You are on page 1of 10

Road Safety: Risk Management Perspective

Alan Nicholson 1

Abstract: Risk is ubiquitous and is associated with a very wide range of human activities. This has resulted in the development of an
internationally agreed set of principles and guidelines for risk management in general. This paper describes the methods and processes
that have been developed for general risk management, along with some of the knowledge upon which those methods and processes are
based. It is argued that road safety could be improved by employing those methods and processes. DOI: 10.1061/AJRUA6.0001027. © 2019
Downloaded from ascelibrary.org by Universitas Indonesia on 12/09/19. Copyright ASCE. For personal use only; all rights reserved.

American Society of Civil Engineers.

Introduction The risk management process involves the following activities


(ISO 2009a):
In the United States in 2015, accidents were the fourth most 1. Establishing the context (i.e., defining the criteria for risk
common cause of death, after heart disease, cancer, and chronic evaluation);
lower respiratory diseases (Murphy et al. 2017). A substantial pro- 2. Risk assessment:
portion of accidental deaths arises from motor vehicle accidents, • Risk identification (i.e., identifying the hazards and what can
and they accounted for 28.6% of accidental deaths in the US in go wrong);
2015 (Murphy et al. 2017). In the US, the rate of fatal crash involve- • Risk analysis (i.e., identifying the probabilities, conse-
ment of drivers varies with age, with the rate (per mile driven) for quences, expectations);
drivers aged 16–29 inclusive being about 2.25 times the rate for • Risk evaluation (i.e., using the risk evaluation criteria to
drivers aged 30–69 inclusive (Tefft 2017). The impact of road decide which risks should be treated);
accidents, unlike other causes of death (e.g., heart disease), is very 3. Risk treatment (i.e., prepare an action plan for treating
high for young people. selected risks);
Elms (1998) noted that humans have long been conscious of 4. Monitoring and review (i.e., evaluating actions and revising
risk, with the first written evidence of systematic risk management, action plan if appropriate); and
the Code of Hammurabi, dating from about 1800 BC. This consid- 5. Communication and consultation with stakeholders.
ered physical risk, specifying that if a building collapsed and the The relationship between these activities is shown in Fig. 1.
owners’ son was killed, then the builder’s son would be executed. While risk assessment is a major part of the risk management
It also considered commercial risk, with investors carrying the risk process, and at least 30 risk assessment techniques have been
of voyages in return for a share of the profits. developed (ISO 2009b), the other risk management activities are
Risk management methods have improved considerably since also important, and both “communication and consultation” and
the development of the theory of probability in the 17th century, “monitoring and review” are also discussed in what follows.
and especially since around 1950. This has led to the development About 80 countries have implemented road assessment programs
of an international standard, ISO 31000 (ISO 2009a), which defines that involve risk assessment. For instance, the New Zealand (NZ)
risk very broadly as the “effect of uncertainty on objectives,” where program has the following objectives (New Zealand Automobile
an effect is “a deviation from the expected” and can be positive or Association 2010, 2012):
negative and can “create or result in opportunities and threats.” Risk 1. Reduce deaths and injuries on NZ’s roads by systematically
management is also defined very broadly as “coordinated activi- assessing risk and identifying safety shortcomings that can
ties to direct and control an organization with regard to risk.” be addressed with practical road improvement measures;
ISO 31000 argues that risk should be expressed in terms of risk 2. Use risk assessment as a key factor in strategic decisions on
sources, potential events, and the consequences and likelihood of road improvements, crash protections, and standards of road
those potential events. management; and
In ISO 31000, likelihood is the chance of an event occurring. 3. Provide meaningful information on where the greatest levels of
It can be defined objectively or subjectively, defined qualitatively risk are faced and, in turn, to influence driver behavior.
or quantitatively, and described in general terms or mathematically. The NZ program defines two types of risk for a section of road:
In this paper, the term probability is used and has the same broad 1. Collective risk is the annual number of fatal and serious injury
meaning as likelihood in ISO 31000. crashes per kilometer of road.
2. Personal risk is the annual number of fatal and serious crashes
1
Professor, Dept. of Civil and Natural Resources Engineering, Univ. per vehicle-kilometer of travel on a given section of road.
of Canterbury, Private Bag 4800, Christchurch 8140, New Zealand. It has been suggested (New Zealand Automobile Association
ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6256-9823. Email: Alan.Nicholson@ 2010, 2012) that road controlling authorities and NZ police be
canterbury.ac.nz interested primarily in collective risk because this kind of risk
Note. This manuscript was submitted on September 19, 2018; approved
on May 3, 2019; published online on October 24, 2019. Discussion period
indicates where the greatest reductions in crashes can be achieved
open until March 24, 2020; separate discussions must be submitted for in- through infrastructure improvement and enforcement. It has also
dividual papers. This paper is part of the ASCE-ASME Journal of Risk been suggested (New Zealand Automobile Association 2010,
and Uncertainty in Engineering Systems, Part A: Civil Engineering, 2012) that drivers will be more interested in the level of personal
© ASCE, ISSN 2376-7642. risk, which is a measure of the danger to each individual, so they

© ASCE 04019017-1 ASCE-ASME J. Risk Uncertainty Eng. Syst., Part A: Civ. Eng.

ASCE-ASME J. Risk Uncertainty Eng. Syst., Part A: Civ. Eng., 2020, 6(1): 04019017
showing that accidents are not linearly related to traffic flows and

Communication and Consultation


Establish the Context that the level of risk is not constant if traffic flow is used as the
measure of exposure.

Monitoring and Review


There is difficulty in defining what constitutes an exposure,
Risk Identification and a range of exposure measures are used. The amount of travel
(i.e., the vehicle-kilometers of travel) is a commonly used measure
Risk Analysis of exposure, but Chapman (1971b) argued that, while it is appro-
priate for single-vehicle crashes on a section of road, it is not ap-
propriate for rear-end or head-on crashes. For example, he showed
Risk Evaluation that the expected number of times a vehicle would encounter a
vehicle traveling in the opposite direction on a two-way road
(i.e., the expected number of exposures to a head-on crash) during
Risk Treatment
a period T is
Downloaded from ascelibrary.org by Universitas Indonesia on 12/09/19. Copyright ASCE. For personal use only; all rights reserved.

Qa Qb ST½ð1=V a Þ þ ð1=V b Þ
Fig. 1. Risk management process.
where Qa and Qb = flow rates in two directions; V a and V b =
speeds in two directions; and S = length of road.
This exposure measure differs from the vehicle-kilometers of
can take greater care on those sections of highway for which the travel, which is simply
personal risk is high.
This paper will discuss the relationship between accidents, risk, ½Qa þ Qb ST
and exposure, along with issues relating to the definition of risk and
exposure. The paper will then discuss the perception of risk, and does not depend on the speed of travel, unlike the exposure
followed by a discussion of risk acceptability and tolerability measure proposed by Chapman (1971b); that measure implies that
(along with the factors affecting them). Issues related to risk com- exposure decreases as vehicle speeds increase.
munication and consultation are then discussed, and some studies Chapman (1971b) showed that for a cross-road junction, the
involving the application of three risk assessment techniques are number of exposures during a period T for a pair of conflicting
described. The paper concludes with a discussion of innovative movements is
technology and how efforts to improve road safety could be made
more effective by applying concepts and methods in use in risk ðT=tÞ½1 − expð−Qa tÞ½1 − expð−Qb tÞ
management.
where Qa and Qb = flow rates for conflicting movements; t = time
to cross collision area; and

Risk and Exposure t ¼ ðL þ WÞ=V

The concept of risk has been employed in road safety studies for where L = vehicle length; W = vehicle width; and V = speed for
many years, frequently in association with the concept of exposure. both conflicting movements.
For instance, Chapman (1967) proposed that the actual number This expression for exposure also implies that the exposure
of collisions is simply the product of the exposure (defined as decreases as speeds increase.
the possible number of collisions) and the probability that, given It appears that Chapman’s exposure measures have not received
the possibility of an accident occurring, the accident does actually much use in road safety studies, perhaps because the decrease in
take place. He subsequently (Chapman 1971a) used the term risk exposure as speed increases has been interpreted as implying a de-
for the number of accidents per exposure. crease in the number of collisions with increasing speeds, which is
Mahalel (1986) suggested there is an inherent methodological counter-intuitive. However, the probability of a collision given an
problem with using the number of accidents per exposure as a risk exposure might well increase with increasing speeds, at a faster
estimator, with the problem arising from the assumption that the rate than the exposure decreases with increasing speeds, with the
number of accidents is linearly related to the exposure, implying net effect being an increase in the probability of a collision with
a constant level of risk. increasing speeds. That is, the exposure measures developed by
Haight (1986) noted that there was “no general agreement on Chapman (1971b) do not necessarily mean a decrease in the prob-
a technical definition of the term risk” and that risk was often de- ability of collisions with increasing speeds.
fined, as in Hauer (1982), as “the probability (chance) of accident The safety of different modes of travel can be compared, but the
occurrence.” Haight suggested that this definition is somewhat results can be sensitive to the exposure measure used. For example,
ambiguous, in that a person may well consider their risk of having Ford (2000) compared the frequency of death for nine modes of
an accident as being low because their level of exposure is low. travel (air, rail, bus/coach, van, car, motorcycle, pedal cycle, foot,
Haight argued that considering risk as a probability is insuffi- and ship/ferry) in the UK using three measures:
cient and that one should consider consequence as well. Haight • deaths per person-kilometer of travel,
argued that risk should be treated as an expectation, i.e., risk equals • deaths per person-trip, and
the sum (over all possible events) of the product of the probability • deaths per person-hour of travel.
and consequence of those events. Defining risk in this manner is Ford ranked each mode using each exposure measure and found
consistent with the definition of risk used in risk management that the rankings of the modes depended upon which exposure
(ISO 2009a). measure was used. For example, travel by aircraft is fairly sensitive
There was subsequently a considerable amount of research to the exposure measure used; it was the safest mode in terms
(e.g., Hauer and Persaud 1996; Maher and Summersgill 1996; of deaths per person-kilometer, the seventh safest mode in terms
Turner and Nicholson 1998; Harwood et al. 2010; Elvik 2015) of deaths per person-trips, and the third safest mode in terms of

© ASCE 04019017-2 ASCE-ASME J. Risk Uncertainty Eng. Syst., Part A: Civ. Eng.

ASCE-ASME J. Risk Uncertainty Eng. Syst., Part A: Civ. Eng., 2020, 6(1): 04019017
the deaths per person-hours. The rankings of some modes of travel Fischhoff et al. (1978) determined the mean perceived risk and
were not sensitive to the measure (e.g., motorcycle travel is the least mean perceived benefit of each activity/technology and found
safe mode for all three measures). Travel by rail or bus/coach was that the perceived risk tended to decline with increasing perceived
found to be relatively safe for all three exposure measures, while benefit. One activity/technology (using motor vehicles) stood out,
travel by pedal cycle or foot was found to be relatively unsafe for all having a high perceived risk and a high perceived benefit, while
three exposure measures. It is likely that different members of the using motorcycles (along with several other activities/technologies,
public have different measures in mind when they assess the safety including smoking and handguns) had a high perceived risk and a
of the alternative modes. low perceived benefit.
In practice, the meaning of the term risk can vary quite widely. A survey of drivers in NZ (New Zealand Automobile
For instance, it was used for the social cost of accidents per vehicle- Association 2010) involved presenting them with photographs of
kilometer in the 2003 road safety strategic plan for New Zealand various roads and getting them to indicate how safe or dangerous
(Ministry of Transport 2003), which recommended that roads with they thought the roads were. Drivers were asked to estimate the
a high “risk” (i.e., a high accident cost per vehicle-kilometer) be potential for death or injury in various scenarios based on various
targeted for equity reasons. roadside hazards being present or safety devices either not being in
Downloaded from ascelibrary.org by Universitas Indonesia on 12/09/19. Copyright ASCE. For personal use only; all rights reserved.

place or not being used.


For each scenario, the percentage of respondents who indicated
Risk Perception, Acceptability, and Tolerability that, in the event of a crash, death or injury was probable or almost
certain was as follows:
Risk Perception • cliff or steep bank (down) 77%,
• no median barrier 73%,
Fischhoff et al. (1982) described the results of a study involving • wall adjacent to road 56%,
getting lay people to estimate the number of deaths/year due to • power poles adjacent to road 43%,
41 causes, where the actual number varied widely (from as low • trees 37%,
as ≤5 to ≥1,000,000). The results showed that lay people tend • deep ditch 32%, and
to overestimate the frequency of uncommon events and underesti- • not wearing a seat belt 80%.
mate the frequency of commonly occurring events. Motor vehicle Crash data for NZ show that about 50% of crashes in rural areas
accidents were a notable exception, with the frequency of these involve a roadside hazard, with most of the crashes involving a
commonly occurring events being overestimated. Fischhoff et al. single vehicle that lost control, with the major roadside hazards
(1982) suggested that those hazards for which the frequency is being cliffs, ditches, trees, and poles (New Zealand Automobile
overestimated tended to be disproportionately mentioned in the Association 2010). The survey results showed that drivers seem
news media. fairly well aware of the increased potential for death or injury as-
Fischhoff et al. (1982) noted that the public is often criticized for sociated with not wearing seat belts, the presence of cliffs or steep
failing to wear seat belts. They commented that while safety experts banks, and the absence of median barriers. However, their aware-
see the many lives that can be saved from a small reduction in each ness of the risk associated with common hazards (power poles,
trip’s probability of ending in death, drivers may see only the very trees, and deep ditches adjacent to the road), which accident sta-
small probability of death during any given trip and believe that a tistics show are strongly associated with serious crashes, was much
reduction in that very small probability does not justify the effort of lower.
buckling up. The survey results indicate that drivers underestimate the risk
A number of studies, such as by Allman (1985) and Slovic associated with some real hazards and highlight the need “to pro-
(1987), have examined the discrepancy between the public’s per- vide meaningful information on where the greatest levels of risk are
ception and the experts’ assessment of risk. They compared the faced,” which is one of the objectives of the NZ road assessment
public’s and experts’ rankings of 30 activities=technologies in program (New Zealand Automobile Association 2010, 2012), if
terms of their risk levels and found substantial differences between driver behavior is to be changed and road safety is to be improved.
the rankings. There was a high level of agreement on the risk as-
sociated with the use of motor vehicles, handguns, and smoking,
but there were some strong disagreements, as shown in Table 1. Acceptability of Risk
Slovic (1987) found that the expert assessments of risk corre-
Starr (1969) undertook one of the earliest studies of the willingness
lated highly with the frequency of fatalities, but the assessments
of individuals to accept risks associated with various activities.
of lay people were related more to other hazard characteristics
Starr studied, for a range of activities (including mining, hunting,
(e.g., the potential for a catastrophic consequence and, for some
skiing, smoking, and traveling by various modes of transport), the
hazards, the threat to future generations).
trade-off between the risk associated with the activity and the
benefit of undertaking those activities. He adopted a revealed pref-
erence approach, which assumed that people had full information
Table 1. Comparison of public perception and expert assessment of risk and that they used that information optimally.
Public perception Expert perception Starr concluded that the risk (measured by the statistical expect-
Activity/technology (ranking out of 30) (ranking out of 30) ation of deaths per person-hour of exposure) was related approx-
Nuclear power 1 20 imately to the third power (cube) of the financial benefit and that
Motor vehicles 2 1 the risk associated with voluntary activities (e.g., hunting, skiing,
Motorcycles 5 6 and smoking) was about 1,000 times greater than the risk associ-
Smoking 4 2 ated with involuntary activities (e.g., the generation and use of elec-
Mountain climbing 15 29 tric power). He also found that the risk for automobile travel was
Bicycles 16 15 similar to the risk of disease and suggested that, given the high per-
X-rays 22 7 centage of the population participating in automobile travel, the
Note: The higher the ranking, the greater the perceived risk. level of risk associated with automobile travel probably reflected

© ASCE 04019017-3 ASCE-ASME J. Risk Uncertainty Eng. Syst., Part A: Civ. Eng.

ASCE-ASME J. Risk Uncertainty Eng. Syst., Part A: Civ. Eng., 2020, 6(1): 04019017
a “true societal judgment” on the acceptability of risk in relation to to mean focusing on roads with substantial traffic flows. Users of
benefit. low-flow roads with a high personal risk (i.e., a high frequency of
Lowrance (1976) suggested 10 factors that affect an individual’s fatal and serious crashes per vehicle-kilometer), who would be ex-
acceptance of risk: pected to be more careful, might well consider the distribution of
• whether the risk is assumed voluntarily or borne involuntarily, risk, and the strategy of traffic control and enforcement authorities,
• whether the effect is delayed or immediate, unfair.
• whether alternatives are available or unavailable,
• whether the risk is known or unknown to scientists and the
public, Tolerability of Risk
• whether the exposure essential or nonessential, Many people treat acceptable risk and tolerable risk as identical, but
• whether the risk is encountered occupationally or the UK Health and Safety Executive (2001) has argued that they are
nonoccupationally, not identical, saying that “tolerable does not mean acceptable : : :
• whether the hazard is common or uncommon, (tolerable) refers to a willingness by society as a whole to live with
• whether the hazard affects average people or sensitive people, a risk so as to secure certain benefits in the confidence that the risk
Downloaded from ascelibrary.org by Universitas Indonesia on 12/09/19. Copyright ASCE. For personal use only; all rights reserved.

• whether the effect is reversible or irreversible, and is one that is worth taking and that it is properly controlled.” A risk
• whether the risk arises from an intended use or misuse of a might not be acceptable to an individual, in which case that indi-
technology. vidual will not take that risk, but it may be acceptable to a large
Fischhoff et al. (1978) undertook a study similar to that of Starr proportion of people and be tolerated by society. The tolerability
(1969), except that they used a stated-preference method instead of of risk to society is likely to be affected by the acceptability of risk
a revealed-preference method, with psychometric procedures being to individuals; the former is likely to decline as the latter declines.
used to obtain quantitative assessments of the perceived risk and This distinction between the acceptability of a risk to an individual
perceived benefit of various activities and technologies. For each and the tolerability of a risk to society is important and has been
of the 30 activities or technologies, participants were also asked followed in this paper.
to indicate the level of voluntariness, familiarity, controllability, The UK Health and Safety Commission (1991) classified risks
potential for major (multiple-fatality) consequences, immediacy of according to their tolerability, based on the annual frequency (F) of
consequences, and knowledge of the consequences of each activity events involving N or more deaths, as follows:
or technology. • Risk is negligible if F < ð0.0001=NÞ;
The revealed-preference and stated-preference approaches each • Risk is not negligible if ð0.0001=NÞ < F < ð0.01=NÞ and should
have their strengths and weaknesses. Fischhoff et al. (1978) dis- be reduced to “as low as reasonably practicable” (ALARP);
cussed the relative merits of both methods, concluding that a multi- • Risk is possibly unjustifiable and should be scrutinized if
method and multidisciplinary approach would be required to answer ð0.01=NÞ < F < ð0.1=NÞ; and
the question “how safe is safe enough?” • Risk is intolerable if ð0.1=NÞ < F.
A number of studies, e.g., Davidshofer (1976) and Fischhoff These limits are shown in Fig. 2 (note the logarithmic scales).
et al. (1982), have explored the consistency of people’s propensity All activities can be assessed in terms of their FN curves (or
for taking risks in different situations and found little or no consis- profiles), showing the annual frequency (F) of events involving
tency. Fischhoff et al. (1982) found that people who are daring in N or more deaths. Two FN profiles (Types A and B) are shown in
one context may be timid in another. Fig. 3. The Type A profile represents an activity resulting in many
Sandman (1993) suggested a list of 12 identified factors that accidents involving a few deaths, and very occasionally accidents
increased an individual’s willingness to accept a risk. Some of the involving more than a few deaths. The Type B profile represents an
factors differed from those of Lowrance (1976), including the activity resulting in a few accidents involving a few deaths, and
risk being: very occasionally accidents involving many deaths. Evans (1994)
• natural rather than industrial or artificial, developed FN profiles for road, rail, and air travel in the UK and
• chronic rather than catastrophic, found that the profile for road travel was similar to the Type A
• controlled by the person rather than someone else,
• distributed fairly rather than unfairly,
• assessed by a trusted source rather than an untrusted source,
ANNUAL FREQUENCY (F) OF N OR MORE DEATHS

1E-1
• assessed transparently rather than nontransparently, and
• nonmemorable rather than memorable (subject to dramatic 1E-2
media coverage). INTOLERABLE
Several of Sandman’s factors are relevant to road safety. For POSSIBLY
instance, whether the risk is controlled by the person rather than 1E-3
UNJUSTIFIED
someone else is relevant when considering the risk for cyclists
and pedestrians exposed to collisions with motor vehicles. Pedes- 1E-4
trians and cyclists, but not motor vehicle occupants, are more vul- ALARP
nerable to death or injury, and it is not surprising that pedestrians
1E-5
and cyclists are consequently less likely to find the level of risk on
roads acceptable. While motor vehicle accidents involve artificial
objects, they are chronic rather than catastrophic. That is, though 1E-6
road accidents are more frequent than aircraft crashes, the latter NEGLIGIBLE
generally involve a greater number of deaths). 1E-7
If traffic control and enforcement authorities do adopt a strategy 1E+0 1E+1 1E+2 1E+3 1E+4

of focusing on reducing the so-called collective risk (i.e., the fre- NUMBER OF DEATHS (N)

quency of fatal and serious crashes per kilometer), as suggested by


Fig. 2. Risk classification.
the New Zealand Automobile Association (2010, 2012), it is likely

© ASCE 04019017-4 ASCE-ASME J. Risk Uncertainty Eng. Syst., Part A: Civ. Eng.

ASCE-ASME J. Risk Uncertainty Eng. Syst., Part A: Civ. Eng., 2020, 6(1): 04019017
ANNUAL FREQUENCY (F) OF N OR MORE DEATHS
1E+4
TYPE ‘A’
Risk Communication and Consultation
PROFILE
1E+3 ISO 31000 (ISO 2009a) notes that communication and consultation
are “continual and iterative processes : : : to provide, share or
1E+2
obtain information and to engage in dialogue with stakeholders
TYPE ‘B’ regarding the management of risk” and defines a stakeholder as
PROFILE “a person or organization that can affect, be affected by or perceive
1E+1 themselves to be affected by a decision or activity.” That is, risk
communication should be a two-way process, with experts listen-
1E+0 ing to the concerns of the public and providing the information
HSE TOLERABILITY sought by the public.
LIMIT
The importance of risk communication has long been recog-
1E-1
nized by those involved in environmental risk management, as evi-
denced by the formulation and adoption of seven so-called cardinal
Downloaded from ascelibrary.org by Universitas Indonesia on 12/09/19. Copyright ASCE. For personal use only; all rights reserved.

1E-2
rules of risk communication (EPA 1988). Recognition of the im-
1E+0 1E+1 1E+2
NUMBER OF DEATHS (N)
portance of risk communication has spread to other areas involving
risk management, including emergency management, for example
Fig. 3. FN profiles. (Janoske et al. 2012).
The first two cardinal rules of risk communication (EPA 1988)
are to accept and involve the public as a legitimate stakeholder (or
partner) in the management of the risk and to listen to them. It is
Table 2. Risk matrix common practice for road safety strategies or plans to be developed
by government officials, sometimes with the aid of selected experts
Consequence or interest groups. This is not conducive to gaining the acceptance
Minor Moderate Major Critical and support of a broad and representative section of the public,
Probability (1) (10) (50) (100) whose acceptance and support are important, given that the strat-
Very unlikely (1) 1 10 50 100 egies or plans invariably involve seeking a change in the behavior
Unlikely (2) 2 20 100 200 of road users.
Moderate (3) 3 30 150 300 Keeney and von Winterfeldt (1986) argue that risk communica-
Likely (4) 4 40 200 400 tion is a separate problem from the problem of deciding how to
Very likely (5) 5 50 250 500 analyze a risk and deciding what should be done about a risk. They
suggest that the objectives of risk communication are
• to educate the public about risks, risk analysis, and risk
management;
profile shown in Fig. 3, with the profiles for rail and for air travel • to inform the public about specific risks and actions taken to
being similar to the Type B profile shown in Fig. 3. He also found alleviate them;
that the profiles for all three modes of travel were well outside the • to encourage personal risk reduction measures;
Health and Safety Commission’s tolerability threshold, as shown • to improve the understanding of public values and concerns;
in Fig. 3. • to increase mutual trust and credibility; and
A risk matrix is a method of classifying risks to help to distin- • to resolve conflicts and controversies.
guish between tolerable and intolerable risks and to help to rank According to Keeney and von Winterfeldt (1986), risk commu-
risks for treatment. A risk matrix combines probability and conse- nication involves three distinct groups of people with fairly differ-
quence, with both being allocated to discrete levels. Scores can be ent characteristics and viewpoints: risk analysts, regulators, and the
allocated to each consequence and probability level, with risk general public. Risk analysts are invariably very familiar with the
scores being the product of the consequence and probability scores concepts of probability and consequence upon which risk analysis
(Table 2). The higher the risk score, the higher is the priority that is based, can be objective in their assessment of probabilities and
should be given to improving safety. consequences, and are generally comfortable conceding uncer-
A risk matrix approach can be used when risk analysts have tainty in their conclusions regarding which risks should be ad-
difficulty arriving at precise estimates of the probability and con- dressed and how they should be addressed.
sequence of events. However, the setting of consequence and prob- Regulators might well have some familiarity with the concepts
ability scores is somewhat arbitrary, and the greater range of of probability and consequence upon which risk analysis is based
consequence scores in Table 2 is consistent with placing greater and might well be objective in their interpretation of the results of
weight on the consequence of an event than the probability of that risk analysis, but they can have strong preference for clear recom-
event. This might well reflect the greater news media interest in mendations regarding which risks should be addressed and how
events with major or critical consequences. they should be addressed.
Using Table 2, road accidents might well be assigned a risk The general public are generally not familiar with the concepts
score of 5–10, corresponding to “very likely and minor” and “very upon which risk analysis is based, are generally not objective, and
unlikely and moderate,” respectively, while rail and air accidents often do not appreciate being regulated, while the media have a role
might well be assigned much higher risk scores of 50–100, corre- in transmitting information (often with some “translating”) from
sponding to “very unlikely and major” (or “unlikely and major”) experts or regulators to the general public.
and “very unlikely and critical,” respectively. Risk communication is complex, reflecting the complexities of
A risk matrix might be more easily understood by the public and the problem being addressed. While the public frequently like to
assist risk communication and make the risk assessment and evalu- express a risk problem in simple terms, such as “safe” or “unsafe,”
ation seem more transparent. risk analysts and regulators must assess the range of possible

© ASCE 04019017-5 ASCE-ASME J. Risk Uncertainty Eng. Syst., Part A: Civ. Eng.

ASCE-ASME J. Risk Uncertainty Eng. Syst., Part A: Civ. Eng., 2020, 6(1): 04019017
actions and balance multiple conflicting objectives. This makes risk Table 3. Relationship between qualitative and quantitative probabilities
communication difficult, particularly when the public are uncom- Qualitative probability Full probability Typical probability
fortable making difficult trade-offs and they expect the experts and term range (%) range (%)
regulators to be omniscient.
“Almost certainly” 50–99 85–99
This often leads regulators (policymakers and politicians) to “Highly likely” 50–99 85–99
become impatient with scientific uncertainty and, according to “Probable” 25–90 55–85
Keeney and von Winterfeldt (1986), asking for “one-armed’ scien- “Likely” 30–85 55–85
tists (i.e., analysts who will not say “on the one hand : : : but on the “Unlikely” 1–45 15–45
other hand : : : ”). Risk communication is also not helped by dis- “Improbable” 1–45 15–45
putes among experts or by experts using technical and bureaucratic “Highly unlikely” 1–30 1–15
language instead of language understandable to the general public. “Chances are slight” 1–45 1–15
Keeney and von Winterfeldt (1986) note that while experts
generally deal with statistical lives lost and average social costs
and benefits, most members of the public prefer to focus on the
Downloaded from ascelibrary.org by Universitas Indonesia on 12/09/19. Copyright ASCE. For personal use only; all rights reserved.

better to compare “4 out of 10,000” with “5 out of 10,000” than


effects on individuals, a tendency that is reinforced by the media
“1 out of 2,500” with “10 out of 20,000” or “1 out of 2,000”).
concentrating on individual drama and so-called human interest
When communicating with the public, it is important to note
stories. They also note that some members of the public have a
that there are major variations in the probabilities assigned to terms
desire for both zero risk and certainty and that this is an unachiev-
commonly used when discussing probability. Von Winterfeldt and
able goal and presents an obstacle to presenting risk information
Edwards (1986) found that there were large variations between lay
responsibly.
people when converting qualitative probability terms (e.g., likely,
Regarding the achievability of zero risk, Allsop (2016) noted
unlikely) to quantitative probabilities.
that the ratio of the probability of death during travel on roads
Table 3 shows, for each qualitative probability term, both the
(per hour of exposure) to the probability of death during all other
full and typical ranges of probabilities; the full range includes
everyday activities (per hour of exposure) is much greater than one,
virtually all responses, and the typical range includes about 85%
but the ratio (i.e., the relative risk of road travel) had decreased in
of the responses (i.e., it excludes so-called outliers and is less wide).
the UK from about seven in 2004 to about three in 2014 (i.e., trav-
It can be seen that the probability ranges (especially the full
eling on roads had become less dangerous compared with the rest
ranges) are very wide and
of everyday life). He suggested that as the ratio approaches one,
• are the same for “almost certainly” and “highly likely,”
“people will look harder at the cost and intrusiveness of further • are similar for “probable” and “likely,”
reducing death and injury on the roads” and “road safety will be • are the same for “unlikely” and “improbable,” and
competing for resources and public attention : : : with other areas • are fairly similar for “highly unlikely” and “chances are slight.”
of preventable death and injury.” If lay people were asked to convert quantitative probabilities
This is entirely consistent with the views of risk management to qualitative probability terms, it seems reasonable to expect a sim-
experts. For instance, Taig (1998) stated that while accidents are ilarly high level of variation between their responses. The consid-
avoidable and strenuous efforts are worthwhile to prevent them erable scope for misinterpretation of quantitative probabilities and
and mitigate their effects, it is important to note that “as soon as qualitative probability terms is not conducive to effective risk
risks are compared and judgements made about them, it becomes communication.
necessary to consider values, not only in respect of the risks them-
selves, but also in respect of the things people do that give rise
to risk : : : striving to eliminate risk, or introducing risk control
Risk Monitoring and Review
solutions that involve stopping the activities that give rise to risk,
are often wholly unacceptable : : : one of the most fundamental ISO 31000 defines monitoring as “continual checking, supervising,
purposes of risk control is, not to eliminate risk itself, but to enable critically observing or determining the status in order to identify
people to undertake activities they wish to pursue but know to be change from the performance level required or expected” and de-
hazardous, in the confidence that the risks are properly managed fines review as an “activity undertaken to determine the suitability,
and controlled : : : this applies across all walks of life, and to every adequacy and effectiveness of the subject matter to achieve estab-
type of risk, whether financial, health and safety, environmental or lished objectives.”
whatever.” The effect of road safety risk management can differ from the
When presenting information to the public about the probability estimated or expected effect, and Evans (1985) suggested that this
of an event, it can be done as a percentage (e.g., 5%) or as a could be due to not taking account of the risk perception issues
frequency (e.g., 5 out of 100), and how a person responds to prob- discussed earlier and how people react to changes in the perceived
ability information depends upon the format that is chosen (Slovic level of risk. He proposed a “human behavior feedback model”:
et al. 2007). This phenomenon is not confined to the general
public; a survey of experienced forensic psychologists/psychiatrists
(Slovic et al. 2007) found that 21% would refuse to discharge a ðActual effectÞ ¼ ð1 þ fÞ × ðEngineering effectÞ
patient if told there was a 20% probability the patient would com-
mit a violent act, while 41% would refuse to discharge if told the where f is the feedback factor.
probability was 20 out of 100. Slovic et al. (2007) concluded that This model implies that
“20%” led to a relatively benign image of the patient, but “20 out of • the actual effect equals the engineering effect if f ¼ 0 (i.e., there
100” led to a frightening image of multiple patients committing is no risk compensation or “feedback”);
violent acts after release. • the actual effect is greater than the engineering effect if f > 0
Slovic et al. (2007) also found that when presenting more than (i.e., there is positive feedback); and
one frequency for comparison, using the same denominators will • the actual effect is less than the engineering effect if f < 0
increase audience differentiation and comprehension (e.g., it is (i.e., there is negative feedback).

© ASCE 04019017-6 ASCE-ASME J. Risk Uncertainty Eng. Syst., Part A: Civ. Eng.

ASCE-ASME J. Risk Uncertainty Eng. Syst., Part A: Civ. Eng., 2020, 6(1): 04019017
The risk homeostasis model (Wilde 1982) is a special case of unexpected traffic congestion). Estimated probabilities can be
Evans’ human behavior feedback model with f equal to −1). It assigned to each of the possible alternatives at each stage, giving
assumes a target risk level for each person and implies that safety a probability of the vehicle occupants being killed.
treatments will be ineffective if drivers succeed in maintaining a Using fault and event trees emphasizes the complex interactions
constant level of risk. As noted by Evans (1985), the risk homeo- that generally exist between the factors contributing to an accident
stasis model is not consistent with empirical data; Evans found and helps identify how those factors operate in the particular cir-
cases where f ≠ −1 (including cases where f > 0). Fuller (2000) cumstances surrounding each accident. One will be less likely to
proposed a model with some similarity to Wilde’s risk homeostasis form a simplistic view of why an accident has occurred than if
model. With Fuller’s so-called task-capability interface model, each one thinks in terms of a single chain of events and will be less likely
driver is considered to have a target level of task difficulty rather to think that accidents can be avoided by focusing on one factor or
than a target level of risk. This model has led to the risk allostasis type of factor (e.g., human factors).
theory (Fuller 2008), in which drivers seek to maintain a feeling of Fault trees, with their focus on understanding why an accident
risk within a preferred range. has occurred, would seem well suited to so-called ex-post accident
The task-capability interface model implies that collisions can investigations and accident reduction. Event trees would appear
Downloaded from ascelibrary.org by Universitas Indonesia on 12/09/19. Copyright ASCE. For personal use only; all rights reserved.

occur when the task demand experienced by a driver exceeds the well suited to ex-ante accident prevention (or safety auditing),
performance that drivers are willing and able to supply. Drivers can given their focus on identifying the possible consequences of an
adjust the task demand so it does not exceed their performance, event (e.g., a vehicle emerging from a minor road where the sight
primarily by adjusting their speed. Hence, an increase in speed after distance along the main road is severely restricted).
implementation of a treatment to improve road safety might be Joshua and Garber (1992) used fault tree analysis for a study of
taken as evidence of risk compensatory behavior. However, an the causes of large truck accidents. They found that the fault tree
increase in speed does not necessarily mean that the treatment analysis increased the level of understanding of the system (particu-
has been ineffective in reducing the likelihood of accidents. For larly how large truck accidents occur), the functional relationships
example, a study of driver behavior on curves before and after between failures, the degree of protection provided by the truck
realignment (Wong and Nicholson 1992) found a reduction in design, and the design requirements from a system safety view-
the likelihood of vehicles sliding off the outside of the curves, point. They suggested the increased understanding allows the iden-
despite an increase in vehicle speeds. This study considered the tification and quantification of potential failures, the identification
basic physics/mechanics of vehicles traversing curves and revealed of cause-and-effect relationships, and informed judgement about
a reduction in the required friction (i.e., the friction being called how, why, and with what frequency systems fail.
upon by drivers), which meant an increase in the safety margin. Washington (2015) investigated the use of event trees to assess
the probability of various types of accidents, including single-
vehicle loss-of-control accidents at curves, head-on collisions at
Risk Assessment Methods curves, and rear-end accidents. He found that, despite the difficulty
in estimating the probabilities of initiating failure events, intermedi-
Risk assessment is a major part of the risk management process, ate events, and outcomes, the construction of an event tree provides
and at least 30 risk assessment techniques have been developed useful insights for those seeking to identify which accident counter-
(ISO 2009b), some of which have been used in road safety risk measures would be effective in reducing accidents.
management for some time (Nicholson 2002). They include event Monte Carlo simulation involves describing uncertain input var-
tree analysis, fault tree analysis, and Monte Carlo simulation. iables using probability distributions and deriving the distribution
Event tree analysis is a structured way of representing the vari- of the dependent output variable. Menzies and Nicholson (2003)
ous possible outcomes of driver behaviors (violations, errors or studied the so-called amber dilemma problem using Monte Carlo
lapses, say) that might result in an accident and can help identifi- simulation, in conjunction with the basic deterministic theory of
cation of the failure process and the full range of outcomes. It in- Gazis et al. (1960). Menzies and Nicholson estimated the proba-
volves identifying initiating failure events, intermediate events, and bility of a conflict between vehicles caught in the amber dilemma
outcomes. Estimated probabilities can be assigned to each of the zone and side road vehicles, where a conflict was defined as a sit-
possible alternatives at each stage, giving a weighted average con- uation where a collision would occur if no evasive action were
sequence. An event tree resembles the trunk and branches of a tree. taken. They allowed for random variations in the uncertain input
The trunk represents the initiating failure event (e.g., a driver enter- variables (i.e., approach speed, acceleration rate, deceleration
ing a curve at too high a speed), where the major branches represent rate, perception-reaction time, length and distance from stop line
the intermediate events (e.g., the vehicle leaves the road, the vehicle for vehicles approaching the signal when amber commences, and
collides with a pole close to the road, some vehicle occupants are position of vehicle on side road). They found the probability of a
not wearing seat belts) and the minor branches represent the out- conflict at a traffic signal change was 5% when the amber time was
comes (e.g., serious injury to some occupants). 3.0 s and the all-red time was 0.0 s, 1% when the amber time was
Fault tree analysis is a structured way of clearly identifying the 3.8 s and the all-red time was 0.0 s, and 0.06% when the amber time
logical structure of the system, so that one can understand how the was 3.8 s and the all-red time was 1.0 s.
factors (or causes) and the preventive measures (or safeguards) are
logically linked. It enables the identification of weaknesses in pre-
ventive measures and the most appropriate means of reducing the Innovative Technology
risk. A fault tree resembles the trunk and roots of a tree. The trunk
represents the event (e.g., some occupants of a vehicle are killed The interest in using innovative technology (e.g., autonomous
after a vehicle collides with a pole alongside a road), the major vehicles) to improve road safety is growing rapidly. However, it
roots represent intermediate factors (e.g., seat belts worn by only is not new, as evidenced by proposals in the past to improve the
some occupants, pole located close to road on outside of curve), safety of vehicles negotiating horizontal curves, by having cars
and the minor roots represent the initiating factors (e.g., the driver automatically tilt (i.e., change the angle between the road and
of the vehicle has been delayed by previously encountered and the car’s chassis) as curves are traversed. This results in a higher

© ASCE 04019017-7 ASCE-ASME J. Risk Uncertainty Eng. Syst., Part A: Civ. Eng.

ASCE-ASME J. Risk Uncertainty Eng. Syst., Part A: Civ. Eng., 2020, 6(1): 04019017
proportion of the centrifugal force, which is experienced by drivers ready to resume manual control if required. They concluded that
as they traverse curves, acting along the so-called axis of the driver drivers are not being properly supported in adhering to their new
(a compressive force) and a smaller proportion acting laterally monitoring responsibilities and instead demonstrate behavior
(i.e., parallel to the surface of the road) and causing body sway. indicative of complacency and overtrust. They suggested that these
Because humans are more sensitive to a force causing body sway attributes may encourage drivers to take more risks on the road.
than to a force along the axis of the body, having a tilting vehicle Of particular concern is the risk to cyclists and pedestrians re-
would reduce driver discomfort and the need to reduce speed to sulting from collisions with autonomous vehicles, and Vissers et al.
avoid discomfort. Hence, drivers will be less likely to reduce speed. (2016) argue that autonomous vehicle technology is currently not
However, a tilting vehicle does not affect the forces on the ve- able to reliably predict the intentions and behaviors of pedestrians
hicle itself (i.e., the gravitational force and frictional force between and cyclists, whose safety is likely to be reduced unless the tech-
the tires and the road surface). Whether the vehicle slides toward nology is able to reliably predict their intentions and behavior.
the outside of the curve depends upon the maximum available Innovative technology, especially when developed in a competi-
frictional force. Hence, the tilting of the vehicle increases the prob- tive environment, can and does malfunction, and there is real merit
ability of drivers not reducing speed sufficiently and consequently in having wise humans with full situational awareness involved in
Downloaded from ascelibrary.org by Universitas Indonesia on 12/09/19. Copyright ASCE. For personal use only; all rights reserved.

calling upon more friction than is available (i.e., it increases the making decisions in situations where the adverse consequences of a
likelihood of an accident). wrong decision are not minor. This is exemplified by the case of the
During the early 1990s, it was proposed to use in-vehicle speed September 1983 malfunction of the USSR’s early warning system,
adaptation (ISA) to manage speeds and improve road safety by when the system indicated that missiles had been launched from the
reducing the probability and severity of crashes. Trials of ISA US. As noted by Chan (2017), it was during one of the most tense
were undertaken in some countries, including Sweden and the periods in the Cold War, with the USSR having recently shot down
Netherlands. The Lund trial (Hjälmdahl 2004) involved speed limit a Korean Air Lines commercial flight after it had crossed into
reminders for drivers and fitting cars with an active accelerator USSR airspace (killing 269 people, including a US congressman),
pedal, which applied a resistance to the accelerator pedal if the the US president having recently declared the USSR an “evil em-
speed limit was exceeded (acceleration was still possible by press- pire,” and the general secretary of the USSR strongly fearing an
ing very hard on the accelerator pedal), making it impossible to attack from the US. Colonel Petrov, the duty officer at the USSR’s
exceed the speed limit inadvertently. The aim of the trial was to early-warning system center, had to decide whether to initiate an
assess the effectiveness and acceptability (to drivers) of the system. action that would probably have resulted in a retaliatory attack from
Based on observed changes in speed, Hjälmdahl (2004) estimated the USSR. Petrov decided to treat the alert as a system malfunction,
that ISA would give reductions of up to 25% and 32% in injury later explaining that it was a “gut decision” (Chan 2017) based on
and fatal crashes, respectively, and noted that driver acceptability his knowledge of the system.
decreased as the intrusiveness increased (e.g., people who drive fast
and have a high accident involvement were less accepting).
Based on in-depth accident investigations (e.g., Sabey and Conclusion
Staughton 1975; Treat 1980; Sabey 1983) having found that around
90% of road accidents involve human error, many advocates of Humans have been living with risk and practicing risk management
autonomous vehicles claim that accidents will be reduced by for a long time, with a wide range of risk management methods
around 90%. Such claims overlook the fact that accidents typically having been developed. The management of transport risk would
involve more than one type of factor (road environment factors and benefit from evaluating those methods that have been developed for
vehicle factors are typically involved in around 30% and around general risk management and implementing those found to be
10%, respectively), and eliminating human error might well not suited to the management of transport risk.
prevent around 90% of accidents. The management of transport risk would be improved if greater
It is not clear how autonomous vehicles will recognize and attention were paid to so-called human factors, including the per-
cope with deficiencies in the road environment, such as warning ception of risk, the acceptability of risk (including the factors
signs being too close to the hazard, inappropriate warning signs, affecting it and the relative importance of those factors), and the
nonstandard temporary traffic management arrangements at road- nature of changes in driver behavior with a change in the perceived
works, localized reductions in superelevation at curves, road sur- risk. Knowledge of how people perceive transport risk and how
face deficiencies (e.g., potholes), and worn pavement markings. their perceptions influence their behavior (e.g., their speed and
Researchers who examine human factors are particularly con- mode choice) would assist effective safety education. It would also
cerned about the transition of control (i.e., the switch from highly assist identification of which exposure measure would be most
automated driving to manual driving while in traffic), which will be appropriately used in risk assessment. More sophisticated perfor-
needed unless autonomous vehicles can master all possible traffic mance measures than accidents per kilometer and per vehicle-
situations and all weather conditions and will never fail. Vlakveld kilometer (currently widely used in transport strategies) might
(2015) notes that there are two types of control transition: planned become appropriate.
(e.g., driver takes control when leaving motorway) and acute A greater emphasis on risk communication and consultation
(i.e., driver takes control when ADS fails or cannot cope). Vlakveld during the development of road safety strategies, to obtain input
notes that in the first situation, driver actions are initially slow and from a broad and representative section of the community, would
error-prone, and in the second situation, the driver is probably inca- assist in achieving a better understanding, by both users and
pable of avoiding an impending crash owing to a lack of situation providers of roads, of how they can contribute to improving road
awareness. safety.
Banks et al. (2018) note that the human factors literature indi- Risk communication and consultation will need to take account
cates that humans are notoriously inefficient at completing pro- of variations between risk management specialists and the public,
longed monitoring tasks. The researchers undertook observations as well as variations between members of the public, in the under-
of on-road driver behavior with a partially automated vehicle re- standing and interpretation of qualitative and quantitative probabil-
quiring drivers/supervisors to remain in an active monitoring state, ity terms. It will also be necessary to grapple with quite complex

© ASCE 04019017-8 ASCE-ASME J. Risk Uncertainty Eng. Syst., Part A: Civ. Eng.

ASCE-ASME J. Risk Uncertainty Eng. Syst., Part A: Civ. Eng., 2020, 6(1): 04019017
issues that appear to affect the acceptability of risk, such as whether Hauer, E., and B. N. Persaud. 1996. Safety analysis of roadway geom-
the distribution of risk is sufficiently fair. etry and ancillary features. Ottawa: Transportation Association of
Canada.
Health and Safety Commission. 1991. Major hazard aspects of the
transport of dangerous substances. London: Her Majesty’s Stationery
References Office.
Health and Safety Executive. 2001. Reducing risks, protecting people—
Allman, W. F. 1985. “We have nothing to fear, but a few zillion things.”
HSE’s decision-making process. London: Health and Safety Executive.
Science 85 (6): 38–41.
Hjälmdahl, M. 2004. “In-vehicle speed adaptation: On the effectiveness of
Allsop, R. E. 2016. “Vision Zero is flawed, but we should harness its spirit
a voluntary system.” Ph.D. thesis, Lund Institute of Technology, Univ.
to keep driving down deaths.” Local Transport Today, August 19, 2016.
of Lund.
Banks, V. A., A. Eriksson, J. O’Donoghue, and N. A. Stanton. 2018.
ISO. 2009a. Risk management—Principles and guidelines. ISO 31000.
“Is partially automated driving a bad idea? Observations from an on-
Geneva: ISO.
road study.” Appl. Ergon. 68 (Apr): 138–145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.apergo.2017.11.010. ISO. 2009b. Risk management—Risk assessment techniques. ISO/IEC
31010. Geneva: ISO.
Chan, S. 2017. “Stanislav Petrov, Soviet officer who helped avert nuclear
Downloaded from ascelibrary.org by Universitas Indonesia on 12/09/19. Copyright ASCE. For personal use only; all rights reserved.

war, is dead at 77.” New York Times, September 18, 2017. Janoske, M., B. Liu, and B. Sheppard. 2012. Understanding risk commu-
nication best practices: A guide for emergency managers and commu-
Chapman, R. A. 1967. “Traffic collision exposure.” In Proc., National
Roads Board Roading Symp., 184–198. Wellington, New Zealand: nicators. College Park, MD: US Dept. of Homeland Security.
National Roads Board. Joshua, S. C., and N. J. Garber. 1992. “A causal analysis of large truck
Chapman, R. A. 1971a. “Accident risks at intersections.” In Vol. 1 of Proc., accidents through fault tree analysis.” Risk Anal. 12 (2): 173–188.
New Zealand Roading Symp., 193–197. Wellington, New Zealand: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1992.tb00666.x.
National Roads Board. Keeney, R. L., and D. von Winterfeldt. 1986. “Improving risk communi-
Chapman, R. A. 1971b. “Some relationships between road accident cation.” Risk Anal. 6 (4): 417–424. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924
frequencies and measures of exposure to risk.” Ph.D. thesis, Transport .1986.tb00954.x.
Studies, Univ. College London. Lowrance, W. W. 1976. Of acceptable risk. Los Altos, CA: William
Davidshofer, C. O. 1976. “Risk-taking and vocational choice: A re- Kaufmann.
evaluation.” J. Couns. Psychol. 23 (2): 151–154. https://doi.org/10 Mahalel, D. 1986. “A note on accident risk.” Transp. Res. Rec. 1068:
.1037/0022-0167.23.2.151. 85–89.
Elms, D. G. 1998. “Overview—Prudence, principles and practice.” In Maher, M. J., and I. Summersgill. 1996. “A comprehensive methodology
Owning the future: Integrated risk management in practice, edited by for the fitting of predictive accident models.” Accid. Anal. Prev. 28 (3):
D. Elms. Christchurch, NZ: Centre for Advanced Engineering, Univ. of 281–296. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-4575(95)00059-3.
Canterbury. Menzies, A., and A. J. Nicholson. 2003. “Analysing the amber dilemma
Elvik, R. 2015. “Some implications of an event-based definition of expo- problem using risk analysis techniques.” In Proc., Institution of Pro-
sure to the risk of road accident.” Accid. Anal. Prev. 76 (Mar): 15–24. fessional Engineers NZ Transportation Group Conf. Wellington,
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2014.12.011. New Zealand: Institution of Professional Engineers New Zealand.
EPA. 1988. Seven cardinal rules of risk communication. Washington, DC: Ministry of Transport. 2003. Road safety to 2010. Wellington, New Zealand:
EPA. Ministry of Transport.
Evans, A. W. 1994. “Evaluating public transport and road safety measures.” Murphy, S. L., J. Xu, K. D. Kochanek, S. C. Curtin, and E. Arias.
Accid. Anal. Prev. 26 (4): 411–428. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-4575 2017. Deaths: Final data for 2015. National Vital Statistics Report.
(94)90033-7. Hyattsville, MD: Dept. of Health and Human Services.
Evans, L. 1985. “Human behaviour feedback and traffic safety.” Hum. Fac- New Zealand Automobile Association. 2010. How safe are our roads:
tors 27 (5): 555–576. https://doi.org/10.1177/001872088502700505. Star rating New Zealand’s state highways. Auckland, New Zealand:
Fischhoff, B., P. Slovic, and S. Lichtenstein. 1982. “Lay foibles and expert New Zealand Automobile Association.
fables in judgments about risk.” Am. Statistician 36 (3): 240–255. New Zealand Automobile Association. 2012. How safe are our Roads:
https://doi.org/10.2307/2683835. Tracking the safety performance of New Zealand’s state highway net-
Fischhoff, B., P. Slovic, S. Lichtenstein, S. J. Read, and B. Combs. 1978. work. Auckland, New Zealand: New Zealand Automobile Association.
“How safe is safe enough? A psychometric study of attitudes towards Nicholson, A. J. 2002. “Road safety: A risk evaluation and management
technological risks and benefits.” Policy Sci. 9 (2): 127–152. https://doi perspective.” In Proc., Int. Conf. of Institute of Transportation Engi-
.org/10.1007/BF00143739. neers. Auburn, VIC, Australia: Institute of Transportation Engineers.
Ford, R. 2000. “Risk, perception and the cold numbers.” Modern Railways, Sabey, B. E. 1983. “Road safety in the 80’s.” In Proc., Symp. on Recent
October 10, 2000. Developments and Research in Road Safety Remedial Measures.
Fuller, R. 2000. “The task-capability interface model of the driving Manchester, UK: Univ. of Salford.
process.” Recherche Transports Sécurité 66 (Jan–Mar): 47–57. https:// Sabey, B. E., and G. C. Staughton. 1975. “Interacting roles of road
doi.org/10.1016/S0761-8980(00)90006-2. environment, vehicle and road user in accidents.” In Proc., 5th Int. Conf.
Fuller, R. 2008. “What drives the driver? Surface tensions and hidden con- of Int. Association for Accident and Traffic Medicine. Stockholm,
sensus.” In Proc., 4th Int. Conf. on Traffic and Transport Psychology. Sweden: International Association for Accident and Traffic Medicine.
Indianapolis: International Association of Applied Psychology. Sandman, P. M. 1993. Responding to community outrage: Strategies for
Gazis, D. C., R. Herman, and A. A. Maradudin. 1960. “The problem of the effective risk communication. Fairfax, VA: American Industrial Hygiene
amber signal in traffic flow.” Oper. Res. 8 (1): 112–132. https://doi.org Association.
/10.1287/opre.8.1.112. Slovic, P. 1987. “Perception of risk.” Science 236 (4799): 280–285. https://
Haight, F. A. 1986. “Risk, especially the risk of traffic accident.” Accid. doi.org/10.1126/science.3563507.
Anal. Prev. 18 (5): 359–366. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-4575(86) Slovic, P., M. L. Finucane, E. Peters, and D. G. MacGregor. 2007. “The
90009-6. affect heuristic.” Eur. J. Oper. Res. 177 (3): 1333–1352. https://doi.org
Harwood, D. W., D. J. Torbic, K. R. Richard, and M. M. Meyer. /10.1016/j.ejor.2005.04.006.
2010. Safety analyst: Software tools for safety management of specific Starr, C. 1969. “Social benefit versus technological risk. What is our society
highway sites. Rep. No. FHWA-HRT-10-063. Washington, DC: Federal willing to pay for safety?” Science 165 (3899): 1232–1238. https://doi
Highway Administration. .org/10.1126/science.165.3899.1232.
Hauer, E. 1982. “Traffic conflicts and exposure.” Accid. Anal. Prev. 14 (5): Taig, T. 1998. “Risk, responsibility and decision—Good practice and good
359–364. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-4575(82)90014-8. business. In Owning the future: Integrated risk management in practice,

© ASCE 04019017-9 ASCE-ASME J. Risk Uncertainty Eng. Syst., Part A: Civ. Eng.

ASCE-ASME J. Risk Uncertainty Eng. Syst., Part A: Civ. Eng., 2020, 6(1): 04019017
edited by D. Elms. Christchurch, New Zealand: Centre for Advanced Vlakveld, W. 2015. Transition of control in highly automated vehicles.
Engineering, Univ. of Canterbury. The Hague, Netherlands: SWOV Institute for Road Safety Research.
Tefft, B. C. 2017. Rates of motor vehicle crashes, injuries and deaths in Von Winterfeldt, D., and W. Edwards. 1986. Decision analysis and behav-
relation to driver age, United States, 2014–2015. Washington, DC: ioural research. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press.
AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety. Washington, A. J. 2015. The ability to quantify rural road crash risk in
Treat, J. R. 1980. “A study of pre-crash factors involved in traffic New Zealand using probabilistic risk assessment methods. Master
accidents.” HSRI Res. Rev. 10 (6): 1–35. of Engineering in Transportation Research Report. Christchurch,
Turner, S. A., and A. J. Nicholson. 1998. “Intersection accident estimation: New Zealand: Univ. of Canterbury.
The role of intersection location and non-collision flows.” Accid. Anal. Wilde, G. J. S. 1982. “The theory of risk homeostasis: Implications for
Prev. 30 (4): 505–517. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0001-4575(97)00095-X. safety and health.” Risk Anal. 2 (4): 209–225. https://doi.org/10.1111/j
Vissers, L., S. van der Kint, I. van Schagen, and M. Hagenzieker. 2016. .1539-6924.1982.tb01384.x.
Safe interaction between cyclists, pedestrians and automated vehicles. Wong, Y.-D., and A. J. Nicholson. 1992. “Driver behaviour at horizontal
Rep. No. R-2016-16. The Hague, Netherlands: SWOV Institute for curves: Risk compensation and the margin of safety.” Accid. Anal. Prev.
Road Safety Research. 24 (4): 425–436. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-4575(92)90053-L.
Downloaded from ascelibrary.org by Universitas Indonesia on 12/09/19. Copyright ASCE. For personal use only; all rights reserved.

© ASCE 04019017-10 ASCE-ASME J. Risk Uncertainty Eng. Syst., Part A: Civ. Eng.

ASCE-ASME J. Risk Uncertainty Eng. Syst., Part A: Civ. Eng., 2020, 6(1): 04019017

You might also like