You are on page 1of 7

Media Multitasking and

Cognitive, Psychological, Neural,


and Learning Differences
Melina R. Uncapher, PhD,​a Lin Lin, PhD,​b Larry D. Rosen, PhD,​c Heather L. Kirkorian, PhD,​d Naomi S. Baron, PhD,​e
Kira Bailey, PhD,​f Joanne Cantor, PhD,​g David L. Strayer, PhD,​h Thomas D. Parsons, PhD,​i Anthony D. Wagner, PhDj

abstract American youth spend more time with media than any other waking activity: an average
of 7.5 hours per day, every day. On average, 29% of that time is spent juggling multiple
media streams simultaneously (ie, media multitasking). This phenomenon is not limited
to American youth but is paralleled across the globe. Given that a large number of media
multitaskers (MMTs) are children and young adults whose brains are still developing,
there is great urgency to understand the neurocognitive profiles of MMTs. It is critical to
understand the relation between the relevant cognitive domains and underlying neural
structure and function. Of equal importance is understanding the types of information
processing that are necessary in 21st century learning environments. The present review
surveys the growing body of evidence demonstrating that heavy MMTs show differences
in cognition (eg, poorer memory), psychosocial behavior (eg, increased impulsivity), and
neural structure (eg, reduced volume in anterior cingulate cortex). Furthermore, research
indicates that multitasking with media during learning (in class or at home) can negatively
affect academic outcomes. Until the direction of causality is understood (whether media
multitasking causes such behavioral and neural differences or whether individuals with
such differences tend to multitask with media more often), the data suggest that engagement
with concurrent media streams should be thoughtfully considered. Findings from such
research promise to inform policy and practice on an increasingly urgent societal issue
while significantly advancing our understanding of the intersections between cognitive,
psychosocial, neural, and academic factors.

One of the first influential reports on the phenomenon of media multitasking (MMT) revealed
that American youth spend more time with media than any other activity: an average of 7.5
hours per day every day of the week, and 29% of that time is spent juggling multiple media
streams simultaneously‍1 (note that “media” refers to all forms of mediated communication of
information or data). Even younger children spend ∼2 hours per day with screen media,​‍2 and
half of 5- to 8-year-olds engage in MMT at least occasionally.‍2 MMT, however, is not just an
NIH

aDepartment of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, Sandler Neurosciences Center, San Francisco, California; Departments of bLearning Technologies and iPsychology,

University of North Texas, Denton, Texas; cDepartment of Psychology, California State University, Dominguez Hills, Carson, California; Departments of dHuman Development and Family
Studies and gCommunication Arts, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, Wisconsin; eCenter for Teaching, Research, and Learning, American University, Washington, DC; fDepartment of
Psychology, Ohio Wesleyan University, Delaware, Ohio; hDepartment of Psychology, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, Utah; and jDepartment of Psychology, Neurosciences Program, Stanford
University, Stanford, California;

Dr Uncapher conceptualized and drafted the initial manuscript, organized and co-chaired the National Academies of Science working group on media multitasking;
Dr Wagner co-chaired the National Academies of Science working group on media multitasking, reviewed and revised the manuscript; Dr Lin reviewed and revised
the manuscript; Drs Rosen, Kirkorian, Baron, Bailey, Cantor, Strayer, and Parsons critically reviewed the manuscript and provided in-depth comments; and all
coauthors approved the final manuscript as submitted and were members of the National Academies of Science working group on media multitasking.
The analysis, conclusions, and recommendations contained in each article are solely a product of the individual working group and are not the policy or opinions of,
nor do they represent an endorsement by, Children and Screens: Institute of Digital Media and Child Development or the American Academy of Pediatrics.

Supplement Article Downloaded from http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/ byPEDIATRICS Volume 140,


guest on November 4, number
2017 s2, November 2017:e20161758
American phenomenon. Kononova‍3 heavy MMT is associated with effects, see ref ‍11,​15), and long-term
investigated MMT behaviors across 3 cognitive differences even when memory.‍10
countries (the United States, Russia, people are performing single tasks.
High degrees of MMT might be
and Kuwait) and reported many These findings raise the possibility
viewed as a form of cognitive training
parallels to the foregoing findings.1,​2‍ that the growing prevalence of MMT
that could confer benefits on other
The ubiquity of media use and may impact everyday cognition
tests of multitasking and/or task-
concomitant MMT have generated beyond the classroom.
switching abilities. However, there is
much scientific and societal interest
Here, we distinguish between currently mixed evidence in support
in determining if and how MMT
investigations of (1) the cognitive, of this idea, with some reports of
impacts affect behavior, cognition,
psychological, and neural profiles HMMs outperforming LMMs during
and brain structure and function.
of individuals who engage in task-switching and/or dual-tasking,​
16
‍ whereas others show the opposite
It is currently unknown whether various levels of MMT, and (2) the
differences in underlying neural and consequences of multitasking with effect,​‍11 and the majority show null
cognitive traits lead to differing levels media while learning (whether effects.‍8–10,​
‍ 12,​
‍ 14,​
‍ 16

of MMT or vice versa. Given that a in classroom or informal learning It is important to note that the
large number of media multitaskers environments; for review of media– heterogeneity in demographics,
(MMTs) are children and young non-MMT, see ref ‍5). tasks, administration methods,
adults, whose brains are still power, and analytic approach,
developing, there is great urgency Cognitive, Psychological, and Neural combined with the small number of
to understand the neurocognitive Profiles of MMTs studies contributing to the literature
profiles of MMTs. It is critical to on each cognitive domain have
build a systematic understanding of Cognitive Profiles of MMTs
contributed to a complex pattern
the relation between the relevant of findings. However, the weight of
Cognitive performance in MMTs
cognitive domains and underlying the evidence overall points to HMMs
has been assessed in the domains
neural structure and function. Of demonstrating reduced performance
of working memory, interference
equal importance is understanding in a number of cognitive domains
management, attention, task-goal
the types of information processing relative to LMMs.
management, inhibitory control,
that are necessary in 21st century
relational reasoning, and long-term
learning environments. Psychological Profiles of MMTs
memory. Working memory studies
reveal that heavier MMTs (HMMs) In addition to cognitive differences,
often underperform relative to heavier MMT is associated with
Current State: Rising Prevalence lighter MMTs (LMMs),​‍6–10 ‍‍‍ although differences in psychosocial variables.
of MMT and Associated Cognitive,
some studies show no difference as For instance, several independent
Psychological, Neural, and
Learning Differences a function of MMT.‍9,​11 ‍ –‍ 13
‍ Likewise, groups have reported that MMT
studies investigating interference behavior positively correlates
A growing body of research management (tasks requiring with trait impulsivity, with HMMs
investigating MMT has generally, filtering out distracting information, exhibiting higher impulsivity
but not always, revealed reduced either from the external environment ratings‍6,​7,​
‍ 10,​
‍ 12
‍ (for null finding, see
performance on cognitive tasks and [ie, from perception] or the internal ref 17). In one study, ratings on the
well-being surveys. Such research environment [ie, from memory]) attentional subscale in particular
has raised concerns among parents reveal that HMMs underperform related to the foregoing memory
and educators regarding the impact relative to LMMs in some contexts,​8,​11,​‍ 14
‍ differences, with higher attentional
of MMT behavior during certain types 7,​
9 – 12
but not in others.‍ ‍ ‍ ‍ In the domain impulsivity predicting worse working
of activities, such as doing homework of attention, HMMs appear to have memory performance and higher
and learning in technology-rich difficulty on tasks that require MMT scores.‍11 Beyond impulsivity,
classrooms. Indeed, many teenagers sustained, goal-directed attention‍15 MMT has been associated with
report at least sometimes doing (for null finding, see ref ‍12). Further increased sensation-seeking‍6 (for a
homework while using another support for this possibility is the null finding, see ref ‍17), social anxiety
medium, including television (51% of finding that an attention-training and depression,​‍18 lower perceived
teenagers), social networking (50%), intervention can partially remediate social success,​19 and neuroticism,​‍18
text messaging (60%), and listening some of the underperformance by as well as a lower belief that
to music (76%).‍4 Importantly, extant HMMs.9 Finally, HMMs underperform intelligence is malleable (growth
evidence suggests that multitasking on tests of relational reasoning,​‍13 mindset).‍7 Given the limited number
disrupts concurrent learning, and inhibitory control‍9,​15
‍ (for null of observations bearing on each of

PEDIATRICS Volume 140, number s2,Downloaded


November 2017
from http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/ by guest on November 4, 2017 S63
these psychosocial variables, there is Multitasking With Media While task and a memorization task. With
a critical need for further research on Learning respect to reading as a primary task,
their associations with MMT. background television interferes
Research from multiple parts of the
with students’ ability to comprehend
world demonstrates that students,
Neural Profiles of MMTs narratives, especially when actively
while attempting to learn academic
watching the television program.‍28
To date, only 2 studies have information, frequently engage with
Similarly, reading while answering
investigated neural profiles media not relevant to the task at
instant messages dramatically
in MMTs.‍14,​17
‍ Loh and Kanai‍20 hand. A majority of college students
reduces reading efficiency. In one
investigated structural profiles in the United States and Israel report
study, the time taken to read a
in MMT adults and showed that, using electronic media while in class,
passage increased from 29 minutes
relative to LMMs, HMMs exhibited studying, or doing homework.‍21,​22‍
when not IM’ing to 49 minutes
less gray matter volume in the Parents of young children report
when IM’ing.‍29 Doing homework
anterior cingulate cortex, a region that multitasking during homework
while sending instant messages may
broadly implicated in cognitive begins by 5 to 8 years of age.‍2 Even
not only slow down and degrade
and/or social-emotional control. when children are not deliberately
performance but also has been
By using functional neuroimaging multitasking with 2 different
reported to negatively impact the
of adolescents and young mediums, they are often required to
learner’s perceived ability to perform
adults performing a sentence ignore background media to focus on
homework.‍22 Other data suggest
comprehension task in the presence a primary task, such as when infants
that accuracy on problem-solving
versus absence of distraction, 1 play with toys while a parent watches
homework tasks suffers as students
study found that heavier MMT was television. However, research
switch more frequently to other
associated with greater distraction- indicates that multitasking with
computer-based tasks.28
related activity in several prefrontal media during learning can negatively
regions implicated in attention affect academic outcomes,​‍23 and
Background television affects even
processing.‍17 The researchers posited background media can reduce the
the youngest children during play,
that greater attentional effort is quality of concurrent activities, such
which is integral to cognitive, social,
required by HMMs when performing as homework24 and toy play.‍25 The
and emotional development during
under conditions of distraction. research outlined below supports
infancy and early childhood (see ref
Although these studies suggest the American Academy of Pediatrics
‍30). Just as older children do not
possible anatomical and functional policy statements that discourage
perform as well on homework in the
differences in people who frequently media use while older children do
presence of background television,
media multitask versus those who homework or while infants and
infants have shorter episodes of
do not, a fuller investigation of how young children play.
play and focused attention when
structural and functional networks
Concurrent Multitasking and background television is present.‍25
manifest differently in HMMs versus
Background Media
LMMs is needed, as are further
investigations of task-based regional College students learn less when Long-Term Development and
Achievement
differences in function. dividing attention between listening
to lectures and interacting with
Interim Summary Fewer studies have examined
handheld devices, whether sending
potential long-term impacts of
or receiving text messages‍26 or social
The foregoing findings suggest that MMT and background media.
networking and instant messaging
there are differences in the cognitive, Results are correlational and
(IM’ing) (ie, exchanging text
psychological, and neural profiles mixed. One dominant method of
messagesin real time).‍27
associated with heavier versus assessing students’ learning is
lighter MMTs, and that this is the Just as MMT disrupts learning grade point average (GPA). Studies
case even when performing single in a classroom setting, MMT investigating multitasking with
tasks. Given that many academic and and background media disrupt media while learning (eg, receiving
career success outcomes depend learning while doing homework. In in-class texts‍22) have demonstrated
on optimizing performance during several studies of eighth graders a negative association with GPA.
single tasks (eg, test-taking or giving in the Netherlands, Pool et al‍24 Although Lin et al‍31 reported a
a presentation), a full understanding demonstrated that watching a Dutch- limited association between MMT
of the multidimensional profiles language soap opera while working and GPA, they did find that Facebook
of HMMs during single-tasking is on a task reduced accuracy and and text message use negatively
imperative. speed on both a paper-and-pencil predicted GPA (see ref ‍22).

S64 Downloaded from http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/ by guest on November 4, 2017 Uncapher et al


Future Research interventions needed for different Clinicians and Educators
An overarching goal of any current populations (eg, age of onset, dose, Until we are able to understand the
national research agenda should type, neurocognitive profile)? What direction of causality, suggest to
be to address existing knowledge are the critical pedagogical issues people of all ages and abilities that
gaps by building a body of research regarding MMT in K-12 education? they give careful consideration to
on the relations between MMT and •• Can we leverage technology to how they engage with media. Note
cognitive, psychosocial, neural, and develop more direct measures that there is an immediate impact
academic factors. A rich body of of MMT behavior, reducing of MMT on concurrent learning.
well-established research on these measurement error? Regarding long-term outcomes, raise
relations is needed to guide evidence- •• How can scholars from awareness of differences associated
based policymaking. Questions different disciplines conduct with MMT but also emphasize that
should include: interdisciplinary research and we currently do not know if such
•• Does MMT change brain and use multiple research methods behavior creates these differences
behavior, or do preexisting state or to better understand MMT and (ie, debunk myth of established
trait variable differences increase learning? causality).
the probability of MMT behavior? Policy Makers
Findings from such research
•• Are younger populations programs promise to inform policy Advocate for increased funding to
particularly vulnerable? Is there examine the relation between MMT,
and practice on an increasingly
a sensitive period wherein MMT cognition, and brain structure and
urgent social issue while significantly
should be limited or avoided? function, and to determine causality.
advancing our understanding of the
•• What makes media multitasking intersections between cognitive,
different from non-media psychosocial, neural, and academic
multitasking? Do specific types of factors. Abbreviations
MMT affect cognitive, psychosocial,
neural, or academic achievement GPA: grade point average
factors differently? Do effects vary Recommendations HMMs: heavier media
depending on activity and context multitaskers
Until the causal directionality is
(eg, listening to lecture, engaging IM’ing: instant messaging
known, it is premature to offer
with social media, studying)? LMMs: lighter media
strong recommendations and
multitaskers
•• What interventions are effective guidelines. However, the following
MMT: media multitasking
in remediating the effects of MMT, may be considered as preliminary
MMTs: media multitaskers
when, and for whom? Are different recommendations:

DOI: https://​doi.​org/​10.​1542/​peds.​2016-​1758D
Accepted for publication Apr 19, 2017
Address correspondence to Melina R. Uncapher, Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, Sandler Neurosciences Center, 675 Nelson
Rising Ln, San Francisco, CA 94158. E-mail: melina.uncapher@ucsf.edu
PEDIATRICS (ISSN Numbers: Print, 0031-4005; Online, 1098-4275).
Copyright © 2017 by the American Academy of Pediatrics
FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.
FUNDING: This special supplement, “Children, Adolescents, and Screens: What We Know and What We Need to Learn,​” was made possible through the financial
support of Children and Screens: Institute of Digital Media and Child Development. Drs Uncapher and Wagner were supported by National Institute of Mental
Health grant R21-MH099812. Funded by the National Institutes of Health (NIH).
POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

References
1. Rideout VJ, Foehr UG, Roberts DF. 2. Rideout V. Zero to eight: children’s zero-​to-​eight-​childrens-​media-​use-​in-​
Generation M2: Media in the Lives of media use in America 2013. america-​2013. Accessed April 19, 2017
8-to 18-Year-Olds. Menlo Park, CA: Henry Available at: https://​www.​ 3. Kononova A. Multitasking across
J. Kaiser Family Foundation; 2010 commonsensemedia.​org/​research/​ borders: a cross-national study of

PEDIATRICS Volume 140, number s2,Downloaded


November 2017
from http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/ by guest on November 4, 2017 S65
media multitasking behaviors, its in heavy media multitaskers. Psychon 22. Junco R, Cotten SR. Perceived
antecedents, and outcomes. Int J Bull Rev. 2013;20(6):1274–1281 academic effects of instant messaging
Commun. 2013;7:1688–1710 use. Comput Educ. 2011;56(2):370–378
13. Baumgartner SE, Lemmens JS, Weeda
4. Rideout VJ. The Common Sense WD, Huizinga M. Measuring media 23. Carrier LM, Rosen LD, Cheever NA, Lim
census: media use by tweens and multitasking: development of a short AF. Causes, effects, and practicalities
teens. Available at: https://​www.​ measure of media multitasking of everyday multitasking. Dev Rev.
commonsensemedia.​org/​research/​ for adolescents. J Media Psychol. 2015;35:64–78
the-​common-​sense-​census-​media-​use-​ 2016;1–10 24. Pool MM, Van der Voort THA, Beentjes
by-​tweens-​and-​teens. Accessed April JWJ, Koolstra CM. Background
19, 2017 14. Moisala M, Salmela V, Hietajärvi L, et al.
Media multitasking is associated with television as an inhibitor of
5. van der Schuur WA, Baumgartner distractibility and increased prefrontal performance on easy and difficult
SE, Sumter SR, Valkenburg PM. The activity in adolescents and young homework assignments. Commun Res.
consequences of media multitasking adults. Neuroimage. 2016;134:113–121 2000;27(3):293–326
for youth: a review. Comput Human 25. Schmidt ME, Pempek TA, Kirkorian HL,
Behav. 2015;53:204–215 15. Ralph BCW, Thomson DR, Seli P,
Carriere JSA, Smilek D. Media Lund AF, Anderson DR. The effects of
6. Sanbonmatsu DM, Strayer DL, multitasking and behavioral measures background television on the toy play
Medeiros-Ward N, Watson JM. Who of sustained attention. Atten Percept behavior of very young children. Child
multi-tasks and why? Multi-tasking Psychophys. 2015;77(2):390–401 Dev. 2008;79(4):1137–1151
ability, perceived multi-tasking ability, 26. Rosen LD, Carrier LM, Cheever NA.
impulsivity, and sensation seeking. 16. Alzahabi R, Becker MW. The association
Facebook and texting made me do it:
PLoS One. 2013;8(1):e54402 between media multitasking, task-
media-induced task-switching while
switching, and dual-task performance.
7. Cain MS, Leonard JA, Gabrieli JD, Finn studying. Comput Human Behav.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform.
AS. Media multitasking in adolescence. 2013;29(3):948–958
2013;39(5):1485–1495
Psychon Bull Rev. 2016;23(6):1932–1941 27. Wood E, Zivcakova L, Gentile P, Archer
17. Shih SI. A null relationship between
8. Cardoso-Leite P, Kludt R, Vignola G, Ma K, De Pasquale D, Nosko A. Examining
media multitasking and well-being.
WJ, Green C, Bavelier D. Technology the impact of off-task multi-tasking
PLoS One. 2013;8(5):e64508
consumption and cognitive control: with technology on real-time
contrasting action video game 18. Becker MW, Alzahabi R, Hopwood CJ. classroom learning. Comput Educ.
experience with media multitasking. Media multitasking is associated with 2012;58(1):365–374
Atten Percept Psychophys. symptoms of depression and social 28. Adler RF, Benbunan-Fich R. Self-
2016;78(1):218–241 anxiety. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. interruptions in discretionary
9. Gorman TE, Green CS. Short-term 2013;16(2):132–135 multitasking. Comput Human Behav.
mindfulness intervention reduces the 19. Pea R, Nass C, Meheula L, et al. Media 2013;29(4):1441–1449
negative attentional effects associated use, face-to-face communication, 29. Bowman LL, Levine LE, Waite BM,
with heavy media multitasking. Sci media multitasking, and social well- Gendron M. Can students really
Rep. 2016;6:24542 being among 8- to 12-year-old girls. Dev multitask? An experimental study
10. Uncapher MR, K Thieu M, Wagner AD. Psychol. 2012;48(2):327–336 of instant messaging while reading.
Media multitasking and memory: 20. Loh KK, Kanai R. Higher media multi- Comput Educ. 2010;54(4):927–931
differences in working memory and tasking activity is associated with 30. Singer DG, Golinkoff RM, Hirsh-Pasek
long-term memory. Psychon Bull Rev. smaller gray-matter density in the K. Play = Learning: How Play Motivates
2016;23(2):483–490 anteriorcingulate cortex. PLoS One. and Enhances Children’s Cognitive and
11. Ophir E, Nass C, Wagner AD. 2014;9(9):e106698 Social-Emotional Growth. New York, NY:
Cognitive control in media Oxford University Press; 2006
21. Jacobsen WC, Forste R. The wired
multitaskers. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. generation: academic and social 31. Lin L, Robertson T, Lee J. Reading
2009;106(37):15583–15587 outcomes of electronic media performances between novices
12. Minear M, Brasher F, McCurdy M, Lewis use among university students. and experts in different media
J, Younggren A. Working memory, Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. multitasking environments. Comput
fluid intelligence, and impulsiveness 2011;14(5):275–280 Sch. 2009;26(3):169–186

S66 Downloaded from http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/ by guest on November 4, 2017 Uncapher et al


Media Multitasking and Cognitive, Psychological, Neural, and Learning
Differences
Melina R. Uncapher, Lin Lin, Larry D. Rosen, Heather L. Kirkorian, Naomi S. Baron,
Kira Bailey, Joanne Cantor, David L. Strayer, Thomas D. Parsons and Anthony D.
Wagner
Pediatrics 2017;140;S62
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2016-1758D

Updated Information & including high resolution figures, can be found at:
Services http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/140/Supplement_2/S62
References This article cites 26 articles, 1 of which you can access for free at:
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/140/Supplement_2/S62.f
ull#ref-list-1
Permissions & Licensing Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures, tables) or
in its entirety can be found online at:
https://shop.aap.org/licensing-permissions/
Reprints Information about ordering reprints can be found online:
http://classic.pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/reprints

Pediatrics is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly publication, it
has been published continuously since . Pediatrics is owned, published, and trademarked by the
American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point Boulevard, Elk Grove Village, Illinois,
60007. Copyright © 2017 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN:
.

Downloaded from http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/ by guest on November 4, 2017


Media Multitasking and Cognitive, Psychological, Neural, and Learning
Differences
Melina R. Uncapher, Lin Lin, Larry D. Rosen, Heather L. Kirkorian, Naomi S. Baron,
Kira Bailey, Joanne Cantor, David L. Strayer, Thomas D. Parsons and Anthony D.
Wagner
Pediatrics 2017;140;S62
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2016-1758D

The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is
located on the World Wide Web at:
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/140/Supplement_2/S62

Pediatrics is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly publication, it
has been published continuously since . Pediatrics is owned, published, and trademarked by the
American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point Boulevard, Elk Grove Village, Illinois,
60007. Copyright © 2017 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN:
.

Downloaded from http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/ by guest on November 4, 2017

You might also like