as a result, perceived brand personality

could be proposed as a relevant predictor of consumer preference for a brand. However, in

the case of low empirical congruity between the concepts of perceived and preferred brand

personality, we suggest that attempting to predict consumer brand preference and usage based

on preferred brand personality is likely to give rise to more promising results, given this

concept’s basic connection with underlying consumer motives.

Second, regarding the study of consumer motives, the use of INDSCAL (e.g. Carroll and

Chang, 1970) as a technique for data analysis provided us with additional insights that are

lacking in other attempts at organizing the motivational domain. As mentioned in Chapter III,

INDSCAL is one of a variety of multidimensional scaling (MDS) techniques, which put or

‘map’ stimuli onto a set of dimensions. The distances between the points represent the

empirical relations among the stimuli. Hence, based on this visual representation of the

empirical distances between stimuli (in our case a set of preferred brand personality traits), the

results of our analysis enabled us to validate a circumplex structure of continuous motive

dimensions in which relations of conflict and compatibility are represented (i.e. Schwartz,

1992: relations of conflict and compatibility among values). As closer distances in the visual

313
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

-mapping indicate stronger conceptual and empirical relationships between stimuli, adjacent

motive positions were postulated to be most compatible, whereas motive dimensions that

emerge in opposing directions from the origin were expected to be in greatest conflict. Given

these relational insights based on the INDSCAL analysis, our findings went beyond the

mainly descriptive nature of previous research on human motives, based on more common

hierarchical clustering or factor analytic techniques. In addition, as INDSCAL generates a

‘subject weight space’ – allowing for comparison of each individual sample in the analysis

with the average group stimulus space – this technique comprised a useful tool for assessing

the cross- cultural validity of Callebaut et al.’s (1999) motivational structure. Both an

examination of the individual dimension weights and NRS measures for each sample and the

detection of a very high Tucker’s coefficient of congruence indicated a good correspondence

of the individual perceptual spaces and the aggregate perceptual map. Hence, based on these

findings structural robustness and consistency were confirmed across the different countries

and different product categories.

3.2. A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives: Construct Validity

The most obvious methodological implication stemming from the studies conducted in the

second research part of the present dissertation (Chapter IV & Chapter V) comprises the

suggestion to use more structured implicit motive assessment procedures in personality

research. Previously, TAT-type implicit measures of motives were mainly used (e.g. Smith,

1992), whereas related constructs such as personal values (Chapter IV) and human personality

traits (Chapter V) were assessed by direct self-report questionnaires (e.g. Schwartz, 1992).

However, a problem related to these traditional measures of respectively motives and other

personality constructs is that they involve different stimuli (i.e. ambiguous pictures versus

314
Chapter VIII : Summary and Discussion

structured questions) and use different response formats (i.e. constructed versus selected

responses), which may contribute to the previously detected lack of empirical correlation

between these measures (e.g. Brunstein and Schmitt, 2004; Tuerlinckx et al., 2002). The aim

of our research was to solve this problem of method variance between measures of motives

and related constructs by proposing an alternative implicit method of motive assessment,

based on respondents’ ratings of a set of 34 motive-expressive personality items which were

applied to the personality of a preferred brand as a third-person (see Chapter IV & Chapter V).

As the same structured response formats (i.e. Likert scales) were used in both cases, with our

particular method of motive assessment, we induced more method congruence between

implicit motive assessments and direct measures of other constructs, hereby resolving some of

the psychometric problems (i.e. reliability, validity) inherent in more traditional TAT-type

measures of motives. Though the results of our studies may indicate that – at least to some

extent – methodological issues contribute to the lack of significant relationships between

measures of motives and other related constructs that was detected in previous studies (e.g.

Biernat, 1989), one must be cautious when drawing conclusions about the impact of choice of

measurement method on correlations between motives and other constructs as no direct

comparisons were assessed between TAT-type measures of motives and the more structured

implicit method of motive assessment that we proposed. Moreover, TAT-type motive

measures are prone to deal with overall human motives that are stable across situations,

whereas an application of our implicit motive assessment procedure provides insight into how

consumer motives are at work in a particular brand category context. Hence, these two

distinct methods of motive assessment can not be considered as measuring exactly the same.

315
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

3.3. A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives: Practical Applications

Although in fact this was beyond the scope of the final research part of the present

dissertation, a brief methodological remark can be made based on our work on motive-related

audience segmentation in Chapter VI and Chapter VII. Traditionally, audience demographics,

psychographics or geography have been used as a common basis for audience segmentation.

Within these approaches, the technique of cluster analysis was considered as the most

appropriate method to segment a heterogeneous group of subjects across these variables (e.g.

Slater, 1996). However, cluster analysis often has been employed as an a-theoretical

statistical tool of analysis designed primarily to discover underlying structures in data without

providing an explanation why they exist (e.g. Silk et al., 2005). As mentioned before, our

research on motive-related audience segmentation went beyond the primarily a-theoretical

nature of previous cluster analytic techniques as we based our new segmentation paradigm on

the application of Callebaut et al.’s (1999) theoretical framework of consumer motives in a

health context. By doing so, theory was added to our particular strategy that assists in

defining different health audience segments and, as a result, the absence of theory was

addressed associated with the technique of cluster analysis. In particular, based on the

interpretation of Callebaut et al.’s (1999) consumer motive taxonomy in the context of health

motivation, several a-priori hypotheses and explanations could be developed regarding the

existence of different health segments with different health-related motive orientations and, in

second instance, regarding the impact of these health-related motive orientations on (health-

related) consumer behavior (see Chapter VI & Chapter VII).

316
Chapter VIII : Summary and Discussion

4. Managerial implications

4.1. A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives: Cross-Validity

Researchers in marketing have become increasingly aware of the strategic importance of

brand image (e.g. Aaker and Biel, 1993; Graeff, 1997). Whereas past research on this topic

has traditionally focused on perceived self-image/brand image similarities and has

investigated the effects of this factor on consumer brand preference and usage (e.g. Malhotra,

1988; Sirgy, 1982; 1985; 1986), based on the implications drawn from Chapter III of this

dissertation, we suggest researchers and practitioners also to consider the level of similarity

between what was called ‘perceived’ and ‘preferred’ brand personality. Though the impact of

the latter type of similarity on consumer brand evaluations or preferences has not been

investigated as such in the present dissertation, it might be recommended to marketing

practitioners to try to build brands imbued with strong ‘actual’ personalities that match the

kind of personality traits that are ‘preferred’ in function of one’s underlying motives. More

specifically, as in our research eight distinct ‘preferred’ brand personality dimensions could

be discovered whereas consumers’ perception of ‘actual’ brand personality is structured

among only five broad dimensions (Aaker, 1997), we posit that marketing practitioners may

benefit from attempting to refine the concept of perceived brand personality by developing

brands with more nuanced personalities. For instance, distinct actual personality profiles

could be created for preferred brand personality dimensions that seem to have been captured

by one common Aaker (1997) factor until now (i.e. Belonging/Conviviality/Pleasure and

Power/Recognition/Control). By doing so, more appropriate brand marketing and positioning

strategies could be developed that are specifically tailored or appeal to the distinct

motivational preferences of particular target audiences. Moreover, as none of the Aaker

317
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

(1997) dimensions seemed to fully capture our dimension of Security, marketing practitioners

may be encouraged to try to incorporate this particular preferred brand personality profile in

future brand image designs. Finally, Aaker’s fifth dimension of Ruggedness seemed to fit

less in our preferred brand personality structure, and as a result, may be less focused upon in

future attempts to develop brands with strong personalities as the latter dimension seems to

tap a perceived brand personality aspect that consumers may not necessarily desire.

Besides these specific implications in the context of brand image creation, from our findings

in Chapter III, also some general practical implications can be derived in the context of

marketing segmentation and targeting. In particular, based on the motivational framework of

Callebaut et al. (1999), marketing practitioners could be recommended to segment a

heterogeneous consumer audience according to its members’ underlying consumer motives or,

more specifically, according to which advantages people are looking for in a given product

environment in the light of underlying motive satisfaction. Afterwards, the resulting

information could be used by brand managers and communication specialists to develop

appropriate advertising campaigns and brand strategies that are responsive to these product-

specific motives.

4.2. A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives: Construct Validity

Analogous to the previous section, an important practical implication from our research

conducted in the second part of this dissertation (Chapter IV & Chapter V) can be considered

in the context of consumer-oriented market segmentation. As commonly acknowledged,

general personality variables (e.g. personality traits, personal values) alone are insufficient to

effectively segment consumer markets, and as a result, many researchers have shifted their

318
Chapter VIII : Summary and Discussion

attention to a segmentation perspective that includes both personality and situation-related

variables (e.g. Dickson, 1982; Mischel, 1977; McDonald and Goldman, 1980). The thrust of

our research is that also more person-related variables, i.e. one’s underlying motive

dispositions could be considered as intervening variables in personality research, and thus, as

useful bases for segmentation in addition to general personality variables. Especially, we

recommend researchers and practitioners to make use of a segmentation framework that

incorporates both personality variables and people’s motive orientations in combination, as

the latter approach will result in a much richer description and understanding of target

markets to our opinion.

4.3. A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives: Practical Applications

From our motive-related audience segmentation approach, applied in Chapter VI and Chapter

VII of the present dissertation, five distinct health segments emerged with different health-

related motive orientations: Energetic Experimenters, Harmonious Enjoyers, Normative

Carers, Rationalists and Conscious Experts. The main objective of the present discussion is to

indicate that, in order to be effective, health practitioners should deliver tailored marketing

health information to each of these segments (e.g. Hastings and Haywood, 1994), rather than

appealing to the obligation of adopting some kind of health-related consumer behavior (e.g.

consuming a sufficient amount of fruits and vegetables) as such. Based on Means-End theory

(e.g. Olson and Reynolds, 1983), we assume that people seek for different product features

and/or health-related benefits in order to achieve distinct health-related motive orientations.

However, seeking products with motive-congruent attributes or benefits is not the same as

perceiving all types of products as such. Rather, the motive-related image of particular health

products or behaviors should be created by appropriate advertising and positioning strategies

319
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

that appeal to the health-related motive orientations of specific target audiences. In essence,

marketing health information directed to audiences with different health-related motive

orientations should stress those attributes/benefits of products or behaviors that are conform to

these underlying motives. Only by doing so, the particular health products or behaviors might

be able to compete with other (less healthy) consumer goods or practices.

Following the above reasoning, from the findings in Chapter VI of this dissertation, some

practical guidelines and recommendations can be derived for health communicators to use

when developing motive-related health advertisements in the context of fruit and vegetable

consumption. In particular, different consumer-oriented advertising strategies can be

proposed for the five health audience segments obtained in our research. Energetic

Experimenters have health-related motive orientations associated with vitality and energy.

Health advertisements designed for these people should focus on individual consumer

experiences such as feeling free, getting energy and power for the body and feeling very much

alive and kicking. A transformational/ self-directed advertising strategy is likely to be most

appropriate in this case. As Harmonious Enjoyers deal with health mainly in terms of

emotional well-being and enjoying life, advertisements geared toward this group should

express how the recommended behavior can be instrumental in achieving a complete and

happy life together with family and friends. A transformational/ other-directed advertising

strategy can be strongly recommended to use for these people. For the Normative Carers we

recommend an informational/ other-directed advertising strategy. As these people are mainly

concerned with the physical aspects of health, recommendations in the advertisements should

focus on functional benefits such as avoiding illness and health problems. In addition,

advertisements designed to these people should emphasize the social responsibility aspect that

is associated with health by communicating how the recommended responses may contribute

320
Chapter VIII : Summary and Discussion

to the preservation of the health of close others (family, children…). In order to counteract

the natural disinterest toward advertising of Rationalists and Conscious Experts,

advertisements designed to them might emphasize one’s own independent capacities to deal

with health. Moreover, mainly verbal arguments should be used focusing on outward

appearance and showing off competence. In this case an informational/ self-directed

advertising strategy can be suggested.

In a similar vein, based on our significant results in Chapter VII, some comprehensive ready

meal positioning strategies can be suggested, that go beyond convenience-related aspects and

also take into account the psychological health-related motive orientations of the target

audience. Particularly, as Normative Carers and Rationalists suffer from feelings of guilt

associated with the consumption of ready meals, to attract these segments, marketing

practitioners should position the latter food category as a valid alternative to self-prepared

family meals, scoring equally high on credence attributes such as healthiness, nutritional

value and naturalness. In addition, promotional strategies (e.g. coupons, price reductions)

should be considered for these segments, as price is valued as an important criterion when

buying ready meals. Harmonious Enjoyers could be appealed by focusing on hedonic criteria

such as freshness and taste and – given the importance of social relationships for these

subjects – ready meals should be positioned as ‘something special’ that can be enjoyed

together with family and friends. In their turn, Energetic Experimenters could be approached

by marketing campaigns focusing on variety and diversity, with particular attention to ethnic

dishes and new recipes. As health-related motive orientations of Conscious Experts imply

less time for shopping and cooking-related activities, marketing campaigns directed to them

should focus on convenience-related aspects of ready meals such as quick to prepare and easy

to clean up. In addition, aspects of sophistication (e.g. stylish package, trendy dish, exclusive

321
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

brand) and low calorie (e.g. vegetarian or steamed dishes) should be stressed to attract these

people, as for Conscious Experts ready meal consumption is associated with modernity and

showing off.

5. Limitations and future research

In each chapter limitations and suggestions for future research were described. In the

following sections, we reconsider the most important ones and, in addition, we posit some

other more general ideas which may inspire motivational researchers in the years to come.

5.1. A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives: Cross-Validity

Regarding the study of brand personality, the distinction made in Chapter III between the

concepts of ‘perceived’ and ‘preferred’ brand personality may provide some future research

opportunities. For instance, it might be interesting to empirically assess the level of similarity

between both concepts and to investigate the effects of this factor on consumer brand

evaluations and preferences. It can be assumed that when congruity is high between a brand’s

‘actual’ personality and the brand personality that is ‘preferred’ in function of one’s

underlying motives, consumers will exhibit more favorable brand evaluations and as a result,

may have a more pronounced preference for the brand. Also, future research studies may

investigate the potential differences in predictive ability between ‘perceived’ and ‘preferred’

brand personality, under the conditions of both low and high congruity between these

constructs (cf. supra: section 3.1.).

322
Chapter VIII : Summary and Discussion

With respect to the validation of Callebaut et al.’s (1999) taxonomy of consumer motives, we

have to recognize that actual behavioral measures were insufficiently included in our research.

Though the results of our studies clearly demonstrated significant effects of people’s

product/domain-specific motive dispositions on self-reported measures of consumer behavior

(e.g. fruit and vegetable intake, ready meal consumption, new-product adoption behavior,

etc.), the role of consumer motives in influencing real consumer preferences and behaviors

remained mainly unexplored. As what people report they do may not always be what they

actually do (e.g. Weber and Gillespie, 1998), future research could be suggested that further

investigates the predictive utility of Callebaut et al.’s (1999) motivational framework and

checks our primary findings against actual choice or purchase behavior. In particular, a

challenging research opportunity can be found in the connection of motivational survey data

with consumers’ longitudinal purchase history which is available from scanner data nowadays.

In addition, though in its most fundamental form Callebaut et al.’s (1999) circumplex

taxonomy of consumer motives has been proven to be valid cross-culturally, it can be

assumed that culture specifics will exist in the ways in which these underlying motives

become manifest in consumer behavior. Hence, we must remark that caution is warranted

when extending the behavioral conclusions of our research to other countries and/or cultures

as differences in motivational expressions might exist due to the predominance of other

cultural dimensions (for an overview of the most important dimensions along which societies

may differ; see Hofstede, 1991). Future research may investigate these cultural differences in

motive expressions and behaviors and, by doing so, may provide additional insights into the

extent to which our behavioral findings can be generalized to other research countries beyond

Belgium.

323
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

5.2. A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives: Construct Validity

An intractable limitation of the studies conducted in the second research part of this

dissertation (Chapter IV & Chapter V) is the measurement issue. As noted earlier, within

these studies an alternative implicit method of motive assessment has been proposed, based

on respondents’ valence ratings of a set of 34 motive-expressive personality items. As

structured response formats (i.e. Likert scales) were used to apply this 34-item scale to the

personality of a preferred brand as a third-person, there is necessarily common method

variance in our measures of motives and other related constructs (i.e. personal values and

human personality traits), which might have biased our results. To account for this problem

to some extent, some unrelated filler tasks were included in the questionnaires that had to be

completed between the independent and dependent variables of interest.

Second, whereas our 34-item Consumer Motive Scale performed rather well in the research

conducted in Chapter IV, in our studies in Chapter V, we were not able to compute composite

construct measures for all eight motive domains due to unsatisfactory Cronbach’s α

coefficients. Hence, within these studies, reliable motive constructs had to be created based

on combinations of items of adjacent positions with high intercorrelations. Given this lack of

measurement invariance across different studies, future research should be conducted that

attempts to improve the general psychometric stability of the Consumer Motive Scale.

Especially, the use of interview administered questionnaires (e.g. face to face, CAPI) could be

proposed in future studies in which respondents are guided through the process of assessing

their underlying motives, as measurement instability in our research might be due to a lack of

experience of respondents with completing rather implicit motive questions in self

administered ways (cf. infra: section 6.2).

324
Chapter VIII : Summary and Discussion

In addition, another interesting pathway for future research implies the direct comparison of

our proposed method of motive assessment with traditional TAT-type measures of human

motives. Only by doing so reliable conclusions can be drawn regarding the impact of choice

of measurement method on the strength of the correlations between motives and other related

constructs such as personal values (Chapter IV) and human personality traits (Chapter V).

Besides the above limitations specifically related to the assessment of consumer motives,

some remarks have to be made with regard to the relationships found between motives and

other personality constructs. Though in general, the findings of our analyses provided support

for the majority of our propositions, especially in Chapter IV, still quite a few predictions

were not supported by the data. Whereas the signs of the multiple regression beta coefficients

truly went in the hypothesized directions, some of the proposed relationships between motives

and personal values appeared to be non-significant. To some extent, multi-collinearity

between the motivational predictor variables has been proposed as a possible contributor to

this problem, as some of the motive dimensions did show significant intercorrelations.

Furthermore, we have to admit that our attempt to trace the effect of underlying motives on

people’s value priorities is plagued by low predictive power (R² ≤ .16). However, as is the

case for previous investigations of individual value differences (e.g. Schiffmann et al., 2003;

Allen and Ng, 1999, 2003), the primary contribution of our research is to show a particular

pattern of relationships, more so than any absolute strength based on the predictive power.

Concerning Chapter V, we should recognize that the predictive utility of contingency models

in personality research that take into account the intervening role of one’s underlying motive

dispositions was not explicitly assessed. However, such models are assumed to have the

325
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

potential to account for considerable more variance in consumer behavior than the 10 percent

explained by trait-behavioral models as such. Hence, we propose that future research studies

should be established that examine this predictive ability more closely.

Finally, it is appropriate to notice that in the second research part of the present dissertation

we only concentrated on consumers’ personal values (Chapter IV) and two distinct

personality traits, i.e. consumer innovativeness and self-control (Chapter V) to assess the

construct validity of Callebaut et al.’s (1999) consumer motive taxonomy. Still a lot of

questions remain about the relationships between consumer motives and a wide range of other

personal and psychological concepts. Clearly, future personality research is needed that

further investigates Callebaut et al.’s (1999) taxonomy’s convergent and discriminant validity

by analyzing the correlations with these other concepts, such as additional personality traits

(e.g. need for uniqueness, impulsiveness, competitiveness, etc.) and motivational constructs.

In the latter context, future research studies may explore the linkages between consumer

motives and Higgins’ self-regulatory focus theory. Basically, this theory predicts that human

behavior is guided by two distinct motivational systems, each employing different strategic

means for self-regulation (Higgins, 1999; 2002). The promotion system, which originates in

the regulation of nurturance needs, relies on approach strategies when regulating toward

desirable ends, i.e. striving for the attainment/presence of positive behavioral consequences.

In contrast, the prevention system, which originates in the regulation of security needs, relies

on avoidance strategies when self-regulating toward desirable ends, i.e. striving for the

avoidance/absence of negative behavioral consequences. Several previous studies have linked

self-regulation to various consumer behaviors and contexts (e.g. Zhou and Pham, 2004; Louro

et al., 2005; Pham and Higgins, 2005). In the context of the present dissertation, one might

wonder how and to what extent Callebaut et al.’s (1999) eight consumer motives are related to

326
Chapter VIII : Summary and Discussion

Higgins’ basic principles of promotion and prevention motivation. Hence, future research can

be recommended that investigates these potential relationships more closely, hereby providing

useful insights into the similarities and differences between the two motivational frameworks.

5.3. A Taxonomy of Consumer Motives: Practical Applications

Similar to the previous chapters, several limitations concerning Chapter VI and Chapter VII

of the present dissertation are worth mentioning. First, only two specific research contexts

were considered to investigate the impact of health-related motive orientations on consumer

behavior, i.e. fruit and vegetable intake (Chapter VI) and ready meal consumption behavior

(Chapter VII). Future research studies may enhance the generalizability of the findings to

other contexts by assessing the relationships between health-related motive orientations and

other aspects of health-related consumer behavior (e.g. buying processes or decision factors)

or the consumption of other food products with a predominant health image (e.g. fish or

functional foods). Moreover, the usefulness of the proposed motive-related audience

segmentation approach could be demonstrated in the context of health prevention instead of

health promotion. For example, when attempting to develop increasingly successful

antismoking or fat reduction advertisements, it may be appropriate for health communicators

to consider the manifest health-related motive orientations of the target audience.

Second, as previous literature regarding the influence of health-related motive orientations on

people’s consumption patterns and behaviors is almost non-existing, the studies conducted in

Chapter VI and Chapter VII were mainly exploratory in nature. Hence, behavioral

propositions in these studies were inferred from our personal a priori knowledge about the

fundamental meaning of health for each of the five health audience segments. Though the

327
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

found results provided support for the majority of our propositions, to some extent, some

minor deviations emerged from what was proposed in advance. In particular, some of the a

priori proposed relationships between health-related motive orientations and consumer

behavior appeared to be non-significant, whereas others were slightly different from our

expectations. Hence, future research is advised to additionally explore these non expected

results. Also, based on the results that did support our basic propositions, an attempt could be

made at further investigating the behavioral impact of health-related motive orientations in a

less exploratory way.

Furthermore, as in the present dissertation only perceived effects of motive-related health

advertising and positioning strategies were assessed on consumers’ self-reported reactions

(i.e. Aad and BI), future experimental intervention studies may be designed that also

investigate actual behavioral effects, by assessing the potential effectiveness of motive-

related health marketing strategies in influencing real consumer behavior.

To conclude, yet a final reflection should be made regarding the practical suggestions and

recommendations raised in Chapter VI and Chapter VII for health communicators and

practitioners. As Callebaut et al.’s (1999) framework of eight consumer motives has been

proven to be valid cross-culturally, the derived segmentation approach based on consumers’

health-related motive orientations allows for more global health marketing strategies where

targeting and positioning are based on universal underlying dimensions of consumer behavior.

However, as the ways in which consumers express their underlying motives are likely to be

very much influenced by factors such as language, local culture and history, the execution of

global marketing strategies based on Callebaut et al.’s (1999) general framework will mostly

remain very local. Hence, caution is warranted when generalizing our practical

328
Chapter VIII : Summary and Discussion

recommendations regarding motive-related health advertising and positioning strategies to

other countries as specific implementation practices might be appropriate for audiences with

different cultural backgrounds.

6. Concluding remarks

To end this dissertation, some additional reflections should be made regarding Callebaut et

al.’s (1999) taxonomy of consumer motives, which scientific validation has been posited as

the main purpose of the present research project. More specifically, in the sections that

follow, an evaluation is provided of the latter motivational framework in the light of previous

human motive taxonomies that were discussed in Chapter II and, in addition, some critical

remarks are formulated regarding the usefulness of this taxonomy and its measurement

instability.

6.1. Link with previous taxonomies of human motives

Through the years, several authors have made fair attempts to classify the consumer motive

domain into a limited number of content categories. Besides some classic theories of

motivation (e.g. Murray, 1938; Maslow, 1943; McClelland, 1961), in Chapter II, we provided

an overview of three more recently developed human motive taxonomies, by respectively

Wicker et al. (1984), Ford and Nichols (1987) and Chulef et al. (2001). Though Callebaut et

al.’s (1999) motivational framework has shown to be compatible with all of these prior

classifications of human motives (cf. supra: Chapter III), the most obvious conceptual

parallels could be discovered with Ford and Nichols’ (1987) motive taxonomy, given these

authors’ primary distinction between within-person (i.e. intrapersonal) and person-

329
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

environment (i.e. interpersonal) motives, respectively consisting of affective (i.e. Pleasure)

versus cognitive (i.e. Control) and self-assertion (i.e. Power) versus affiliation (i.e. Belonging)

sub-domains. Hence, the latter motive taxonomy could be considered as providing the best

alternative to the consumer motive taxonomy of Callebaut et al. (1999), at least on a

conceptual level.

As a major dissimilarity between the motivational frameworks of Ford and Nichols (1987)

and Callebaut et al. (1999), it should be noted that the former taxonomy is primarily based on

the theoretical preconceptions of its developers, whereas Callebaut et al.’s (1999) consumer

motive taxonomy has been validated on an empirical basis, taking into account true consumer

thoughts and perceptions. Moreover, Callebaut et al.’s (1999) motivational framework could

be considered as more satisfactory than the Ford and Nichols (1987) taxonomy, as it

comprises a dynamic structure of continuous motive dimensions, rather than a static

framework of discrete motive categories. In addition, given its link with consumer brand

perceptions 32 , Callebaut et al.’s (1999) taxonomy could be positioned as a sophisticated

instrument to assess how consumer motives are at work in a particular brand category context,

rather than a basic framework of overall motive classification. Hence, based on the latter

characteristics, the use of the motivational framework of Callebaut et al. (1999) might be

recommended over the use of previous human motive classifications (e.g. Ford and Nichols,

1987) in a marketing context. However, some critical remarks should be taken into account

regarding the application of Callebaut et al.’s (1999) consumer motive taxonomy, especially

when considering the moderate psychometric properties (i.e. measurement instability) of the

particular Consumer Motive Scale that was developed in the context of the present

dissertation (cf. infra: section 6.2).

32
In the context of the present dissertation consumer motives were operationalized by specific brand personality items instead of more
general motive units (cf. supra: Chapter III).

330
Chapter VIII : Summary and Discussion

6.2. Additional critical reflections

Regarding the usefulness of Callebaut et al.’s (1999) taxonomy of consumer motives, this

framework has proven to have a lot of practical relevance, as many multinational companies

make use of it as an effective marketing tool (cf. supra: Chapter I). For instance, the

application of the latter framework may provide advantages in the context of consumer-

oriented market segmentation and targeting or may have impact on portfolio management

and/or communication and advertising development. Besides these managerial implications,

the motivational framework of Callebaut et al. (1999) should also be considered as having

scientific importance, as was indicated by the results of the research conducted in the present

dissertation (cf. supra: Chapter III – Chapter VII). However, some critical reflections should

be made regarding the scientific validation of Callebaut et al.’s (1999) taxonomy, as particular

psychometric problems could be detected in the context of measuring its eight underlying

constructs. Whereas our 34-item Consumer Motive Scale performed rather well in the studies

conducted in Chapter III, measurement instability appeared to be a problem in the remaining

chapters, as particular scale items failed to capture their underlying motive dimensions (i.e.

Chapter IV) and/or unsatisfactory reliability estimates restrained us from computing

composite construct measures for all eight motive domains (i.e. Chapter V & Chapter VI).

Though the specific product environment in which the research was conducted might play a

role in the bad performance of some items and, hence, it might be appropriate to reformulate

particular items in the context of different product categories, a lack of experience of

respondents with completing our implicit motive questions could be posited as a more

obvious reason for measurement instability to occur. Whereas in the research conducted in

Chapter III, fieldworkers of the market research company Synovate/Censydiam employed

331
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

personal interviews to guide respondents through the process of assessing their underlying

motives, in the context of the studies in the remaining chapters, our 34-item Consumer Motive

Scale had to be filled out in a self administered way (i.e. random walk methods, web surveys),

which may have implied difficult abstractions for our respondents in the context of the

projective nature of this scale. Hence, based on these insights, future research interventions

could be proposed that make use of interview administered questionnaires in an attempt to

improve the general psychometric stability of our Consumer Motive Scale. In particular,

computer or web-based questioning techniques might be recommended that allow for a more

interactive application of the latter scale, hereby providing more guidance for the respondents

and, as a result, a more reliable/stable assessment of people’s underlying motive orientations.

332
Chapter VIII : Summary and Discussion

References

Aaker, D.A., & Biel, A.L. (1993). Brand Equity and Advertising: Advertising’s role in

building strong brands. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Aaker, J.L. (1997). Dimensions of brand personality. Journal of Marketing Research, 34

(aug), 347- 356.

Aaker, J.L. (1999). The malleable self: the role of self-expression in persuasion. Journal of

Marketing Research, 36 (feb), 45-57.

Aaker, J.L., Benet-Martinez, V., & Garolera, J. (2001). Consumption symbols as carriers of

culture: a study of Japanese and Spanish brand personality constructs. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 81(3), 492-508.

Allen, N.W., & Ng, S.H. (1999). The direct and indirect influences of human values on

product ownership. Journal of Economic Psychology, 20(1), 5-39.

Allen, N.W., & Ng, S.H. (2003). Human Values, Utilitarian Benefits and Identification: the

case of meat. European Journal of Social Psychology, 33(1), 37-56.

Alpert, M.I. (1972). Personality and the determinants of product choice. Journal of

Marketing Research, 5(feb), 89-92.

Atkin, C.K., & Freimuth, V. (1989). Formative evaluation research in campaign design. In

R.E. Rice, & C.K. Atkin (Eds.). Public communication campaigns (2nd ed.) (pp. 131-

150). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Barsalou, L.W. (1985). Ideals, central tendency and frequency of instantiation as

determinants of graded structure in categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology,

11(4), 629-654.

333
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Batra, R., Lehmann, D.R., & Singh, D. (1993). The brand personality component of brand

goodwill: some antecedents and consequences. In D.A. Aaker, & A. Biel (Eds.),

Brand Equity and Advertising. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Belk, R.W. (1975). Situational variables and consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer

research, 2, 157-164.

Berrigan, J., & Finkbeiner, C. (1992). Segmentation marketing: new methods for capturing

business markets. New York: Harper Business.

Biel, A. (1993). Converting image into equity. In D.A. Aaker, & A. Biel (Eds.), Brand

Equity and Advertising (pp. 67-82). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Biernat, M. (1989). Motives and values to achieve: different constructs with different effects.

Journal of Personality, 57(1), 69-95.

Bilsky, W., & Schwartz, S.H. (1994). Values and personality. European Journal of

Personality, 8, 163-181.

Blackwell, R.D., Miniard, P.W., & Engel, J.F. (2001). Consumer Behavior (9th edition). Fort

Worth: Harcourt College Publishers.

Boslaugh, S.E., Kreuter, M.W., Nicholson, R.A., & Naleid, K. (2005). Comparing

demographic, health status and psychosocial strategies of audience segmentation to

promote physical activity. Health Education Research, 20(4), 430-438.

Brody, R.P., & Cunningham, S.M. (1968). Personality variables and the consumer decision

process. Journal of Marketing Research, 5(feb), 50-57.

Brunstein, J.C., & Schmitt, C.H. (2004). Assessing individual differences in achievement

motivation with the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Research in Personality, 38,

536-555.

Callebaut, J., Janssens, M., Op de Beeck, D., Lorré, D., & Hendrickx, H. (1999).

Motivational marketing research revisited. Leuven-Apeldoorn: Garant.

334
Chapter VIII : Summary and Discussion

Carroll, J.D., & Chang, J.J. (1970). Analysis of individual differences in multidimensional

scaling. Psychometrika, 35, 238–319.

Chulef, A.S., Read, S.J., & Walsh, D.A. (2001). A hierarchical taxonomy of human goals.

Motivation and Emotion, 25(3), 191-232.

Costa, A.I.A., Schoolmeester, D., Dekker, M., & Jongen, W.M.F. (2005). To cook or not to

cook: a means-end study of motives for choice of meal solutions. Food Quality and

Preference, xxx, xxx-xxx (available online 4 oct 2005 at www.sciencedirect.com).

Croft, M.J. (1994). Market segmentation. London: Routledge.

Cronbach, L.J., & Meehl, P.E. (1955). Construct validity in psychological tests.

Psychological Bulletin, 52, 281-302.

de Boer, M., McCarthy, M., Cowan, C., & Ryan, I. (2004). The influence of lifestyle

characteristics and beliefs about convenience food on the demand for convenience

foods in the Irish market. Food Quality and Preference, 15, 155-165.

Dickson, P.R. (1982). Person-situation: segmentation’s missing link. Journal of Marketing,

46, 56-64.

Dollinger, S.J., Leong, F.L., & Ulicni, S.K. (1996). On traits and values: with special

reference to openness to experience. Journal of Research in Personality, 30, 23-41.

Evans, F.B. (1959). Psychological and objective factors in the prediction of brand choice.

Journal of Business, 32 (oct), 340-369.

Fennis, B.M. (2003). Advertising, consumer behaviour and health: exploring possibilities for

health promotion. International Journal of Medical Marketing, 3,4, 316-326.

Ferrandi, J.M., Fine-Falcy, S., & Valette-Florence, P. (1999). L’échelle de personnalité des

marques de Aaker appliquée au contexte français: un premier test. Actes de l’AFM

(Mai).

335
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Ferrandi, J.M., Fine-Falcy, S., & Valette-Florence, P. (2001). Transposition d’ une échelle de

personnalité humaine aux marques: un premier test. Actes de l’AFM (Mai).

Ford, M.E., & Nichols, C.W. (1987). A taxonomy of human goals and some possible

applications. In M.E. Ford, & D.H. Ford (Eds.), Humans as Self-constructing

Systems: Putting the Framework to work. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Freimuth, V., Cole, G., & Kirby, S. (2001). Issues in evaluating mass media health

communication campaigns. In I. Rootman, M. Goodstadt, B. Hyndman, D.V.

McQueen, L. Potvin, J. Springett, & E. Ziglio (Eds.). Evaluation in health promotion:

Principles and perspectives (pp. 475-492). Copenhagen: WHO Regional Office for

Europe.

Freud, S. (1933). New introductory lectures on psycho-analysis. New York: W.W. Norton.

Graeff, T.R. (1997). Consumption situations and the effects of brand image on consumers’

brand evaluations. Psychology and Marketing, 14(jan), 49-70.

Grier, S., & Bryant, C.A. (2005). Social marketing in public health. Annual Review of Public

Health, 26, 319-339.

Gutman, J. (1982). A means–end chain model based on consumer categorization processes.

Journal of Marketing, 46(Spring), 60–72.

Haley, R.I. (1968). Benefit segmentation: a decision-oriented research tool. Journal of

Marketing, 32(july), 30-35.

Hastings, G.B., & Haywood, A.J. (1994). Social marketing: a critical response. Health

Promotion International, 9(1), 59-63.

Helgeson, J.G., & Supphellen, M. (2004). A conceptual and measurement comparison of

self-congruity and brand personality. International Journal of Market Research,

46(2), 205-233.

336
Chapter VIII : Summary and Discussion

Herringer, L.G. (1998). Relating values and personality traits. Psychological Reports, 83,

953-954.

Higgins, E.T. (1999). Promotion and prevention as a motivational duality: implications for

evaluative processes. In S. Chaiken, & Y. Trope (Eds.). Dual-process theories in

social psychology. New York: Guilford.

Higgins, E.T. (2002). How self-regulation creates distinct values: the case of promotion and

prevention decision-making. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 12(3), 177-191.

Hofstede, G. (1991). Cultures and Organizations. Berkshire: McGraw-Hill Book Company

Europe.

Jacoby, J. (1971). Multiple-indicant approaches for studying new product adopters. Journal

of Applied Psychology, 55, 384-388.

Kalar, S. (2003). Brands as complements to the self. Advances in Consumer Research, 31,

269.

Kassarjian, H.H., & Sheffet, M.J. (1991). Personality and consumer behavior: an update. In

H.H. Kassarjian and T.S. Robertson (Eds.), Perspectives in consumer behavior (p.

281-303). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Keller, K.L. (1993). Conceptualizing, measuring and managing customer-based brand equity.

Journal of marketing, 57 (jan), 1-22.

Kernan, J. (1968). Choice criteria, decision behavior and personality. Journal of Marketing

Research, 5(may), 155-164.

Kim, C.K., Han, D., & Park, S. (2001). The effect of brand personality and brand

identification on brand loyalty: applying the theory of social identification. Japanese

Psychological Research, 43(4), 195-206.

Knafo, A., & Schwartz, S.H. (2001). Value socialization in families of Israeli-born and

Soviet-born adolescents in Israel. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32, 213-228.

337
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Koebel, M.N., & Ladwein, R. (1999). L’échelle de personnalité de la marque de Jennifer L.

Aaker: adaption au contexte français. Décisions Marketing, 16, 81-88.

Kotler, P. (1997). Marketing management: analysis, planning, implementation and control.

New Jersey: Prentice-Hall Inc.

Landy, F.J. (1986). Stamp collecting versus science: validation as hypothesis testing.

American Psychologist, 41, 1183-1192.

Lastovicka, J.L., & Joachimsthaler, E.A. (1988). Improving the detection of personality-

behavior relationships in consumer research. Journal of Consumer Research,

14(March), 583-587.

Levy, S.J. (1959). Symbols for sales. Harvard Business Review, 37(4), 117-124.

Louro, M.J., Pieters, R., & Zeelenberg, M. (2005). Negative returns on positive emotions: the

influence of pride and self-regulatory goals on repurchase decisions. Journal of

Consumer Research, 31(4), 833-840.

Luk, C.L., & Bond, M.H. (1993). Personality variation and values endorsement in Chinese

university students. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 429-437.

Maibach, E., & Parrott, R.L. (1995). Designing health messages: approaches from

communication theory and public health practice. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Malhotra, N.K. (1988). Self-concept and product choice: an integrated perspective. Journal

of Economic Psychology, 9, 1-28.

Maslow, A. (1943). A theory of human motivation. Psychological Review, 50, 370-396.

Maslow, A.H. (1970). Motivation and personality (2nd ed.). New York: harper and Row.

McAdams, D.P., & Powers, J. (1981). Themes of intimacy in behavior and thought. Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 573-587.

McClelland, D.C. (1961). The achieving society. Princeton, New Jersey: Van Nostrand.

338
Chapter VIII : Summary and Discussion

McClelland, D.C. (1980). Motive dispositions: the merits of operant and respondent

measures. In L. Wheeler (Ed.), Review of Personality and Social Psychology (Vol. 1,

p. 10-41). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

McClelland, D.C., Koestner, R., & Weinberger, J. (1989). How do self attributed and implicit

motives differ? In F. Halisch, & J.H.L. Van Den Bercken (Eds.), International

perspectives on achievement and task motivation (p. 259-289). Amsterdam: Swets &

Zeitlinger.

McCrae, R.R., & Costa, P.T. (1996). Toward a new generation of personality theories:

theoretical contexts for the five-factor model. In J.S. Wiggins (Ed.), The five-factor

model of personality: theoretical perspectives (p. 51-87). New York: Guilford.

McDonald, S.S., & Goldman, A.E. (1980). Strategies of segmentation research. In G.B.

Hafer (Ed.), A look back, a look ahead (p. 30-42). Chicago: American Marketing

Association.

McGuire, W. (1974). Psychological motives and communication gratification. In J.F.

Blumer & Katz (Eds.). The uses of mass communication: current perspectives on

gratification research (pp.110-121). Beverly Hills: Sage.

Messick, S. (1980). Test validity and ethics of assessment. American Psychologist, 35, 1012-

1027.

Miller, K.E., & Ginter, J.L. (1979). An investigation of situational variation in brand choice

and attitude. Journal of Marketing Research, 16, 111-123.

Mischel, W. (1977). On the future of personality measurement. American Psychologist, 32,

246-254.

Murray, H.A. (1938). Explorations in Personality. New York: Oxford University Press.

339
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Olson, J.C., & Reynolds, T.J. (1983). Understanding consumers’ cognitive structures:

implications for marketing strategy. In L. Percy, & A.G. Woodside (Eds.).

Advertising and consumer psychology (pp. 77-90). Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.

Olver, J.M., & Mooradian, T.A. (2003). Personality traits and personal values: a conceptual

and empirical integration. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 109-125.

Pham, M., & Higgins, E.T. (2005). Promotion and prevention in consumer decision-making.

In S. Ratneshwar and D.G. Mick (Eds.), Inside consumption (p. 8-43). New York:

Routledge.

Plummer, J.T. (1985). How personality makes a difference. Journal of Advertising Research,

24, 27-31.

Rappoport, L., Peters, G.R., Downey, R., McCann, T., & Huff-Corzine, L. (1993). Gender

and age differences in food cognition. Appetite, 20, 33-52.

Read, S.J., & Lalwani, N. (2000). Testing a narrative model of trait inferences: the role of

goals. Unpublished manuscript, University of Southern California.

Read, S.J., Jones, D.K., & Miller, L.C. (1990). Traits as goal based categories, the

importance of goals in the coherence of dispositional categories. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 58(6), 1048-1061.

Robertson, T.S., & Myers, J.H. (1969). Personality correlates of opinion leadership and

innovative buying behavior. Journal of Marketing research, 7(May), 164-168.

Rohan, M.J., & Zanna, M.P. (1996). Value transmission in families. In C. Seligman, J.M.

Olson, & M.P. Zanna (Eds.), The Psychology of Values: the Ontario Symposium (Vol.

8) (p. 253-276). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Schiffman, L.G., Sherman, E., & Long, M.M. (2003). Toward a better understanding of the

interplay of personal values and the internet. Psychology and Marketing, 20(2), 169-

186.

340
Chapter VIII : Summary and Discussion

Schoenpflug, U. (2001). Perspectives on cultural transmission (special issue). Journal of

Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32(2).

Schultheiss, O.C., & Brunstein, J.C. (2001). Assessment of implicit motives with a research

version of the TAT: picture profiles, gender differences, and relations to other

personality measures. Journal of Personality Assessment, 77(1), 71-86.

Schwartz, S.H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: theoretical advances

and empirical tests in 20 countries. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 25,

1–65.

Schwartz, S.H., & Bilsky, W. (1987). Toward a universal psychological structure of human

values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 550-562.

Schwartz, S.H., & Sagiv, L. (1995). Identifying culture specifics in the content and structure

of values. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 26, 92-116.

Silk, K.J., Weiner, J., & Parrott, R.L. (2005). Gene Cuisine or Frankenfood? The theory of

reasoned action as an audience segmentation strategy for messages about genetically

modified foods. Journal of Health Communication, 10, 751-767.

Sirgy, M.J. (1982). Self-concept in consumer behavior: a critical review. Journal of

Consumer Research, 9, 287-300.

Sirgy, M.J. (1985). Using self-congruity and ideal congruity to predict purchase motivation.

Journal of Business Research, 13, 195-206.

Sirgy, M.J. (1986). Self-Congruity. New York: Praeger.

Slater, M., & Flora, J. (1994). Is health behavior consumer behavior? Health behavior

determinants, audience segmentation, and designing media health campaigns. In E.M.

Clark, T.C. Brock, & D.W. Stewart (Eds.). Attention, Attitude and Affect in Response

to Advertising (pp.273-285). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

341
Towards a Better Understanding of Motivational Consumer Behavior

Slater, M.D. (1996). Theory and method in health audience segmentation. Journal of Health

Communication, 1, 267-283.

Smith, C.P. (1992). Motivation and Personality: Handbook of thematic content analysis.

New York: Cambridge University Press.

Smith, G.T. (2005). On construct validity: issues of method and measurement. Psychological

Assessment, 17(4), 396-408.

Steptoe, A., Pollard, T.M., & Wardle, J. (1995). Development of a measure of the motives

underlying the selection of food: the food choice questionnaire. Appetite, 25, 267-284.

Tuerlinckx, F., De Boeck, P., & Lens, W. (2002). Measuring needs with the Thematic

Apperception Test: a psychometric study. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 82(3), 448-461.

Van Kenhove, P., De Wulf, K., & Van Waterschoot, W. (1999). The impact of task-

definition on store-attribute saliences and store choice. Journal of Retailing, 75(1),

125-137.

Verlegh, P.W.J., & Candel, M.J.J.M. (1999). The consumption of convenience foods:

reference groups and eating situations. Food Quality and Preference, 10, 457-464.

Weber, J., & Gillespie, J. (1998). Differences in ethical beliefs, intentions and behaviors.

Business and Society, 37(dec), 447-467.

Wicker, F.W., Lambert, F.B., Richardson, F.C., & Kahler, J. (1984). Categorical goal

hierarchies and classification of human motives. Journal of Personality, 52(3), 285-

305.

Williams, R.M. (1968). Values. In E. Sills (Ed.). International encyclopedia of the social

sciences (pp. 89-103). New York: Macmillan.

342
Chapter VIII : Summary and Discussion

Winter, D.G., John, O.P., Stewart, A.J., Klohnen, E.C., & Duncan, L.E. (1998). Traits and

motives: toward an integration of two traditions in personality research.

Psychological Review, 105(2), 230-250.

Yik, M.S.M., & Tang, C.S. (1996). Linking personality and values: the importance of a

culturally relevant personality scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 767-

774.

Youniss, R.P., Lorr, M., & Stefic, E.C. (1979). Assessment of motives and orientations.

Psychological Reports, 45, 555-561.

Zhou, R.R., & Pham, M.T. (2004). Promotion and prevention across mental accounts: when

financial products dictate consumers’ investment goals. Journal of Consumer

Research, 31(1), 125-135.

343

You might also like