You are on page 1of 10

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/51191417

Cortisol, sleep, and recovery - Some gender


differences but no straight associations

Article in Psychoneuroendocrinology · June 2011


DOI: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.05.003 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

17 79

5 authors, including:

Frida Eek Björn Karlson


Lund University Lund University
35 PUBLICATIONS 523 CITATIONS 58 PUBLICATIONS 1,423 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Anne Helene Garde Åse Marie Hansen


National Research Centre for the Working En… University of Copenhagen
121 PUBLICATIONS 2,288 CITATIONS 235 PUBLICATIONS 4,223 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Working hours, health, well-being and participation in working life (WOW) – Creating new working
time models and solutions for Nordic countries View project

Current research projects View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Åse Marie Hansen on 04 August 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. All in-text references underlined in blue are added to the original document
and are linked to publications on ResearchGate, letting you access and read them immediately.
Psychoneuroendocrinology (2012) 37, 56—64

a v a i l a b l e a t w w w. s c i e n c e d i r e c t . c o m

j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w. e l s e v i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / p s y n e u e n

Cortisol, sleep, and recovery — Some gender


differences but no straight associations
Frida Eek a,*, Björn Karlson a, Anne Helene Garde b, Åse Marie Hansen b,
Palle Ørbæk b

a
Department of Occupational and Environmental Medicine, Barngatan 2, Lund University Hospital, SE-221 85 Lund, Sweden
b
National Research Centre for the Working Environment, Lersø Parkallé 105, DK-2100 København Ø, Denmark

Received 26 October 2010; received in revised form 5 May 2011; accepted 6 May 2011

KEYWORDS Summary
Cortisol; Background: Work related fatigue has been suggested as a link in the assumed sequence of events
Recovery; between repeated adverse work demands and the development of work related stress, which may
Sleep; be associated with changes in concentrations of cortisol, psychological overload and, in the long
Fatigue run, health problems. Insufficient sleep is a contributing factor to lack of recovery, but previous
studies on associations between subjective aspects of sleep and recovery, and cortisol, have been
inconclusive. The aim with the present study was to examine possible associations between
cortisol measures and (I) self-rated recovery, (II) occupational fatigue and (III) subjective sleep
quality the night preceding cortisol sampling. Further, possible gender differences were tested.
Methods: Salivary cortisol was measured in 581 persons during a working day, at awakening,
+30 min and in the evening. Various measures of subjective sleep and recovery were analyzed in
relation to cortisol.
Results: Few correlations between cortisol and any sleep- or recovery parameters were found.
However, some significant associations were found between cortisol and a few measures of more
chronic aspects of sleep and recovery. Gender stratified analyses showed somewhat differing
associations among men and women. This indicates that possible associations and pathways
between lack of recovery/sleepiness and cortisol, and in the long run, unhealth, may not be
similar for men and women.
# 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction consequences of exposure to stress depend on cognitive


mechanisms (Ursin and Eriksen, 2001). Therefore, it is logical
According to many stress theories, such as the Cognitive to expect an association between subjective reports of stress
Activation Theory of Stress (CATS), the responses to and and physiological stress responses such as alterations in the
HPA-axis’ release of cortisol. One of the most early stress
theories, formulated by Selye, is the general adaptation
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +46 46 177437; fax: +46 46 177285. syndrome (GAS). According to GAS, exposure to a stressor
E-mail address: frida.eek@med.lu.se (F. Eek). is followed by three stages, with similar patterns regardless

0306-4530/$ — see front matter # 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.05.003
Cortisol and recovery 57

of the nature of the stressor (Browne et al., 1939). The acute effects. Insufficient sleep may also in itself be an effect of
phase (phase one), often described as the ‘‘fight or flight’’ ongoing stress activation and hence likely to be associated
phase, is triggered by exposure to any kind of potentially with cortisol measures. The association between sleep and
harmful stimulus. In this phase, the body quickly mobilizes cortisol levels have previously been examined in various
energy to handle the potential threat (Cannon, 1929). After laboratory settings. Total sleep deprivation in laboratory
this acute and rapid mobilization comes a stage of resistance studies have been found to result in a significant decrease
or adaptation, when the physiological response decline in plasma cortisol levels the next day, while indices of sleep
(Selye, 1938). If the duration of the stressor exposure con- disturbance, i.e. wake and stage 1 sleep, were positively
tinues, the body may eventually reach a stage of exhaustion, correlated with cortisol levels (Vgontzas et al., 1999). Partial
the third phase. This stage is considered as a dysfunctional and total sleep deprivations have also been shown to result in
state with hormonal imbalance. In this stage, when exposed increased evening cortisol (Leproult et al., 1997; Spiegel
to a stressor that would normally result in a strong response, et al., 1999). Polysomnographically recorded sleep in the
the physiological response is instead weak, i.e. the body has home environment have shown associations between
lost the ability to mobilize the energy needed to handle a increased frequency of micro arousals during sleep and
potential threat. increased concentrations of cortisol upon awakening the
Another term, frequently used in stress theory, is the following morning (Ekstedt et al., 2004). Previous studies
concept of allostasis, ‘‘stability through change’’. Most phy- on associations between subjective aspects of sleep and
siological variables show a diurnal variation, determined by recovery, and cortisol, have been inconclusive. Subjective
specific behavioral states and environmental events (Sterling sleep disturbances have been found to correlate with
and Eyer, 1988). Important for maintaining health is the decreased awakening cortisol concentrations (Backhaus
ability to respond by appropriate arousal when facing an et al., 2004). Subjective measures of insufficient recovery
environmental challenge. Hence, health may be defined as a from work were in one study associated with high morning
state of responsiveness, including the ability to restitute as cortisol in 25 female and male white collar workers (Gus-
well. Sustained arousal, resulting from an insufficient resti- tafsson et al., 2008). Another study found a long-term rela-
tution may eventually lead to the inability to respond appro- tionship between subjective need for recovery and evening
priately (Sterling and Eyer, 1988). cortisol among women, but not among men. However, the
Hence, according to the stress theories mentioned above, association was in the opposite direction of what was
an acute stress response could be expected to result in expected; high need for recovery was associated with lower
increased release of cortisol. On the other hand, a prolonged evening cortisol levels four years later (Rydstedt et al.,
exposure to stress not being adequately coped with or suffi- 2009). In a study of shift workers morning cortisol showed
ciently recovered from has been suggested to result in a significant positive correlations with ‘‘diurnal type’’ (morn-
gradually decreased cortisol levels and eventually a flattened ing person) and sufficient sleep before the morning shift, but
day profile, and in the long run increase the risk for negative several sleep- and recovery parameters such as exhaustion
health effects (McEwen, 1998; Sluiter et al., 2001). Although upon awakening, disturbed sleep, and sleepiness problem did
the cortisol awakening response (CAR) has frequently been not show any significant general correlation with morning
used as a measure of (anticipatory) stress, later research has cortisol in this study (Axelsson et al., 2003).
suggested that the most important aspect of a ‘‘healthy’’ Some gender differences have previously been found in
cortisol pattern may be the dynamic over the day, i.e. the sleep-endocrine regulation as well as in the overall regula-
ability to mobilize energy when appropriate, but also to tion and secretion of cortisol and other stress related
restitute during periods of rest, reflected in low evening hormones. Women were found to have higher plasma cor-
cortisol values. Hence, diurnal deviation measures have tisol levels during the first part of the night, and showed
recently gained more attention. slightly different EEG-recorded sleep parameters compared
Work related fatigue, and thus a high need for recovery, has to men, although this difference was not reflected in
been suggested as a link in the assumed sequence of events different HPA axis activity during the second half of the
between repeated adverse work demands and the develop- night (or the night as a whole) (Antonijevic et al., 1999).
ment of work related stress, psychological overload and, in the Pre-menstrual women appeared to have slightly lower mean
long run, health problems (Sluiter et al., 2003). Work char- cortisol levels over the day, compared to men in similar
acteristics are, however, not the only cause of need for ages. This difference was mostly attributable to lower
recovery; work characteristics alone explained 39% of the morning peak values. Among female students, the cortisol
variation in subjective need for recovery (Sluiter et al., awakening response was higher during a period of high
2001). Persons suffering from low levels of self-rated recovery, examination stress, while the CAR was not affected among
thus classified as fatigued, have been found having an men (Weekes et al., 2008). Women, but not men, also
increased risk for high allostatic load, although cortisol was showed an age-related elevation in the morning cortisol
not included in this measure (von Thiele et al., 2006). Allo- levels (Van Cauter et al., 1996). However, a later study
static load was here defined as an aggregated index of various instead found age related changes (increase) in CAR among
biomarkers including measures of cardiovascular activity, men but not among women (Almeida et al., 2009), and
blood lipids and cholesterol, serum DHEAS, glucose and HbA1c, other studies have not either shown any general differences
prolactin and waist/hip ratio. However, there were no signifi- in cortisol levels among men and women (Hansen et al.,
cant differences regarding individual biomarkers between 2003b). According to Lundberg (2005) previous studies have
recovered and unrecovered (von Thiele et al., 2006). shown that gender roles and psychological factors appear to
Insufficient sleep is a contributing factor to lack of recov- be more important than biological factors for the differing
ery, and may as such contribute to long-term negative health stress responses between men and women.
58 F. Eek et al.

Given the assumed complex role of fatigue, sleep and 2.2.2. Logbook
recovery in relation to stress, cortisol and negative health The participants also filled in a logbook during the day of
outcomes, and the so far inconclusive results the associations cortisol sampling. In the morning notes, they specified their
between these factors need further clarifications. The aim awakening time. The morning scoring further included the
with the present study was to examine possible associations Karolinska Sleep Diary (KSD), which assesses a number of
between cortisol measures and (I) self-rated recovery, (II) sleep parameters during the most recent night (Akerstedt
occupational fatigue and (III) subjective sleep quality the et al., 1994). In the present study, the two indices Disturbed
night preceding cortisol sampling. Further, possible gender sleep and Ease of awakening were examined. The disturbed
differences were tested. sleep score was calculated as the mean score of four items
asking whether the person during the most recent night had
2. Participants and methods (i) difficulties falling asleep; (ii) disturbed/restless sleep; (iii)
repeated awakenings; and (iv) premature awakenings.
Higher scores represent increasingly disturbed sleep (score
2.1. Participants
range 1—5). The awakening score was calculated as the mean
score for three items concerning (i) ease of awakening; (ii)
The study group consisted of 581 persons working day time;
whether the respondent felt well rested; and (iii) subjective
352 women and 229 men aged 19—65 years (mean 46.3 SD
sleep quality, where higher scores represented a more satis-
10.7 years), recruited from seven companies, all located in
factory awakening (score range 1—5). A correlation matrix of
the Southern part of Sweden: one high school (n = 172), one
the included KSD and SOFI scales is presented in Table 1.
telecommunication company (costumer service) (n = 101),
one regional social insurance office (n = 39), one pharmaceu-
2.2.3. Definition of dichotomized lack-of-recovery
tical company (n = 91), one wood industry (n = 34), one
variables
medical clinic (n = 53), one manufacturing plant (n = 42)
The six single-item questions concerning work related recov-
and one construction work place (n = 49).
ery were dichotomized, with the two most negative response
alternatives as outcome. ‘‘Often mentally fatigued after
2.2. Data sampling methods work’’ was defined as responding ‘‘Always’’ or ‘‘Mostly’’ on
the question ‘‘How often do you feel tired in your head
2.2.1. Questionnaire data (mental fatigue) after work?’’. ‘‘Long time to recover’’
The questionnaires were distributed and responded to at was defined as responding ‘‘more than 3 days’’ or ‘‘never
the workplace during a work day, and then collected by the get recovered’’ to the question ‘‘How long time, after each
researchers. The questionnaire included six single item work week, does it take until you feel recovered?’’. ‘‘Poor
questions about recovery rated on five-point scales with sleep quality’’ was defined as responding ‘‘quite poor’’ or
verbal labeling at each point but varying across items; ‘‘very poor’’ on the question ‘‘How would you, in general,
‘‘How often do you feel tired in your head (mental fatigue) rate your sleep quality?’’. ‘‘Not enough sleep’’ was defined as
after work’’ (response alternatives from ‘‘always’’ to responding ‘‘definitely not enough’’ or ‘‘far from enough’’ on
‘‘never’’), ‘‘How long time, after each work week, does the question ‘‘Do you get enough sleep?’’. ‘‘Need to sleep
it take until you feel recovered?’’ (response alternatives through’’ was defined as responding ‘‘mostly’’ or ‘‘always’’
‘‘less than one day’’, 1 day, 2—3 days, more (than 3 days), on the question ‘‘How often do you feel a need to sleep
and ‘‘never get recovered’’), ‘‘How would you, in general, through?’’. ‘‘No possibility to sleep through’’ was defined as
rate your sleep quality’’ (response alternatives ‘‘very good’’ responding ‘‘never’’ or rarely’’ to the question ‘‘How often do
to ‘‘very poor’’), ‘‘Do you get enough sleep’’ (response you have the possibility to sleep through when you feel in
alternatives from ‘‘definitely enough’’ to ‘‘far from need for it?’’
enough‘‘), How often do you feel a need to sleep through
(response alternatives ‘‘never’’ to ‘‘always (most days)’’) 2.2.4. Cortisol sampling
and ‘‘How often do you have the possibility to sleep through The participants collected saliva at three pre-specified
when you feel in need for it (response alternatives ‘‘never’’ time points during one work day; at awakening, 30 min
to ‘‘always (almost every time)’’. The internal non- after awakening, and at 2100 h. In order to standardize
responses to these questions were between 0.9% (n = 5) the sampling procedure as much as possible, participants
and 2.8% (n = 16). The recovery items are not previously were instructed to collect saliva during an ordinary work
validated, but are similar to questions used in previous day, excluding the first and last day of the work week (hence
studies that has found significant associations with cortisol most samples are collected during Tuesday—Thursday). A
(Gustafsson et al., 2008). previous study showed that there were no differences in
Work related fatigue was measured by Swedish Occupa- cortisol values these between week days (Garde et al.,
tional Fatigue Inventory (SOFI-20), an instrument that 2003b). Verbal instructions were given to each participant
assesses five dimensions of fatigue after a typical workday: how to place the swab from the saliva sampling tube in the
lack of energy, physical exertion, physical discomfort, lack of mouth until hydrated, but no longer than 5 min. Each
motivation, and sleepiness, together with a global scale person also received written information together with
computed as the subject’s mean score across all items (Åhs- the set of sampling tubes (Salivette1, Sarstedt Ltd., Lei-
berg et al., 1997). SOFI-20 contains 20 items, four items for cester, UK). Restrictions were issued concerning teeth-
each of the five subscales, and all items are rated on a seven- brushing, smoking, and heavy meals 1 h prior to saliva
point scale with two verbal anchors: (i) ‘‘not at all’’ and (vii) sampling. After completed sampling, the samples were
‘‘to a very high degree’’. returned in the pre-stamped envelope together with the
Cortisol and recovery 59

completed questionnaire. On arrival to the clinic, the

8. SOFI global
samples were stored at 20 8C until analyzed.

2.3. Analysis of cortisol in saliva


index

2.42
1.09
1
The assay used for determination of cortisol in saliva was a
competitive radio immuno-assay (RIA) (Spectria Cortisol
sleepiness

Coated Tube RIA) purchased from Orion Diagnostica, Espoo,


7. SOFI

Finland. The assay was designed for quantitative in vitro

0.83
2.84
1.53
measurement of cortisol in serum, plasma, urine and saliva.

1
A method evaluation of certified reference material in water
showed no bias of the method, with a 97% recovery (CI 94%;
6. SOFI lack of

100.9%). Limit of detection (LOD) was 1.59 nmol/l. Between-


motivation

run CVs were 19% at 11.5 nmol/l and 16% at 49.2 nmol/l. To
show equivalence between different runs, natural saliva
0.59
0.77
2.06
1.30
samples at two concentrations (11.5 nmol/l and
1

49.2 nmol/l) were used as control materials and analyzed


together with the samples. Westgard control charts were
5. SOFI physical

used to monitor and control variation, and ensure that the


analytical methods remained stable. The performance of the
discomfort

methods was further evaluated by participation in inter-


laboratory comparison schemes (Westgard et al., 1981;
0.49
0.54
0.81
2.33
1.33

Garde et al., 2003a; Hansen et al., 2003a).


1

The cortisol variables analyzed in the present study were:


(i) the mean cortisol concentration over the day, (ii) the
Correlation matrix and descriptives (mean, SD) of included sleep, fatigue and recovery scales.

4. SOFI physical

cortisol concentration (nmol/l) at the three time points, (iii)


the relative awakening response (defined as % increase
between the first and the second sample (CAR)), (iv) the
exertion

cortisol morning peak (CMP) (nmol/l), (v) the mean morning


0.59
0.36
0.40
0.65
1.48
0.87

concentration (that is, the mean of the two morning samples)


1

and (vi) the cortisol decline over the day (CDD) (nmol/l
difference between the CMP and the 2100 h sample).
3. SOFI lack of

2.4. Statistical analyses


energy

0.47
0.64
0.60
0.67
0.88
3.41
1.72

For the dichotomized variables, the linear MIXED models


1

module in SPSS 14 (SPSS Inc., 2001) was used to specify a


repeated measures model for the analysis of cortisol con-
2. KSD disturbed

centration over the day. The models were solved using the
restricted maximum likelihood (REML) method. The catego-
rical predictors were group (‘‘non-recovered’’ or ‘‘recov-
ered’’ category for each of the single-item questions), and
sleep

time point of sampling. The statistical modeling also


0.28
0.15
0.31
0.15
0.20
0.28
2.07
0.82

included the two-way interaction group  time point of


1

sampling, to find possible differential patterns of reactions


between recovery profiles. For the analyses, a series of first-
of awakening
1. KSD ease

order autoregressive covariance structures was tested, as


well as a compound symmetry covariance structure (CS). The
0.54
0.41
0.22
0.35
0.26
0.31
0.40
3.20
0.77

Schwarz’ Bayesian Information Criterion was used to guide


1

the final selection of covariance structure. The non-repeated


cortisol variables (CAR, CMP and mean morning concentra-
tion, and CDD) were analyzed by General Linear Modeling
5. SOFI physical discomfort
6. SOFI lack of motivation
1. KSD ease of awakening

4. SOFI physical exertion

(GLM) Univariate ANOVA. Awakening time (i.e. time at the


2. KSD disturbed sleep
3. SOFI lack of energy

first sample), age, and gender were introduced as covariates


8. SOFI global index

in the model. The modeling also included the interaction


7. SOFI sleepiness

group by gender, to examine possible differences in associa-


tions between recovery and cortisol for men and women. The
limit of significance of covariates to be included was set to
Table 1

0.20 in all models, that is, variables or interactions not


Mean

reaching a p-value below 0.20 were in the next step elimi-


SD

nated from the model (Greenland, 1989; Maldonado and


60 F. Eek et al.

Greenland, 1993). For the study variables, p-values 0.05 on the KSD ease of awakening index, indicating a less satis-
were considered statistically significant. Due to skewed data factory awakening, compared to men while the disturbed
distribution, the cortisol concentrations were logarithmically sleep index score did not differ much between men and
transformed before carrying out the statistical analyses. women (Table 2).
Regarding the variables CAR and CDD, which also were skewly
distributed but did include negative values, the data was 3.1. Dichotomized lack-of-recovery variables
ranked before statistical analysis. and cortisol
For the continuous measures of sleep quality and fatigue
(SOFI subscales and KSD subscales), bivariate correlations There were no significant differences between any of the
with cortisol measures were analyzed with Pearson correla- dichotomized groups of lack-of-recovery variables, regarding
tion coefficients (r). Since poor sleep and recovery is sup- mean cortisol concentration during the day, and no signifi-
posed to be associated with mainly morning cortisol cant interactions between group and time point, indicating
concentrations, analyses were performed for the two morn- similar cortisol pattern over the day for recovered- and non-
ing samples separately, as well as for the mean morning recovered profiles. For the non-repeated cortisol measures
concentration. Also correlations with CMP, CAR and CDD were CMP, CAR, and CDD there were only differences between a
tested. In a second step, gender-stratified correlations were few of the lack-of-recovery dichotomies. One of these was a
analyzed. significant interaction effect between gender and Long time
to recover for the variable CAR ( p < 0.001). Women who
2.5. Ethics needed more than three days to recover after a work week
had significantly higher CAR compared to women who felt
The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration recovered in less than three days ( p < 0.001), while men who
of Helsinki. All participation in the study was completely needed long time to recover had a significantly flattened CAR
voluntary and with the adequate understanding of the sub- compared to men who recovered in less than three days
jects. ( p < 0.001) (Fig. 1a and b). Descriptive cortisol values for
men and women are presented in Table 2.
3. Results
3.2. Correlations between cortisol and
Of the six lack-of-recovery variables Need to sleep through continuous variables of fatigue and sleep
was the most frequently reported (Table 2). However, the disturbances
gender difference was evident; almost half of the women
reported an almost constant need to sleep through, while There were no significant correlations between the KSD
only 22% of the men did. Among men, Lack of possibility to subscales disturbed sleep and Ease of awakening, and any
sleep through was the most frequent type of lack of recovery of the examined cortisol variables. However, a few significant
reported, followed by Mental fatigue after work. correlations were found between SOFI-subscales and CAR,
Few persons, only 5%, experienced five or six of the lack- which was significantly positively correlated with Lack of
of-recovery conditions measured, while 38.6% experienced energy (r = 0.11, p = 0.024), Lack of motivation (r = 0.10,
none of them, indicating a fulfilled recovery. 42.1% experi- p = 0.036) and Physical exertion (r = 0.11, p = 0.023). Physi-
enced one or two of the conditions, indicating a somewhat cal exertion was also significantly negatively correlated with
unmet need for recovery. Women also had lower mean scores cortisol concentration at awakening (r = 0.10, p = 0.032).

Table 2 Proportion of persons experiencing various aspects of lack of recovery and mean Karolinska Sleep Diary scores for ‘‘Ease of
awakening’’ and ‘‘Disturbed sleep’’ and descriptive cortisol values.

Men Women Total


Often mentally fatigued after work (%) 21.9 31 27.4
Need >3 days to recover after work week (%) 11.2 13.7 12.7
Poor sleep quality (%) 12.1 20.5 17.2
Not enough sleep (%) 9.8 16.5 13.9
Need to sleep through (%) 21.6 41.8 33.9
No possibility to sleep through (%) 25.5 26.4 26.0
KSD ease of awakening (mean; SD) 3.4; 0.7 3.1; 0.8 3.2; 0.8
KSD Disturbed sleep score (mean; SD) 2.0; 0.8 2.1; 0.8 2.1; 0.8
Cortisol awakening response CAR (MD (10;90th 31.5 ( 40.5; 219) 51.6 ( 38.7; 261) 45.7 ( 38.8; 251.0)
percentile)) %
Cortisol morning peak CMP (MD (10; 90th 22.5(12.5; 37.7) 23.1 (13.4; 41.2) 22.7 (13.3; 39.9)
percentile)) nmol/l
Cortisol decline over day CDD (MD (10; 90th 18.2 ( 35.1; 6.3) 18.6 ( 36.9; 8.2) 18.4 ( 36.0; 7.6)
percentile)) nmol/l
Mean morning concentration (MD (10; 90th 18.0 (10.6; 28.0) 18.6 (10.8; 31.1) 18.1 (10.6; 29.9)
percentile)) nmol/l
Cortisol and recovery 61

a 30,0 women than among men. Among women, the correlation


between CAR and lack of energy (r = 0.17, p = 0.007),
between CAR and physical exertion (r = 0.16, p = 0.010) as
well as between cortisol concentration at awakening and
25,0 Physical exertion (r = 0.17, p = 0.005) was stronger than in
short
the total group. None of the correlations turned out signifi-
long (>3 days)
cant among men, when analyzed separately (r = 0.023,
r = 0.02, r = 0.006, respectively, all p > 0.75). No other
20,0 correlations were significant among either men or women.
Cortisol (nmol/l)

4. Discussion
15,0
Several previous studies have found associations between
sleep and cortisol, when sleep is manipulated and registered
in laboratory settings. However, subjective ratings of sleep
10,0 and recovery in every day life may be quite different from
variables and conditions measured in laboratory settings.
One previous small group study reported rather strong asso-
ciations also between self rated recovery, and cortisol (Gus-
5,0 tafsson et al., 2008). Another study, including 24 women,
found rather strong long term associations between need for
recovery and evening cortisol, where high need for recovery
was associated with lower evening cortisol concentrations
0,0 (Rydstedt et al., 2009). The present study intended to cor-
05:00 11:00 17:00 23:00 roborate these findings and further explore possible relation-
Time ships between cortisol and various measures of subjective
sleep and recovery by including a larger group of participants
b 30,0 and additional measures of both cortisol and sleep/recovery.
Women did more frequently than men report all aspects of
lack of recovery. The gender difference was most evident
regarding constant need to sleep through, which was
25,0
short reported by almost half of the women, but only 22% of the
men. Lack of possibility to sleep through when needing it was
long (>3 days)
instead the factor most often reported among the men,
20,0 although it was even more frequently experienced by the
women. However, if the men more rarely feel a need to sleep
Cortisol (nmol/l)

through, the lack of possibility may be of limited impact. The


constant need to sleep through among many women may
15,0 reflect their ‘‘double burden’’ by still being the ones that
most often take the largest responsibility of child care and
house hold work. Women also showed a general tendency
towards more frequent reports of various physical symptoms,
10,0 which may be considered as an increased symptom percep-
tion that may also be reflected in the higher report of lack of
recovery (Gijsbers Van Wijk and Kolk, 1997).
Regarding the association between on the one hand sleep
5,0 and recovery, and on the other cortisol, we failed to confirm
previous findings of rather strong associations. The few sig-
nificant findings in the present study should be interpreted in
the light of the mass significance issue rising from the vast
0,0
amount of analyses performed. However, some of the gender
05:00 11:00 17:00 23:00 interaction effects do, for women, point in the same direc-
Time tion as in the previous study. Gustafsson et al. (2008) found
high morning cortisol being associated with self-reported
Figure 1 Cortisol profile (median nmol/l) over the day for men insufficient recovery from work in 25 female and male white
(a) and women (b) who experience a need for long (more than 3 collar workers. Recovery was in that study measured by
days) and short time to recover after a work week. questions like ‘‘Do you feel thoroughly rested when you start
working in the morning’’ and ‘‘Do you feel rested and recov-
The correlations remained significant after controlling for
ered when you return to work after a weekend’’. Thus the
awakening time.
questions were quite similar to the ones used in the present
Gender stratified analyses showed that the association
study, where only few significant correlations were found
between cortisol and SOFI-subscales were stronger among
62 F. Eek et al.

between cortisol and any sleep- or recovery parameters, Taken together, these interpretations seem quite spec-
despite the much larger study group. ulative and even dubious, considering both the mass signifi-
No significant correlations were found among cortisol and cance issue and the lack of consistency in the results. Still,
‘‘acute’’ sleep parameters, as measured with the Karolinska regarding gender interaction effects, they resemble the
Sleep Diary (KSD). This instrument intends to measure the results from the mentioned study by Gustafsson et al.
sleep quality during the night/morning preceding the cortisol (2008), reporting higher morning cortisol values among 12
sampling in terms of disturbed sleep and ease of awakening women who rarely felt rested after a weekend, but no such
after the night’s sleep. However, some significant associations positive association among 13 men, who instead showed a
were found between cortisol and a few other measures of more weak tendency towards the opposite pattern. In that study
chronic aspects of sleep, fatigue, and recovery. A higher Lack CAR was not analyzed, since the first sample was collected
of energy, Lack of motivation, and Physical exertion were 15—30 min after awakening. The most comparable measure
correlated with a higher CAR; Physical exertion also with a in our study would be the second morning sample or the
lower cortisol concentration at awakening. This finding was maximum morning value not being significantly associated
supported in a recent study among 389 white collar-workers with any of the different recovery variables.
which found high concentration of salivary cortisol was asso- Even though a few significant associations between sleep
ciated with high perceived energy and that vigorous physical or recovery and cortisol emerged in the present analyses, the
activity reduced cortisol concentration. Vigorous physical picture was far from clear. In total, we were not able to
activity was associated with high perceived energy (Hansen confirm previous findings of clear associations between
et al., 2009). Gender stratified analyses showed that these sleep- or recovery parameters and cortisol, even though
correlations were found only among women. This was not the present study group is relatively large, and the methods
simply a question of power, as may be expected with the used for analyses were comparable to previous studies (i.e.
smaller group of men participating in the study. The correla- Gustafsson et al., 2008) both regarding measures of cortisol
tion coefficients were not only insignificant but also very close and self-reported recovery. Nor did we find any association
to 0 for the male group, while it was higher, although not between the additional measures of sleep and cortisol,
impressively so, in the female group. This indicates that included in the present study. With the starting point in
possible associations and possibly pathways between lack of the hypothesis that the development of exhaustion may go
recovery/fatigue and cortisol, and in the long term, unhealth, from initially increased cortisol release towards a gradually
may not be similar for men and women. flattening day profile, there is an obvious problem with cross
Also analyses of the different lack-of-recovery variables in sectional studies. Persons may have come variously far in this
relation to cortisol showed differences among men and development. Some persons in the initial phase may have
women. Regarding association between Long time to recover increased cortisol response while others who have experi-
and CAR, the men showed the expected pattern according to enced lack of recovery and exhaustion during a longer period
our theoretical assumptions. Among males, those needing a may instead have lower cortisol concentrations and flatter
long time to recover after a work week tended to show a day profiles. However, on group level, such differential
flatter awakening response compared to males needing responses may level each other out, resulting in support of
shorter time to recover. This result corresponds to the the null hypothesis.
hypothesis that a prolonged state of lack of recovery may This illustrates the precarious point in the measure and
result in flattened cortisol day profiles. A previous study in interpretation of salivary cortisol, and its relation to sub-
line with this hypothesis found associations between lack of jective experiences of stress or strain. In order to elucidate
vitality and a flatter cortisol profile over the day (i.e. CDD). the complexity of the interplay between cortisol and self
However, in the present study, no significant associations reported measures of strain and recovery, it is important to
between the CDD parameter and any of the examined publish also inconsistent findings, or lack of significant
lack-of-recovery variables were found, which, although lack results. However, the previous study by Gustavsson et al.
of recovery and lack of vitality is not exactly synonymous, was based on a very homogenous sample of white collar
also points towards the disparity in results within this topic. workers with similar occupations, whereas the present study
This disparity is also illustrated by an opposite pattern of included workers from several different companies with
associations in women: those needing a longer time to varying occupational categories, forming a more heteroge-
recover after a work week showed a significantly higher nous group of workers with a wide variety of work tasks.
CAR compared to women needing shorter time to recover, Possible differences in salivary cortisol related to differences
which is in contrast to our expectations. Striving for an in recovery among these workers could have been masked by
interpretation, this result could possibly be discussed in the influence from other work related factors and individual
terms of a high taxing of available resources in women. These differences. Perhaps it could be possible to demonstrate a
may be reflected in fatigue after work, a frequent inability to stronger association between recovery and cortisol with a
sleep through, a high need for energy mobilization in the more homogenous group of workers.
morning, and a need for longer time to recover after the work
week. It has previously been suggested that women may be 4.1. Study limitations
more sensitive than men to work stress with respect to
cortisol excretion (Alderling et al., 2006). However, due to Given the large number of statistical analyses performed in
the relative nature of the CAR measure, lower awakening the present study, it is inevitable to reflect on the possibility
concentrations may lead to relatively higher CAR. Hence, the of mass significance findings. Still, the significant results may
correlations are not in themselves contradictory but still not nevertheless be reflected on and discussed. The present
self-evident how to interpret. study examines variations of normal self-reported sleep
Cortisol and recovery 63

patterns and states of recovery during everyday circum- cortisol and acute measures of sleep quality preceding night.
stances, contrary to laboratory studies recording more exact Few associations were found, among women only, between
measures of sleep quality, often during quite extremely cortisol and some measures of subscales of the occupational
manipulated sleep. The aim of the study was, accordingly, fatigue index. However, these findings should be interpreted
to study associations between cortisol and various self in light of the potential mass significance effect. The present
reported measures of everyday sleep, fatigue and recovery, results points out the importance of replication of especially
and our results should not be extrapolated to sleep depriva- small sample studies, in order to further confirm possible
tion situations. Self reports of sleep may not be as exact as associations. More research is needed before the final con-
e.g. polysomnography recordings, but since there are other clusion about possible associations between cortisol and self-
limitations with laboratory studies such as limited general- rated sleep and recovery can be drawn, by examining large,
izability to every day life, and restriction in possible (realis- and perhaps more homogenous, study samples.
tic) study groups, the use of self reports of sleep and recovery
are still highly relevant. Some of the measures used in the Role of funding sources
present study, such as the SOFI and KSD-scales, are well
established and validated measures (Akerstedt et al.,
There were no impact from funding source(s) on any aspect of
1994; Åhsberg et al., 1997), while the recovery items are
the work with the current manuscript (design, data collec-
not previously validated. The reason for analysing individual
tion, analyse, interpretation, writing, or submission).
(and dicothomised) items was that the initial and primary aim
with the present study was an attempt to reproduce the
results from Gustafsson et al. (2008), who found strong Conflict of interest
associations between cortisol and individual recovery items,
similar to those used in our study, despite including only a There are no conflicts of interest for any of the authors.
small study sample. In that study, the most consistent results
emerged for dicothomised items. Analyzing individual items References
also allows for an exploration of whether different aspects of
(lacking) recovery may show differing associations with cor-
Åhsberg, E., Gamberale, F., Kjellberg, A., 1997. Perceived quality of
tisol. Including both well established scale measures of fatigue during different occupational tasks. Development of a
fatigue and sleep, and these individual recovery items, questionnaire. Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 20, 121—135.
allows for analyses of a wide range of different aspects of Akerstedt, T., Hume, K., Minors, D., Waterhouse, J., 1994. The
sleep, fatigue and recovery. meaning of good sleep: a longitudinal study of polysomnography
Previous studies have shown that there is a high intrain- and subjective sleep quality. J. Sleep Res. 3 (3), 152—158.
dividual variation in cortisol, especially during the morning Alderling, M., Theorell, T., de la Torre, B., Lundberg, I., 2006. The
(Carlsson Eek et al., 2006). Ideally, cortisol samples should be demand control model and circadian saliva cortisol variations in a
collected during several days for each participant. In the Swedish population based sample (the PART study). BMC Public
present study, saliva was collected during one day only, which Health 6, 288.
Almeida, D.M., Piazza, J.R., Stawski, R.S., 2009. Interindividual
makes it more vulnerable to the high intraindividual varia-
differences and intraindividual variability in the cortisol awaken-
tion. Compliance with cortisol sampling instructions is ing response: an examination of age and gender. Psychol. Aging 24
another important aspect in field studies. Lacking compli- (4), 819—827.
ance, for example a delay between awakening and first Antonijevic, I.A., Murck, H., Frieboes, R., Holsboer, F., Steiger, A.,
morning sample may, due to the relatively rapid changes 1999. On the gender differences in sleep-endocrine regulation in
in cortisol concentration during early morning hours, affect young normal humans. Neuroendocrinology 70 (4), 280—287.
especially the CAR-measure, but also other cortisol mea- Axelsson, J., Akerstedt, T., Kecklund, G., Lindqvist, A., Attefors, R.,
sures. All participants were carefully instructed about the 2003. Hormonal changes in satisfied and dissatisfied shift workers
importance of taking the first sample immediately at awa- across a shift cycle. J. Appl. Physiol. 95 (5), 2099—2105.
kening, however, we had no possibility to control whether Backhaus, J., Junghanns, K., Hohagen, F., 2004. Sleep disturbances
are correlated with decreased morning awakening salivary corti-
instructions were followed or not. Poor compliance may
sol. Psychoneuroendocrinology 29 (9), 1184—1191.
hence possibly have contributed to the lack of significant Browne, J.S., Karady, S., Selye, H., 1939. The effect of noxious
associations. There could also be gender differences in com- agents on creatine, creatinine, chloride and water excretion. J.
pliance that may explain why some significant associations Physiol. 97 (1), 1—7.
were found among women only. The study sample consists of Cannon, W.B., 1929. Bodily changes in pain, hunger, fear and rage; an
persons with a wide spread in age. Age was included as account of recent researches into the function of emotional
covariate in the analyses of individual recovery items and excitement. D. Appleton and company, New York and London,
cortisol, while scale measures was analyzed by zero-order (or xvi, p. 404.
gender stratified) correlations. Hence, age differences could Carlsson Eek, F., Garde, A.H., Hansen, A.M., Persson, R., Ørbæk, P.,
possibly have affected some of the results. Karlson, B., 2006. The cortisol awakening response — an explora-
tion of intra- and interindividual variation. Scand. J. Work Envi-
Even though some methodological issues may have
ron. Health 32 (Suppl. 2), 15—21.
affected the result, there is no reason to believe that meth- Ekstedt, M., Akerstedt, T., Soderstrom, M., 2004. Microarousals
odological limitations were the full explanation for the lack during sleep are associated with increased levels of lipids, corti-
of significant findings. In the present study, we could not sol, and blood pressure. Psychosom. Med. 66 (6), 925—931.
confirm previous findings of associations between cortisol Garde, A.H., Hansen ÅM, Nikolajsen, T.B., 2003a. An inter-laboratory
and lack of recovery, although we found some gender inter- comparison for determination of cortisol in saliva. Accredit. Qual.
actions. Not either could we find any associations between Assur. 8 (16), 20.
64 F. Eek et al.

Garde, A.H., Hansen, A.M., Persson, R., Ohlsson, K., Ørbæk, P., Selye, H., 1938. Blood sugar and blood chloride changes in thyroid-
2003b. The influence of production systems on physiological ectomized rats following exposure to various damaging agents.
responses measured in urine and saliva. Stress Health 19, Biochem. J. 32 (12), 2071—2074.
297—306. Sluiter, J.K., de Croon, E.M., Meijman, T.F., Frings-Dresen, M.H.,
Gijsbers Van Wijk, C.M.T., Kolk, A.M., 1997. Sex differences in 2003. Need for recovery from work related fatigue and its role in
physical symptoms: the contribution of symptom perception the development and prediction of subjective health complaints.
theory. Soc. Sci. Med. 45 (2), 231—246. Occup. Environ. Med. 60 (Suppl. 1), i62—70.
Greenland, S., 1989. Modeling and variable selection in epidemio- Sluiter, J.K., Frings-Dresen, M.H., van der Beek, A.J., Meijman, T.F.,
logic analysis. Am. J. Public Health 79 (3), 340—349. 2001. The relation between work-induced neuroendocrine reac-
Gustafsson, K., Lindfors, P., Aronsson, G., Lundberg, U., 2008. tivity and recovery, subjective need for recovery, and health
Relationships between self-rating of recovery from work and status. J. Psychosom. Res. 50 (1), 29—37.
morning salivary cortisol. J. Occup. Health 50 (1), 24—30. Spiegel, K., Leproult, R., Van Cauter, E., 1999. Impact of sleep debt
Hansen, A.M., Blangsted, A.K., Hansen, E.A., Sogaard, K., Sjogaard, on metabolic and endocrine function. Lancet 354 (9188),
G., 2009. Physical activity, job demand-control, perceived stress- 1435—1439.
energy, and salivary cortisol in white-collar workers. Int. Arch. Sterling, P., Eyer, J., 1988. Allostasis: a new paradigm to explain
Occup. Environ. Health 83 (2), 143—153. arousal pathology. In: Fisher, S., Reason, J. (Eds.), Handbook of
Hansen, ÅM, Garde, A.H., Christensen, J.M., Eller, N., Netterstrøm, Life Stress, Cognition and Health. John Wiley & Sons Ltd., New
B., 2003a. Validation of a radio-immunoassay and establishment York, pp. 629—649.
of a reference interval for salivary cortisol in healthy subjects in Ursin, H., Eriksen, H.R., 2001. Sensitization, subjective health com-
Denmark. Scand. J. Clin. Lab. Invest. 63, 1—9. plaints, and sustained arousal. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 933,
Hansen, A.M., Garde, A.H., Christensen, J.M., Eller, N.H., Netter- 119—129.
strøm, B., 2003b. Evaluation of a radioimmunoassay and estab- Van Cauter, E., Leproult, R., Kupfer, D.J., 1996. Effects of gender and
lishment of a reference interval for salivary cortisol in healthy age on the levels and circadian rhythmicity of plasma cortisol. J.
subjects in Denmark. Scand. J. Clin. Lab. Invest. 63 (4), 303—310. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 81 (7), 2468—2473.
Leproult, R., Copinschi, G., Buxton, O., Van Cauter, E., 1997. Sleep Vgontzas, A.N., Mastorakos, G., Bixler, E.O., Kales, A., Gold, P.W.,
loss results in an elevation of cortisol levels the next evening. Chrousos, G.P., 1999. Sleep deprivation effects on the activity of
Sleep 20 (10), 865—870. the hypothalamic—pituitary—adrenal and growth axes: potential
Lundberg, U., 2005. Stress hormones in health and illness: the roles clinical implications. Clin. Endocrinol. (Oxf.) 51 (2), 205—215.
of work and gender. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30 (10), von Thiele, U., Lindfors, P., Lundberg, U., 2006. Self-rated recovery
1017—1021. from work stress and allostatic load in women. J. Psychosom. Res.
Maldonado, G., Greenland, S., 1993. Simulation study of confounder- 61 (2), 237—242.
selection strategies. Am. J. Epidemiol. 138 (11), 923—936. Weekes, N.Y., Lewis, R.S., Goto, S.G., Garrison-Jakel, J., Patel, F.,
McEwen, B.S., 1998. Protective and damaging effects of stress Lupien, S., 2008. The effect of an environmental stressor on
mediators. N. Engl. J. Med. 338 (3), 171—179. gender differences on the awakening cortisol response. Psycho-
Rydstedt, L.W., Cropley, M., Devereux, J.J., Michalianou, G., 2009. neuroendocrinology 33 (6), 766—772.
The effects of gender, long-term need for recovery and trait Westgard, J.O., Barry, P.L., Hunt, M.R., Groth, T., 1981. A multi-rule
inhibition—rumination on morning and evening saliva cortisol Shewhart chart for quality control in clinical chemistry. Clin.
secretion. Anxiety Stress Coping 22 (4), 465—474. Chem. 27 (3), 493—501.

View publication stats

You might also like