You are on page 1of 4

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/224587353

Cognitive Robotics: New Insights into Robot and Human Intelligence by


Reverse Engineering Brain Functions

Article  in  IEEE Robotics & Automation Magazine · October 2009


DOI: 10.1109/MRA.2009.933617 · Source: IEEE Xplore

CITATIONS READS

6 265

5 authors, including:

Will Neil Browne Kazuhiko Kawamura


Victoria University of Wellington Vanderbilt University
154 PUBLICATIONS   2,023 CITATIONS    202 PUBLICATIONS   2,873 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Jeff Krichmar William Harwin


University of California, Irvine University of Reading
142 PUBLICATIONS   2,430 CITATIONS    171 PUBLICATIONS   3,140 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

mobile robotics education, human-robot interfaces View project

CARLsim: A GPU-accelerated spiking neural network simulator View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Hiroaki Wagatsuma on 19 March 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Authorized licensed use limited to: IEEE Xplore. Downloaded on September 24, 2009 at 01:32 from IEEE Xplore. Restrictions apply.
F R O M T H E G U E S T E D I T O R S

Cognitive Robotics: New Insights into Robot and Human Intelligence


by Reverse Engineering Brain Functions
William Browne, Kazuhiko Kawamura, Jeffrey Krichmar, William Harwin, and Hiroaki Wagatsuma

C
ognitive robotics is a fascinating field as it seeks to cap- behavior. Cognitive robotics reinforces the concept that the
ture humanlike behaviors in artificial creations for the best way to understand something is by building it.
benefit of society. The advent of advances in mecha- The brain is a very capable parallel-processing machine. In
tronic systems, low-cost computational power, and understand- an approximately 1,300 g of neurons and glial cells of the brain,
ing of natural processes has coincided with the need for there are about 1011 neurons and 1015 synapses. Although imag-
autonomous systems to undertake complex, nuanced, and semi- ing techniques are improving, it is impossible to measure how
structured tasks. Using cognitive inspiration facilitates robust- spike trains, the fundamental units of neuron communication,
ness and flexibility for real-world applications of robotics. and flow in this network in response to stimuli. Cognitive
The term cognition can be applied to a collection of proc- robotics offers a two-fold benefit: first, by being embedded in
esses that are involved in perception and thinking. Not only do the physical world, the sensory stimuli are guaranteed to be
cognitive robots attempt to replicate useful behaviors, but they representative of the stimuli received by animals, and second,
are inspired by the underlying processes in natural cognition to track the flow of information is simply a matter of providing
achieve efficiency and effectiveness benefits such as robustness storage for the computational representation of the brain. Thus,
to subtly changing problem conditions. it becomes possible to infer what is happening at the individual
There is a long history in robotics of trying to emulate the cell or synapse of a particular cell.
performance of humans. This may be at a macroscale, such as At the moment, the benefits of cognitive robotic techni-
robots copy waling or arm functions, or at subsystem level, such ques, although important, are only a small component of neu-
as using compliance, to design robotic arms that attempt to roscience. On the other hand, the benefit of neuroscience to
mimic the way the muscles, tendons, and neurons control the the design of cognitive robotics is in providing a rich source of
movement of an individual joint. Similarly, the biological inspi- inspiration. This enables cognitive robots to integrate with
ration may be derived from different levels of abstraction, e.g., humans in nuanced, changeable, and complex environments,
from psychological behavior analysis to neuronal functionality. leading them to work more independently.
There has also been a long history of trying to mimic the In this special issue on cognitive robotics, we begin with an
processes within the brain. Early work in cognitive robotics was article that uses neuroscience to inspire new methods for robot
done by the American neuroscientist Grey Walter, who, in mapping and navigation (Wyeth and Milford) and end with the
1948, produced a number of very simple light-reactive mobile article where the raison d’^etre for the robot is an instrument to
robots. The brains of the robots, which Grey Walter labeled study the processes in the brain (Cox and Krichmar). The issue
Machina spectulatrix (M. spectulatrix), had essentially two also has a representation of processing in animals (Prescott et al.
neurons, each built around a vacuum tube (thermionic valve). and Srinivasan et al.) and observations on the processes in humans
One excitatory neuron caused the robot to move forward in (Fleischer and Edelman, and Barakova and Chonnaparamut).
response to light. A second inhibitory neuron suppressed the One key area of the brain is the hippocampus, and a seminal
behavior that M. spectulatrix used to scan for a light source, work by Maguire [2] showed its strong relationship to naviga-
and thus in combination the Grey Walter’s robots displayed a tion in humans by showing that the size of the region is
general affinity for light [1]. enlarged in London taxi drivers who must have the knowl-
Cognitive robotics has now emerged as a strong interdisci- edge, the ability to plan any route in London, before they are
plinary research field that seeks to exploit and expand knowl- licensed. Wyeth and Milford have based their work on specific
edge of both neuroscience and robotics. The majority of cells in the hippocampus, place cells, that fire in response to a
cognitive robotic work considers replicating human and ani- specific location. By observing the response of the place cells
mal behaviors in robots, but there is a growing interest in using in rats and linking these to similar grid cells and head-direction
robotic analogs of the brain to provide insight into the neuro- cells, the authors have formulated a flexible program for simul-
science. This special issue has articles that represent both the taneous location and mapping that they have called RatSLAM.
approaches. A unique advantage of cognitive models is the It would be impossible to do a faithful reproduction of the
ability to track information flow through the brain in ways that processes associated with rat navigation, because it is impossi-
cannot be done with real brains. This makes cognitive robotics ble to know how this information arrives at the cells in the hip-
a powerful tool for understanding brain function and human pocampus, nor how the cells create viable solutions that enable
rats to map and navigate. However, by reproducing the general
Digital Object Identifier 10.1109/MRA.2009.933617 concepts and by making no distinction between learning and

SEPTEMBER 2009 IEEE Robotics & Automation Magazine 17

Authorized licensed use limited to: Hiroaki Wagatsuma. Downloaded on September 22, 2009 at 07:22 from IEEE Xplore. Restrictions apply.
recall, the authors have created a mapping and navigation system The bee forms the inspiration for the article by Srinivasan
that is adaptable to both short- and long-term changes in the et al. Bees have fixed eyes, with fixed focus, and because of
environment. They demonstrated their work on a delivery their small size, minimal binocular stereo, yet, are able to
robot in the laboratory environment. To evaluate the technique return to the hive from several miles away and land safely. Bees
for a more complex problem, they then demonstrate RatSLAM appear to use a combination of two simple rules to achieve suc-
by creating a map of a Brisbane suburb, using a camera in a note- cessful landing. The first is that the visual flow velocity is kept
book computer mounted on the roof of the car. constant when landing and the second is that the forward and
The second article is by Fleischer and Edelman. Edelman descent velocity are kept in proportion. Having provided evi-
won the 1972 Nobel prize in medicine and established a repu- dence of these two rules, Srinivasan et al. test the rules on a
tation for excellence in neuroscience research that led to a number of R/C models as well as a laboratory test bed. The
series of test bed cognitive robots (the Darwin series) to test article then continues with details of an optical design for a
hypotheses on specific processes in the brain. The article reit- sensor for visual flow-based landings that can accommodate
erates the fact that neurophysiologists have great difficulty data contamination from yaw and roll and supports the idea
looking at the topology and timings of an animal brain, that, by understanding the processes involved in human and
whereas a brain-based device controlling a robot platform can animal cognition, we may be able to abstract the key aspects
be analyzed in great detail. The authors note that the brain is a and use these in furthering the design of competent robots.
good model for intelligence and discuss the progress of two The final article by Cox and Krichmar provides a strong
robots in the Darwin series (X and XI) designed to test a reminder that advances in neuroscience are possible using
hypothesis of how brain structures relate to the formation of robot-based models. The theory underpinning this study is on
spatial and episodic memory. The authors also provide an the role of neuromodulators in learning in the brain. Neuromo-
excellent demonstration of the benefits of using the brain as a dulation refers to the role of a set of neurotransmitters that have
model for intelligence by reporting on the control structures a strong and lasting effect on behaviors. This contrasts with most
of a soccer-playing robot based on a Segway base. This robot neurotransmitters that have a local effect across a single synapse
proved the prowess of a cognitive approach by winning all five between the axon of a transmitting neuron and dendrites on the
of its demonstration games in the 2005 RoboCup. receiving neuron. The role of the neuromodulating transmitters
Touch is an ignored sense in most robots. If a bump sensor appears to be to influence aspects such as reward, response to
on a mobile robot is activated, it is usually an indication of an risks, levels of attention, novelty, and the anticipation of reward.
unexpected event and closely tied to the safety. Although CARL-1 is a test-bed robot that sets out to explore the role of
many animals use whiskers, feelers, antennae, etc., the sophisti- these neuromodulators on behavior. CARL-1 has neural areas
cation of a rat’s whisking system makes it particularly interest- to control Flee and Find behaviors and then explores the proc-
ing for a neurological study and as a model for active touch in esses modulating these behaviors and attributes these to levels of
robotics. Prescott and coauthors have studied in some detail the associated neuromodulating transmitter.
the structure of rat whiskers along with the typical exploratory Advances in computing, neuroscience, and robotics have
behaviors used by the rats and the associated neurological combined to make research in cognitive robotics fruitful with
structures in the brain and sensors. They describe two of the valuable reward for success, because we need to develop robots
biomimetic models they are using to study robot whiskering: that can integrate with humans in a nuanced, changeable, and
whiskerbot, and scratchbot. complex-world. Cognitive robotics provide a synergy between
Disorders in humans are often a window to the working of empirical and simulated data and may provide platforms that
the mind. Early topographical maps such as Penfield’s Homoncu- can be directly tested against animal models. The importance
lus are based on knowledge of brain damage caused by traumatic of this area of research is highlighted by the U.S. National
brain injury [3]. Autism is a spectrum disorder primarily mani- Academy of Engineering 21st Century Grand Challenge to
fested in males, where one of the symptoms is a difficulty in reverse-engineer the brain. The European Union has a similar
movement preparation and execution. Barakova and Chonnapar- agenda with calls in the latest framework program for cognitive
amut test the hypothesis that this symptom may be a consequence systems, interaction, and robotics in FP7.
of differences in the overall delay in processing sensory data caus-
ing a discordance in stimuli. The authors test this hypothesis on References
two platforms. The first is the Webot simulated environment and [1] W. G. Walter, The Living Brain. London: Duckworth, 1953.
the second a physical robot, the e-puck. In both cases, they look [2] E. A. Maguire, D. G. Gadian, I. S. Johnsrude, C. D. Good, J. Ash-
at the consequences of the delay in proprioceptive and visual burner, R. S. Frackowiak, and C. D. Frith, ‘‘Navigation-related
structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers,’’ Proc. Natl. Acad.
information using a continuous approximation to the individual Sci. USA, vol. 97, no. 8, pp. 4398–4403, 2000.
neurons—the dynamic neural field model. The hypothesis [3] W. Penfield and T. Rasmussen, The Cerebral Cortex of Man. New York:
appears to be borne out, but more work is needed. Macmillan, 1950.

18 IEEE Robotics & Automation Magazine SEPTEMBER 2009

Authorized
View publication stats licensed use limited to: Hiroaki Wagatsuma. Downloaded on September 22, 2009 at 07:22 from IEEE Xplore. Restrictions apply.

You might also like