You are on page 1of 10

Acta Psychiatr Scand 2006: 113: 350–359 Copyright  2005 Blackwell Munksgaard

All rights reserved ACTA PSYCHIATRICA


DOI: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00626.x
SCANDINAVICA

Neuropsychological test profiles in


schizophrenia and non-psychotic depression
Rund BR, Sundet K, Asbjørnsen A, Egeland J, Landrø NI, Lund A, B. R. Rund1, K. Sundet1,
Roness A, Stordal KI, Hugdahl K. Neuropsychological test profiles in A. Asbjørnsen2, J. Egeland1,
schizophrenia and non-psychotic depression. N. I. Landrø1, A. Lund3, A. Roness3,
K. I. Stordal3, K. Hugdahl4
Objective: The study examined to what degree schizophrenia is 1
Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo and
characterized by a neuropsychological (NP) test profile specific in Departments of 2Psychosocial Sciences, 3Psychiatry and
shape and level compared with depression and normal functioning. 4
Biological and Medical Psychology, University of
Method: Fifty-three patients with schizophrenia, 45 with non- Bergen, Bergen, Norway
psychotic depression, and 50 normals were assessed with a
comprehensive NP test battery and clinical instruments. NP test scores
were factor analyzed into seven composite scores.
Results: Schizophrenia patients performed significantly below normals
across all seven composite scores, whereas depression patients were
impaired in two. Verbal memory was most impaired. Sixty-two percent
of schizophrenia patients were moderately or severely impaired, the
corresponding figure for depression was 28%. Impairment was Key words: schizophrenia; depression; cognition;
moderately associated with IQ level and clinical symptom load in neuropsychology; intelligence; symptoms
schizophrenia, but not in depression. Bjørn Rishovd Rund, Department of Psychology, PO Box
Conclusion: Schizophrenia is characterized by deficits across a wide 1094 Blindern, University of Oslo, N-0317 Oslo, Norway.
range of NP functions. Thirty-eight percent of the patients are within E-mail: b.r.rund@psykologi.uio.no
normal limits. A mild and limited NP disturbance is apparent in
depression. Accepted for publication July 19, 2005

Significant outcomes
• Patients with schizophrenia showed selective impairments in all four higher order neurocognitive
domains, while the depressed patients evidenced impairment only on measures of processing speed.
• Neurocognitive function constitutes a core trait in schizophrenia as impairments are not fully
explained by symptom load or level of intelligence.
• Subgroups within each diagnostic entity is suggested as nearly two of five patients with schizophrenia
and three of four patients with depression did not show significant neuropsychological (NP)
impairments.

Limitations
• We did not have an adequate control over medication effects.
• Possible different discriminating power of the various NP tasks represents a psychometric limitation.
• Exploratory data analysis may have inflated the chance of type I error.

deficits on a wide range of neuropsychological (NP)


Introduction
tasks compared with normal controls (1, 2). It is also
Studies of neurocognitive function in patients with a common finding that a substantial proportion of
schizophrenia have consistently reported pervasive patients perform within the normal range (3).

350
Neuropsychological test profiles

In order to draw firm conclusions regarding the on the question of whether or not neurocognitive
magnitude and specificity of cognitive dysfunction dysfunction is a universal feature of schizophre-
in schizophrenia, this group should be compared nia.
with other psychiatric control groups (4, 5). One The last aim of the study was to examine the
highly relevant comparison group is patients with relationship between NP impairment and symptom
recurrent unipolar major depression. Several stud- load. Some studies have found a correlation
ies have reported that this group also shows between cognitive deficits and negative symptoms
cognitive impairment (6–8), and there is empirical (24–27), while others have not found such an
evidence that some of the same brain areas, i.e. association (28–30), suggesting that the two
temporal lobe areas and the hippocampus, are domains are relatively independent. We addressed
affected in schizophrenia and the recurrent type of this question by investigating to what degree
depressive disorder (9–11). clinical symptoms influence NP deficits within the
To identify deficits that discriminate between the schizophrenia and depression groups separately.
two groups of patients, a wide range of cognitive
functions have to be assessed. By analyzing differ-
Aims of the study
ences in test profiles rather than measures of
separate functions, it is possible to identify func- Four questions were addressed. The first was to
tions that are jointly impaired from those that are what degree patients with schizophrenia present a
uniquely affected for each diagnostic group. We specific NP profile. The second was to enhance our
have previously presented results on single tests understanding of how depression per se, free from
(12–15). The present study expands the analysis psychosis, has a negative effect on neurocognitive
across all cognitive domains and takes into con- performance. The third was whether variation in
sideration demographic confounders. Thus, the IQ is associated with NP test performance in these
first question addressed in the present study was to groups. The fourth was whether degree of NP
what degree patients with schizophrenia present a impairment correlates with clinical symptom load.
specific NP profile, i.e. whether the impairment
varies not only in level but also in type from that of
Material and methods
patients with recurrent depression.
In all but two studies (16, 17) patients with The study was approved by the Regional Com-
schizophrenia have been compared with psychotic mittee for Medical Research Ethics.
or mixed groups of psychotic and non-psychotic
depression. Findings from these two studies, as
Participants
well as from comparisons of psychotic and non-
psychotic depression patients (18, 19), provide Patients were recruited for research purposes from
support for the conclusion that psychotic depres- four clinics in Norway covering two catchment
sion is a distinct diagnostic category different areas. Fifty-three patients who met the DSM-IV
from non-psychotic depression. By comparing criteria (31) for schizophrenia (38 in-patients), 50
schizophrenia patients with non-psychotic depres- patients with a major depressive disorder, recurrent
sion patients we contribute to the understanding type (296.3) (34 in-patients), and 50 healthy control
of which aspects of cognition is uniquely impaired subjects invited to participate from newspaper and
in depression. This was the second aim of the community advertisements and recruited from the
study. same area as the patients, were included. The
Our next objective was to analyze how variation Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I
in IQ is associated with NP test performance. A Disorders (32) was used for diagnostic assessment.
large body of research shows that low premorbid Five senior psychiatrists conducted the interviews.
IQ increases the risk for developing schizophrenia Consensus was reached for selected cases by
(20). To what degree IQ level changes in parallel clinical discussion and no diagnostic disagreement
with manifestation of schizophrenia symptoms is occurred. The schizophrenic sample had the
still debated (21). Further, little is known about following subdiagnoses: 43 paranoid, three
how patterns of NP function differ at various disorganized, one catatonic, two residual, four
levels of current IQ in schizophrenia (22). Weick- undifferentiated.
ert et al. (23) and Kremen et al. (22) found that Individuals with a history of hypomanic epi-
also patients with normal IQ manifested substan- sodes were excluded from the depression group.
tial cognitive deficits. We wanted to replicate their Number of depressive episodes ranged from two
analyses of how current IQ-level affects NP to five. Five of the depression patients were
performance in schizophrenia and thus shed light psychotic at the time of assessment and were

351
Rund et al.

excluded from the present analyses. Thus, the function. Control participants had no previous or
total number of patients with recurrent major present psychiatric disorders.
depression was 45. Attempts were made to match the three groups
Psychiatric ratings included the Extended Brief on demographic variables, and there were no
Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS) (33), the Global significant differences (P > 0.05) between the
Assessment of Functioning (GAF) Scale (31), three groups regarding age, education, hand dom-
Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HRSD) inance (according to self report), or gender.
(34), and the Positive and Negative Syndrome Demographic and clinical characteristics are
Scale (PANSS) (35). Inter-rater reliability was given in Table 1. A Norwegian short form of the
calculated based on five cases. Average intra-class Wechlser Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised (WAIS-
correlations were over 0.80 for all rating scales. All R) (digit span, similarities, picture completion) (36)
patients with depression had a minimum score of with documented psychometric properties (37) was
18 on HRSD at the time of testing, indicating that administered to estimate current IQ level. Patients
they were above the cut-off score for moderate with schizophrenia scored significantly lower than
depression. Four of the schizophrenics did not use the other two groups, but none scored below 70 IQ
antipsychotic medication. Eleven patients used points. The schizophrenics had higher BPRS and
typical antipsychotics, 36 atypical antipsychotics, PANSS scores and lower GAF scores, indicating
and two patients both. Three participants with greater functional disturbance than the depressed
schizophrenia were using drugs with known anti- patients. The depressed group scored significantly
cholinergic effect, and three patients were taking higher than the schizophrenia group on HRSD.
benzodiazepines. All but two of the schizophrenia patients scored
All but six of the depressed patients were within the valid range on a measure sensitive to
medicated at the time of NP testing. Thirty-six poor motivation or malingering (Victoria Symp-
patients were using antidepressants (selective sero- tom Validity Test, VSVT) (38) indicating that the
tonin reuptake inhibitor, mianserin, nefazodone, NP test scores may be regarded as reliable
venlafaxine or moclobemide), and none were on estimates of the groupsÕ neurocognitive perform-
tricyclic antidepressants. As additional medication, ance level.
19 patients were taking benzodiazepines and nine
antipsychotic medication (as a hypnotic). Informa-
Neuropsychological tests
tion regarding medication was missing for three
patients. All subjects were tested with a comprehensive NP
All participants were Norwegian-speaking adults test battery. In selecting the NP tests we focused on
between 19 and 51 years old with normal vision cognitive domains that have proved to be sensitive
and hearing. Exclusion criteria were: i) history of to dysfunction in these two illnesses. In order of
head trauma, neurologic disorder or developmen- administration the test battery included: Picture
tal dysfunction, ii) alcohol or substance abuse, iii) Completion (WAIS-R) (36), California Verbal
medical disease likely to affect nervous system Learning Test (39), Tower of London (40), Paced

Table 1. Characteristics of patients with schizophrenia (n ¼ 53) or recurrent major depression (n ¼ 45) compared with normal control subjects (n ¼ 50)

Group 1: schizophrenia Group 2: depression Group 3: controls Analysis

N N N Chi-square P

Gender (M/F) 33/20 18/27 25/25 v2ð2;N¼148 Þ ¼ 4.9 n.s.


Hand dominance (R/L) 48/4 41/4 42/4 v2ð2;N¼143Þ ¼ 0.1 n.s.
N Mean SD N Mean SD N Mean SD ANOVA Scheff

Age (years) 53 31.5 8.4 45 35.6 8.4 50 32.9 9.0 F2,145 ¼ 2.9 n.s.
Education (years) 53 13.3 3.2 45 13.7 2.8 50 13.9 2.5 F2,145 ¼ 0.6 n.s.
WAIS-R IQ estimate 53 91.0 10.3 45 98.9 10.9 50 103.3 7.6 F2,145 ¼ 21.4*** 1 „ 2, 3
GAF 52 39.3 11.3 45 46.5 8.8 – – – F1,95 ¼ 11.8*** 1 „ 2
BPRS 52 51.9 16.9 45 43.0 6.5 – – – F1,95 ¼ 11.0*** 1 „ 2
PANSS 53 75.6 23.2 45 56.3 11.6 – – – F1,96 ¼ 25.5*** 1 „ 2
HDRS 52 11.1 5.4 44 22.4 4.4 – – – F1,94 ¼ 124.4*** 1 „ 2

Valid test performance: >12 of 16 correct responses on VSVT (5 s delay).


BPRS, Extended Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale; GAF, Global Assessment of Function; HDRS, Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression; PANSS, Positive and Negative Syndrome
Scale; VSVT, Victoria Symptom Validity Scale; WAIS-R, Wechlser Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised.
***P < 0.001.

352
Neuropsychological test profiles

Auditory Serial Addition Test (41), Stroop Color tests and groups). High scores indicate better
and Word Test (Stroop) (42), Rey Complex Figure performance; where this was not the case, the
Test (43), Visual Backward Masking (14), Span of direction of the scores was reversed. The intercor-
Apprehension (R.F. Asarnow and K.H. Nuechter- relations between the NCCs were mainly between
lein, unpublished data), Continuous Performance 0.0 and 0.2, except for EF and ViM, which showed
Test–Degraded Stimulus (44), California Compu- a correlation of 0.4. A mean NCC score was
terized Assessment Package (45), Similarities computed across the seven cognitive domains for
(WAIS-R) (36), Digit Span (WAIS-R) (36), correlating cognitive function and clinical symp-
Controlled Oral Word Association Test (46), tom load.
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (47), Warrington The seven NCCs were entered as dependent
Recognition Memory Test (48), and the VSVT variables in a multivariate analysis of variance
(38). In order to carry out the profile analysis on a (MANOVA) with diagnostic group as the between-
smaller set of empirically defined neurocognitive group factor. If the main effect was significant,
composite (NCC) scores representative indices univariate analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were
from each test (49, 50), 16 in total, were selected performed for each NCC. Significant univariate
for principal component analysis (PCA). effects were followed by post hoc comparisons
(Scheffé) between each pair of groups.
A second set of MANCOVA and follow-up
Procedure
ANCOVAs with IQ as covariate were conducted in
After a complete description of the study, written order to control for a spurious group effect on the
informed consent was obtained. The NP tests were NCCs due to differences in intellectual capacity.
administered by a licensed clinical neuropsycholo- Subjects were then divided into subgroups defined
gist, a graduate psychology student or by an MD by IQ level. None of the control subjects scored
supervised by a neuropsychologist, all trained in below 85, compared with 12 schizophrenia and
standardized testing procedure. Examiners attemp- four depression patients. These 16 individuals were
ted to obtain maximal performance. The subjects excluded when analyzing how the interaction
were tested individually. Time taken to complete between IQ group (below or above mean) and
the test battery was approximately 4 h. Subjects diagnostic group affected NCC scores. Dividing
were allowed breaks between the tests, as needed. diagnostic groups according to IQ level (below
All subjects had at least one break, with test mean: 85–100, above mean: 100–125), the follow-
administration typically divided into 1.5–2 h ses- ing group sizes appeared: 29 and 12 schizophrenia
sions. patients, respectively; 20 and 21 depression
patients, respectively; and 20 and 30 healthy
control subjects, respectively. More schizophrenia
Statistical analysis
patients were classified in the below mean IQ group
Analyses were conducted using the statistical compared with the other two groups [v2(2, N ¼
package SPSS for Windows (version 11; SPSS 132) ¼ 8.8, P ¼ 0.012], confirming the general
Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Scores from the normal finding that schizophrenia patients have lower IQ
control group on the 16 NP measures were scores.
grouped into six principal components (eigenvalues To assess within-group contrasts each group’s
>1) by a PCA with Varimax rotation, accounting score for each NCC was compared with the mean
for 69.3% of the variation in the data set. Five of the group’s remaining six NCCs by pairwise
components constituted established cognitive t-tests. This procedure has the potential of identi-
dimensions (working memory, early visual infor- fying selective deficits (48), as opposed to a
mation processing, verbal memory, reaction time, generalized deficit.
attention and vigilance). One component com- To assess the percentage of patients considered
prised measures from two separate cognitive func- neuropsychologically impaired on each NCC, a
tions (Visual Memory and Executive Function) cut-off score of 1.5 SD below mean in the normal
and was thus split in two. Measures loading >0.4 control group was used as a threshold. To be
on each component were used to compute seven classified as moderately impaired a subject had to
composite scores as listed in Table 2, using mean perform deviantly on two to four NCCs, and to be
values and standard deviations from the control classified as severely impaired in five to seven.
group as norms. Missing data were replaced with Finally, NP impairment was correlated with
group mean values in order to secure a complete symptom ratings, i.e. GAF for both clinical
data set for multivariate analyses of NCCs (see groups, HRSD for the depression group, and
Table 2 for the prevalence of missing data across BPRS and PANSS for the schizophrenia group.

353
Rund et al.

Table 2. Neuropsychological test results and Z-transformed neurocognitive composite scores for patients with schizophrenia (n ¼ 53) or recurrent major depression (n ¼ 45)
compared with normal control subjects (n ¼ 50)

Group 1: schizophrenia Group 2: depression Group 3: controls


Post hoc
N Mean SD N Mean SD N Mean SD Analysis: ANOVA comparison

Component 1: working memory


PASAT: R3 + 2 s 51 60.8 28.7 43 79.2 24.3 50 95.4 16.6 F2,141 ¼ 27.0*** 1 „ 2 „ 3
Digit span backwards 53 5.0 2.1 45 5.8 1.7 50 6.9 2.3 F2,145 ¼ 11.1*** 1, 2 „ 3
Stroop: color word (s) 52 61.4 2r3.3 45 54.8 13.0 50 43.7 9.6 F2,144 ¼ 14.7*** 1, 2 „ 3
COWAT: F + A 53 21.5 7.8 45 23.8 7.7 50 31.0 8.9 F2,145 ¼ 18.6*** 1, 2 „ 3
Composite score 53 )1.5 1.1 45 )0.9 0.9 50 0.0 0.8 F2,145 ¼ 31.1*** 1 „ 2 „ 3
Component 2: executive function
WCST: Perseverative responses 50 31.8 25.3 45 15.1 11.0 49 11.8 9.2 F2,141 ¼ 19.6*** 1 „ 2, 3
Tower of London (trials) 53 14.1 2.7 45 15.8 1.9 50 16.5 1.4 F2,145 ¼ 18.1*** 1 „ 2, 3
Composite score 53 )2.0 1.6 45 )0.5 1.1 50 0.0 0.8 F2,145 ¼ 35.2*** 1 „ 2, 3
Component 3: visual memory
ROCF: delayed recall 50 14.2 6.8 45 20.6 6.6 50 24.5 5.8 F2,142 ¼ 32.7*** 1 „ 2 „ 3
RMT: faces 52 37.1 6.2 45 41.0 6.8 50 42.4 4.4 F2,144 ¼ 11.3*** 1 „ 2, 3
Composite score 53 )1.5 1.0 45 )0.5 1.1 50 0.0 0.8 F2,145 ¼ 32.7*** 1 „ 2, 3
Component 4: verbal memory
CVLT: total A1–A5 53 43.2 13.3 45 55.4 11.0 50 57.3 9.6 F2,145 ¼ 23.0*** 1 „ 2, 3
RMT: words 52 45.9 4.8 45 48.0 3.2 50 49.0 1.2 F2,144 ¼ 10.6*** 1 „ 2, 3
Composite score 53 )2.0 2.2 45 )0.5 1.6 50 0.0 0.8 F2,145 ¼ 19.7*** 1 „ 2, 3
Component 5: early visual information processing
VBM: no-mask condition 50 16.6 2.4 42 17.5 1.9 46 17.8 1.8 F2,135 ¼ 4.7* 1 „ 3
VBM: masking conditions 50 15.7 3.6 42 17.0 3.3 46 17.7 3.0 F2,135 ¼ 4.5* 1 „ 3
Composite score 53 )0.6 1.0 45 )0.2 0.9 50 0.0 0.9 F2,145 ¼ 4.7** 1 „ 3
Component 6: reaction time
CalCAP: SRT 51 356.2 93.8 44 365.3 85.7 48 311.5 68.0 F2,140 ¼ 5.6** 1, 2 „ 3
CalCAP: CRT (mean) 51 591.7 90.9 44 554.8 98.8 48 498.2 77.6 F2,140 ¼ 13.7*** 1, 2 „ 3
Composite score 53 )1.0 1.1 45 )0.8 1.1 50 0.0 0.8 F2,145 ¼ 12.4*** 1, 2 „ 3
Component 7: attention/vigilance
SoA total 53 103.5 14.9 45 109.0 8.8 48 111.4 7.0 F2,143 ¼ 6.9*** 1 „ 3
CPT-DS total dÕ 53 2.26 0.82 44 2.33 0.71 47 2.69 0.92 F2,141 ¼ 3.9* 1 „ 3
Composite score 53 )0.8 1.3 45 )0.4 0.8 50 0.0 0.8 F2,145 ¼ 8.7*** 1 „ 3

MANOVA across the 16 neuropsychological test variables (missing data replaced with group mean): F ¼ 4.763 (k ¼ 0.397), d.f. ¼ 32, 260, P < 0.001.
MANOVA across the seven composite scores: F ¼ 9.239 (k ¼ 0.466), d.f. ¼ 14,278, P < 0.001.
CalCAP, California Computerized Assessment Package; COWAT, Controlled Oral Word Association Test (F + A); CPT-DS, Continuous Performance Test-Degraded Stimuli; CVLT,
California Verbal Learning Test; PASAT, Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test; ROCF, Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test; RMT, Warrington Recognition Memory Test; SoA, Span
of Apprehension Test; VBM, Visual Backward Masking Test; WCST, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.
*P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001.

Results 0.5

Neuropsychological test scores and NCC scores for 0.0


each group are presented in Table 2. The NCC –0.5
z-scores

Z-score profiles of the groups are presented in –1.0


Fig. 1. There was a clear difference in performance
–1.5 Con
across groups, with the overall MANOVA being Dep
–2.0
significant for both the set of 16 test measures [F(32, Sch
260) ¼ 4.8, P < 0.001] and the seven composite –2.5
Working Executive Visual Verbal Visual lnfo Reaction Attention /
scores [F(14, 278) ¼ 9.2, P < 0.001]. Follow-up memory function memory memory processing time vigilance
ANOVAs on the composite scores revealed a
Fig. 1. Scores on seven neurocognitive composite scores for
significant difference between groups on all seven patients with schizophrenia (n ¼ 53) or recurrent major
NCCs (Table 2). Post hoc comparisons (Scheffé) depression (n ¼ 45) compared with normal control subjects
showed that patients with schizophrenia performed (n ¼ 50).
significantly below normal controls on all measures,
and below patients with depression on four NCCs The planned profile contrasts indicated that
(working memory, executive function, visual working memory [t(52) ¼ 2.7, P ¼ 0.01], executive
memory, and verbal memory). Patients with depres- function [t(52) ¼ 4.2, P < 0.001], visual memory
sion performed significantly below normal controls [t(52) ¼ 3.0, P ¼ 0.004], and verbal memory
on two domains (working memory, reaction time). [t(52) ¼ 3.3, P ¼ 0.002] were selectively impaired

354
Neuropsychological test profiles

60 1.0

Sch 0.5
50
% Impaired subjects

Dep
0.0
Con

z-scores
40
–0.5
30 –1.0

20 –1.5 Con
Dep
–2.0
10 Schi
–2.5
0 Working Executive Visual Verbal Viusal info Reaction Attention /
Working Executive Visual Verbal Visual lnfo Reaction Attention / memory function memory memory processing time vigilance
memory function memory memory processing time vigilance
Fig. 3. Scores on seven neurocognitive composite scores for
Fig. 2. Percentages of subjects scoring 1.5 SD below control patients with schizophrenia (n ¼ 29) or recurrent major
group mean (classified as impaired) on the seven neurocogni- depression (n ¼ 20) compared with normal control subjects
tive composite scores. (n ¼ 20) with current IQ between 85 and 100.

1.0
areas in the schizophrenia group. The depressed 0.5
patients were selectively impaired in working 0.0

z-scores
memory [t(44) ¼ 6.2, P < 0.001] and reaction –0.5
time [t(44) ¼ 2.4, P ¼ 0.022]. –1.0
The percentages of subjects in each group –1.5 Con
Dep
scoring more than 1.5 SD below the mean of the –2.0
Schi
normal group (cognitively impaired), are shown in –2.5
Working Executive Visual Verbal Viusal info Reaction Attention /
Fig. 2. More than 40% of the schizophrenia memory function memory memory processing time vigilance

patients showed deficits in working memory, exe-


cutive function, visual memory, or verbal memory, Fig. 4. Scores on seven neurocognitive composite scores for
whereas approximately 25% of the patients with patients with schizophrenia (n ¼ 12) or recurrent major
depression showed impairments in working depression (n ¼ 21) compared with normal control subjects
(n ¼ 30) with current IQ between 100 and 125.
memory or reaction time. In total, 62% of the
schizophrenia group showed impairments within
two or more domains, i.e. 45% were classified as executive functioning (P ¼ 0.006), visual memory
moderately impaired and 17% as severely (P ¼ 0.004) and verbal memory (P ¼ 0.003).
impaired, compared with 24% moderately and Mean NCC score in the schizophrenia group
4% severely impaired in the depression group. correlated moderately with total BPRS score
When using IQ as a covariate in the analysis of [r(52) ¼ )0.42, P ¼ 0.002] as well as all the
group differences, the effect of diagnostic group PANSS scores: total score [r(53) ¼ )0.42, P ¼
remained significant for the set of single tests [F(32, 0.002], positive symptom score [r(53) ¼ )0.28,
258) ¼ 3.3, P < 0.001] and the NCCs [F(14, P ¼ 0.042], negative symptom score [r(53) ¼
276) ¼ 5.8, P < 0.001]. Group differences on )0.37, P ¼ 0.006], and general symptom score
early visual information processing and reaction [r(51) ¼ )0.42, P ¼ 0.002]. No significant corres-
time became non-significant when controlling for pondence between cognitive function and HDRS
IQ. score was found in the depression group. Global
When reiterating the analysis with IQ as the functioning (GAF) showed a weak correlation with
grouping factor instead of as the covariate for the cognitive performance in schizophrenia [r(53) ¼
NCCs, significant main effects for both diagnosis 0.29, P ¼ 0.038], and no correlation in depression.
[F(14, 242) ¼ 8.1, P < 0.001] and IQ group [F(7, Group differences between the two clinical groups
120) ¼ 8.9, P < 0.001] were found. The interac- on executive function and visual memory were still
tion between diagnosis and IQ was non-significant significant when controlling for symptom ratings
(P > 0.05), confirming that the profile for each (BPRS, PANSS total score) in addition to IQ.
diagnostic group remained more or less the same in No significant (P > 0.05) difference was found
shape even though the level of performance on any NCC between patients on conventional and
improved with higher IQ. The NCC scores for atypical antipsychotic medication.
subjects with current IQ between 85 and 100 are
displayed in Fig. 3, and for those with IQ scores
Discussion
between 100 and 125 in Fig. 4. The high-function-
ing schizophrenia group scored below controls on The neurocognitive profiles of the two patient
several measures. This was significantly for groups provide evidence for a pervasive deficit in

355
Rund et al.

schizophrenia and for a mild and restricted distur- beyond the intellectual decline is evident in schi-
bance in non-psychotic depression. This implies zophrenia. In accordance with Kremen et al. (22)
that neurocognitive deficits found in previous we conclude that these findings provide strong
studies of patients with affective disorder may support for the argument that neurocognitive
partly be due to psychosis and not the depression dysfunction is a core deficit in schizophrenia.
per se (16). Consistent with several previous reports The finding that degree of NP impairment
(25, 49, 50, 52–54) the schizophrenia group showed among schizophrenia patients was significantly
selective impairments in all four higher order associated with level of clinical symptoms is in
neurocognitive domains (executive function, work- accordance with some previous studies (5, 25, 51).
ing memory, visual memory, verbal memory). The However, as symptom load (BPRS and PANSS)
presence of selective deficits in both the executive/ correlated 0.42 with mean NCC score and thus
working memory domain and the verbal/visual explained <20% of the variance in neurocogni-
memory domain, may implicate both frontal and tion, the two dimensions seem to be independent of
temporal-hippocampal involvement in the patho- each other and should be assessed and treated
physiology of schizophrenia. separately.
Thirty-eight percent of the patients with schizo- While the impairment in schizophrenia indicates
phrenia did not show an impaired NP profile, dysfunction in several aspects of higher order
demonstrating that schizophrenia may exist in the cognitive domains, depression evidence impair-
context of preserved cognition. The percentage of ment primarily on measures of processing speed.
unimpaired patients in the present study is higher Our findings show that depressed patients exhibit
than in most previous studies where the corres- problems within two domains, e.g. working
ponding figure has varied from approximately 10% memory and reaction time. This is in concert
(55, 56) to approximately 55% (3, 57), with with the study of Landrø et al. (6). They examined
25–30% as the most common estimate (5, 23, 58). unmedicated patients with non-psychotic major
It is possible that the high frequency of paranoid depression and found that these patients exhibited
symptoms in the present sample has contributed to an overall NP deficit, but performed dispropor-
the relatively high percentage of unimpaired tionately worse in the two domains of working
patients. Patients with a paranoid type of schizo- memory and selective attention (a choice reaction
phrenia are generally less neuropsychologically time task). Thiery et al. (62) also found that
impaired than non-paranoid patients (59, 60). It subjects with non-psychotic depression were
is also possible that these patients are impaired impaired on a choice reaction time task, and
relative to their premorbid level of functioning but recently Harvey et al. (63) confirmed a specific
that our research design was unable to detect such working memory deficit in non-psychotic depres-
subtle deficits. sion. These patterns of dysfunction suggest
Employing the concept of a Ôcontinuum of involvement of fronto-temporal cortical areas in
neurocognitive functioningÕ (3) may be a meaning- schizophrenia and subcortical areas in depression.
ful way of understanding these results. This model Although studies of pathogenesis suggest overlap-
is substantiated in the present study by the finding ping afflicted brain areas in schizophrenia and
of 17% of the schizophrenia patients being severely depression (9–11), our findings of differences in
disturbed, 45% moderately disturbed, and 38% type and magnitude of impairment is supported by
unimpaired. Further, we cannot rule out the findings of different fMRI activation pattern (64).
possibility that a small subsample of patients Moreover, impaired working memory and reaction
shows no evidence of any decline at all in cognitive time in depression did not correlate with ratings of
functioning relative to their premorbid level. affective disturbance. This strongly suggests that
Finally, cognitive dysfunction also varies with the neurocognitive impairment is not caused by the
phase of the illness, being more pronounced in affective symptoms characterizing the diagnosis.
acute psychotic phases (61). One limitation of this study is that the schizo-
At a group level patients with schizophrenia phrenia group scored significantly lower than the
show a decline in intellectual functioning measured two other groups on current IQ, and unfortunately
by IQ tests. We replicated and confirmed the we do not have an estimate of premorbid IQ. After
findings of Kremen et al. (22) that neurocognitive controlling for present IQ level by covariance
dysfunction is present in this patient group at all analysis, group differences in neurocognitive
levels of intellectual ability. The present results impairment remained significant for almost all
show that even patients with an intellectual ability NCCs. However, some researchers have suggested
above average, manifest substantial neurocognitive that covariance analysis is invalid for pre-existing
deficits. Thus, a neurocognitive dysfunction disparate groups that differ on the variable to be

356
Neuropsychological test profiles

covaried for (65, 66). Moreover, available data


Acknowledgements
suggest a decline of 1/2 to 1 SD from premorbid IQ
(21, 67). If 10 IQ points were added to the current This study was supported by grants from the
IQ an estimated average premorbid IQ would be Norwegian Research Council (#122974/320) and
approximately 103 in the present sample. Thus, from the National Council for Mental Health/
this sample is probably fairly representative with Norwegian Foundation for Health and Rehabilit-
respect to premorbid IQ. ation (#1997/007) to Dr Rund.
Another limitation is that we did not have
adequate control over medication effects. Moha-
References
med et al. (68) compared the performance of
medicated and non-medicated schizophrenia 1. Heinrichs RW. In search of madness. Schizophrenia and
neuroscience. New York: Oxford University Press, 2001.
patients. They found no significant differences in 2. Rund BR, Borg NE. Cognitive deficits and cognitive train-
their performance on any of the NP tasks. In the ing in schizophrenic patients: a review. Acta Psychiatr
current study, no differences were found between Scand 1998;100:85–95.
patients on conventional and those on atypical 3. Palmer BW, Heaton RK, Paulsen JS et al. Is it possible to be
drugs. Considering the positive effects of atypical schizophrenic yet neuropsychologically normal? Neuro-
psychology 1997;11:437–446.
antipsychotics reported in several studies (69) it is 4. Riley EM, McGovern D, Mockler D et al. Neuropsycho-
unlikely that antipsychotic medication can explain logical functioning in first-episode psychosis – evidence of
the poor performance found in the schizophrenia specific deficits. Schizophr Res 2000;43:47–55.
group. Further, long-term use of benzodiazepines 5. Saykin AJ, Shtasel DL, Gur RE et al. Neuropsycholo-
seems to be associated with impairment in some gical deficits in neuroleptic navie patients with first-
episode schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994;51:
cognitive functions, specifically sustained attention 125–131.
(70). Finally, findings are inconclusive with respect 6. Landrø NI, Stiles TC, Sletvold H. Neuropsychological
to the effects of antidepressants (71). However, the function in nonpsychotic unipolar major depression.
newer type of antidepressant medication is known Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 2001;
to have a less deleterious effect on neurocognitive 14:233–240.
7. Veiel HOF. A preliminary profile of neuropsychological
functions than older types (72). In this study, all deficits associated with major depression. J Clin Exp
depressed patients were on newer types of antide- Neuropsychol 1997;19:587–603.
pressants. It is therefore unlikely that antidepres- 8. Zakzanis KK, Leach L, Kaplan E. On the nature and pattern
sants have had any significant effect on NP of neurocognitive functions in major depressive disorder.
performance. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 1998;11:
111–119.
The neurocognitive test battery in the present 9. Bremner JD. Does stress damage the brain? Biol Psychiatry
study also has a psychometric limitation. The 1999;45:797–805.
different discriminating power of the various tasks 10. Kirschbaum C, Wolf OT, May M, Wippich W, Hellhammer
represents a confounding variable that does not DH. Stress- and treatment induced elevations of cortisol
allow direct interpretation of differential deficits levels associated with impaired declarative memory in
healthy adults. Life Sci 1996;58:1475–1483.
(73). 11. Nelson MD, Saykin AJ, Flashman LA, Riordan HJ. Hippo-
Finally, conducting as many as seven different campal volume reduction in schizophrenia as assessed by
comparisons for the profile contrasts may have magnetic resonance imaging: a metanalytic study. Arch
resulted in an inflated type I error rate, as these Gen Psychiatry 1998;55:433–440.
seven variables to a certain extent are intercorre- 12. Egeland J, Sundet K, Rund BR et al. Sensitivity and spe-
cificity of memory dysfunction in schizophrenia: a com-
lated. The strength of the present study is that it is parison with major depression. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol
the first comparison of large, well-defined groups 2002;25:79–93.
of subjects with schizophrenia and recurrent non- 13. Egeland J, Rund BR, Sundet K et al. Attention profile in
psychotic depression using a comprehensive NP schizophrenia compared with depression: differential
test battery. The group sizes are large enough to effects of processing speed, selective attention and vigil-
ance. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2003;107:1–9.
lend adequate power to the analyses. 14. Rund BR, Melle I, Friis S et al. Neurocognitive dysfunction
In conclusion, the present study confirms previ- in first-episode psychosis: correlates with symptoms, pre-
ous findings that cognitive impairment is a core morbid adjustment, and duration of untreated psychosis.
deficit in schizophrenia as well as provide evidence Am J Psychiatry 2004;161:466–472.
suggesting a great deal of individual variability in 15. Stordal KI, Mykletun A, Asbjørnsen A et al. Executive
functioning in major depression and schizophrenia: Is
cognition within schizophrenia in that a subgroup impairment determined by general psychopathology or
of patients performs within a normal range. This diagnosis? Acta Psychiatr Scand 2005;111:22–28.
finding suggests that the schizophrenia comprise 16. Albus M, Hubman W, Wahlheim C, Sobizack N, Franz U,
subgroups that should be identified for optimal Mohr F. Contrasts in neuropsychological test profile
treatment and counseling. between patients with first-episode schizophrenia and

357
Rund et al.

first-episode affective disorders. Acta Psychiatr Scand 35. Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. The Positive and Negative
1996;94:87–93. Syndrome Scale for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull
17. Jeste DV, Schelley C, Heaton BA et al. Clinical and neu- 1987;13:261–276.
ropsychological comparison of psychotic depression with 36. Wechsler D. Wechsler adult intelligence scale – revised.
nonpsychotic depression and schizophrenia. Am J Psychi- New York: The Psychological Corporation, 1981.
atry 1996;153:490–496. 37. Ward LC, Ryan JJ. Validity and time savings in the selec-
18. Basso MR, Bornstein RA. Neuropsychological deficits in tion of short forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale
psychotic versus nonpsychotic unipolar depression. Neu- – Revised. Psychol Assess 1996;8:69–72.
ropsychology 1999;13:69–75. 38. Slick D, Hopp G, Strauss E, Thompson GB. Victoria symp-
19. Schatzberg AF, Posener JA, DeBattista C, Kalehzan BM, tom validity test – version 1.0 professional manual. Odessa,
Rotschild AJ, Shear PK. Neuropsychological deficits in TX: Psychological Assessment Resources, 1997.
psychotic versus nonpsychotic major depression and no 39. Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. California verbal
mental illness. Am J Psychiatry 2000;157:1095–1100. learning test (CVLT) manual. New York: The Psycholo-
20. Zammit S, Allebeck P, David AS et al. A longitudinal gical Corporation, 1987.
study of premorbid IQ score and risk of developing 40. Shallice T. From neuropsychology to mental structure.
schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, severe depression, and New York: Cambridge University Press, 1988.
other nonaffective psychosis. Am J Psychiatry 2004;61: 41. Gronwall D, Wrightson P. Cumulative effects of concus-
354–360. sion. Lancet 1975;22:995–997.
21. Sheitman BB, Murray MG, Snyder JA et al. IQ scores of 42. Comalli PE, Wapner S, Werner H. Interference effects of
treatment-resistant schizophrenia patients before and Stroop Color-Word Test in childhood, adulthood, and
after the onset of the illness. Schizophr Res 2000;46: aging. J Gen Psychol 1962;100:47–53.
203–207. 43. Meyers JE, Meyers KR. Rey complex figure test and
22. Kremen WS, Seidman LJ, Farone SV, Tsuang MT. Intelli- recognition trial – professional manual. Odessa, TX: Psy-
gence quotient and neuropsychological profiles in patients chological Assessment Resources, 1985.
with schizophrenia and in normal volunteers. Biol Psy- 44. Nuechterlein KH. Signal detection in vigilance tasks and
chiatry 2001;50:453–462. behavioral attributes among offspring of schizophrenic
23. Weickert TW, Goldberg TE, Gold JM, Bigelow LB, Egan mothers and among hyperactive children. J Abnorm Psy-
MF, Weinberger DR. Cognitive impairment in patients with chol 1983;92:4–28.
schizophrenia displaying preserved and comprised intel- 45. Miller EN. CalCAP: California computerized assessment
lect. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000;57:907–913. package manual, rev. 4.0. Los Angeles, CA: Eric N Miller,
24. Andreasen NC, Olsen SA, Dennert JW, Smith MR. Ven- PhD, and Norland Software, 1993.
tricular enlargement in schizophrenia: relationship to 46. Spreen O, Strauss E. A compendium of neuropsychological
positive and negative symptoms. Am J Psychiatry tests: administration, norms, and commentary, 2nd edn.
1982;139:297–302. New York: Oxford University Press, 1998.
25. Bilder RM, Goldman RS, Robinson D et al. Neuropsycho- 47. Heaton RK, Chelune GJ, Talley JL, Kay GG, Curtiss G.
logy of first-episode schizophrenia: initial characterization Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Manual. Revised and
and clinical correlates. Am J Psychiatry 2000;157:549–559. expanded. Odessa, TX: Psychological Assessment
26. Johnstone EC, Crow TJ, Frith CD, Husband J, Kreel L. Resources, 1993.
Cerebral ventricular size and cognitive impairment in 48. Warrington EK. Recognition memory test. Windsor:
chronic schizophrenia. Lancet 1976;11:924–926. NFER-Nelson, 1994.
27. Perlick D, Mattis S, Stastny P, Silverstein B. Negative 49. Saykin AJ, Gur RC, Gur RE et al. Neuropsychological
symptoms are related to both frontal and nonfrontal function in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry
neuropsychological measures in chronic schizophrenia. 1991;48:618–624.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1992;49:245–246. 50. Øie M, Rund BR. Neuropsychological deficits in adolescent-
28. Binder J, Albus M, Hubmann W et al. Neuropsychological onset schizophrenia compared with attention deficit
impairment and psychopathology in first-episode schizo- hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1999;156:1216–
phrenic patients related to the early course of illness. Eur 1222.
Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1998;248:70–77. 51. Addington J, Addington D. Positive and negative symptoms
29. Hoff AL, Harris D, Faustman WO et al. A neuropsycho- of schizophrenia: their course and relationship over time.
logical study of early onset schizophrenia. Schizophr Res Schizophr Res 1991;5:51–59.
1996;20:21–28. 52. Addington J, Brooks BL, Addington D. Cognitive func-
30. Rund BR, Egeland J, Sundet K et al. Early information tioning in first episode psychosis: initial presentation.
processing in patients with schizophrenia or major Schizophr Res 2003;62:59–64.
depressive disorder. Schizophr Res 2004;68:111–118. 53. Goldberg TE, Gold JM, Greenberg R et al. Contrasts
31. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statis- between patients with affective disorders and patients with
tical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV), 4th edn. schizophrenia on a neuropsychological test battery. Am J
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association, 1994. Psychiatry 1993;150:1355–1362.
32. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. The 54. Sullivan EV, Shear PK, Zipursky RB, Sagar HJ, Pfefferbaum
structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders – A. A deficit profile of executive, memory, and motor
patient edition (SCID I/P, version 2.0). New York: Bio- functions in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 1994;36:641–
metrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric 653.
Institute, 1995. 55. Seltzer J, Conrad C, Cassens G. Neuropsychological pro-
33. Lukoff D, Ventura D. Manual for the expanded BPRS. files in schizophrenia: paranoid versus undifferentiated
Symptom monitoring in the rehabilitation of schizophrenic distinctions. Schizophr Res 1997;23:131–138.
patients. Schizophr Bull 1986;12:594–602. 56. Torrey EF, Bowler AE, Taylor EH, Gottesman II. Schizo-
34. Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neu- phrenia and manic-depresive disorder. New York: Basic
rosurg Psychiatry 1960;23:56–62. Books, 1994.

358
Neuropsychological test profiles

57. Strauss BS, Silverstein ML. Luria-Nebraska measures in 65. Miller GA, Chapman JP. Misunderstanding analysis of
neuropsychologically nonimpaired schizophrenics: a com- covariance. J Abnorm Psychol 2001;110:40–48.
parison with normal subjects. Int J Clin Neuropsychol 66. Cohen J, Cohen P. Applied multiple regression/correlation
1986;8:35–38. analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erl-
58. Taylor MA, Abrams R. Cognitive dysfunction in schizo- baum, 1975.
phrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1984;141:186–201. 67. Frith C, Leary J, Cahill C, Johnstone E. Performance on
59. Rund BR. The effect of distraction on focal attention in psychological tests: demographic and clinical correlates of
paranoid and non-paranoid schizophrenic patients com- the results of these tests. Br J Psychiatry 1991;13:26–29.
pared to normals and non-psychotic psychiatric patients. 68. Mohamed S, Paulsen JS, O’Leary D, Arndt S, Andreasen
J Psychiat Res 1983;17:241–250. NC. Generalized cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. A
60. Silverstein ML, Zervick MJ. Clinical psychopathologic study of first-episode patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry
symptoms in neuropsychologically impaired and intact 1999;56:749–754.
schizophrenics. J Consult Clin Psychol 1985;53:288–296. 69. Purdon SE, Jones BDW, Stip E et al. Neuropsychological
61. Rund BR. A review of longitudinal studies of cognitive change in early phase schizophrenia during 12 months
functions in schizophrenia patients. Schizophr Bull treatment with olanzapine, risperidone, or haloperidol.
1998;24:425–435. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000;57:249–258.
62. Thiery P, Axman D, Giedke H. Slow brain potentials and 70. Golombok S, Moodley P, Lader M. Cognitive impairment in
psychomotor retardation in depression. Electroencepha- long-term benzodiazepine users. Psychol Med
logr Clin Neurophys 1986;63:570–581. 1988;18:365–374.
63. Harvey PO, Le Bastard G, Pochon JB, Allilaire JF, Dubois 71. Amadore-Boccara J, Gougoulis N, Littrè MFP, Galinowski
B, Fossati P. Executive functions and updating of the A, Loo H. Effects of antidepressants on cognitive functions:
contents of working memory in unipolar depression. a review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1992;19:479–493.
J Psychiat Res 2004;38:567–576. 72. Elliot R. The neuropsychological profile in unipolar
64. Hugdahl K, Rund BR, Lund A et al. fMRI brain activation depression. Trends Cogn Sci 1998;2:447–454.
to a mental arithmetic task in schizophrenia and major 73. Chapman LJ, Chapman JP. The measurement of differential
depression. Am J Psychiatry 2004;161:286–293. deficit. J Psychiat Res 1978;14:303–311.

359

You might also like