You are on page 1of 9

Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Copyright 1998 by the Educational Publishing Foundation

1998, Vol. 2, No. 1,48-56 1089-2699/98/S3.00

Group Norms and Attitude-Behavior Consistency: The Role of Group


Salience and Mood
Jackie M. Wellen, Michael A. Hogg, and Deborah J. Terry
University of Queensland

This study tested the social identity-self-categorization theory reconceptualization of


the role of norms in attitude-behavior relations. Specifically, the study investigated how
the effects of in-group norms on the relationship between people's attitudes and their
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

behavior vary as a function of the salience of group membership and mood. Partici-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

pants' (N= 131) attitudes toward students being responsible for picking up litter on
campus grounds were examined. As expected, the effects of the attitudinal congruency
of norms varied as a function of group salience under neutral mood (i.e., deliberative
processing) conditions. In-group norms were more influential for high-salience indivi-
duals than for low salience individuals in a neutral mood. These findings indicate
that in-group norms influence behavioral decision making for individuals high in
group salience only when there is an opportunity to carefully process the normative
information.

When people get together in groups, social and view them as having largely additive effects.
interaction produces norms that regulate behav- As an applied example, the theory of reasoned
ior (Sherif, 1936; Turner, 1991), a process that is action-planned behavior would propose that
applicable to small face-to-face groups (e.g., people who intend to quit smoking are likely to
sports teams) in which people interact directly evaluate quitting as a favorable behavior (atti-
with one another as well as large-scale social tude) and perceive that significant others would
categories (e.g., ethnic groups) in which "inter- endorse their decision to give up smoking
action" is often indirect and channeled through (subjective norm).
mass media. Group norms are emergent proper- Although these theories have been influential
ties of groups. They influence people even when in the study of attitude-behavior relations,
other group members are not present, and thus research reveals that the link between norms and
they are cognitively stored as attitudes about behavioral intentions is much weaker than the
beliefs, feelings, and behaviors. link between attitudes and intentions; norms
Exactly how such attitudes influence behavior only weakly influence people's intentions to
is one of the chief questions facing contempo- behave in certain ways (see Ajzen, 1991). One
rary social psychology (e.g., Eagly & Chaiken, reason for this apparent lack of influence of
1993; Terry & Hogg, in press). Probably the best norms on behavior may be that Fishbein and
known attempt at answering this question has Ajzen (1975) conceptualized norms in an
been made by the theory of reasoned action additive fashion (a norm exists if a number of
(Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975) and its recent significant others endorse a particular course of
extension, the theory of planned behavior action) and viewed norms and attitudes as
(Ajzen, 1991). These theories separate attitudes independently influencing behavior.
(individual factors) from norms (social factors) Norms can, however, be conceived in a
different way that is tied more closely to
contextually salient membership in specific
Jackie M. Wellen, Michael A. Hogg, and Deborah J. Terry,
School of Psychology, University of Queensland, Brisbane, social groups. According to social identity
Queensland, Australia. theory and self-categorization theory (e.g.,
Thanks are due to Anne Wellen, who helped with the Hogg & Abrams, 1988; Turner, 1982; Turner,
collection of the data. Hogg, Oakes, Reicher, & Wetherell, 1987),
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad- groups influence people's attitudes and behav-
dressed to Jackie M. Wellen, School of Psychology,
University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland 4072, iors because the process of psychologically
Australia. Electronic mail may be sent to jackie@psy.uq.edu.au. belonging to a group involves categorization of
48
NORM CONGRUENCY AND ATTITUDE-BEHAVIOR RELATIONS 49

oneself as a group member (i.e., group identifi- contrasts with other current approaches to
cation), which in turn transforms one's self- normative influence. For example, Miller and
concept and concomitant attitudes, feelings, and Prentice (1996; Miller, Monin, & Prentice, in
behavior such that they are consistent with, or press) have emphasized the importance of the
assimilated, by the group prototype (i.e., the immediate context and associated cognitive
cognitive representation of the group norm). representations in the construction of group
Norms are treated as inextricable properties of norms. According to this approach, norms are
social groups derived from shared attitudes and highly context specific and depend largely on
behaviors that have become prescriptive and contextually salient representations. In contrast,
descriptive of group membership. our treatment of norms emphasizes contextually
salient membership in specific social groups and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Building on social identity theory and self-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

categorization theory (e.g., Hogg & Abrams, regards the content of norms as emanating from
1988; Turner, 1982; Turner et al., 1987), we group prototypical attitudes and behaviors
have proposed that the attitude-behavior relation- rather than from information available in the
ship becomes significantly stronger under condi- immediate context.
tions in which people categorize themselves and Social identity and self-categorization analy-
identify with an in-group that defines member- ses of group processes and intergroup relations
ship in terms of specific behaviorally and and of the social self have good support (eg.,
attitudinally prescriptive norms (Hogg, 1996; Abrams & Hogg, 1990; Hogg, 1992; Hogg &
Terry & Hogg, 1996; White, Terry, & Hogg, Abrams, 1988; Oakes, Haslam, & Turner, 1994;
1994). From our perspective, attitudes and Turner, 1991; Turner et al., 1987). The extension
norms are not independent. Attitudes (and to explain attitude-behavior relations as a
behaviors) become normative to the extent that function of normative depersonalization is
they characterize group membership, and they relatively new but already has some initial
influence people to the extent that the group they support (see Terry, Hogg, & White, in press).
characterize is the contextually salient in-group For example, in two tests of the theory of
in terms of which people categorize themselves planned behavior, Terry and Hogg (1996) found
(i.e., identify). Our treatment of attitudes that the perceived norms of a behaviorally
diverges from the definition used by Fishbein relevant reference group (friends and peers at a
and Ajzen (1975) largely in terms of the scope of university) predicted intentions to engage in
the concept; we envisage that attitudes can be regular exercise (Study 1) and women's inten-
personal, idiosyncratic judgments of the favor- tions to engage in sun-protective behaviors
ableness of a behavior, but they can also be (Study 2), but only for people who identified
socially shared and tied to specific group strongly with the group. For people who did not
memberships, in which case they are perceived identify strongly with the group, personal
to be normative (i.e., part of the content of group factors (perceived behavioral control in Study 1
norms). In applying our example of quitting and attitude in Study 2) were more influential
smoking to social identity-self-categorization than norms in predicting behavior. Using a
theory, we would propose that individuals may slightly different but related theoretical perspec-
be more likely to give up smoking if they tive, Trafimow and Finlay (1996) obtained
perceive that a salient in-group with whom they similar findings in a within-subjects analysis of
strongly identify would provide normative the relative importance of attitudes and subjec-
support for not smoking. If the general group tive norms in the prediction of behavioral
attitude toward smoking is negative and individu- intentions; norms were more influential than
als believe that most group members do not attitudes for people who had a stronger "collec-
smoke, their own attitudes and behavior should tive self" (i.e., cognitions about group member-
gradually align with the group norm to the ship were more salient than cognitions about
extent that the group provides an important basis people's own traits and states). The study
for self-definition. reported in this article sought to replicate Terry
and Hogg's (1996) prior research by providing a
In addition to differing from more traditional further test of the proposed reconceptualization
attitude-behavior theory (Ajzen, 1991; Fishbein of the role of norms in attitude-behavior
& Ajzen, 1975), our conceptualization of the relations along the lines suggested by social
role of norms in attitude-behavior relations also
50 WELLEN, HOGG, AND TERRY

identity-self-categorization theory. In addition, processing, as suggested by the MODE model


the present study developed this research further (Fazio, 1990). Specifically, when group member-
by incorporating a consideration of the role of ship has low personal relevance, norms should
mode of processing of attitudinal and normative have the most impact when normative informa-
information in attitude-behavior relations as a tion is spontaneously processed; when group
function of social identity salience. membership has high personal relevance, norms
Fazio (1986, 1990) has developed a theory of should have the most impact when normative
how mode of processing affects the relationships information is deliberatively processed. If the
among attitudes, norms, and behavior. The group membership is highly self-relevant, people
MODE (motivation and opportunity as determi- will be motivated to scrutinize the information
effortfully. In support of this idea, Forgas and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

nants of mode of behavioral decision making)


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

model distinguishes between two modes of Fiedler (1996) demonstrated that, under low
processing: (a) spontaneous processing, in personal relevance, positive mood participants
which low levels of motivation or ability cause (i.e., spontaneous processing) showed more
people to rely on context cues and other intergroup discrimination, and when relevance
heuristics, and (b) deliberative processing, was high, negative mood individuals (i.e.,
which involves careful and effortful consider- deliberative processing) showed more discrimi-
ation of all available information under condi- nation. Again, in applying our example of
tions of high motivation and ability. On the basis quitting smoking, this perspective would pro-
of Fazio's (1990) MODE model, it is possible to pose that when normative pressure to give up
derive predictions concerning the relative impor- smoking emanates from a group with which the
tance of attitudes and norms in behavioral person strongly identifies, it is likely to be more
decision making. Spontaneous processing is influential when the person is in a neutral mood
proposed to lead to the automatic activation of a (and presumably engages in systematic process-
relevant attitude from memory; thus, attitudes, ing). However, when social pressure to quit
to the extent that they are accessible, should be smoking emanates from a less important source,
more influential in determining behavior. When it is likely to have more impact when the
deliberative processing occurs, norms are likely individual is in a positive mood.
to be more influential because all sources of
The present study drew on ideas about mode
available information are considered. Empirical
of processing and behavioral decision making,
tests of the MODE model (Fazio, 1990) support
as well as Forgas's (1995; Forgas & Fiedler,
the assumption that degree of processing
1996) work on mood, to test our own social
influences the extent to which attitudes, or other
identity-self-categorization model of the role of
specific attribute information, guide behavior
norms in attitude-behavior relations. Building
(see Sanbonmatsu & Fazio, 1990; Schuette &
on Terry and Hogg's (1996) original study, in
Fazio, 1995).
which levels of identification with the in-group
One factor that has been shown to affect mode were measured, we also manipulated the sa-
of processing is mood (Forgas, 1989; Stroessner, lience of group membership in relation to an
Hamilton, & Mackie, 1992); positive mood is assumed behaviorally relevant reference group.
associated with spontaneous processing, and Furthermore, we included an additional experi-
neutral and negative moods are associated with mental manipulation of mode of processing by
deliberative processing (Bless, Bohner, Schwarz, inducing either positive or neutral moods in
& Strack, 1990). A model of how mood may participants (presumed to engender spontaneous
affect behavioral decision making has been and deliberative modes of processing). Our
proposed by Forgas (1995; Forgas & Fiedler, participants and the focal group members were
1996). Forgas's (1995; Forgas & Fiedler, 1996) local university students, and the focal attitude-
work integrates mood-related differences in behavior was picking up litter on campus.
processing with ideas related to social identity Participants had the salience of their group
and self-categorization theory. This view pro- membership raised or lowered, were placed in a
poses that the effects of norms should vary as a positive or a neutral mood, and were provided
function of both the personal relevance of group with in-group normative information that was
membership, in line with the social identity-self- congruent or incongruent with their own atti-
categorization perspective, and the mode of tude. Attitude-behavior consistency on a num-
NORM CONGRUENCY AND ATTITUDE-BEHAVIOR RELATIONS 51

ber of behavioral measures served as the attitude that emerged from the normative
dependent variable. On the basis of Forgas's information. Three versions of each set of
(1995; Forgas & Fiedler, 1996) work, we normative information were generated to con-
predicted that the effect of norms on attitude- trol for order effects.
behavior consistency would be strongest for Mode of processing was manipulated by
high salience participants when they were in a inducing either positive or neutral moods.
neutral mood. Participants viewed 5-min video segments from
either a popular comedy series (positive mood
condition) or a documentary (neutral mood
Method
condition). As a check for the effectiveness of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Introductory psychology students from a the mood manipulation, participants rated their
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

large Australian university (N = 131; 29 men, current mood on three 9-point semantic differen-
and 102 women) participated in a laboratory tials (happy-unhappy, bad-good, and pleasant-
experiment involving a 2 (low salience vs. unpleasant); see Wegener, Petty, & Smith,
high salience) X 2 (positive mood vs. neutral 1995). The items were combined into a measure
mood) X 2 (norm congruent vs. norm incongru- of mood (a = .88).
ent) between-subjects design. The research was Participants were then asked to move to a
introduced as a study of general student and different room in an adjoining building to
campus issues, and participants completed a complete the study. While they waited, individu-
four-item pretest measure of their attitude ally, in a specific area of a courtyard where some
toward the focal issue of students being litter had been placed near a bin, an assistant
responsible for picking up litter on campus unobtrusively observed their behavior for ap-
(9-point semantic differentials: good-bad, un- proximately 15 s and recorded their reactions to
pleasant—pleasant, undesirable-desirable, favor- the litter using a 3-point Guttman-type scale
able-unfavorable). The items were combined (1 = did not notice litter, 2 = looked at litter,
into a measure of initial attitude (a = .84). 3 = picked up litter). Three independent assis-
There was also a binary item asking participants tants, unaware of participants' assigned experi-
whether they opposed (rated as 1) or supported mental conditions, were involved in obtaining
(rated as 2) students being responsible for the measure of Utter behavior, and each received
picking up litter on campus; this item allowed us training in the use of the scoring method before
to unambiguously classify participants and the study. Attitude-behavior consistency was
subsequently distribute appropriate materials to measured by recoding participants' scores in
manipulate norm congruency. terms of their initial preference on the oppose-
Group salience was manipulated via a self- support item (1 = no attitude—behavior consis-
description task. We instructed participants in tency, 3 = strong attitude-behavior consistency).
the high salience condition to consider their In this second room, we administered another
identity as psychology students (an assumed questionnaire, ostensibly from the Students'
behaviorally relevant reference group) and to Union, which was interested in students' atti-
describe how they were similar to other tudes toward a number of campus issues. This
psychology students. Participants in the low questionnaire served as a quasi-behavioral
salience condition considered their identity as measure of participants' willingness to volunteer
unique individuals and described what made time (i.e., none, extra 1 hour, extra 5 hours,
them stand out as individuals. extra 10 hours, extra 20 hours, or unlimited) to a
Participants in the norm-congruent condition project aimed at reducing litter on campus (see
were presented with eight statements, ostensibly Fazio & Zanna, 1978). Responses were recoded
from other introductory psychology students. to form a binary measure of volunteering
Seven of the statements supported their attitude behavior (1 = did not volunteer, 2 = did volun-
toward the focal issue, and one statement was teer). Attitude—behavior consistency was mea-
attitudinally incongruent. Participants in the sured by expressing the score in terms of
norm-incongruent condition were presented whether participants' behavior was inconsistent
with seven attitudinally incongruent statements with (rated as 1) or consistent with (rated as 2)
and one statement that supported their attitude. their initial preference on the binary oppose-
We asked participants to summarize the general support scale.
52 WELLEN, HOGG, AND TERRY

The final questionnaire assessed participants' congruent vs. norm incongruent) between-
willingness to engage in three litter-related groups analyses of variance (ANOVAs) on the
behaviors (e.g., becoming a member of an action manipulation check items established the effi-
group that meets to pick up litter on campus, cacy of the experimental manipulations. The
attending a rally about the importance of main analyses involved the same 2 X 2 X 2
students taking responsibility for litter on between-groups ANOVAs on the three measures
campus, and having their name given to the of attitude-behavior consistency (i.e., litter
coordinator of an action group aimed at keeping behavior, volunteering behavior, and willing-
the campus tidy) on a scale ranging from not ness to engage in litter-related behaviors).
willing at all (1) to very willing (9; a = .87). An additional series of analyses in which
Scores were recoded to obtain a measure of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

perceived identification as a psychology student


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

attitude-behavior consistency (1 = weak atti- (dichotomized on the basis of a median split)


tude-behavior consistency, 9 = strong attitude- was substituted for group salience enabled a
behavior consistency) depending on partici- more direct assessment of the role of identifica-
pants' initial preference on the oppose-support tion with a relevant group in the adoption of
scale. The three willingness items were com- group norms. Theoretically, identification, rather
bined to form a composite measure (a = .91). than group salience, is the crucial variable that
Thefinalquestionnaire also contained manipu- should determine the impact of norms on
lation checks. As a context-specific check on the attitudes and behavior. These analyses com-
salience manipulation, participants indicated prised 2 (low identifiers vs. high identifiers) X 2
how often during the study their thoughts were (positive mood vs. neutral mood) X 2 (norm
drawn to their status as psychology students congruent vs. norm incongruent) between-
(1 = not at all, 9 = all the time). Six additional groups ANOVAs on each dependent measure.
items were used to obtain a measure of For the purpose of analysis, the continuous
perceived group identification (based on those identification variable was dichotomized (on the
used by Hogg, Cooper-Shaw, & Holzworth, basis of a median split) to form groups of high
1993). These items were designed to assess the and low identifiers. Although use of the
extent to which being a psychology student was dichotomized variable could have limited the
a relatively more enduring component of power of the analysis to detect possible effects
participants' self-concepts (e.g., "How much do due to identification, this was believed to be
you feel strong ties with other psychology necessary to enhance the comparability of any
students?" [1 = not very much, 9 = very much] findings for group salience and identification.
and "How much do you feel yourself as
Preliminary checks revealed that there were
belonging to the group of people who study
no differences among cells in initial attitude
psychology?" [1 = very much so, 9 = not at
all]). Factor analysis of these six items indicated toward the focal issue or in terms of gender.
that five of them combined well (the exception Because of the small number of male partici-
was an item that asked participants how much pants, analyses could not be conducted to
they liked other psychology students) to produce determine whether sex, in interaction with the
an identification scale (a = .80). Finally, as a independent variables, was related to attitudinal
means of assessing the efficacy of the norm- or behavioral responses.
congruency manipulation, participants rated the
extent of similarity between their attitude
toward the focal issue and the attitude among Manipulation Checks
lst-year students (1 = not similar at all, 9 = ex-
tremely similar). Norm congruency. A significant main effect
for norm congruency emerged on the measure of
participants' perceived consistency between
Results their initial attitude and the group norm, F(l,
122) = 39.75, p< .001, if = .25. Participants
Data Analysis Overview in the norm-congruent condition perceived
greater consistency between their attitude and
Two (low salience vs. high salience) X 2 the group norm (M = 6.95) than participants in
(positive mood vs. neutral mood) X 2 (norm the norm-incongruent condition (Af = 4.71).
NORM CONGRUENCY AND ATTITUDE-BEHAVIOR RELATIONS 53

Mood. A significant main effect for mood


emerged from the analysis of the composite
score for mood, F(l, 122) = 24.42, p < .001,
•n2 = .17. Individuals in the positive mood
condition rated their current mood as signifi- Low Salience
cantly more positive (Af = 7.48) than individu-
als in the neutral mood condition (M = 6.25). High Salience
Group salience. There was also a signifi-
cant main effect for group salience on the
salience manipulation check item, F(l, 122) =
4.81, p < .05, TI2 = .04; in comparison with
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

participants in the low salience condition


(Af = 3.59), participants in the high salience
condition indicated that their status as psychol-
ogy students was emphasized more frequently
throughout the study (Af = 4.50).
Norm- Norm-
congruent incongruent
Effects of Group Salience, Norm
Nonn-congruency
Congruency, and Mood-Mode
of Processing Figure 1. Effects of group salience and norm congruency
on attitude-behavior consistency in participants in the
Analysis of the measure of litter behavior neutral mood condition on the measure of volunteering to
revealed a significant main effect for salience participate in a litter project.
only, F(l, 122) = 5.74, p < .05, TI2 = .04.
Participants in the high salience condition there was a tendency for low salience partici-
(Af = 2.05) displayed greater attitude-behavior pants in a neutral mood to show increased
consistency than participants in the low salience attitude-behavior consistency when they were
condition (Af = 1.88). There were no main or provided with norm-incongruent information.
interactive effects for the ANOVA conducted on These participants may have behaved more in
willingness to engage in litter-related behaviors. accordance with their attitudes in an attempt to
The ANOVA conducted on volunteering psychologically distance themselves from a
behavior revealed a significant main effect for relatively unimportant group whose members
norm congruency, F(l, 121) = 4.84, p < .05, were perceived to hold contrasting attitudes.
•n2 •= .04. Participants in the norm-congruent A similar pattern of findings emerged from
condition (M = 1.66) displayed greater attitude- the analysis of the measure of willingness to
behavior consistency than participants in the engage in litter-related behaviors (Figure 1).
norm-incongruent condition (Af = 1.47). There There was a significant Identification X Mood X
was also a significant Salience X Mood X Norm Norm Congruency interaction, F(l, 122) =
Congruency interaction, F(l, 121) = 5.56, p < 4.37, p < .05, T|2 = .03. Further analysis
.05, T\2 = .04. Further analysis revealed a revealed that the interaction between identifica-
significant Salience X Norm Congruency inter- tion and norm congruency was significant under
action for participants in a neutral mood, F(l, neutral mood conditions, F(l, 122) = 4.61, p <
121) = 5.52, p < .05, if = .04, but not for .05, T|2 = .04, but not under positive mood
participants in a positive mood. For participants conditions. The pattern of means for high and
in a neutral mood, who had presumably engaged low identifiers was similar to the pattern for high
in deliberative processing, exposure to attitudi- and low salience individuals in the previous
nally inconsistent normative information de- three-way interaction; however, significant dif-
creased attitude-behavior consistency more for ferences emerged when individuals were ex-
participants in the high salience condition posed to norm-congruent rather than to norm-
(Af = 1.27) than for participants in the low incongruent information. As expected, high
salience condition (Af=1.71), F(l, 121) = identifiers (Af = 6.10) displayed greater attitude-
6.77,p<.Ol,T| 2 = .O5. behavior consistency than low identifiers
Although a significant effect was not found, (Af = 4.07) when they processed attitudinally
54 WELLEN, HOGG, AND TERRY

congruent normative information in a neutral was personally relevant, participants engaged in


mood. Hence, participants in the neutral mood more intergroup discrimination when they were
condition, who had presumably engaged in in a neutral mood rather than a positive mood.
deliberative processing of attitudinally consis- Building on previous research by Terry and
tent normative information, were more likely to Hogg (1996), the present study demonstrated
behave in accordance with their attitudes when that norms had more impact on attitude-
they strongly identified with the group than behavior relations for high salience individuals
when their levels of perceived identification under neutral mood (i.e., deliberative process-
were lower. ing) conditions. Theoretically, these findings
provide support for a link between a number of
important theories in social psychology that
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Discussion
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

have each separately been extensively re-


The purpose of the study reported in this searched but have rarely been investigated
article was to examine the effects of salience of interdependently. Social identity-self-categori-
group membership as a moderator of the effects zation theory provided the fundamental frame-
of in-group norms on attitude-behavior rela- work for the perspective adopted in the present
tions under positive and neutral mood condi- article. In line with social identity-self-
tions (proposed to engender spontaneous and categorization theory, norms received renewed
deliberative modes of processing). Specifically, recognition as an important factor in attitude-
the study was designed to test the prediction, behavior relations, and the critical importance of
derived from research by Forgas and his die salience of group membership was acknowl-
colleagues (Forgas, 1995; Forgas & Fiedler, edged. Fazio's (1990) MODE model provided
1996), that the effect of norms should be most recognition of the implications of mode of
marked for high salience participants under processing for the impact of norms on attitude-
neutral mood (i.e., deliberative processing) behavior relations. Recent work by Forgas
conditions. (1995; Forgas & Fiedler, 1996) on how the
effect of norms varies as a function of mood
Findings from the present study provided
(i.e., mode of processing) and the personal
support for this prediction; norms were found to
relevance of group membership provided a
have the most impact on attitude-behavior
theoretical framework to allow the integration of
consistency for high salience individuals in a
social-identity-related concepts and research on
neutral mood. On the measure of volunteering
mode of processing.
behavior, high salience individuals showed
greater reduction in their attitude-behavior Overall, the present study demonstrates that
consistency than low salience individuals when when an individual's social identity is based on
they were provided with attitudinally incongru- a salient category membership, information
ent norms under neutral mood conditions. from the social category—represented as group
Further support for this finding emerged in a norms—is an important source of information
supplementary analysis of the willingness mea- about the self. Hence, it is likely that, when high
sure in which identification was substituted for salience individuals are exposed to group
group salience. High identifiers showed greater norms, they become motivated to systematically
attitude-behavior consistency than low identifi- contemplate the information and are provided
ers when they were presented with attitudinally with the opportunity to do so only when
congruent norms under neutral mood condi- conditions favor deliberative processing. Under
tions. Although the three-way interactions dif- positive mood conditions, high salience individu-
fered in terms of the effect of the attitudinal als are impeded in both their ability and
congruence of norms (possibly as a result of die motivation to process normative information in
measure of volunteering behavior being more the preferred manner, which means that norma-
closely related to the behavioral criterion), both tive information is ignored.
interactions demonstrated that norms were more There were a number of limitations to the
influential for high salience individuals under design of our study. First, it was difficult to
neutral mood conditions. This finding is consis- determine die extent of normative influence for
tent with results reported by Forgas and Fiedler individuals in the norm-congruent condition;
(1996), who found that when group membership these participants received normative informa-
NORM CONGRUENCY AND ATTITUDE-BEHAVIOR RELATIONS 55

tion that was similar to their attitude. Future behavior? In R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins
studies would benefit from the inclusion of a (Eds.), The handbood of motivation and cognition:
no-norm condition. Second, the assumed link Foundations of social behavior (pp. 204-243).
between mood and mode of processing adopted New York: Guilford Press.
in the present study was based on theory and Fazio, R. H. (1990). Multiple processes by which
attitudes guide behavior: The MODE model as an
past research. Future research is required to
integrative framework. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.),
further establish the nature of mood implications Advances in experimental psychology (Vol. 23, pp.
for mode of processing, especially in relation to 75-109). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
behavioral decision making. Fazio, R. H , & Zanna, M. P. (1978). Attitudinal
Nevertheless, the results are important in that qualities relating to the strength of the attitude-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

they provide further evidence that norms behavior relationship. Journal of Experimental
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

emerging from a salient in-group do influence Social Psychology, 14, 389-408.


the degree of attitude-behavior consistency for Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude,
in-group members (see Terry & Hogg, 1996). intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory
Also, this research was extended by showing and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
that such an effect is most marked under neutral Forgas, J. P. (1989). Mood effects on decision making
mood conditions, which are assumed to engen- strategies. Australian Journal of Psychology, 41,
197-214.
der deliberative processing of the normative
Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgement: The affect
information. Further investigation of the mode infusion model (ATM). Psychological Bulletin,
of processing implications for the social identity 117, 39-66.
theory conceptualization of the role of norms Forgas, J. P., & Fiedler, K. (1996). Us and them:
provides a new direction for future research on Mood effects on intergroup discrimination. Journal
attitude-behavior relations (see Hogg & Terry, of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 28-40.
in press). These findings have important practi- Hogg, M. A. (1992). The social psychology of group
cal implications for all types of social interac- cohesiveness: From attraction to social identity.
tions involving groups. In a clinical context, Hemel Hempstead, England: Harvester Wheat-
these findings are especially informative about sheaf.
the functioning of therapy groups. Normative Hogg, M. A. (1996). Intragroup processes, group
support from fellow group members in group structure and social identity. In W. P. Robinson
therapy situations may be effective only for (Ed.), Social groups and identities: Developing the
legacy of Henri Tajfel (pp. 65-93). Oxford,
those who strongly identify with the group (i.e.,
England: Butterworth-Heinemann.
being a member of the therapy group is an
Hogg, M. A., & Abrams, D. (1988). Social identifica-
important basis for self-definition). The effect of tions: A social psychology of intergroup relations
such group norms is likely to diminish for and group processes. London: Routledge.
individuals who do not categorize themselves as Hogg, M. A., Cooper-Shaw, L., & Holzworth, D. W.
group members. (1993). Group prototypicality and depersonalized
attraction in small interactive groups. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 452-^65.
References Hogg, M. A., & Terry, D. J. (in press). Social
contextual influences on attitude-behavior corre-
Abrams, D., & Hogg, M. A. (1990). Social identifica- spondence, attitude change and persuasion. In D. J.
tion, self-categorization, and social influence. Terry & M. A. Hogg (Eds.), Attitudes, behavior and
European Review of Social Psychology, 1, 195- social context: The role of norms and group
228. membership. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Miller, D. T, Monin, B., & Prentice, D. A. (in press).
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Pluralistic ignorance and inconsistency between
Processes, 50, 179-211. private altitudes and public behaviors. In D. J.
Bless, H., Bohner, G., Schwarz, N., & Strack, F. Terry & M. A. Hogg (Eds.), Attitudes, behavior and
(1990). Mood and persuasion: A cognitive response social context: The role of norms and group
analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulle- membership. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
tin, 16, 331-345. Miller, D. T, & Prentice, D. A. (1996). The
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of construction of social norms and standards. In E. T.
attitudes. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace Jovanov- Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social
ich. psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp.
Fazio, R. H. (1986). How do attitudes guide 799-829). London: Guilford Press.
56 WELLEN, HOGG, AND TERRY

Oakes, P. J., Haslam, S. A., & Turner, J. C. (1994). group membership. In D. J. Terry & M. A. Hogg
Stereotyping and social reality. Oxford, England: (Eds.), Attitudes, behavior and social context: The
Blackwell. role of norms and group membership. Mahwah,
Sanbonmatsu, D. M , & Fazio, R. H. (1990). The role NJ: Erlbaum.
of attitudes in memory-based decision making. Trafimow, D., & Finlay, K. A. (1996). The importance
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, of subjective norms for a minority of people.
614-622. Between-subjects and within-subjects analyses.
Schuette, R. A., & Fazio, R. H. (1995). Attitude Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22,
accessibility and motivation as determinants of 820-828.
biased processing: A test of the MODE model. Turner, J. C. (1982). Towards a cognitive redefinition
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21,
of the social group. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), Social
704-710.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

identity and intergroup relations (pp. 15-40).


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Sherif, M. (1936). The psychology of social norms.


Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
New York: Harper.
Stroessner, S. J., Hamilton, D. L., & Mackie, D. M. Turner, J. C. (1991). Social influence. Milton Keynes,
(1992). Affect and stereotyping: The effect of England: Open University Press.
induced mood on distinctiveness-based illusory Turner, J. C , Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher,
correlations. Journal of Personality and Social S. D., & Wetherell, M. S. (1987). Rediscovering the
Psychology, 62, 564-576. social group: A self-categorization theory. Oxford,
Terry, D. J., & Hogg, M. A. (1996). Group norms and England: Blackwell.
the attitude-behavior relationship: A role for group Wegener, D. T., Petty, R. E., & Smith, S. M. (1995).
identification. Personality and Social Psychology Positive mood can increase or decrease message
Bulletin, 22, 776-793. scrutiny: The hedonic contingency view of mood
Terry, D. J., & Hogg, M. A. (Eds.), (in press). and message processing. Journal of Personality
Attitudes, behavior and social context: The role of and Social Psychology, 69, 5-15.
norms and group membership. Mahwah, NJ: White, K. M., Terry, D. J., & Hogg, M. A. (1994).
Erlbaum. Safer sex behavior: The role of attitudes, norms,
Terry, D. J., Hogg, M. A., & White, K. M. (in press). and control factors. Journal of Applied Social
Attitude-behavior relations: Social identity and Psychology, 24, 2164-2192.

You might also like