You are on page 1of 9

Psychological Science http://pss.sagepub.

com/

Positive Affect and Cognitive Control: Approach-Motivation Intensity Influences the Balance Between
Cognitive Flexibility and Stability
Ya Liu and Zhenhong Wang
Psychological Science 2014 25: 1116 originally published online 26 March 2014
DOI: 10.1177/0956797614525213

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://pss.sagepub.com/content/25/5/1116

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of:

Association for Psychological Science

Additional services and information for Psychological Science can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://pss.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://pss.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

>> Version of Record - May 8, 2014

OnlineFirst Version of Record - Mar 26, 2014

What is This?

Downloaded from pss.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF BRIGHTON on July 16, 2014


525213
research-article2014
PSSXXX10.1177/0956797614525213Liu, WangPositive Affect and Cognitive Flexibility-Stability Balance

Research Article

Psychological Science

Positive Affect and Cognitive Control: 2014, Vol. 25(5) 1116­–1123


© The Author(s) 2014
Reprints and permissions:
Approach-Motivation Intensity Influences sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0956797614525213

the Balance Between Cognitive Flexibility pss.sagepub.com

and Stability

Ya Liu and Zhenhong Wang


Shaanxi Normal University

Abstract
In most prior research, positive affect has been consistently found to promote cognitive flexibility. However, the
motivational dimensional model of affect assumes that the influence of positive affect on cognitive processes is
modulated by approach-motivation intensity. In the present study, we extended the motivational dimensional model
to the domain of cognitive control by examining the effect of low- versus high-approach-motivated positive affect on
the balance between cognitive flexibility and stability in an attentional-set-shifting paradigm. Results showed that low-
approach-motivated positive affect promoted cognitive flexibility but also caused higher distractibility, whereas high-
approach-motivated positive affect enhanced perseverance but simultaneously reduced distractibility. These results
suggest that the balance between cognitive flexibility and stability is modulated by the approach-motivation intensity
of positive affective states. Therefore, it is essential to incorporate motivational intensity into studies on the influence
of affect on cognitive control.

Keywords
positive affect, approach-motivation intensity, cognitive flexibility, cognitive processes, cognitive stability,
attentional-set-shifting paradigm, emotions, motivation

Received 8/29/13; Revision accepted 1/29/14

Ample evidence in the literature on affect and cognitive 1984; Talarico, Berntsen, & Rubin, 2009), whereas high-
control suggests that positive affect, compared with nega- approach-motivated positive affect has the opposite effect
tive or neutral affect, has a robust beneficial effect on (Gable & Harmon-Jones, 2008, 2010a, 2010b, 2011; Price &
cognitive flexibility (for reviews, see Ashby, Isen, & Harmon-Jones, 2010).
Turken, 1999; Fredrickson, 2001; Isen, 2009). Previous Given the motivational dimensional model and relevant
researchers examining the role of affect in cognitive flex- empirical studies, it seems plausible that positive affective
ibility have focused mainly on the valence dimension of states of varying approach-motivation intensity have differ-
affective states and have rarely examined the underlying ent influences on cognitive flexibility. Initial support
dimension of motivation. for this hypothesis has come from Price and Harmon-
In the motivational dimensional model of affect recently Jones (2010), whose results showed that low-approach-
proposed by Gable and Harmon-Jones (2010c; Harmon- motivated positive affect increased cognitive broadening
Jones, Gable, & Price, 2012, 2013), the effects of positive and flexibility, as measured by cognitive categorization
affect on cognitive processing are assumed to be modu-
lated by approach-motivation intensity. Converging with
this theoretical view, a growing number of studies have
Corresponding Author:
demonstrated that low-approach-motivated positive affect Zhenhong Wang, School of Psychology, Shaanxi Normal University,
broadens attentional and cognitive scope (Fredrickson & 199 South Chang’an Rd., Xi’an 710062, China
Branigan, 2005; Gasper & Clore, 2002; Isen & Daubman, E-mail: wangzhenhong@snnu.edu.cn

Downloaded from pss.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF BRIGHTON on July 16, 2014


Positive Affect and Cognitive Flexibility-Stability Balance 1117

tasks, whereas high-approach-motivated positive affect should decrease switch costs. In the distraction condi-
decreased cognitive broadening and flexibility. In a similar tion, flexible cognitive control should again bias partici-
vein, Wang, Liu, and Jiang (2013) found that low-approach- pants’ attention toward stimuli in the novel color, but
motivated positive affect facilitated response inhibition because these are the distractors, flexibility should
and task switching, whereas high-approach-motivated increase distractibility and therefore increase switch costs.
positive affect impaired task switching. The opposite pattern is expected in the stable-cognitive-
Higher flexibility may lead to higher distractibility (i.e., control mode, in which reactivity toward new informa-
lower stability), especially when new information is irrel- tion is inhibited: Stable cognitive control should increase
evant to the ongoing task. Some studies (Dreisbach, 2006; switch costs in the perseveration condition and decrease
Dreisbach & Goschke, 2004; Marien, Aarts, & Custers, switch costs in the distraction condition.
2012) have investigated the impact of positive affect on We hypothesized that in previous studies (Dreisbach,
the balance between cognitive flexibility and stability, 2006; Dreisbach & Goschke, 2004; Müller et al., 2007),
which is critical for goal-directed behavior in humans. low-approach-motivated positive affect increased reactiv-
These studies have consistently shown that positive affect ity toward novel stimuli, thereby enhancing cognitive
enhances cognitive flexibility but also increases distracti- flexibility, presumably because low-approach-motivated
bility (Dreisbach, 2006; Dreisbach & Goschke, 2004), positive affect occurs after a goal has been achieved or
especially when participants’ responses are represented when there is no goal (Gable & Harmon-Jones, 2010c). In
in terms of goals (Marien et al., 2012). However, these such a positive affective state, a flexible control mode can
studies have primarily examined the impact of low- rather assist organisms in exploring new goals or opportunities.
than high-approach-motivated positive affect on the flex- The flexible control mode should reduce perseverance
ibility-stability balance, because the induced affect has when new information identifies targets but increase dis-
been a relatively passive state not associated with goal tractibility when new information identifies distractors.
pursuit. The aim of the present research was therefore to Thus, low-approach-motivated positive affect should
investigate whether the influences of positive affect on facilitate cognitive flexibility, reducing switch costs in the
the cognitive flexibility-stability balance are modulated perseveration condition and increasing switch costs in
by approach-motivation intensity. the distraction condition.
To systematically examine this issue, we conducted High-approach-motivated positive affect, which often
two experiments using the attentional-set-shifting para- occurs during goal pursuit, should reduce reactivity
digm originally developed by Dreisbach and Goschke toward novel stimuli and promote cognitive stability
(2004). This paradigm, designed to identify the costs and (Gable & Harmon-Jones, 2010c). Enhancing flexibility
benefits of increased cognitive flexibility, consists of two during goal pursuit might prove maladaptive, because it
phases: the preswitch and postswitch phases. In the pre- might lead one away from the current goal. Conversely, a
switch phase, participants are instructed to respond to stable control mode can assist organisms in shielding
target stimuli appearing in a prespecified color (e.g., red) goal pursuit from irrelevant stimuli, perceptions, and cog-
and to ignore distractor stimuli, which appear in a differ- nitions. The stable control mode should enhance perse-
ent color (e.g., blue). Then, in the postswitch phase, par- veration, even in the case of relevant new changes in the
ticipants are given instructions for the perseveration or environment, but simultaneously reduce distractibility
distraction condition. In the perseveration condition caused by irrelevant new stimuli. Thus, high-approach-
(Experiment 1), targets are in a new color (e.g., green), motivated positive affect should enhance cognitive stabil-
and distractors appear in the formerly relevant color ity, increasing switch costs in the perseveration condition
(e.g., red). In the distraction condition (Experiment 2), and reducing switch costs in the distraction condition. In
targets are in the formerly ignored color (e.g., blue), and sum, we hypothesized that the influence of positive affect
distractors appear in a new color (e.g., green). The two on the balance between cognitive flexibility and stability
switching conditions are widely used to assess the degree is modulated by approach-motivation intensity.
of flexibility and stability of cognitive control (Dreisbach
& Goschke, 2004; Dreisbach et al., 2005; Müller et al.,
2007). Experiment 1
More precisely, the attentional-set-shifting paradigm Method
assesses reactivity toward new stimuli, a main aspect of
cognitive control (Dreisbach & Goschke, 2004; Dreisbach Thirty-two undergraduate students (19 women, 13 men)
et al., 2005; Müller et al., 2007). In the perseveration con- participated in Experiment 1 in return for 10 Chinese
dition, flexible cognitive control facilitates disengage- yuan. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal
ment from the previous target color and a bias toward the vision and were not red-green color-blind. They all pro-
novel color, which identifies new targets. Thus, flexibility vided informed consent and were debriefed after the

Downloaded from pss.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF BRIGHTON on July 16, 2014


1118 Liu, Wang

experiment. In order to control the possible confounding the screen, one above the other, until a response was
effect of hunger on the effectiveness of the dessert pic- given (maximum of 1,500 ms). One number was shown in
tures we used to elicit high-approach-motivated positive the target color (e.g., red), and the other was shown in the
affect, we allowed only participants who had eaten 2 to distractor color (e.g., blue). Participants were asked to
4 hr before the experiment to take part. indicate whether the target digit was odd or even by press-
The perseveration condition of the attentional-set- ing either the left “ALT” key or the right “ALT” key on a
shifting paradigm was used in this experiment to assess computer keyboard with the corresponding index finger.
flexibility and stability of cognitive control. Participants Assignment of responses to these keys was counterbal-
performed a digit-categorization task in which they cat- anced across participants. Participants were instructed to
egorized digits of the target color as odd or even. In this respond as quickly as possible while avoiding errors. After
task, the imperative stimuli consisted of two simultane- a response, a blank screen appeared for 500 ms, and a
ously presented digits, randomly selected from a set of new trial began.
eight (2–9); the digits appeared one above the other in At the end of the preswitch phase (i.e., after the 40th
the center of the screen. The selection of stimuli was trial), a 3,000-ms cue informed participants of the new
completely randomized on each trial, the only constraint target color (e.g., green). The procedure for the post-
being that the target and distractor digits were always switch trials was otherwise the same as that for the pre-
mapped to different responses (i.e., if the target digit was switch trials, except that the new distractor color was the
odd, the distractor digit was even, and vice versa). The former target color (e.g., red). Participants rested for 1
location of the target (above or below the distractor) was min between blocks. Figure 1a presents a diagram depict-
determined at random. The digits were always presented ing the course of trials in the preswitch and postswitch
in two different colors selected from a pool of three col- phases.
ors: red, blue, and green. After completing the experiment, participants viewed
There were three experimental blocks: neutral, low- the pictures again (3,000 ms each) and rated each for
approach-motivated positive affect, and high-approach- how pleasant it was (1 = very unpleasant, 9 = very pleas-
motivated positive affect. The order of these three blocks ant), how arousing it was (1 = calm, 9 = exciting), and
was counterbalanced across participants. Each 60-trial how intense their approach motivation was (1 = do not
block consisted of two phases: a preswitch phase (40 tri- want to approach, 9 = really want to approach).
als) and a postswitch phase (20 trials). Low-approach-
motivated positive affect was elicited using 30 pictures of
beautiful landscapes; high-approach-motivated positive
Results
affect was elicited using 30 pictures of delicious desserts. Affective picture ratings.  The ratings for pleasantness
In addition, 30 pictures of household objects were used (i.e., valence), arousal, and approach-motivation inten-
in the neutral block. The experimental blocks were pre- sity were subjected to three separate within-subjects
ceded by a 20-trial practice block in which 10 other pic- repeated measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs) with
tures of household objects were shown. Each picture was picture type as a within-participants factor. For valence
presented twice per block. These pictures were validated ratings, a significant effect of picture type was observed,
in previous studies (Gable & Harmon-Jones, 2008, 2010a, F(2, 62) = 59.20, p < .001, ηp2 = .66. Post hoc Bonferroni
2010b; Gable & Poole, 2012; Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, tests demonstrated that both the dessert pictures (M =
2005; Wang et al., 2013). 6.22, SD = 0.91; p < .001) and the landscape pictures
The experimental program was administered to each (M = 6.44, SD = 0.79; p < .001) had significantly higher
participant individually. Participants were informed that valence ratings than the neutral pictures (M = 4.64, SD =
the target color would be switched during the experiment. 0.84). The difference between the dessert pictures and
The assignment of relevant color (i.e., the preswitch target the landscape pictures was far from reliable, p = .62.
color), irrelevant color (i.e., the preswitch distractor color), Likewise, for arousal ratings, there was a significant effect
and new color (i.e., not used in the preswitch phase) of picture type, F(2, 62) = 16.73, p < .001, ηp2 = .35. Both
remained constant for a given participant but was counter- the dessert pictures (M = 5.35, SD = 1.42; p < .001) and
balanced across participants. At the beginning of each the landscape pictures (M = 4.93, SD = 1.25; p = .002) had
block, a 3,000-ms cue informed participants of the target significantly higher arousal ratings than the neutral pic-
color. Then, the trials began. On each trial, participants tures (M = 3.95, SD = 0.88); there was no difference
saw a picture of a landscape (low-approach-motivation between the dessert and landscape pictures, p = .30. Rat-
block), a dessert (high-approach-motivation block), or a ings of approach-motivation intensity also showed a sig-
household object (neutral block) for 2,000 ms. This was nificant effect of picture type, F(2, 62) = 45.69, p < .001,
followed by a fixation cross for 500 ms. Then, the impera- ηp2 = .60. Average motivational-intensity ratings were sig-
tive stimulus was presented: Two numbers appeared on nificantly higher for dessert pictures (M = 6.09, SD = 0.83)

Downloaded from pss.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF BRIGHTON on July 16, 2014


Positive Affect and Cognitive Flexibility-Stability Balance 1119

a
Preswitch Phase Postswitch Phase
(Trials 1–40) (Trials 41–60)

Target 4 Switch 2
+ +
Is Red 7 to Green 9

3,000 ms 2,000 ms 500 ms 1,500 ms 500 ms 3,000 ms 2,000 ms 500 ms 1,500 ms 500 ms

Time

b Preswitch Phase Postswitch Phase


(Trials 1–40) (Trials 41–60)

Target 4 Switch 3
+ +
Is Red 5 to Blue 8

3,000 ms 2,000 ms 500 ms 1,500 ms 500 ms 3,000 ms 2,000 ms 500 ms 1,500 ms 500 ms

Time
Fig. 1.  Schematic diagrams illustrating trial sequences in (a) Experiment 1, the perseverance condition, and (b) Experiment 2, the distraction
condition. Each block began with a cue indicating the target color. During the preswitch phase, each trial began with a picture, which was fol-
lowed by a fixation cross and then two digits, one in the target color (e.g., red) and the other in the distractor color (e.g., blue). The task was
to indicate whether the digit in the target color was odd or even. In separate blocks, landscape pictures, illustrated in (a), were used to induce
low-approach-motivation positive affect, and dessert pictures, illustrated in (b), were used to induce high-approach-motivation positive affect.
After 40 trials, participants were informed of the new target color. In the perseveration condition (a), the former target color (e.g., red) became
the new distractor color, and a new color (e.g., green) became the target color. In the distraction condition (b), the former distractor color (e.g.,
blue) became the new target color, and a new color (e.g., green) became the new distractor color. Otherwise, the procedure for the trials in
this postswitch phase was the same as that for the preswitch trials.

than for landscape pictures (M = 5.54, SD = 0.87; p = .01) Reaction times were submitted to a 3 (block: neutral,
and were significantly higher for landscape pictures low-approach-motivated positive affect, high-approach-
than for neutral pictures (M = 4.22, SD = 0.92; p < .001). motivated positive affect) × 2 (phase: preswitch, post-
Consistent with results from previous studies (Gable switch) repeated measures ANOVA. Results revealed
& Harmon-Jones, 2008, 2010a, 2010b; Gable & Poole, significant main effects of both block, F(2, 62) = 3.63, p =
2012; Wang et al., 2013), our results indicate that the .03, ηp2 = .10, and phase, F(1, 31) = 72.61, p < .001, ηp2 =
landscape and dessert pictures we used to evoke low- .70. Moreover, these two main effects were qualified by a
and high-approach-motivated positive affect, respec- significant interaction, F(2, 62) = 37.85, p < .001, ηp2 = .55.
tively, were effective. Relative to the neutral block (M = 61 ms, SD = 57), switch
costs were lower in the low-approach-motivation block
Reaction times.  Incorrect responses (7.92% of the (M = 14 ms, SD = 61; p = .002) but higher in the high-
total) and responses with a reaction time more than 3 approach-motivation block (M = 120 ms, SD = 58; p <
standard deviations from the mean (1.67%) were excluded .001). The mean reaction times for each block and phase
from further analyses. Following the method of previous are presented in Figure 2.
studies (Dreisbach & Goschke, 2004; Dreisbach et al., Simple-effects analyses revealed that in the preswitch
2005; Müller et al., 2007), we calculated switch costs by phase, there was no simple main effect of block, F(2, 62) =
subtracting the mean reaction time of the last five pre- 0.15, p = .86, ηp2 = .005. Conversely, in the postswitch
switch trials from the mean reaction time of the first five phase, the simple main effect of block was significant, F(2,
postswitch trials. In practice, this method is commonly 62) = 13.36, p < .001, ηp2 = .30. A post hoc Bonferroni test
used in this type of attentional-set-shifting paradigm. As demonstrated that, compared with reaction times in the
described earlier, cognitive flexibility is reflected in neutral block (M = 709 ms, SD = 107), reaction times were
decreased switch costs in the perseveration condition. lower in the low-approach-motivation block (M = 664 ms,

Downloaded from pss.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF BRIGHTON on July 16, 2014


1120 Liu, Wang

Preswitch In hopes of providing more compelling evidence for our


Postswitch hypothesis, we conducted Experiment 2, the distraction
800 condition.

Experiment 2
750
Method
Mean Reaction Time (ms)

Thirty-six undergraduate students (24 women, 12 men)


participated in Experiment 2 in exchange for 10 yuan.
700 Experiment 2 was identical to Experiment 1 with one
exception: The perseveration condition was replaced
with the distraction condition (Fig. 1b). The difference
between the two conditions is that in the postswitch
650 phase of the distraction condition, the former distractor
color becomes the new target color, and a new color
becomes the distractor color. For example, in the pre-
switch phase, the target color might be red and the dis-
600 tractor color blue; in the postswitch phase, the target
Neutral Low Approach High Approach color would then be blue and the distractor color green.
Motivation Motivation
Fig. 2.  Experiment 1 (perseveration condition) results: mean reaction
time as a function of block and phase. Error bars represent within-
Results
subjects 95% confidence interval. Affective picture ratings.  Results of affective picture
ratings were similar to those in Experiment 1. There were
SD = 113; p = .042) but higher in the high-approach- significant effects of picture type on ratings of valence,
motivation block (M = 761 ms, SD = 114; p = .036). These F(2, 70) = 65.58, p < .001, ηp2 = .65; arousal, F(2, 70) =
results suggest that the opposite effects of low- and high- 13.85, p < .001, ηp2 = .28; and approach-motivation inten-
approach-motivated positive affect on reaction time switch sity, F(2, 70) = 32.56, p < .001, ηp2 = .48. Results again
costs were driven by postswitch rather than preswitch suggested that the affective pictures used in this study
trials. could effectively evoke low- and high-approach-moti-
vated positive affect.
Error rates.  Error data were entered into an analogous
3 × 2 repeated measures ANOVA. Neither the main effects Reaction times.  Incorrect responses (7.13% of the
of block, F(2, 62) = 0.18, p = .83, ηp2 = .006, and phase, total) and responses with a reaction time more than 3
F(1, 31) = 1.95, p = .17, ηp2 = .06, nor their interaction, standard deviations from the mean (1.11%) were excluded
F(2, 62) = 1.28, p = .28, ηp2 = .04, reached statistical from the analyses. As mentioned earlier, cognitive stabil-
significance. ity is reflected in decreased switch costs in the distraction
condition, unlike the perseveration condition.
Reaction times were subjected to a 3 (block: neutral,
Discussion low-approach-motivated positive affect, high-approach-
Experiment 1 revealed that in the perseveration condition, motivated positive affect) × 2 (phase: preswitch, post-
low-approach-motivated positive affect reduced switch switch) repeated measures ANOVA. The analysis yielded
costs, whereas high-approach-motivated positive affect significant main effects of block, F(2, 70) = 7.27, p = .001,
increased switch costs. These results provided initial sup- ηp2 = .17, and phase, F(1, 35) = 49.49, p < .001, ηp2 = .59.
port for our hypothesis that the impact of positive affect on Furthermore, the two main effects were qualified by a
the balance of cognitive flexibility and stability is modu- significant interaction of block and phase, F(2, 70) =
lated by approach-motivation intensity. Previous studies 18.63, p < .001, ηp2 = .35. Relative to the neutral block
have confirmed that higher cognitive flexibility goes along (M = 58 ms, SD = 70), switch costs were higher in the
with increased distractibility, whereas increased cognitive low-approach-motivation block (M = 97 ms, SD = 70; p =
stability can reduce distractibility caused by irrelevant .008) but lower in the high-approach-motivation block
novel stimuli (Dreisbach & Goschke, 2004; Dreisbach et (M = 16 ms, SD = 61; p = .01). The mean reaction times
al., 2005; Müller et al., 2007). Thus, it seems reasonable to for each block and phase are presented in Figure 3.
assume that positive affective states varying in approach- Simple-effects analyses revealed that the simple main
motivation intensity have different effects on distractibility. effect of block was nonsignificant for the preswitch

Downloaded from pss.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF BRIGHTON on July 16, 2014


Positive Affect and Cognitive Flexibility-Stability Balance 1121

Preswitch increased distractibility, as reflected in higher switch


Postswitch costs, whereas high-approach-motivated positive affect
800 reduced distractibility, as reflected in lower switch costs.
This pattern of results is fully compatible with our
hypothesis that low-approach-motivated positive affect
enhances cognitive flexibility but also incurs a comple-
750 mentary cost in the form of increased distractibility. In
Mean Reaction Time (ms)

contrast, positive affect high in approach motivation


enhances perseverance but also reduces distractibility.

700
General Discussion
The present results indicate that the influence of positive
affect on the balance between cognitive flexibility
650 and stability is modulated by approach-motivation
intensity. Specifically, low-approach-motivated positive
affect enhanced cognitive flexibility but simultaneously
increased distractibility, whereas high-approach-moti-
600 vated positive affect enhanced perseveration but also
Neutral Low Approach High Approach reduced distractibility.
Motivation Motivation The finding that low-approach-motivated positive
affect promoted cognitive flexibility is well in line with
Fig. 3.  Results from Experiment 2 (distraction condition): mean reac-
tion time as a function of block and phase. Error bars represent within- much previous research (Dreisbach, 2006; Dreisbach &
subjects 95% confidence interval. Goschke, 2004; Müller et al., 2007; Price & Harmon-Jones,
2010; Wang et al., 2013). The presence of low-approach-
motivated positive affect suggests that there is a stable
phase, F(2, 70) = 0.86, p = .43, ηp2 = .02, but highly sig- and comfortable environment or that things are going
nificant for the postswitch phase, F(2, 70) = 14.43, p < well (Carver, 2003; Fredrickson, 2001; Gable & Harmon-
.001, ηp2 = .29. A post hoc Bonferroni test demonstrated Jones, 2010c). Such a free-floating positive state allows
that, compared with postswitch reaction times in the neu- organisms to be open to new information and thereby
tral block (M = 692 ms, SD = 95), postswitch reaction enhances their cognitive flexibility. This increased flexi-
times were higher in the low-approach-motivation block bility promotes free and full engagement with the envi-
(M = 741 ms, SD = 113; p = .01) but lower in the high- ronment, which might build enduring personal resources
approach-motivation block (M = 639 ms, SD = 111; p = and ultimately improve well-being (Fredrickson, 2001).
.02). These results suggest that the opposite effects of However, the flexible control mode may cause distracti-
low- and high-approach-motivated positive affect on bility when novel stimuli are irrelevant for the ongoing
switch costs in the distraction condition were driven by task, which in turn impedes goal pursuit.
postswitch rather than preswitch trials. Thus, the findings The finding that high-approach-motivated positive
of Experiment 2 conceptually replicated the findings of affect enhances perseverance, or reduces flexibility, con-
Experiment 1 in a different switching condition. verges with results from some prior studies (Price &
Harmon-Jones, 2010; Wang et al., 2013). High-approach-
Error rates.  Error data were entered into an analogous motivated positive affect is very relevant to biologically
3 × 2 repeated measures ANOVA. A marginal main effect important outcomes such as reproduction, social attach-
of phase was observed, F(1, 35) = 3.23, p = .08, ηp2 = .08; ment, and eating and drinking (Gable & Harmon-Jones,
there was a tendency for error-rate switch costs to be reli- 2010c; Harmon-Jones et al., 2012). Such an appetitive
able. Neither the main effect of block, F(2, 70) = 2.12, p = state encourages tenacious goal pursuit. During goal pur-
.13, ηp2 = .06, nor the interaction of phase and block, F(2, suit, a flexible control mode might prove maladaptive,
70) = 0.38, p = .69, ηp2 = .01, proved reliable. because it might increase distractibility caused by irrele-
vant new stimuli and consequently delay or hinder acqui-
sition of the desired goal. So, high-approach-motivated
Discussion positive affect should reduce flexibility or, rather, enhance
In Experiment 2, low- and high-approach-motivated pos- perseverance, so that organisms are less distracted or
itive affect clearly had opposite effects on distractibility. even not distracted by irrelevant new information. This
In particular, low-approach-motivated positive affect enhanced stability might assist organisms in achieving

Downloaded from pss.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF BRIGHTON on July 16, 2014


1122 Liu, Wang

desired goals by shutting out irrelevant distractors. Funds of Graduate Programs, Shaanxi Normal University (Grant
However, the stable control mode might cause rigid 2013CXB005).
behavior when novel stimuli are critical for completing a
task. References
The opposite patterns of effects observed for low- and Ashby, F. G., Isen, A. M., & Turken, A. U. (1999). A neuro-
high-approach-motivated positive affect fit exactly with psychological theory of positive affect and its influence on
the motivational dimensional model, according to which cognition. Psychological Review, 106, 529–550.
approach-motivation intensity modulates the influences Carver, C. S. (2003). Pleasure as a sign you can attend to some-
of positive affect on cognitive processing. Low-approach- thing else: Placing positive feelings within a general model
motivated positive affect broadens attentional and cogni- of affect. Cognition & Emotion, 17, 241–261.
Dreisbach, G. (2006). How positive affect modulates cognitive
tive scope to better enable perception of novel stimuli. In
control: The costs and benefits of reduced maintenance
turn, this enhances flexibility when novel stimuli serve as
capability. Brain and Cognition, 60, 11–19.
new targets but increases distractibility when novel stim- Dreisbach, G., & Goschke, T. (2004). How positive affect modu-
uli serve as distractors. In contrast, high-approach-moti- lates cognitive control: Reduced perseveration at the cost of
vated positive affect narrows attentional and cognitive increased distractibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
scope to shield perception of the chosen target from Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 343–353.
irrelevant novel stimuli. In turn, this reduces flexibility Dreisbach, G., Müller, J., Goschke, T., Strobel, A., Schulze, K.,
and enhances perseverance when novel stimuli serve as Lesch, K. P., & Brocke, B. (2005). Dopamine and cognitive
new targets but reduces distractibility when novel stimuli control: The influence of spontaneous eyeblink rate and
serve as distractors. Accordingly, the motivational dimen- dopamine gene polymorphisms on perseveration and dis-
sional model could well explain the increased flexibility tractibility. Behavioral Neuroscience, 119, 483–490.
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in posi-
and higher distractibility associated with low-approach-
tive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive
motivated positive affect and the enhanced perseverance
emotions. American Psychologist, 56, 218–226.
and lower distractibility associated with high-approach- Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions
motivated positive affect. broaden the scope of attention and thought-action reper-
In summary, the current research provides further sup- toires. Cognition & Emotion, 19, 313–332.
port for the motivational dimensional model of affect and Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2008). Approach-motivated
extends this model to the cognitive-control domain. positive affect reduces breadth of attention. Psychological
Along with previous work on cognitive scope, this Science, 19, 476–482.
research suggests that positive affect should not be Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010a). The effect of low
regarded as a unitary construct that is beneficial only to vs. high approach-motivated positive affect on memory for
cognitive broadening and flexibility. Positive affect can peripherally vs. centrally presented information. Emotion,
10, 599–603.
broaden or narrow cognitive scope and can make for
Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010b). Late positive poten-
flexible or stable cognitive control, depending on the
tial to appetitive stimuli and local attentional bias. Emotion,
approach-motivation intensity of the positive affect. Thus, 10, 441–446.
the present research highlights the importance of incor- Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010c). The motivational
porating motivational intensity into studies on the influ- dimensional model of affect: Implications for breadth of
ence of affect on cognitive control. attention, memory, and cognitive categorization. Cognition
& Emotion, 24, 322–337.
Author Contributions Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2011). Attentional conse-
Z. Wang developed the study concept. Both authors contrib- quences of pregoal and postgoal positive affects. Emotion,
uted to the study design. Y. Liu conducted the experiments and 11, 1358–1367.
analyzed and interpreted the data under the supervision of Gable, P. A., & Poole, B. D. (2012). Time flies when you’re hav-
Z. Wang. Y. Liu drafted the manuscript, and Z. Wang provided ing approach-motivated fun: Effects of motivational intensity
critical revisions. Both authors made revisions to the manu- on time perception. Psychological Science, 23, 879–886.
script and approved the final version of the manuscript for Gasper, K., & Clore, G. (2002). Attending to the big picture:
submission. Mood and global versus local processing of visual informa-
tion. Psychological Science, 13, 34–40.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests Harmon-Jones, E., Gable, P. A., & Price, T. F. (2012). The influ-
ence of affective states varying in motivational intensity
The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with on cognitive scope. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience,
respect to their authorship or the publication of this article. 6, Article 73. Retrieved from http://www.frontiersin.org/
Journal/10.3389/fnint.2012.00073/full
Funding Harmon-Jones, E., Gable, P. A., & Price, T. F. (2013).
This study was supported by the National Natural Science Does negative affect always narrow and positive affect
Foundation of China (Grant 30970912) and by Innovation always broaden the mind? Considering the influence

Downloaded from pss.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF BRIGHTON on July 16, 2014


Positive Affect and Cognitive Flexibility-Stability Balance 1123

of motivational intensity on cognitive scope. Current implicitly motivates the pursuit of goals or means.
Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 301–307. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 277–
Isen, A. M. (2009). A role for neuropsychology in understanding 283.
the facilitating influence of positive affect on social behav- Müller, J., Dreisbach, G., Goschke, T., Hensch, T., Lesch, K. P.,
ior and cognitive processes. In S. Lopez & C. R. Snyder & Brocke, B. (2007). Dopamine and cognitive control: The
(Eds.), Oxford handbook of positive psychology (2nd ed., prospect of monetary gains influences the balance between
pp. 503–518). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. flexibility and stability in a set-shifting paradigm. European
Isen, A. M., & Daubman, K. A. (1984). The influence of affect Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 3661–3668.
on categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Price, T. F., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The effect of embod-
Psychology, 47, 1206–1217. ied emotive states on cognitive categorization. Emotion, 10,
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2005). International 934–938.
Affective Picture System (IAPS): Digitized photographs, Talarico, J. M., Berntsen, D., & Rubin, D. C. (2009). Positive
instruction manual and affective ratings (Technical Report emotions enhance recall of peripheral details. Cognition &
No. A-6). Gainesville: University of Florida Center for Emotion, 23, 380–398.
Research in Psychophysiology. Wang, Z., Liu, Y., & Jiang, C. (2013). The effect of low versus
Marien, H., Aarts, H., & Custers, R. (2012). Being flexible high approach-motivated positive affect on cognitive con-
or rigid in goal-directed behavior: When positive affect trol. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 45, 546–555.

Downloaded from pss.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF BRIGHTON on July 16, 2014

You might also like