You are on page 1of 8

Journal of Experimental Psychology

Vol. 59, No. 4, 1960

EFFECT OF DISCRIMINATION TRAINING ON


AUDITORY GENERALIZATION
HERBERT M. JENKINS
Bell Telephone Laboratories, Incorporated

AND ROBERT H. HARRISON


Massachusetts Mental Health Center

Two types of training procedures with these two procedures (Hovland,


have been used in studies of stimulus 1937a; Schlosberg & Solomon, 1943);
generalization. In the nondifferen- it was found that differential training
tial procedure a response is simply produced a steeper gradient. Al-
reinforced repeatedly in the presence though the differential training in-
of, or closely following the presenta- volved nonreinforcement with respect
tion of, a stimulus of a particular to a stimulus that differed from the
value. In the differential procedure, reinforced stimulus along the dimen-
on the other hand, a discrimination sion on which the gradient was ob-
is developed between two stimulus tained, it is not clear that the effect
values by reinforcing the response depends entirely on this feature.
to one value and extinguishing the The purpose of the present experi-
response when it occurs to the other ments was to examine the possibility
value. that differential training not involving
Lashley and Wade (1946) argued an explicit discrimination of this kind
that differential training was required also produces a steeper gradient.
for the development of a gradient of The method was similar to the one
generalization. The several experi- used by Guttman and Kalish (1956).
ments which faced this issue (Gran- The key-response of the pigeon was
dine & Harlow, 1948; Grant & reinforced in the presence of a pure
Schiller, 1953; Grice, 1948) have tone. Generalization gradients along
demonstrated, however, that a sloping the dimension of frequency were
gradient of generalization can be compared following differential or
obtained following a nondifferential nondifferential training. Differential
training procedure. training was used to produce a dis-
In experiments comparing gradients crimination between the presence of
of generalization following training tone and its absence; it did not in-
1 volve explicit training to discriminate
This research was done at the Massa-
chusetts Institute of Technology and was frequencies. The expectation that
supported by the United States Air Force training of this form sharpens the
through the Air Force Office of Scientific gradient of generalization was based
Research of the Air Research and Develop- on the following reasoning. The es-
ment Command under Contract AF 18 tablishment of a discrimination be-
(603)-85. The present article is a condensed
version of the final report on that contract; tween presence and absence ensures
TN 58-443, ASTIA Document 158248. Re- that the tone has discriminative
production for any purpose of the United control over the response. This pro-
States Government is permitted. vides the potential for an increase
The authors wish to express their apprecia-
tion to Carl Hovland for his helpful criticisms in discriminative control by any of
of a draft of this article. the several properties of the tonal
246
EFFECT OF DISCRIMINATION TRAINING ON GENERALIZATION 247

stimulus; properties which might be first session to 125 in later sessions. The
expected to include frequency as well SD and SA periods were presented in a
random order. Daily training sessions were
as intensity, rate of interruption, and continuedD until the average rate of response
so on. in the S periodsAwas at least four times
greater than in S periods in each of two
EXPERIMENT I successive sessions. When testing the dis-
crimination against this criterion, the sessions
Method consisted of 25 SD periods and 50 SA periods.
Subjects. The 5s were eight male, white Five 5s received differential training. From
Carneau pigeons maintained by restricted six to nine sessions were required to reach
feeding at 80% of their body weight under the criterion of discrimination.
free feeding. In testing for generalization, the stimuli
Apparatus. A Skinner automatic key- were presented for 33-sec. periods separated
pecking apparatus was used. The general by 7-sec. blackouts as in training. Eight test
features of the apparatus have been described stimuli were used. They consisted of seven
elsewhere (Ferster & Skinner, 1957). The tones approximately equally spaced along a
reinforcement was a 4-sec. period of access logarithmic scale of frequency. Their fre-
to a tray of mixed grain. The occurrence of quencies were 300 cps, 450 cps, 670 cps, 1500
a reinforcement was signaled by turning off cps, 2250 cps, 3500 cps, and 1000 cps (SD in
the illumination of the response-key, and by training). The eighth stimulus was no tone.
turning on illumination for the food tray. In a test session, the eight stimuli were pre-
The auditory stimuli were pure tones sented in eight blocks, making a total of 64
sounded by a 5-in. speaker suspended 3 in. presentations. The order of presentation
behind the response-key. The tone was was governed by 8 X 8 latin squares so that
interrupted for 0.25 sec. in each second in each stimulus appeared once in every ordinal
order to reduce possible adaptation to a position in the block.
steady tone. The amplification of tones at Tests for generalization were conducted
different frequencies was adjusted to give without reinforcement except where noted.
a sound pressure level of 70 decibels re 0.0002 Three tests for generalization separated by
dynes/cm.2 as measured with the aid of a single retraining sessions were usually made.
calibrated microphone. The procedure during retraining was the
Preliminary training. Four sessions of same as that used during training.
preliminary training were given. The key-
response was conditioned using a method of Results
successive approximations and then placed
on a variable interval (VI) schedule of rein- Nondifferential training. The per-
forcement. The tone which served as the centage of responses made to each
reinforced stimulus, or SD, was presented as test stimulus following nondifferential
soon as several responses occurred. The
third and fourth sessions were divided into training is shown in Fig. 1. Prior to
25 working periods of 33-sec. duration by the the generalization tests, S?o received
introduction of 7-sec. blackout intervals. In 10 training sessions, 5?i received 20,
each working period, the SD was presented
and responses were reinforced on the VI
schedule which delivered from 1 to 3 reinforce- o NO. 70
ments with an average of 1.7 reinforcements. • NO. 71
Procedure. In the nondifferential training V NO. 72
method, sessions consisted of 25 working
periods in which a tone at 1000 cps (SD) was
presented and responses were reinforced on
the VI schedule. Three 5s received 10, 20,
or 40 daily sessions of nondifferential training. 300 450 670 IOOO I50O 2250 350O NO
In the differential training sessions, the FREQUENCY IN CYCLES PER SECOND TONE

working periods were of two types: SAD periods


as in nondifferential training, and S periods FIG. 1. Generalization gradients follow-
in which no tone was presented and responses ing nondifferential training with a 1000-cps
were nonreinforced. Training sessions con- tone as SD. Individual gradients are based
sisted of 25 SD periods, and a number of SA on the means of several generalization tests
periods which was increased from 25 in the (see text).
248 HERBERT M. JENKINS AND ROBERT H. HARRISON

evidence for a shallow gradient follow-


ing nondifferential training. Large
and highly significant main effects
were found for tests (F = 229.54;
1/273 df, P < .001) and for blocks
within tests (F = 36.36; 7/273 df,
P < .001). The effect due to tests
reflects a sharp decline in the number
of responses from the first to the
second test. This was expected on
the basis of the well established
300 450 670 1000 1500 2250 3500 NO
FREQUENCY IN CYCLES PER SECOND TONE finding that extinction occurs more
rapidly in successive extinctions fol-
FIG. 2. Generalization gradients follow- lowing reconditioning (Bullock &
ing differential training with a 1000-cps tone
as SD; no tone as SA. Individual gradients Smith, 1953). The effect of blocks
are based on the means of three generalization reflects a decline in the rate of response
tests. due to nonreinforcement during the
tests of generalization.
and .$72 received 40. Gradients are Differential training. Generaliza-
based on three tests on 870 and 5?i tion gradients following differential
and on two tests for S?2. Tones training are shown in Fig. 2. During
widely separated in frequency from the test sessions, Ss, Sw, and Su
the SD, as well as no tone, were received Dfour additional presentations
responded to in approximately equal of the S in which one reinforcement
measure. Increasing the amount of was delivered. These presentations
nondifferential training from 10 to 40 were excluded from the data. Dif-
sessions did not have a marked effect ferential training resulted in well
upon the gradient. defined gradients along the dimension
There was some evidence for a very of frequency. The percentage of
shallow gradient of response strength. responses to extreme frequencies (300
An average of the individual curves cps or 3500 cps) was only slightly
would show the highest percentage greater than to no tone. The pos-
of response at the SD with a slight but sibility that tones at these frequencies
systematic decrease at tones of higher were inaudible, can be ruled out on
or lower frequency. the basis of data to be presented from
A four-way analysis of variance Exp. II.
was performed on the number of The analysis of variance for the dif-
responses in 33-sec. test periods. The ferential group was based on S® and
variables were 5s confounded with S6o which received no reinforcement
amount of training, stimuli (eight during the tests for generalization.
values including no tone), tests (first Three tests for generalization were in-
and second generalization tests), and cluded in this and subsequent analy-
blocks within tests (eight blocks of ses. As is apparent from Fig. 2, the
eight stimulus presentations). The effect of stimuli was large (F = 47.74;
main effect of stimuli was significant 7/273 df, P < .001). The effects of
at the .05 level (F = 2.38; 7/273 df). blocks within tests (F = 8.68; 7/273
Since the simple interactions involving df, P < .001) and of tests (F = 14.05;
stimuli were not significant, the small 2/273 df, P < .001) again reflect,
main effect of stimuli provides some respectively, extinction within tests,
EFFECT OF DISCRIMINATION TRAINING ON GENERALIZATION 249

and more rapid extinction in succes- and no tone. Generalization tests were
sive tests. Significant interactions conducted without reinforcement. Three
tests were made with a single retraining
were obtained for the effects of stimuli session between tests.
with blocks (F = 1.80; 49/273 df,
P < .01), and of stimuli with tests Results
(F = 2.28; 14/273 df, P < .01). The
interaction effects were due in part Nondifferential training. The gra-
to a progressive sharpening of the dients obtained following nondiffer-
generalization gradient both from the tial training are shown in Fig. 3.
beginning to the end of a test, and in Prior to the generalization tests, S&
successive tests. Similar trends are received 10 training sessions, Sao re-
examined in more detail with the ceived 20, and S38 received 40. The
results of Exp. II. gradients are similar to those obtained
following nondifferential training with
EXPERIMENT II respect to a single frequency in that
they show a nearly equal percentage
Experiment II was designed to of response to all test stimuli, includ-
extend the comparison of generaliza- ing no tone. Again, it is apparent
tion gradients obtained following dif- that increasing the amount of training
ferential or nondifferential training did not greatly alter the result. As
to a case in which responses are rein- in Exp. I, there was some evidence
forced to tones at two frequencies for a shallow gradient; each 5 had a
(cf. Kalish & Guttman, 1957). local peak in response strength at 450
cps, although the curves over all test
Method stimuli were irregular.
The same method was used as in Exp. I
An analysis of variance for this
except for the details noted below.. condition shows effects similar to
Procedure. Nondifferential training ses- those for the nondifferential condi-
sions consisted of 30 SD periods. A 450-cps tion in Exp. I. The effect of stimuli
tone was presented in IS of these periods; a was again significant (F = 3.23; 7/448
2500-cps tone was presented in the remaining
15 periods. The tones were presented in a df, P < .01), and the simple inter-
random order. Three 5s received 10, 20, or actions of stimuli with blocks or tests
40 daily sessions of nondifferential training. were not significant.
The differential training procedure in- Differential training. Generaliza-
volved the same SD periods, and in addition, tion gradients following differential
A
S periods in which no tone was presented
and responses were nonreinforced. The training are shown in Fig. 4. Differ-
number of SA periods was increased from 30
in the early training sessions to ISO in later 30
sessions. This procedure does not involve o NO. 29
• NO. 30
explicit training to discriminate between the V NO. 36
two tones used as an SD.
Daily sessions were continued until the
criterion of discrimination was reached (4 to i 10
1 ratio of SD to SA rate of response) for each
tone with respect to no tone. Six 5s received
"• 250 450 670 1000 1500 250O 4000" NO
differential training. From 7 to 11 training FREQUENCY IN CYCLES PER SECOND TONE
sessions were required to reach the criterion
of discrimination. FIG. 3. Generalization gradients follow-
The tests for generalization were made ing noiidifferential training with a 450-cps
with the following stimuli: tones at fre- tone and a 2500-cps tone as SDs. Individual
quencies of 250 cps, 450 cps (SD), 670 cps, gradients are based on the mea.ns of three
1000 cps, 1500 cps, 2500 cps (SD), 4000 cps, generalization tests.
250 HERBERT M. JENKINS AND ROBERT H. HARRISON

present experiment, response strength


at 1000 cps was only slightly greater
than that for no tone; a stimulus
which approximated silence. Al-
though these observations do not
eliminate the possibility that differ-
ences in loudness affected the gradients
obtained, they make it appear un-
likely that such differences were a
major source of variance.
250 450 670 1000 1500 2500 4000" NO An analysis of variance shows
FREQUENCY IN CYCLES PER SECOND TONE effects that parallel those obtained
FIG. 4. Generalization gradients follow- with differential reinforcement in Exp.
ing differential training with a 450-cps tone I with some exceptions. In particu-
and a 2500-cps tone as SDs; no tone as SA. lar, the interaction of 5s with stimuli
Individual gradients are based on the means was highly significant (F = 5.44;
of three generalization tests.

ential training resulted in much


sharper gradients with peaks in re-
sponse strength occurring at the
frequencies reinforced in training.
The effect of differential training in
sharpening the gradient is consistent
with the effect obtained in Exp. I.
The gradients with peaks near the
extremes of the frequency range
permit us to reject an interpretation 25O 450 67O 1000 1500 2500 4000 "NO
of the low response strength obtained FREQUENCY IN CYCLES PER SECOND TONE
in Exp. I at the extreme frequencies FIG. 6. Average generalization gradients for
of 300 cps and 3500 cps (see Fig. 2) successive blocks within tests; Exp. II.
as resulting from the inaudibility of
these frequencies to the pigeon. It is 35/973 df, P < .001), whereas it was
also of interest to note that, in the not significant in Exp. I. The major
source of this interaction was in the
so
» « BLOCKS 1-2
difference among the 5s in the SD
o——o BLOCKS 3-4 (450 cps or 2500 cps) at which they
40 57 T T BLOCKS 5-6
- V——-V BLOCKS 7-8
responded most frequently in the
generalization tests. A change in the
distribution of responses to the test
stimuli both in successive blocks
within tests and in successive tests
was reflected in significant interac-
tions of stimuli with blocks (F = 2.07;
49/973 df, P < .01) and of stimuli
250 450 670 1000 1500 2500 4000 NO with tests (F = 4.01; 14/973 df,
FREQUENCY IN CYCLES PER SECOND TONE P < .001). These effects were also
FIG. 5. Average generalization gradients for obtained in the differential group in
successive tests; Exp. II. Exp. I, and in both cases, represent
EFFECT OF DISCRIMINATION TRAINING ON GENERALIZATION 251

a general sharpening of the gradient ential, these stimuli are repeatedly rein-
from the first to later tests, and within forced and may predominate in control
the course of a single test. Figure 5 of the response with the result that the
shows the mean percentage of re- gradient of generalization observed upon
sponses to the test stimuli in succes- varying the experimental stimulus is flat
or nearly so. When, on the other hand,
sive tests. Figure 6 plots these data training is differential, these incidental
for successive blocks within the tests. stimuli are both reinforced in the pres-
ence of SD and nonreinforced in its
DISCUSSION absence. This reduces their effective-
ness in controlling the response. Hull
The experiments provide clear evi- assumed, as does Restle (1955), that
dence that differential training can stimuli accompanying reinforced and
greatly sharpen a gradient of generaliza- nonreinforced responses adapt out, or
tion even when this training does not in some manner become nonfunctional.
involve an explicit discrimination be- The resulting increase in the relative
tween stimulus values along the dimen- weight of the experimental stimulus in
sion of the gradient. The conclusion controlling the response makes it possible
stated by Lashley and Wade (1946, to obtain a sharply sloping gradient of
p. 74): "the 'dimensions' of a stimulus generalization along some dimension of
series are determined by a comparison the experimental stimulus. It is of
of two or more stimuli and do not exist interest to note that, according to this
for the organism until established by account, differential training involving
differential training" is weakened by neighboring stimulus values would also
this result if the phrase "two or more weaken the control by incidental stimuli.
stimuli" is taken to mean stimuli having The effect of that procedure in sharpen-
values on the dimension of the gradient. ing the gradient might be largely due
It now seems likely that the absence to this factor rather than due to the
or near absence of a sloping gradient presumed development of a counter
along some dimension of a stimulus does gradient of conditioned inhibition cen-
not imply that training to discriminate tered around the value of the nonrein-
values on this dimension is required. forced stimulus. In an experiment by
The present results suggest that the. Reinhold and Perkins (1955) it was
failure to obtain such a gradient may be found that differential training on a
due to a lack of control over the occur- feature of a compound stimulus unre-
rence of the response by the experimental lated to the feature used in testing for
stimulus. When presenting or removing generalization produced a gradient with
the stimulus has little or no effect upon a steeper slope than did nondifferential
the occurrence of the response, we can training. They suggested that a set to
only conclude that the stimulus, and discriminate was developed by differen-
consequently any dimension of the tial training and that this resulted in less
stimulus, is irrelevant to the response. generalization. However, their results
The results pose a theoretical question: could also be accounted for by the reduc-
how account for the effectiveness of non- tion in effectiveness of incidental stimuli
reinforcement in the absence of the tone through differential training.
in- placing the tone in control of the A weakness in the account offered
response? A discussion of stimulus gen- by Hull and Restle lies in the specifica-
eralization by Hull (1952, p. 59 ff) is tion of the conditions for adaptation
relevant here. He pointed out that in of incidental stimuli. It is not sufficient
addition to the experimental stimulus, to state that a stimulus accompanying
incidental stimuli (e.g., stimuli arising both the reinforcement and nonreinforce-
from constant features of the apparatus) ment of a response adapts out, since
accompany the reinforcement of the in the present experiment responses to
response. When training is nondiffer- the SD were reinforced only inter-
252 HERBERT M. JENKINS AND ROBERT H. HARRISON

mittently, yet with differential training logarithmic scale of frequency. The


the SD showed rather precise discrimina- approximate symmetry of the auditory
tive control over the response. gradients when plotted in this way may
Regardless of the interpretation placed be related to the fact that human
upon the present results, it is clear that discriminability of frequency changes is
a property of a stimulus that shows proportional to the logarithm of fre-
little or no influence on a response after quency for tones above 1000 cps.
prolonged nondifferential training may The present results, showing that the
gain rather precise discriminative con- generalization gradients became sharper
trol by means of differential training in the course of testing, are at variance
to discriminate the presence from the with those reported by Kalish and Gutt-
absence of the stimulus. man (1957), who found no systematic
The auditory generalization gradients change in the relative frequencies of
may be compared with the visual gen- response to test stimuli in the course
eralization gradients along the dimension of extinction. Some investigators have
of wavelength obtained by Guttman found sharpening (Brown, 1942; Hov-
and Kalish (1956) using pigeons. In land, 1937b; Littman, 1949), as in the
contrast with the present results, these present case, while others have found
investigators found steep, well-defined that gradients flatten during extinction
gradients following nondifferential train- (Grant & Schiller, 1953; Reinhold &
ing. Since the training procedures ap- Perkins, 1955). The question of what
pear to be the same in all important determines how extinction affects a
respects it may be concluded that the gradient of generalization is still open.
difference lies in the use of a visual—
as compared with an auditory stimulus. SUMMARY
It does not appear that a lack of physio-
logical capacity to detect or to make An operant conditioning method was used
discriminations among auditory stimuli to obtain auditory generalization gradients
can account for the contrast in the along the frequency dimension of pure tones
results obtained with visual and with for the pigeon. Two procedures for training
prior to testing for generalization in extinc-
auditory stimuli. The more important tion were compared. In the differential
role of visual stimuli in the normal procedure responses were reinforced in the
activities of pigeons may be a factor. presence of a tone and nonreinforced in the
In locating food or in flying, visual dis- absence of a tone so that an explicit dis-
criminations are more heavily involved. crimination between presence and absence
It should be noted, however, that the was formed. In the nondifferential pro-
visual stimulus appeared directly on the cedure responses were simply reinforced in
response key, whereas the auditory the presence of a tone.
stimulus was diffuse and probably un- In Exp. I the 8° was a tone at 1000 cps.
In Exp. II equal reinforcement was given
localized. Nondifferential training with with respect to two SDs: a tone at 450 cps
a visual stimulus presented in an analo- and one at 2SOO cps. Test stimuli were tones
gous manner, as an over-all illuminant, higher and lower in frequency than the SD
might also result in a flat generalization (or SDs) and the absence of a tone.
gradient. Gradients of generalization following non-
Auditory generalization gradients fol- differential training were nearly flat; that is,
lowing differential training and visual tones of different frequency, as well as the
generalization gradients following non- absence of a tone, were responded to about
differential training are similar in some equally. On the other hand, well-defined
gradients of generalization with relatively
respects. In each case the typical steep slopes were obtained following differen-
gradient is negatively accelerated and tial training. When responses were rein-
approximately symmetrical. However, forced in the presence of two tones of different
the visual gradients were plotted on a frequency, the gradients showed two peaks
linear scale of wavelength, while the in the strength of response, one at each of the
auditory gradients were plotted on a frequencies reinforced in training. To a
EFFECT OF DISCRIMINATION TRAINING ON GENERALIZATION 253

first approximation, the gradients were sym- GUTTMAN, N., & KALISH, H. I. Discrimin-
metrical about the SD or SDs when plotted ability and stimulus generalization. /.
on a logarithmic scale of frequency. exp. Psychol, 1956, 51, 79-88.
It is concluded that training to dis- HOVLAND, C. I. The generalization of con-
criminate the presence from the absence of a ditioned responses: I. The sensory gen-
stimulus can greatly enhance the amount of eralization of conditioned responses with
control exerted over a response by a specific varying frequencies of tone. J. gen. Psy-
property of the reinforced stimulus. chol, 1937, 17, 125-148. (a)
HOVLAND, C. I. The generalization of con-
ditioned response. IV. The effects of
REFERENCES varying amounts of reinforcement upon
BROWN, J. S. Generalized approach and the degree of generalization of conditioned
avoidance responses as a function of responses. /. exp. Psychol, 1937, 21, 161-
stimulus intensity and strength of motiva- 176. (b)
tion. /. comp. Psychol, 1942, 33, 209- HULL, C. L, A behavior system. New Haven:
226. Yale Univer. Press, 1952.
BULLOCK, D. H., & SMITH, W. C. An effect KALISH, H. I., & GUTTMAN, N. Stimulus
of repeated conditioning-extinction upon generalization after equal training on two
operant strength. /. exp. Psychol., 1953, stimuli. /. exp. Psychol, 1957, 53,139-144.
46, 349-352. LASHLEY, K. S., & WADE, M. The Pav-
lovian theory of generalization. Psychol.
FERSTER, C. B., & SKINNER, B. F. Schedules Rev., 1946, 53, 72-87.
of reinforcement. New York: Appleton- LITTMAN, R. A. Conditioned generalization
Century-Crofts, 1957.
of the galvanic skin reaction to tones.
GRANDINE, L., & HARLOW, H. F. Generali- J. exp. Psychol, 1949, 39, 868-882.
zation of the characteristics of a single REINHOLD, D. B., & PERKINS, C. C., JR.
learned stimulus by monkeys. /. comp. Stimulus generalization following different
physiol. Psychol, 1948, 41, 327-338. methods of training. J. exp. Psychol,
GRANT, D. A., & SCHILLER, J. J. Generali- 1955, 49, 423-427.
zation of the conditioned galvanic skin RESTLE, F. A theory of discrimination
responses to visual stimuli. J. exp. Psy- learning. Psychol. Rev., 1955, 62, 11-19.
chol., 1953, 46, 309-313. SCHLOSBERG, H., & SOLOMON, R. L. Latency
GRICE, G. R. The acquisition of a visual of response in a choice discrimination.
discrimination habit following responses to /. exp. Psychol, 1943, 33, 22-39.
a single stimulus. /. exp. Psychol., 1948,
38, 633-642. (Received May 11, 1959)

You might also like