You are on page 1of 8

Physiology & Behavior 103 (2011) 51–58

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Physiology & Behavior


j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w. e l s ev i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / p h b

Learning and the persistence of appetite: Extinction and the motivation to


eat and overeat☆
Mark E. Bouton ⁎
Department of Psychology, University of Vermont, 2 Colchester Ave., Burlington, VT 05405-0134, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The modern world is saturated with highly palatable and highly available food, providing many opportunities
Received 15 September 2010 to associate food with environmental cues and actions (through Pavlovian and operant or instrumental
Received in revised form 31 October 2010 learning, respectively). Basic learning processes can often increase the tendency to approach and consume
Accepted 19 November 2010
food, whereas extinction, in which Pavlovian and operant behaviors decline when the reinforcer is withheld,
weakens but does not erase those tendencies. Contemporary research suggests that extinction involves an
Keywords:
Eating
inhibitory form of new learning that appears fragile because it is highly dependent on the context for
Overeating expression. These ideas are supported by the phenomena of renewal, spontaneous recovery, resurgence,
Pavlovian and operant conditioning reinstatement, and rapid reacquisition in appetitive learning, which together may help explain why
Extinction overeating may be difficult to suppress permanently, and why appetitive behavior may seem so persistent.
© 2010 Published by Elsevier Inc.

Human food intake is influenced by a large and very complex set of conditional stimuli (CSs), and as CSs, they would be expected to
factors (e.g., [1]). However, there is little doubt that learning and engage approach behavior (e.g., [9]) and any of a number of
conditioning processes are important among them. As Stephen conditioned responses that function to get the system ready for the
Woods and others have emphasized, conditioning can play a very meal (e.g., [10]). They might also trigger eating, even when the
important role in determining the onset or timing of meals, individual is full or satiated (e.g., [11–15]). Table 1 also illustrates
preferences and aversions for different foods, and other aspects of some voluntary, operant behaviors available for reinforcement by the
eating and appetite (e.g., [2–4]; see also [5–7]). The purpose of this pleasures of eating—choosing a restaurant at the mall, purchasing
article is to selectively review some basic research on learning with an food, opening the packaging to get at the food, and of course handling
emphasis on extinction, a major inhibitory process in conditioning, and eating it. Presentation of a Pavlovian CS would further excite or
and discuss some of the possible implications for understanding the motivate these behaviors through Pavlovian-instrumental transfer
persistence of appetite, difficulties in losing weight and maintaining (e.g., [16,17]), for example, by eliciting the “wanting” of food (e.g.,
weight loss, and the contemporary obesity epidemic. [18,19]). Given the abundance and high palatability of the food in the
The modern world is saturated with food and with opportunities environment today, basic learning processes could easily support
to consume it. As noted by Kessler [8], we are constantly exposed to strong appetite and food-motivated behavior.
highly palatable, high-calorie foods that have been engineered with Table 1 also lists a few examples of the kinds of “contexts” in which
multiple layers of sugar, salt, and fat. From the perspective of learning food consumption occurs. Contexts are usually “background” cues in
theory, the high abundance of such tasty foods means that the world which Pavlovian CSs are presented and operant behaviors are emitted.
is full of very effective conditioning trials, i.e., opportunities to Thus, as illustrated in Table 1, CSs and operant behaviors never occur
associate the food with cues in the environment (through Pavlovian in a vacuum; they always occur in a context like a shopping mall, an
conditioning) or with voluntary actions that lead to it (through airport, the couch in front of the television, or a social event such as a
operant or instrumental learning). Table 1 illustrates some of the Superbowl party. Contexts like these may be directly associated with
many cues and operant behaviors that are available for association food, and have all the behavioral consequences of CSs noted above.
with food. In the Pavlovian case, it seems hard to avoid exposure to Contexts can also provide a kind of hierarchical control over behavior
colorful food packaging, advertising logos, and the sight, smell and that occurs in them (e.g., [20]), as I will illustrate below. In that case,
taste of foods. Any or all such stimuli can serve as Pavlovian they can engage behavior not by directly eliciting it, but by signaling
whether or not the CS or the operant behavior will be associated with
food.
☆ For the Special Issue of Physiology & Behavior, The implications of conditioning and
metabolism for addiction and obesity: A Festschrift honoring Stephen C. Woods.
Research in my laboratory and in others has discovered that
⁎ Tel.: +1 802 656 4164; fax: +1 802 656 8783. contexts play an especially important role in extinction. In extinction,
E-mail address: mark.bouton@uvm.edu. the reinforcer (food) is withdrawn, and the Pavlovian CS or the

0031-9384/$ – see front matter © 2010 Published by Elsevier Inc.


doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.11.025
52 M.E. Bouton / Physiology & Behavior 103 (2011) 51–58

Table 1 strong. In either case, there is often surprisingly little effect on the
Some natural conditioning events available for association with food. strength of the response to the CS when the CS is switched from
Conditional stimuli (CSs) Context A to Context B at the beginning of extinction. That is, the first
Advertising logos context switch has little influence on conditioned responding. Like the
Food packaging ABC and AAB renewal phenomena themselves, this feature of
Sight of food
behavior suggests that conditioned responding can transfer surpris-
Smell of food
Taste of food ingly well to new contexts, while extinction is more context-specific.
Operant (instrumental) actions The asymmetry or imbalance may be fundamental to the persistence
Approaching food court or restaurant of motivated behavior and appetite.
Purchasing food
Weight loss is notoriously difficult to maintain after clinical
Handling food
Contexts:
treatment, and Wilfley et al. [31] have recently noted how renewal
Airports effects might contribute to making maintenance difficult. The renewal
Shopping malls effect might also work in more natural settings, outside the domain of
Sporting events clinical treatment. For example, a friend of mine was able to eat
Superbowl parties
sensibly and extinguish a number of habitual appetitive behaviors
Couch in front of the television
while she was at school for several months, away from a home
environment in which the family often gathered socially with great
operant response consequently occurs without it. The strength of quantities of food. At school, she stopped shopping for (and cooking)
Pavlovian or operant behavior therefore declines. An important fact large amounts of food when she was expecting guests, and refrained
about extinction is that it does not erase the original learning; a great from over-indulging at potlucks and dinner parties. On return home to
deal of research suggests that the CS or behavior's original meaning is the family over the holidays, however, all the original behaviors
still available in the brain, even after extensive exposure occasions in returned. In a similar way, those of us who feel cravings in the
which the CS or the behavior has occurred without the reinforcer (e.g., presence of the odor of cinnamon buns may learn to suppress the urge
[21,22]). This means that an individual may find it hard to unlearn to eat in their presence when we visit the shopping mall. But the odor
behaviors and habits that have accumulated over years of drinking may trigger the desire again when we encounter it at the airport.
soda and eating junk food. Instead, research suggests that extinction The renewal effect is clearly evident in operant as well as Pavlovian
depends at least partly on the organism actively learning something conditioning. Interestingly, its importance in operant learning has
new that inhibits performance based on the original learning. As I been a focus of several behavioral pharmacology laboratories
illustrate below, the organism seems to learn that the CS or the investigating drug self-administration. The “compulsion” that some
behavior is no longer paired with the reinforcer in the present context. individuals have to take drugs might bear some resemblance to the
Behavior can thus return in other contexts, and this fact (among “compulsion” that others have to eat food. ABA renewal effects have
others) makes extinguished behavior seem persistent and vulnerable now been documented after extinction when rats have been
to relapse. I will illustrate the idea by reviewing several phenomena in reinforced for lever pressing with injections of heroin (e.g., [32]),
extinction. The phenomena and their implications have been cocaine (e.g., [33]), a mixture of heroin and cocaine (e.g., [34]), or oral
discussed elsewhere (e.g., [23,24]); what follows emphasizes recent delivery of ethanol (e.g., [35,36]). Specifically, when the rat learns to
research with food reinforcers and attempts to develop some specific take the drug via lever pressing in one context and then is
implications for appetite and overeating. extinguished in another, drug seeking returns when the animal is
simply returned to the original context. ABA renewal has also been
1. Renewal of extinguished appetitive behavior demonstrated in operant experiments in which rats have learned to
lever press for food pellets instead of drugs (e.g., [37,38,76]).
One phenomenon that is especially central to the contemporary Nakajima et al. [37] also found the renewal effect in discriminated
understanding of extinction is the renewal effect. Renewal has been operant conditioning, where rats first learned to perform the operant
studied most extensively in Pavlovian learning. In its simplest form, if response in the presence of a light discriminative stimulus. In this
a CS is associated with a reinforcer in one context (e.g., Context A), case, renewal took the form of increased responding in the presence of
and then extinguished in a second one (Context B), the CS elicits the light in Context A after it had been fully extinguished in Context B.
behavior again when it is returned to the original context (Context A). The context thus controlled the tendency of the light to instigate food-
This is called ABA renewal. Renewal can also be observed if seeking behavior.
conditioning, extinction, and testing respectively occur in Contexts Most of the initial operant work focused on ABA renewal, ignoring
A, B, and C (ABC renewal) and A, A, and B (AAB renewal). Although the the ABC and AAB forms. In fact, there were a number of failures to
latter forms of renewal are often somewhat weaker than the classic produce the AAB renewal effect [32,34,37]. This created a puzzle,
ABA form, they are interesting because the final return of the behavior because as noted above, ABC and AAB renewals are important in
occurs in a context other than the original conditioning context. ABC suggesting that removal from the extinction context is sufficient to
and AAB renewals thus indicate that simple removal from the context renew behavior. However, my colleagues and I have recently
of extinction can be sufficient to cause a return of conditioned produced good evidence of each of the renewal effects in free operant
responding. Something learned in, and specific to, the extinction learning [39]. For example, in one experiment rats were first trained
context inhibits or suppresses the conditioned response. Hence, to lever press in Context A on a variable-interval (VI) 30-s schedule of
extinction is at least partly a context-dependent form of learning. food-pellet reinforcement. After several daily sessions, they received
Many well-known studies of renewal used fear conditioning four sessions of extinction training in either Context A, the same
techniques, where a tone is first paired with footshock and then context, or Context B, a second context. After extinction was complete,
consequently evokes fear (e.g., [25,26]). The renewal of fear has the rats were tested in extinction in both Contexts A and B
implications for the treatment and understanding of anxiety disorders (counterbalanced). As shown in Fig. 1, when the rats extinguished
(e.g., [23,27]). However, we have also demonstrated various forms of in Context B were tested in Context A (the ABA renewal condition),
renewal in appetitive conditioning, where a CS like a tone is paired there was a robust return of extinguished lever pressing. And when
with food pellets (e.g., [28–30]). In this case, food responding is the rats extinguished in Context A were tested in Context B (the AAB
elicited by the CS again when it is removed from the extinction renewal condition), there was also an increase in responding.
context. The parallel between aversive and appetitive conditioning is Although the AAB effect was significantly weaker than the ABA effect,
M.E. Bouton / Physiology & Behavior 103 (2011) 51–58 53

Fig. 1. The renewal effect after the extinction of operant conditioning. Left: Mean lever-press responding in rats for a food-pellet reinforcer during each 30-min session of acquisition
(Context A) and extinction. Right: Mean responding during two 10-min test sessions in the extinction context and the non-extinction (renewal) context (counterbalanced; note
change in the y axis). AAB = renewal in Context B after extinction in Context A (n = 16); ABA = renewal in Context A after extinction in Context B (n = 16). Both groups increased
responding when moved to the nonextinction context (ps b 0.005); there was significantly more responding in the renewal context in the ABA condition than in the AAB condition
(p b 0.005).
From Bouton et al. [39], Experiment 1. Reprinted with permission.

100% of the animals in either condition responded more in the an experiment in which two groups of rats received operant lever
renewal context than in the extinction context. In another exper- press-food training in Context A before receiving extinction (lever
iment, we found that tripling the amount of extinction in the AAB press-no food) in Context B. A control group received the same
design did not weaken the strength of the AAB renewal effect. And in a training that the ABA group in Fig. 1 had. But in an experimental
third experiment, we found evidence of ABC renewal—when operant group, the rats were returned to Context A (with response lever
conditioning occurred in Context A, extinction occurred in B, and removed and unavailable) four times (30-min sessions each) after
testing occurred in B and C, the rats recovered some responding in C. every extinction session in Context B. This treatment created
In this case, 94% of the subjects showed more responding in the substantial exposure to the food-associated box without food.
nonextinction context than in the extinction context. Thus, there is no Although all that exposure should have weakened the influence of
doubt that mere removal from a context of extinction can be sufficient the context-food association through extinction, as shown in Fig. 2, it
to cause recovery of an extinguished appetitive operant. The had no discernible impact on the strength of the renewal effect that
extinguished craving for a cinnamon bun, and the urge to get up occurred there during a final test. Although we know that a direct
and buy one, can be renewed at the airport even if I've never eaten a association between a context and a reinforcer can in fact augment
cinnamon bun there. One does not have to return to the original extinguished operant responding ([40], see below), the role of
context in which the operant behavior was learned. Context A in the ABA renewal experiment may depend on another
Although the confirmation of operant ABC and AAB renewal is mechanism. It seems likely that the context is playing the role of an
important, there is little doubt that ABA is a stronger effect than occasion setter, a stimulus that signals that the response–reinforcer
either AAB or ABC. One obvious reason is that testing in the ABA association is now in force, rather than simply that the context
design occurs in a context (Context A) that has been directly means food. In Pavlovian experiments, such occasion setting is not
associated with food during the original operant training. Thus, the diminished by simple exposure to the occasion setting stimulus itself
rat is returned to a food-associated context that can motivate (e.g., [41]), but instead requires disconfirmation of a target CS–
responding (through Pavlovian-instrumental transfer) via its direct reinforcer relation in the presence of the occasion setter in order to
Pavlovian association with food (e.g., [17]). To test this idea, we ran extinguish it. In the analogous operant case, simple passive exposure

Fig. 2. Lack of an effect of exposure to Context A on the ABA renewal effect in operant conditioning. Left: Mean lever-press responding during each 30-min session of acquisition
(Context A) and extinction (Context B). Right: Mean responding during the 10-min test sessions in the extinction context (Context B) and the non-extinction (renewal) context
(Context A; note change in y axis). EXP = extra exposures to Context A (n = 16) during the extinction phase; NoEXP = no such exposures (n = 16). There was a reliable increase in
responding in either group in Context A (p b 0.005), and no difference between the groups or group × test session interaction (Fs b 1).
From Bouton et al. [39], Experiment 4. Reprinted with permission.
54 M.E. Bouton / Physiology & Behavior 103 (2011) 51–58

to the cues of a fast food restaurant might not be as good at 3. Resurgence of extinguished appetitive behavior
weakening operant food-seeking as extinction of at least one
operant behavior in that context. We need more basic research on Another post-extinction phenomenon is worth addressing at this
how to get rid of occasion setting in both Pavlovian and operant point, because it seems especially relevant to the issue of overeating.
learning. In resurgence, an organism first learns one operant response (e.g.,
It is worth explicitly summarizing what renewal might suggest for pressing a lever in a Skinner box) and then, while that behavior is
our understanding of overeating. First, the renewal effect tells us that being extinguished, a second “replacement” behavior is reinforced
extinction is not unlearning. The potential for old behaviors may (e.g., pressing a second lever in the box). In a third phase, the second
remain, perhaps indefinitely, in the memory system. Second, it behavior is then extinguished, and the first response recovers (e.g.,
illustrates and supports the idea that extinction depends on learning [51–53]). Once again, extinction—in this case coupled with the
about the extinction context. Thus, as the overeater learns not to eat in explicit reinforcement of a new behavior—does not erase the original
the presence of a trigger cue, he or she really learns not to eat in the memory, but only suppresses it. An example of resurgence is
presence of the trigger cue in that context. Third, the ABC and AAB presented in Fig. 3 (Winterbauer and Bouton, unpublished). Rats
effects further tell us that removal from the extinction context is were first trained to press on a lever (Lever 1) reinforced on a VI 30
sufficient to cause a return of the behavior—it is not necessary to schedule (Panel A). Then, presses on Lever 1 were extinguished while
return to the context in which indulgence has occurred in the past. presses on a second lever were reinforced (or, in a control group,
Finally, although a return to the original context (ABA) does produce ignored; Panel B). When responses on Lever 2 were then in turn
the strongest renewal effect, simple exposure to the context without extinguished, responses on Lever 1 returned in the experimental
emission of the operant behavior (as in the experiment shown in group (Panel D at right in Fig. 3). New experiments (in progress) have
Fig. 2) is not sufficient to eliminate the renewal effect. This might be found that resurgence is even stronger when the first response
one reason why simple exposure to cues associated with drugs and receives more training than the rather minimal amount shown. The
perhaps food might be ineffective at reducing appetite (see [42]). phenomenon also has some generality. For example, a number of
More research is needed to further understand how to eliminate different schedules of reinforcement on Levers 1 and 2 support the
behavioral control by occasion setting. effect [53], and resurgence has also been shown to occur in rats
reinforced in Phase 1 with other reinforcers, like alcohol [55].
In the world outside the laboratory, reinforcement of other
2. Spontaneous recovery of extinguished appetitive behavior behaviors during extinction of a target behavior is probably the rule
rather than the exception. A dieter who is reducing his or her calorie
Spontaneous recovery, another post-extinction behavior recovery intake might be encouraged to take up new behaviors, such as
phenomenon, was first described by Pavlov ([43], e.g., p. 58). In either walking or running or playing volley ball. Any such replacement
Pavlovian or operant learning, allowing time to elapse after extinction behavior has its own payoff according to its own natural schedule of
has occurred can allow the behavior to at least partially return. In the reinforcement. Extinction of the new behavior could allow the bad
animal lab, the CS elicits responding again, or the subject now returns habits to resurge. In the clinic, weight loss and self-regulatory
to the lever and emits some responding. Extinguished appetitive behaviors can be explicitly reinforced via social contact with a
behavior can thus return after a delay. clinician, or even more effectively, with money (e.g., [56]). Although
Several explanations of spontaneous recovery are available (e.g., the evidence suggests that such strategies help with weight loss, the
[44]). One idea is that the organism pays less and less attention to the implication of resurgence is that the extinguished appetitive behavior
CS (or perhaps its own behavior) as extinction training continues, and should be expected to lapse to some extent if the new behavior is put
this attention recovers spontaneously over time (e.g., [45]). However, on extinction. Consistent with this, weight is regained at least partially
spontaneous recovery also occurs after counterconditioning, where a in the first few months after monetary incentives are withdrawn (e.g.,
CS associated with food is associated with a qualitatively different [56]).
event (e.g., footshock) in the second phase [46]. The subject shows, by Resurgence can result from several behavioral processes. An early
its appropriate shock-related behavior when the CS is presented, that idea [57] was that the reinforcement of the replacement behavior
it is still attending to the CS during Phase 2. My students and I have might reduce the frequency of the behavior it is replacing so radically
instead emphasized that time may be part of the context. Just as the that the individual has fewer opportunities to learn about extinction.
organism might learn that the CS or operant response is no longer The replacement behavior, or the availability of the incentive
paired with food here, it might learn that the CS is no longer paired motivating it, suppresses the original response so much that the
with food now. Spontaneous recovery is essentially the renewal effect subject insufficiently samples the new extinction contingency. We
that occurs when testing occurs in a temporal context that is different have recently shown, however, that resurgence readily occurs even
from the temporal context of extinction. Consistent with this view, we when the new behavior is reinforced on a reinforcement schedule that
have recently shown that temporal intervals can indeed play the role is lean enough so that it does not suppress the original behavior
of context and control performance to CSs embedded in them [47–49]. beyond the suppression that occurs through extinction alone [53]. As
In a sense, the animal fails to retrieve the extinction learning (or an alternative, we have therefore proposed that resurgence can be
counterconditioning) as time elapses after extinction. Consistent with considered a subtle example of the renewal effect (see also [58]). The
this possibility, we have shown that presentation of a retrieval cue original behavior is extinguished in the “context” of the new one
associated with extinction can reduce the strength of either being reinforced. Then, when reinforcement of the new behavior is
spontaneous recovery caused by changing the temporal context discontinued, and its frequency begins to decline, the context changes
(e.g., [50]) or renewal caused by changing the physical context [30]. again. Resurgence might thus be interpreted as another example of
Similar reminders could be built into weight loss programs to help the ABC renewal effect—conditioning generalizes more than extinc-
individuals remember to choose newly-learned healthy behaviors, tion does to the new context.
rather than older less healthy ones, in new contexts [31]. Perhaps the most straightforward implication of this contextual
The general implication for appetite is that the urge to eat or seek analysis of resurgence is that it highlights the importance of the
food may return sometime after the temptation has been successfully transition between the extinction plus replacement behavior training
suppressed. Once again, the conditioning processes that trigger and the test in which the replacement is put on extinction. A
motivated behavior seem to generalize better across contexts than procedure that makes the transition less abrupt, e.g., by fading in the
the extinction process that suppresses them. extinction phase so that the new reinforcer gradually becomes less
M.E. Bouton / Physiology & Behavior 103 (2011) 51–58 55

Fig. 3. Resurgence after extinction in operant conditioning. A target operant behavior (presses to Lever 1, or L1) was first reinforced on a VI 30 reinforcement schedule over several
30-min acquisition sessions (Panel A). Then, in a 4-day extinction phase, presses to L1 were extinguished under normal conditions (Group EXT, n = 7), or while a second behavior
(presses to Lever 2, or L2) was also reinforced on a fixed-ratio 10 schedule (Group FR10, n = 7). Responding on Lever 1 is shown in Panel B, whereas responding on Lever 2 is shown
in Panel C. In a final 30-min test session, presses on Lever 2 were then extinguished. As shown in Panel D, responding on Lever 1 increased during this session compared to the final
extinction session, F (1,6) = 39.09, p b 0.005; there was no such increase in responding in the control group (EXT), which pressed Lever 1 reliably less than Group FR10 during the
resurgence test, F (1,12) = 11.00, p b 0.01. Lever 2 responses are shown in Panel E. Unpublished data by Winterbauer and Bouton.

frequent, would produce a less salient context change and ultimately (but see [63]). A mechanism we have emphasized is the fact that
allow extinction to be connected with contexts beyond the artificial presentation of the reinforcer allows it to be associated with the
one in which monetary incentives are earned. We are currently context, and this contextual conditioning triggers the extinguished
running experiments in the animal laboratory that investigate such response. Consistent with a role for context conditioning, in both
methods. Pavlovian and operant learning, presentation of the reinforcer in a
different context has very little impact on the extinguished response
4. Reinstatement of extinguished appetitive behavior in the context where it has been extinguished (e.g., [26,28,40,59,64]).
Similarly, extended exposure to the context after presentation of the
We also know that extinguished behavior can readily return if the reinforcer can reduce its reinstating impact [40,64]. Theoretically, a
organism is exposed to the reinforcer itself again after extinction. In context made “hot” by recent association with the reinforcer might
Pavlovian learning, the food reinforcer can be presented and motivate and invigorate the Pavlovian or instrumental response
consumed again, independently of the CS, after extinction. When (exposure to the context without the reinforcer would effectively
the CS is then tested, it can trigger responding again [28,59,60]. “cool” the context down). Or a hot context might set the occasion for
Reinstatement also occurs after operant extinction. If the reinforcer is the response, because the context was also hot when the response
simply presented after lever pressing has been extinguished, the rat was originally reinforced, and thus provides a kind of contextual
may return to lever pressing [40,61,62]. In the laboratory, the stimulus for another ABA renewal effect.
reinstating reinforcers are usually presented somewhat artificially; Another mechanism behind reinstatement, though, is direct
they are delivered freely, that is, not contingent on a particular occasion setting by the reinforcer itself. When the rat lever presses
behavior. Humans probably rarely receive rewards that are truly free in an operant conditioning session, or the human takes a tasty chip
and noncontingent. But we have recently shown that reinforcers from the bag, the taste and feel of the reinforcer provide a stimulus
presented contingent on a second behavior also reinstate a separate that is associated with reinforcement of the next response: The rat
extinguished operant [54]. Because of reinstatement, it is easy to see therefore returns to the lever, and the human reaches into the bag
why an overeater might begin bingeing again if he or she eats some again. Presentation of food reinforcers after extinction might
tasty guacamole at a Superbowl party. The presentation of the reinvigorate appetitive behavior because they are thus part of the
reinforcer would increase Pavlovian responding as well as voluntary, context that supported them. One implication is that providing
food-seeking operant behavior. occasional reinforcers during extinction, so they are now decoupled
There are several mechanisms underlying reinstatement. In the from reinforcement of the next response, can reduce the reinstate-
drug self-administration literature, the operating assumption is ment effect (e.g., [54,62]). I will return to this somewhat paradoxical
usually that presentation of the drug primes neural reward circuits idea in the next section.
56 M.E. Bouton / Physiology & Behavior 103 (2011) 51–58

5. Rapid reacquisition of extinguished appetitive behavior with food pellets on a VI 30 schedule, the rats received either simple
extinction or extinction with occasional response–reinforcer pairings
We (e.g., [23]) have previously noted that the phenomena (the reinforcement schedule was gradually made leaner, until the rats
discussed above can cause extinguished behavior to lapse or “slip.” were responding on a VI 32-min schedule). Responding declined with
But one very important feature of eating and appetitive behavior in either method, although the occasional response–reinforcer pairings
the natural world is that the act of eating during a lapse will inevitably led to less complete decline (as illustrated by “before” at left in Fig. 4).
bring the stimuli and behavior into contact with the reinforcer again. Importantly, when the response was then paired with the reinforcer
Response recovery by renewal, spontaneous recovery, resurgence, or again in a reacquisition phase, the partial reinforcement procedure
reinstatement will bring the individual back in contact with new CS– reduced the impact of each new pairing. That is, while rats that had
reinforcer or action–reinforcer pairings. received simple extinction increased their responding during the
This observation is important, because we know that behaviors minutes after each new action–reinforcer pairing, the partially
that have received extensive training, like those presumably involved reinforced rats did not. In this way, occasional reinforcement of the
in overeating, are quick to return to full force when the CS or the action during extinction may slow down relapse when the action
operant behavior is paired with the reinforcer again [65]. Such a regularly leads to the reinforcer again.
return to the full-blown, reinforced, behavior would be the start of The implications for overeating (and for drug taking) are
true relapse. Many investigators have been satisfied to assume that interesting. If a person has learned to eat unhealthy foods (or take
rapid reacquisition is merely evidence that the original learning is drugs) repeatedly in binges, then eating a snack (or taking a drink)
“saved” or retained after extinction. But this doesn't really explain becomes a cue for reinforcement of the next one. A snack is a stimulus
why the behavior rapidly returns. Instead, at a more mechanistic level, for additional intake. Therefore, an extinction procedure that includes
the reintroduction of the CS–reinforcer or action–reinforcer contin- the consumption of occasional snacks in a way that allows them to be
gency may return the subject to the original acquisition context, associated with an overall reduction in intake may help reduce the
because conditioning originally occurred in the presence of a memory risk of a lapse becoming a relapse.
of recent reinforced trials. In this sense, rapid reacquisition may be an
ABA renewal effect [65–67]. 6. Discussion and conclusion
An important implication of this analysis is analogous to one we
discussed a few paragraphs ago. If we are to protect an individual from Each of the phenomena described above illustrates at least three
relapse via rapid reacquisition, it is best to try to associate the points. First, extinction is not the same as erasure; the tendency to
renewing cues—CS–reinforcer or action–reinforcer pairings—with respond can readily return. Second, each phenomenon suggests a
extinction. One can do this by inserting occasional CS–reinforcer or possible mechanism or lapse and/or relapse. And third, the overall
action–reinforcer pairings into an extinction procedure. In Pavlovian message is that extinguished responding is highly sensitive to
experiments [66], rats received extensive extinction in which the CS manipulations of the context. In this regard, it is worth noting that
was paired with the reinforcer very infrequently, in the midst of many our discussion of “context” has assumed that the concept is very
extinction trials. The idea was to associate the reinforced trials with broad. Although many experiments in the animal learning literature
ensuing extinction trials. Consistent with our hypothesis, the define context as the box or immediate environment in which
treatment slowed down the reacquisition that occurred when CS learning and remembering take place, I have discussed the role of a
and US were again paired relative to a group that had received simple number of possible background stimuli as part of the context,
extinction. The new pairings were a feature of extinction. Fig. 4 shows including time (in explaining spontaneous recovery), other behaviors
the results of a test phase in an analogous experiment done in operant and their reinforcement (in explaining resurgence), presentations of
conditioning [67]. Here, after initial reinforcement of lever pressing the reinforcer or context–reinforcer associations (in explaining
reinstatement), and CS–reinforcer and action–reinforcer pairings (in
explaining rapid reacquisition). Other research indicates that emo-
tions and drug states can provide contexts too (e.g., see [23]). And it is
noteworthy that Davidson [68,69] has argued that the state of food
deprivation can also provide a contextual stimulus; this may help
explain how hunger can modulate behavior (see also [16]). In our
view, it is advisable to think of “context” as any of a large number of
background stimuli.
It is also appropriate to think more broadly about the inhibitory
processes that are engaged by extinction. Is the context important at
influencing all forms of inhibitory learning? The answer to this
question turns out to be complex. Learning theorists often study
inhibition by means of so-called feature-negative discriminations in
which one CS (X) is paired with a reinforcer when it is presented alone
(X+), but without the reinforcer when it is combined with a second
CS (Y) (XY−). In the world of eating and appetite, a child might learn
that the sight of a bright blue box of sugary cereal is associated with
something wonderful (X+), but not if a disapproving adult is also
Fig. 4. Reacquisition of an operant lever-pressing response following eight 60-min present (XY−). The box will therefore generate more excitement
sessions of either simple extinction (EXT) or extinction with occasional reinforced trials
when the disapproving adult is not around. Learning theories
(ns = 8) (lever pressing had originally been reinforced on a VI 30 schedule during five
30-min acquisition sessions). Responding is shown as the percent of the response rate attribute the behavioral pattern to the development of conditioned
achieved during the last acquisition session. Baseline behavior immediately before the inhibition to stimulus Y (the adult) (X might also develop a little
beginning of reacquisition is shown at left (“before”). The remaining data are response inhibition of its own after it first acquires an association with food).
rates during successive 4-min periods after each new response–reinforcer pairing. Interestingly, laboratory experiments on feature-positive discrimina-
Notice that the rats given simple extinction responded substantially more after each
new pairing than the rats given extinction with occasional reinforced trials (ps b 0.05).
tions suggest that inhibition to Y is not specific to the original context;
Adapted from Woods and Bouton [67], Experiment 2, with permission from the inhibition to Y transfers without loss when tested in a new context
publisher (El). [70,71]. But surprisingly, in the new context, responding to X becomes
M.E. Bouton / Physiology & Behavior 103 (2011) 51–58 57

more difficult to inhibit. Thus, the bright blue box may stimulate more motivation, and cognition: the functional behaviorism of Robert C. Bolles.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1997. p. 99–115.
excitement and eating at the vacation cottage than at home, even in [8] Kessler DA. The end of overeating: taking control of the insatiable American
the presence of the disapproving adult. appetite. New York: Rodale; 2009.
A more general implication of this digression is that inhibition per [9] Hearst E, Jenkins HM. Sign-tracking: the stimulus-reinforcer relation and directed
action. Austin, TX: The Psychonomic Society; 1974.
se is not context-specific. It turns out that the second thing learned [10] Bouton ME. Learning and behavior: a contemporary synthesis. Sunderland, MA:
about a stimulus is often more context-specific than the first thing Sinauer Associates, Inc.; 2007.
learned [72]—as if the memory system treats the second thing learned [11] Birch LL. Obesity and eating disorders: a developmental perspective. Bull Psychon
Soc 1991;29:265–72.
about a stimulus as a conditional, context-specific exception to the rule [12] Holland PC, Petrovich GD, Gallagher M. The effects of amygdale lesions on
[23]. Extinction is thus relatively context-specific because it is the conditioned stimulus-potentiated eating in rats. Physiol Behav 2002;76:117–29.
second thing the organism learns about the CS or the operant action. In [13] Petrovich GD, Ross CA, Gallagher M, Holland PC. Learned contextual cue
potentiates eating in rats. Physiol Behav 2007;90:363–7.
counterconditioning, where a CS is first associated with one reinforcer
[14] Weingarten HP. Conditioned cues elicit feeding in sated rats: a role for learning in
and then a second reinforcer in a subsequent phase, the principle is the meal initiation. Science 1983;220:431–3.
same: the CS's association with the second reinforcer is more context- [15] Weingarten HP. Meal initiation controlled by learned cues: basic behavioral
specific than the association with the first [73]. It is thus retroactive properties. Appetite 1984;6:387–401.
[16] Balleine BW. Neural bases of food-seeking: affect, arousal and reward in
inhibition (or retroactive interference), not conditioned inhibition, that corticostriatolimbic circuits. Physiol Behav 2005;86:717–30.
is specific to its context. What I have been claiming for extinction could [17] Rescorla RA, Solomon RL. Two-process learning theory: relationships between
apply to any situation in which new learning replaces old learning. Pavlovian conditioning and instrumental learning. Psychol Rev 1967;74:151–82.
[18] Berridge KC. ‘Liking’ and ‘wanting’ food rewards: brain substrates and roles in
As I stated earlier, the causes of eating and overeating are numerous eating disorders. Physiol Behav 2009;97:537–50.
and enormously complex. Yet, a learning analysis of conditioning and [19] Berridge KC, Robinson TE, Aldridge JW. Dissecting components of reward: ‘liking’,
extinction may provide a useful perspective on them. Although ‘wanting’, and learning. Curr Opin Pharmacol 2009;9:65–73.
[20] Holland PC. Occasion setting in Pavlovian conditioning. In: Medin DL, editor. The
conditioning mechanisms have almost certainly evolved to allow psychology of learning and motivation, Vol. 28. San Diego, CA: Academic Press;
organisms to cope effectively with biologically-significant events (e.g., 1992. p. 69–125.
[2,74,75]) the bets are off in the modern world, given the hyperpalat- [21] Bouton ME. Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Learn Mem 2004;11:
485–94.
ability of modern food, its abundance, and the ease with which it can [22] Rescorla RA. Experimental extinction. In: Mowrer RR, Klein SB, editors. Handbook
be eaten and ingested [8]. Given the number of palatable foods out of contemporary learning theories. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2001. p. 119–54.
there, and the known behavioral effects of Pavlovian and operant [23] Bouton ME. Context, ambiguity, and unlearning: sources of relapse after
behavioral extinction. Biol Psychiatry 2002;52:976–86.
learning processes, it is not surprising that we eat, and are tempted to
[24] Bouton ME, Woods AM. Extinction: behavioral mechanisms and their implica-
eat, in the presence of so many CSs and contexts. tions. In: Byrne JH, Sweatt D, Menzel R, Eichenbaum H, Roediger H, editors.
The idea developed here is that extinction, whether it is Learning and memory: a comprehensive reference (vol. 1, learning theory and
deliberately produced through weight loss regimes or more naturally behaviour). Oxford: Elsevier; 2008. p. 151–71.
[25] Bouton ME, Bolles RC. Contextual control of the extinction of conditioned fear.
by naturally-occurring extinction trials, does not permanently undo Learn Motiv 1979;10:445–66.
the effects of conditioning. At the most general level, we should not [26] Bouton ME, King DA. Contextual control of the extinction of conditioned fear: tests
suppose that it erases, even partially, the learning allowed by our for the associative value of the context. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 1983;9:
248–65.
food-saturated world. Instead, in both Pavlovian and instrumental [27] Bouton ME. Context and ambiguity in the extinction of emotional learning:
situations, extinction is best thought of as new learning that is at least implications for exposure therapy. Behav Res Ther 1988;26:137–49.
partly context-dependent. There seems to be a bias or imbalance in [28] Bouton ME, Peck CA. Context effects on conditioning, extinction, and reinstatement
in an appetitive conditioning preparation. Anim Learn Behav 1989;17:188–98.
basic learning processes such that conditioning tends to generalize [29] Bouton ME, Ricker ST. Renewal of extinguished responding in a second context.
across contexts, whereas extinction generalizes less completely. Anim Learn Behav 1994;22:317–24.
Renewal, spontaneous recovery, resurgence, reinstatement, and [30] Brooks DC, Bouton ME. A retrieval cue for extinction attenuates response recovery
(renewal) caused by a return to the conditioning context. J Exp Psychol Anim
rapid reconditioning all suggest that lapse and relapse are under- Behav Process 1994;20:366–79.
standable (and perhaps inevitable) after extinction. And they may [31] Wilfley DE, Van Buren DJ, Theim KR, Stein RI, Saelens BE, Ezzet F, et al. The use of
contribute to why behavior, once learned, is so persistent. This point biosimulation in the design of a novel multilevel weight loss maintenance
program for overweight children. Obesity 2010;18:S91–8.
has been made before concerning anxiety disorders and drug
[32] Bossert JM, Liu SY, Lu L, Shaham Y. A role of ventral tegmental area glutamate in
dependence. The arguments may apply equally to our understanding contextual cue-induced relapse to heroin seeking. J Neurosci 2004;24:10726–30.
of eating, appetite, and their apparent potency today. [33] Hamlin AS, Clemens KJ, McNally GP. Renewal of extinguished cocaine-seeking.
Neuroscience 2008;151:659–70.
[34] Crombag HS, Shaham Y. Renewal of drug seeking by contextual cues after
Acknowledgments prolonged extinction in rats. Behav Neurosci 2002;116:169–73.
[35] Hamlin AS, Newby J, McNally GP. The neural correlates and role of D1 dopamine
Supported by Grant R01 MH064847 from the National Institute of receptors in renewal of extinguished alcohol-seeking. Neuroscience 2007;146:
525–36.
Mental Health. I thank Leonard Epstein, Lilya Sitnikov, Travis Todd, [36] Zironi I, Burattini C, Aicardi G, Janak PH. Context is a trigger for relapse to alcohol.
Drina Vurbic, and Neil Winterbauer for their discussion and Behav Brain Res 2006;167:150–5.
comments, and Stephen C. Woods for his friendship and inspiration. [37] Nakajima S, Tanaka S, Urushihara K, Imada H. Renewal of extinguished lever-press
responses upon return to the training context. Learn Motiv 2000;31:416–31.
[38] Nakajima S, Urushihara K, Masaki T. Renewal of operant performance formerly
eliminated by omission or noncontingency training upon return to the acquisition
References context. Learn Motiv 2002;33:510–25.
[39] Bouton ME, Todd TP, Vurbic D, Winterbauer NE. Renewal after the extinction of
[1] Woods SC. The control of food intake: behavioral versus molecular perspectives. free operant behavior. Learn Behav 2011;39:57–67.
Cell Metab 2009;9:489–98. [40] Baker AG, Steinwald H, Bouton ME. Contextual conditioning and reinstatement of
[2] Woods SC. The eating paradox: how we tolerate food. Psychol Rev 1991;98: extinguished instrumental responding. Q J Exp Psychol 1991;43B:199–218.
488–505. [41] Rescorla RA. Extinction of facilitation. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 1986;12:
[3] Strubbe JH, Woods SC. The timing of meals. Psychol Rev 2004;111:128–41. 16–24.
[4] Woods SC, Strubbe JH. The psychobiology of meals. Psychon Bull Rev 1994;1: [42] Conklin CA, Tiffany ST. Applying extinction research and theory to cue-exposure
141–55. addiction treatments. Addiction 2002;97:155–67.
[5] Capaldi ED, editor. Why we eat what we eat. Washington, DC: American [43] Pavlov IP. Conditioned reflexes. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 1927.
Psychological Association; 1996. [44] Rescorla RA. Spontaneous recovery. Learn Mem 2004;11:501–9.
[6] Holland PC, Petrovich GD. A neural systems analysis of the potentiation of feeding [45] Robbins SJ. Mechanisms underlying spontaneous recovery in autoshaping. J Exp
by conditioned stimuli. Physiol Behav 2005;86:747–61. Psychol Anim Behav Process 1990;16:235–49.
[7] Seeley RJ, Ramsay DS, Woods SC. Regulation of food intake: interactions between [46] Bouton ME, Peck CA. Spontaneous recovery in cross-motivational transfer
learning and physiology. In: Bouton ME, Fanselow MS, editors. Learning, (counterconditioning). Anim Learn Behav 1992;20:313–21.
58 M.E. Bouton / Physiology & Behavior 103 (2011) 51–58

[47] Bouton ME, García-Gutiérrez A. Intertrial interval as a contextual stimulus. Behav [63] Wise RA, Wang B, You Z-B. Cocaine serves as a peripheral interoceptive
Process 2006;71:307–17. conditioned stimulus for central glutamate and dopamine release. PLoS ONE
[48] Bouton ME, Hendrix MC. Intertrial interval as a contextual stimulus: further 2008;3:1–9.
analysis of a novel asymmetry in temporal discrimination learning. J Exp Psychol [64] Bouton ME, Bolles RC. Role of conditioned contextual stimuli in reinstatement of
Anim Behav Processes 2011;37:79–93. extinguished fear. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 1979;5:368–78.
[49] Todd TP, Winterbauer NE, Bouton ME. Interstimulus interval as a discriminative [65] Ricker ST, Bouton ME. Reacquisition following extinction in appetitive condition-
cue: evidence of the generality of a novel asymmetry in temporal discrimination ing. Anim Learn Behav 1996;24:423–36.
learning. Behav Process 2010;84:412–20. [66] Bouton ME, Woods AM, Pineño O. Occasional reinforced trials during extinction
[50] Brooks DC, Bouton ME. A retrieval cue for extinction attenuates spontaneous can slow the rate of rapid reacquisition. Learn Motiv 2004;35:371–90.
recovery. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 1993;19:77–89. [67] Woods AM, Bouton ME. Occasional reinforced responses during extinction can
[51] Leitenberg H, Rawson RA, Bath K. Reinforcement of competing behavior during slow the rate of reacquisition of an operant response. Learn Motiv 2007;38:56–74.
extinction. Science 1970;169:301–3. [68] Davidson TL. The nature and function of interoceptive signals to feed: toward
[52] Lieving GA, Lattal KA. Recency, repeatability, and reinforcer retrenchment: an integration of physiological and learning perspectives. Psychol Rev 1993;100:
experimental analysis of resurgence. J Exp Anal Behav 2003;80:217–33. 640–57.
[53] Winterbauer NE, Bouton ME. Mechanisms of resurgence of an extinguished [69] Davidson TL. Hunger cues as modulatory stimuli. In: Schmajuk NA, Holland PC,
instrumental behavior. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 2010;36:343–53. editors. Occasion setting: associative learning and cognition in animals.
[54] Winterbauer NE, Bouton ME. Mechanisms of resurgence II: response-contingent Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1998. p. 223–48.
reinforcers can reinstate a second extinguished behavior. Submitted for [70] Bouton ME, Nelson JB. Context-specificity of target versus feature inhibition in a
publication. Learn Motiv. feature-negative discrimination. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 1994;20:
[55] Podlesnik CA, Jimenez-Gomez C, Shahan TA. Resurgence of alcohol seeking 51–65.
produced by discontinuing non-drug reinforcement as an animal model of drug [71] Nelson JB, Bouton ME. The effects of a context switch following serial and
relapse. Behav Pharmacol 2006;17:369–74. simultaneous feature-negative discriminations. Learn Motiv 1997;28:56–84.
[56] Volpp KG, John LK, Troxel AB, Norton L, Fassbender J, Loewenstein G. Financial [72] Nelson JB. Context specificity of excitation and inhibition in ambiguous stimuli.
incentive-based approaches for weight loss: a randomized trial. J Am Med Assoc Learn Motiv 2002;33:284–310.
2008;300:2631–7. [73] Peck CA, Bouton ME. Context and performance in aversive-to-appetitive and
[57] Rawson RA, Leitenberg H, Mulick JA, Lefebvre MF. Recovery of extinction appetitive-to-aversive transfer. Learn Motiv 1990;21:1–31.
responding in rats following discontinuation of reinforcement of alternative [74] Hollis KL. Pavlovian conditioning of signal-centered action patterns and
behavior: a test of two explanations. Anim Learn Behav 1977;5:415–20. autonomic behavior: a biological analysis of function. In: Rosenblatt RA, Hinde
[58] Bouton ME, Swartzentruber D. Sources of relapse after extinction in Pavlovian and RA, Beer C, Busnel MC, editors. Advances in the study of behavior, Vol. 12. New
instrumental learning. Clin Psychol Rev 1991;11:123–40. York: Academic Press; 1982. p. 1–64.
[59] Bouton ME. Differential control by context in the inflation and reinstatement [75] Hollis KL. Contemporary research on Pavlovian conditioning: a “new” functional
paradigms. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 1984;10:56–74. analysis. Am Psychol 1997;52:956–65.
[60] Rescorla RA, Heth CD. Reinstatement of fear to an extinguished conditioned [76] Welker RL, McAuley K. Reductions in resistance to extinction and spontaneous
stimulus. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 1975;1:88–96. recovery as a function of changes in transportational and contextual stimuli. Anim
[61] Reid RL. The role of the reinforcer as a stimulus. Br J Psychol 1958;49:202–9. Learn Behav 1978;6:451–7.
[62] Rescorla RA, Skucy JC. Effect of response-independent reinforcers during
extinction. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1969;67:381–9.

You might also like